Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Teaching Children with Autism to

Prefer Books or Toys Over


Stereotypy or Passivity

Robin Nuzzolo-Gomez Abstract: Two experiments were conducted with 4 students with autism to test the relationship
between either toys or books as conditioned reinforcers for observing or playing and their effect
Mandy A. Leonard
on stereotypy and passivity. Experiment 1 consisted of a single preschool student who emitted
Eyleen Ortiz
and frequent intervals of passive behavior and infrequent intervals of looking at books in a free play
Celestina M. Rivera setting. After systematic training sessions involving pairings of reinforcers with looking at
The David Gregory School books, he engaged in looking at books significantly more than in his baseline in free play and
decreased intervals of passivity. Experiment 2 involved a multiple baseline across 3 students.
R. Douglas Greer Baseline data were followed by toy-play conditioning sessions run concurrently with free-play
Columbia University observations. The 2 students who emitted frequent rates of stereotypy in baseline had signifi-
cantly fewer intervals of stereotypy after toys were conditioned as reinforcers and toy play
increased for all 3 students.

Stereotypy is a prevalent behavior of children with autism. chik, 1979), spaced responding differential reinforcement
Greer, Becker, Saxe, and Mirabella (1985) defined stereo- of low rates (Singh, Dawson, & Manning, 1981), and teach-
typy as cycles of repetitive movements that have no appar- ing children functionally equivalent communication (Du-
ent consequences for the individual who is emitting the rand & Carr, 1987). Two experiments utilizing positive
responses beyond the movement itself. Some children with treatment approaches showed that high rates of stereotyp-
autism engage in abnormally frequent rates of stereotypy ical or passive behaviors in children or young adults with
(Baumeister & Forehand, 1972). Research has been con- autism or pervasive developmental disorder could be re-
ducted on stereotypical behavior and its interference with duced when the participants were taught to play (Eason,
more adaptive behaviors such as toy play
(Koegel, Fire- White, & Newsom, 1982; Greer et al., 1985). Eason et al.
stone, Kramme, & Dunlap, 1974) and academic instruc- taught children to play with toys, and the instruction re-
tion (Koegel & Covert, 1972). Numerous treatments have sulted in increases in toy play and decreases in stereotypy.
been utilized to decrease stereotypical behaviors and most Their data showed that learning to play with the toy re-
of these have been punishment procedures, including sulted in decreases in stereotypy when the children had the
physical restraint (Marholin & Townsend, 1978), overcor- toys and were reinforced with extrinsic reinforcers for toy
rection (Foxx & Azrin, 1973), and even the use of electric play. Greer et al. (1985) sought to determine whether the
shock (Baumeister & Forehand; Lovaas, Schaeffer, & Sim- toys or play objects could reinforce play that replaced
mons, 1965). Punishment procedures have been effective stereotypy when teachers were not present. In one experi-
in decreasing stereotypy but often have limited mainte- ment they found that students whohad never engaged in
nance of initial treatment effects.
stereotypy would do when their preferred toy was re-
so
Positive approaches, although less prevalent in the moved, suggesting that the function of stereotypy was play.
corpus of literature, have also been successfully imple- In a second study, they found that conditioning the rein-
mented to decrease stereotypical behavior in children with forcement effects of playing with a ball resulted in the re-
autism. These positive treatment approaches have included duction of stereotypy and physical assaults by young adults
differential reinforcement of other behavior (Harris & Wol- who had been institutionalized for many years. Moreover,

80
Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at NORTH CAROLINA STATE UNIV on May 15, 2015
81

after 6 months of not having access to the ball, reintroduc- disorder. He was toilet trained, ambulatory, and able to
tion of the ball immediately reduced stereotypy for several perform activities of daily living with partial physical assis-
sessions. Together, these studies suggested that stereotypy tance. He followed one-step vocal commands and could
was a form of play and that instruction that conditioned vocally mand and tact single words. He was chosen from a
play objects as reinforcement was a valid and nonpunitive group of seven students because systematic observations
approach to the treatment of stereotypy or passivity in showed that he emitted frequent intervals of passivity and
free-play settings. few intervals of looking at books in a free-play setting. The
In other laboratory studies of both children and participant also imitated movements modeled by the ex-
adults, researchers found that durable preference could be perimenter, which was a characteristic he needed to have
developed for previously nonpreferred stimuli by pairing in order to teach him to look at books.
conditioned reinforcers (praise or tokens) with nonpre-
ferred stimuli until the nonpreferred stimuli become the SETTING
preferred stimuli, as measured by moment-to-moment re-
inforcement control (Greer, 1981; Greer, Dorow, & Randall, The observation and training took place in a self-
1975). These data showed that new choices could be condi- contained special education classroom in a publicly
tioned. Greer et al. (1985) showed that newly conditioned funded, private day school for preschool children with
reinforcers could replace stereotypy and, consequently, ob- autism. The classroom had a one-to-one student to staff
serving or playing behavior could displace the formerly member ratio. There were six students and six staff mem-
preferred stereotypical behavior. bers present in the classroom when the participant was ob-
The following two experiments were designed to de- served and conditioned to look at books. The books that
termine whether the findings of Greer et al. could be ex- were available were constant in type and number through-
tended to preschool children with autism. In the following out the entire experiment. The conditioning of books was
experiments, toys and books were conditioned as rein- done at a large table in the back of the classroom. All other
forcers through training sessions in which prosthetic rein- data were taken in a play area referred to as the free-play
forcement was paired with the toys or books until the setting. The free-play setting was a 10 by 5 foot area in the
students chose to use the conditioned items. We then ob- back of the student’s classroom in which there was a single
served whether the conditioning procedure affected the toy box containing toys and a bookshelf containing ap-
free-play choices of stereotypy, passivity, or play. Can stu- proximately 100 books written on a kindergarten level.
dents’ choices be conditioned so that newly conditioned
reinforcers lead to more appropriate independent play that DEPENDENT VARIABLES
replaces aberrant behavior? Experiment 1 investigated the
effect of conditioning looking at books and the choice of Two categories were observed: (a) looking at books and
passivity or looking at books in a free-play setting. Experi- (b) passivity. Looking at books was defined as exhibiting be-
ment 2 sought to determine whether students who showed haviors such as touching and making eye contact with a
little or no toy play in a free-play setting would replace book. Passivity was defined as not looking at, touching, or
stereotypy with toys when the toys were conditioned as re- searching for a book, or sitting and not responding to toys
inforcers for play in training sessions. Would the stimulus or books. If both of the two response categories occurred
control of playing with toys or looking at books replace the during the interval, both were recorded for the interval.
stimulus control of the effects of stereotypy or passivity in
free-play settings as a function of training sessions devoted DATA COLLECTION
to conditioning the new stimuli as reinforcers?
Data were collected during 5-minute observation sessions
in the free-play setting. A total of 60 intervals were con-
EXPERIMENT 1 ducted in each 5-minute session. Observers who directly
collected the data were cued in 5-second intervals with a
Method staff member’s vocal command, &dquo;Record.&dquo; A staff member
PARTICIPANT would set a digital timer for 5 minutes and at the end of
each 5-second interval would give the vocal command,
One student (Student A) participated in the experiment. &dquo;Record.&dquo; This vocal command served as the prompt for
Student A was a 3-year-old boy diagnosed with autism. He the observers to record the data by hand on grid sheets
was diagnosed by a medical doctor using the Childhood with columns for the two dependent variables-looking at
Autism Rating Scale (Schopler, Reichler, & Renner, 1988), books and passivity. There were 60 consecutive 5-second
on which he achieved an overall score that placed him in intervals. Partial interval recording was used, and intervals
the moderate to severe range for an autism spectrum could contain more than one response class. For example,

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at NORTH CAROLINA STATE UNIV on May 15, 2015
82

if theparticipant looked at a book for 2 seconds and en- EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN


gaged in passive behavior for the following 3 seconds, both
response classes were recorded for that interval. The con-
An ABCA design and two follow-up probes were used to
compare the effects of books versus no books on the stu-
ditioning of books was then started and data were taken in dent’s passivity. Phase A represents observation of the
the same manner. The experimenter ran one session of
book conditioning, waited at least 30 minutes, and then free-play setting with books available. Phase B represents
placed the child in the free-play setting. Data were taken in post-book conditioning with books present. Phase C rep-
resents post-book conditioning without books present.
this format for each session of treatment.
The final phase represents a reversal to baseline conditions
with books present. The dependent variables were looking
INTEROBSERVER AGREEMENT at books and passivity. The independent variable was a

The percentage of interobserver agreement was calculated book conditioning procedure.


for both of the dependent variables by dividing the num-
ber of interval agreements by the total number of interval Results
agreements and disagreements and multiplying this num- During baseline conditions, the student looked at books
ber by 100. A second observer conducted independent for a range of 0 to 25 intervals with a mean of 14.5 and was
agreement observations for 38% of the total sessions. The passive for a range of 21 to 57 intervals with a mean of
mean agreement was 97% with a range of 90% to 100%.
38.8. In Phase B, post-book conditioning, the student
showed an immediate increase in looking at books and a
BOOK CONDITIONING PROCEDURES significant decrease in passivity. The student looked at
books for a range of 39 to 57 intervals with a mean of 55.9
The student was conditioned to look at books using a set and was passive for a range of 2 to 21 intervals with a mean
of scripted procedures that were part of a program de- of 10.1. To test for a functional relationship, in Phase C,
signed to teach toy conditioning (Greer et al., 1985). Each books were removed from the free-play setting and only
session consisted of 20 learn units (Greer & McDonough,
passivity was measured. The overall trend was ascending
1999), which were scored in terms of whether the student with a range of 10 to 54 and a mean of 38 intervals of pas-
self-initiated, independently looked at the book following
the experimenter antecedent, &dquo;You do it,&dquo; or required phys-
sivity. A reversal to baseline condition was then conducted.
In the reversal phase the looking at books data ranged
ical prompting to look at the book. To begin each learn from 33 to 59 intervals present with a mean of 50.8. Two
unit, if necessary, the experimenter prompted the student
to look toward the book. A book was then placed in front
follow-up probes were conducted 30 days and 90 days after
the experiment, and resulting data showed that the books
of the child, and he was given 5 seconds to initiate looking maintained stimulus control as a function of the book
at the book. If he did, vocal praise and edible reinforce-
ment in the form of pretzels and crackers were delivered
conditioning with the student looking at books in 58 and
60 intervals (see Figure 1).
and a plus was recorded on the data sheet for self-
initiation. If he did not respond, the experimenter mod-
eled turning the pages and looking at the book. The book Discussion
was then placed back in front of the student and he was The data showed a functional relationship be-
resulting
told, &dquo;You do it.&dquo; If he then looked at the book, vocal praise tween the book conditioning procedure and the student’s
and edible reinforcement were delivered and a plus was increase in looking at books and decrease in passivity. Data
recorded on the data sheet for independently looking at showed that books acquired stimulus control as a function
books. If he did not look at the book, a physical prompt of the book conditioning procedures. When books were
was used, a vocal correction of, &dquo;This is looking at the removed from the environment, the student remained
book&dquo; was delivered, and a minus was recorded on the data passive for the majority of the sessions. It can also be
sheet for independently looking at books. Training ses- stated that the absence of books resulted in the student’s
sions were continued throughout the entire conditioning passivity.
phase. Separate free-play sessions were run concurrently
but were separated from the training sessions by at least
30 minutes. Concurrent free-play sessions were conducted EXPERIMENT 2
following the exact procedures applied in baseline sessions. Method
No vocal praise and edible reinforcement were delivered to
the student in the free-play setting for all conditions. Data PARTICIPANTS
from the book conditioning procedure are not present for
brevity’s sake, but criterion for the student was at least 90% Three students (Students A, B, and C) participated in the
self-initiated looking at books for two consecutive sessions. experiment. All three were diagnosed by a medical doctor
Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at NORTH CAROLINA STATE UNIV on May 15, 2015
83

Figure 1. intervals of looking at books and passivity in the free-play setting.

using the Childhood Autism Rating Scale, on which they SETTING


achieved an overall score that placed them in the moderate
to severe range for an autism spectrum disorder. Student A The observation and training took place in a self-
was a 4-year-old boy. He was toilet trained, ambulatory,
contained special education classroom in a publicly
and able to perform activities of daily living with minimal funded, private day school. The classroom had a one-to-
one student to staff member ratio. There were five students
assistance. He followed multistep vocal commands and
could vocally mand and tact using multiple autoclitics. and seven staff members present in the classroom when
Student B was a 7-year-old girl. She was toilet trained and the participants were observed and conditioned to play
with toys. The toys available were blocks, balls, puzzles,
ambulatory but unable to perform activities of daily living musical instruments, cars, trucks, a toy garage, dolls, doll-
without partial physical assistance. She followed one-step
commands and could vocally mand and tact using multi- houses, and stuffed animals. The conditioning of toys was
done at a large table in the back of the classroom. All other
ple autoclitics. Student C was a 7-year-old boy. He was toi-
let trained, ambulatory, and able to perform activities of data were taken in a toy area referred to as the free-play
daily living with minimal assistance. He followed multistep setting.
vocal commands and could vocally mand and tact using
multiple autoclitics. They were chosen from a group of RESPONSE CLASSES
eight students because systematic observations showed
that they emitted the highest rates of stereotypy and the Two categories of responses were observed: (a) toy play
lowest rates of toy play in a free-play setting. All partici- and (b) stereotypy. Toy play was defined as exhibiting be-
pants were able to imitate 10 movements modeled by the haviors such as touching the toy or using it in the manner
experimenter, which was a characteristic they needed to for which it was designed with no end other than the ma-
have in order to teach them to play with toys. The toys were nipulation of the toy itself. Stereotypy was defined as hand
used to be conditioned for stimulus control. flapping or clapping, finger-flicking, making nonsensical
Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at NORTH CAROLINA STATE UNIV on May 15, 2015
84

vocal noises, object mouthing, and rocking. A toy-play re- necessary, the experimenter prompted the student to look
sponse could not involve the student concurrently emit- at the toy. The toy was then placed in front of the child and
ting any stereotypical behavior. If the student utilized the he or she was given 5 seconds to initiate toy play. If he or
toy in a stereotypical manner, this was recorded as an oc- she did so, vocal praise and edible reinforcement in the
currence of stereotypy for that interval. If both of the two form of candy, pretzels, or popcorn were delivered and a
response categories occurred separately during that inter- plus was recorded for self-initiation on the data sheet. If
val, both behaviors were recorded for the interval. the child did not respond, the experimenter then modeled
playing with the toy. The toy was then placed back in front
DATA COLLECTION of the student and he or she was told, &dquo;You do it.&dquo; If the
child then played with the toy, vocal praise and edible re-
Data were collected during 5-minute observation sessions. inforcement were delivered and a plus was recorded on the
A total of 60 intervals were conducted in each 5-minute data sheet for independently playing with the toy. If he or
session. Observers who directly recorded the data were she did not play with the toy, a physical prompt was used,
cued in 5-second intervals with a staff member’s vocal a vocal correction of, &dquo;This is playing with the toy&dquo; was de-

command, &dquo;Record.&dquo; A staff member set a digital timer for livered, and a minus was recorded on the data sheet for in-
5 minutes and at the end of each 5-second interval gave the dependently playing with the toy. Training sessions were
vocal command, &dquo;Record.&dquo; This vocal command served as continued throughout the entire conditioning phase. Sep-
a prompt for the observers to record the data
by hand on arate free-play sessions were run concurrently but were
grid sheets of the two response classes-toy play and separated from the training sessions by at least 30 minutes.
stereotypy. There were 60 consecutive 5-second intervals. Concurrent free-play sessions were conducted following
Partial interval recording was used, and intervals could the exact procedures applied in baseline sessions. No pros-
contain more than one response class. For example, if the thetic consequences were delivered to the student in the
participant played with a toy for 2 seconds and engaged in free-play setting for all conditions. Data from the toy con-
stereotypy for the following 3 seconds, both responses ditioning procedure are not present for brevity’s sake, but
were recorded for that interval. The conditioning of toys criterion for the student was at least 90% self-initiated toy
was then started and data were taken in the same manner. play for two consecutive sessions.
The experimenter ran one session of toy conditioning,
waited at least 30 minutes, and then placed the child in the EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN

free-play setting. Data were taken in this format for each


session of treatment. The experimental design used was a multiple baseline across
participants (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 1987). Phase A
INTEROBSERVER AGREEMENT represents observation of the free-play setting. Phase B
represents the onset of conditioning toy play and the free-
The percentage of interobserver agreement was calculated play observations continued. The dependent variables were
for both of the dependent variables by dividing the num- toy play and stereotypy. The independent variable was a
ber of interval agreements by the total number of interval toy-play conditioning procedure.
agreements and disagreements and multiplying this num-
ber by 100. A behavior analyst supervisor conducted
Results
agreement observations for all three participants. The per-
centage of agreement was calculated for 46% of Student A’s Baseline data showed that Student A had a range of 0 to 50
sessions, with the mean agreement being 100%; 39% of intervals with toy play present, with a mean of 20. His
Student B’s sessions, with the mean agreement being stereotypy ranged from 0 to 60 intervals present, with a
100%; and 37% of Student C’s sessions, with the mean mean of 32.6. During the toy-conditioning phase there was

agreement being 100%. an ascending trend, and


toy play continued at high rates
with only one session of overlap at Session 14. The range
TOY PLAY CONDITIONING PROCEDURES of toy play in the conditioning phase was 10 to 60 intervals
present with a mean of 36.6. The range of stereotypy in the
The students were conditioned to play with toys using a set conditioning phase was 0 to 21 with a mean of 6.7.
of scripted procedures that were part of a program de- Student B had a range of 0 to 42 baseline intervals with
signed to teach toy play (Greer et al., 1985). Each session toy play present and a mean of 15.8. Her stereotypy
consisted of 20 learn units (Greer & McDonough, 1999), ranged from 1 to 43, with a mean of 13.4. During the toy-
which were scored in terms of whether the student self- conditioning phase there was an immediate increase in toy
initiated, independently played with the toy following the play with the data remaining at high steady rates through-
experimenter antecedent, &dquo;You do it,&dquo; or required physical out the remainder of the experiment. The range of toy play
prompting to play with the toy. To begin each learn unit, if in the conditioning phase was 29 to 58 intervals present,
Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at NORTH CAROLINA STATE UNIV on May 15, 2015
485

with a mean of 41.5. The range of stereotypy in the condi- behaviors with more appropriate activities, such as playing
tioning phase was 0 to 13, with a mean of 6.8. with toys and looking at books. However, further research
Student C had a range of 0 to 41 baseline intervals on reinforcement conditioning should be conducted.
with toy play present, only one session over 12 intervals,
with a mean of 7.3. His stereotypy ranged from 34 to 59, GENERAL DISCUSSION
with a mean of 41.1. During the toy-conditioning phase
there was a sharp increase in toy play, with the data re- One of the trends in behavior analysis and special educa-
maining at high steady rates throughout the remainder of tion over the last few years has been giving students choices
the experiment. The range of toy play in the conditioning in daily activities. Certainly this is important, but we as
phase was 0 to 56 intervals present, only one session under educators are concerned with the expansion of choices. For
35 intervals, with a mean of 37. The range of stereotypy in example, it is not enough to teach children to read; we
the conditioning phase was 0 to 30, with only one session want to teach so that children choose to read in their free
higher than 11 intervals present, with a mean of 12.7 (see time. In effect, we argue that this goal can be achieved by
Figure 2). expanding the student’s community of reinforcers because
All three participants’ data showed an increase in toy the availability of conditioned reinforcers strongly affects
play and a decrease in stereotypy after the conditioning the student’s choice. Thus, research that expands choice
procedure. is critical.
For students like those studied in these experiments
and those of Greer et al. (1985), it is not necessary to use
Discussion
punitive procedures or treat stereotypy directly. Punitive
The data from this experiment showed that the toys ac- procedures temporarily suppress stereotypy; we need pro-
quired stimulus control for play behavior as a function of cedures that replace the function of stereotypy for individ-
the conditioning procedure. The toys functioned as rein- uals. Parents and teachers of children with autism need
forcers for the children, and over time they sought out the positive tools to deal with how children with autism spend
toys in the free-play setting, rather than engaging in stereo- their free time. Many individuals with autism engage in
typical behavior. The data demonstrated that stereotypical stereotypy or passivity in their free time. One of the prob-
behaviors have a play function and can be replaced with lems of designing treatments for stereotypy is that the
play behavior. Student A played with toys in only three of function of the behavior in free time has not been sub-
the five baseline sessions. After the toy-play conditioning jected to analysis (Greer et al., 1987). However, when chil-
sessions began, toy play increased significantly, with all but dren engage in stereotypy during free time, the behavioral
one session having higher intervals of toy play than stereo- effects themselves are controlling the behavior. Most if not
typy. Stereotypy remained at low, stable intervals through- all individuals engage in brief episodes of stereotypy, par-
out the conditioning phase. ticularly when we are waiting and have nothing interesting
Student B had three out of nine baseline sessions that in which to engage ourselves. These episodes are usually
showed high rates of toy play, but all other sessions had less brief because we can entertain ourselves by engaging in
than 11 intervals of toy play present. After the toy-play various activities such as reading, playing solitaire, doing
conditioning sessions began, toy play remained at high and crossword puzzles, or working on our laptops. Perhaps we
stable rates throughout the remainder of the experiment. engage in stereotypy when our community of reinforcers is
Stereotypy also remained at low, stable rates throughout not readily available to break up our continual behavior.
the remainder of the experiment. We speculate that stereotypy occurs as a function of a
Student C showed the least amount of toy play for all paucity of preferred activities that is a result of a lack of re-
participants, with only 1 of 10 baseline sessions with more inforcement conditioning.
than 11 intervals of toy play present. After Session 1 of the The data in these experiments and in those of Greer
toy-play conditioning phase, Student C showed steady, et al. (1985) and Eason et al. (1982) suggest that this is a
high rates of toy play throughout the remainder of the ex- tenable theory. If this were proven to be the case, it would
periment. be of critical importance to the educational curricula of
The resulting data showed that the conditioning of toy children with autism. That is, one of the goals for educa-
play resulted in a decrease in stereotypy for all three stu- tors of individuals with stereotypy should be to provide
dents. This suggests that the consequences of toy play had systematic and direct instruction that involves the use of
stronger stimulus control than those of stereotypy and that reinforcement conditioning procedures. Thus, rather than
stereotypy has a play function and therefore can be re- merely being taught to read or play with toys, children
placed with play behavior. For these participants, it was not would be taught under pairing conditions until the toys or
necessary to use an aversive procedure or treat stereotypy books control choice in free time. Indeed, one of the most
directly. These results indicate that behavior analysts can important goals of education for all students is to expand
use a student’s known reinforcers to replace nonpreferred students’ preferences for activities of all types.
Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at NORTH CAROLINA STATE UNIV on May 15, 2015
86

Figure 2. Intervals of toy play and stereotypy prior to, concurrent with, and after toy-play conditioning
sessions.

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at NORTH CAROLINA STATE UNIV on May 15, 2015
87

ABOUT THE AUTHORS Foxx, R. M., & Azrin, N. H. (1973). The elimination of autistic self-
stimulatory behavior by overcorrection. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy-
Robin Nuzzolo-Gomez, MPhil, is the behavior analyst di- sis, 6, 1-14.
Greer, R. D. (1981, October). An operant approach to motivation and affect:
rector of the David Gregory School, a CABASO model school
of research in music learning. Paper presented at the Ann Arbor
Ten years
for students with autism and other developmental disabili- Symposium on Psychology and the Teaching and Learning of Music.
ties. Her current research interest includes the role of the Reston, VA: Music Educators’ National Conference.
supervision learn unit on teachers’ scientific tacts and con- Greer, R. D., Becker, J. B., Saxe, C. D., & Mirabella, R. F. (1985). Conditioning
histories and setting stimuli controlling engagement in stereotypy or toy
tingency shaped repertoires. Mandy A. Leonard, MA,
Eyleen Ortiz, MA, and Celestina M. Rivera, MA, are cur- play. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 5, 269-284.
Greer, R. D., Dorow, L. G., & Randall, A. (1975). Music listening preferences
rently PhD candidates at Columbia University, Teachers Col- of children from nursery school through grade six. Journal of Research in
lege. Their current research interest is in decreasing echolalia Music Education, 22(4), 284-291.
and palilalia in preschool students with autism. R. Douglas Greer, R. D., & McDonough, S. H. (1999). Is the learn unit a fundamental
Greer, PhD, is a professor of education and psychology and measure of pedagogy? The Behavior Analyst, 22 5-16.
(1),
is the coordinator of the Behavior Analysis Program at Harris, S. L., & Wolchik, S. A. (1979). Supression of self-stimulation: Three
alternative strategies. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 185-198.
Teachers College, Columbia University. His current interests
Koegel, R. L., & Covert, A. (1972). The relationship of self-stimulation to
include CABAS model of education and the learn unit. Ad- learning in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5,
dress : R. Douglas Greer, Columbia University, Teachers Col- 381-387.

lege, PO Box 76, New York, NY 10027. Koegel, R. L., Firestone, P. B., Kramme, K. W., & Dunlap, G. (1974). Increas-
ing spontaneous play by suppressing self-stimulation in autistic children.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 521-528.
REFERENCES Lovaas, O. I., Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J. Q. (1965). Experimental studies in
childhood schizophrenia: Building social behavior in autistic children by
Baumeister, A. A., & Forehand, R. (1972). Effects of contingent shock and use of electric shock. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 1,
verbal command on stereotyped body rocking of retardates. Journal of 99-109.
Clinical Psychology, 28, 586-590. Marholin, D., & Townsend, N. M. (1978). An experimental analysis of side
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (1987). Applied behavior analy- effects and response maintenance of a modified overcorrection procedure:
sis. Columbus, OH: Merrill. The case of the persistent twiddler. Behavior Therapy, 9, 383-390.
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1987). Social influences on "self-stimulatory" Schopler, E., Reichler, R., & Renner, B. (1988). The childhood autism rating
behavior: Analysis and treatment application. Journal of Applied Behavior scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Analysis, 20, 119-132. Singh, N. N., Dawson, M. J., & Manning, P. (1981). Effects of spaced re-
Eason, L. J., White, M. J., & Newsom, C. (1982). Generalized reduction of self- sponding DRL on the stereotyped behavior of profoundly retarded per-
stimulatory behaviors: An effect of teaching appropriate play to autistic sons. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 521-526.
children. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 157-
169. Action Editor: Robert L. Koegel

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at NORTH CAROLINA STATE UNIV on May 15, 2015

You might also like