Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

Volume 130, Number 2, February 2023

British Journal of Anaesthesia, 130 (2): e209ee212 (2023)


doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.09.010
Advance Access Publication Date: 4 November 2022
© 2022 British Journal of Anaesthesia. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

EDITORIALS

Presumption of insensibility during general anaesthesia


Andrew E. Hudson
Department of Anesthesiology, Veterans Affairs Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles, CA, USA and
Department of Anesthesiology and Perioperative Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA

E-mail: ahudson@mednet.ucla.edu

This editorial accompanies: Connected consciousness after tracheal intubation in young adults: an international multicentre cohort study by
Lennertz et al., Br J Anaesth 2023:130:e217ee224, doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.04.010

Summary
Whilst the general presumption of the public is that general anaesthesia prevents awareness of any sensory stimuli,
Lennertz and colleagues have shown in this issue of the British Journal of Anaesthesia that 11% of young adults were able to
respond to auditory commands when neuromuscular blocking drugs were prevented from reaching one arm using the
isolated forearm technique. This occurred with anaesthetic regimens that followed usual clinical practice in each of the
10 countries that enrolled patients, and it was significantly more common in women than in men. This high incidence
demands attention. Further characterisation of the experience of these patients is essential to our understanding of the
state of general anaesthesia.

Keywords: accidental awareness during anaesthesia; cognitiveemotor dissociation; consciousness; general anaesthesia;
isolated forearm technique; qualia

‘The operation lasted 4 or 5 min, during which time the patient patients is that they will be unconscious (‘insensible to sur-
betrayed occasional marks of uneasiness, but upon subse- rounding objects’) during general anaesthesia. Anaesthetists
quently regaining her consciousness, she professed not only to routinely affirm that patients during general anaesthesia will
have felt no pain but to have been insensible to surrounding not be aware during the procedure. Yet, that claim may be
objects, to have known nothing of the operation, being only overconfident and unjustified; after all, consciousness is a
uneasy about a child left at home’.1 subjective experience that is not directly ascertainable by an
Since Bigelow’s initial breathless report of the demonstra- outside observer, so why are we so sure our patients are un-
tion of ether anaesthesia at the Massachusetts General Hospi- aware during their surgical procedures? Our only way to
tal by William Morton in 1846, the expectation amongst surgical determine that someone is conscious is to judge whether their
reactions to their environment accord with our understanding
of what a conscious being would do under those circumstances.
DOI of original article: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.04.010. Given that balanced anaesthetic regimens prevent movement,

For Permissions, please email: permissions@elsevier.com

e209
e210 - Editorials

they largely obliterate the ability of patients to respond to their one of connected consciousness, although as we will see later,
environment, so our patients certainly appear unconscious, it is unclear how IFT responders experience the phenomenon.
regardless of their subjective experience during the procedure. Consciousness and awareness can occur without explicit
Fine, but patients must certainly remember if they endure memory formation, as when not recalling your drive home
something as jarring as awareness during anaesthesia, right? from work despite awareness of the environment sufficient to
Overwhelmingly, our determination of a patient’s unintended avoid an accident, and false explicit memories can be formed
awareness comes after the fact when patients recall experi- of events that did not occur. These represent a challenge for
ences from the period when they were anaesthetised. Yet, most reliable eye witness testimony14 and for those interested in
sedativeehypnotic agents are amnestic even at sub-hypnotic studying awareness during anaesthesia.15
doses,2 so the relative rarity of reports must necessarily form Lennertz and colleagues11 proposed to prospectively
a lower limit on the incidence of accidental awareness during investigate specific risk factors for connected consciousness
anaesthesia. The 5th National Audit Project in the UK and during general anaesthesia, including female sex and whether
Ireland found that one in 19 000 general anaesthetics yielded a anaesthesia is continuously delivered during the time be-
spontaneous report of accidental awareness by the patient, tween induction of anaesthesia and tracheal intubation, and
with the incidence increasing to one in 8000 cases when to characterise the IFT response with a mix of multiple audi-
neuromuscular blocking drugs were used.3 When actively tory stimulus types and tests for implicit and explicit memory
soliciting memories of the intraoperative period using a formation. Administration of anaesthetic drugs was at the
modified Brice structured interview format,4 the apparent discretion of the anaesthetist, and enrolling centres were
incidence increases in high-risk populations by more than an located in 10 different countries, suggesting a relatively high
order of magnitude to one in 500e600.5e7 Given that a struc- external validity. Target enrolment was 500 patients, but
tured interview can prompt such a huge increase in the inci- funding constraints forced the study to stop short at 344
dence of probable awareness during anaesthesia, a number of enrollees. Even with the decreased power, the group was able
anaesthetists have wondered about studying patients who to show that young adults have an 11% response rate after
retain a communication channel during a balanced anaesthetic intubation, with an adjusted odds ratio of 2.7 for female vs
to allow direct querying of the patient’s mental status in real male sex (95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.1e7.6), despite almost
time during anaesthetic administration. no differences in dosing of anaesthetics between the sexes.
Tunstall8 proposed the isolated forearm technique (IFT) as Delivery of anaesthesia during the interval between induction
a way to query patients directly for conscious experience and intubation, whether by i.v. infusion, intermittent boluses,
during general anaesthesia in 1977. In brief, anaesthetic in- or inhalation, decreased the risk of IFT responsiveness, with
duction takes place as normal, but before administration of a an adjusted odds ratio of 0.43 (95% CI: 0.20e0.96). Responses
neuromuscular blocking drug a BP cuff is inflated above arte- were more likely to occur with commands than with random
rial pressure in one arm, sparing it from delivery of the non-command verbal stimuli. Perhaps most concerning, 18 of
neuromuscular blocking drug. This hand is then able to 37 responders endorsed being in pain when given the com-
squeeze in response to queries from the investigators. mand, ‘If you are in pain, squeeze my hand two times’. There
Through careful titration, as many as one in three patients was no evidence for implicit memory formation in the word
receiving a light general anaesthetic have IFT responsiveness retrieval assay used by the team. Finally, the Brice structured
(i.e. they respond appropriately to questioning with a hand interview yielded an incidence of definite explicit recall of 0.3%
squeeze).9 In an attempt to understand the real-world inci- (95% CI: 0.03e1.3%), consistent with that reported previously.
dence of IFT responsiveness during general anaesthesia, It should be noted that assessors of IFT responsiveness were
Sanders and colleagues10 reported in 2017 that the incidence of not blinded to anaesthetic dosing, and assessors of intra-
IFT responsiveness in a multicentre prospective cohort study operative recall were not blinded to IFT responsiveness.
of 254 patients was 4.6%, and that responders were younger These results, corroborating the 2017 report, are quite
than non-responders. Interestingly, none of the IFT re- disconcerting. Note that comparing incidence directly be-
sponders had any memories of the experience. tween the two studies is not possible, as the definition of a
In this issue of the British Journal of Anaesthesia, an expanded response differs between the reports. If accidental awareness
research group has submitted a follow-up to their 2017 study: does occur in ~1 in 10 young adults receiving general anaes-
an international multicentre prospective cohort study thesia, that is orders of magnitude greater than the incidence
designed to assess IFT responsiveness shortly after tracheal that most anaesthetists expect and wildly discordant with
intubation in patients 18e40 yr old.11 For technical and philo- public expectations. Moreover, these results suggest ~1 in
sophical reasons, the authors describe conscious awareness of 20 such adults are experiencing pain during this period of
study stimuli generating motor responses during general awareness.
anaesthesia as ‘connected consciousness’ to emphasise that An important element of this study is the serial sampling of
the conscious experience is connected to the outside envi- responsiveness over time. Such an approach is an important
ronment, as distinct from the ‘disconnected consciousness’ of step for several reasons. One, effect-site concentrations are
dreaming. Thus, ‘connected consciousness’ indicates some fluctuating during the period after induction, making a varying
level of conscious awareness of the environment. This has level of consciousness quite understandable, so patients
perhaps added to the terminological clutter in the field, where might respond at some time points and not at others. A
definitions are overlapping and vary from author to author. In steady-state drug concentration does not guarantee stable
general, awareness has a long history as a term to describe the brain electrical behaviour,16 but at least it is more predictable.
contents of, as opposed to level of, consciousness.12,13 Thus, We therefore cannot infer that this ability to parse statements
connected consciousness is another name for awareness of was necessarily stable over time. Second, multiple repeated
the external environment. This does not mean that connected probes of responsiveness allow some attempt to understand
consciousness implies a particular type of experience (e.g. pain the patient’s conscious experience. Are they responding to any
during the operation). Normal wakeful conscious experience is auditory stimulus because they were instructed in advance to
Editorials - e211

do so, even if they are not parsing the experience? A few pa- but very few (1/338) remembered the experience. The low
tients (4/338) responded to the nonsense statement but not the incidence of complaints of accidental awareness with recall
command to squeeze the investigator’s hand, suggesting that certainly suggests that, for the majority of these patients, the
this could plausibly be true, but more patients (13/338) benefits of undergoing surgery with general anaesthesia
responded only to the command and not to the nonsense outweigh the risk of the potential experience of connected
statement. Use of more complicated probes (i.e. ‘If stones float consciousness during anaesthesia, and a disclosure of this
on water, squeeze my hand’) could be even more revealing, poorly defined potential experience might dissuade patients
but it would require more statistical power to interpret. from undergoing necessary procedures. Yet, a 5% incidence of
The incongruity of patients who respond to direct queries, connected consciousness associated with pain is high enough
including queries about pain, in the affirmative but do not in this cohort that it could be argued that a reasonable person
necessarily move spontaneously in response to surgery has might choose to decline general anaesthesia to avoid a
prompted some to question whether the IFT response is suf- potentially uncomfortable experience. As a result, disclosure
ficient as an indication of consciousness, and whether the around the risks of awareness during general anaesthesia has
degree of consciousness that it indicates is necessarily unac- been controversial.22 I foresee heated discussions ahead as we
ceptable.17 Whilst consciousness is not necessarily all or none, wrestle with the implications of this new information. Within
with gradations in level of arousal and content potentially a shared decision-making model, I would argue that, for the
mediated by anaesthetic drug effects, it would seem that patient that expresses concern about being aware during
decoding semantic content from an auditory stimulus and anaesthesia, it is reasonable to consider beginning a
then selecting an appropriate motor response (‘If you are in riskebenefit discussion with the low incidence of accidental
pain, squeeze my hand twice’) clearly indicate some degree of awareness with recall, to discuss the data of Lennertz and
consciousness. Perhaps whatever that consciousness feels like colleagues11 as we know it, to assure the patient that we will
(in philosophical terms, its qualia) is not the same as normal act if their vital signs suggest discomfort and to assure them
wakeful consciousness, but if it is experienced by one in 10 we will take steps to minimise the chance of an episode of
young adults it seems prudent to prepare patients for that connected consciousness. For example, the results reported by
possibility. Lennertz and colleagues11 do offer one immediate suggestion
This disconcerting revelation of how much we do not un- for clinical practice: providing anaesthesia continuously after
derstand about the conscious experience in anaesthetised induction decreases the risk of IFT response by roughly a
patients has a clear parallel in the discovery of covert aware- factor of 2. It is reasonable to recommend that an induction
ness in patients with disorders of consciousness after a brain bolus dose of hypnotic should be followed by additional
injury, also known as cognitiveemotor dissociation. This is an anaesthetic administration, i.v. or inhaled, until the airway is
active area of research in disorders of consciousness.18 In- secured and maintenance anaesthetic delivery begins.
vestigators have identified that a subset of brain-injured pa-
tients have electroencephalographic or functional imaging
Declaration of interest
evidence of brain activity congruent with following a com-
mand in response to verbal directions as opposed to closely The author declares that they have no conflict of interest.
matched but scrambled auditory stimuli.19,20 The ongoing
work in cognitiveemotor dissociation may offer some insights
and methodologic inspiration for anaesthetists interested in Funding
characterising the conscious experience of patients undergo- US National Institute for General Medical Science (1R01
ing general anaesthesia, and any resulting studies could GM135420).
certainly inform investigations into mechanisms of other
disorders of consciousness.21 A number of open questions
References
with immediate clinical relevance clearly remain. Why are
women twice as likely to be IFT responders? Why does 1. Bigelow HJ. Insensibility during surgical operations pro-
neuromuscular block increase the incidence of awareness? Is duced by inhalation. Boston Med Surg J 1846; 35: 309e17
it simply that less hypnotic is administered, or is there 2. Veselis RA, Pryor KO, Reinsel RA, Li Y, Mehta M,
something intrinsically ‘arousing’ about paralysis? Johnson Jr R. Propofol and midazolam inhibit conscious
Anaesthetists, we are now in an uncomfortable place memory processes very soon after encoding: an event
where it is clear that some fraction of young patients may related potential study of familiarity and recollection in
experience connected consciousness during general anaes- volunteers. Anesthesiology 2009; 110: 295e312
thesia, but we are not yet able to determine the qualia of those 3. Pandit JJ, Andrade J, Bogod DG, et al. 5th National Audit
connected consciousness episodes, so we cannot counsel pa- Project (NAP5) on accidental awareness during general
tients about what to expect from a first-person perspective in anaesthesia: summary of main findings and risk factors.
the event of recall. At a minimum, further study into the na- Br J Anaesth 2014; 113: 549e59
ture of whatever conscious experience of IFT responders is 4. Brice DD, Hetherington RR, Utting JE. A simple study of
necessary. This line of work may ultimately need to include awareness and dreaming during anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth
setting different expectations for patients undergoing general 1970; 42: 535e42
anaesthesia, such that there is some non-zero chance that 5. Myles PS, Leslie K, McNeil J, Forbes A, Chan MTV. Bis-
they experience ‘connected consciousness’ during anaes- pectral index monitoring to prevent awareness during
thesia, but what shall we say they might be aware of? As of anaesthesia: the B-Aware randomised controlled trial.
now, all we know are the raw rates reported by Lennertz and Lancet 2004; 363: 1757e63
colleagues11: 11% (37/338) responded to auditory stimuli at 6. Avidan MS, Zhang L, Burnside BA, et al. Anesthesia
some point, almost half of whom (18/338) responded when awareness and the bispectral index. N Engl J Med 2008;
asked to squeeze the investigator’s hand if they were in pain, 358: 1097e108
e212 - Editorials

7. Avidan MS, Jacobsohn E, Glick D, et al. Prevention of 16. Hudson AE. Metastability of neuronal dynamics during
intraoperative awareness in a high-risk surgical popula- general anesthesia: time for a change in our assumptions?
tion. N Engl J Med 2011; 365: 591e600 Front Neural Circuits 2017; 11: 58
8. Tunstall ME. Detecting wakefulness during general 17. Pandit JJ. Isolated forearmdor isolated brain? Interpreting
anaesthesia for caesarean section. Br Med J 1977; 1: 1321 responses during anaesthesiador ‘dysanaesthesia’.
9. Russell IF. The ability of bispectral index to detect intra- Anaesthesia 2013; 68: 995e1009
operative wakefulness during isoflurane/air anaesthesia, 18. Claassen J, Akbari Y, Alexander S, et al. Proceedings of the
compared with the isolated forearm technique. Anaes- First Curing Coma Campaign NIH Symposium: chal-
thesia 2013; 68: 1010e20 lenging the future of research for coma and disorders of
10. Sanders RD, Gaskell A, Raz A, et al. Incidence of connected consciousness. Neurocrit Care 2021; 35: 4e23
consciousness after tracheal intubation: a prospective, 19. Owen AM. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging
international, multicenter cohort study of the isolated and electroencephalography to detect consciousness after
forearm technique. Anesthesiology 2017; 126: 214e22 severe brain injury. Handb Clin Neurol 2015; 127: 277e93
11. Lennertz R, Pryor KO, Raz A, et al. The incidence of con- 20. Boly M, Laureys S. Functional “unlocking”: bedside
nected consciousness after tracheal intubation in young detection of covert awareness after severe brain damage.
adults: an international multicenter cohort study. Br J Brain 2018; 141: 1239e41
Anaesth 2022 21. Luppi AI, Cain J, Spindler LRB, et al. Mechanisms under-
12. Fromm E. Awareness versus consciousness. Psychol Rep lying disorders of consciousness: bridging gaps to move
1965; 16: 711e2 toward an integrated translational science. Neurocrit Care
13. Zeman A. What in the world is consciousness? Prog Brain 2021; 35: 37e54
Res 2005; 150: 1e10 22. Pandit JJ. Acceptably aware during general anaesthesia:
14. Loftus EF. Make-believe memories. Am Psychol 2003; 58: ‘dysanaesthesia’dthe uncoupling of perception from
867e73 sensory inputs. Conscious Cogn 2014; 27: 194e212
15. Cook TM, Pandit JJ. Pitfalls of comparing incidences of
awareness from NAP5 and from Brice studies. Br J Anaesth
2015; 115: 471e2

British Journal of Anaesthesia, 130 (2): e212ee214 (2023)


doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.08.024
Advance Access Publication Date: 29 September 2022
© 2022 British Journal of Anaesthesia. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Microf luidics-assisted bloodebrain barrier device: a powerful tool to


study perioperative neurocognitive disorder
Zhongcong Xie
Department of Anesthesia, Critical Care and Pain Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School,
Charlestown, MA, USA

E-mail: zxie@mgh.harvard.edu

This editorial accompanies: Protective effects of omega-3 fatty acids in a bloodebrain barrier-on-chip model and on postoperative delirium-
like behaviour in mice by Yang et al., Br J Anaesth 2023:130:e370ee380, doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.05.025

Summary
The dysfunction of the blood-brain barrier could contribute to the pathogenesis of the perioperative neurocognitive
disorder. In a recent study published in the British Journal of Anaesthesia, Yang and colleagues developed an innovative
microfluidics-assisted blood-brain barrier device to investigate the effects of neuroimmune interactions on blood-brain
barrier opening. The findings are important and timely to understanding the mechanistic insights of perioperative
neurocognitive disorder.

Keywords: anaesthesia; blood-brain barrier; interleukin-1b; neurocognition; omega-3 fatty acids; surgery

Perioperative neurocognitive disorder (PND),1 one of the most potentially associated with Alzheimer’s disease, impairments
common postoperative complications in older adults,2,3 is in daily functioning, government assistance dependency, and
increased morbidity and mortality.4 PND includes post-
operative delirium, delayed neurocognitive recovery (dNCR),
DOI of original article: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.05.025. mild neurocognitive disorder (NCD), and major NCD.1

You might also like