Acceptance and Commitment Therapy Alteri

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

The Behavior Analyst 1994, 17, 289-303 No.

2 (Fall)

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy:


Altering the Verbal Support for Experiential Avoidance
Steven C. Hayes and Kelly G. Wilson
University of Nevada
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) is a behavior-analytically-based psychotherapy approach
that attempts to undermine emotional avoidance and increase the capacity for behavior change. An
overview of this approach is given, followed by several specific examples of the techniques used within
ACT. In each instance the behavioral rationale of these techniques is described. A contemporary view
of verbal relations provides the basis for new approaches to adult outpatient psychotherapy.
Key words: rule-governed behavior, emotions, psychotherapy, Acceptance and Commitment Therapy,
cognition

In our previous article in this series chotherapies that have consciously been
(Hayes & Wilson, 1993), we described a based on behavior-analytic thinking (R.
relational frame conception of verbal Kohlenberg, Hayes, & Tsai, 1993; R.
events, and related this theory to an anal- Kohlenberg, Tsai, & Dougher, 1993).
ysis of rule-governed behavior. We de- In this article we will briefly summa-
veloped the implications of this view for rize our approach. We will then explore
several clinical topics, including mean- two issues in greater detail, showing the
inglessness and existential angst, suicide, kinds of things we do and providing a
insight and self-knowledge, and the hu- behavioral rationale.
man tendency toward emotional avoid-
ance. Together, these lines of thinking ACCEPTANCE AND
create the outlines of a theory of psycho- COMMITMENT THERAPY
pathology, in which many common forms The essential goal of ACT is to treat
of psychopathology are interpreted as be- emotional avoidance, excessive literal re-
ing the natural result of human verbal sponse to cognitive content, and the in-
behavior. ability to make and keep commitments
Over the last 15 years we have devel- to behavior change (Hayes, 1987; Hayes,
oped a set of techniques designed to alter Kohlenberg, & Melancon, 1989; Hayes
the way verbal relations function, based & Melancon, 1989; Zettle & Hayes, 1986).
on this analysis. Taken together, these
techniques form a behavior analytically Conventional Support for a Causal
derived psychotherapy approach: Accep- Role of Private Events
tance and Commitment Therapy or ACT
(pronounced "act" not "A-C-T"). (As this Most psychotherapies deal, implicitly
approach was being developed, it was or explicitly, with the effect of client
known as comprehensive distancing, but thoughts and feelings on overt behavior.
the therapy was renamed to avoid the In the usual view, certain undesirable
undesirable and inaccurate dissociative emotions or thoughts are believed to
connotations of that term.) ACT is one cause undesirable patterns of living. On
of a very few comprehensive verbal psy- that basis these thoughts or emotions are
targeted for change, control, or elimina-
tion. Avoidance of a variety of private
Preparation of this manuscript was supported in events can be understood as both a so-
part by Grant DA08634 from the National Institute cially encouraged outcome and as a nat-
on Drug Abuse. ural outcome for verbal organisms as re-
Correspondence concerning this article should be sult of bidirectional transformation of
addressed to Steven C. Hayes, Department of Psy-
chology, University of Nevada, Reno, NV 89557- stimulus functions through equivalence
0062. and other derived relational responses
289
290 STEVEN C. HAYES & KELLY G. WILSON

(see Hayes & Wilson, 1993, for a more Functions given to one member of a re-
complete discussion). Rather than trying lational network will transfer to others in
to change the form or frequency of pri- terms ofthe underlying relation. Such ef-
vate events, ACT attempts to alter their fects have been repeatedly documented
psychological functions by altering the in the equivalence literature (Hayes,
social/verbal context in which private Brownstein, Devany, Kohlenberg, &
events occur. Shelby, 1987; Hayes, Kohlenberg, &
According to an ACT perspective, the Hayes, 1991; B. Kohlenberg, Hayes, &
controlling effects of private events over Hayes, 1991; Wulfert & Hayes, 1988). In
overt behavior are supported by the so- common language, words mean the things
cial/verbal context that supports the lit- to which they relate, and many functions
erality of language, and the resultant need that would adhere to the situation be-
to alter the form of private events. Sup- come present with regard to the words.
pose a panic-disordered person thinks "If The verbal community arbitrarily es-
I get too anxious I will go crazy." Given tablishes the specific relation between
a context (both historical and situational) words and other events. These conven-
that supports the domination of derived tions are what we mean by "the context
stimulus functions based on such rela- of literality." In many situations, this
tional responses over other sources of be- ability to respond to verbal formulations
havior, the private event called "anxi- in some ways as if one were responding
ety" may occasion a variety of disruptive to the actual contingencies described is
behavior, such as running out of the sit- extremely adaptive. "Don't drink that-
uation "in order to reduce the anxiety" it's poison," for example, can establish a
and thus avoid going crazy. From an ACT behavioral topography that would be dif-
perspective, however, the behavior reg- ficult to acquire through direct contact
ulatory function of anxiety and thoughts with the contingencies described. Other
about anxiety is not mechanical or direct. descriptions are less clearly adaptive and,
When the context supporting excessive in some instances, may be psychologi-
literality is changed, the link between pri- cally destructive, such as "Don't feel X,"
vate events and overt action can be re- or "Don't remember X." The problem
duced, even in the presence of such is that in some contexts derived stimulus
thoughts, and attention can then shift functions can dominate over other, more
from emotional or cognitive manipula- direct forms ofbehavioral influence. ACT
tion to the more direct consequences of seeks to undermine literality, such that
overt action. In short, the issue becomes literal meaning is no longer the necessary
doing what works, particularly in the long basis for responding-rather, it is the
term, rather than feeling, thinking, re- psychological context for responding only
membering, or sensing only certain things on the basis of experienced workability.
in order to do what works. In other words, we seek to limit rational
At least three aspects of the normal responding to situations in which ratio-
social/verbal context for human action nality is helpful as a basis for responding.
are thought to contribute to the estab- To do this, however, derived stimulus
lishment of undesirable control by pri- functions must come more into balance
vate behavior: (a) the impact of literal with those acquired directly. To give a
meaning and evaluation, (b) the accep- slightly simplified analysis of the exam-
tance of verbal reason giving as a valid ple of the panic-disordered person we de-
explanation for individual behavior, and scribed earlier, the impact of "if I get too
(c) social training that cognitive and emo- anxious I will go crazy" is based on at
tional control can, and should, be least three kinds of verbal relations: (a)
achieved as a means to successful living. a strong equivalence relation between
Literality. Words enter into stimulus various bodily sensations and behavioral
equivalence and other derived relations predispositions and the term anxiety, (b)
(Hayes & Hayes, 1989; Sidman & Tailby, a similarly tight equivalence class be-
1982) with verbal and nonverbal events. tween highly undesirable and socially
ACCEPTANCE AND COMMITMENT THERAPY 291

condemned behavior in others and "go- Most of these functions are conven-
ing crazy," and (c) the ability to apply an tional, however, rather than necessary.
"if... then" relation as an operant class Depressed feelings need not necessarily
based on cues to do so rather than on lead to behavior change, and under other
direct experience with the sequential re- conditions they may not. One such con-
lation. The panic-disordered person's di- dition may be the weakening of support
rect history is almost never that anxiety by the verbal community for emotional
itself leads to going crazy. Indeed, the or other experiential reasons. The social/
direct history is usually more that at- verbal community of therapist and client
tempting to avoid anxiety creates anxiety created in an ACT session gives little or
(in part, because such avoidance para- no support for reason giving ofthis kind.
doxically supports the if... then relation Training in experiential control. The
between anxiety and aversive ends, and process of emotional and cognitive reg-
anxiety is a natural response to imminent ulation as a means of behavior control
aversive stimulation). The domination begins quite early. Even babies are often
ofderived over direct stimulus functions evaluated according to how little they ex-
is based, we argue, on the domination of press negative affective states (e.g., "She's
contextual support for literal (i.e., highly such a good baby, she never cries"). Chil-
relational) responding. dren are told, regularly and often, that
Reason giving. A second source of the they can and ought to control negative
relation between private events and overt affective states. Punishment and rein-
behavior is reason giving. Thoughts and forcement are frequently doled out ac-
feelings are commonly pointed to as val- cording to the ability to control and sup-
id and sensible causes of overt behav- press at least the outward signs ofaversive
ior-an explanation for behavior that is emotional states ("Stop crying or I'll give
well supported by the culture. A person you something to cry about"). Siblings
saying "I was too anxious to stay at the and schoolmates support the ongoing
mall" will certainly be thought to have purposeful control of emotion. State-
said something reasonable and under- ments such as "Don't be a baby" or "Just
standable. He or she may even gamer forget about X" will be backed up by a
sympathy or reassurance for this for- variety of socially mediated conse-
mulation. "I have no idea why I left" will quences (e.g., being hit, being shamed,
probably receive a much less positive re- etc.). Clients often arrive in therapy fo-
sponse. In this way, the verbal commu- cused on this agenda: "I can't control my
nity establishes discriminative and mo- depression" or "I'm too anxious." Even
tivational functions for a variety of in the therapeutic milieu, the therapist
private events that are conventionally may overtly tell the client to emote, ex-
part of reasons and explanations. The press, and report negative emotions, but
verbal community that reinforces (or does in subtle ways may punish the client's
not punish) behavior because a reason negatively evaluated affect, thoughts, or
points to a private event as a cause for memories. Furthermore, the therapeutic
that behavior, is also reinforcing two dis- agenda itself may imply as much, because
tinct things. First, it reinforces the be- it is common to accept as a goal of ther-
havior regulatory function of the private apy the reduction or alteration of emo-
event itself. Second, it reinforces the oc- tional and cognitive events. Given these
currence of the private event that is ac- contingencies, private events gain more
tually related to the behavior that this behavior regulatory power, because they
event supposedly controls. For example, occasion such determined efforts to alter
if one can successfully avoid a difficult their form or frequency of occurrence.
job interview "because I am just too de-
pressed to go," then (a) feeling depressed SYNOPSIS OF ACT
is more likely to lead to the avoidance of
difficult tasks, and (b) depressed feelings Clients naturally come into therapy
are more likely when such tasks occur. with the intent to control what they view
292 STEVEN C. HAYES & KELLY G. WILSON

TABLE 1
Essential components of acceptance and commitment therapy
Behavioral/
rule-governed
behavior (RGB)
Component principles Purpose Technique
Creative hopelessness Augmentals, al- Disruption of ongoing avoidance Paradox, confusion,
tering dis- repertoires, disruption of social metaphor, affirma-
criminative verbal support for avoidance tion of underlying
value of (SD, EO), making psychological- fears about hope-
avoided pri- ly present the futility of the pur- lessness
vate events as suit of relief in providing relief
SD's for
avoidance
repertoires
Control of private events Establishing Describe contingencies generating Direct description, ex-
as problem tracks, dis- and maintaining avoidance, de- periential exercises
ruption of scribing ways avoidance inhibits (e.g., polygraph
pliance life functioning metaphor), descrip-
tion of inherent
paradox
Discriminating observing Altering the Establishing a context from which Experiential exercises
self from content ob- context (SD, psychological acceptance is pos- (e.g., observer exer-
served EO) for con- sible, and where avoidance is cise), deliteraliza-
trol unnecessary tion exercises (e.g.,
milk-milk-milk)
Choosing and valuing a Establishing Supporting the client in making Direct description, or-
direction tracks linked contact with what he or she dinary life examples
to verbally would choose to value in life,
constructed establish valuing as an activity,
consequences distinguishing choices from de-
(see Hayes & cisions and describing the ap-
Wilson, propriate arena for each
1993), aug-
menting
Letting go of the strug- Facilitating con- Facilitate the direct shaping of Metaphor, willingness
gle/embracing symp- tact with di- repertoires possible in the ab- exercises, experien-
toms rect contin- sence of the second-order agen- tial exercises
gencies da of avoiding classically condi-
(versus RGB) tioned responses
Commitment and be- Establishing Facilitating the client's choosing Making behavioral
havior change tracks based directions that he or she values, commitments, will-
upon results while noticing the various pri- ingness exercises
of above pro- vate events that emerge, as what
cesses they are, not what they say they
are, and doing what is there to
be done

as the determinants of their problematic contact with other sources of reinforce-


life condition. Taken together, the above ment. ACT attempts to disrupt this prob-
circumstances and contingencies focus lematic stimulus control, opening up the
attention on the apparent need for more possibility of contact with alternative
successful avoidance repertoires (i.e., sources of reinforcement.
elimination of disturbing emotions, We will first describe in very general
thoughts, memories, impulses, and so on). ways the components of ACT, arranged
The presence of these learned stimulus into several short sections (Table 1). We
functions for private events can prevent will then explore aspects of two of these
ACCEPTANCE AND COMMITMENT THERAPY 293
components in detail, so that the reader verbal consequence and thereby increase
can get a better sense of what actually the probability that other repertoires
goes on in ACT and the reasons for it. might emerge, through which other con-
sequences might be contacted.
Creative Hopelessness The following is an actual example from
a session early in ACT. It has been slight-
In the first component of ACT, the ly edited for clarity and to protect con-
therapist attempts to establish a condi- fidentiality:
tion of "creative hopelessness," in which Therapist: Right. That doesn't make sense. It's
former "solutions" begin to be seen by not logical-it's not like that anywhere else in your
the client as problems in themselves. Un- life. You've worked hard. If you're building some-
der such conditions, the client is more thing, you work on it, it gets better.
open to entirely new courses of action. thoughtClient: Uh hum-it doesn't make sense-I've
about it a lot, a lot of long hours just of
When all "solutions" are no longer so- hard thinking just about this.
lutions, the client may view the situation Therapist: We have to consider the possibility
as hopeless, but it is a creative condition that what is the most obvious thing to you, the most
because now fundamentally new ap- you obvious thing, is actually not so; the most clear thing
proaches are possible. have to do in terms of moving ahead, that that's
actually the problem, not the solution, it's the prob-
Because the client's solutions are near- lem.
ly all both logical and reasonable, the Client: My mind stops working here.
therapist must behave in ways that are Therapist: Good, good-actually that may be
neither. The purpose ofthis phase of ACT helpful. Because again, if what I'm saying right now
makes sense, you'd say "oh yeah, that's right," but,
is to confront the social/verbal supports if it makes sense that's probably not it. Because
for the client's means-ends formulations you've already done everything that makes sense.
(based on these contexts of literality, rea- Confusion is used deliberately to prevent
son giving, and experiential control). All
of the client's efforts at emotional control the client from intellectualizing and com-
are explored in detail and in each case partmentalizing his or her dilemmas into
the same
the client is asked if, in their experience, failed. In solutions that have already
addition
this approach truly solved the problem. interventions, many these to paradoxical
To some degree, the answer must be "no," interventions in
or the client would not be in therapy. The ACT are nonlinear and metaphorical.
therapist emphasizes the great exertion This other,
use of metaphor fits fairly well with
more humanistic uses of metaphor
and minimal benefit of efforts to control
emotions, thoughts, sensations, memo- in the clinical setting (see McCurry &
ries, and other private events. Because in Hayes, 1993, for a review), but it differs
the client has already attempted logical, that specific ACT-relevant themes are
commonsense solutions, some change always at the focus of such metaphorical
beyond ordinary verbal logic is clearly talk.
needed. Clients often have an underlying Trying to Control Private
fear that the situation is hopeless. This Events as the Problem
sense is brought out and affirmed. Within
the context in which the client has been In the second component of ACT,
working, the therapist agrees that the sit- therapists target emotional and cognitive
uation is hopeless. The client's sense of control as the core obstacle preventing
being stuck is also brought out and af- successful solution ofthe problems in liv-
firmed. ing faced by the client. As discussed
In behavioral terms, "relief' is a ver- above, by the time the client comes to
bally constructed consequence for the therapy, he or she has been well trained
client's avoidance repertoires (see Hayes to view control of private events as im-
& Wilson, 1993, for a discussion of ver- portant. At this stage of therapy, the ther-
bal vs. nonverbal purpose). The purpose apist gives examples of ways in which we
of this phase of ACT is motivative. We are socialized to exercise such control.
seek to deestablish "relief' as a valued Among those emphasized are:
294 STEVEN C. HAYES & KELLY G. WILSON

1. We experience direct instruction to cial sanctions to avoid "unpleasant" pri-


control emotion: "stop that crying," "just vate events.
forget about it," "put it behind you."
2. Significant persons have modeled Establishing Self as Context
apparent control of thoughts, emotions, Rather Than Content
and the like. Adults are often adept at
hiding their emotional responses from The third component of ACT helps the
children. From the perspective of the client to discriminate the person he or
child, emotional responses appear to be she calls "I," and the problem behavior
absent: "See, daddy isn't afraid." that the client wants to eliminate. This
3. There is likely to be generalization topic is behaviorally difficult, and space
from situations in which children are en- limitations preclude a detailed discussion
couraged and are successful at controlling (see Hayes, 1984, 1987). The essential
environmental events. A repertoire of idea is that verbal training leads to a form
verbally governed control efforts works of self-awareness that consists of the lo-
very well in dealing with the world, but cus of verbal events, rather than the con-
it works horribly in dealing with one's tent of such events. That is, humans learn
own history or one's own private events. not only to observe their own actions (one
4. Emotional control may actually be form of self-awareness) but also to do so
effective in limited or temporary ways. from a consistent locus or point of view-
Distraction, for example, may be an ef- what is usually called "I."
fective strategy for the management of ACT uses various experiential exercis-
aversive emotional responding when the es and metaphors to help this sense of
source of that response is temporary (i.e., "I" come into focus. Most people can
an impending dental appointment). experientially recognize the essential
However, if the source of the response is continuity between the "I"s referred to
persistent or permanent, such as an incest in the statements "I went to first grade"
history, these strategies become less ten- and "I am in therapy now," even if many
able both because the temporary effects decades have passed from one to the oth-
of distraction wear off, setting the stage er and virtually everything in the realm
for another round of distraction efforts, of content has changed.
and because such deliberate avoidance Other techniques are used at this point
actually increases the behavior regula- in ACT to begin to separate thoughts,
tory function and thus the psychological emotions, and other private events from
importance of the avoided event. the person having them. For example, we
Clients are encouraged to examine their ask clients at least temporarily to adopt
own experience to see if the rule that a particular verbal style in therapy in
works in the world of objects ("If you which the type of verbal event is named,
don't like something, figure out how to rather than simply stating the content of
get rid of it and then get rid of it") has that event. For example, clients are taught
worked in the world of private events. to say "I'm having the thought that I can't
We suggest that a more accurate rule for go to the mall" (as opposed to simply
the arena of private events is "Ifyou aren't stating, "I can't go to the mall"), or "I'm
willing to have it, then you've got it." having the evaluation that I'm a bad per-
Again, metaphor and paradox are used son" (as opposed to "I'm a bad person").
to highlight this dilemma (e.g., see the Clients have often attempted to separate
polygraph metaphor below). themselves from negatively evaluated
The therapist, in describing the client's thoughts and emotions. Here, however,
experience, elaborates tracks that the cli- the separation is to increase contact with
ent is following, along with the actually the private event rather than to decrease
contacted, rather than verbally con- it-which has been the client's agenda.
structed, consequences of following those A commonly used ACT metaphor may
tracks. In addition, the therapist attempts help to clarify this point. In the meta-
to undermine the client's pliance with so- phor, the client's private events (both
ACCEPTANCE AND COMMITMENT THERAPY 295

negatively and positively evaluated) are tions as they discuss the issue of valuing.
said to be like pieces on a chess board. Among these is the distinction between
Within this metaphor, the counterpart for valuing as a feeling and valuing as an
"self as context" is not the chess pieces activity. These two are often thoroughly
(the content) but the board itself. If the connected for clients. The verbal support
client is observing events "from piece for this connection between private events
level," certain pieces on the board may related to values and active valuing is
be obscured or altogether invisible de- attacked. The example of valuing a lov-
pending on their placement on the board. ing relationship with one's spouse pro-
The board, however, is fully in contact vides an instructive example. One's feel-
with all of the pieces. It is also worth ings of love may wax and wane across
noting that pieces on the board may time and situation. To behave lovingly
threaten one another: A white piece such (i.e., respectfully, thoughtfully, etc.) only
as "I'll get through this" may be threat- when one had feelings of love, and to
ened by a large black queen shouting "Not behave in opposite ways when the op-
in this lifetime!" The board, however, posite feelings emerged, would be likely
contains and is fully in contact with, but to have problematic effects on a mar-
not threatened by, any piece, be it posi- riage. We ask our clients "Could you be-
tive or negative. Such metaphors are have lovingly, even when you were not
aimed at altering the verbally established feeling loving?" We are careful here to
discriminative and motivative context in assure the client that we are not talking
which control of private events is nec- about faking love-duplicity would
essary. hardly characterize loving behavior.
What we are attempting to highlight is
Choosing and Valuing a Direction the client's ability to control some things
and not others, and to get the client to
In the fourth component of ACT, the exercise control in the areas where con-
therapist supports the client in establishing trol is effective.
tracks linked to verbally constructed con- The ACT therapist focuses on valuing
sequences. Often the goals that a client has and choosing as activities, not because
for therapy emerge in the context of ex- the feeling aspect of a client's valuing is
ploring his or her avoidance repertoires. unimportant, but because efforts at con-
The client may come to therapy wanting trol are effective in the domain of activ-
to be rid of depression or anxiety, and that ities, whereas they are problematic in the
wish will often be explicitly linked to some areas of thoughts and feelings. In tech-
outcome he or she would choose-if only nical terms, valuing as an activity is op-
these problematic thoughts and feelings erant behavior, whereas valuing as a feel-
would abate. So an agoraphobic client ing is more likely to be respondent. We
might say that he or she needs to get rid further characterize valuing and choosing
of panic so that he or she can really move as activities that are unavoidable. Among
forward in a career. Or the incest survivor the techniques used are questions such as
might say that the memories need to stop "What do you want your life to stand
so that he or she can develop a truly in- for?" or "If your epitaph were to be writ-
timate relationship with a spouse. At other ten, based upon what you are doing to-
times, we may see clients for whom being day, what would it read?" Clients will
psychologically present to what they val- seldom endorse an epitaph such as "She
ue-and believe that they cannot have- dedicated her life to avoiding anxiety."
is so aversive that they might pronounce We often speak ofthis active component
loudly that they want the opposite. It has of valuing as "valuing with your feet."
been our experience in the application of We ask them, "In the direction of what
this therapy, however, that as avoidance valued end are your feet currently taking
repertoires subside, valued outcomes you?" Sometimes clients try to avoid
emerge. choosing a direction, and at those times
ACT therapists make several distinc- we point out that choosing not to choose
296 STEVEN C. HAYES & KELLY G. WILSON

is also a choice -in the very direct sense associated with that history need not be
of a selection among behavioral alter- changed first, the focus can be on the
natives. In such cases, the epitaph might client's chosen behavior change. A client
read "He dedicated his life to not choos- at this point in ACT therapy has lost so-
ing." cially sanctioned "reasonable" problem-
atic private events as causes for a failure
Letting Go of the Struggle/Embracing to follow through. Nor will any attempt
Symptoms be made to punish recalcitrant clients or
In the fifth component of ACT, we to trick them into keeping their com-
encourage clients to begin deliberately mitments. Rather, a verbal environment
experiencing thoughts, feelings, bodily has been created in therapy that allows
sensations, memories, or behavioral pre- no logical escape -the central issue is en-
dispositions that, if taken literally, must tirely what works rather than what is rea-
be avoided. There are many times when sonable even if it doesn't work. ACT tries
self-rules point to ineffective actions. to establish a discrimination between self-
When we encourage clients to give up the rules that cannot be followed effectively
struggle with experiential control, we are (i.e., rules of emotional avoidance) and
not asking them to "grin and bear it," or self-rules that can be followed effectively,
to "tough it out" until their symptoms and if followed will lead to positive con-
can be endured. Rather, we are asking sequences (e.g., commitments to behav-
the client to lean forward into the "symp- ior change). Promises usually work best
toms." We encourage them not only to when they are kept. This contingency is
stop struggling but to embrace the very direct and natural, rather than imposed
things that they most dread. This trans- by the therapist.
forms the functional meaning of these
events, without changing their form. The Therapeutic Relationship
When a private event no longer occasions therapeutic relationship is crucial
avoidance, it begins to lose its impor- to The ACT for several reasons. First, ther-
tance. The purpose of this component of apists cannot
ACT is to help the client make contact not model. Wereadily cannot
train what they do
ask clients to open
with the shaping impact of directly ex- themselves up to experiencing deliteral-
perienced contingencies present when the ized thoughts and feelings undefended
second-order agenda of avoiding certain without doing so as therapists. Our cli-
private events is absent. come to us with seemingly insur-
Many techniques are used in this pro- ents
cess, especially "willingness exercises." deal of pain.difficulties.
mountable
No one
They are in a great
open to the reality
Avoided private events are brought into of human misery can be in a therapy room
the therapy room (via imagery or exer- without often feeling sad or anxious. If,
cises) and disassembled into component from the opening moments of therapy,
pieces: bodily sensations, thoughts, be- the therapist can be present to the client's
havioral predispositions, memories, and burden and not balk or turn away, the
so on. In all cases, the goal is not to gain
control over them but to experience them client is already contacting the process
and purpose
without any attempt to modify or escape the part of the of ACT. This openness on
them. therapist establishes a
powerful and intimate therapeutic alli-
Commitment and Behavior Change ance.
Second, we consider that given the
The sixth goal of ACT is to aid the contingencies that established and main-
client in making commitments to action. tain emotional avoidance, careful atten-
All forms of psychopathology are asso- tion to the therapeutic relationship is
ciated with ineffective styles of living and necessary to the goals ofACT. Emotional
behaving. Now that one's history and the avoidance is, in many respects, a social
resulting elicited thoughts and feelings act. The historical context in which emo-
ACCEPTANCE AND COMMITMENT THERAPY 297

tional avoidance is established begins in It is written much as an ACT therapist


early childhood in the context of intense- might deliver it:
ly intimate and dependent interpersonal Suppose I had you hooked up to the best polygraph
relationships. Behaviorally speaking, machine that's ever been built. This is a perfect
treating behavior in the actual context in machine, the most sensitive ever made. When you
which it was learned and continues to are all wired up to it there is no way you can be
occur improves generalization to rele- aroused or anxious without the machine knowing
it. You have a very simple task here: All you have
vant nontherapy contexts. In a number to do is stay relaxed. If you get the least bit anxious,
of respects, an intimate therapeutic re- I will know it. But I know you want to try hard and
lationship may mirror the context in I want to give you an incentive to do so, so I also
which avoidance repertoires were estab- have a .44 magnum that I'll hold to your head. If
lished in the first place. Many of our cli- you just stay relaxed, I won't blow your brains out,
but if you get nervous (and I'll know it because
ents indicate difficulties in their inter- you're wired up to this perfect machine), I'm going
personal relationships, especially intimate to have to kill you. So, just relax! What do you
ones. Thus an intimate therapeutic re- think would happen? Guess what you'd get? Bamm!
lationship may bring to bear important The tiniest bit of anxiety would be terrifying. You'd
be going "Oh, my God! I'm getting anxious! Here
contextual variables that have been as- it comes!" BAMM! How could it work otherwise?
sociated with problematic functioning.
Finally, ACT attempts to disrupt prob- The purpose of this metaphor is to ex-
lematic verbal control in its paradoxical pose the client to the actual contingencies
and metaphorical attacks on the context operating when emotional control is cru-
of literality. If problematic verbal control cial. Metaphors are useful because they
is interfering with a client's probability are forms of verbal activity that are not
of making contact with important non- rule-like, and thus are unlikely to initiate
verbal contingencies, then an attack on pliance to please the therapist, but in-
this control, if successful, makes the cli- stead present pictures or stories that are
ent more susceptible to nonverbal con- more like a verbal approximation of ex-
tingencies in the therapeutic interaction. perienced contingencies. In the above
Thus, emotional avoidance on the part metaphor, almost everyone can imme-
of the therapist can be extremely prob- diately see, in a commonsense way, the
lematic because it would provide a dis- futility of the situation. The ACT ther-
torted and unhealthy learning experience apist may bring this out in high relief,
to a person newly opened up to direct, once the client has already contacted the
contingent influences on behavior. central point:
Now, you have the perfect polygraph machine al-
EXAMPLES OF SPECIFIC ACT ready hooked up to you: It's you own nervous sys-
TECHNIQUES tem. It is better than any machine humans have
ever made. You can't really feel something and not
The ACT protocol is a book-length have your nervous system in contact with it, almost
document (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, in by definition. And you've got something pointed at
press), comprising over a hundred spe- you that is more powerful and more threatening
than any gun-your own self-esteem, self-worth,
cific procedures organized around these and the workability of your life. So you actually are
key principles. Thus, it is not possible to in a situation very much like this. You're holding
describe ACT in detail here. What fol- the gun to your head and saying, "Relax!" So guess
lows, however, is a sample of a few spe- what you get?
cific metaphors, exercises, and other pro-
cedures. In each case we will describe Deliteralization
what an ACT therapist does or says, and The essence of deliteralization is the
will then attempt briefly to analyze this weakening of the response functions of
material behaviorally. verbal events that depend upon a strong
The Polygraph Metaphor relational network, and the weakening of
the transfer of psychological functions
The following is an example of a spe- through this relational network. If a lan-
cific metaphor that is used early in ACT. guage history is a method of training or
298 STEVEN C. HAYES & KELLY G. WILSON
(one might say) creating a mind, then de- through your mind were things about actual milk
literalization is a method of blowing a and your experience with milk. All that happened
was that we made a strange sound, and lots of these
mind or opening a mind. things showed up. Notice that there isn't any milk
No amount of deliteralization will in this room. None at all. Yet milk was in the room
eliminate derived relations, nor is this psychologically. You and I were seeing it, tasting
desirable. To make this point, ACT ther- it, feeling it-yet only the word was actually here.
apists challenge clients to try to hear what Now, here is the little exercise, if you're willing to
try it. The exercise is a little silly, and so you might
is being said in therapy in the same way feel a little embarrassed doing it, but I am going to
they would hear words spoken in an un- do the exercise with you so we can be silly together.
known foreign language. They can't. The What I am asking you to do is to say the word
same could be asked of our readers at "milk," out loud, rapidly, over and over again and
then notice what happens. Are you willing to try
this moment: Kindly read this sentence it?
we are now writing, but as you read each Client: I guess so.
word in this sentence we challenge you Therapist: OK. Let's do it. Say "milk" over and
to see each word in the same way that a over again.
person sees words in a totally foreign lan- [As the client does so the ACT therapist does too,
periodically interjecting things like: "As fast as you
guage. Read this sentence but have no can go until I tell you to stop. Faster! Keep going!
idea of what you are reading, or even that Faster!" or "Louder! Keep it up." Between these
there are words. This does not occur, and interjections, the therapist also is repeatedly and
no ACT client has yet met the challenge. loudly saying the word. This continues for at least
An inherent difference exists between two or three minutes.]
Therapist: OK, now stop. Did you notice what
"this is a sentence" and "ouwnm aoi polU happened to the psychological aspects of milk that
slhk." Once a relational repertoire is ac- were here a few minutes ago? What happened to
quired and applied to the conventional actual milk?
stimuli within a language community, Client: After about 40 times it disappeared. All
I could hearwas the sound. It sounded very strange-
these stimuli are-to a degree -mean- in fact, I had a funny feeling that I didn't even know
ingful by virtue of the arbitrarily appli- what words I was saying for a few moments. It
cable relations they sustain. sounded more like a bird making a sound than a
Deliteralization has the goal of weak- word.
ening-not eliminating-the relational Therapist: Right. The creamy, cold, gluggy stuff
just goes away. The first time you said it, it was as
response such that other response forms if milk were actually here, in the room. But all that
can coexist with verbal forms. The fol- really happened was that you said a word. The first
lowing exercise can begin to make the time you said it, it was really meaning-full, it was
point. It was first used by Titchener to almost solid. But when you said it again and again
and again, you began to lose that meaning and the
demonstrate his "context theory of words began to also be just a sound.
meaning." It is presented in transcript Client: I see that, but I don't quite see your point.
form to show how it is used in actual Therapist: Well, when you say things to yourself
sessions. in addition to any meaning behind those words isn't
it also true that these words are just words? The
The "Milk, Milk, Milk" Exercise words are just smoke. There isn't anything solid in
Therapist: Let's do a little exercise. I'm going to them. How is "milk" any different from "I'm bad"?
ask you to say a word. Then you tell me what comes
to mind. I want you to say the word, "Milk." Say What we are doing in ACT is training
it once. humans to weaken derived relations, and
Client: Milk.
Therapist: Good. Now what came to mind when to suspend the "sense-making" effort in
you said that? strategically important areas. This weak-
Client: I have milk at home in the refrigerator.
Therapist: OK. What else. What shows up when
ening is what we mean by deliteraliza-
we say "milk?" tion. Literality is an automatic process in
Client: I picture it. which the world has fimctions that emerge
Therapist: Good. What else? from derived stimulus relations. The
Client: I can taste it. Sort of. "milk, milk, milk" exercise creates a con-
Therapist: Exactly. And can you feel what it might text in which sense making is not appli-
feel like to drink a glass? Cold. Creamy. Coats your cable. After 50 or 100 times of saying
mouth. Goes "glug, glug" as you drink it. Right?
Client: Sure. something, the next time the word is said,
Therapist: OK, so let's see if this fits. What shot nothing is added by operating on the ba-
ACCEPTANCE AND COMMITMENT THERAPY 299

sis of a derived relation, and the direct Here is a language convention drawn
functions of the word (e.g., auditory func- from the ACT protocol that we call "Get-
tions) begin to dominate. This exercise ting Off Our Buts":
opens up a truth: In addition to any ver- There is another verbal convention I'd like us to
bal meaning a word may have, it is also adopt in here. This is one that we can use through-
just a sound. Just as there is no reason out our time together. It has to do with our use of
to avoid sounds, there is no necessary the word "but." This is a word that draws us into
reason to avoid even the most negative the struggle with our thoughts and feelings because
words, when they are considered to be ithavior is so commonly associated with explaining be-
on the basis of private events and then pit-
words. ting one set of private events against another. "But"
literally means that what follows the word contra-
Language Conventions dicts what went before the word. It originally came
from the words be out. When we use it we often say
Many language conventions are adopt- "this private event be out that private event. It's
ed in ACT. The purpose of these con- literally a call to fight, so it is no wonder it pulls us
ventions is to disentangle the client from up into the piece level-into the war zone. [Note
to readers: this phrase "piece level" is a reference
the conventional emotional control agen- to the chessboard metaphor described earlier.] Let's
da built into normal locution. A good consider some examples. Here is one: "I love my
example is the use of the word "but." husband, but I get so angry with him." Here is
This word commonly carries with it an another: "I want to go, but I am too anxious."
implicit statement about the organiza- Notice that although both say "this be out that,"
what the person actually experienced in both cases
tion of psychological events. Consider the was two things: this and that. The "be out" part
statement, "I want to go, but I am anx- isn't a description of what happened-it is a pro-
ious." This simple statement carries a scription about how private events should go to-
deep message about the role of feelings gether. This proscription, however, is exactly what
we are trying to back out of. No one experienced
in human action. Considered literally, the that two private events have to be resolved-in-
statement says that although wanting to stead two private events were experienced. If the
go would normally lead to going, anxiety word "but" is replaced by the word "and," it is
contradicts the effect of wanting to go. almost always much more honest. So in our ex-
Going cannot occur with anxiety. amples, it is much more honest and directly in con-
tact with what actually happened to say "I love my
The etymology of the word "but" re- husband and I get angry with him" or to say "I
veals this dynamic quite clearly. Accord- want to go and I am anxious." So the little con-
ing to the Oxford English Dictionary vention I'd like us to adopt is to say "and" instead
"but" is from the Old English "be-utan" of "but" when we talk. If you try it, you'll see that
almost always "and" is more true to your experi-
meaning "on the outside, without." In ence. "I want to go and I am anxious." Both things
Middle English this became "bouten" and are true, the wanting to go and the feeling of anxiety.
was gradually phonetically weakened to By calling attention to what we're saying with the
buten, bute, and thus "but." The Old En- use of this little convention, it will help make you
glish word "be-'utan" is a combination of more sensitive to one of the ways that people get
pulled into the piece-level struggle with themselves.
"be" (meaning something quite similar It will help us a lot to get off our buts in here. If
to the modem word "be") and "'utan" you really must say the word "but" at some point,
which is a form of "(ut"-an early form then at least we should say it in a way that empha-
of our modem word "out." Etymologi- sizes what we are actually doing. The original form
does this pretty well, so if we really have to say
cally, "but" means "Be out." It is a call "but" we will say it in here as "be out."
for whatever follows the word to "go
away" or else threaten whatever precedes This convention greatly opens up the
the word. In other words, "but" is a fight- psychological space within which clients
ing term. It says that two reactions that and therapists can work. "And" is a de-
do exist cannot coexist and still be as- scriptive rather than a proscriptive term,
sociated with effective action. One or the and thus can be associated with many
other must go. This is a conventional courses of action. All possibilities are
agenda that is directly contradictory to open. The motivated avoidance of pri-
the ACT perspective and more generally vate events that results from their con-
to a behavioral view of the role of private ventional connection to action can be re-
events in behavioral regulation. duced.
300 STEVEN C. HAYES & KELLY G. WILSON
Reactions as Barriers to accept is not what it says it is but what you
experience it to be. Now what did you actually ex-
Here is a lengthy edited excerpt from perience?
an ACT session transcript in which the Client: The fear that I'm getting confused and it
client raises barrier after barrier. Instead might not ever come back. I might not ever un-
derstand.
of these barriers being problems to ther- Therapist: is that OK?
apeutic work, the ACT therapist treats Client: The fear is OK, um. So right, um, when
them as the focus of therapy, as problems I blanking, when I blank out, I'm stuck behind the
being treated in the ACT work. This cli- words. I couldn't have told you that. There weren't
ent came in complaining of getting con- any thoughts there to describe.
Therapist: Isn't that the most amazingthing; that's
fused and anxious in work settings. He true. The most amazing thing is that when you look
was clinically depressed. The same con- at the world from words you don't actually see the
tent-confusion and anxiety-emerged words.
in therapy and was quite distressing there Client: Yeah, there weren't any. I was just con-
fused. I get into a place and my mind is just nothing,
as well. Note how the therapist repeat- zero.
edly undermines psychological avoid- Therapist: Go with that.
ance and turns the issue from the literal Client: And that anything we're talking about here
content of thoughts and feelings to the in the last hour is gone, it's not-
Therapist: Stay with that.
unwillingness of the client to experience Client: I can't remember anything.
the psychological content that is imme- Therapist: OK. Good!
diately present. Client: Yeah. The thought is that my mind is a
blank and I can't remember.
Client: It's hard to hang onto what we're going Therapist: OK "my mind is blank" um-any-
through. thing else your mind has to share?
Therapist: So, don't try to. Client: I'm confused.
Client: It's hard not to try to. (chuckle or sigh) Therapist: Go with that, go right this moment
Therapist: So, notice that you have the thought with that confusion.
that you want to try to. Client: I have a blank wall in front of me. Oh-
Client: OK. Oh, I had it a second ago. I can't remember what
Therapist: And is it OK to think that you want it is.
to try to hang onto it? That you need to hang on to Therapist: What was that? Isn't that a thought?
it? Is it OK to think that? Client: Yeah. I had the thought that I can't re-
Client: I would like to say it's OK, but it's really member what that is.
not. I feel like I should hold on to it. (sigh) Therapist: OK.
Therapist: OK, but now let'sjust think that. We've Client: (19-s pause) I feel like I don't even know
got this thing "I got to hang on to this." Is it OK what we're talking about right now.
to think "I've got to hang on to this?" Therapist: OK, good. Is that OK-to feel that?
Client: Sure (sigh and chuckle). No-I guess I'm Client: I need to keep my mind working.
afraid that I won't get it back if I can't hang on to Therapist: OK. So you're now having a thought
something. that you have to keep your mind working.
Therapist: OK, so you have the thought that it Client: (8-s pause) I'm thinking I should try and
won't come back ... Is it OK to have those words, get back into the groove of where we were. I'm
"it won't come back?" confused.
Client: If it didn't come back that wouldn't be Therapist: As you think that, can you let that be
OK. a thought-not a thing that you are lookingfrom,
Therapist: But you didn't experience that it didn't but as a thing you're looking at. Just watch what
come back, right? comes up. (12-s pause)
Client: Right, just the fear. [Client starts to say something and then shakes his
Therapist: The fear, right. head vigorously as if to "shake it off"]
Client: Uh huh. Therapist: And at that moment, just when you
Therapist: And you also experienced some words grab it, what are you shaking of'?
in your head called "But it wouldn't be OK if it Client: I'm going backwards.
didn't come back." Therapist: You're going backwards. I'm slipping!
Client: Right. Client: And, I'm-struggling extra hard.
Therapist: Is it OK to experience the fact that Therapist: Great thought.
you have the words called, "but it wouldn't be OK Client: Because I am going backwards. All I can
if it didn't come back." think about is "I've got to stop it."
Client: Sure, it's . . . it's OK to have that feeling. Therapist: OK, and as you do that, let that be
Therapist: Great. Next thought. what it is. This is the thought that pushes you around,
Client: But what if it doesn't come back? (giggle) and not just in here.
Same thing? Client: I just thought I had to stop it. I have
Therapist: That's the next thought. What's here decided it is hopeless, I think.
ACCEPTANCE AND COMMITMENT THERAPY 301
Therapist: Great! It is hopeless. In a funny kind
vorable to ACT but not significantly so
of way. This is a healthy kind of hopeless. That's
(Zettle & Raines, 1989). ACT has been
where we started, remember?
used to treat the emotional distress of fam-
Client: Oh yeah. I remember. Yes, I do. The first
session. ilies with severely physically handicapped
children-a situation in which removal of
Therapist: Yeah, and that wasn't a trick. That's
real; it is hopeless, in a healthy way. So, let's see
the stressor is not possible (Biglan, 1990).
what is hopeless. What are we trying to do here?
ACT has also been found to be effective
Client: Just to look at my thoughts.
with several different anxiety disorders on
Therapist: Right, and all we are really trying to
do is just be here with whichever ones come upstandard anxiety measures (Hayes, 1987;
Hayes, Afari, McCurry, & Wilson, 1990)
without struggling with them. Whatever shows up.
No particular thing has to show up. Notice howin studies using either single-case analyses
hard that was. Each one kept inviting you to struggle
and run away. or group designs with pre- and posttreat-
Client: Right. ment measures.
Process research on the change se-
This client was successfully treated by quence with ACT
ACT. The core of the work was an aban- that are very much clients shows results
in line with theoret-
donment of this obsessive effort to get ical expectations. Compared to clients in
clear or to remember -an effort that itself cognitive therapy, ACT clients being
created confusion and distress. Ironical- treated for depression show a somewhat
ly, when we accept a feeling of confusion, slower drop in the frequency of self-re-
clarity emerges because it is very clear ported depressive thoughts but show a
that we are confused. Clarity, in other much more rapid drop in the believability
words, comes from being present with of those thoughts (Zettle & Hayes, 1986).
what is already present, and in this client's This makes sense, because ACT does not
case what was present was a string of focus at all on changing the form or fre-
thoughts about how he needed to be quency of private events supposedly re-
somewhere else. His task in the ACT work lated to poorer life functioning. The focus
was to learn to get present to these instead is entirely on changes in thefunc-
thoughts as thoughts and to do nothing tion of these events. Taking a thought
with them other than to notice them. literally in a technical sense (as described
OUTCOME AND PROCESS earlier) is quite similar to what is dis-
DATA ON ACT cussed in lay language as believing a
thought. Being able to have a thought
The impact of ACT has been studied without necessarily having to adopt it as
with several populations, but the outcome a basis for action -without believing it-
data are still limited. In a randomized clin- is a core goal ofACT. Similar differences
ical trial, ACT was shown to be more ef- in process between ACT and cognitive
fective than Beck's cognitive therapy in the therapy have been shown in other studies
treatment of depression (Zettle, 1984), as (Zettle, 1984; Zettle & Raines, 1989). Also
measured by standard self-report measures in line with ACT theory, beneficial out-
of depression, when presented in a 12-week comes have been shown to be related to
course of individual therapy. This was an reductions in emotional avoidance (Kho-
extraordinary finding in its own way, be- rakiwala, 199 1; McCurry, 199 1). In these
cause a major focus of the ACT protocol process studies, transcripts of ACT ses-
was to open up the client to feeling de- sions conducted with successful ACT cli-
pressed feelings without defense, while at ents were generated. Actual behavioral
the same time doing what needed to be measures ofemotional willingness in ses-
done overtly. Paradoxically, opening up to sion were developed and were reliably
depressed feelings reduced their intensity applied to client verbalizations presented
and impact. When conducted in a group simultaneously both in audiotape and
format, a controlled investigation showed transcript form. Emotional willingness
ACT to be essentially equivalent to cog- increased over time, both from the be-
nitive therapy on standard depression ginning of sessions to the end of sessions
measures-several of the means were fa- and from the beginning of therapy to the
302 STEVEN C. HAYES & KELLY G. WILSON
end of therapy (Khorakiwala, 1991). The domain (e.g., Khorakiwala, 1991; Mc-
expansion of the ability to experience pri- Curry, 1991), and finally an intervention
vate events without avoidance was as- technology (e.g., Kohlenberg, Hayes, &
sociated with other key ACT concepts, Tsai, 1993) and preliminary tests of its
such as the ability to make and keep com- efficacy.
mitments. In other words, we argue that much of
The empirical analysis ofACT is in its the current support for the ACT ap-
infancy, in part because the treatment de- proach can be found in the foundational
velopment strategy we have followed is work that underlies the technique. Fol-
unusual and lengthy. We have conscious- lowing an inductive, technique-building
ly built ACT in a six-step process in- strategy, we are only now to the point at
volving philosophy, methodology, basic which larger scale outcome studies seem
theory and research, applied theory and to be worthwhile. We have recently re-
research, assessment technology, and in- ceived a grant from the National Institute
tervention technology. on Drug Abuse to launch such an effort.
In the usual empirical approach to ap- Whether ACT will be more helpful than
plied intervention techniques, a specific other approaches is not presently clear,
applied problem is defined and a prelim- but it does seem clear that ACT can have
inary approach to its amelioration is de- at least some beneficial impact and that
veloped based on existing techniques and it may act according to different pro-
data. This approach is tested and refined cesses than other forms of psychothera-
and then compared to other approaches py-
for the same problem. In the process, ad-
ditions are made to the intervention and
it becomes a treatment package. Group- CONCLUSION
comparison treatment-outcome studies Behavior analysis has had a very lim-
are then conducted. If it works, a series ited impact on adult outpatient psycho-
of package-dismantling studies are con- therapy work, compared to its dramatic
ducted to determine the essential com- impact on work with children or insti-
ponents. Finally, possible theoretical tutionalized populations. This is largely
mechanisms are examined to determine because the core of adult outpatient ther-
the means through which clinical impact apy is work on the role of human verbal
occurs. behavior. Behavior therapists have large-
Our approach has been quite different. ly "gone cognitive" because of this core,
We have attempted to clarify and extend but they have adopted a mechanistic and
our philosophical assumptions so that mentalistic model in part because no oth-
consistent units of analysis and truth cri- er approaches have presented them-
teria tied together our basic and applied selves. Contemporary behavior-analytic
work. Our work on functional contex- views of verbal behavior provide a pow-
tualism was the result (e.g., Hayes, Hayes, erful alternative. In the examples above,
& Reese, 1988; Hayes, Hayes, Reese, & ACT therapists at times use mentalistic
Sarbin, 1993). We have tried to develop and dualistic terms (e.g., "mind") to
an inductive and intensive approach to communicate most readily with clients,
treatment development methodology but at every point the underlying ratio-
(e.g., Barlow, Hayes, & Nelson, 1984). nale and theory is both nonmentalistic
We have developed a program of basic and contextualistic. Behavior-analytic
behavioral research on how verbal rela- psychotherapies provide a new domain
tions change the way contingencies guide within which to explore the applied im-
behavior (e.g., Hayes, 1989; Hayes & pact of contemporary behavioral theory.
Hayes, 1992). It is in that context that
we have developed the beginnings of a
theory of psychopathology (e.g., Hayes & REFERENCES
Wilson, 1993), assessment devices need- Barlow, D. H., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. 0. (1984).
ed to assess that theory in the applied The scientist-practitioner: Research and account-
ACCEPTANCE AND COMMITMENT THERAPY 303
ability in clinical and educational settings. New prehensive distancing, paradox, and the treat-
York: Pergamon. ment ofemotional avoidance. In M. Ascher (Ed.),
Biglan, A. (1990). A contextual approach to treat- Paradoxical procedures inpsychotherapy(pp. 184-
ing family distress. In G. Singer & L. Irvin (Eds.), 218). New York: Guilford.
Supporting the family: Enabling a positive ad- Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. D. (in
justment to children with disabilities (pp. 299- press). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy.
31 1). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. New York: Guilford.
Hayes, S. C. (1984). Making sense of spirituality. Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1993). Some ap-
Behaviorism, 12, 99-1 10. plied implications of a contemporary behavior-
Hayes, S. C. (1987). A contextual approach to analytic account of verbal events. The Behavior
therapeutic change. In N. S. Jacobson (Ed.), Psy- Analyst, 16, 283-301.
chotherapists in clinical practice: Cognitive and Khorakiwala, D. (1991). An analysis ofthe process
behavioralperspectives (pp. 327-387). New York: of client change in a contextual approach to ther-
Guilford. apy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer-
Hayes, S. C. (Ed.). (1989). Rule-governed behav- sity of Nevada, Reno.
ior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J.
control. New York: Plenum. (1991). The transfer of contextual control over
Hayes, S. C., Afari, N., McCurry, S. M., & Wilson, equivalence classes through equivalence classes:
K. (1990, May). The efficacy of comprehensive A possible model of social stereotyping. Journal
distancingin the treatment ofagoraphobia. Poster ofthe ExperimentalAnalysis ofBehavior, 56,505-
presented at the 16th annual convention of the 518.
Association for Behavior Analysis, Nashville, TN. Kohlenberg, R. J., Hayes, S. C., & Tsai, M. (1993).
Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A. J., Devany, J. M., Radical behavioral psychotherapy: Two contem-
Kohlenberg,B.S.,&Shelby,J. (1987). Stimulus porary examples. Clinical PsychologyReview, 13,
equivalence and the symbolic control of behav- 579-592.
ior. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 13, Kohlenberg, R. J., Tsai, M., & Dougher, M. J.
361-374. (1993). The dimensions of clinical behavior
Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1989). The verbal analysis. TheBehaviorAnalyst, 16, 271-282.
action of the listener as a basis for rule-gover- McCurry, S. M. (1991). Client metaphor use in a
nance. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed be- contextualform oftherapy. Unpublished doctoral
havior: Cognition, contingencies, and instruction- dissertation, University of Nevada, Reno.
al control (pp. 153-190). New York: Plenum. McCurry, S. M., & Hayes, S. C. (1993). Metaphor:
Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1992). Verbal rela- A basic and applied analysis. Clinical Psychology
tions and the evolution of behavior analysis. Review, 12, 763-785.
American Psychologist, 47, 1383-1395. Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional
Hayes, S. C., Hayes, L. J., & Reese, H. W. (1988). discrimination versus matching to sample: An
Finding the philosophical core: A review of Ste- expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the
phen C. Pepper's World hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5-22.
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 97- Wulfert, E., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). The transfer
111. of conditional sequencing through conditional
Hayes, S. C., Hayes, L. J., Reese, H. W., & Sarbin, equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental
T. R. (Eds.). (1993). Varieties ofscientific con- Analysis of Behavior, 50, 125-144.
textualism. Reno, NV: Context Press. Zettle, R. D. (1984). Cognitive therapy of depres-
Hayes, S. C., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Hayes, L. J. sion: A conceptual and empirical analysis ofcom-
(1991). Transfer of consequential functions ponent and process issues. Unpublished doctoral
through simple and conditional equivalence dissertation, University of North Carolina at
classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Greensboro.
Behavior, 56, 119-137. Zettle, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1986). Dysfunc-
Hayes, S. C., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Melancon, S. M. tional control by client verbal behavior: The con-
(1989). Avoiding and altering rule control as a text of reason giving. The Analysis of Verbal Be-
strategy of clinical treatment. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), havior, 4, 30-38.
Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingen- Zettle, R. D., & Raines, J. C. (1989). Group cog-
cies, and instructional control(pp. 359-385). New nitive and contextual therapies in treatment of
York: Plenum. depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45,
Hayes, S. C., & Melancon, S. M. (1989). Com- 438-445.

You might also like