Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat.

41 (2010) 125–134

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Behavior Therapy and


Experimental Psychiatry
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbtep

Adopting the perspective of another in belief attribution: Contribution of


Relational Frame Theory to the understanding of impairments in schizophrenia
Matthieu Villatte a, *, Jean-Louis Monestès b, Louise McHugh c, Esteve Freixa i Baqué a, Gwenolé Loas b
a
Departement de Psychologie, Université de Picardie Jules Verne, Chemin du Thil, Amiens, France
b
Service Universitaire de Psychiatrie – Neurosciences Fonctionnelles & Pathologies – CNRS UMR 8160 – Centre Hospitalier Ph. Pinel, France
c
University of Wales, Swansea, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Impaired ability of identifying mental states is a characteristic of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. In
Received 24 April 2009 particular, people suffering from this illness tend to fail at attributing a belief to another, which has been
Received in revised form linked to difficulties in changing interpersonal perspective. Following the view of Relational Frame
13 September 2009
Theory on perspective-taking skills, the current study aimed at examining the involvement of social
Accepted 17 November 2009
anhedonia, one of the frequent features of schizophrenia, in the development of deficits in reversing the
I-YOU relation (i.e., adopting the perspective of another). A task consisting of attributing a belief to
Keywords:
another or to the self was employed with 30 non-clinical participants with a high level of social anhe-
Schizophrenia
Social anhedonia donia and with 15 patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. In comparison to two control groups, both
Theory of mind experimental groups showed significant poorer performance when adopting the perspective of another.
Deictic relational responding These results constitute important indications to target specific relational repertoires when attempting
Belief attribution to remediate impairments in mental states attribution linked to schizophrenia.
RFT Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Cross, and Watson (2001) revealed that children below the age of
four usually fail at false-belief tasks, suggesting that these situa-
1.1. Belief attribution tions require complex cognitive skills that young children do not
yet possess in their repertoire.
The ability to understand that we act on the basis of what we Cognitive psychologists have been the first to study the mech-
believe is true, even when such belief is actually false, has been anisms underlying attribution of belief in general and false-belief in
one of the most studied competencies in the field of Theory of particular. An important issue in this field of research concerns the
Mind (i.e., the capacity to infer the beliefs, intentions and involvement of language in such competency. Some authors argue
emotions of others in order to explain and predict their behavior that one needs an ability to infer mental states of others to acquire
(Premack & Woodruff, 1978)). In one typical false-belief task, the language since the use of symbols require attributing a state of
Unexpected Transfer Task (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985), knowledge to the listener regarding the meaning of these symbols
a participant is told a story about Sally who places her marble in (Baron-Cohen, 1999; Hobson, 2000). Conversely, de Villiers (2000)
a basket and then leaves the room. In her absence, Anne moves proposed that the structure of language plays a key-role in the
the marble from the basket to a box. The participant is then asked development of belief understanding. Notably, the mastery of
‘‘Where will Sally look first to find her marble?’’. Another version of a specific aspect of syntax called tensed complements might be
this task, the Deceptive Container Task (Perner, Frith, Leslie, & a prerequisite to the acquisition of Theory of Mind. A tensed
Leekam, 1989), consists of showing a Smarties box to a participant complement is a clausal object following communication or mental
and asking ‘‘What is inside the box?’’. After the box has been verbs (e.g., say or think) that, according to de Villiers, provides the
opened, the participant can see that the box contains pencils opportunity to experience the relativity of mental states because
instead of Smarties and is asked ‘‘Before we opened the box, what the overall sentence may be true even if the complement is false.
did you think was inside the box?’’. A meta-analysis by Wellman, For example, in ‘‘Paul thinks that Mary is not there’’, it is necessary to
distinguish the truth value of each proposition: ‘‘Paul thinks’’ is true,
whereas ‘‘Mary is not there’’ may be false. Thus, such sentences lead
the participant to adopt the perspective of others and to under-
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: matthieu.villatte@u-picardie.fr (M. Villatte).
stand that this belief (‘‘Mary is not there’’) is true from Paul’s

0005-7916/$ – see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.11.004
126 M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134

perspective but may be false from another’s. Consistent with this of accuracy across ages follows a trend consistent with literature
view, de Villiers and Pyers (2002) observed in a longitudinal study using traditional false-belief tasks.
with children from the age of 3 to 5 years that early complement More recently, McHugh, Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, Stew-
understanding and later false-belief performance were related. art, and Dymond (2007) have assumed that the attribution of belief
Further supporting the language false-belief connection, a study by to another involves more relational complexity than self-attribu-
Hale and Tager-Flusberg (2003) revealed that training in a specific tion of belief. In the following example: ‘‘If you (the participant) put
form of grammatical language, that is, tensed complements the pencils in the Smarties box and I am not there, I (the experi-
improved false-belief understanding in preschoolers. menter) would think the Smarties box contains.’’, the participant
has to derive ‘‘If I were you, I would think that.’’ (i.e., reversing the
frame of I-YOU) in order to adopt the perspective of the experi-
1.2. A relational frame analysis of belief attribution menter. Consistent with their hypothesis, McHugh et al. (2007)
observed longer response latencies for attribution to another than
Recently, the account of language and cognition provided by for self-attribution in a sample of adult participants.
Relational Frame Theory (RFT) in terms of arbitrarily applicable Hence, RFT provides a useful behavioral account of belief
relational responding has lead McHugh, Barnes-Holmes, Barnes- attribution underlying the functional role of language that could
Holmes, and Stewart (2006) to propose a behavior analytic overlap with de Villiers’ linguistic approach. That is, verbs of
approach to belief attribution. At the core of RFT lies the concept of communication and verbs of mental states probably require
framing, which consists of responding to a stimulus in relation to deictic relational responding because the listener needs to reverse
another stimulus according to characteristics that can be arbitrary the frames of I-You, Here-There or Now-Then to judge accurately
(i.e., defined by social community) or non-arbitrary (i.e., indepen- the truth value of the whole sentence, as in the following exam-
dent of social community) (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001). ples: ‘‘Paul thinks that Mary is not there’’ (I know that Mary is there
The simplest form of such relating can be referred to as coordina- but if I were Paul, I would think that Mary is not there), ‘‘Paul is
tion framing, which can be exemplified as follows: a child could behind his desk (here) and not in front of the window (there). He
learn that the written word ‘‘DOG’’ is the same as the spoken word thinks that Mary is not in the garden.’’ (Paul is here and cannot see
‘‘DOG’’ in line with the arbitrary characteristics of these two stimuli Mary through the window but if here was there, Paul would know)
while their non-arbitrary properties are very different. According to or ‘‘I did not know this morning that you would be here tonight’’
RFT, it is the ability to learn to relate objects and events based on (I know now that you are here but if now was then I would not
arbitrary properties that allows the massive generativity of know). RFT constitutes an approach to language and cognition
language in human beings. which explains these phenomena, including mental states attri-
Among the different ways in which two stimuli can be related bution, in terms of generalized relational responding. While RFTs
(such as relations of ‘coordination’ and ‘opposition’ (Dymond & explanation of the provenance of these abilities is unique, its
Barnes, 1996); ‘distinction’ (Roche & Barnes, 1996); ‘comparison’ empirically guided conception of the behavioral patterns involved
(Dymond & Barnes, 1995; O’Hora, Roche, Barnes-Holmes, & Smeets, in these abilities is potentially compatible with mainstream
2002), ‘hierarchy’ (Griffee & Dougher, 2002); ‘analogy’ (Barnes, conceptions.1
Hegarty, & Smeets, 1997; Stewart, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2004);
‘temporality’ (O’Hora, Barnes-Holmes, Roche, & Smeets, 2004;
O’Hora et al., 2002)), Barnes-Holmes, Hayes, and Dymond (2001) 1.3. Impairments in belief attribution in schizophrenia spectrum
have underlined the role of a specific class of relational responding disorders
termed deictic frames, which are assumed to account for
perspective-taking skills. Specifically, the frames of I-YOU, HERE- Although deficits in Theory of Mind have first been studied in
THERE and NOW-THEN would be learned through a history of autistic disorders (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985), weaker performance
exposure to verbal events allowing us to adopt different points of in a series of tasks requiring attributing mental states, including
view along interpersonal, spatial, and temporal dimensions. For false-belief, have been observed more recently in schizophrenia
example, responding to questions such as ‘‘What did you do in town (see Brüne, 2005a for a review and Sprong, Schothorst, Vos, Hox, &
yesterday? Did you see the same movie as me?’’ requires the listener Van Engeland, 2007 for a meta-analysis). Such difficulties are linked
to change perspective along the three dimensions as follows: ‘‘I am to a deficit in social functioning and integration, which constitute
here now but when I was there and then, I did.’’ and ‘‘You saw that some of the most problematic features of this pathology (Bora,
movie but I saw.’’. Eryavuz, Kayahan, Sungu, & Veznedaroglu, 2006; Brüne, 2005b,
Barnes-Holmes, McHugh, and Barnes-Holmes (2004) have 2006; Hyronemus, Penn, Corrigan, & Martin, 1998; Roncone et al.,
argued that belief attribution might be conceptualized as deictic 2002; Schenkel, Spaulding, & Silverstein, 2005). According to
relational responding since one needs to adopt different perspec- Langdon and Coltheart (2001) and Langdon, Coltheart, Ward, and
tives along the spatial and temporal dimensions in these types of Catts (2001), these impairments in mental states attribution in
tasks. Relational complexity is also added by the implication of the schizophrenia spectrum disorders are due to inefficient abilities to
frame of logical NOT for the attribution of a false-belief. In the reconstruct another first-person experience (allocentric simula-
example of a correct response to the Deceptive Container Task, the tion). Indeed, Langdon et al. observed that schizotypal participants
involvement of these specific relational frames can be outlined as and participants with schizophrenia performed less accurately than
follows: ‘‘I did not see inside there and then, but I do see inside here controls on a visual perspective-taking task when the principle was
and now’’. In concert with this interpretation, McHugh et al. (2006) to imagine the appearance of an object under a rotation of the
observed that accuracy in a series of tasks designed to assess deictic
relational responding involved in true- and false-belief attribution
1
increased as a function of age. In particular, 3- to 5-year-old chil- Although comparing models of mental states attribution would require a more
dren demonstrated a weaker performance than all the older age extended discussion than the current paper allows, it should be mentioned that the
RFT approach shares notably certain similarities with Simulation Theory (Gordon,
groups and the 5- to 7-year-olds, although better than the younger 1996; Harris, 1992) since both models put perspective-taking at the core of this
group, were significantly worse than the three older groups (8–10, ability (see Villatte, Monestès, McHugh, & Freixa i Baqué, 2009 for a systematic
12–14, and 18–35 year-olds). As noted by the authors, this evolution comparison between the RFT approach and other main models of ToM).
M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134 127

viewer (allocentric simulation), whereas no deficit appeared when should present a subtle deficit in belief attribution to others, which
participants were required to imagine the appearance of an object might precede more severe difficulties when the illness arises.
under a rotation of the object (egocentric simulation). Thus, The current research aims at assessing deictic relational
perspective-taking skills would be impaired in schizophrenic responding involved in belief attribution in people diagnosed with
spectrum disorders when it is required to adopt the perspective of schizophrenia. If the RFT approach to this type of mental states
another. In RFT terms, the ability to respond in accordance with the understanding is accurate, these participants should demonstrate
deictic frame of I-YOU would be impaired when additional rela- deficits in false-belief attribution similar to those reported in
tional responding is necessitated by the inclusion of the IF-THEN mainstream cognitive studies. In addition, the specific design of RFT
frame (i.e., ‘‘If I were you then I would believe that.’’). protocols will allow distinguishing between two different rela-
tional repertoires, that is, attribution to the self and attribution to
another (i.e., simple vs. reversed I-YOU). Because individuals
1.4. The role of social anhedonia in the development of ToM suffering from schizophrenia usually perform weaker than controls
impairments on attribution of false-belief and on attribution of others’ mental
states in general, we expect these participants to be impaired both
According to dimensional approaches to schizophrenia, this when the frame of logical NOT or an inversion of the frame of I-YOU
illness reflects an extreme level on a continuum between normal is involved. In order to test the prediction that deficits in belief
and pathological functioning. That is, characteristics of schizo- attribution to another are linked to social anhedonia, deictic rela-
phrenia (including impulsive non-conformity, perceptual aberra- tional responding involved in belief attribution will also be assessed
tion, magical ideation, physical and social anhedonia (Chapman, in a sample of non-clinical participants scoring two standard
Chapman, & Kwapil, 1995)) can be present in non-clinical pop- deviations above the mean or higher on a social anhedonia scale;
ulation but at different levels. The higher young individuals score poorer performances being expected when an inversion of the
on these schizotypal dimensions, the more the risk to develop frame of I-YOU is required.
symptoms of schizophrenia increases. For instance, Chapman,
Chapman, Kwapil, Eckblad, and Zinser (1994) observed that among 2. Study I
an initial sample of over five hundred individuals, those with high
scores on schizotypal dimensions (i.e., a score of two standard 2.1. Method
deviations above the mean) developed more psychoses and
psychotic-like experiences across a 10-year longitudinal study. 2.1.1. Measures
Gooding, Tallent, and Matts (2005) reported similar results in The Revised Social Anhedonia Scale (SAS) (Eckblad et al., 1982) is
a more recent 5-year longitudinal study. In concert with the a true-false self-report questionnaire that measures social with-
symptoms amplification between high levels of schizotypy and drawal, a lack of interest in social relationships and/or a lack of
schizophrenic syndrome, several studies have reported the exis- pleasure derived from interpersonal relationships with 40 items
tence of slight deficits in mental states attribution in non-clinical such as ‘‘I sometimes become deeply attached to people I spend a lot of
populations with a high profile of schizotypy (Langdon & Coltheart, time with’’ (keyed false) or ‘‘If given the choice, I would much rather
1999 – with a false-belief task; Platek, Critton, Myers, & Gallup, be with others than be alone’’ (keyed false).
2003 – with a ‘‘faux-pas’’ task based on a higher level of false-belief One thousand two hundred and fifty five first-year psychology
understanding). students participated in the study by completing the SAS. Thirty
Among schizotypal dimensions, social anhedonia, which is participants chosen randomly among individuals with a high score
characterized by social disinterest, withdrawal and a lack of plea- (i.e., scoring at or above two SD’s above the mean of the same sex
sure from social contact (Eckblad, Chapman, Chapman, & Mishlove, sample) constituted the experimental group. Thirty participants
1982), constitutes a main factor of vulnerability to the development chosen randomly among individuals scoring not higher than 0.5 SD
of schizophrenic spectrum disorders (Gooding et al., 2005; Kwapil, from the mean of the same sex sample were retained to constitute
1998), even when it is associated with low scores on other sub- the control group.2 Exclusion criteria included any head injury or
scales of schizotypy (Horan, Brown, & Blanchard, 2007). In addition, psychiatric illness and French as a second language.
certain features of schizophrenia are also present at a lower level in Since intellectual competencies were demonstrated to impact
non-clinical populations with high social anhedonia, such as slight on ToM performance of people with schizophrenia in several
impairments in working memory (Gooding & Tallent, 2001) and in studies (see Sprong et al., 2007), Raven’s Progressive Matrices
sustained attention (Kwapil & Diaz, 2000), or perseverating errors (Raven, 1960) were employed to assess intellectual competencies of
using the WCST (Barrantes-Vidal et al., 2003; Gooding, Kwapil, & all the participants selected for the experimental protocol in order
Tallent, 1999; Tallent & Gooding, 1999). Considering the lack of to control for effects of IQ and thus, isolate the effect of social
social interactions experienced by individuals with high social anhedonia.
anhedonia (Brown, Silvia, Myin-Germeys, & Kwapil, 2007) and the
crucial role of social experiences in the development of accurate 2.1.2. Participants3
mental states attribution (Bartsch, 2002), it is possible to conceive Experimental group: 10 males and 20 females. Ages ranged from
this specific dimension as a key factor of deficits in Theory of Mind 18 to 21 years (mean 19.33 years; SD: 0.8). Mean score on the SAS:
in schizophrenia. In line with this view, slight impairments in 22.07 (SD: 4.76). Mean score on Raven’s Progressive Matrices: 48.7
attribution of mental states have been observed in studies (SD: 4.47).
employing a traditional Theory of Mind task (Monestès, Villatte,
Moore, Yon, & Loas, 2007; Villatte, Monestès, McHugh, Freixa i
Baqué, & Loas, 2008). According to RFT, because social interactions 2
The inclusion criteria for the constitution of the two groups were the same as
constitute opportunities to adopt another individual’s point of those of most of the studies on social anhedonia and schizotypy (see for instance
Chapman et al., 1994; Collins, Blanchard, & Biondo, 2005; Gooding & Tallent, 2003;
view, the ability to switch perspective between the self and another
Gooding et al., 1999, 2005; Horan et al., 2007; Kwapil, 1998; Tallent & Gooding,
(i.e., the ability to reverse the frame of I-YOU) should be particularly 1999).
impaired by a deficit in this kind of experience. If this prediction is 3
The participants took part also in another study. However, the presentation
correct, young individuals with a profile of high social anhedonia order of the two protocols was counterbalanced.
128 M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134

Table 1
Examples of trials of the belief-attribution task.

Self-attribution Attribution to other

True-belief False-belief True-belief False-belief


If I put the pencils in the Smarties box If I put the pencils in the Smarties If You put the pencils in the Smarties If You put the pencils in the Smarties
and you are here, you would think box and you are not here, you would box and I am here, I would think the box and I am not here, I would think
the Smarties box contains think the Smarties box contains Smarties box contains the Smarties box contains
Pencils/smarties Pencils/smarties Pencils/smarties Pencils/smarties

Control group: 10 males and 20 females. Ages ranged from 18 to respectively. To ensure that all participants responded in the same
22 years (mean 19.27 years; SD: 1.05); there was no significant way, after the session, each of the participants was asked if ‘‘I’’
difference in age between experimental and control groups: corresponded to him/her or to anyone else (and the same question
(t(58) ¼ 0.28, p > 0.05). Mean score on the SAS: 7.13 (SD: 1.17). Mean for ‘‘You’’). Anyone who did not respond correctly to these two
score on Raven’s Progressive Matrices: 48.6 (SD: 4.37); there was no questions was excluded from the analyses.
significant difference between experimental and control groups:
(t(58) ¼ 0.03, p > 0.05). 2.2. Results
No significant correlation emerged between participants scores
on the SAS and scores on Raven’s Progressive Matrices (r ¼ 0.08, 2.2.1. Accuracy
p > 0.05). The percentage of accuracy was calculated for each participant.
All the participants took part in the study to meet part of their These results were then grouped by SAS level and trial-type. The
course requirements. data indicate that the two groups demonstrated a rate of accuracy
above 0.85 for all trial-types. Nevertheless, while the two groups
2.1.3. Procedure produced almost the same number of errors on self-attribution of
An E-PrimeÒ (version 1.1) program was compiled in order to true- and false-belief, the group with high social anhedonia per-
present the task to the participants on a Personal Computer with formed poorer on both true- and false-belief attributions to another
a 660 MHz processor, a 15-inch color monitor and a numeric pad. All than the control group: participants with high social anhedonia
trials in the program were presented in French (black letters, font 26). produced 75% more errors on true-belief and three times more
The experiment consisted of one block of 48 true-false trials that errors on false-belief than control participants.
were divided in four categories, according to two types of attribu- When regrouping true- and false-beliefs rates of accuracy (see
tion (self and other) crossed with two types of belief (true and Fig. 1), it appeared that experimental participants produced almost
false). Thus, among trials of self-attribution, there were twelve three times more errors than controls on attribution to another
trials for true-beliefs and twelve trials for false-beliefs. There was (0.95 vs. 0.86), whereas both groups obtained a similar mean score
the same ratio of true- and false-beliefs for trials of attribution to on attribution to the self (0.95 vs. 0.94).
other and there was an equal number of true and false statements A 2  2  2 mixed repeated measures multivariate analyses of
(see examples in Table 1). Three objects sets appeared in the variance (MANOVA) was employed with group-type (low vs. high
statements: a Smarties box and pencils; a farina box and cacao; score on the SAS) as the between subject variable, attribution-type
a cacao box and farina. As in McHugh et al. (2007), these objects (self vs. other) and belief-type (true vs. false) as within subject
were selected for the purpose of false-belief attribution because variables and with accuracy and response latencies as the two
one would normally expect to find farina rather than cacao, for dependent variables. The main effect of group-type was significant
example, inside a farina box. (Wilk’s (2,57) ¼ 5.08, p < 0.01). There was a significant main effect
The instruction given to the participants was as follows: ‘‘You of attribution-type (Wilk’s F(2,57) ¼ 19.69, p ¼ 0.000). There was
will see appear on the screen the first part of a statement. Once you a statistical tendency for an interaction between group-type and
have read the first part, you must press the key ‘‘enter’’ in order for the attribution-type (Wilk’s F(2,57) ¼ 2.97, p ¼ 0.06), suggesting that
end of the statement to appear. Then your job will be to press 1 if the the two groups were differently affected by the type of attribution.
whole statement is true and 2 if it is false. In the different statements
that you will read, ‘‘You’’ refers always to you and I to anyone else who
would be talking to you’’. 1
Both accuracy rates and response times were recorded (with
Mean rate of accuracy

longer response latencies predicted to reflect poorer performance). 0,9


Response latencies were recorded as follows: once the participant
had read the first part of the statement (example: ‘‘If I put the pencils
0,8
in the Smarties box and you are here, you would think the Smarties box
contains’’), s/he had to press the key ‘‘Enter’’. Then, the end of the
0,7
statement and the two allowable responses appeared on screen
(example: ‘‘PENCILS’’ True/False). Response latencies were recorded
between the participant pressing ‘‘Enter’’ after having read the first 0,6
part of the statement and his/her response by pressing one of the
two activated keys (response latencies that exceeded two SD’s 0,5
above the mean were removed from the statistical analyses). All Self Other
trials were presented randomly and no feedback was given after the Attribution type
participant’s response.
Control Experimental
In this task, it is important for the participant to understand that
‘‘You’’ and ‘‘I’’ represent the perspective of the participant and the Fig. 1. Accuracy (proportion correct) for experimental and control participants on each
perspective of anyone who would be talking to the participant, type of attribution (self vs. other).
M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134 129

another: 97% vs. 83% for true-belief (statistical tendency,


Proportion of participants scoring above 0.67

c2(1) ¼ 2.96; p ¼ 0.08) and 100% vs. 83% for false-belief (significant
1
difference, c2(1) ¼ 5.45; p < 0.05). These results are consistent with
the previous analyses of variance, revealing that the experimental
0,8
participants produced more errors than controls only on tasks that
involve attribution to another.
0,6
Finally, correlation analyses (using Bonferroni corrections)
0,4 revealed that the score on the SAS correlated negatively with
accuracy on attribution to another (r ¼ 0.36; p ¼ 0.02) but not on
0,2 self-attribution (r ¼ 0.03; p > 0.05). These results indicate a link
between higher levels of social anhedonia and poorer performance
0 on adopting another’s perspective, consistent with the analysis of
Self/True belief Self/False belief Other/True belief Other/False belief variance conducted earlier.
Trial type
Control Experimental
2.2.2. Response latencies4
The data indicate that the response latencies of the two groups
Fig. 2. Proportion of participants from the two groups scoring over 0.67 on each type were very close on all trial-types. In general, the response latencies
of attribution and belief. were longer on attribution to another than on self-attribution.
Across the self-attributions, response latencies were slightly longer
The effect of belief-type was not significant (Wilk’s F(2,57) ¼ 0.18, on false-belief than on true-belief. An inverse trend appeared
p ¼ 0.84), neither was the interaction between group-type and across the attributions to another.
belief-type (Wilk’s F(2,57) ¼ 0.01, p ¼ 0.91), indicating that the two Univariate analysis revealed that the main effect of group-type
groups of participants were not affected by the type of belief to was not significant (Wilk’s F(1,58) ¼ 0.26; p ¼ 0.61). The main effect
report. The interaction between attribution-type and belief-type of attribution-type was significant (Wilk’s F(1,58)¼ 34.19;
was significant (Wilk’s F(2,57) ¼ 6.88, p < 0.01), indicating that the p ¼ 0.000), but not the interaction between group-type and attri-
effect of attribution-type that influenced all the participants bution-type (Wilk’s F(1,58) ¼ 2.55, p ¼ 0.12). There was no effect of
performances was influenced by the type of belief. Finally, the belief-type (Wilk’s F(1,58) ¼ 0, p ¼ 0.95) and no interaction
interaction between subject, attribution-type and belief-type was between group-type and belief-type (Wilk’s F (1,58) ¼ 0.03,
not significant (Wilk’s F(2,57) ¼ 0.67, p ¼ 0.5). p ¼ 0.85). The interaction between attribution-type and belief-type
Univariate analysis revealed a significant main effect of group- was significant (Wilk’s F(1,58) ¼ 11.72, p < 0.01). Finally, no inter-
type (Wilk’s F(1,58) ¼ 6.1, p ¼ 0.02). The main effect of attribution- action appeared between the types of group, attribution, and belief
type was also significant (Wilk’s F(1,58) ¼ 6.2, p ¼ 0.02) as well as (Wilk’s F (1,58) ¼ 0.26, p ¼ 0.61). To summarize, the two groups
the interaction between group-type and attribution-type were faster on self-attribution than on attribution to another,
(F(1,58) ¼ 6.2, p ¼ 0.02). There was no significant effect of belief- which was influenced by the type of belief to report.
type (Wilk’s F(1,58)¼0.34, p ¼ 0.56) and no interaction between Planned comparisons were conducted (using Bonferroni
belief-type and group-type (Wilk’s F(1,58)¼1.35, p ¼ 0.25). Finally, corrections) to examine the interaction between attribution-type
there was no significant interaction between attribution-type and and belief-type in the whole sample of participants. These analyses
belief-type (Wilk’s F(1,58)¼1.63, p ¼ 0.21) and between attribution- revealed that participants were faster on self-attribution than on
type, belief-type and group-type (Wilk’s F(1,58) ¼ 0, p ¼ 1). In brief, attribution to another (t(59) ¼ 5.13; p ¼ 0.000 for true-belief and
accuracy of the two groups differed from each other as a function of t(59) ¼ 3.5; p < 0.01 for false-belief). When analysing self-attribu-
the type of attribution involved, whereas the type of belief had no tion, it appeared that participants were faster on true-belief than on
influence. false-belief (t(59) ¼ 2.76; p < 0.04). No variation emerged as
Though no difference appeared between the score of the two a function of belief-type on attribution to another (t(59) ¼ 2.18;
groups on Raven’s test, we conducted an ANCOVA to examine if the p ¼ 0.13). In brief, participants were faster on self-attribution and
effect of group-type remained significant. The results of this anal- particularly on true-belief.
ysis revealed a significant effect of group-type (F(1,57) ¼ 3.34;
p < 0.05) and no significant effect of the score on Raven’s test
2.3. Discussion
(F(1,57) ¼ 2.47; p > 0.05). Thus, the difference in performance
observed between the two groups was not due to a variation of
The results of this first study supported our main prediction.
general intellectual competencies between the participants.
Participants with a high level of social anhedonia demonstrated
Post-hoc tests (Fishers PLSD) were conducted to analyse
poorer performance than controls when they were required to
comparisons between the two groups among the different types of
reverse the frame of I-YOU, that is, when attributing a belief to
attribution (using Bonferroni corrections to control for Type I
another. In addition, these participants produced more errors on
errors). These analyses revealed that experimental participants
attribution to another than on attribution to the self, whereas no
were significantly less accurate than controls on attribution to
difference between these two types of attribution appeared in
another (p ¼ 0.02); but no difference appeared on self-attribution
controls. Such findings are in line with Villatte et al. (2008), in which
(p > 0.05). Furthermore, experimental participants were less
participants with high social anhedonia were significantly less accu-
accurate on attribution to another than on self-attribution
rate than controls on highest levels of deictic relational complexity in
(p < 0.01), whereas no significant difference emerged between the
a perspective-taking task. Our results thus support once again the
two types of attribution for the control group (p > 0.05).
view that fewer social interactions impact on the development of
Because accuracy rates of 0.5 can be interpreted as chance level
responding, we calculated the proportion of each group’s partici-
pants whose scores were over 0.67 (see Fig. 2). These proportions 4
Response latencies that exceeded two SD’s above the mean were removed from
were not significantly different between the two groups for self- statistical analyses. However, the exclusion of these data had no effect on the
attribution but were superior in the control group for attribution to statistical analyses.
130 M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134

deficits in mental states attribution. Because people characterized by an RFT based belief-attribution task will be similar to those
a lack of interest in social contact tend to engage in fewer interactions demonstrated using cognitively based ToM tasks. In addition to this
with others, they practice less often changing perspective, which employing the RFT task will afford a direct comparison between the
results in anomalies when compared to the control population. different aspects of belief attribution (i.e., attribution to the self vs.
Such interpretation of our results seems consistent with to another, true-belief vs. false-belief).
previous research on the links between social interactions and ToM
in children. Indeed, social experiences like disputes seem to
enhance social cognition; the presence of adults using terms of 3.1. Method
intention predicts the age of children’s references to beliefs and the
presence of siblings is correlated with better performance on ToM 3.1.1. Measures
task (see Bartsch, 2002 for a review). In turn, impairments in ToM As in study I, IQ of all the participants was estimated in the
likely lead to important difficulties in social interactions, in current study using Raven’s Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1960) in
particular in schizophrenic spectrum disorders. As Brüne, Abdel- order to control for effects of intellectual competencies.
Hamid, Lehmkämper, and Sonntag (2007) have shown, perfor-
mance in mental states attribution is indeed a strong predictor of 3.1.2. Participants5
poor social competence in this population. Hence, social anhedonia The study was approved by the local ethics committee (CCPPRB
might be even increased by its own consequences, that is, a lack of de Picardie) and all participants provided informed consent.
interest in social contact leads to less training in mental states Patients were recruited from the University-Psychiatric Hospital
attribution, which then leads to poorer social competence and less Ph. Pinel of Amiens (France) and controls from personal acquain-
interest in social contact. tances of the experimenters. Participants did not receive any
The causal link that, as we suggest, defines the relation between payment for participation.
social anhedonia and deficits in mental states attribution might Experimental group: 15 patients (8 males and 7 females) diag-
also be confirmed by comparative research between humans and nosed with schizophrenia according to ICD-10 (World Health
great apes. Indeed, observations of apes severely lacking social Organization, 1992). Ages ranged from 22 to 53 years (mean 33.3
interactions due to their captivity have already revealed a decrease years; SD: 8.4). Estimated IQ: 99.13 (SD: 12.4). Mean duration of
in social competences (see Brüne, Brüne-Cohrs, McGrew, & Pre- illness: 5.13 (SD: 4.8). Mean age of onset of the disease was at 28.1
uschoft, 2006); using experimental tasks akin to ToM assessments (5.5). Fourteen of the 15 patients were treated with conventional or
(see Scott, 2001) might show poorer skills of subjects experiencing atypical neuroleptics.
less social contact independently of their interest in such contact. Control group: 15 healthy participants (8 males and 7 females).
However, the question of the primary origins of a lack of plea- Ages ranged from 20 to 63 years (mean 30.08 years; SD: 11.2); there
sure for social interaction still remains. According to Lebreton et al. was no significant difference in age between the experimental and
(2009), favorable dispositions towards social relationships are the control group: t(28) ¼ 0.68; p > 0.5. Estimated IQ: 110.07 (SD:
linked to specific neuro-anatomical characteristics (i.e., the density 10.78). The difference in IQ between both groups was significant
of gray matter in the orbitofrontal cortex, basal ganglia, and (t(28) ¼ 2.58; p < 0.02); however, analyses of co-variance were
temporal lobes). Although the study demonstrated only a correla- conducted with IQ as the co-variate (as reported in the results
tion between these two variables, one can hypothesize that section).
conversely, in the case of social anhedonia, certain neuro- Exclusion criteria for all participants: any brain injury, age below
anatomical anomalies constitute a starting point in a sequence of 18 or above 65, and French as a second language.
variables that ends in impairments in adopting the perspective of Exclusion criteria for control participants: any history of psychi-
another. atric illness.
Contrary to our prediction, no difference appeared on latencies
between the two groups as a function of attribution-type, which 3.1.3. Procedure
suggests that latencies do not constitute a sufficiently discrimina- The procedure employed in this task was the same as that of
tive dependent variable to distinguish between two non-clinical study I except for the number of trials which was of 24. However,
populations with this type of task. However, since participants with the proportion of trials per type of attribution and type of belief was
high social anhedonia were as fast as controls, it is possible to rule the same as in study I (6 trials per trial-type). The duration of the
out the hypothesis that the more important number of errors they task was reduced in comparison with study I to adapt the protocol
produced was caused by impulsivity. In addition, latencies results to constraints linked to the hospitalisation of the patients.
were consistent with the data reported by McHugh et al. (2007):
participants responded more quickly on tasks involving self-attri-
3.2. Results
bution than on tasks involving belief attribution to another, which
supports the RFT prediction that participants need to reverse the
3.2.1. Accuracy
frame of I-YOU in the latter and hence, need more time to derive
The percentage of accuracy was calculated for each participant.
their responses. The involvement of the frame of logical NOT
These results were then grouped by participant-type and trial-type.
seemed also to have a slight effect on latencies of both groups,
The data indicate that participants of the control group demon-
whereas such effect was not observed in McHugh et al.’s study.
strated a rate of accuracy comprised between 0.91 and 0.95 for all
Since a greater number of trials was employed in this previous
trial-types. Although patients performed very similarly with
research, the repetition of trials might have trained participants to
controls on self-attribution of true-belief, they produced more
respond as fast on both types of belief, thus explaining the
errors on attribution to another (accuracy rates of 0.73 on true-belief
discrepancy between our respective results.
and 0.74 on false-belief) and on self-attribution of false-belief (0.69).

3. Study II
5
The participants took part also in another study (Villatte, Monestès, McHugh,
The aim of Study II is to examine whether the pattern of Freixa i Baqué, & Loas, in press). However, the presentation order of the two
performance observed in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia in protocols was counterbalanced.
M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134 131

Proportion of participants scoring over 0.67


1
Mean rate of accuracy

0,9
0,8

0,8
0,6

0,7
0,4

0,6 0,2

0,5 0
Self/True belief Self/False belief Other/True belief Other/False belief
Self Other
Trial type
Trial type
Controls Patients
Controls Patients
Fig. 4. Proportion of participants from the two groups scoring over 0.67 on each type
Fig. 3. Accuracy (proportion correct) for patients and controls on each type of attri- of attribution and belief.
bution (self vs. other).

Furthermore, patients tended to be less accurate on self-attri-


bution of false-belief than on self-attribution of true-belief
When comparing the two types of attribution independently of (t(14) ¼ 2.91, p ¼ 0.12; significant before correction). A similar trend
the type of belief as in Study I (see Fig. 3), the difference between appeared when comparing self-attribution and attribution to
the two groups was more important on attribution to another (0.95 another of true-belief: (t(14) ¼ 2.82, p ¼ 0.12 significant before
for controls vs. 0.73 for patients) than on self-attribution (0.93 for correction). No difference emerged as a function of attribution- or
controls vs. 0.78 for patients). belief-type in control participants. In brief, while patients’ perfor-
A 2  2  2 mixed repeated measures multivariate analyses of mance seemed affected by the types of belief and attribution,
variance (MANOVA) was employed with group-type (patients vs. controls were equally accurate on all trial-types.
controls) as the between subject variable, attribution-type (self vs. As in study I, the proportion of each group’s participants whose
other) and belief-type (true vs. false) as within subject variables scores were over 0.67 was calculated (see Fig. 4). These proportions
and with accuracy and response latencies as the two dependent were not significantly different between the two groups for self-
variables. The main effect of group-type was significant (Wilk’s attribution of true-belief (87% for controls vs. 73% for patients,
(2,27) ¼ 8.69, p ¼ 0.001). There was a significant main effect of c2(1) ¼ 0.83; p ¼ 0.9), but were superior in the control group for
attribution-type (Wilk’s F(2,27) ¼ 6.6, p < 0.01) and a tendency for self-attribution of false-belief (100% for controls vs. 40% for
an effect of belief-type (Wilk’s F(2,27) ¼ 2.97, p ¼ 0.07). Because patients, c2(1) ¼ 12.86; p ¼ 0.001) and both attributions to another
accuracy’s variances were not homogeneous across the two groups, (true-belief: 100% for controls vs. 40% for patients, c2(1) ¼ 12.86;
interaction effects were not analysed with the MANOVA. p ¼ 0.001; false-belief: 100% for controls vs. 33% for patients,
Univariate analysis revealed a significant main effect of group- c2(1) ¼ 15.00; p < 0.001). These results are consistent with the
type (Wilk’s F(1,28) ¼ 17.12, p < 0.001). The main effect of attribu- previous analyses of variance: patients were less accurate than
tion-type was not significant (Wilk’s F(1,28) ¼ 0.51, p ¼ 0.48), controls when reporting either a false-belief or the belief of another
neither was the effect of belief-type (Wilk’s F(1,28)¼2.16, p ¼ 0.15). but demonstrated a performance similar to controls’ when attrib-
Again, interaction effects were not analysed due to the lack of uting a true-belief to the self.
homogeneity in variance.
Since the two groups differed on the score on Raven’s test, an 3.2.2. Response latencies6
ANCOVA was carried out to examine if the effect of group-type The data indicate that the response latencies of the two groups
remained significant. The results of this analysis revealed a signifi- were very close on all trial-types. In general, the response latencies
cant effect of group-type (F (1,27) ¼ 3.93; p ¼ 0.01) and no signifi- were longer on attribution to another than on self-attribution.
cant effect of the score on Raven’s test (F(1,27) ¼ 1.26; p ¼ 0.31). Across the self-attributions, response latencies were slightly longer
Thus, the difference in performance observed between the two on false-belief than on true-belief.
groups was not due to a variation of general intellectual compe- Univariate analysis revealed that the main effect of group-type
tencies between the participants. was not significant (Wilk’s F(1,28) ¼ 1.41, p ¼ 0.24). The main effect
Although no analysis of interaction was conducted with the of attribution-type was significant (Wilk’s F(1,28)¼ 13.44,
ANOVA, a series of non-parametric planned comparisons (Mann- p ¼ 0.001), but not the interaction between group-type and attri-
Whitney U with Bonferroni corrections) were carried out in order bution-type (Wilk’s F(1,28) ¼ 0.05, p ¼ 0.83). The effect of belief-
to examine if the difference in performance between the two type was significant (Wilk’s F(1,28) ¼ 4.35, p < 0.05) but not the
groups varied as a function of attribution- and belief-type, as interaction between group-type and belief-type (Wilk’s F
descriptive analyses suggested earlier. These analyses revealed that (1,28) ¼ 1.13, p ¼ 0.3). The interaction between attribution-type and
patients were significantly less accurate than controls on self- belief-type was not significant (Wilk’s F(1,28) ¼ 2.59, p ¼ 0.12), and
attribution of false-belief (Z ¼ 2.98; p ¼ 0.04); they also produced no interaction appeared neither between the types of group,
more errors than controls on attribution to another (statistical attribution and belief (Wilk’s F(1,28) ¼ 0.74, p ¼ 0.4). To summarize,
tendencies: Z ¼ 2.78; p ¼ 0.06 for true-belief and Z ¼ 2.8;
p ¼ 0.06 for false-belief). Conversely, no difference appeared on
self-attribution of true-belief (Z ¼ 0.25; p > 0.9). Thus, patients 6
Response latencies that exceeded two SD’s above the mean were removed from
were less accurate than controls when they had to report a false- statistical analyses. However, the exclusion of these data had no effect on the
belief and/or the belief of another. statistical analyses.
132 M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134

the two groups were faster on self-attribution than on attribution study, we suggest that it would be useful to conduct further
to another and on true-belief than on false-belief. research to address this question.
Although the results strongly support the involvement of social
3.3. Discussion anhedonia in the development of an impaired ability to adopt
another’s perspective, certain limitations of the current studies
The results of this second study were consistent with our should be mentioned. First, since other schizotypal dimensions
predictions. Patients with schizophrenia were clearly less accurate were not assessed in the sample of participants with high social
than controls when required to reverse the frame of I-YOU (attri- anhedonia, high levels on some of these dimensions might be
bution of belief to another) and/or to respond in accordance with responsible (at least in part) for the poorer performances observed.
the frame of Logical not (attribution of a false-belief). These find- However, recent research on social anhedonia indicate that this
ings are line with the growing literature on mental states attribu- dimension is particularly crucial to understand the progressive
tion in schizophrenia showing that patients who suffer from this apparition of psychotic symptoms, even when it is not associated
condition present a deficit in attribution of a false-belief (see Brüne, with other dimensions of schizotypy (Horan et al., 2007). Replica-
2005a and Sprong et al., 2007). It also supports the idea that these tion of our results controlling levels on the different dimensions
patients have an important disability of adopting another’s point of will allow clarifying this issue.
view, as already suggested by Langdon et al. (2001). Similarly, although our data bring certain arguments to explain
As in the first study, latencies recordings did not reveal the same the impairments of people with schizophrenia in changing inter-
trend as mean rates of accuracy, both groups performing very personal perspective in terms of fewer social interactions, we
closely on all trial-types (in a manner generally similar to partici- cannot determine if this variable effectively impacted on patients’
pants from study I and McHugh et al. (2007) when comparing performance. Indeed, social anhedonia was not assessed in our
attribution to the self vs. to another). This result suggests once sample of patients and it is possible that this dimension was not
again that response latencies might not be a sufficiently discrimi- present at a high level, even though social disinterest is reported as
native dependent variable in this context. However, such findings a main characteristic of schizophrenia in the classification of mental
tend to rule out the hypothesis that accuracy differences observed disorders (DSM-IV-TR – American Psychiatric Association, 2000;
between group mean scores may have been a by product of ICD-10 – World Health Organization, 1992). With regard to the view
neuroleptic treatment rather than participants clinical condition that only disorganized patients are impaired in mental states
per se. That is, if medication would have significantly handicapped attribution (Hardy-Baylé, Sarfati, & Passerieux, 2003), other
the patients of the current research, they would probably have measures, such as disorganized thought (using the TLC – Andreasen,
demonstrated longer latencies than controls on all trial-types. In 1979), might be also conducted to disentangle the influence of the
addition, and as in study I, impulsivity is unlikely responsible for specific compounds of schizophrenia on deictic relational
poorer accuracy of the patients since they were neither faster than responding involved in belief attribution. Finally, since an influence
controls on any trial-type. of deficits in executive functioning on ToM performance in schizo-
phrenia has been mentioned (see Pickup, 2008 for a review), the
4. General discussion absence of assessment in this domain of competency in the current
study leads to the possibility that such influence exists in our sample
The current research aimed at using a relational frame approach of participants. It is worth noting, however, that Pickup’s review
in order to examine the involvement of social anhedonia in the suggests that impairments in mental states attribution cannot be
impaired capacity to adopt another’s point of view of people with fully explained by executive functioning deficits in this illness.
schizophrenia. Results showed that participants with a profile of Using RFT to study impairments in belief attribution aimed at
high social anhedonia were, as patients with schizophrenia, less providing data regarding the performance of participants in specific
accurate than controls when reversing the I-YOU relation. These aspects of this competency. Indeed, the functional conceptualiza-
findings are compatible with the hypothesis that the lack of social tion of belief attribution offered by RFT allowed a distinction to be
interactions linked to social disinterest might be responsible for the made between the specific relational repertoires involved (i.e.,
development of deficits in attribution of mental states to others in simple vs. reversed I-YOU relation; Logical NOT) and helped shed
schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Although non-clinical partici- some light on an area frequently considered as a whole (i.e.,
pants demonstrated only slight impairments (all mean rates without separating the different aspects of mental state attribution,
reaching at least 85%), such subtle anomalies match the data such as attribution to the self vs. to another). Findings from the
reported by the literature on mental states attribution in schizotypy current research indicate that reversing the I-YOU relation consti-
(Irani et al., 2006; Langdon & Coltheart, 1999; Platek et al., 2003) and tutes a key behavioral target for remediating deficits demonstrated
might be viewed as a predictor of more severe impairments when by people with schizophrenia and high social anhedonia. Thus
psychotic symptoms develop, as suggested by results from Study II. procedures recently developed from the RFT perspective (see
The fact that participants with high social anhedonia were as Rehfeldt & Barnes-Holmes, 2009) might be employed to train
accurate as controls on false-belief attribution, whereas patients deictic relational responding in these populations. So far, several
with schizophrenia demonstrated a clear impairment in this task training studies of this kind have proven efficiency in accelerating
contributes to specify the possible role of this dimension. Experi- the development of these repertoires in normal developing chil-
encing less social interactions would cause difficulties in appreci- dren (Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, & McHugh, 2004) as well as
ating the point of view of others but would have no influence on in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder
understanding that people can act on the basis of a false-belief. This (Rehfeldt, Dillen, Ziomek, & Kowalchuk, 2007). In addition, a study
particular competency might be deteriorated by other schizotypal by Weil, Hayes, and Capurro (2007) revealed that using similar
dimensions, such as magical ideation or perceptual aberration. So tasks to those of the current research in the frame of training
far, while Theory of Mind impairments in schizophrenia have been procedures positively impacted on post-test performances on
linked to anhedonia (see Frith, 2004) and disorganized thought traditional false-belief tasks. Future research will show if these
(Sarfati & Hardy-Baylé, 1999) the impact of these different spheres promising recent developments in the field of behavioral remedi-
of the symptomatology on specific aspects of mental states attri- ation can be applied usefully to schizophrenia spectrum disorders.
bution is still unknown. Based on the findings from the current Finally, we propose that the likely implication of a lack of social
M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134 133

interactions in the development of deficits in adopting anothers’ more-than, and less-than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64,
163–184.
perspective (and consequently in attributing mental states to
Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1996). A transformation of self-discrimination response
others) should be taken into account in remediation programs for functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness
patients with schizophrenia. Numerous studies have demonstrated and opposition. The Psychological Record, 46, 271–300.
the utility of role-playing to train social skills among people with Eckblad, M. L., Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., & Mishlove, M. (1982). The revised
social anhedonia scale. Unpublished test, University of Wisconsin, Madison.
psychosis (Dilk & Bond, 1996; Kurtz & Mueser, 2008); findings from Frith, C. D. (2004). Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychological Medicine, 34,
our research suggest that patients might benefit from this kind of 385–389.
exercise to specifically enhance their capacity of accurately iden- Gooding, D. C., Kwapil, T. R., & Tallent, K. A. (1999). Working memory and Wisconsin
Card Sorting Test performance in schizotypic individuals: a replication and
tifying others’ mental states. extension. Psychiatry Research, 89, 161–170.
Gooding, D. C., & Tallent, K. A. (2001). The association between anti-saccade task
and working memory task performance n schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
References Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 8–16.
Gooding, D. C., & Tallent, K. A. (2003). Spatial, object, and affective working memory
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental in social anhedonia: an exploratory study. Schizophrenia Research, 63, 247–260.
disorders. Text revision (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Gooding, D. C., Tallent, K. A., & Matts, C. W. (2005). Clinical status of at-risk indi-
Association. viduals 5 years later: further validation of the psychometric high-risk strategy.
Andreasen, N. C. (1979). Thought, language and communication disorders. Archives Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 170–175.
of General Psychiatry, 36, 1315–1330. Gordon, R. M. (1996). ‘‘Radical’’ simulationism. In P. Carruthers, & P. K. Smith
Barnes, D., Hegarty, N., & Smeets, P. M. (1997). Relating equivalence relations to (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 11–21). Cambridge: Cambridge
equivalence relations: a relational framing model of complex human func- University Press.
tioning. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 14, 57–83. Griffee, K., & Dougher, M. J. (2002). Contextual control of stimulus generalization
Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayes, S. C., & Dymond, S. (2001). Self and self directed rules. In and stimulus equivalence in hierarchical categorization. Journal of the Experi-
S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A post- mental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 433–447.
Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 119–139). New York: Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of
Plenum. mind: a training study. Developmental Science, 6, 346–359.
Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & McHugh, L. (2004). Teaching derived Hardy-Baylé, M.-C., Sarfati, Y., & Passerieux, C. (2003). The cognitive basis of disor-
relational responding to young children. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior ganization symptomatology in schizophrenia and its clinical correlates: toward
Intervention, 1, 4–13. a pathogenic approach to disorganization. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29, 459–471.
Barnes-Holmes, Y., McHugh, L., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking and Harris, P. L. (1992). From simulation to folk psychology. Mind and Language, 7,
theory of mind: a relational frame account. The Behavior Analyst Today, 5, 15–25. 120–144.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Evolution of a theory of mind? In M. Corballis, & S. Lea Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-
(Eds.), The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum.
(pp. 261–277) Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hobson, R. P. (2000). The grounding of symbols: a social-developmental account. In
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory P. Mitchell, & K. J. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 11–35).
of mind? Cognition, 21, 37–46. Hove: Psychology Press.
Barrantes-Vidal, N., Fañanás, L., Rosa, A., Caparrós, B., Riba, M. D., & Obiols, J. E. Horan, W. P., Brown, S. A., & Blanchard, J. J. (2007). Social anhedonia and schizotypy:
(2003). Neurocognitive, behavioral and neurodevelopmental correlates of the contribution of individual differences in affective traits, stress and coping.
schizotypy clusters in adolescents from the general population. Schizophrenia Psychiatry Research, 149, 156.
Research, 61, 293–302. Hyronemus, G., Penn, D. L., Corrigan, P. W., & Martin, J. (1998). Social perception and
Bartsch, K. (2002). The role of experience in children’s developing folk episte- social skill in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 80, 275–286.
mology: review and analysis from the theory-theory perspective. New Ideas in Irani, F., Platek, S. M., Panyavin, I. S., Calkins, M. E., Kohler, C., Siegel, S. J., et al.
Psychology, 20, 145–161. (2006). Self-face recognition and theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia
Bora, E., Eryavuz, A., Kayahan, B., Sungu, G., & Veznedaroglu, B. (2006). Social and first-degree relatives. Schizophrenia Research, 88, 151–160.
functioning, theory of mind and neurocognition in outpatients with schizo- Kurtz, M. M., & Mueser, K. T. (2008). A meta-analysis of controlled research on social
phrenia; mental state decoding may be a better predictor of social functioning skills training for schizophrenia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76,
than mental state reasoning. Psychiatry Research, 145, 95–103. 491–504.
Brown, L. H., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). When the need to Kwapil, T. R. (1998). Social anhedonia as a predictor of the development of schizo-
belong goes wrong: the expression of social anhedonia and social anxiety in phrenia-spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 558–565.
daily life. Psychological Science, 18, 778–782. Kwapil, T. R., & Diaz, M. A. (2000). Development of a new prospective study of risk
Brüne, M. (2005a). ‘‘Theory of Mind’’ in schizophrenia: a review of the literature. for schizophrenia-spectrum disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 41, 178.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 21–42. Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (1999). Mentalising, schizotypy and schizophrenia.
Brüne, M. (2005b). Emotion recognition, ‘‘theory of mind’’, and social behaviour in Cognition, 71, 43–71.
schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 133, 135–147. Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Visual perspective-taking and schizotypy:
Brüne, M. (2006). Theory of mind and social competence in schizophrenia. Clinical evidence for a simulation-based account of mentalizing in normal adults.
Neuropsychiatry, 2, 132–138. Cognition, 82, 1–26.
Brüne, M., Abdel-Hamid, M., Lehmkämper, C., & Sonntag, C. (2007). Mental state Langdon, R., Coltheart, M., Ward, P. B., & Catts, S. V. (2001). Visual and cognitive
attribution, neurocognitive functioning, and psychopathology: what predicts poor perspective-taking impairments in schizophrenia: a failure of allocentric
social competence in schizophrenia best? Schizophrenia Research, 92, 151–159. simulation? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 6, 241–269.
Brüne, M., Brüne-Cohrs, U., McGrew, W. C., & Preuschoft, S. C. (2006). Psychopa- Lebreton, M., Barnes, A., Miettunen, J., Peltonen, L., Ridler, K., Veijola, J., et al. (2009).
thology in great apes: concepts, treatment options and possible homologies to The brain structural disposition to social interaction. European Journal of
human psychiatric disorders. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 30, Neuroscience, 29, 2247–2252.
1246–1259. McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2006). False belief
Chapman, J. P., Chapman, J. L., & Kwapil, T. R. (1995). Scales for the measurement of as a generalized operant. The Psychological Record, 56, 341–354.
schizotypy. In A. Raine, T. Lencz, & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Schizotypal personality McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Stewart, I., & Dymond, S. (2007).
(pp. 79–106). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Deictic relational complexity and the development of deception. The Psycho-
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., Kwapil, T. R., Eckblad, M., & Zinser, M. C. (1994). logical Record, 57, 517–531.
Putatively psychosis-prone subjects 10 years later. Journal of Abnormal Monestès, J. L., Villatte, M., Moore, A., Yon, V., & Loas, G. (2007). Decisions in
Psychology, 103, 171–183. conditional situation and theory of mind. L’Encéphale, 34, 116–122.
Collins, L. M., Blanchard, J. J., & Biondo, K. M. (2005). Behavioral signs of schizoı̈dia O’Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2004). Derived relational
and schizotypy in social anhedonics. Schizophrenia Research, 78, 309–322. networks and control by novel instructions: a possible model of generative
de Villiers, J. (2000). Language and theory of mind: what are the developmental verbal responding. The Psychological Record, 54, 437–460.
relationships. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Under- O’Hora, D., Roche, B., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2002). Response latencies
standing other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience to multiple derived stimulus relations: testing two predictions of relational
(2nd ed.). (pp. 83–123) Oxford: Oxford University Press. frame theory. The Psychological Record, 52, 51–76.
de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: a longitudinal Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. (1989). Exploration of the autistic
study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-under- child’s theory of mind: knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Develop-
standing. Cognitive Development, 17, 1037–1060. ment, 60, 689–700.
Dilk, M. N., & Bond, G. R. (1996). A meta-analytic evaluation of skills training Pickup, G. J. (2008). Relationship between theory of mind and executive function in
research for individuals with severe mental illness. Journal of Consulting and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Psychopathology, 41, 206–213.
Clinical Psychology, 64, 1337–1346. Platek, S. M., Critton, S. R., Myers, T. E., & Gallup, G. G. (2003). Contagious yawning:
Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1995). A transformation of self-discrimination response the role of self-awareness and mental state attribution. Cognitive Brain Research,
functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, 17, 223–227.
134 M. Villatte et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 125–134

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Sprong, M., Schothorst, P., Vos, E., Hox, J., & Van Engeland, H. (2007). Theory of mind
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526. in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 5–13.
Raven, J. C. (1960). Standard progressive matrices. London: Lewis. Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2004). A functional-analytic model of analogy
Rehfeldt, R. A., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). Derived relational responding applica- using the relational evaluation procedure. The Psychological Record, 54, 531–552.
tions for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities. New Tallent, K. A., & Gooding, D. C. (1999). Working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting
Harbinger Publications. Test performance in schizotypic individuals: a replication and extension.
Rehfeldt, R. A., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M. M., & Kowalchuk, R. K. (2007). Assessing Psychiatry Research, 89, 161–170.
relational learning deficits in perspective-taking in children with high-func- Villatte, M., Monestès, J. L., McHugh, L., & Freixa i Baqué, E. (2009). Studying theory
tioning autism spectrum disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23–47. of mind through relational frame theory: a review of the literature on deictic
Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1996). Arbitrarily applicable relational responding and relational responding. Acta Comportamentalia, 17, 117–136.
sexual categorization: a critical test of the derived difference relation. The Villatte, M., Monestès, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2008).
Psychological Record, 46, 451–475. Assessing deictic relational responding in social anhedonia: a functional
Roncone, R., Faloon, I. R., Mazza, M., De Risio, A., Pollice, R., Necozione, S., et al. approach to the development of theory of mind impairments. International
(2002). Is theory of mind in schizophrenia more strongly associated with Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 4, 360–373.
clinical and social functioning than with neurocognitive deficits? Psychopa- Villatte, M., Monestès, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G . Assessing
thology, 35, 280–288. perspective taking in schizophrenia using Relational Frame Theory. The
Sarfati, Y., & Hardy-Baylé, M. C. (1999). How do people with schizophrenia explain the Psychological Record, in press.
behaviour of others? A study of theory of mind and relationship to thought and Weil, T. M., Hayes, S. C., & Capurro, P. The impact of training deictic frames on
speech disorganization in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 29, 613–620. perspective taking in children: a relational frame approach to theory of mind.
Schenkel, L. S., Spaulding, W. D., & Silverstein, S. M. (2005). Poor premorbid social Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis
functioning and theory of mind deficit in schizophrenia: evidence of reduced Conference, 2007, San Diego, California.
context processing? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39, 499–508. Wellman, H., Cross, D., & Watson, J. K. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind
Scott, S. (2001). Chimpanzee theory of mind: a proposal from the armchair. Carleton development: the truth about false-beliefs. Child Development, 72, 655–684.
University Cognitive Science Technical Report, 2001–2006. Available from. World Health Organization. (1992). ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural
www.Carleton.ca/iis/Techreports. disorders. Geneva: World Health Organization.

You might also like