Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

European Journal of Neuroscience

European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 33, pp. 1328–1338, 2011 doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07619.x

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Technology, expertise and social cognition


in human evolution

Dietrich Stout,1 Richard Passingham,2 Christopher Frith,3 Jan Apel4 and Thierry Chaminade5
1
Department of Anthropology, Emory University, 1557 Dickey Drive, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
2
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
3
Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK
4
Department of Archaeology and Osteology, Gotland University College, Visby, Sweden
5
Mediterranean Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, CNRS – Université Aix-Marseille, Marseille Cedex, France

Keywords: cognitive processes, functional MRI, gesture, imitation, learning comprehension

Abstract
Paleolithic stone tools provide concrete evidence of major developments in human behavioural and cognitive evolution. Of particular
interest are evolving cognitive mechanisms implied by the cultural transmission of increasingly complex prehistoric technologies,
hypothetically including motor resonance, causal reasoning and mentalizing. To test the relevance of these mechanisms to specific
Paleolithic technologies, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of Naı̈ve, Trained and Expert subjects
observing two toolmaking methods of differing complexity and antiquity: the simple ‘Oldowan’ method documented by the earliest
tools 2.5 million years ago; and the more complex ‘Acheulean’ method used to produce refined tools 0.5 million years ago. Subjects
observed 20-s video clips of an expert demonstrator, followed by behavioural tasks designed to maintain attention. Results show that
observational understanding of Acheulean toolmaking involves increased demands for the recognition of abstract technological
intentions. Across subject groups, Acheulean compared with Oldowan toolmaking was associated with activation of left anterior
intraparietal and inferior frontal sulci, indicating the relevance of resonance mechanisms. Between groups, Naı̈ve subjects relied on
bottom-up kinematic simulation in the premotor cortex to reconstruct unfamiliar intentions, and Experts employed a combination of
familiarity-based sensorimotor matching in the posterior parietal cortex and top-down mentalizing involving the medial prefrontal
cortex. While no specific differences between toolmaking technologies were found for Trained subjects, both produced frontal
activation relative to Control, suggesting focused engagement with toolmaking stimuli. These findings support motor resonance
hypotheses for the evolutionary origins of human social cognition and cumulative culture, directly linking these hypotheses with
archaeologically observable behaviours in prehistory.

Introduction
Neither toolmaking (Beck, 1980) nor cultural transmission (Whiten opposed to intention reading (Saxe, 2005; Grafton, 2009), nor at
et al., 2007) is unique to humans. Yet there is a vast gulf between the which level(s) action representations are shared between individuals
accumulated (Tennie et al., 2009) complexity of human technology (Jacob & Jeannerod, 2005; de Vignemont & Haggard, 2008).
and that of any other living species. This disparity has been attributed To address these issues, we conducted a neuroimaging study in
to uniquely human physical (Johnson-Frey, 2003) or social (Tomasello which human subjects observed the making of Paleolithic stone tools.
et al., 2005) cognition, or both (Passingham, 2008). Stone toolmaking is the earliest known uniquely human behaviour
Motor hypotheses of action understanding (Gallese & Goldman, (Roux & Bril, 2005), dating back at least 2.6 million years (Semaw
1998; Blakemore & Decety, 2001) suggest a possible unification of et al., 2003). Previous research (Stout & Chaminade, 2007) used
these explanations. The ‘Motor Cognition Hypothesis’ (Gallese et al., FDG-positron emission tomography (PET) to study brain activation
2009) proposes that human social cognition has its phylogenetic and during stone toolmaking. In the earliest, ‘Oldowan’, technology a
ontogenetic origins in ‘motor resonance’. Distinctive human capacities ‘hammerstone’ held in the dominant hand is used to strike sharp
for technology, language and intersubjectivity might thus have a single ‘flakes’ from a cobble ‘core’ manipulated by the other hand. We found
origin in evolutionary modifications of a primate ‘mirror neuron this method to be associated with activation of parietal and frontal
system’ (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). However, it is not clear to brain regions involved in sensorimotor coordination, grip selection
what extent action understanding relies on motor resonance as and 3D shape perception. After that, 1.7 million years ago, more
complex ‘Acheulean’ technology developed. Here cores were inten-
tionally shaped into large cutting tools known as ‘handaxes’. We
Correspondence: Dr D. Stout, as above. found this method to be associated with activation of the right inferior
E-mail: dwstout@emory.edu
frontal gyrus (Stout et al., 2008), a region implicated in the
Received 22 April 2010, revised 15 December 2010, accepted 23 December 2010 hierarchical organization of action (Koechlin & Jubault, 2006).

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Technology and cognition in human evolution 1329

In the present study we used functional magnetic resonance imaging individuals. All subjects were right-handed by self-report and had no
(fMRI) to compare brain activation during the observation of Oldowan history of neurological illness. The study was approved by the
and Acheulean toolmaking. The Motor Cognition Hypothesis pro- National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery and the Institute of
poses that action understanding is tied to motor expertise (Gallese Neurology joint Ethics Committee. Twenty-one individuals with no
et al., 2009), but learning clearly requires understanding of actions not prior experience of stone toolmaking were recruited via advertise-
yet in the observer’s repertoire. Our design crossed observer expertise ments posted to electronic mailing lists maintained by the University
(Naı̈ve, Trained, Expert) with technological sophistication (Oldowan, College London Functional Imaging Lab and Institute of Archaeology.
Acheulean) to examine the contribution of resonance and interpreta- Respondents chose to participate in the Naı̈ve or Trained group.
tion in understanding actions of varying familiarity and complexity. Individuals who elected training attended 16 1-h training sessions over
An account in terms of motor resonance predicts expertise effects in an 8-week period, in groups of 2–3 subjects per session. During
the putative human mirror neuron system (Rizzolatti & Craighero, training, subjects were provided with tools and raw materials for
2004) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Buccino et al., 2004; Vogt practice, as well as demonstrations and interactive verbal and gestural
et al., 2007), regardless of complexity. An inferential account (Saxe, instruction by the first author. Subjects improved with training, but
2005) predicts complexity effects in brain regions associated with none achieved expertise in shaping handaxes (Supporting Information
mental state attribution, including the medial prefrontal cortex (Frith & Fig. S1). Products of the 1st, 8th and 16th sessions of each subject
Frith, 2006). A mixed model (Grafton, 2009) makes less exclusive were collected for further analysis (forthcoming). Because it became
predictions, but might involve a shift from resonance to inference with clear during training that subjects would not achieve the desired
increasing complexity and expertise. expertise, a group of five subjects with pre-existing expertise was
included in the ongoing study. Expert subjects were drawn from the
small extant community of academic and craft stone toolmakers, and
Methods were contacted directly. Imaging sessions for Naive, Trained and
Paleolithic toolmaking Expert subjects were interspersed over the course of the study.
Subjects in all groups received the same instructions before
The Paleolithic technologies investigated here are the same that were scanning, consisting of a scripted briefing, accompanying PowerPoint
addressed in previous FDG-PET studies of subjects actually making presentation, and Cogent script showing instructions and exemplar
stone tools (Stout & Chaminade, 2007; Stout et al., 2008). Oldowan stimuli (not used in experiment) as presented in the scanner. Crucially,
flaking, known from approximately 2.6–1.6 million years ago, is a instructions included a description of the methods and aims of
simple process of striking sharp cutting flakes from a stone core using Paleolithic stone toolmaking so that even Naı̈ve subjects had basic
direct percussion. However, even this simple technology requires conceptual knowledge of the technology.
substantial visuomotor coordination, including visual evaluation of
core morphology (e.g. edge angles, location of convexities and
concavities) in order to select appropriate targets for percussion, as
well as active proprioceptive sensation and precise bimanual coordi- Stimuli
nation to guide forceful blows to small targets on the core. Twenty-second video clips (Supporting Information Video S1) were
After approximately 1.7 million years ago, flake-based Oldowan extracted from full-length videos of an expert toolmaker (right-
technology began to be replaced by ‘Acheulean’ technology, involving handed) engaged in Oldowan flaking (n = 6), Acheulean shaping
the intentional shaping of cores into large cutting tools known as (n = 6) and the Control condition (n = 6). All videos were recorded on
‘picks’, ‘handaxes’ and ‘cleavers’. Such shaping requires greater the same day with constant camera position and lighting. The
perceptual-motor skill to precisely control stone fracture patterns and demonstrator was seated facing the camera, and supported the core on
more complex action plans that relate individual flake removals to his left thigh or above his lap in his left hand. The field of view
each other in pursuit of a distal goal. By 500 000 years ago, some included this workspace and the full range of arm movements, but did
Acheulean tools exhibit a high level of refinement that additionally not extend to the face. Flint from a single quarry in Suffolk, UK was
requires the careful preparation of edges and surfaces, known as used for all toolmaking, and video segments were deliberately selected
‘platform preparation’, before flake removals. Platform preparation is from early stages of flaking ⁄ shaping (e.g. prior to establishment of
often done on the face opposite a planned flake removal: the core is symmetrical ‘handaxe’ shape) so that size, shape, colour and other
flipped over (‘inverted’) and a new hammerstone and ⁄ or hammerstone large-scale visual characteristics of cores did not differ systematically
grip is selected and used to abrade ⁄ micro-flake the edge through light, across stimulus types. Nevertheless, action sequences portrayed in the
tangential blows. This preparatory operation introduces a new sub- clips clearly reflected technological differences.
routine to toolmaking action plans, increasing their hierarchical depth. Nine types of technological action were identified in the videos, and
It is the ‘Late Acheulean’ method that is studied here. their frequencies in the actual stimuli used recorded using the
As in previous FDG-PET studies, the current study also includes a EthoLog 2.2.5 behavioural transcription tool (Table 1). These are:
control condition that consists of simple bimanual percussion of an (i) percussive strikes with the right hand; (ii) shifts of the left-hand
unmodified core without any attempt to detach flakes. This condition core grip; (iii) rotations of the core in the left hand; (iv) shifts of the
is designed to include general demands of striking and manipulating a right-hand hammerstone grip; (v) inversions (flipping over) of the core
core, while omitting any more specific demands for percussive with the left hand; (vi) changing of the hammerstone (here the
accuracy, core support, target selection and strategic planning involved demonstrator reached off camera to exchange one hammerstone for
in actual toolmaking. another, see Supporting Information Video S1); (vii) abrasion ⁄ micro-
flaking of core edges with right hand; (viii) sweeping of detached
flakes and fragments off the thigh with the right hand (the
Subjects and training hammerstone itself or an extended finger may be used); (ix) grasping
Three subject groups were included in the study, comprising of a detached flake or fragment with the right hand to remove it from
technologically Naı̈ve (n = 11), Trained (n = 10) and Expert (n = 5) the thigh, usually with a side-to-side ‘scissor’ grip of index and middle

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
1330 D. Stout et al.

Table 1. Frequency of technological actions in fMRI stimuli indication plus a 1-s fixation screen, giving a total duration of 15 s.
Trials were interleaved so that in each session, experimental trials took
Action Hand Control Oldowan Acheulean place in blocks of two or three. Each session comprised one repetition
of each of the six conditions; the order was pseudo-randomized to
Percussion R 81 36 26
Shift core grip L 39 12 13 avoid repetition of the type of stimulus in consecutive trials and of
Rotate core L 37 16 7 individual stimulus videos within a session. Presentation of stimuli
Shift hammerstone grip R 3 0 8 and recording of participants’ responses were carried out using Cogent
Invert core L 0 2 7 (http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent_graphics.php) running in Mat-
Change hammerstone R 0 1 3
lab 6.5 (MathWorks).
Abrade ⁄ micro-flake R 0 0 12
Sweep R 0 14 8
Grasp flake R 0 8 3
Flakes detached N⁄A 0 29 16 fMRI recording and preprocessing
In each of the six experimental sessions, a T2*-weighted, gradient-
echo, echo-planar imaging sequence was used to acquire 164 40-slice
fingers, rarely (twice) with a pad-to-pad ‘pincer’ grip of thumb and (2 mm thickness and 1 mm gap; TE = 65 ms; a = 90 ) volumes
index finger (Supporting Information Video S1). Importantly, the total covering the whole brain and cerebellum with an in-plane resolution
number of actions declines from Control to Oldowan to Acheulean of 3 · 3 mm (64 · 64 matrix, fov 192 · 192 · 144 mm3;
stimuli. This is also true for right- and left-hand actions considered TR = 2600 ms). A high-resolution (1 · 1 · 1 mm3) structural image
separately. Thus, increasing activation across conditions must be (MPRAGE sequence) was also collected.
explained in terms of the increasing diversity and causal ⁄ intentional fMRI data were analysed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/
complexity of actions rather than their simple quantity. spm) procedures, running in Matlab 7.6 (MathWorks), after
There are four action types that are substantially more numerous in, discarding the first four dummy volumes in each session to allow
or unique to, Acheulean stimuli: hammerstone grip shifts; hammer- for T1 equilibrium effect. Slice timing correction was applied to
stone changes; core inversions; and abrasion ⁄ micro-flaking. These correct for offsets of slice acquisition. EPI volumes were realigned to
actions are all components of the distinctive ‘platform preparation’ the first volume for each subject to correct for interscan movement,
operation discussed above, and their frequency directly reflects the and unwarped for movement-induced inhomogeneities of the magnetic
greater technological complexity of Late Acheulean toolmaking. This field using realignment parameters (Andersson et al., 2001). EPI
complexity includes increased contingency on detailed variation in volumes were stereotactically normalized into the standard space
hammerstone properties, grips and gestures, and in core morphology, defined by the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) using a two-step
orientation and support, as well as a greater hierarchical depth of procedure: the mean EPI image created during realignment was
action planning. coregistered with the structural image, which was spatially normalized
to the SPM T1 template using a 12-parameter affine and non-linear
cosine basis function transformation, both transformations being
fMRI paradigm subsequently applied to all EPI volumes. Normalized images were
Subjects lay supine in the 3T Siemens Allegra MRI scanner at the smoothed using an 8-mm isometric Gaussian kernel to account for
Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, pads positioned on the side residual inter-subject differences in functional anatomy (Friston et al.,
of the head to reduce movement. Subjects underwent six sessions of 2007).
approximately 7 min, and each session comprised 12 trials, corre-
sponding to one repetition of six experimental conditions defined by a
three · two factorial plan. fMRI statistical analysis
Analysis of the functional imaging data entailed the creation of
1. Stimulus: 20-s video clips of the Control stimulus, Oldowan or
statistical parametric maps representing a statistical assessment
Acheulean toolmaking.
of hypothesized condition-specific effects (Friston et al., 1994).
2. Task: following stimulus presentation, subjects were instructed
A random effect procedure was adopted for data analysis. Within
either to simulate themselves continuing to perform the action they
individual subjects, the 20-s stimulations were modelled for the three
saw (Imagine) or to decide whether, in their opinion, the actor was
types of stimuli (Control, Oldowan, Acheulean), the 5-s tasks were
successful in achieving his goal (Evaluate).
modelled for the three types of stimuli and two tasks (Imagine, Evaluate),
Prior to entering the scanner, subjects were instructed to watch each and the motor responses were modelled as events (duration 0) irrespective
video ‘carefully’, to ‘try to understand what the demonstrator is doing’ of the experimental condition. Rest was modelled as a 12-s condition.
and that after each video they would be ‘asked to do one of two Each condition was defined with a boxcar function convolved with SPM8
things’, which were then explained. In the scanner, each trial was canonical haemodynamic response function to estimate condition-
started by the presentation of the stimulus, followed by: (i) 1.5 s of a specific effects with the General Linear Model. Low-frequency drifts
fixation cross; (ii) a written instruction indicating the Task (‘Imagine’ were removed by a high-pass filtering with a cut-off of 128 s.
or ‘Evaluate’) that remained on screen for 5 s; and (iii) a response The current analysis focused on the response to Acheulean,
screen displaying the appropriate question (‘Did you finish?’ or ‘Was Oldowan and Control stimuli, with individual subjects’ statistical
he successful?’). The side for yes and no responses was randomly maps for the four contrasts [Toolmaking: (Acheulean–Control) +
assigned to the left and right button press and indicated by the position (Oldowan–Control); Acheulean–Control, Oldowan–Control, Acheu-
of the words ‘Yes’ and ‘No’ on screen. The response screen remained lean–Oldowan] entered in second-level analyses of variance, with the
visible for 1.5 s or until subjects replied, and was followed by a group (Naı̈ve n = 11, Trained n = 10, Expert n = 5) as between-
fixation screen (minimum 1 s) for a total trial duration of 29 s. In subjects factor. As all groups comprise neurotypical adults, we
addition, each session included four 12-s rest trials, each of which hypothesized equal variance between populations in order to control
started with a 1-s ‘Rest’ indication, and ended with a 1-s ‘End of rest’ for differences in group size (Penny & Holmes, 2003).

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
Technology and cognition in human evolution 1331

Common brain response irrespective of expertise was investigated Results


using a minimum statistic conjunction (Nichols et al., 2005) between Toolmaking–Control
the three groups. Brain response specific of each group was assessed
by masking exclusively the effect of this group by a global null Common response
conjunction (P < 0.05 uncorrected) of the other two groups; for Across all subject groups, the contrast of Toolmaking conditions with
instance, the contrast between Acheulean and Oldowan in Naı̈ve is Control yielded activations is a series of cortical regions, including a
exclusively masked by a conjunction of the same contrast in Trained large cluster extending from the primary visual and lateral occipital
and Expert subjects. Our procedure used exclusive masking instead of cortices to the inferior temporal cortices, intraparietal sulci, inferior
interactions, which were not significant at the threshold used, to favour parietal cortices and postcentral gyrii bilaterally. In the frontal cortex,
the effects within the group of interest over the reversed effects in the responses were found in right pars triangularis and bilateral pars
other groups, which are included in the statistics of interactions opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus, as well as in the dorsal
(Culham, 2006). All contrasts were thresholded at P < 0.05 FDR- precentral gyrii bilaterally (Fig. 1; Table 2).
corrected with an extent threshold of 20 voxels. Two-way (Stimulus, Group) analysis of variance of the extracted
Anatomical localization was performed using a brain atlas (Duver- percent signal change in the right pars triangularis revealed a main
noy, 1999) and, in particular for inferior frontal and parietal clusters, effect of Stimulus (P < 0.001), with no effect of Group (P = 0.9) or
functional localization made use of distribution analysis of the interaction between Stimulus and Group (P = 0.5; see Fig. 1, right).
activated voxels on the basis of probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps All pairwise comparisons between stimuli are significant (Oldowan vs.
(Eickhoff et al., 2007) implemented in SPM (Eickhoff et al., 2005). Control P = 0.001; Acheulean vs. Control P < 0.001; Acheulean vs.
For the sake of consistency, only anatomical labels are used in the Oldowan P = 0.016).
tables. Thus, clusters attributed to Brodmann area (BA) 44 were
labelled ‘pars opercularis’ (Amunts et al., 1999), those attributed to Effect of expertise
BA45 were labelled ‘pars triangularis’ (Amunts et al., 1999), and The exclusive masking procedure used to isolate brain responses to the
those attributed to BA6 were labelled ‘precentral gyrus’ (Geyer, 2003). observation of Toolmaking stimuli unique to each level of expertise
In the parietal cortex, clusters attributed to areas PF and PG (Caspers identified clusters (Fig. 2; Table 2) in the bilateral ventral precentral
et al., 2006) were labelled ‘inferior parietal lobule’, and those gyrus and left middle occipital gyrus in the Naı̈ve group, and in the left
attributed to hIP1 and hIP2 (Choi et al., 2006) were labelled ‘anterior superior parietal and right postcentral gyrus of Experts. Activations
intraparietal sulcus’. While recognizing that functional localization unique to the Trained group were much more numerous particularly in
and anatomical landmarks may not strictly overlap in individuals, the frontal cortices, including medial frontal cortex, the right pars
these conventions were adopted in the interest of coherence in the orbitalis, left pars triangularis, bilateral pars opercularis, right
presentation of results. Statistical maps were rendered on FreeSurfer’s anterior insula, left posterior middle frontal gyrus and left precentral
fsaverage pial surface with 50 inflation steps (http://surfer.nmr. gyrus, as well as left middle temporal gyrus and right inferior temporal
mgh.harvard.edu). gyrus.

Local activity Acheulean vs. Oldowan


In order to assess the effect of Group, local activity in clusters of Common response
interest was further characterized using the SPM extension toolbox The minimum statistic conjunction between the three groups for the
MarsBar (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/) to extract percentage contrast Acheulean–Oldowan identified increases in activity in the
signal change in 5-mm radius volumes centred on the maximum of anterior part of the left intraparietal sulcus (Fig. 3; Table 3), and in
each cluster, then analysed with spss. the left prefrontal cortex within the inferior frontal sulcus.

Fig. 1. Left: local brain activity in Toolmaking–Control irrespective of subject expertise (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20). Rendered with freesurfer on top, lateral and
ventral views of the two hemispheres in neurological convention (details in Methods and Table 1). Right: percent signal change across the three technologies in the
right pars triangularis (***P < 0.001; *P < 0.05).

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
1332 D. Stout et al.

Table 2. Brain activity in response of the observation of Toolmaking


compared with Control stimuli, common to the three groups (minimum
statistic conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive masking)

Location x y z Z-score Extent

Conjunction
Frontal lobe
Left Dorsal precentral gyrus )32 )8 58 4.39 421
Right Dorsal precentral gyrus 30 )8 54 4.66 1003
Right Pars opercularis 60 10 26 5.68 747
Left Pars opercularis )54 8 22 4.89 612
Right Pars triangularis 50 44 10 3.23 53
Right Insula 40 )4 8 5.40 328
Left Insula )42 )4 2 4.04 Fig. 2. Local brain activity in Toolmaking–Control for Naı̈ve (left), Trained
Parietal lobe (centre) and Expert (right) subjects (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20).
Left Intraparietal sulcus )26 )52 62 5.25 –
Left Postcentral gyrus )30 )44 62 5.91 19 098 the pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus (Fig. 4; Table 3). The
Right Intraparietal sulcus 32 )60 56 5.39 –
Right Postcentral gyrus 36 )40 56 5.06 –
latter activation was in a similar location to that previously reported for
Right Supramarginal gyrus 66 )20 36 5.82 – the actual performance (as opposed to observation) of stone toolmaking
Left Supramarginal gyrus )64 )20 36 5.46 – (Stout & Chaminade, 2007). No cluster survived the thresholds used in
Temporal lobe this analysis for Trained subjects.
Left Inferior temporal gyrus )48 )64 )10 5.40 – In Experts (Fig. 4; Table 3), there were clusters in the right medial
Left Fusiform gyrus )48 )50 )20 5.57 – frontal and parietal cortices. The latter were localized in the inferior
Right Fusiform gyrus 34 )46 )28 5.72 – parietal lobule, and in the anterior intraparietal sulcus area hIP1 (Choi
Occipital lobe et al., 2006; see also Jubault et al., 2007).
Right Middle occipital gyrus 44 )84 12 5.23 –
Left Calcarine fissure )8 )84 4 3.40 209
Right Middle occipital gyrus 40 )78 )4 5.12 –
Discussion
Basal ganglia
Right Thalamus 14 )26 8 4.89 947 To identify brain systems involved in the observation of Paleolithic
Left Thalamus )16 )32 )4 3.32 114 toolmaking, we examined contrasts of toolmaking observation with a
Naı̈ve only control condition. Results were remarkably similar to those obtained
Left Precentral gyrus )58 )2 30 3.81 34 from previous FDG-PET studies of toolmaking execution, despite the
Right Precentral gyrus 64 0 28 3.80 29 different experimental tasks and imaging modalities used. This
Left Middle occipital gyrus )22 )88 14 4.76 31
indicates that neural systems involved in the observational under-
Trained only standing of Paleolithic toolmaking are very similar to those involved
Left Precentral gyrus )18 )10 64 4.44 242 in execution.
Left Middle frontal gyrus )24 4 54 4.19 –
Left Medial frontal cortex )12 4 48 3.95 – To investigate the effects of expertise on toolmaking observation,
Left Precentral gyrus )38 )6 42 4.04 – we examined the unique responses of each subject group (Naı̈ve,
Right Intraparietal sulcus 44 )48 40 3.83 31 Trained and Expert) to Toolmaking stimuli. This provided evidence
Left Pars opercularis )42 8 30 4.19 369 for the functional ‘reorganization’ (Kelly & Garavan, 2005) of
Right Pars opercularis 60 20 12 3.78 –
Left Pars triangularis )54 40 8 4.81 287
activation between groups, reflecting expertise-dependent shifts in
Right Anterior insula 34 20 2 3.94 165 cognitive strategy.
Right Pars orbitalis 48 48 )4 3.77 107 To investigate the specific demands of understanding increasingly
Left Middle temporal gyrus )60 )62 )6 4.04 36 complex Paleolithic technologies, we examined the contrast in brain
Right Inferior temporal gyrus 62 )56 )10 4.24 38 response to Acheulean vs. Oldowan stimuli in Naı̈ve, Trained and
Experts only Expert subjects. This revealed a significant main effect of technological
Left Superior parietal lobule )10 )52 74 4.46 159 complexity across groups, as well as distinct responses in the Naı̈ve and
Right Postcentral gyrus 48 )30 62 3.87 25
Expert groups. The localization of these expertise-dependent effects
All results are FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20. All coordinates MNI. suggests that stone toolmaking action understanding depends on a
complex mixture of top-down, bottom-up, conceptual and embodied
In agreement with SPM whole-brain investigation, analysis of processes (cf. Grafton, 2009).
variance of activity extracted in these clusters indicated a main effect
of the stimulus (both P < 0.001), while there was no effect of Group
or interaction between Group and Stimulus (all P > 0.3) in these Contrasts of Toolmaking with Control condition
regions. Activity in Acheulean was significantly increased compared Common response
with Oldowan (P < 0.001) and Control (P < 0.05) for the left Contrasts of toolmaking stimuli with Control yielded activations in a
prefrontal cortex cluster, and all pairwise comparisons were significant series of cortical regions, notably including inferior frontal gyrus,
(P < 0.001) for the anterior intraparietal sulcus. dorsal premotor cortex, intraparietal sulcus and the inferior parietal
lobule (Fig. 1; Table 1). Activations in these regions have com-
Effect of expertise monly been reported in imaging studies of action observation
In Naı̈ve subjects, there were activations for Acheulean–Oldowan in the (Grezes & Decety, 2001; Grafton, 2009; Caspers et al., 2010),
left frontal cortex, dorsally in the superior frontal gyrus and ventrally in and they are thought to comprise a network supporting action

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
Technology and cognition in human evolution 1333

Fig. 3. Left: local brain activity in Acheulean–Oldowan irrespective of subject expertise (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20). Right: percent signal change across the
three technologies in the left anterior intraparietal sulcus (top) and inferior frontal sulcus (bottom; ***P < 0.001; *P < 0.05).

Table 3. Brain activity in response of the observation of Acheulean compared with Oldowan toolmaking stimuli, common to the three groups (minimum statistic
conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive masking)

Location x y z Z-score Extent

Conjunction
Left Anterior intraparietal sulcus )50 )36 42 4.74 53
Left Pars triangularis )46 32 26 6.02 94
Naı̈ve only
Left Superior frontal gyrus )24 22 56 4.21 51
Left Pars opercularis )60 12 24 4.46 83
Experts only
Right Medial frontal cortex 6 42 50 5.25 24
Right Anterior intraparietal sulcus 46 )48 46 5.65 44
Right Inferior parietal lobule 62 )52 36 4.80 22

All results are FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20. All coordinates MNI.

understanding through the covert simulation of observed behaviours.


In keeping with this, the observed activations closely match (see
also Supporting Information Fig. S2; Tables S1 and S2) those
reported in previous FDG-PET studies, in which subjects actively
produced tools rather than simply observing toolmaking (Stout &
Chaminade, 2007; Stout et al., 2008).
Particularly notable is activation of the pars triangularis of the right
inferior frontal gyrus. Pars triangularis activation is more typically
associated with linguistic processing (e.g. Bookheimer, 2002; Musso
et al., 2003), but has been reported during action observation (Johnson-
Frey et al., 2003; Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2005; Caspers et al., 2010). It
has been proposed (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004) that such activation
reflects the ‘syntactic’ processing of hierarchically organized actions
(cf. Koechlin & Jubault, 2006). This leads to the expectation that pars
triangularis activity should respond to variation in the complexity of
observed actions (Caspers et al., 2010). Such an effect of stimulus
complexity is observed here (Fig. 1), in keeping with previous findings
Fig. 4. Local brain activity in Acheulean–Oldowan for Naı̈ve (left) and Expert of pars triangularis activation during the execution of Acheulean, but
(right) subjects (FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20). not Oldowan, toolmaking (Stout et al., 2008; Table 2).

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
1334 D. Stout et al.

Effect of stone toolmaking method representing episode-specific intentions, causal relations and multi-
component action sequences during toolmaking observation. The
Across groups, the increased technological complexity of Acheulean apparent abstraction (Hamilton & Grafton, 2006; Badre & D’Esposito,
stimuli compared with Oldowan (Table 1) was associated with 2009) of causal ⁄ intentional processing in this circuit may be compared
activation of the anterior intraparietal sulcus and inferior frontal with a proposed ‘intermediate’ level representing ‘intentions in action’
sulcus, both in the left hemisphere (Fig. 3; Table 3). The anterior as goal-oriented sequences of motor commands and predicted
intraparietal sulcus is a core component of the putative human mirror outcomes (de Vignemont & Haggard, 2008).
neuron system (Grafton & Hamilton, 2007). It is thought to contribute
to the understanding of ‘immediate’ action goals, such as grasping to Expertise effects on response to stimuli
eat vs. to place in macaque monkeys (Fogassi et al., 2005), or taking a Varying expertise across subject groups was associated with qualitative
cookie vs. a diskette in humans (Hamilton & Grafton, 2006). shifts in the set of brain regions activated in response to Acheulean
In monkeys, the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus changes its compared with Oldowan stimuli (Fig. 4; Table 3). These differences
connectivity and response patterns when the animals train to use tools suggest a functional reorganization (Kelly & Garavan, 2005) involving
(Hihara et al., 2006), enabling an integration of visual and somato- the adoption of different cognitive strategies for action understanding.
sensory stimuli. This is argued to support tool use through assimilation Naı̈ve subjects show activation in core motor resonance structures
of the tool into the monkey’s body schema (Maravita & Iriki, 2004), together with the ventral prefrontal cortex, as expected for a low-level
such that ‘tools become hands’ (Umiltà et al., 2008). However, human strategy of novel action understanding through kinematic simulation.
left anterior inferior parietal lobule displays a specific response to Trained subjects show strong, statistically indistinguishable responses
observed tool use (as opposed to unassisted manual prehension) that is to both Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli, perhaps reflecting the
absent in monkeys (Peeters et al., 2009). This suggests that hominoid particular social context and motivational set associated with training.
anterior inferior parietal cortex may be evolutionarily derived to play a Finally, Expert subjects display activation in the medial prefrontal
new role in coding the distinct functional properties of hand-held tools cortex, a classic ‘mentalizing’ region, suggesting a relatively high-
(Johnson-Frey et al., 2005; Peeters et al., 2009; Jacobs et al., 2010; level, inferential strategy of intention reading.
Povinelli et al., 2010).
The centre of anterior inferior parietal cortex activation reported Naı̈ve subjects
here is somewhat posterior ()50, )36, 42 vs. )52, )26, 34) to that of
Peeters et al. (2009); however, extraction of the volume of interest One cluster exclusive to technologically Naı̈ve subjects occurred in the
used by Peeters et al. (coordinates from Orban, pers. comm.) confirms pars opercularis of the left posterior inferior frontal gyrus (Fig. 4,
that the same effect of stimulus is indeed present in this region. This left). Pars opercularis is another core component of the putative
response to increasingly complex Paleolithic toolmaking is consistent human mirror neuronal system (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), which,
with the hypothesis that human technological evolution was sup- in contrast with the performance-monitoring functions of the anterior
ported, at least in part, by the emergence of enhanced neural inferior parietal cortex described above, is thought to be responsible
mechanisms for representing the causal properties of hand-held tools for the generation of the kinematic models used to execute (Fagg &
(Johnson-Frey, 2003; Wolpert, 2003; Peeters et al., 2009). Arbib, 1998) or simulate (Carr et al., 2003; Grafton & Hamilton,
The main effect in the prefrontal cortex was centred on the inferior 2007; Kilner et al., 2007) motor acts.
frontal sulcus. In macaques, this region is heavily interconnected with Also unique to Naı̈ve subjects was activation of the superior frontal
the anterior inferior parietal lobule (Pandya & Seltzer, 1982) and the gyrus, anterior to the frontal eye field (Lobel et al., 2001). Activation
parietal operculum (Preuss & Goldman-Rakic, 1989), in keeping with of this area is associated with the selection among competing
the co-activation observed here, and suggesting involvement in the responses (Petrides, 2005), and the more superior portion activated
integration of visuospatial and somatosensory information. In an fMRI here is especially involved in the spatial domain (Volle et al., 2008).
study with macaques, there was activation in this area during the During imitation, this region may serve to maintain a representation of
observation of actions (Nelissen et al., 2005). In contrast to more the the observed goal in short-term working memory for later execution
posterior premotor cortex (F5c) where mirror neurons were originally (Chaminade et al., 2002). Co-activation of the superior frontal gyrus
recorded, the ventral prefrontal cortex also responded to abstract or and posterior inferior frontal gyrus may thus reflect Naı̈ve reliance on
context-free stimuli, including isolated hands, robotic hands and kinematic simulation and top-down direction of attention to task-
shapes (Nelissen et al., 2005), indicating representation and integra- relevant spatial cues. When combined with the anterior inferior
tion of actions at a relatively high level. In humans, activation of parietal and ventral prefrontal activations observed across all groups,
similar coordinates is reported during observation and preparation to these Naı̈ve activations match the general expectations of a simulation
imitate complex hand postures (guitar chords), perhaps indicating a model of novel action understanding (Buccino et al., 2004; Vogt
role for this region in the selection and combination of motor elements et al., 2007).
into novel actions (Vogt et al., 2007).
It is thus likely that the increase in prefrontal activation for Trained subjects
Acheulean–Oldowan reflects the greater temporal and relational No activations exclusive to Trained subjects were observed in the
complexity of Acheulean toolmaking actions, which, to a greater Acheulean–Oldowan contrast. Comparison with the numerous activa-
extent than Oldowan flaking, are organized into flexible and internally tions observed in the contrast of Toolmaking–Control for Trained
variable action chunks, such as ‘platform preparation’ vs. ‘primary flake subjects (Table 2; Fig. 2) indicates that this result derives from the
removal’ (Pelegrin, 2005; Stout, 2011). No significant prefrontal presence of similar responses to Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli
activation was observed for Oldowan–Control, in keeping with rather than from the absence of significant differences from Control.
previous conclusions regarding the relative simplicity of Oldowan This is corroborated by the observation of similar activations in
action sequences (Stout & Chaminade, 2007; Stout et al., 2008). separate contrasts of Oldowan–Control and Acheulean–Control
On this interpretation, the anterior inferior parietal cortex and the (Supporting Information Figs S3 and S4; Tables S1 and S2). The
inferior frontal sulcus form a parieto-frontal circuit involved in Trained response to both Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli includes:

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
Technology and cognition in human evolution 1335

(i) clusters in the anterior insula, lateral premotor cortex, frontal Broader implications
eye field and supplementary eye field likely related to attentional
Contrasts with Control show that the observation of Paleolithic
and affective engagement with the stimuli; and (ii) ventral
toolmaking recruits cognitive control mechanisms in the pars
prefrontal clusters likely associated with parsing of observed action
triangularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus, and that this response
sequences.
increases with the technological complexity of the observed actions.
Insular activations unique to Trained subjects are in an anterior
This matches results from earlier studies of subjects actively making
region associated with interoception, subjective feeling and perceptual
stone tools, and is consistent with an evolutionary scenario in which
awareness (Kikyo et al., 2002; Ploran et al., 2007; Craig, 2009).
manual and perceptual-motor adaptations were critical to the earliest
Activations of the left medial frontal cortex (close to y = 0) and
stages of human technological evolution (Wynn & McGrew, 1989;
posterior middle frontal gyrus appear to fall within the supplementary
Ambrose, 2001; Byrne, 2004; Bril & Roux, 2005; Stout & Chamin-
and frontal eye fields (Tehovnik et al., 2000), functional regions
ade, 2007), but later developments were more dependent on enhanced
associated with saccades, visual attention and visual learning
cognitive control (Faisal et al., 2010; Stout, 2010). These findings
(Tehovnik et al., 2000; Grosbras et al., 2005). Together with activa-
support long-standing intuitions regarding the cognitive sophistication
tion of the precentral gyrus, a region commonly recruited during
of Acheulean technology (e.g. Oakley, 1954; Wynn, 1979; Gowlett,
action observation (Grezes & Decety, 2001; Caspers et al., 2010),
1986), and specifically highlight the complex hierarchical organization
these activations likely indicate intense engagement by Trained
(Holloway, 1969; Stout et al., 2008) of Acheulean action sequences.
subjects with the Toolmaking stimuli. These effects of training were
This interpretation is further supported by the main effect of stimulus
not predicted, but are consistent with the pragmatic social and
in the anterior inferior parietal and ventral prefrontal cortices across
motivational context created by the training programme.
subject groups.
Also unique to Trained subjects were inferior frontal gyrus
Differing responses to stimulus complexity between groups provide
activations of bilateral pars opercularis, left pars triangularis and
insight into the effects of expertise on action observation strategies.
right pars orbitalis. These are probably best understood in terms of the
Activations specific to Naı̈ve subjects suggest a strategy reliant on
putative role of the inferior frontal gyrus in the multi-level processing
kinematic simulation (inferior frontal gyrus) and the top-down
of stimuli along a posterior to anterior gradient of increasing
direction of visuospatial attention (superior frontal gyrus). This
hierarchical complexity (Koechlin & Jubault, 2006; Caspers et al.,
supports an account of early observational learning in which
2010), and may reflect the intense processing of all Toolmaking
simulation of low-level action elements interacts with representations
stimuli by highly motivated Trained subjects.
of mid-level intentions in action to produce a ‘best-fit’ understanding
of complex, unfamiliar actions (cf. Vogt et al., 2007).
Expert subjects Interestingly, Trained subjects responded equally to Oldowan and
Activations exclusive to Expert subjects were observed in the medial Acheulean stimuli, activating a set of frontal regions related to
frontal cortex, anterior intraparietal sulcus and inferior parietal lobule of subjective awareness, visual attention and multi-level action parsing.
the right hemisphere (Fig. 4, right). The medial frontal cortex is a core This unexpected result may reflect a strong motivation to attend to,
element in the network of brain regions associated with the attribution of analyse and understand all Toolmaking stimuli, generated by the social
mental states (Frith & Frith, 2006), suggesting that Expert subjects rely and pragmatic context of being a ‘learner’ (cf. Lave & Wenger, 1991;
on top-down interpretation of the demonstrator’s intentions in order to Stout, 2002). There is increasing awareness of the importance of such
differentiate Acheulean from Oldowan toolmaking. The activation is social and affective dimensions in understanding human cognitive
centred at the border between a posterior region associated with the evolution (Holloway, 1967; Hare & Tomasello, 2005; Burkart et al.,
attribution of ‘private’ action intentions and an anterior region 2009; Stout, 2010).
associated with communicative intentions (Grèzes et al., 2004a,b; Unlike Naive and Trained subjects, Experts recruited a mixture of
Amodio & Frith, 2006), in a position closely approximating that bottom-up, familiarity-based posterior parietal mechanisms for visuo-
activated when mentalizing about the internal states of a dissimilar other spatial attention (right inferior parietal lobule) and sensorimotor
(Mitchell et al., 2006). It may reflect inference about the private matching (anterior intraparietal sulcus) with high-level inference
technological ‘prior intentions’ of the demonstrator (Chaminade et al., regarding technological ‘prior intentions’ in the medial frontal cortex.
2002), rather than meta-cognition about the demonstrator’s communi- In this context, shared pragmatic skills may provide the foundation for
cative intentions toward the observer (Amodio & Frith, 2006: 274). sharing of higher level intentions, in keeping with the Motor
Activation of the right anterior intraparietal sulcus in Experts is Cognition Hypothesis (Gallese et al., 2009). More broadly, the
comparable to expertise effects found in studies of dance observation apparent shift in observation strategy from Naive kinematic simulation
(Calvo-Merino et al., 2005, 2006; Cross et al., 2006). The more anterior to Expert mentalizing is consistent with a ‘mixed’ model of action
location the current activation may reflect somatotopy of response to the understanding (Grafton, 2009) involving contextually variable inter-
observation of upper vs. lower limb actions (Buccino et al., 2001). This actions between bottom-up resonance and top-down interpretation.
particular region of right anterior intraparietal sulcus has also been Complex, pragmatic skills like stone toolmaking can only be
linked with the preparation of successive sensorimotor task-sets during acquired through deliberate practice (Pelegrin, 1990; Whittaker, 1994)
action sequence execution (Jubault et al., 2007). and experimentation (Ericsson et al., 1993), leading to the discovery of
Also activated in Experts was a region of right inferior parietal subtle causal relations that would remain ‘opaque’ (Gergely & Csibra,
lobule known to support the stimulus-driven allocation of spatial 2006) to observation and simulation alone. Mid-level ‘intentions in
attention (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Mort et al., 2003) during action’ represented in the anterior inferior parietal and the ventral
visuospatial sequence learning (Rosenthal et al., 2009). This activa- prefrontal cortices, though likely to be inaccurate at first, appear to be
tion is posterior to the region associated with action outcome important across skill levels and may play an important role in guiding
monitoring by Hamilton & Grafton (2008), and together with the such practice, perhaps contributing to the high fidelity of human social
right anterior intraparietal sulcus activation probably reflects Expert learning (the ‘ratchet effect’: Tomasello, 1999; Tennie et al., 2009). The
recognition of familiar toolmaking action sequences. effect of Toolmaking complexity in the anterior inferior parietal lobule

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
1336 D. Stout et al.

in particular suggests that this phylogenetically derived (Peeters et al., Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., Burgel, U., Mohlberg, H., Uylings, H.B. & Zilles,
2009) region may have played a key role in human technological K. (1999) Broca’s region revisited: cytoarchitecture and intersubject
variability. J. Comp. Neurol., 412, 319–341.
evolution 2.6–0.5 million years ago. Andersson, J.L., Hutton, C., Ashburner, J., Turner, R. & Friston, K. (2001)
Modeling geometric deformations in EPI time series. Neuroimage, 13, 903–
919.
Supporting Information Badre, D. & D’Esposito, M. (2009) Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe
Additional supporting information may be found in the online version hierarchical? Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 10, 659–669.
Beck, B.B. (1980) Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools
of this article: by Animals. Garland STPM Press, New York.
Fig. S1. Handaxes produced (a–c) by Trained subjects, (d) by the Blakemore, S.-J. & Decety, J. (2001) From the perception of action to the
expert demonstrator, and (e) from the Middle Pleistocene (approxi- understanding of intention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 2, 561–567.
mately 500 000 years ago) site of Boxgrove, West Sussex, UK. Bookheimer, S. (2002) Functional MRI of language: new approaches to
understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu. Rev.
Fig. S2. Local brain activity in Oldowan–Control (left) and Acheu- Neurosci., 25, 151–188.
lean–Control (right) irrespective of subject expertise (FDR P < 0.05, Bril, B. & Roux, V. (2005) Synthesis and speculations. In Roux, V. & Bril, B.
extent k > 20). To more directly compare current results with previous (Eds), Stone Knapping: The Necessary Conditions for a Uniquely Hominin
FDG-PET studies of Oldowan and Acheulean toolmaking execution, Behaviour.McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp.
we examined separate contrasts of Oldowan and Acheulean toolmak- 353–355.
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G.R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V.,
ing with the Control. This yielded activations of left ventral premotor Seitz, R.J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G. & Freund, H.-J. (2001) Action
cortex in both contrasts (Oldowan: )56, 8, 22; Acheulean: )58, 10, observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner:
32), and of right pars triangularis in the Acheulean (46, 36, 4) but not an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci., 13, 400–404.
Oldowan contrast. This directly matches results from the execution of Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G.R., Zilles, K., Freund, H.-J. &
Rizzolatti, G. (2004) Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand
Oldowan (ventral premotor cortex: )52, 6, 28) and Acheulean (ventral actions: an Event-Related fMRI Study. Neuron, 42, 323–334.
premotor cortex: )52, 6, 28; pars triangularis: 48, 34, 10) toolmaking Burkart, J.M., Hrdy, S.B. & Schaik, C.P.V. (2009) Cooperative breeding and
(Stout et al., 2008; Table 2). human cognitive evolution. Evol. Anthropol., 18, 175–186.
Fig. S3. Local brain activity in Oldowan–Control for Naı̈ve (left), Byrne, R. (2004) The manual skills and cognition that lie behind hominid tool
Trained (centre) and Expert (right) subjects (FDR P < 0.05, extent use. In Russon, A.E.B. & David, R. (Ed), The Evolution of Thought:
Evolutionary Origins Of Great Ape Intelligence. Cambridge University
k > 20). Press, Cambridge, pp. 31–44.
Fig. S4. Local brain activity in Acheulean–Control for Naı̈ve (left), Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D.E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R.E. & Haggard, P.
Trained (centre) and Experts (right) subjects (FDR P < 0.05, extent (2005) Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI Study with
k > 20). expert dancers. Cereb. Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.
Calvo-Merino, B., Grezes, J., Glaser, D.E., Passingham, R.E. & Haggard, P.
Table S1. Brain activity in response of the observation of Oldowan (2006) Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action
compared with Control stimuli, common to the three groups observation Curr. Biol., 16, 1905–1910.
(minimum statistic conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J.C. & Lenzi, G.L. (2003)
masking). All results are FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for
Table S2. Brain activity in response of the observation of Acheulean imitation to limbic areas. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 100, 5497–5502.
Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K. & Zilles, K.
compared with Control stimuli, common to the three groups (2006) The human inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonic parcellation and
(minimum statistic conjunction) and by subject expertise (exclusive interindividual variability. Neuroimage, 33, 430–448.
masking). All results are FDR P < 0.05, extent k > 20. Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A.R. & Eickhoff, S.B. (2010) ALE meta-analysis
Video S1. Examples of Control, Oldowan and Acheulean stimuli used of action observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage, 50,
1148–1167.
in the experiment. Chaminade, T., Meltzoff, A.N. & Decety, J. (2002) Does the end justify the
Please note: As a service to our authors and readers, this journal means? A PET exploration of the mechanisms involved in human imitation
provides supporting information supplied by the authors. Such NeuroImage, 15, 318–328.
materials are peer-reviewed and may be re-organized for online Choi, H.-J., Zilles, K., Mohlberg, H., Schleicher, A., Fink, G.R., Armstrong, E.
delivery, but are not copy-edited or typeset by Wiley-Blackwell. & Amunts, K. (2006) Cytoarchitectonic identification and probabilistic
mapping of two distinct areas within the anterior ventral bank of the human
Technical support issues arising from supporting information (other intraparietal sulcus. J. Comp. Neurol., 495, 53–69.
than missing files) should be addressed to the authors. Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G.L. (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-
driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 3, 201–215.
Craig, A.D. (2009) How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human
Acknowledgements awareness Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 10, 59–70.
Cross, E.S., Hamilton, A.F.d.C. & Grafton, S.T. (2006) Building a motor
This research was funded by European Union project HANDTOMOUTH. We simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. NeuroImage, 31,
thank Bruce Bradley for acting as the expert demonstrator, and Stefan Vogt and 1257–1267.
an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments. Culham, J.C. (2006) Functional neuroimaging: experimental design and
analysis. In Cabeza, R. & Kingstone, A. (Eds), Handbook of Functional
Neuroimaging of Cognition, 2nd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 53–
Abbreviations 82.
BA, Brodmann area; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; PET, Duvernoy, H.M. (1999) The Human Brain: Surface, Blood Supply, and Three-
positron emission tomography. dimensional Anatomy, 2nd edn, completely revised. Springer-Verlag, Wien
New York.
Eickhoff, S.B., Stephan, K.E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G.R., Amunts,
K. & Zilles, K. (2005) A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic
References cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage, 25, 1325–
Ambrose, S. (2001) Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science, 291, 1335.
1748–1753. Eickhoff, S.B., Paus, T., Caspers, S., Grosbras, M.-H., Evans, A.C., Zilles, K.
Amodio, D.M. & Frith, C.D. (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal & Amunts, K. (2007) Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic
cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 7, 268–277. cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. NeuroImage, 36, 511–521.

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
Technology and cognition in human evolution 1337

Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T. & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993) The role of deliberate Jubault, T., Ody, C. & Koechlin, E. (2007) Serial organization of human
practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol. Rev., 100, 363–406. behavior in the inferior parietal cortex. J. Neurosci., 27, 11028–11036.
Fagg, A. & Arbib, M.A. (1998) Modeling parietal-premotor interaction in Kelly, A.M. & Garavan, H. (2005) Human functional neuroimaging of brain
primate control of grasping. Neural Netw., 11, 1277–1303. changes associated with practice. Cereb. Cortex, 15, 1089–1102.
Faisal, A., Stout, D., Apel, J. & Bradley, B. (2010) The Kikyo, H., Ohki, K. & Miyashita, Y. (2002) Neural correlates for feeling-of-
Manipulative complexity of lower paleolithic stone Toolmaking. PLoS knowing: an fMRI parametric analysis. Neuron, 36, 177–186.
ONE, 5, e13718. Kilner, J., Friston, K. & Frith, C. (2007) Predictive coding: an account of the
Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P.F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F. & Rizzolatti, G. mirror neuron system. Cogn. Process., 8, 159–166.
(2005) Parietal Lobe: from Action Organization to intention understanding. Koechlin, E. & Jubault, T. (2006) Broca’s area and the hierarchical organization
Science, 308, 662–667. of human behavior. Neuron, 50, 963–974.
Friston, K.J., Holmes, A.P., Worsley, K.J., Poline, J.P., Frith, C.D. & Lave, J. & Wenger, E. (1991) Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral
Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1994) Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: Participation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp., 2, 189–210. Lobel, E., Kahane, P., Leonards, U., Grosbras, M.-H., Lehéricy, S., Bihan,
Friston, K.J., Ashburner, J.T., Kiebel, S., Nichols, T.E. & Penny, W.D. (Eds) D.L. & Berthoz, A. (2001) Localization of human frontal eye fields:
(2007) Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain anatomical and functional findings of functional magnetic resonance
Images. Elsevier, London, UK. imaging and intracerebral electrical stimulation. J. Neurosurg., 95, 804–
Frith, C.D. & Frith, U. (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50, 815.
531–534. Maravita, A. & Iriki, A. (2004) Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn. Sci.,
Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of 8, 79–86.
mind-reading. Trends Cogn. Sci., 2, 493–501. Mitchell, J.P., Macrae, C.N. & Banaji, M.R. (2006) Dissociable medial
Gallese, V., Rochat, M., Cossu, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2009) Motor cognition and prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others.
its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. Dev. Neuron, 50, 655–663.
Psychol., 45, 103–113. Molnar-Szakacs, I., Iacoboni, M., Koski, L. & Mazziotta, J.C. (2005)
Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. (2006) Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and Functional segregation within pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus:
pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In Enfiled, N.J. & evidence from fMRI studies of imitation and action observation. Cereb.
Levenson, S.C. (Eds), Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Cortex, 15, 986–994.
Human Interaction. Berg Publishers, Oxford, pp. 229–255. Mort, D.J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S.K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard,
Geyer, S. (2003) The microstructural border between the motor and the C. & Husain, M. (2003) The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain, 126, 1986–
cognitive domain in the human cerebral cortex. In Advances in Anatomy, 1997.
Embryology and Cell Biology, vol. 174. Springer, Berlin, 92 pp. Musso, M., Moro, A., Glauche, V., Rijinties, M., Reichenback, J., Buchel, C. &
Gowlett, J.A.J. (1986) Culture and conceptualisation: the Oldowan-Acheulian Weiller, C. (2003) Broca’s area and the language instinct. Nat. Neurosci., 6,
gradient. In Bailey, G.N. & Callow, P. (Eds), Stone Age Prehistory: Studies 774–781.
in Memory of Charles McBurney. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, Nelissen, K., Luppino, G., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G. & Orban, G.A. (2005)
pp. 243–260. Observing others: multiple action representation in the frontal lobe. Science,
Grafton, S.T. (2009) Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to 310, 332–336.
understand others. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci., 1156, 97–117. Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T. & Poline, J.B. (2005) Valid
Grafton, S.T. & Hamilton, A.F.d.C. (2007) Evidence for a distributed hierarchy conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage, 25, 653–660.
of action representation in the brain. Hum. Mov. Sci., 26, 590–616. Oakley, K.P. (1954) Skill as a human possession. In Singer, C., Holmyard, E.J.
Grezes, J. & Decety, J. (2001) Functional anatomy of execution, mental & Hall, A.R. (Eds), A History of Technology. Volume I, From Early Times to
simulation, observation, and verb generation of action: a meta-analysis. Fall of Ancient Empires. Clarendon press, Oxford, pp. 1–37.
Hum. Brain Mapp., 12, 1–19. Pandya, D.N. & Seltzer, B. (1982) Intrinsic connections and architectonics of
Grèzes, J., Frith, C. & Passingham, R.E. (2004a) Brain mechanisms for posterior parietal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol., 204, 196–
inferring Deceit in the actions of others. J. Neurosci., 24, 5500–5505. 210.
Grèzes, J., Frith, C. & Passingham, R.E. (2004b) Inferring false beliefs from the Passingham, R.E. (2008) What is Special about the Human Brain? Oxford
actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 21, 744–750. University Press, Oxford; New York.
Grosbras, M.-H., Laird, A.R. & Paus, T. (2005) Cortical regions involved in Peeters, R., Simone, L., Nelissen, K., Fabbri-Destro, M., Vanduffel, W.,
eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Hum. Brain Mapp., Rizzolatti, G. & Orban, G.A. (2009) The representation of tool use in
25, 140–154. humans and monkeys: common and uniquely human features. J. Neurosci.,
Hamilton, A.F.d.C. & Grafton, S.T. (2006) Goal representation in human 29, 11523–11539.
anterior intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci., 26, 1133–1137. Pelegrin, J. (1990) Prehistoric lithic technology: some aspects of research.
Hamilton, A.F.d.C. & Grafton, S.T. (2008) Action outcomes are represented in Archaeol. Rev. Cambridge, 9, 116–125.
human inferior frontoparietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex, 18, 1160–1168. Pelegrin, J. (2005) Remarks about archaeological techniques and methods of
Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2005) Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends knapping: elements of a cognitive approach to stone knapping. In Roux, V. &
Cogn. Sci., 9, 439–444. Bril, B. (eds), Stone Knapping: The Necessary Conditions for a Uniquely
Hihara, S., Notoya, T., Tanaka, M., Ichinose, S., Ojima, H., Obayashi, S., Fujii, Human Behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cam-
N. & Iriki, A. (2006) Extension of corticocortical afferents into the anterior bridge, pp. 23–34.
bank of the intraparietal sulcus by tool-use training in adult monkeys. Penny, W. & Holmes, A. (2003) Chapter 12, Random effect analysis. In
Neuropsychologia, 44, 2636–2646. Ashburner, J., Friston, K. & Penny, W. (Eds), Human Brain Function, 2nd
Holloway, R.L. (1967) The evolution of the human brain: some notes toward a edn, Academic Press, London, pp. 843–850.
synthesis between neural structure and the evolution of complex behavior. Petrides, M. (2005) Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional
Gen. Syst., 12, 3–19. organization. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 360, 781–795.
Holloway, R. (1969) Culture: a human domain. Curr. Anthropol., 10, 395–412. Ploran, E.J., Nelson, S.M., Velanova, K., Donaldson, D.I., Petersen, S.E. &
Jacob, P. & Jeannerod, M. (2005) The motor theory of social cognition: a Wheeler, M.E. (2007) Evidence accumulation and the moment of recogni-
critique. Trends Cogn. Sci., 9, 21–25. tion: dissociating perceptual recognition processes using fMRI. J. Neurosci.,
Jacobs, S., Danielmeier, C. & Frey, S.H. (2010) Human anterior intraparietal 27, 11912–11924.
and ventral premotor cortices support representations of grasping with the Povinelli, D.J., Reaux, J.E. & Frey, S.H. (2010) Chimpanzees’ context-
Hand or a Novel tool. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 22, 2594–2608. dependent tool use provides evidence for separable representations of hand
Johnson-Frey, S.H. (2003) What’s so special about human tool use? Neuron, and tool even during active use within peripersonal space. Neuropsycholo-
39, 201–204. gia, 48, 243–247.
Johnson-Frey, S.H., Maloof, F.R., Newman-Norlund, R., Farrer, C., Inati, S. & Preuss, T.M. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1989) Connections of the ventral
Grafton, S.T. (2003) Actions or hand–object interactions? human inferior granular frontal cortex of macaques with perisylvian premotor and somato-
frontal cortex and action observation Neuron, 39, 1053–1058. sensory areas: anatomical evidence for somatic representation in primate
Johnson-Frey, S.H., Newman-Norlund, R. & Grafton, S.T. (2005) A distributed frontal association cortex. J. Comp. Neurol., 282, 293–316.
left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev.
Cereb. Cortex, 15, 681–695. Neurosci., 27, 169–192.

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338
1338 D. Stout et al.

Rosenthal, C.R., Roche-Kelly, E.E., Husain, M. & Kennard, C. (2009) Tomasello, M. (1999) The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard
Response-dependent contributions of human primary motor cortex and University Press, Cambridge, MA.
angular gyrus to manual and perceptual sequence learning. J. Neurosci., 29, Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005)
15115–15125. Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition.
Roux, V. & Bril, B. (Eds) (2005) Stone Knapping: The Necessary Conditions Behav. Brain Sci., 28, 675–691.
for a Uniquely Hominin Behaviour. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Umiltà, M.A., Escola, L., Intskirveli, I., Grammont, F., Rochat, M., Caruana, F.,
Research, Cambridge. Jezzini, A., Gallese, V. & Rizzolatti, G. (2008) When pliers become fingers
Saxe, R. (2005) Against simulation: the argument from error. Trends Cogn. in the monkey motor system. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 105, 2209–2213.
Sci., 9, 174–179. de Vignemont, F. & Haggard, P. (2008) Action observation and execution: what
Semaw, S., Roger, M.J., Quade, J., Renne, P.R., Butler, R.F., Dominguez- is shared? Soc. Neurosci., 3, 421–433.
Rodrigo, M., Stout, D., Hart, W.S., Pickering, T. & Simpson, S.W. (2003) Vogt, S., Buccino, G., Wohlschlager, A.M., Canessa, N., Shah, N.J., Zilles, K.,
2.6-Million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS- Eickhoff, S.B., Freund, H.-J., Rizzolatti, G. & Fink, G.R. (2007) Prefrontal
7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. J. Hum. Evol., 45, 169–177. involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: effects of practice and
Stout, D. (2002) Skill and cognition in stone tool production: an ethnographic expertise. NeuroImage, 37, 1371–1383.
case study from Irian Jaya. Curr. Anthropol., 45, 693–722. Volle, E., Kinkingnehun, S., Pochon, J.-B., Mondon, K., Thiebaut de Schotten,
Stout, D. (2010) The evolution of cognitive control. Top. Cogn. Sci., 2, 614–630. M., Seassau, M., Duffau, H., Samson, Y., Dubois, B. & Levy, R. (2008) The
Stout, D. (2011) Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and functional architecture of the left posterior and lateral prefrontal cortex in
cognition. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B, 366, 1050–1059. humans. Cereb. Cortex, 18, 2460–2469.
Stout, D. & Chaminade, T. (2007) The evolutionary neuroscience of tool Whiten, A., Spiteri, A., Horner, V., Bonnie, K.E., Lambeth, S.P., Schapiro, S.J.
making. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1091–1100. & Waal, F.B.M.d. (2007) Transmission of multiple traditions within and
Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K.D. & Chaminade, T. (2008) Neural correlates of between chimpanzee groups. Curr. Biol., 17, 1038–1043.
Early Stone Age tool-making: technology, language and cognition in human Whittaker, J.C. (1994) Flintknapping: Making and Understanding Stone Tools.
evolution. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B, 363, 1939–1949. University of Texas Press, Austin.
Tehovnik, E.J., Sommer, M.A., Chou, I.H., Slocum, W.M. & Schiller, P.H. Wolpert, L. (2003) Causal belief and the origins of technology. Philos. Trans.
(2000) Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev., R Soc. Lond. A, 361, 1709–1719.
32, 413–448. Wynn, T. (1979) The intelligence of later acheulean hominids. Man, 14,
Tennie, C., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the 371–391.
evolution of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., Wynn, T. & McGrew, W. (1989) An ape’s view of the Oldowan. Man, 24,
364, 2405–2415. 383–398.

ª 2011 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2011 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1328–1338

You might also like