Occupational Therapy - Performance Participation, and Well-Being

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Chapter Four

TIME U SE AND
PATTERNS OF OCCUPATIONS

Charles H. Christiansen, EdD, OTR, OT(C), FAOTA ________________________________________

T his chapter considers the relationship between time and human occupation, presenting time use as anoth-
er approach for understanding people as occupational beings. The chapter begins with the observation
that perceived time is subjective and may be influenced by various environmental and person-related factors
that influence attention and action. The body’s internal clocks, studied within the field of chronobiology, are
identified as an important person-related influence on time use patterns. The chapter proceeds with a discus-
sion of the nature and purpose of habits, routines, rituals, and lifestyles, and the consequences of their dis-

Don't miss the companion Web site to Occupational Therapy: Performance, Participation, and Well-Being, Third Edition.
Please visit us at http://www.cb3e.slackbooks.com.

Christiansen, C. H. (2005). Time use and patterns of occupation. In C. H. Christiansen, C. M. Baum, and J. Bass-Haugen (Eds.),
Occupational therapy: Performance, participation, and well-being (3rd ed.). Thorofare, NJ: SLACK Incorporated.
72 Chapter Four

INTRODUCTION importance for navigation and scheduling. Yet even


before technology created these needs, humans were
The word occupation is derived from the Latin occupare, interested in keeping track of time (Whitrow, 1989).
meaning to seize or occupy. The admonition carpe diem, Early humans recognized the relationship between time
meaning “seize the day,” refers to the use of time and place and activities—that time elapsed as activities were under-
to leave a mark on the world. This “leaving a mark” taken and that occupations needed to occur or recur at
occurs through agency or the active doing through daily particular times. For example, sailors and fishermen found
human occupations (Emirbayer & Mische, 1998). it convenient to know when the tides would be changing.
From the moment people become aware of their places It was also useful to know when the sun would be setting
in the world, they most often experience their lives as an because, before electricity, most outdoor activity could
uninterrupted stream of events. Memories of past experi- not be undertaken once the darkness of night arrived.
ences lead to present activities that encourage anticipa- Even today, our dependence on sunlight for everyday
tions for the future. Events, which consist of recognizable activity is made evident in the use of daylight savings time
activity experiences, mark the passage of time. The round (known in some parts of the world as summer time). This
of daily activities, typically beginning with a morning rou- was an idea first conceived by Benjamin Franklin in 1784
tine invited by the new light of the day and ending with in a humorous essay he wrote while living in Paris
an extended period of sleep prodded by darkness and (Smyth, 1905). Now, with the exception of countries in
fatigue, cycles continuously through lifetimes.1 the tropical regions (where it has no practical benefit),
In some parts of the world, the cycles of the seasons are the practice of adjusting clocks (and lifestyles) during the
marked by different occupations, providing another indi- summer months has been adopted around the world. That
cation that time has elapsed. Traditions, celebrations, and groups of people would agree to adjust clocks (and their
rituals may also serve as reminders of time’s passage. lives) to preserve activity time (and energy) provides an
Examples include birthdays, regular religious services, or excellent example of the inextricable relationship
New Year celebrations. Clearly, one very important between time and everyday occupations.
dimension of human occupation is its temporal nature.
Considered together, time and daily occupations are
two sides of the same coin. How occupations occupy time THE EXPERIENCE OF TIME
determines the patterns and routines of daily life. These
are the central concepts of this chapter. How people experience the passage of time has been a
In the following sections, various concepts related to topic of interest to both philosophers and scientists for
time and human occupation will be reviewed, including centuries. Despite the standardization of time-keeping,
how time is customarily perceived and measured, patterns people differ in the manner in which time is perceived.
of time use, influences on time use, and recurring habits, Individual differences appear to be based on age, person-
routines, and rituals, and their characteristics and purpos- ality, cultural experiences, and attitudes. Psychophysical
es (Figure 4-1). and physiological variables, such as the temperature of the
body, the state of arousal, and the effect of drugs (Eisler &
Eisler, 1994) can also affect time perception. The extent
THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN to which accurate perception of the passage of time influ-
ences the performance of everyday occupations can be
TIME AND OCCUPATIONS shown among musicians, dancers, and skilled athletes.
The skilled performance of music, dance, and sport
It is possible that we are more precisely aware of the depends on the precise timing of human movements.
passage of time today than our ancestors were because Scientists believe they have now identified areas in the
analog and digital clocks, watches, and timekeepers are brain that are responsible for time perception. The basal
widespread in the modern world. Of all inventions used ganglia located deep within the base of the brain and the
by modern cultures today, the clock may be the most uni- parietal lobe located on the surface of the right side of the
versal and significant. Regardless of when or where they brain are critical areas for this time-keeping system (Rao,
occur, the moment and duration of events can now be Mayer, & Harrington, 2001).
described consistently and precisely. This has practical

1The idea that a person moves through time seamlessly as in a stream of consciousness portrays a decidedly Western view of philosophy. The author
is aware of Eastern philosophies and religions (Taoism, Confucianism, Buddhism, Hinduism, and others) that interpret time as a number of sequen-
tial moments, each distinct in the characteristics and circumstances of what is experienced as the here and now. In this chapter, the experience of
time is described in the manner that will be familiar to most readers. This should not be taken to represent a devaluing of the divergent views of
time represented in other important philosophies.
Time Use and Patterns of Occupations 73

habits, social and cultural factors, age, physical status, and


role expectations. One obvious influence on time use is
available energy. Energy levels in the morning may
encourage activities requiring more exertion, such as
housecleaning or heavy chores. Societal expectations and
worker status also influence time use. For example, retired
people may have more time available for discretionary or
freely chosen occupations than those employed outside
the home or paid through a fixed hour per week schedule.
Activities may also be influenced by environmental
factors. For example, in agricultural areas, the different
seasons influence the time of year when planting and har-
vesting take place, so that different kinds of activities may
occur at specific times of the year. The recurring pattern
of these seasonal variations influences a lifestyle that is
highly dependent on nature.
Figure 4-1. Chronobiology is the study of the body's
Physiological factors, including hormones, may change
internal clocks. One approach for studying these tem-
levels of arousal, also influencing activity levels and
poral patterns orchestrated within the brain is through
choices. Human activity typically shows recurring pat-
measuring the electrical signals of brain waves.
terns, producing a certain daily rhythm. This natural cycle
(Reproduced with permission of Getty Images.)
is influenced by internal biological clocks, which are
thought to be influenced by hormonal changes and envi-
Physiological factors such as lower body temperatures, ronmental factors. The study of internal clocks is done
high levels of arousal, and concentrations of dopamine are within a specialized field known as chronobiology.
thought to influence the perception of time through the
body’s internal biological clocks (Schleidt & Kien, 1997). Chronobiology
Experiments have shown that, as people age, their esti-
The term chronobiology is derived from the Greek words
mates of the amount of time that has passed, particularly
chronos (time), bios (life), and logos (reason). Although the
while doing an activity, are higher than people at younger
first known experiment on biological rhythms was done on
ages. This suggests that as people mature, they perceive
the heliotrope plant by a French astronomer in the 18th
that time is moving faster than it actually is, which may
century, serious advances in understanding biological
explain why there is the nearly universal perception that
rhythms in humans did not begin until the late 1950s
years get shorter as we get older.
(Pittendrigh, 1960). By then, it was well known that the
The noted psychologist William James was one of the
human body was influenced by many rhythms and that
first people to write about the perception of time in his
daily patterns of activity were influenced by a rest activity-
classic work, Principles of Psychology, published in 1890
rhythm that was circadian in nature (i.e., having a 24-hour
(James, 1890a). In a chapter on time perception, James
cycle). The term circadian is from the Latin words circa
discussed a concept widely known as the “specious pres-
(meaning around) and dia (meaning day).
ent,” during which he acknowledged that the present is
During any one day, a person goes through periods of
really an interval of time kept in short-term memory
higher and lower arousal or activity. This is known as the
because as soon as an event happens and is perceived, it
rest/activity cycle and has a profound impact on occupa-
becomes part of the immediate past. Scientists continue
tional engagement. Environmental changes can disrupt
to debate the metaphysics of perceived time. Differing
that cycle and can cause physiological disturbances.
points of view include those who argue that there is objec-
Scientists are now aware that biological rhythms provide
tive time that exists independently of individual percep-
optimal times for human function and can significantly
tion (even though the measurement of time is a human
influence the value of medical or therapeutic interven-
invention and units of time are relatively arbitrary and
tions. This knowledge is the basis for an emerging focus in
have evolved through consensus) and those who believe
medicine called chronotherapeutics (Decousus, 1994).
that perceived time is a result of how people interpret
Most people know about jet lag, which occurs when
events in their mind as having an order (Mellor, 1998).
travelers cross many time zones. The disruption that
sometimes occurs following such travel is called circadian
desynchronization. This disruption of internal biological
INFLUENCES ON TIME USE clocks can also occur as a result of shift work and can be
The use of time is influenced by a number of factors. A of limited duration or longer term. Common symptoms of
person’s selection of activities may be influenced by desynchronization include sleep loss, fatigue, diminished
74 Chapter Four

performance, loss of appetite, nervous tension, and a feel- PURPOSES FOR STUDYING TIME USE
ing of malaise or ill health.
Scientists have now discovered that the biological Much of the current information on time use comes
clocks inside humans depend upon everyday events to from studies by governments, often gathered for the pur-
“set themselves.” Exposure to light, eating meals, and poses of understanding productivity and economic trends.
other routines or patterns of social activity are now Some consumer research and studies by social scientists
known to be important to daily rhythms of activity have also made useful contributions to the general under-
because they serve as time-setters or zeitgebers for internal standing of how people spend their time. The expression
clocks (Aschoff, 1960; Halberg, 1960). The study of “spending time” is interesting because it conveys a view of
human occupation often shows the connection between time that equates it with other commodities or resources
internal and external influences (such as social factors) that are consumed. The emphasis on consuming goods
on activity selection and time use (Christiansen, 1993; and services in many modern societies has influenced
Halberg, 1994). time use to the point that individuals may neglect to real-
ize the extent to which they fail to pursue daily endeavors
that promote health and/or are meaningful to them
THE WHEN, WHAT, AND WHERE (Peloquin, 1990).
OF HUMAN OCCUPATIONS Historically, the lack of consistent approaches to clas-
sifying or describing everyday time use has made detailed
People seldom stop to think about how they actually comparisons among these various studies difficult
spend their time. Most people can tell you what they did (Harvey, 1990). However, cooperative efforts have been
during the day, and they may recall some of their specific started under the auspices of the United Nations
activities during the past week. If they are goal-oriented Statistics Division to develop standardized nomenclature
and structure their time use through careful planning, and approaches for time use surveys (Bediako & Vanek,
they will be better informed, but a typical person may 1999). The Trial International Classification of Activities
develop a routine that varies little unless he or she expe- for Time Use Statistics provides a recommended strategy
riences a significant life change, such as a relocation, new for collecting time use data from large populations
job, marriage or divorce, significant illness or injury, or (Bediako & Vanek, 1999) and represents an important
death of someone close to them. Thus, unless an activity first step in this global effort (Figure 4-2).
is notable for some reason or changes from the usual rou- The United Nations uses a conceptual approach known
tine, it will fade from memory rather quickly. As a result, as the System of National Accounts (SNA) to identify
most people have only a vague idea about how they allo- and account for the activities that contribute to the econ-
cate their time. omy of a country. Typically, activities that contribute to
Yet, there are practical benefits to understanding time wealth include both market- and nonmarket-related activ-
use. Because time allocations for different occupations ities. Therefore, measures other than traditional economic
influence the consumption of goods and services, people statistics are required to gain a full understanding of the
who manufacture and sell products are interested in time human productivity in a population. Time use studies can
use data to determine the potential for marketing their help contribute to a more complete overall picture of a
products, and people who provide services are interested in nation’s economic standing (Ironmonger, 1999).
tailoring their businesses to the lifestyles of their customers. Of the 10 major groups of the Trial International
Because there is often a close association between how time Classification for Time Use Research Survey, three relate
is used and a person’s location, city planners and architects to production and income-producing activities.
may have an interest in this information. Economists are Data from time use studies collected for economic pur-
interested in the production of goods and services to meas- poses are often useful for discerning cultural changes and
ure the wealth of regions and nations and to understand patterns of work and leisure. Each major group contains
trends in economic development. Public health officials are subcategories that represent clusters of everyday occupa-
also interested in time use statistics to determine potential tions with related purposes. For example, the major group
exposure to hazardous substances or pathogens. labeled “personal care and self-maintenance” comprises
Psychologists, anthropologists, sociologists, and occupa- activities required to meet a person’s biological needs,
tional scientists are interested in time use data as everyday which include sleep, rest periods and naps, personal
measures of typical behavior, cultural patterns and trends, hygiene, bathing and dressing, meals and self-adminis-
and lifestyles. How people use time also indicates their tered health-related care, and relaxing.
underlying beliefs, values, interests, needs, and personality
dispositions (Little, Lecci, & Watkinson, 1992).
Time Use and Patterns of Occupations 75

Figure 4-2. Major categories identified within a United Nations sanctioned approach to
standardize studies of time use across nations. (Adapted from Statistics Division,
Department of Economic and Social Affairs, United Nations.)

GATHERING TIME USE DATA information provided by the International Association of


Time Use Research (IATUR), nearly 100 countries have
There are a number of approaches to studying how sponsored studies of time use during the past 30 years.
people spend their time, including observation, structured These government-sponsored studies are done for purpos-
questions, activity logs, and time diaries. According to es of understanding economic and social trends and for
76 Chapter Four

informing public policy. Other studies, done by social sci- approaches have been used by social scientists interested
entists or businesses, seek to understand the time use of in the occupations of special populations, such as, for
individuals for behavioral analysis or marketing purposes example, those with physical or mental disabilities or
(Robinson, 1999; Robinson & Nicosia, 1991). retirees. Cynkin (1979) and Neville (1980) each pro-
Occasionally, when time logs or diaries are used, the accu- posed the use of a segmented circle (viewed as a clock or
racy of these logs or diaries is then checked by random a pie graph) to record a day’s activities. Baum and
observations or interviews of family members (i.e., proxy Edwards (1993) developed a card sort to understand time
reporting) to establish their validity. use based on the percent of activities retained as a meas-
However, there is general agreement that time diaries, ure of time use among people with senile dementia.
capturing the flow of daily activity, are the best approach for It is apparent that in cultures where time is viewed in
capturing data to support understanding and theory devel- a less structured manner, such as with American Indians,
opment in human occupations. This is because time diaries Australian aborigines, and some cultures in the Pacific
allow the collection of multiple dimensions of human occu- Islands (to cite a few), the determination of time use may
pation (Robinson, 1999). A limitation of diaries, of course, be more difficult, not only for conceptual reasons, but
is that they often fail to capture the context in which a par- because such cultures may not have access to or use
ticular daily occupation is being performed. clocks, radios, or other devices that would enable the rec-
Szalai (1972) provides an early account of multinational ollection of exactly when certain activities occurred.
time use studies. More recently, to derive a more complete
understanding of how time is used, researchers have asked
subjects to report additional data that would provide con- HOW PEOPLE USE TIME
textual information about their activities (Harvey, 2000).
Thus, subjects may be asked to report where, when, for Just as occupations are influenced by the contextual
whom, and with whom a reported activity was being done. features of the situations in which they are performed,
In some cases, researchers also collect information on fea- time use is also subject to the same contexts (Harvey,
tures of time use that would provide distinguishing informa- 2000). Time use statistics are therefore influenced by the
tion, such as whether or not the nature of an occupation was social and economic conditions within the countries and
work- or leisure-related. As an example, if a person record- regions in which they are conducted. Surveys indicate
ed that he or she was reading a book, the researchers might that, on average, within developed countries, approxi-
try, through requesting information on the title of the book, mately 30% of a typical 24-hour day is spent sleeping;
to determine if it was being read for enjoyment or work- 10% is allocated to self-maintenance and self-care activi-
related purposes. ties (which includes eating); and another 10% is allocat-
Because individuals often are doing more than one ed to household maintenance, such as cooking, laundry,
activity at a time, scientists are interested in distinguish- housecleaning, and shopping. Approximately 25% of a
ing between primary and secondary activities. In time use person’s daily time is spent on work or work-related trav-
research, primary activities represent those endeavors that el. Thus, 60% of the waking day is devoted to what can be
constitute the principal pursuit of the moment. Secondary termed obligatory or required activities, including produc-
activities are defined as other activities done concurrent- tive employment or work, self-care, and household main-
ly with the main or primary activity being reported for a tenance. Of course, such statistics provide summary
particular interval of activity. For example, a person may descriptions of time use that are typical of the average per-
be listening to music and talking on a cellular phone son. In truth, few individuals will spend their time in a
while driving to work. In this example, driving to work manner exactly as described by this average profile.
would be the primary activity, while listening to music Examination of individual time use diaries makes it clear
and talking on the phone would be secondary because that there is infinite variety and scope to the pattern and
they are nested or enfolded within the primary act of driv- duration of individual lives.
ing to work. Concurrent activities add complexity to the With this qualification in mind, time use averages
study of time use and increase the importance of discern- seem to demonstrate some international consistency
ing the context within which a particular occupation is across developed or industrialized nations. An analysis of
being performed. percentages of time reported in certain activities for
Others studies of time use have collected subjective adults in Canada and the United States closely resembles
information, such as the level of perceived tension associ- data collected in other countries, including Sweden
ated with each daily activity (Michelson, 1985). (Sjoberg, 1990), Germany (Baltes, Wahl, & Schmid-
Although time diaries and structured interviews are the Furstoss, 1990), and Australia (Castles, 1994; McLennan,
most frequently used approaches to studying the patterns 1997). These data show remarkably consistent patterns,
of time use among the general population, other both for required and discretionary categories of daily
occupation. Analysis of time use by population subgroups
Time Use and Patterns of Occupations 77

TEMPORAL DIMENSIONS OF
OCCUPATIONAL BEHAVIOR
The order and duration of daily occupations often has a
familiar pattern, influenced by environmental changes and
biological clocks. Thus, the predictability of lives from day
to day is derived from the consistency of occupational pat-
terns. Necessary occupations, such as self-care, eating, and
sleeping, are typically repeated as part of daily life. Paid
employment outside the home also provides a temporal
structure that encourages a regular round of occupations.
The patterns of daily life, constructed from the order and
duration of occupations, can be either healthful or harmful.
Included in the various patterns are those whose duration is
influenced by attention (resulting in either too much or too
little engagement) and those whose order is repeated to a
Table 4-1. Time Use Data from the UK Time Use person’s benefit or potential harm.
Survey, Year 2000. This graph compiled from a recent People differ in the frequency with which they engage
time use survey in the United Kingdom shows how in certain activities, the length of time they typically
people change their time use on weekdays and week- spend doing them, and the order in which occupations are
ends. The graph depicts time use by persons 8 years pursued. Collectively, these temporal characteristics or
old and above. The graph reveals that, on average, dimensions can be described as patterns of occupational
people sleep an extra 43 minutes on a weekend day behavior. In everyday language, we describe such patterns
compared to a week day. Employment makes up less with words like habit, custom, routine, ritual, ceremony, reg-
than an hour on a weekend day compared to an aver- imen, lifestyle, and way of life. These terms specify charac-
age of 3 hour and 15 minutes on a weekday. teristics relating to duration, frequency, order, and repeti-
Weekends typically involve more housework, TV tion. Scientists have not yet agreed on definitions for
watching, social life and sport. (Reprinted with per- these patterns and whether or not some terms currently in
mission of Her Majesty’s Stationery Office.) use describe distinct characteristics.

by age, gender, and educational level may show variations


in the amount of time devoted to various activities. Table PATTERNS OF
4-1 summarizes the average time reported in selected
activities for a population sample in the United States DURATION AND FREQUENCY
(Tsang & Klepeis, 1995). What compels a person to engage in an occupation
Studies have shown that the number of activities peo- with great frequency or for extended periods? Some occu-
ple perform during any given time segment may differ sig- pations become so engaging they are pursued continuous-
nificantly. Also, the duration of time spent in occupations ly to the exclusion of eating, sleeping, or self-care, occa-
seems to vary by category and can reveal significant age, sionally leading to negative health consequences. People
gender, cultural, and role differences. who become compulsive gamblers exhibit this type of
For example, a study of adults in Germany (Baltes et al., behavior, but there are also people who watch television
1990) found that, on average, people who reported engag- compulsively, play computer games, or surf the Internet
ing in more obligatory activities also engaged in a greater for long durations. The obsessive nature of these patterns
number of discretionary activities. Thus, active people tend- may indicate the presence of mental illness.
ed to be busier than less active people in all aspects of their While there may be biological causes of obsessive behav-
lives, not just in one area or another. Researchers have the- ior, there also seem to be characteristics of activities them-
orized that some people have natural tendencies to be selves that impel prolonged engagement. A study involving
active, and this theory seems to be supported by studies of university students suggested that one important element of
activity frequency (Stones & Kozma, 1989). this phenomenon, which Carlson (1995) terms occupational
perseverance (p. 145), was the individual’s perceived progress
toward meeting an important or valued goal. This tendency
to continue pursuing the activity seems to be distinct from
habits and is a byproduct of the interests of the individual
78 Chapter Four

and the nature of the activity. Some occupations encourage patterned predispositions that enable individuals to
engagement because of the pleasure they bring in balancing respond to their situations with economy of thought and
challenges and skills. Chess masters of similar levels of skill action: they can act while focusing attention elsewhere.
would probably lose track of time were it not necessary for His thoughtful analysis, influenced greatly by the work of
them to execute their moves within designated intervals. William James (James, 1890b), recognized that habits can
Many hobbyists find a similar escape to interests in their serve important supportive purposes as humans respond to
leisure pursuits. environmental demands.
Csikszentimihalyi, a psychologist at the University of The supportive nature of habit has been further dis-
Chicago, has studied the characteristics of activities that cussed by Michael Young (Young, 1988). He proposed the
engage people by providing a level of challenge that following:
encourages the development of higher levels of mastery • Habits increase skill in action by enabling a person
(Csikszentimihalyi, 1978). His name for this level of to focus less on a given action and more on its elab-
attention and the perception of timelessness it engenders oration.
is flow. While engaged in flow activities, people frequent- • Habits reduce fatigue because they require an econ-
ly lose track of time (Csikszentimihalyi, 1990). Yet, flow omy of effort.
as defined by Csikszentimihalyi does not represent a pat- • Habits free attention for the unpredictable,
tern of occupational engagement but rather a property or enabling a person quickly to detect novel or threat-
characteristic of such engagement as experienced by the ening stimuli.
doer or participant.
• Habits enable a person to exercise functions with-
At the other end of the continuum are people who
out having to recall and attend to specific elements
spend so little time engaged in what they do that they
of a given practice.
move seemingly randomly from one activity to the next,
doing many things but unable to attend for long to any These perceived benefits of habit have been extended
one activity. This type of behavior suggests the presence by Clark, who observes that habits enable attention and
of distractibility, either due to anxiety or the presence of energy to be devoted to individual creativity, thereby
a short-term memory disorder that causes people to forget enriching opportunity for self-expression and creation of
what they were doing in the moments immediately pre- identity (Clark, 2000).
ceding the present. Seeking to provide a neurological basis for explaining
the performance supporting nature of habit, Dunn (2000)
proposed that a continuum exists governed by a person’s
PATTERNS OF sensitivity to stimuli and the stability of his or her behav-
ioral responsiveness to such thresholds. According to her
ORDER AND REPETITION formulation, under normal (or habit utility) conditions,
repetitive patterns of occupational engagement are bal-
It is not uncommon in everyday language to hear peo-
anced to be supportive of human performance as required
ple discuss patterns of behavior pertaining to order and
by varying situations. Her continuum is anchored at one
repetition. The words habit, routine, ritual, and lifestyle are
end by habit impoverishment, characterized by over- and
used variously (and sometimes interchangeably) to refer
under-responsiveness to stimuli, as found in depression
to different instances of these patterns. At this point,
and disorders of attention. At the other extreme, habit
however, there is insufficient research and agreement
domination results from over-responsiveness to stimuli as
among scientists to provide conclusive definitions for
characterized by addictive behaviors (Dunn, 2000).
these terms or authoritative descriptions of similarities
Although the word habit has been used to describe behav-
and differences among them. With this caution in mind,
iors of very short duration (such as biting a fingernail),
the following sections summarize preliminary thinking
the term has also been used to describe longer sequences
about these concepts and their associated phenomena.
of actions or practices that typify character and personal-
ity, such as repeatedly demonstrating initiative or being
Habits polite and respectful of others. While such traits are
Habit is a concept that embraces notions of both order repeated in different situations and may involve similar
and repetition in human behavior that has been discussed sequences of specific acts, it is unclear how they would
and studied for a century. Despite this, little consensus has qualify for inclusion in Dunn’s continuum. Therefore, the
been achieved on the definition of habit. Although habits suitability of using the term in this sense is uncertain even
are typically connected with observed behaviors, it is if habit is defined in the very general manner proposed by
important to acknowledge that there are habits of Dewey. The ambiguity surrounding precise definitions
thought as well as action. Dewey (1922) defined habits as may explain why the terms habit and routine are sometimes
Time Use and Patterns of Occupations 79
used interchangeably. In the next section, we address this and that a disruption to any one life would become disrup-
apparent confusion. tive to the overall patterns of daily life for many of the resi-
dents, whose lives had become highly routinized in this
Routines place. This “place-related” environmental influence on
time use is geographical in the sense that it locates people in
Routines have been defined as “habitual, repeatable, proximity, who then adopt routines of occupation that have
and predictable ways of acting” (Corbin, 1999). collective influences on others in the community. Other
According to Clark (2000), not all habits are routines and studies have reported findings similar to those described by
not all routines are habits. Yet, she asserts, all routines Rowles (Cutchin, 2000; Rubenstein, 1986).
consist of actions or occupations:
Habits are the relatively automatic things a per-
son thinks or does repeatedly. Routines, in con-
Routinization
trast, are a type of higher order habit that involves The term routinization is used in the psychological lit-
sequencing and combining processes, procedures, erature to refer to a trait or disposition to depend on rou-
steps, or occupations. Routines specify what a per- tine for everyday function. Reich and Zautra have studied
son will do and in what order and therefore con- the trait of routinization, which they hypothesize is a
stitute a mechanism for achieving given outcomes characteristic representing the extent to which people
and an orderly life. (Clark, 2000, p. 128S) (particularly at later stages of life) become so attached to
Clark offers that routines provide a structure that serves their regular routines that they react aversively to changes
to organize and maintain individual lives and that occupa- in that routine (Reich & Zautra, 1991). They define rou-
tions can be thought of as the building blocks of daily rou- tinization as the extent to which people are “motivated to
tines (Clark, 2000). This structural, supportive view of the maintain the daily events of their lives in relatively
function of routines in daily life is shared by other scien- unchanging and orderly patterns of regularity” (Reich &
tists, including Bond and Feather (Bond, 1988) and Zautra, 1991, p. 161).
Ludwig (1997, 1998) based on qualitative and quantitative There appears to be two factors involved in this dispo-
studies of individuals in their natural environments. sition sameness, including the need for order and routine
It is clear that routines are supported by, and perhaps in daily life and an aversion or dislike of disruption of such
even dependent on, the presence of environmental con- routine. Studies of routinization suggest that they are
ditions or situational stimuli. For example, mealtime cus- related to coping styles and the effectiveness of dealing
toms are clearly evident in cultures and influence these with stressful life events and may have implications for
behaviors independently of hunger or physiological needs. mental health and well-being, particularly for people with
Businesses and other employers may prescribe working chronic illness, disability, or functional declines due to
hours that impose a temporal structure that creates neces- aging (Reich, 2000).
sary routines. This is noted later in the section on occu-
pational deprivation. Ritual
A ritual is a type of occupational behavior that shares
Place and Routine characteristics with habits and routines but seems clearly
Another aspect of environment that influences rou- distinct. Of the patterned occupations described in the
tine is the place in which a person lives. Social geogra- literature, there is most agreement surrounding the con-
phers have studied the impact of place on human behav- cept of ritual. Rituals are prescribed occupations that are
ior and have found that it serves to interconnect the rou- intentional in nature and that typically hold special sig-
tines of individuals within a location so that a routine of nificance and meaning for those performing them. Rituals
one person may be an important environmental feature exist for many purposes and trace their origins to a point
that supports the routine of another. For example, Rowles before recorded history (Chase & Dibble, 1987). While
(2000), a social geographer, has extensively studied the many rituals are cultural and therefore easily recognizable
routine patterns of behavior of seniors in a small rural to others within the culture, others are more personal and
town. On a typical day, a resident of the community may may be undertaken privately within specific families or by
make his or her way to a given community destination, individuals to commemorate special occasions or events
such as the store or coffee shop, to be predictably followed or to accomplish other purposes.
by a journey to the community senior center, to converse It is not unusual for rituals to mark significant begin-
with friends for a set period, then to return home using a nings, such as the commissioning of a ship, the birth or
familiar path at the same time each day. baptism of a newborn, the union of two people who agree
Rowles observed that the elderly participants involved in to share a life together, or the beginning of a new job.
his study were part of a larger community pattern of events Rituals are also undertaken to mark the end of a period or
80 Chapter Four

way of life (such as bachelor parties, divorce parties,


funerals, cremations, retirements), the completion of
important tasks and the attainment of goals (including
graduation ceremonies, negotiated contracts, or extensive
projects), and the transition from one state or time period
to another (including birthdays, anniversary celebrations,
centennials, and days commemorating the birth of certain
heroes and celebrities of state). Some rituals, particularly
those marking transitions (such as graduations and retire-
ments) may be celebrated for a combination of purposes,
such that they simultaneously may mark the completion
of one period of life and the beginning of another.
Widely practiced cultural rituals are typically marked
by performances that may include prescribed language,
symbolic objects and places, ceremonial dress (such as for-
malwear or uniforms), and attendance by people occupy-
ing specific societal positions (such as spiritual leaders,
educational officials, particular family members, special
friends, or officials sanctioned by the state).
Personal rituals may include language, symbolic Figure 4-3. One common habit is often part of
objects and place, and certain dress, and may be practiced a daily routine in the morning and afternoon.
to celebrate any number of significant events in a person’s Walking pets, when done at a consistent time,
life, such as a successful surgery, the beginning of a friend- can also serve as a zeitgeber, or timekeeper,
ship, or the attainment of a personal goal. Whether cul- to help entrain biological clocks. (Repro-
tural or personal in nature, the celebration of rituals is duced with permission of Getty Images.)
often viewed as important to the experience of time and
is a characteristic of daily life that can be used to describe
lifestyles (Figure 4-3).
promotion and disease prevention because links between
Lifestyles lifestyle characteristics (such as activity engagement, nutri-
tional habits, and coping behaviors) have been shown to be
Habits, routines, and rituals help define lifestyles. A related to life expectancy, well-being, and health
lifestyle can be defined as a distinctive mode of living that (Amundson, Hart, & Holmes, 1981; Baltes et al., 1990;
is both observable and recognizable and over which the Bartley, 1994; Dean, Colomer, & Perez-Hoyos, 1995).
individual has choice (Sobel, 1981). Elliott (1993) notes Targeted groups for lifestyle studies include segments of
that a routine or established way of dealing with personal the population selected for characteristics such as age,
needs and the demands of the environment, as well as an geographic location, gender, ethnicity, economic status,
established and consistent pattern of involvement in a employment, and leisure participation and interests. Time
particular type of behavior, are also important character- use studies constitute one particular form of lifestyle
istics of lifestyles. research.
Fidler (1996) suggests that each individual has a
unique lifestyle profile that should be understood in terms
of the extent to which it permits attention to four essen- DISRUPTED PATTERNS OF TIME USE
tial domains of living, which she identifies as self-mainte-
nance, intrinsic gratification, social contribution, and What happens when established patterns of time use
interpersonal relatedness. are disrupted? This question is of interest to social scien-
Clearly, lifestyles are associated with cultures, places, his- tists and health professionals because of the known rela-
tories (and thus age groups), genders, personalities, and tionships between lifestyle, health, and happiness.
other personal characteristics. Attempts to generalize spe- Disruptions in lifestyle routines can be commonplace and
cific patterns are probably optimally done within specific temporary, such as that experienced during travel to dif-
cultural groups or subcultures. Thus, lifestyle research has ferent environments, changes in close relationships, expe-
tended to focus on specific behaviors of interest to urban riencing minor illness or injury, disruptions caused by
planners, economists, marketing analysts, and those inter- severe weather, construction, or renovation projects, or
ested in human development. There has been considerable even the need for car repairs (Whiteford, 2004). More
interest in lifestyles as these pertain to understanding health significant disruptions in occupational pattern or lifestyle
Time Use and Patterns of Occupations 81
can be caused by permanent changes of location, through use studies have not been concerned with specific contexts
isolated living circumstances, unemployment, retirement, or qualities of experience during occupational engagement.
imprisonment, refugeeism, cultural restrictions on partic- Patterns of time use were also described based on char-
ipation, or severe illness or injury leading to disability. acteristics of frequency, duration, sequence, and repeti-
These more serious restrictions to a person’s use of time tion. Using these characteristics, the concepts of habit,
can have significant consequences and have been routine, routinization, ritual, and lifestyle were defined
described in the literature as forms of occupational depri- and discussed as special approaches to discussing patterns
vation (Whiteford, 2000). of occupation. It was noted that the relative dearth of
Holmes and Rahe studied the relationship between life research in this area and the variety of disciplines with
events and health in a classical study published in 1967 interest in human time use has precluded the develop-
(Holmes, 1967; Holmes & Rahe, 1967). In their Social ment of consensus surrounding concepts and definitions.
Readjustment Rating Scale (also known as the schedule or The chapter concluded with a brief discussion of disrup-
recent events), they identified events that require a tions to occupational engagement, including life changes
change in daily life patterns and viewed these changes as and environments that are occupationally deprived.
potentially stressful because of their disruption of typical
patterns of time use. They found that people who experi-
enced more disruptions in their typical lifestyle during the REFERENCES
previous year tended to experience greater numbers of
adverse health episodes in the ensuing months, which Amundson, M. E., Hart, C. A., & Holmes, T. H. (1981). About
the schedule of recent experience. Psychiatric Annals, 11, 210.
they attributed to reduced resistance to illness. Their
research led to further development of theories viewing Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health, stress, and coping: New perspec-
tives on mental and physical well-being. San Francisco, CA:
life stress as resulting from disruption of ordinary routines
Jossey-Bass.
and the manner in which individual characteristics per-
Aronoff, M. S. (1991). Sleep and its secrets: The river of crystal
mit effective coping and adaptation to such events
light. Los Angeles, CA: Insight Books.
(Amundson et al., 1981; Antonovsky, 1979).
Aschoff, J. (1960). Exogenous and endogenous components in
Some extended changes in environment, such as insti-
circadian rhythms. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia, 25, 11-28.
tutionalization, incarceration, or homelessness, can result
Babkoff, H., Caspy, T., Mikulincer, M., & Sing, H. C. (1991).
in deprived or impoverished conditions for time use. Such
Monotonic and rhythmic influences: A challenge for sleep
occupational deprivation can result in significant physical deprivation research. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 411-428.
and psychological consequences (Whiteford, 1995, 1997,
Baltes, M. M., Wahl, H. W., & Schmid-Furstoss, U. (1990). The
2000). It is possible that sleep difficulties can be viewed as daily life of elderly Germans: Activity patterns, personal
a special category of occupational deprivation because control, and functional health. The Journals of Gerontology,
sleep occupies a significant portion of each day and is 45, 173-179.
known as a necessary routine for self-maintenance and Bartley, M. (1994). Unemployment and ill health:
survival (Aronoff, 1991; Babkoff, Caspy, Mikulincer, & Understanding the relationship. Journal of Epidemiology and
Sing, 1991). There is also some indication that activity Community Health, 48, 333-337.
patterns during alert states influence the nature and qual- Baum, C. M., & Edwards, D. (1993). Cognitive performance in
ity of sleep (Horne, 1985). senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: The kitchen task
assessment. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 47,
431-436.
SUMMARY Bediako, G., & Vanek, J. (1999). Trial international classifica-
tion of activities for time use statistics. Paper presented at
In this chapter, the important relationship between the International Conference on Time-Use (ICTU),
occupations and time use has been examined. It was noted University of Luneburg.
that the experience or perception of time is different from Bond, M. J. F. (1988). Some correlates of structure and purpose
measured or clock time and has an influence on individual in the use of time. Journal of Personality and Social
behavior. Other influences on time use were described, Psychology, 55, 321-329.
including cultural, biological, and social factors. Prominent Camporese, R., Freguja, C., & Sabbadini, L. L. (1998). Time use
among these are internal clocks or physiological mecha- by gender and quality of life. Social Indicators Research, 44,
nisms that are studied in the field of chronobiology. 119-144.
Studies of how people use time were described and sum- Carlson, M. (1995). The self-perpetuation of occupations. In R.
marized. It was noted that the purposes of such studies have Zemke & F. Clark (Eds.), Occupational science: The emerging
traditionally ranged from understanding national economic discipline (pp. 143-158). Philadelphia, PA: F. A. Davis.
conditions to marketing and public health. Typically, time Castles, I. (1994). How Australians use their time. Canberra:
Australia Bureau of Statistics.
82 Chapter Four

EVIDENCE WORKSHEET
Author(s) Year Topic Method Conclusion
Camporese et al. 1998 Time use by gender and Descriptive Suggests that a disparity in roles related to
quality of life study time spent in housework between men
DES and women contributes to a difference in
perceived quality of life
Monk et al. 1990 The Social Rhythm DES Older adults have distinct rhythms influ-
Metric—An instrument enced by routines, and disruption in these
to Quantify the Daily routines may lead to difficulties that com-
Rhythms of Life promise health and well-being
Reich & Williams 2003 Exploring the properties DES Factor analysis of responses from a sample
of habits and routines of students suggested that underlying fac-
in daily life tors of habit pertain to cognition and sen-
sory reactions, habitual behaviors, and
motivations of approach and avoidance
Robinson & 1994 The overestimated work- DES Comparisons of estimates and time diaries
Bostrom week: What time diary suggest that people perceive that they are
measures suggest working more than they actually are
Rowles 2000 Habituation and being DES From an ethnographic study of growing old
in place in an Appalachian community, it was shown
that older people make habitual adjust-
ments in the use and the meaning of the
spaces and the places of their lives as they
accommodate to changing circumstances

Chase, P., & Dibble, H. (1987). Middle Paleolithic symbolism: Decousus, H. (1994). Chronobiology in hemostasis. In H. E.
A review of current evidence and interpretations. Journal of Touitou (Ed.), Biologic rhythms in clinical and laboratory med-
Anthropological Archaeology, 6, 263-296. icine (pp. 555-565). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Christiansen, C. (1993). Three perspectives on balance in occu- Dewey, J. (1922). Human nature and conduct. New York, NY:
pation. In F. C. R. Zemke (Ed.), Occupational science: Henry Holt.
Selections from the symposia. Philadelphia, PA: F. A. Davis. Dunn, W. W. (2000). Habit: What’s the brain got to do with it?
Clark, F. A. (2000). The concepts of habit and routine: A pre- The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research,
liminary theoretical synthesis. The Occupational Therapy 20(Supplement 1), 2S-5S.
Journal of Research, 20, 123S-138S. Eisler, A. D., & Eisler, H. (1994). Subjective time scaling:
Corbin, P. K. A. (1999). The incorporation of physical disabili- Influence of age, gender, and Type A and Type B behavior.
ty into the self (adolescents, adjustment, identity develop- Chronobiologia, 21, 185-200.
ment). Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: the Elliott, D. S. (1993). Health enhancing and health compromis-
Sciences & Engineering, 60(4-B), 1884. ing lifestyles. In A. C. Millstein (Ed.), Health enhancing and
Csikszentimihalyi, M. (1978). Intrinsic rewards and emergent health compromising lifestyles (pp. 119-145). New York, NY:
motivation. In M. L. D. Green (Ed.), The hidden costs of Oxford University Press.
rewards. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum. Emirbayer, M., & Mische, A. (1998). What is agency? American
Csikszentimihalyi, M. (1990). Flow—The psychology of optimal Journal of Sociology, 102, 962-1023.
experience. New York, NY: Harper and Row. Fidler, G. S. (1996). Lifestyle performance: From profile to con-
Cutchin, M. P. (2000). Retention of rural physicians: Place inte- ceptual model. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,
gration and the triumph of habit. The Occupational Therapy 50(2), 139-147.
Journal of Research, 20(Supplement 1), 106S-111S. Halberg, F. (1960). Temporal coordination of physiologic func-
Cynkin, S. (1979). Occupational therapy: Toward health through tion. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia, 25, 289-310.
activities (1st ed.). Boston, MA: Little, Brown & Co. Halberg, F. (1994). Introduction to chronobiology. Minneapolis,
Dean, K., Colomer, C., & Perez-Hoyos, S. (1995). Research on MN: Medtronic.
lifestyles and health: Searching for meaning. Social Science
and Medicine, 41(6), 845-855.
Time Use and Patterns of Occupations 83

Harvey, A. S. (1990). Guidelines for time use data collection. Reich, J. W., & Zautra, A. (1991). Analyzing the trait of rou-
Social Indicators Research, 30, 97-228. tinization in older adults. International Journal of Aging and
Harvey, A. S. (2000). Use of context in time use research. Paper Human Development, 32, 161-180.
Given at Expert Group Meeting on Methods for Conducting Robinson, J. P. (1999). The time-diary method. In A. S.
Time-Use Surveys. New York, NY: United Nations Pentland & M. A. M. Lawton (Eds.), Time use research in
Secretariat-Statistics Division. the social sciences (pp. 47-89). New York, NY: Kluwer
Holmes, T. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Academic Plenum Publishers.
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-218. Robinson, J. P., & Bostrom, A. (1994). The overestimated work-
Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The Social Readjustment week: What time diary measures suggest. Monthly Labor
Rating Scale (SRRS). Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, Review, 117(8), 11-23.
213-218. Robinson, J. P., & Nicosia, F. N. (1991). Of time, activity, and con-
Horne, J. A. (1985). Sleep and sleepiness following a behav- sumer behavior: An essay on findings, interpretations, and
iourally “active” day. Ergonomics, 28, 567-575. needed research. Journal of Business Research, 22, 171-186.
Ironmonger, D. (1999). An overview of time use surveys. Rowles, G. (2000). Habituation and being in place. The
International Seminar on Time Use Studies. Ahmedabad, Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 20(Supplement
India: Ministry of Statistics and Programme 1), 52S-67S.
Implementation, Government of India. Rubenstein, R. L. (1986). The construction of a day by elderly
James, W. (1890a). The perception of time. In Principles of psy- widowers. International Journal Aging and Human
chology (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Henry Holt. Development, 23, 161-193.
James, W. (1890b). Principles of psychology. New York, NY: Holt. Schleidt, M., & Kien, J. (1997). Segmentation in behavior and
Little, B. R., Lecci, L., & Watkinson, B. (1992). Personality and what it can tell us about brain function. Human Nature, 8,
personal projects—Linking big 5 and pac units of analysis. 77-111.
Journal of Personality, 60, 501-525. Sjoberg, L. M. (1990). Action and emotion in everyday life.
Ludwig, F. M. (1997). How routine facilitates well-being in older Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 31, 9-27.
women. Occupational Therapy International, 4, 213-228. Smyth, A. H. (Ed.). (1905). The writings of Benjamin Franklin.
Ludwig, F. M. (1998). The unpackaging of routine in older women. New York, NY: MacMillan.
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 52, 168-174. Sobel, M. E. (1981). Lifestyles and social structure. Concepts, def-
McLennan, W. (1997). How Australians use their time. Canberra, initions, analyses. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Australia: Australian Bureau of Statistics. Stones, M. J., & Kozma, A. (1989). Happiness and activities in
Mellor, D. H. (1998). Real time II. London: Routledge. later life: A propensity formulation. Canadian Psychology,
30, 526-537.
Michelson, W. (1985). From sun to sun: Daily obligations and
community structure in the lives of employed women and their Szalai, A. (Ed.). (1972). The use of time. The Hague,
families. Ottawa, Canada: Rowman and Allanheld. Netherlands: Mouton.
Monk, T. H., Flaherty, J. F., Frank, E., Hoskinson, K., & Kupfer, Tsang, A. M., & Klepeis, N. E. (1995). Descriptive Statistics
D. J. (1990). The social rhythm metric—An instrument to Tables from a Detailed Analysis of the National Human
quantify the daily rhythms of life. The Journal of Nervous Activity Pattern Survey (NHAPS) Data (No. U.S. EPA
and Mental Disease, 178(2), 120-126. Contract No. 68-W5-0011). Las Vegas, NV: U.S.
Environmental Protection Agency.
Neville, A. (1980). Temporal adaptation: Application with
short-term psychiatric patients. The American Journal of Whiteford, G. (1995). A concrete void: Occupational depriva-
Occupational Therapy, 34, 328-331. tion and the special needs inmate. Journal of Occupational
Science, 2, 80-81.
Peloquin, S. (1990). Time as a commodity. American Journal of
Occupational Therapy, 43, 775-782. Whiteford, G. (1997). Occupational deprivation and incarcera-
tion. Journal of Occupational Science: Australia, 4, 126-130.
Pittendrigh, C. S. (1960). Circadian rhythms and the circadian
organization of living systems. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia, Whiteford, G. (2000). Occupational deprivation: Global chal-
25, 159-182. lenge in the new millennium. British Journal of Occupational
Therapy, 64, 200-210.
Rao, S. M., Mayer, A. R., & Harrington, D. L. (2001). The evo-
lution of brain activation during temporal processing. Whiteford, G. (2004). When people cannot participate:
Nature Neuroscience, 4, 317-323. Occupational deprivation. In C. Christiansen & E.
Townsend (Eds.), Introduction to occupation: The art and sci-
Reich, J. W. (2000). Routinization as a factor in the coping and
ence of living (pp. 221-242). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
the mental health of women with fibromyalgia. The
Prentice-Hall.
Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 20(Supplement
1), 41S-51S. Whitrow, G. J. (1989). Time in history—Views of time from pre-
history to the present day. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Reich, J. W., & Williams, J. (2003). Exploring the properties of
habit and routine in daily life. OTJR: Occupation, Young, M. (1988). The metronomic society. Cambridge, MA:
Participation and Health, 23(2), 48-56. Harvard University Press.

You might also like