The Role of Pragmatic Language Use in

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

JSLHR

Article

The Role of Pragmatic Language Use in


Mediating the Relation Between Hyperactivity
and Inattention and Social Skills Problems
Melinda A. Leonard,a Richard Milich,b and Elizabeth P. Lorchb

Purpose: In the present study, the authors explored whether problems. Further, pragmatic language use provided a unique
pragmatic language use was associated with, and perhaps contribution in the estimate of children’s social skills of 21.6%
accounted for, the social skills problems that children with varying above and beyond the contribution of hyperactivity and 17.2%
levels of hyperactivity and inattention experience. above and beyond the contribution of inattention.
Method: A community sample of 54 children aged 9–11 years Conclusions: Possible explanations for these mediation results are
participated. Pragmatic language use, hyperactivity and discussed in terms of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
inattention, and social skills were examined utilizing data disorder and the problems that they experience with social
collected from standardized parent-report rating scales. relations.
Results: Pragmatic language use fully mediated the relation
between hyperactivity and social skills problems and partially Key Words: pragmatic language use, hyperactivity, inattention,
mediated the relation between inattention and social skills social skills, mediation

P
eer relationships are the primary context in which sample of children. To do that, we identified two well-
children learn the social skills (e.g., cooperation, ne- documented “at-risk” dimensions for attention-deficit/
gotiation, and conflict resolution) that are critical hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), hyperactivity, and in-
for effective social functioning throughout life (Rubin, attention, and we examined how well each one predicts
Bukowski, & Parker, 1998). Childhood peer problems social skills problems. More importantly, we investigated
predict a wide variety of later negative outcomes, includ- whether these at-risk dimensions predict pragmatic
ing academic difficulties, delinquency, dropping out of language use (PLU; an aspect of communication) and
school, substance abuse, and psychological maladjust- whether pragmatic language problems may mediate the
ment (Rubin et al.; Parker & Asher, 1987). One factor relations between these at-risk dimensions and social
that may be contributing to social skills problems is defi- skills problems. At-risk symptoms for ADHD were chosen
ciencies in communication. Typically developing children because ADHD has been reliably associated with signifi-
who are good communicators also find it relatively easy cant peer problems that are rapid in onset and stable over
to establish and maintain friendships (Gottman, 1983). time (Whalen & Henker, 1985).
The goal of the present study was to understand factors The bulk of work on communication deficiencies
that lead to the social skills problems of a community among children with and without ADHD has been in
the area of general language impairment—more specifi-
a
University of Louisville, Louisville, KY cally, receptive and expressive language. Receptive lan-
b
University of Kentucky, Louisville, KY guage skill is the ability to understand or comprehend
Correspondence to Melinda A. Leonard: spoken language. Expressive language skill is the ability
melinda.leonard@louisville.edu to communicate thoughts, needs, or wants. Children who
Editor: Janna Oetting are deficient in these skills are not able to communicate at
Associate Editor: Sean Redmond the same level or with the same complexity as their peers.
Received February 26, 2010 Several investigations are consistent in demonstrating no
Revision received June 24, 2010 significant difference in receptive language abilities be-
Accepted July 30, 2010 tween typically developing children and those with ADHD
DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0058) (Barkley, DuPaul, & McMurray, 1990; Kim & Kaiser,

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 567–579 • April 2011 • D American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 567

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
2000). In contrast, impairments in oral expressive abili- implicated in children’s success at making friends and
ties among children with ADHD have been documented being accepted by peers (Hemphill & Siperstein, 1990).
with standardized language tests (Kim & Kaiser, 2000) When interacting with peers, upper elementary school
and storytelling tasks (Purvis & Tannock, 1997). children are able to maintain a topic of conversation; pro-
Communication in social interaction is part of the duce more topic continuations, topic invitations, and re-
pragmatic aspect of language. The pragmatic domain re- sponses; produce fewer silent pauses; and become more
fers to the practical use of language in social interaction proficient at controlling the processes of planning, pro-
(Prutting & Kirchner, 1987). According to Prutting and duction, and comprehension (McLaughlin, 1998).
Kirchner (1987), pragmatics includes topic initiation, topic Appropriate pragmatic communication skills are crit-
maintenance, turn taking, use of context, interruptions, ical in academic tasks requiring cooperative group learn-
amount of talk, intensity (tone and volume), eye contact, ing and nonacademic social occasions (Westby & Cutler,
facial expression, physical proximity, and gestures. Prag- 1994). The children who are able to develop these skills
matic impairments are not restricted to spoken language. are more successful in their social interactions with peers,
A broader definition of pragmatics incorporates behav- family, and teachers (Bierman, 2004). Children who in-
iors that encompass social, emotional, and communicative vite others to play and take turns in dyadic interactions
aspects of social interaction (Adams, Baxendale, Lloyd, are viewed as attractive friends (Gottman, 1983). Success-
& Aldredge, 2005), such as screaming, crying, or yelling ful communicators are able to share information about
dyspragmatically. Not only can the assessment of prag- themselves and their feelings and opinions, and they ask
matics provide a complementary window into aspects of questions to elicit information from others. Thus, these
social and cognitive functioning (Adams, Green, Gilchrist children are able to establish and maintain effective
& Cox, 2002) that observation of nonverbal behaviors friendships (Rubin et al., 1998).
alone cannot, but it can also make a sound contribution
Consideration of implications of pragmatic language
to communication and social intervention strategies for
use may be informative in understanding social diffi-
children with ADHD (Camarata & Gibson, 1999).
culties among children who experience problems with
Pragmatic language development is an ongoing in- hyperactivity and inattention. Empirical research has
teraction in child and peer behaviors. The literature on confirmed that hyperactivity and inattention are associ-
typically developing children’s language shows the be- ated with ADHD, and ADHD has been documented as
ginning of pragmatic competence at a surprisingly early one of the most well-known and stable risk factors for
age. For example, 2-year-olds can adapt their message to problematic peer relationships (Melnick & Hinshaw,
what the listener knows or does not know and can re- 1996; Whalen & Henker, 1985). The primary character-
spond to listener feedback (Furrow, 1984). From 2 years istics that define ADHD in children are developmentally
of age, children can maintain a topic in an interaction inappropriate levels of socially disruptive behavior, inat-
with an adult, and by age 5, children make turn-taking tention, impulsiveness, hyperactivity, or a combination
repairs (Ervin-Tripp, 1979). Between ages 6 and 7, meta- thereof (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994).
pragmatic skills (the ability to reflect on one’s own com- Children with ADHD are more likely to experience dis-
munication) are present (Andersen-Wood & Smith, 1997). turbed peer relationships, rejection by peers, and failure
The school-age years bring new settings, audiences, to attain peer acceptance (Melnick & Hinshaw, 1996).
roles, and experiences that provide the motivation for The diagnostic criteria for ADHD include behaviors
further refinement of communication skills. Conversa- that suggest pragmatic dysfunction, such as talking ex-
tional skills mature as students improve their ability to cessively, interrupting others, not listening to what is
perceive others’ abilities and knowledge and their ability being said, blurting out answers to questions before they
to shift topics in subtle ways (McLaughlin, 1998). Chil- are completed, and experiencing difficulty waiting for
dren are able to achieve their pragmatic goals such as turns (APA, 1994). Although the diagnosis for ADHD
asserting, denying, sharing information, and bonding does not include problematic peer relationships, the symp-
with others, and they learn forms that help them achieve tomatology included under the diagnosis has major im-
these goals (Dore, 1974; Searle, 1979) As children tra- plications for peer relationships. Specifically, difficulty
verse the school system, their cognitive abilities have de- sustaining attention in tasks or play activities, difficulty
veloped to the point that they are able to describe, compare waiting their turn, and talking and fidgeting excessively
and contrast, explain, analyze, hypothesize, deduce, and (Parker, Rubin, Price, & DeRosier, 1995) may compro-
evaluate. mise children’s relationships with others. Understand-
As children move from preschool to upper elemen- ing pragmatic language use difficulties as a function of
tary school, conversation becomes more important for at-risk dimensions of ADHD among typically develop-
establishing and maintaining social relationships. Age- ing children is important in understanding the poten-
appropriate skill in carrying on conversations has been tial precursors of social skills problems.

568 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 567–579 • April 2011

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Bignell and Cain (2007) investigated pragmatic as- on the Conners’ Parent Rating Scales—Revised: Short
pects of communication and language comprehension in Version (CPRS–R:S; Conners, 2001); (c) inattention (predic-
relation to poor attention and/or high hyperactivity in tor variable)—the Inattention subscale score as reported
a nondiagnosed population of 7- to 11-year olds. Three by the parent on the CPRS–R:S (Conners, 2001); and
groups were formed: poor attention group, high hyperac- (d) PLU mediator variable—the Pragmatic Composite
tivity group, and poor attention/high hyperactivity group. (unstandardized), as reported by the parent on the Chil-
Their performance as reported by their classroom teacher dren’s Communication Checklist—Second Edition (CCC–2;
was compared with that of same-age controls. These re- U.S. Edition; Bishop, 2003). The present study explored
searchers reported that the poor attention group and the whether PLU was associated with, and perhaps accounted
poor attention/high hyperactivity group were impaired for, the social skills problems that these typically devel-
in both their comprehension of figurative language and oping children experience.
in pragmatic aspects of communication. The high hyper- The following questions and hypotheses were inves-
activity group was impaired in their comprehension of tigated. First, are hyperactivity and inattention correlated
figurative language, but they did not exhibit communi- with social skills problems? Second, are hyperactivity and
cation impairments. These results extend work with inattention correlated with PLU? Third, is PLU corre-
clinical populations of children with ADHD to a non- lated with social skills? Fourth, does PLU mediate the
diagnosed sample of children, in that poor attention and relation between hyperactivity or inattention and social
elevated levels of hyperactivity are associated with prag- skills problems? It was predicted that hyperactivity and
matic language weaknesses. inattention would be negatively correlated with social
To date, virtually no empirical research has been skills and PLU, PLU would be positively correlated with
published documenting the relation between pragmatic social skills, and PLU would mediate the relation between
language use and the social skills problems among chil- hyperactivity or inattention and social skills problems.
dren with varying levels of hyperactivity and inattention.
Cohen et al. (1998) suggested that language impairments
could possibly cause the social problems experienced by Method
children with ADHD, but this relation has not been for-
mally tested. A possible link may be that children with
Recruitment
ADHD seem significantly less adaptive in their ability to The University of Kentucky Institutional Review
adjust social communication behaviors (Landau & Milich, Board (IRB) approved the research protocol and recruit-
1988). It has been documented that deficiencies in these ment flyer. Written approval for the distribution of the
skills have resulted in negative consequences such as recruitment flyer was obtained from the superintendent
being teased, victimized, and rejected by peers (Bierman, of the county public school district and from the prin-
2004), regardless of diagnostic status. Thus, the focus of cipal of the private school. Copies of the recruitment flyer
the present study was to develop a deeper understand- were delivered to the respective schools for distribu-
ing of the complexities of pragmatic language develop- tion to all 4th, 5th, and 6th grade children (9-, 10-, and
ment in children with varying levels of hyperactivity and 11-year-olds) to take home to their parent(s). All chil-
inattention—specifically, the use of pragmatic language dren, regardless of special educational needs, were in-
in communicative contexts and its association with es- vited to participate. This grade/age range was selected
tablishing and maintaining successful peer relationships. because these children have had an opportunity to ex-
perience elementary school for a few years and have
had time to establish relationships with their peers as
well as develop more mature pragmatic communication
The Present Study skills.
The goal of the present study was to investigate the The recruitment flyer invited families to take part in
social skills of a community sample of children varying a research study about children’s communication and be-
in their levels of hyperactivity and inattention, with a havior patterns. Interested parents initially were screened
specific focus on their PLU. The operational definitions over the phone for whether their child spoke English as his
used for the variables of interest are as follows: (a) Social or her primary language or had a history of speech or
skills (outcome variable)—the Social Skills Standard hearing problems. Only two children were reported as
Score (Cooperation, Assertion, Responsibility, and Self- having a speech problem. Based on the parent’s report
Control [CARS]) as reported by the parent on the Social that their child should have no problem completing the
Skills Rating System—Parent version (SSRS; Gresham required tasks, these two children were allowed to par-
& Elliott, 1990); (b) hyperactivity (predictor variable)— ticipate in the study. There was one child reported as
the Hyperactivity subscale score as reported by the parent having a confirmed diagnosis of ADHD. This child also

Leonard, Milich, & Lorch: Pragmatic Language Use 569

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
was allowed to participate in the study. As requested by Assessment of social skills ability. The SSRS (Gresham
the school district, teachers were informed of the pur- & Elliott, 1990) was developed for the purpose of identi-
pose of the research study by their respective school fying and classifying children suspected of having social
principals. behavior problems and is used to evaluate a broad range
of socially validated behaviors, including those behav-
iors that affect peer acceptance in children. The SSRS—
Participants Parent version emphasizes positive behaviors, or pro-
The final study sample included 54 children from one social skills, and includes the Social Skills Scale, which
private and 11 public schools in southeastern commu- is composed of four subscales: Cooperation, Assertion,
nities; the children ranged in age from 9 to 11 (M age = Responsibility, and Self-Control (CARS). The subscales
10;6 [years;months], SD = 0.81). There were 29 boys include key symptoms such as “volunteers to help family
(53.70%) and 25 girls (46.30%). Fifty-one children were members with tasks,” “accepts friends’ ideas for play-
Caucasian (94.45%), one was African American, one was ing,” “acknowledges compliments or praise from friends,”
Hispanic, and one was biracial. Demographic character- and “politely refuses unreasonable requests from others.”
istics are presented in Table 1. The result from the four subscales results in the Social

Table 1. Description of sample: Demographic and diagnostic information.

Sample
(n = 54)

Factor n % M SD Range

Gender
Male 29 53.70
Female 25 46.30
Ethnicity
Caucasian 51 94.45
African American 1 1.85
Hispanic 1 1.85
Biracial 1 1.85
Age (years) 10.50 0.81 9.0–11.9

KBIT–2
IQ Composite Standard Score 109.20 11.78 81–137
Verbal Standard Score 110.85 11.73 70–138
Nonverbal Standard Score 104.59 14.00 56–130

CPRS–R:S
Oppositional 51.17 11.37 39–87
Inattention 51.48 9.87 42–79
Hyperactivity 54.30 10.11 44–90
ADHD Index 52.81 9.26 40–77
OWLS
Standard Score (OLS) 106.98 15.46 78–144
Listening Comprehension (LC) Standard Score 104.00 15.98 77–137
Oral Expression (OE) Standard Score 109.00 15.83 81–145
CCC–2
Pragmatic Composite (Scales E through J) 59.54 11.44 36–82

SSRS—Parent version
Social Skills Standard Score (CARS) 108.81 17.20 59–131

Note. KBIT–2 = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test—Second Edition; CPRS–R:S = Conners’ Parent Rating System—
Revised: Short Version; ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; OLS = combined OE and LC Standard
Score; OWLS = Oral and Written Language Scales; CCC–2 = Children’s Communication Checklist—Second Edition;
SSRS = Social Skills Rating System; CARS = Cooperation, Assertion, Responsibility, and Self-Control.

570 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 567–579 • April 2011

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Skills Standard Score. The SSRS Social Skills Standard CPRS–R:S assess three relatively distinct dimensions
Score (CARS) has been standardized such that M = 100 (Conners, 2001). Findings are consistent with the di-
and SD = 15. The national median coefficient alpha re- agnostic criteria for ADHD, and definitions of CPRS–R
liability for the Social Skills Scale was .90, whereas the subscales. The four subscales include Oppositional, In-
internal consistency estimates ranged from .83 to .94, attention, Hyperactivity, and a combined ADHD Index.
representing a relatively high degree of scale homoge- Scoring is based on the following scale: 44 and below =
neity. Test–retest reliability for the Social Skills Scale no concern; 45–55 = average; 56–60 = slightly atypical—
ranged from .77 to .84 for parents, demonstrating good should raise concern; 61–65 = mildly atypical—possible
stability. According to the instrument’s authors, the inter- significant problem; 66–70 = moderately atypical—
nal relationships of the SSRS subscales and scales are significant problem; and 70 and above = markedly
highly consistent with the concepts underlying the devel- atypical—significant problem. In this study, the Hyper-
opment of the system. Reliability analysis of the ques- activity and Inattention subscale scores were used as the
tions making up the SSRS Social Skills Standard Score primary at-risk measures of ADHD. The subscales in-
(CARS) from this sample revealed a good Cronbach’s clude key symptoms such as “hard to control in malls or
alpha (a) of .81. In this study, the SSRS Social Skills Stan- while grocery shopping” and “avoids, expresses reluc-
dard Score (CARS) was used as the primary measure of tance about, or has difficulties engaging in tasks that re-
social skills. Table 2 represents the breakdown in the quire sustained mental effort (such as schoolwork or
SSRS Social Skills Standard Score (CARS). homework).” Table 3 represents the breakdown of all
Assessment of hyperactivity and inattention. The four subscales of the CPRS–R:S.
CPRS–R:S (Conners, 2001) has long been the standard Assessment of PLU. The CCC–2 (U.S. Edition; Bishop,
instrument for the assessment of ADHD and related prob- 2003) is a measure designed to assess children’s commu-
lem behaviors in children. Total internal reliability co- nication skills in the areas of pragmatics, syntax, mor-
efficients for the development of the CPRS–R:S ranged phology, semantics, and speech. CCC–2 is used with
from .857 to .938. The data for the CPRS–R:S items had children ages 4;0 to 16;11 who speak in sentences and
excellent fit to the three-factor model. The parameter whose primary language is English. It is administered
estimates for the relationships among the three factors using a Caregiver Response Form on which the caregiver
were as follows: Oppositional With Cognitive Problems/ rates the frequency that the child demonstrates the com-
Inattention = .58, Oppositional With Hyperactivity = .60, munication behavior described in each item. The CCC–2
and Cognitive Problems/Inattention With Hyperactivity = is designed to (a) identify children with pragmatic lan-
.58 (all loadings were significant; p < .05). These results guage impairment, children who may have a speech and
support the view that the factor-derived subscales of the language impairment, and children whose receptive and
expressive language skills should be further evaluated
with a comprehensive speech and language assessment,
Table 2. Social Skills Rating System (SSRS): Social Skills Standard or (b) assist in identifying children who may require fur-
Score (CARS)—Description of sample. ther assessment for an autism spectrum disorder (ASD).
Although there are many tests that are excellent for as-
Factor sessing aspects of speech and language, the CCC–2 has
(n = 54) Cooperation Assertion Responsibility Self-Control demonstrated high sensitivity to communication impair-
ments under the domain of pragmatics (Bignell & Cain,
Boys (n = 29)
M 12.10 16.72 14.86 13.79
2007). In a selective review and critique of current formal
SD 3.35 3.00 2.90 3.34 and informal testing methods and pragmatic analytic
Fewer 6 5 1 2 procedures, Adams (2002) states that the Children’s Com-
Average 19 19 18 20 munication Checklist (CCC) developed by Bishop (1998)
More 4 5 10 7 appears to be the only pragmatics checklist that has pro-
Girls (n = 25)
vided satisfactory estimates of internal consistency and
M 13.36 16.76 15.32 14.24 interrater reliability and has rapidly become the instru-
SD 3.96 3.48 2.97 3.67 ment of choice for the identification of pragmatic lan-
Fewer 3 3 2 2 guage impairment.
Average 17 19 13 16 The questionnaire covers mainly pragmatic skills
More 5 3 10 7 and contains 70 items (each with seven items) that are
Note. The ratings “fewer, average, and more” can be directly
grouped into 10 subscales: (a) Speech, (b) Syntax, (c) Se-
interpreted as referring to amounts, or frequencies, of behaviors. mantic, (d) Coherence, (e) Inappropriate Initiation, (f ) Ste-
“More” on the CARS can be thought of as exhibiting more positive reotyped Language, (g) Use of Context, (h) Nonverbal
social skills than the average for the group. Communication, (i) Social Relations, and ( j) Interests.
For each subscale, five items address difficulties in

Leonard, Milich, & Lorch: Pragmatic Language Use 571

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Table 3. Conners’ Parent Rating System Revised—Short Version (CPRS–R:S): Description of sample.

Opposition Inattention Hyperactivity ADHD


Factor
(n = 54) n n n n

70 and above = markedly atypical—significant problem 5 5 3 3


66–70 = moderately atypical—significant problem 1 2 2 2
61–65 = mildly atypica possible significant problem 4 2 4 5
56–60 = slightly atypical—should raise concern 3 6 8 9
45–55 = average 22 19 33 23
44 and below = no concern 19 20 4 12

communication skills that children may have, and Context—“Is too literal (e.g., if asked ‘Is it hard to
two items focus on strengths. The first four subscales get up in the morning?’ replies, ‘No, you just put one leg
(a through d) assess aspects of articulation and phonol- out of the bed and then the other and stand up’; or if told
ogy, language structure, vocabulary, and discourse. These ‘watch your hands’ when using scissors, proceeds to stare
are areas that are often impaired in children with specific at his or her fingers)”
language impairment (SLI). The next four subscales Nonverbal Communication—“Does not look at the
(e through h) address pragmatic aspects of communica- person he or she is talking to”
tion that are not readily assessed by conventional language
assessments (Bishop & Baird, 2001). Pragmatic skills Social Relations—“Appears anxious in the company
can be impaired in children with or without structural of other children”
language difficulties. In other words, Subscales e, f, g, Interests—“Moves the conversation to a favorite topic,
and h identify children whose language is not impaired even if others do not seem interested in it.”
in the areas of content and form but whose language is Table 4 reflects the breakdown in the unstandardized
impaired in the area of use. The last two subscales (i and PC scores of the CCC–2.
j) assess behaviors that are usually impaired in children
Assessment of general verbal and nonverbal intel-
with ASD. Because pragmatic difficulties do occur in chil-
ligence. The Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test—Second
dren with high-functional autism (HFA) and ADHD, these
Edition (KBIT–2; Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004) was admin-
items have been shown to discriminate case identifica-
istered individually to each child to obtain an estimate of
tion in children with ADHD, HFA, and typically develop-
two distinct cognitive abilities: general verbal (crystal-
ing controls (Geurts et al., 2004). The CCC–2 has been
lized) and nonverbal (fluid) intelligence. The Verbal score
found to have strong reliability and validity, including
contains two subtests: Verbal Knowledge and Riddles.
good interrater agreement (r = .79).
These subtests include receptive and expressive vocab-
The standardized Pragmatic Composite (PC) included ulary items that do not require reading or spelling. The
in the CCC is no longer available in the CCC–2, although Nonverbal score is composed of a single subtest: Matrices,
the six subscales (e through j) relating to the pragmatic which is composed of several types of items involving
aspects of communication are retained. For this study, visual stimuli, both meaningful (people and objects) and
an unstandardized PC was calculated by averaging the abstract (designs and symbols). All items require under-
six subscales to determine whether this score was asso- standing of relationships among the stimuli, and all are
ciated with the Hyperactivity and Inattention subscale multiple choice, requiring the person either to point to
scores. Scoring from the original PC is based on M = 60 the correct response or to say its letter. Scores were pro-
and SD = 18. Reliability analysis of the questions mak- vided on a familiar scale, where M = 100 and SD = 15.
ing up the unstandardized PC from this sample revealed
Assessment of general receptive and expressive lan-
a good Cronbach’s alpha of .88. In this study, the un-
guage abilities. The Oral and Written Language Scales:
standardized PC was used as the primary measure of
Listening Comprehension (OWLS: LC) subscale (recep-
PLU. Key symptoms of the PC include the following:
tive language) and the OWLS Oral Expression (OWLS:
Initiation—“Talks repetitively about things that no OE) subscale (expressive language; Carrow-Woolfolk,
one is interested in” 1995) were administered individually to each child. The
Scripted Language—“Provides over-precise infor- assessment of receptive and expressive language abili-
mation in his or her talk (e.g., if asked ‘When did you go ties provided a broad picture of the children’s general
on vacation?’ may say ‘July 13, 2005’ instead of ‘in the language abilities. OWLS scoring is based on correct re-
summer’)” sponse (e.g., preferred, acceptable, or no differentiation)

572 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 567–579 • April 2011

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Table 4. Children’s Communication Checklist—Second Edition, U.S. (CCC–2): Description of sample.

Factor Initiation Scripted Language Context Nonverbal Communication Social Interests


(n = 54) E F G H I J

CCC–2: PC
M 9.35 9.94 10.22 10.11 9.93 9.98
SD 2.38 2.40 2.11 2.15 2.59 2.81
Range 4–15 4–13 6–14 3–13 3–13 3–16
Possible scores n n n n n n
0–5 4 3 0 1 3 4
6–10 35 29 33 31 25 27
11–15 15 22 21 22 26 23

Note. PC = Pragmatic Composite.

and incorrect response (e.g., grammatical error, semantic/ language abilities (M = 106.98, SD = 15.46, range = 78–
pragmatic error, or no response). Three standardized 144), and social skills (M = 108.81, SD = 17.20, range =
scores were calculated: OE Standard Score, LC Stan- 59–131). The Hyperactivity and Inattention subscale
dard Score, and a combined OE and LC Standard Score scores from the CPRS-R:S (Conners, 2001) represented
(OLS). Scores were provided on a familiar scale, where a normal distribution of scores with an average of 54.30
M = 100 and SD = 15. (SD = 10.11) and 51.48 (SD = 9.87), respectively. The
sample averaged above the mean on PLU (M = 59.54,
SD = 11.44, range = 88–152). These distributions repre-
Procedure sented enough variability to examine the relations among
All study procedures were approved by the Univer- the measures.
sity of Kentucky Institutional Review Board. One child Analyses proceeded in two steps. First, correlation co-
with a confirmed diagnosis of ADHD as reported by the efficients were calculated to examine associations among
parent participated in this study. The child was medi- language measures, hyperactivity and inattention, and so-
cation free for 24 hr prior to the study. This is considered cial skills. Second, following procedures established by
an acceptable washout period for stimulant medication. MacKinnon, Krull, and Lockwood (2000), we computed a
Informed consent was obtained from the parent, and the series of multiple linear regressions to test the prediction
parent was then provided with instructions on complet- that PLU would mediate the relation between hyperac-
ing the PLU, hyperactivity and inattention, and social tivity or inattention and social skills problems. Four con-
skills standardized measures. While the parent was com- ditions must be met for PLU to be a mediator: First,
pleting the rating forms, the child was taken to the test- hyperactivity or inattention (predictor) is significantly as-
ing room, where he or she was seated at a table across sociated with social skills problems (outcome). Second, hy-
from the experimenter. Prior to the start of the session, peractivity or inattention is significantly associated with
the experimenter read aloud the Child Assent Form to PLU (mediator). Third, PLU is significantly associated
the child and obtained verbal assent for participation with social skills problems (after controlling for hyper-
from the child. To determine general verbal and non- activity or inattention). Fourth, the impact of hyperactiv-
verbal intelligence and general expressive and receptive ity or inattention on social skills problems is significantly
language abilities, children completed the KBIT–2 and less after controlling for PLU. An alpha level of .05 was
the OWLS. Altogether, the testing lasted approximately used for all statistical tests. Power analysis using an alpha
35 min. Following the testing, the child was returned to of .05, an effect size (r) of .5, and a total sample size of
his or her parent. Each family was paid $15 for partici- 54 reveals a power greater than .90 to find a large effect.
pating in the study.

Analyses Results
Ms and SDs describing the sample on each of these Intercorrelations Among
measures are listed in Table 1. As is evident in this table,
the sample averaged somewhat above the mean on general
Language Measures
verbal and nonverbal intelligence (M = 109.20, SD = Results presented in Table 5 indicate that there
11.78, range = 81–137), general receptive and expressive were significant positive correlations among the general

Leonard, Milich, & Lorch: Pragmatic Language Use 573

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Table 5. Intercorrelations among language measures. or inattention and social skills, such that as the Hyper-
activity or Inattention subscale scores increased, the
Scale 1 2 3 4 5 social skills scores (e.g., CARS) decreased; (b) hyperactivity
or inattention and PLU, such that as the Hyperactivity
1. KBIT–2: VSS —
or Inattention subscale scores increased, PLU scores (e.g.,
2. OWLS: LC .488** —
3. OWLS: OE .438** .453** —
CCC–2: PC) decreased; and (c) PLU and social skills, such
4. OWLS: OLS .550** .851** .853** — that as PLU scores (e.g., CCC–2: PC) increased, social
5. CCC–2: PC .148 .083 .256 .202 — skills scores (e.g., CARS) increased. Thus, mediation test-
ing was warranted.
Note. VSS = Verbal Standard Score; LC = Listening Comprehension;
OE = Oral Expression.
**p < .01, two-tailed. Test of the Mediation Model
With Hyperactivity
We used multiple linear regression analyses to de-
language measures: the KBIT–2: Verbal Standard Score
velop a model for predicting children’s social skills from
(VSS), the OWLS: LC Standard Score, the OWLS: OE
their hyperactivity levels and PLU. The continuous pre-
Standard Score, and the OLS Standard Score. These ex-
dictor (hyperactivity) and mediating (PLU) variables were
pected correlations indicate that the higher the child’s
centered at their means for these analyses. Basic descrip-
verbal intelligence as measured by the KBIT–2, the higher
tive statistics and regression coefficients are shown in
the score obtained on the receptive and expressive lan-
Table 6. The predictor variable (hyperactivity) and the
guage domains as measured by the OWLS.
mediator variable (PLU) each had a significant ( p < .01)
There were no significant correlations between ver- zero-order correlation with the outcome variable (social
bal intelligence (KBIT–2: VSS) and PLU (CCC–2: PC– skills problems), and hyperactivity and PLU had sig-
Unstandardized) or between PLU and general language nificant ( p < .01) full mediating effects in the complete
abilities (OWLS: LC Standard Score, OWLS: OE Standard model. The correlation between hyperactivity and social
Score, and OLS Standard Score). These findings indicate skills dropped from r = –.462, p < .01, to r = –.228, p = .059,
that the pragmatic communication skills measured by when PLU was entered as a mediator in the model,
the CCC–2: PC–Unstandardized are distinct from more suggesting that PLU mediates the relation between hy-
general verbal abilities. Thus, PLU as measured by the peractivity and social skills problems. In addition, hyper-
CCC–2 is not simply synonymous with general language activity provided a unique contribution in the estimate
proficiencies. of children’s social skills of approximately 4% above and
beyond the contribution of PLU, sr 2 = .0416, p < .01, and
Intercorrelations Among the Predictor, PLU provided a unique contribution in the estimate of
children’s social skills of 22% above and beyond the con-
Mediator, and Outcome Variables tribution of hyperactivity, sr2 = .2162, p < .01. The full
As hypothesized and reflected in Tables 6 and 7, a model was able to account for 42.9% of the variance in
set of preliminary zero-order correlations demonstrated children’s social skills, F(2, 51) = 19.174, p < .01. The 95%
the expected predictive relations between (a) hyperactivity confidence interval (CI) for R2 was [.425, 1.140].

Table 6. Mediation analyses predicting social skills problems from hyperactivity and PLU.

Zero-order r

Variable Hyperactivity PLU SS b sr 2 B

PLU .623** .520** .216 .782


Hyperactivity –.449** .462** –.228 .042 –.388
M 54.30 59.54 108.81
SD 10.11 11.44 17.20 R2 = .429

Note. PLU = pragmatic language use as measured by the CCC–2: PC (unstandardized); SS = social skills as measured by the Social
Skills Rating System: Social Skills Standard Score. Hyperactivity and PLU were centered at their means for the mediation analyses.
**p < .01, one-tailed.

574 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 567–579 • April 2011

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Table 7. Mediation analyses predicting social skills problems from inattention and PLU.

Zero-order r

Variable Inattention PLU SS b sr 2 B

PLU .623** .408** .172 .614


Inattention –.460** –.654** –.467** –.131 –.814
M 51.48 59.54 108.81
SD 9.87 11.44 17.20 R2 = .559

Note. Inattention and PLU were centered at their means for the mediation analyses.
**p < .01, one-tailed.

Test of the Mediation Model With Inattention problems and that the findings are not due to a more
general language impairment.
We used multiple linear regression analyses to de-
velop a model for predicting children’s social skills from
their inattention levels and PLU. The continuous predictor Test of the Moderation Model
(inattention) and mediating (PLU) variables were cen- Was it possible that general language impairment was
tered at their means for these analyses. Basic descrip- acting as a moderating variable in the relation between
tive statistics and regression coefficients are shown in hyperactivity or inattention and social skills problems?
Table 7. The predictor variable (inattention) and the Moderation analyses determine under what conditions
mediator variable (PLU) each had a significant ( p < .01) of the moderator variable (general language ability as
zero-order correlation with the outcome variable (social measured by the OWLS: LC or OWLS: OE) the predictor
skills problems), and inattention and PLU had signifi- variable (hyperactivity or inattention) is significantly asso-
cant ( p < .01) partial mediating effects in the complete ciated with the outcome variable (social skills problems).
model. The correlation between inattention and social In contrast to mediation analyses, significant correla-
skills dropped from r = –.654, p < .01, to r = –.467 p < .01, tions do not have to exist among the predictor, modera-
when PLU was entered as a mediator in the model but tor, and outcome variables before conducting moderation
still remained significant, suggesting that PLU partially analyses. We conducted multiple linear regression anal-
mediates the relation between inattention and social skills yses with the OWLS: LC Standard Score and the OWLS:
problems. In addition, inattention provided a unique con- OE Standard Score as potential moderators between social
tribution in the estimate of children’s social skills of ap- skills and hyperactivity or inattention. Before conducting
proximately 13% above and beyond the contribution of the analyses, the continuous predictor and moderator var-
PLU, sr2 = .1310, p < .01, and PLU provided a unique iables were centered at their means to eliminate multi-
contribution in the estimate of children’s social skills of collinearity effects between the predictor and moderator
17.2% above and beyond the contribution of inattention, as well as between the interaction terms. The predictor
sr2 = .1722, p < .01. The full model was able to account for and moderator variables along with their interaction
55.9% of the variance in children’s social skills, F(2, 51) = terms were entered into the regression equation. The
32.38, p < .001. The 95% CI for R2 was [.298, .930]. interaction was interpreted using predicted values at
Because PLU mediated the relation between hyper- 1 SD below and above the means of the predictor and
activity and inattention and social skills problems, it was moderator variables and was probed using simple slope
necessary to determine whether the interpretation of the analyses. No moderation was evident.
mediational findings were specific to PLU or were due
to general language impairment (receptive or expressive
language dysfunction). Preliminary analyses revealed
that no significant correlations existed among any of the
Discussion
three scores from the OWLS: LC Standard Score, OWLS: The primary hypotheses of this study were confirmed,
OE Standard Score, and OLS Standard Score, and either in that (a) both hyperactivity and inattention were neg-
social skills or hyperactivity and inattention, so addi- atively associated with social skills, (b) hyperactivity and
tional mediational analyses were not warranted. These inattention both had a significant negative relation with
findings demonstrate that PLU mediates the relation PLU, (c) PLU had a significant positive relation with social
between hyperactivity and inattention and social skills skills, and (d) the relation of hyperactivity or inattention

Leonard, Milich, & Lorch: Pragmatic Language Use 575

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
and social skills problems were fully and partially (re- comprehend the contextual norms of the public context
spectively) mediated by PLU. It is not surprising that where peer group influence is the predominant force of
hyperactivity and inattention deficits and social skills children’s play and interaction. This underdeveloped
deficits are associated, as current literature clearly pragmatic competence may lead to disruption in the de-
has established this relation (Hinshaw & Melnick, 1995; velopment of appropriate social skills. Because the de-
Landau, Milich, & Diener, 1998). A unique aspect of this sign of the present study did not measure the quality
study is that in a community sample of children, both and frequency of the children’s interactions with care-
hyperactivity and inattention are associated with PLU, givers or peers, this alternative explanation as to why
and PLU mediates the relation between hyperactivity PLU and social skills are related remains open to fu-
and social skills problems as well as the relation between ture investigation.
inattention and social skills problems.

Why Might PLU Mediate the Relation


Why Are Hyperactivity and Inattention Between Hyperactivity and Inattention
Associated With Pragmatic and Social Skills Problems?
Language Difficulties? Perhaps it is not surprising that a measure of PLU
Theoretical models of the relations between inatten- would be important in accounting for the deficits that
tion, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language skills have children with increased levels of hyperactivity and inat-
come from the ADHD literature. These models propose tention have in developing social skills. Much, if not most,
that children with increased levels of hyperactivity and social interaction is achieved through the use of language.
inattention may experience pragmatic language difficul- A competent language user knows not only rules of social
ties because ADHD involves poor behavioral inhibition, interaction, but also how and when to apply them—and
which affects executive control and is related to problems how to vary communicative style and content according
with attention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity (Barkley, to moment-by-moment changes in the social environment
1997, 1999). (Lapadat, 2001).
Although the present study did not incorporate a Children with varying levels of hyperactivity and in-
measure of response inhibition, Barkley’s (1997, 1999) attention may engage in a social context with an interac-
assumption of defective behavioral inhibition has become tional goal in mind. However, for some children with high
levels of inattention, impulsivity, or hyperactivity, an in-
a current view of ADHD and has had an impact on new
effective filtering mechanism may create problems in that
intervention programs focusing on pragmatic language
steps are skipped (e.g., a shortcut is taken) to achieve the
and metacognition (Bruce, Thernlund, & Nettelbladt,
interactional goal. Stressors that may cause the filtering
2006). Because ADHD consists of developmental defi-
mechanism to drop may include prior experience of and
ciencies in the regulation and maintenance of behavior
anticipation for name calling, insults, rejection, victim-
by rules and consequences, these deficiencies are asso-
ization, peer alienation, and rudeness from their social
ciated with problems with inhibiting, initiating, or sus-
partner(s). As stressors are encountered, the filtering
taining responses to tasks or stimuli and adhering to rules
mechanism—often perceived by social partners as a mea-
or instructions (Barkley, 1990).
sure of self control (or lack thereof )—redirects children
such that pragmatic language difficulties occur (e.g., in-
terruptions, deficiencies in topic initiation and mainte-
Why Is PLU Associated With Social Skills? nance, lack of eye contact, or reduction in politeness).
Regardless of hyperactivity and inattention levels, The children then can fail to follow the subtle nuances
the quality and the frequency of children’s interactions associated with appropriate PLU, which can result in
with caregivers and peers also may explain why prag- disruptive social skills.
matic language difficulties and social skills deficits are Once children with increased levels of hyperactivity
related. Pragmatic language skills are important for de- and inattention establish a history of ineffective social
veloping relationships with others and for communicat- experiences, these children need more time to learn and
ing with a range of interlocutors in a variety of contexts require additional practice to incorporate appropriate
(Hyter, 2007). The way in which a child’s language is used pragmatic language utterances and behaviors while at
in the important contexts and encounters in their so- the same time attempting to control the behaviors spe-
cial environment (e.g., home, school, peer environments) cific to hyperactivity and inattention. If these children
may be more relevant to adjustment and social success encounter those already knowledgeable of their pragmatic
than their competence in the more traditionally assessed difficulties, an offensive stance (e.g., aggressiveness) or
language areas (Russell & Grizzle, 2008). Children ex- defensive stance (e.g., withdrawal) may ensue, dependent
pressing a low degree of pragmatic competence may not on the specific social group. Over the years and through

576 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 567–579 • April 2011

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
the process of trial and error (potentially more error than
trial), children with increased levels of hyperactivity and Future Directions
inattention must either learn how to effectively manage Poor peer relationships have been found to predict
their pragmatic language abilities through this learned serious difficulties in later life, including truancy, anti-
filtering process or suffer the long-term consequences of social behavior, social anxiety, and an increased need for
problematic social relations. mental health services (Parker & Asher, 1987). Children
experiencing these relationship difficulties—specifically,
children with ADHD—often present with comorbid disor-
Limitations ders such as aggression, conduct disorder, oppositional de-
fiant disorder, and multiple learning disabilities. Because
The results of this study need to be considered in empirical evidence exists documenting problematic peer
light of two key limitations. A stronger design would relationships in children presenting with these comor-
have been to utilize multiple indices of the outcome, pre- bid difficulties (Hinshaw & Melnick, 1995), it is critical
dictor, and mediator variables. However, in this regard, that research continues to disentangle the relations among
we were limited by the small number of standardized social skills problems, at-risk dimensions for ADHD, and
measures with adequate psychometric properties. PLU difficulties.
What might it take to capture the nuances in these In conducting future research, if clinically signif-
variables? Because the assessment of social skills prob- icant pragmatic deficits are detected, one could argue
lems, at-risk factors for ADHD, and pragmatics is complex that interventions for these deficits should be a priority
and problematic, multimethod, multisource assessment (Camarata & Gibson, 1999). The development of interven-
procedures are desirable. Additional assessment using tions for children with increased levels of hyperactivity
natural social contexts may provide greater sensitivity or inattention symptoms and social skills problems should
in detecting social skills problems, at-risk factors for be reformulated to integrate children’s participation in
ADHD, and language and social–communicative abili- focused communicative interactions in multiple commu-
ties. Further, language assessment needs to be far more nicative contexts that clarify and expand on the appro-
broadly based than the formal testing or analysis of priate use of pragmatic utterances and behaviors. Specific
spoken narrative that is commonly thought to suffice targets for intervention include topic initiation, topic
(Mathers, 2006) or the structural (grammatical) aspects of maintenance, and the appropriate use and timing of
language often assessed on standardized tests (Camarata pauses and intensity. In order for children with in-
& Gibson, 1999). creased levels of hyperactivity and inattention to change
A second limitation exists in that results are based behavior and increase the appropriateness of their PLU
solely on a community sample of children with varying and social skills, it is necessary that the intervention al-
low children to (a) understand the purpose for the skills,
levels of hyperactivity and inattention. Regardless of
(b) understand the effect that the inappropriate use of
this limitation, the predicted relations were evident in
the skills may have on the perceptions that others may
the population utilized. It is possible that the relations
garner toward them, and (c) realize the benefits that the
would be stronger if future samples were to include a
skills have on their social relationships—specifically,
clinically diagnosed population and, thus, a wider range
those with their peers. Meaning must be attached to
of ADHD symptomatology and social skills problems. In
strategies that are taught so that children develop an
addition, children with a diagnosis of ADHD can be clas-
understanding of the relation between behavior and
sified into three unique subtypes: predominately inatten-
consequences (Westby & Cutler, 1994).
tive, predominately hyperactive/impulsive, and combined.
Because the children classified into these unique ADHD In a similar vein, future studies can examine the
subtypes have been shown to display different behav- role that treatment with stimulant medication may have
ioral profiles (Milich, Balentine, & Lynam, 2001), it is on the relation between pragmatic language difficulties
crucial that future researchers make these distinctions in and peer problems. Such treatment clearly improves the
evaluating the relations among ADHD symptoms, PLU, attention and overall behavior of children with ADHD,
and social skills problems. Children with ADHD and chil- but as Derefinko, Bailey, Milich, Lorch, and Riley (2009)
dren with pragmatic language deficits are a relatively reported, medication treatment may not have an impact
heterogeneous population (Tirosh & Cohen, 1998). Al- on higher order cognitive processing, such as goal-based
though some effort (Bignell & Cain, 2007) has been di- story recall. Thus, it is reasonable to wonder whether
rected at disentangling how hyperactivity/impulsivity stimulant medication has a positive impact on these
and poor attention are separately related to school chil- children’s language skills.
dren’s pragmatic language skills, future research should In addition to interventions with children, both par-
also try to develop further understanding of these chil- ents and teachers also should be provided with feedback
dren’s peer difficulties as they relate to PLU. and instruction as well as strategies to monitor the child’s

Leonard, Milich, & Lorch: Pragmatic Language Use 577

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
progress. Such feedback and instruction may lead to References
changes in the home and at school, thus resulting in
Adams, C. (2002). Practitioner review: The assessment of
changes to parent and teacher perceptions and affective
language pragmatics. Journal of Child Psychology and
behavior toward the child. In light of the problematic Psychiatry, 43, 973–987.
social relationships that children with increased levels
Adams, C., Baxendale, J., Lloyd, J., & Aldredge, C. (2005).
of hyperactivity and inattention experience, such inter- Pragmatic language impairment: Case studies of social and
ventional efforts may guide children to more positive pragmatic language therapy. Child Language Teaching &
and successful interactions with peers, family, and teach- Therapy, 21, 227–250. doi:10.1191/0265659005ct290oa.
ers, thus resulting in improved communicative abilities Adams, C., Green, J., Gilchrist, A., & Cox, A. (2002).
and social developmental outcomes in this population. Conversational behaviour of children with Asperger syn-
drome and conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology
Finally, future investigations can begin to examine and Psychiatry, 43, 679–690. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00056.
why it is that children with ADHD have such pragmatic
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and
language difficulties. There are many symptoms asso- statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington,
ciated with ADHD that may contribute to these diffi- DC: Author.
culties, including poor executive functioning, attention Andersen-Wood, L., & Smith, B. R. (1997). Working with
problems, impulsivity, and problems with affect regula- pragmatics. London, United Kingdom: Winslow Press.
tion (Barkley, 1997). If research can identify factors that Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder:
may contribute to the development of pragmatic language A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York, NY:
problems, then it may be possible to intervene early to Guilford.
prevent or attenuate the onset of these difficulties. Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained
attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying
theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65.
Conclusion Barkley, R. A. (1999). Response inhibition in attention-deficit
In closing, this is the first known study that directly hyperactivity disorder. Mental Retardation and Develop-
mental Disabilities Research Review, 5, 177–184. doi:10.1002/
evaluates the relations among PLU, hyperactivity and
(SICI)1098-2779(1999)5:3<177::AID-MRDD3>3.0.CO;2-G.
inattention symptoms, and social skills problems utiliz-
Barkley, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, M. B. (1990). A
ing a community sample of children with varying levels
comprehensive evaluation of attention deficit disorder with
of hyperactivity and inattention. The current results and without hyperactivity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
highlight the complex interactive nature of PLU, hy- Psychology, 58, 775–789. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.58.6.775.
peractivity and inattention, and social skills. Given that Bierman, K. L. (2004). Peer rejection: Developmental processes
peer relations, both successful and unsuccessful, are com- and intervention strategies. New York, NY: Guilford.
plex and have many contributing factors, it is reasonable Bignell, S., & Cain, K. (2007). Pragmatic aspects of commu-
to assume that the treatment of disturbed relations re- nication and language comprehension in groups of children
quires an intensive, multimodal approach (Landau, Milich, differentiated by teacher ratings of hyperactivity and in-
& Diener, 1998). Such an approach may benefit from the attention. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25,
499–512. doi:10.1348/026151006X171343.
inclusion of a pragmatic language assessment beyond
the typically accessed receptive and expressive language Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children’s
Communication Checklist (CCC): A method of assessing
abilities and a more rigorous social skills assessment, qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children.
including the quality and frequency of interactions with Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–891.
caregivers and peers. Expanded assessment procedures doi:10.1017/S0021963098002832.
may provide a more fully elaborated model regarding re- Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Children’s Communication Checklist—
lations among the subtypes of ADHD, pragmatic lan- Revised (2nd ed.). London, United Kingdom: The Psycho-
guage ability, and social development. logical Corporation.
Bishop, D., & Baird, G. (2001). Parent and teacher report of
pragmatic aspects of communication: Use of the Children’s
Acknowledgments Communication Checklist in a clinical setting. Develop-
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 809–818.
Funding for this research was provided, in part, by the
Bruce, B., Thernlund, G., & Nettelbladt, U. (2006). ADHD
University of Kentucky Women’s Club 2008 Endowed Fellow- and language impairment: A study of the parent question-
ship, awarded to the first author for dissertation completion. naire FTF (Five to Fifteen). European Child Adolescent
We would like to thank Darrell Leonard and Jesse Yule for Psychiatry, 15, 52–60. doi:10.1007/s00787-006-0508-9.
their assistance in collecting, transcribing, and coding videos Camarata, S. M., & Gibson, T. (1999). Pragmatic language
and transcripts. We would also like to thank the parents and deficits in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
children who participated in this study. Without their willing- Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities
ness to take part in cognitive developmental research, this Research Reviews, 5, 207–214. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-
project would not have been possible. 2779(1999)5:3<207::AID-MRDD7>3.0.CO;2-O.

578 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 54 • 567–579 • April 2011

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1995). Oral and Written Language Lapadat, J. C. (2001). Pragmatic language skills of students
Scales manual. Circle Pines, MN: AGS. with language and/or learning disabilities: A quantitative
Cohen, N. J., Menna, R., Vallance, D. D., Barwick, M. A., Im, synthesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 147–158.
N., & Horodezky, N. B. (1998). Language, social cognitive MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000).
processing, and behavioral characteristics of psychiatrically Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppres-
disturbed children with previously identified and unsuspected sion effect. Prevention Science, 1, 173–181. doi:10.1023/
language impairments. Journal of Child Psychology and A:1026595011371.
Psychiatry, 39, 853–864. doi:10.1017/S0021963098002789. Mathers, M. E. (2006). Aspects of language in children with
Conners, C. K. (2001). Conners’ Parent Rating Scales— ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 9, 523–533.
Revised technical manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi- McLaughlin, S. (1998). Introduction to language develop-
Health Systems. ment. San Diego, CA: Singular.
Derefinko, K. J., Bailey, U. L., Milich, R., Lorch, E. P., & Melnick, S. M., & Hinshaw, S. P. (1996). What they want
Riley, E. (2009). The effects of stimulant medications on and what they get: The social goals of boys with ADHD and
the online story narrations of children with ADHD. School comparison boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Mental Health, 1, 171–182. doi:10.1007/s12310-009-9017-6. 24, 169–185. doi:10.1007/BF01441483.
Dore, J. (1974). A pragmatic description of early language Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD
development. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 343–350. combined type and ADHD predominately inattentive type
doi:10.1007/BF01068169. are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology:
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1979). Children’s verbal turn-taking. In Science and Practice, 8, 463–488.
E. Ochs & B. Schiefflin (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later
(pp. 391–414). New York, NY: Academic Press. personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psycho-
Furrow, D. (1984). Social and private speech at 2 years. Child logical Bulletin, 102, 357–389. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.102.3.357.
Development, 55, 355–362. doi:10.2307/1129948. Parker, J. G., Rubin, K. H., Price, J. M., & DeRosier, M. E.
Geurts, H. M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., (1995). Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment:
Hartman, C. A., Mulder, E. J., . . . Sergeant, J. A. (2004). A developmental psychopathology perspective. In D. Cicchetti
Can the Children’s Communication Checklist differentiate & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk,
between children with autism, children with ADHD, and disorder, and adaptation (Vol. 2; pp. 357–389). New York,
normal controls? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- NY: Wiley.
try, 45, 1437–1453. Prutting, C., & Kirchner, D. (1987). A clinical appraisal of
Gottman, J. M. (1983). How children become friends. the pragmatic aspects of language. Journal of Speech and
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop- Hearing Disorders, 52, 105–117.
ment, 48(2, Serial No. 201). doi:10.2307/1165860. Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (1997). Language abilities in
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, reading
System manual. Circle Pines, MN: AGS. disabilities, and normal controls. Journal of Abnormal Child
Hemphill, L., & Siperstein, G. N. (1990). Conversational Psychology, 25, 133–144. doi:10.1023/A:1025731529006.
competence and peer response to mildly retarded children. Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer in-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 1–7. doi:10.1037/ teractions, relationships, and groups. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),
0022-0663.82.1.128. Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and
Hinshaw, S. P., & Melnick, S. M. (1995). Peer relationships personality development (pp. 619–700). New York, NY: Wiley.
in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with Russell, R. L., & Grizzle, K. L. (2008). Assessing child and
and without comorbid aggression. Development and Psy- adolescent pragmatic language competencies: Toward evidence-
chopathology, 7, 627–647. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006751. based assessments. Clinical Child and Family Psychology
Hyter, Y. D. (2007). Pragmatic language assessment: A Review, 11(1–2), 59–73. doi:10.1007/s10567-008-0032-1.
pragmatics-as-social practice model. Topics in Language Searle, J. R. (1979). Expression and meaning: Studies in the
Disorders, 27, 128–145. theory of speech acts. Cambridge, MA: University Press.
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Brief Tirosh, E., & Cohen, A. (1998). Language deficit with
Intelligence Test—Second Edition manual. Minneapolis, attention-deficit disorder: A prevalent comorbidity.
MN: NCS Pearson. Journal of Child Neurology, 13, 493–497. doi:10.1177/
Kim, O. H., & Kaiser, A. P. (2000). Language characteristics 088307389801301005.
of children with ADHD. Communication Disorders Quar- Westby, C. E., & Cutler, S. K. (1994). Language and ADHD:
terly, 21, 154–166. doi:10.1177/152574010002100304. Understanding the bases and treatment of self-regulatory
Landau, S., & Milich, R. (1988). Social communication pat- deficits. Topics in Language Disorders, 14, 58–76.
terns of attention-deficit-disordered boys. Journal of Abnor- Whalen, C. K., & Henker, B. (1985). The social worlds of
mal Child Psychology, 16, 69–81. doi:10.1007/BF00910501. hyperactive children. Clinical Psychology Review, 5, 1–32.
Landau, S., Milich, R., & Diener, M. B. (1998). Peer rela- doi:10.1016/0272-7358(85)90004-2.
tions of children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Diffi-
culties, 14, 83–105. doi:10.1080/1057356980140105.

Leonard, Milich, & Lorch: Pragmatic Language Use 579

Downloaded From: http://pubs.asha.org/ by a Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino User on 02/05/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

You might also like