Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 54

Asking Questions in Biology A Guide to

Hypothesis Testing Experimental


Design and Presentation in Practical
Work and Research Projects 5th Edition
Chris Barnard
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://textbookfull.com/product/asking-questions-in-biology-a-guide-to-hypothesis-tes
ting-experimental-design-and-presentation-in-practical-work-and-research-projects-5t
h-edition-chris-barnard/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Interior Design Visual Presentation a Guide to Graphics


Models and Presentation Techniques 4th Edition Mitton

https://textbookfull.com/product/interior-design-visual-
presentation-a-guide-to-graphics-models-and-presentation-
techniques-4th-edition-mitton/

Communication Research Asking Questions Finding Answers


Joann Keyton

https://textbookfull.com/product/communication-research-asking-
questions-finding-answers-joann-keyton/

Practical Cloud Security A Guide for Secure Design and


Deployment 1st Edition Chris Dotson

https://textbookfull.com/product/practical-cloud-security-a-
guide-for-secure-design-and-deployment-1st-edition-chris-dotson/

The good research guide for small scale research


projects 5th Edition Denscombe

https://textbookfull.com/product/the-good-research-guide-for-
small-scale-research-projects-5th-edition-denscombe/
Introduction to Robust Estimation and Hypothesis
Testing 4th Edition Rand Wilcox

https://textbookfull.com/product/introduction-to-robust-
estimation-and-hypothesis-testing-4th-edition-rand-wilcox/

How to talk to angels a practical guide to asking for


guidance comfort strength First Edition Gabriel

https://textbookfull.com/product/how-to-talk-to-angels-a-
practical-guide-to-asking-for-guidance-comfort-strength-first-
edition-gabriel/

Philosophy - Asking questions Seeking Answers F3Thinker


!

https://textbookfull.com/product/philosophy-asking-questions-
seeking-answers-f3thinker/

Design A Practical Guide to RIBA Plan of Work 2013


Stages 2 and 3 RIBA Stage Guide 1st Edition Tim Bailey

https://textbookfull.com/product/design-a-practical-guide-to-
riba-plan-of-work-2013-stages-2-and-3-riba-stage-guide-1st-
edition-tim-bailey/

Groups A Guide to Small Group Work in Healthcare


Management Education and Research 1st Edition Glyn
Elwyn

https://textbookfull.com/product/groups-a-guide-to-small-group-
work-in-healthcare-management-education-and-research-1st-edition-
glyn-elwyn/
A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 1 10/27/16 6:45 PM
Asking Questions in Biology

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 1 19/10/2016 16:23


Chapter 2 Asking Questions

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 2 19/10/2016 16:23


Asking Questions in Biology
A Guide to Hypothesis Testing, Experimental Design
and Presentation in Practical Work and Research Projects

Fifth Edition

Chris Barnard, Francis Gilbert


School of Biology, University of Nottingham

and Peter McGregor


Cornwall College, Newquay

Harlow, England • London • New York • Boston • San Francisco • Toronto • Sydney • Dubai • Singapore • Hong Kong
Tokyo • Seoul • Taipei • New Delhi • Cape Town • Sao Paulo • Mexico City • Madrid • Amsterdam • Munich • Paris • Milan

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 3 19/10/2016 16:23


PEARSON EDUCATION LIMITED
Edinburgh Gate
Harlow CM20 2JE
United Kingdom
Tel: +44 (0)1279 623623
Web: www.pearson.com/uk

First published under the Longman imprint 1993 (print)


Second edition 2001 (print)
Third edition published 2007 (print)
Fourth edition published 2011 (print)
Fifth edition published 2017 (print and electronic)

© Longman Group Limited 1993 (print)


© Pearson Education Limited 2001, 2007, 2011 (print)
© Pearson Education Limited 2017 (print and electronic)

The rights of Chris Barnard, Francis Gilbert and Peter McGregor to be identified as authors of this work have been asserted by them in
accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.

The print publication is protected by copyright. Prior to any prohibited reproduction, storage in a retrieval system, distribution or
transmission in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording or otherwise, permission should be obtained from the
publisher or, where applicable, a licence permitting restricted copying in the United Kingdom should be obtained from the Copyright
Licensing Agency Ltd, Barnard’s Inn, 86 Fetter Lane, London EC4A 1EN.

The ePublication is protected by copyright and must not be copied, reproduced, transferred, distributed, leased, licensed or publicly
performed or used in any way except as specifically permitted in writing by the publishers, as allowed under the terms and conditions
under which it was purchased, or as strictly permitted by applicable copyright law. Any unauthorised distribution or use of this text may
be a direct infringement of the authors’ and the publisher’s rights and those responsible may be liable in law accordingly.

The screenshots in this book are reprinted by permission of Microsoft Corporation.

Pearson Education is not responsible for the content of third-party internet sites.

ISBN: 978-1-292-08599-9 (Print)


978-1-292-08602-6 (PDF)
978-1-292-13239-6 (ePub)

British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data


A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data


Names: Barnard, C. J. (Christopher J.), author. | Gilbert, Francis S. (Francis Sylvest), 1956- , author. | McGregor, Peter, author.
Title: Asking questions in biology : a guide to hypothesis testing, experimental design and presentation in practical work and research
projects / Chris Barnard, Francis Gilbert, School of Biology, University of Nottingham, Peter McGregor, Cornwall College, Newquay.
Description: New York : Pearson Education, 2017. | Includes bibliographical references and index.
Identifiers: LCCN 2016040750| ISBN 9781292085999 (Print) | ISBN 9781292086026 (PDF) | ISBN 9781292132396 (ePub)
Subjects: LCSH: Science–Methodology. | Biology–Methodology.
Classification: LCC Q175 .B218 2016 | DDC 507.2/4–dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2016040750

10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
21 20 19 18 17

Print edition typeset in Times NR MT Pro 10/12.5 pts by SPi Global


Printed and bound in Malaysia

NOTE THAT ANY PAGE CROSS REFERENCES REFER TO THE PRINT EDITION

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 4 19/10/2016 16:23


Contents

Preface vii

1 Doing science:
Where do questions come from? 1
1.1 Science as asking questions 2
1.2 Basic considerations 3
1.3 The skill of asking questions 9
1.4 Where do questions come from? 11
1.5 What this book is about 16

References 17

2 Asking questions:
The art of framing hypotheses and predictions 18
2.1 Observation 18
2.2 Exploratory analysis 23
2.3 Forming hypotheses 46
2.4 Summary 54

References 54

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 5 19/10/2016 16:23


vi Contents

3 Answering questions:
What do the results say? 55
3.1 Confirmatory analysis 55
3.2 What is statistical significance? 57
3.3 Significance tests 60
3.4 Testing hypotheses 129
3.5 Testing predictions 135
3.6 Refining hypotheses and predictions 141
3.7 Summary 147

References 148

4 Presenting information:
How to communicate outcomes and conclusions 150
4.1 Presenting figures and tables 150
4.2 Presenting results in the text 163
4.3 Writing reports 164
4.4 Writing for a more general readership 179
4.5 Presenting in person: spoken papers and poster presentations 181
4.6 Plagiarism 194
4.7 Summary 195

Reference 196

Test finder and help guide 197


Some self-test questions 200
Appendix I: Table of confidence limits to the median 206
Appendix II: How to calculate some simple significance tests 207
Appendix III: Significance tables 226
Appendix IV: The common codes for the important graphical
parameters of R® 233
Answers to self-test questions 234

Index 241
Quick test finders 249

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 6 19/10/2016 16:23


Preface

Science is a process of asking questions, in most cases precise, quantitative


questions that allow distinctions to be drawn between alternative explanations
of events. Asking the right questions in the right way is a fundamental skill in
scientific enquiry, yet in itself it receives surprisingly little explicit attention in
scientific training. Students being trained in scientific subjects, for instance in sixth
forms, colleges and universities, learn the factual science and some of the tools of
enquiry such as laboratory techniques, mathematics, statistics and computing, but
they are taught little about the process of question-asking itself.
The first edition of this book had its origins in a first-year undergraduate
practical course that we, and others since, ran at the University of Nottingham
for many years. The approach adopted there now also forms the basis for a more
advanced second-year course. It is also the approach for FdSc- and BSc-level
research methods courses at Cornwall College Newquay. The aim of all these
courses is to introduce students in the biological sciences to the skills of observation
and enquiry, but focusing on the process of enquiry – how to formulate hypotheses
and predictions from raw information, how to design critical observations and
experiments, and how to choose appropriate analyses – rather than on laboratory,
field and analytical techniques per se. This focus is maintained in the fifth edition.
However, as in previous editions, we have responded to a number of positive
suggestions from people who have used the book, either as teachers or as students,
which we think enhance further its usefulness in teaching practical biology generally.
Again, the largest change has been with respect to the presentation of statistical
tests. In the fourth edition we replaced the previous boxes based on the procedures
and output of the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS®) with the
freeware package ‘R®’, obtainable from www.r-project.org. This collaborative
project of biologists and statisticians worldwide is now pretty much the industry
standard in biology. It also has the enormous benefit of an extensive user base who
can provide advice rapidly when you hit a snag.
Thus in the fifth edition we have decided to base the analyses on R® alone,
to encourage undergraduates to use it. Unlike most modern programs, R® is not
a ‘point-and-click’ process, but you have to write the commands in yourself. This
means you need to know what you are doing, otherwise you will not get any
meaningful results. Several colleagues have suggested that this will be a barrier to
modern undergraduates who have become used to simpler ways of obtaining output

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 7 19/10/2016 16:23


viii Preface

from such programs. However, our experience teaching it to undergraduates shows


that they learn it almost as quickly as any other package, and so the ‘barrier’ is not
as large as many think. And using R® will be much more useful to students in the
longer term.
The book website (www.pearsoned.co.uk/barnard) contains instructions for
using SPSS®, Minitab®, Genstat® and the Excel®-based AQB package from the
fourth edition, as well as data files. The formulae and hand calculations remain
because, as previously, we consider it important that the underlying arithmetic of
the tests is understood.
The range of tests remains as in the fourth edition, i.e. it includes, among other
things, repeated-measures designs, analysis of covariance, multiple regression and
principal components analysis. This is a wider spectrum of designs than is likely to
be encountered in a first course in experimental design, but caters for many kinds
of data collected in field-course and final-year projects. The Test Finder, Quick
Test Finder and Help sections should enable students to find what test they need
to carry out, while simultaneously underlining the principles involved.
With increasing emphasis on the wider communication and public understanding
of science, we have retained the sections on presenting information. We include
giving talks or presenting posters at scientific meetings, and writing for broader
non-specialist readers such as newspapers and magazines. To the sections on using
online literature databases such as the World of Knowledge, we have added some
practical tips on how to extract the information you require from the literature
such searches throw up. We have retained sections on the ethical implications of
working with biological material, now an essential consideration in any study as
legal regulation of biological experiments in both teaching and research becomes
ever more stringent.
The book looks at the process of enquiry during its various stages, starting with
unstructured observations and working through to the production of a complete
written report. In each section, different skills are emphasised and a series of main
examples runs through the book to illustrate their application at each stage.
The book begins with a look at scientific question-asking in general. How
do we arrive at the right questions to ask? What do we have to know before
we can ask sensible questions? How should questions be formulated to be
answered most usefully? Chapter 1 addresses these points by looking at the
development of testable hypotheses and predictions and the sources from
which they might arise.
Chapter 2 looks at how hypotheses and predictions can be derived from
unstructured observational notes. Exploratory analysis is an important first
step in deriving hypotheses from raw data, and the chapter introduces plots and
summary statistics as useful ways of identifying interesting patterns on which to
base hypotheses. The chapter concludes by pointing out that although hypotheses
and their predictions are naturally specific to the investigation in hand, testable
predictions in general fall into two distinct groups: those dealing with some kind of
difference between groups of data, and those dealing with a trend in the quantitative
relationship between groups of data.

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 8 19/10/2016 16:23


Preface ix

The distinction between difference and trend predictions is developed further


in Chapter 3, which discusses the use of confirmatory analyses. The concept of
statistical significance is introduced as an arbitrary but generally agreed yardstick as
to whether observed differences or trends are interesting, and a number of broadly
applicable significance tests are explained. Throughout, however, the emphasis is
on the use of such tests as tools of enquiry rather than on the statistical theory
underlying them. Having introduced significance tests and some potential pitfalls
in their use, the book uses the main worked examples to show how some of their
predictions can be tested and hypotheses refined in the light of testing.
In Chapter 4, the book considers the presentation of information. Once
hypotheses have been tested, how should the outcome be conveyed for greatest
effect? The chapter discusses the use of tables, figures and other modes of
presentation, and shows how a written report should be structured. The chapter
then moves on to consider the presentation of material in spoken and poster paper
formats, and how to recast written reports of results for a general, rather than
specialist, readership.
At the end of the book are a number of appendices. These provide expanded
self-test questions and answers sections based on the material in the previous
chapters and some statistical tables for use in significance testing.
We said that the book had its inception in our introductory practical course.
This course was developed in response to an increasingly voiced need on the part
of students to be taught how to formulate hypotheses and predictions clearly and
thus design properly discriminating experiments and observations. As we descend
into ever more neurotically prescribed teaching and assessment procedures, the
need for students to be given clear guidance on such aspects of their work becomes
correspondingly greater. As always, both our practical teaching and the book have
continued to benefit enormously from our ongoing and enjoyable interaction with
our undergraduates. Their insights and enquiries continue to hone the way we
teach, and have been the guiding force behind all the discussions in the book.
Finally, we should like to thank all the people who have commented on the
book since its first appearance, and encouraged us to think about the further
amendments we have made in this present edition. We particularly thank Kelly
Haynes and Angus Jackson and the 2013 and 2014 cohorts of BSc Applied
Zoology students at Newquay for input to the ‘Getting key information from
papers quickly’ section. In particular, we thank Tom Reader for the generous
amount of time he has spent discussing the book with us, commenting on drafts
of many of the amendments and giving freely of his experience in and enthusiasm
for the business of communicating science. Rufus Curnow at Pearson Education
encouraged us to produce a fifth edition, and guided us as to how it should be
modified. James Gilbert and Lucy Browning made useful changes to the R®
descriptions.

Francis Gilbert
Peter McGregor
January 2016

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 9 19/10/2016 16:23


x Preface

Publisher’s Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the following for permission to reproduce copyright material:

Figures

Figure 2.7 and Figure 3.3 modified from Choosing and Using Statistics: A
Biologist’s Guide 3 ed., Blackwell (Dytham, C. 2010) pp. 54 and 41; Reproduced
with permission of Wiley-Blackwell in the format Educational/Instructional
Program via Copyright Clearance Center.

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 10 19/10/2016 16:23


Dedicated to the memory of Chris Barnard

Chris Barnard succeeded in completing the third edition before his


untimely death in June 2007 at the age of 55. We dedicate this
latest edition to his memory: his talents as a scientist, educator and
communicator of science, raconteur, poet, artist and musician are
greatly missed.

A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 11 19/10/2016 16:23


A01_BARN5999_05_SE_FM.indd 12 19/10/2016 16:23
1 Doing science
Where do questions come from?

You’re out for a walk one autumn afternoon when you notice a squirrel picking up
acorns under some trees. Several things strike you about the squirrel’s behaviour.
For one thing it doesn’t seem to pick up all the acorns it comes across; a sizeable
proportion is ignored. Of those it does pick up, only some are eaten. Others are
carried up into a tree, where the squirrel disappears from view for a few minutes
before returning to the supply for more. Something else strikes you: the squirrel
doesn’t carry its acorns up the nearest tree but instead runs to one several metres
away. You begin to wonder why the squirrel behaves in this way. Several possibilities
occur to you. Although the acorns on the ground all look very similar to you, you
speculate that some might contain more food than others, or perhaps they are
easier to crack. By selecting these, the squirrel might obtain food more quickly
than by taking indiscriminately any acorn it encountered. Similarly, the fact that
it appears to carry acorns into a particular tree suggests this tree might provide a
more secure site for storing them.
While all these might be purely casual reflections, they are revealing of the way
we analyse and interpret events around us. The speculations about the squirrel’s
behaviour may seem clutched out of the air on a whim, but they are in fact
structured around some clearly identifiable assumptions, for instance that achieving
a high rate of food intake matters in some way to the squirrel and influences its
preferences, and that using the most secure storage site is more important to it than
using the most convenient site. If you wanted to pursue your curiosity further, these
assumptions would be critical to the questions you asked and the investigations
you undertook. If all this sounds very familiar to you as a science student, it
should, because, whether you intended it or not, your speculations are essentially
scientific. Science is simply formalised speculation backed up (or otherwise) by
equally formalised observation and experimentation. In its broadest sense most of
us ‘do science’ all the time.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 1 19/10/2016 14:54


2 Chapter 1 Doing Science

1.1 Science as asking questions

Science is often regarded by those outside it as an open-ended quest for objective


understanding of the universe and all that is in it. But this is so only in a rather trivial
sense. The issue of objectivity is a thorny one and, happily, well beyond the scope
of this book. Nevertheless, the very real constraints that limit human objectivity
mean that use of the term must at least be hedged about with serious qualifications.
The issue of open-endedness is really the one that concerns us here. Science is open-
ended only in that its directions of enquiry are, in principle, limitless. Along each
path of enquiry, however, science is far from open-ended. Each step on the way is,
or should be, the result of refined question-asking, a narrowing down of questions
and methods of answering them to provide the clearest possible distinction between
alternative explanations for the phenomenon in hand. This is a skill, or series of
skills really, that has to be acquired, and acquiring it is one of the chief objectives
of any scientific training.
While few scientists would disagree with this, identifying the different skills and
understanding how training techniques develop them are a lot less straightforward.
With increasing pressure on science courses in universities and colleges to teach
more material to more people and to draw on an expanding and increasingly
sophisticated body of knowledge, it is more important than ever to understand
how to marshal information and direct enquiry. This book is the result of our
experiences in teaching investigative skills to university undergraduates in the
life sciences. It deals with all aspects of scientific investigation, from thinking
up ideas and making initial exploratory observations, through developing
and testing hypotheses, to interpreting results and preparing written reports.
It is not an introduction to data-handling techniques or statistics, although it
includes a substantial element of both; it simply introduces these as tools to aid
investigation. The theory and mechanics of statistical analysis can be dealt with
more appropriately elsewhere.
The principles covered in the book are extraordinarily simple, yet,
paradoxically, students find them very difficult to put into practice when taught
in a piecemeal way across a number of different courses. The book has evolved
out of our attempts to get over this problem by using open-ended, self-driven
practical exercises in which the stages of enquiry develop logically through the
desire of students to satisfy their own curiosity. However, the skills it emphasises
are just as appropriate to more limited set-piece practicals. Perhaps a distinction
– admittedly over-generalised – that could be made here, and which to some
extent underpins our preference for a self-driven approach, is that with many
set-piece practicals it is obvious what one is supposed to do but often not why
one is supposed to do it. Almost the opposite is true of the self-driven approach;
here it is clear why any investigation needs to be undertaken, but usually less clear
what should be done to see it through successfully. In our experience, developing
the ‘what’ in the context of a clear ‘why’ is considerably more instructive than
attempting to reconstruct the ‘why’ from the ‘what’ or, worse, ignoring it
altogether.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 2 19/10/2016 14:54


1.2 Basic Considerations 3

1.2 Basic considerations

Scientific enquiry is not just a matter of asking questions; it is a matter of asking


the right questions in the right way. This is more demanding than it sounds. For a
start, it requires that something is known about the system or material in which
an investigator is interested. A study of mating behaviour in guppies, for instance,
demands that you can at least tell males from females and recognise courtship and
copulation. Similarly, it is difficult to make a constructive assessment of parasitic
worm burdens in host organisms if you are ignorant of likely sites of infection and
can’t tell worm eggs from faecal material.
Of course, there are several ways in which such knowledge can be acquired: e.g.
the Internet/World Wide Web, textbooks, specialist academic journals (mostly now
available electronically through licensed subscribers like universities and colleges, or
free on the Internet), asking an expert, or simply finding out for yourself through
observation and exploration.
These days, the first choice for browsing information is often the Internet/
World Wide Web. The advantages of such ‘online’ searching in terms of speed and
convenience hardly need detailing here, but there are dangers, as we indicate later. A
good way of accessing reliable scientific information like this is to use one or more
of the professional Web-based literature databases, such as the Web of Knowledge
(http://wok.mimas.ac.uk), PubMed (www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/), Google Scholar
(http://scholar.google.co.uk) or BIOSIS. These search the peer-reviewed (and
therefore quality-controlled) academic journals for articles containing information
relevant to your request. Each of these provides tips on how best to use them, but a
handful of basic ones is given in Box 1.1.
Whichever mode of acquiring information is preferred, however, a certain
amount of background preparation is usually essential, even for the simplest
investigations. In practical classes, some background is usually provided for you
in the form of handouts or accompanying lectures, but the very variability of
biological material means that generalised and often highly stylised summaries are
poor substitutes for hard personal experience. Nevertheless, given the inevitable
constraints of time, materials and available expertise, they are usually a necessary
second best. There is also a second, more important, reason why there is really no
substitute for personal experience: the information you require may not exist or, if it
does exist, it may not be correct. The Internet/World Wide Web is a particular hazard
here because of the vast amount of unregulated information it makes available,
often dressed up to appear professional and authoritative. Such material should
always be treated with caution and verified before being trusted. Where academic
information is concerned, a first step might be to check the host site to see whether
it is a recognised institution, like a university or an academic publisher; another
might be to look for other research cited in the information, for instance in the form
of journal citations (see section 4.3.1), which can be cross-checked. Entering the
author’s name into the search field of one of the web-based professional literature
databases (Box 1.1) to see whether this person has a published research track record
can be another approach. Using general-purpose search engines, like Yahoo! or

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 3 19/10/2016 14:54


4 Chapter 1 Doing Science

BOX 1.1 Searching online literature databases

Searchable online literature databases, like the However, the process inevitably involves some
Web of Knowledge, Google Scholar, BIOSIS compromises.
or PubMed, allow you to search for articles For example, suppose you were interested
by particular authors, or on particular topics, in steroid hormone secretion as a cause of
or according to some other category, such as immune depression in laboratory mice. You
a journal title or research organisation. An might start, seemingly reasonably, by typing
example of the kind of search fields on offer, ‘steroid hormone AND immune depression
in this case for the Web of Knowledge, is shown AND laboratory mice’ into the ‘Topic’ search
in Fig. (i). field in Fig. (i) and hitting the ‘Search’ button.
The key to using the search fields effectively Disappointingly, and rather to your surprise,
lies in the precision with which you specify this yields nothing at all – apparently nobody
your terms: too general and you will be has published anything on steroid hormones
swamped with articles that are of little or no and immune depression in mice. At the
interest; too narrow and you will wind up with other extreme, a search for ‘immune AND
only one or two and miss many important mice’ yields over 45,000 articles, a wholly
ones. To help with this, the search fields unmanageable number, of which many can
provide various means of linking terms so be seen at a glance to be irrelevant to your
that searches can be focused (the AND, OR, needs. Clearly, something between the two is
NOT options – called ‘operators’ – in Fig. (i)). what is required.

Figure (i) A screen capture from the Web of Knowledge as it appeared in 2016. Like other similar sites, it is
regularly updated, so the exact appearance of the search field screen may change.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 4 19/10/2016 14:54


1.2 Basic Considerations 5

The reason the first search turned up phrase are recognised, in this case, say, ‘immune
nothing is not, of course, because nobody depression’, ‘depressed immunity’ or ‘depressed
has published anything on the topic, but immune response’. Running the search again
because the search term was restricted to in this form yields around 450 articles, much
a very specific combination of phrases. It better than zero or 45,000, but with quite
could well be that people have published on a lot of them still redundant. If the search
the effects of steroid hormones on immune is specified a little more tightly as ‘immun*
depression in mice but didn’t use the precise SAME depress* AND mice AND hormone’,
phrases selected. For instance, they may have however, it turns up around 40 articles, and all
reported ‘depressed immune responsiveness’ much more on target.
or ‘depressed immunity’, rather than ‘immune All the searchable databases use these kinds
depression’, and referred to specific hormones, of approaches for refining searches, some
such as testosterone or cortisol, rather than the very intuitive, some less so. One thing you
generic term ‘steroid’. There are various ways will quickly notice, though, is that exactly the
of catering for this. In the Web of Knowledge, same search can turn up a different number
the form ‘immun* SAME depress* AND mice’ and selection of articles depending on which
in the ‘Topic’ search field allows the system to database you are using – BIOSIS, for example,
search for any term beginning with ‘immun’ manages to find something under the initial
or ‘depress’, such as ‘immune’, ‘immunity’, over-specific search that drew a blank on
‘immunocompetence’, ‘depression’, ‘depressed’ the Web of Knowledge. For this reason, it is
and so on, thus picking up all the variants. The good practice to run searches on a selection
term ‘SAME’ ensures similar combinations of of databases.

Google, can often turn up information from the professional literature too, but
just as often you’re likely to get information from unregulated personal websites,
or other sources of uncertain provenance. Taking received wisdom at face value
can be a dangerous business – something even seasoned researchers can continue
to discover, the famous geneticist and biostatistician R®. A. Fisher among them.
In the early 1960s, Fisher and other leading authorities at the time were
greatly impressed by an apparent relationship between duodenal ulcer and certain
rhesus and MN blood groups. Much intellectual energy was expended trying to
explain the relationship. A sceptic, however, mentioned the debate to one of his
blood-group technicians. The technician, for years at the sharp end of blood-
group analysis, resolved the issue on the spot. The relationship was an artefact
of blood transfusion! Patients with ulcers had received transfusions because of
haemorrhage. As a result, they had temporarily picked up rhesus and MN antigens
from their donors. When patients who had not been given transfusions were tested,
the relationship disappeared (Clarke, 1990).
Where at all feasible, therefore, testing assumptions yourself and making up
your own mind about the facts available to you is a good idea. Indeed, science
is often characterised as systematic scepticism – a demand for evidence for every
assertion. It is impossible to draw up a definitive list of what it is an investigator
needs to know as essential background; biology is too diverse a subject, and every
investigation is to some extent unique in its factual requirements. Nevertheless, it

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 5 19/10/2016 14:54


6 Chapter 1 Doing Science

is useful to indicate the kinds of information that are likely to be important. Some
examples might be as follows:
Question Can the material of interest be studied usefully under laboratory conditions or will
unavoidable constraints or manipulations so affect it that any conclusions will have
only dubious relevance to its normal state or functions?
For instance, can mating preferences in guppies usefully be studied in a small plastic
aquarium, or will the inevitable restriction on movement and the impoverished
environment compromise normal courtship activity?
Or, if nutrient transfer within a plant can be monitored only with the aid of
a vital dye, will normal function be maintained in the dyed state or will the dye
interfere subtly with the processes of interest?
Question Is the material at the appropriate stage of life history or development for the desired
investigation?
There would, for instance, be little point in carrying out vaginal smears on female
mice to establish stages of the oestrous cycle if some females were less than 28 days
of age. Such mice may well not have begun cycling.
Likewise, it would be fruitless to monitor the faeces of infected mice for the eggs
of a nematode worm until a sufficient number of days have passed after infection
for the worms to have matured.

Question Will the act of recording from the material affect its performance?

For example, removing a spermatophore (package of sperm donated by the male)


from a recently mated female cricket in order to assay its sperm content may
­adversely affect the female’s response to males in the future.
Or, the introduction of an intracellular probe might disrupt the aspect of cell
physiology it was intended to record.

Question Has the material been prepared properly?

If the problem to be investigated involves a foraging task (e.g. learning to find


cryptic prey), has the subject been trained to perform in the apparatus and has it
been deprived of food for a short while to make it hungry?
Similarly, if a mouse of strain X is to be infected with a particular blood
­parasite so that the course of infection can be monitored, has the parasite been
passaged in the strain long enough to ensure its establishment and survival in the
experiment?

Question Does the investigation make demands on the material that it is not capable of meeting?

Testing for the effects of acclimation on some measure of coping in a new


environment might be compromised if conditions in the new environment are
beyond those the organism’s physiology or behaviour have evolved to meet.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 6 19/10/2016 14:54


1.2 Basic Considerations 7

Likewise, testing a compound from an animal’s environment for carcinogenic


properties in order to assess risk might not mean much if the compound is administered
in concentrations or via routes that the animal could never experience naturally.

Question Are assumptions about the material justified?

In an investigation of mating behaviour in dragonflies, we might consider using


the length of time a male and female remain coupled as an index of the amount of
sperm transferred by the male. Before accepting this, however, it would be wise to
conduct some pilot studies to make sure it was valid; it might be, for instance, that
some of the time spent coupled reflected mate-guarding rather than insemination.

By the same token, assumptions about the relationship between the staining
characteristics of cells in histological sections and their physiological properties
might need verifying before concluding anything about the distribution of
physiological processes within an organ.
The list could go on for a long time, but these examples are basic questions of
practicality. They are not very interesting in themselves but they, and others like
them, need to be addressed before interesting questions can be asked. Failure to
consider them will almost inevitably result in wasted time and materials.
Of course, even at this level, the investigator will usually have the questions
ultimately to be addressed – the whole point of the investigation – in mind, and
these will naturally influence initial considerations. Before we develop this further,
however, there is one further, and increasingly prominent, issue we must address,
and that is the ethics of working with biological material.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 7 19/10/2016 14:54


8 Chapter 1 Doing Science

1.2.1 Ethical considerations


Because biological material is either living, or was once living, or is derived from
something that is or was living, we are sensitive to the possibility that another
living organism may be harmed in some way as a result of what we are doing. Of
particular concern is the possibility that our activity might cause such an organism
to suffer, physically or psychologically. We try very hard to avoid suffering ourselves
because, by definition, it is extremely unpleasant, so the question arises as to
whether we should risk inflicting it on another living being simply because we are
interested in finding something out about it. This is not an easy question to answer,
not least because of the difficulty of knowing whether species very different from
ourselves, such as invertebrates, are capable of experiencing anything that might
reasonably be called suffering in the first place. However, good science is mindful
of the possibility, and works to various guidelines and codes of practice, some
enforced by law, to give organisms the benefit of the doubt. While minimising
the risk of suffering is important in itself, there is also a straightforward practical
reason why we should take care of the organisms we use, whatever they may be,
since any results we obtain from them could be affected if the organism is damaged
or in some way below par.
Suffering may not be the only potential ethical concern. If material is coming
from the field, for example, there could be conservation issues. Is the species
concerned endangered? Is the habitat it occupies fragile? Are there unwelcome
consequences for populations or habitats of removing material and/or returning
it afterwards? Questions like this can lead to acute dilemmas. For instance, the
fact that a species is becoming endangered may mean there is a desperate need for
more information about it, but the very means of acquiring the information risks
further harm.
As awareness of these issues increases, ethical considerations are beginning
to play a more explicit role in the way biologists approach their work, not
just in terms of taking greater care of the organisms they use, and being better
informed about their needs, but at the level of how investigations are designed
in the first place. Take sample size, for instance. Deciding on a suitable sample
size is a basic problem in any quantitative study. It might involve an informal
judgement on the basis of past experience or the outcome of other studies, or
it might depend on power tests (see section 3.4.1) to calculate a sample size
statistically. Where there are ethical concerns, a power test would arguably be
better than ‘guesstimation’ because it would provide an objective means of
maximising the likelihood of a meaningful result while minimising the amount
of material needed (a smaller sample would risk the outcome being swamped by
random noise, while a larger one would use more material than necessary). But,
of course, the ideal sample size indicated by the power test might demand more
material than can be sustained by the source, or involve a very large number of
animals in a traumatic experimental procedure. The value of proceeding then
has to be judged against the likely cost from an ethical perspective, a task with
considerable room for debate. Detailed discussion of these issues is beyond the
scope of this book, but a good idea of what is involved can be found in Bateson

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 8 19/10/2016 14:54


1.3 The Skill of Asking Questions 9

(1986, 2005), who provides a digestible introduction to trading off scientific


value and ethical concerns, and the extensive ethical guidelines for teachers and
researchers in animal behaviour published by the Association for the Study
of Animal Behaviour (ASAB) and its North American partner, the Animal
Behavior Society (ABS) (see www.asab.org or www.animalbehaviorsociety.org
.uk or each January issue of the academic journal Animal Behaviour). It is also
well worth looking at the website of the UK National Centre for the 3Rs (www
.nc3rs.org; the three Rs stand for the Replacement, Refinement and Reduction in
the use of animals in research), a government-funded organisation dedicated to
progressing ethical approaches to the use of animals in biology. For discussion of
more philosophical issues, see, for example, Dawkins (1980, 1993) and Barnard
& Hurst (1996). It is important to stress that, tricky as these kinds of decision
can be, ethical considerations should always be part of the picture when you are
working with biological material.

1.3 The skill of asking questions

1.3.1 Testing hypotheses


Charles Darwin once remarked that without a hypothesis a geologist might as
well go into a gravel pit and count the stones. He meant, of course, that simply
gathering facts for their own sake was likely to be a waste of time. A geologist is
unlikely to profit much from knowing the number of stones in a gravel pit. This
seems self-evident, but such undirected fact-gathering (not to be confused with the
often essential descriptive phase of hypothesis development) is a common problem
among students in practical and project work. There can’t be many science teachers
who have not been confronted by a puzzled student with the plea: ‘I’ve collected
all these data, now what do I do with them?’ The answer, obviously, is that the
investigator should know what is to be done with the data before they are collected.
As Darwin well knew, what gives data collection direction is a working hypothesis.
Theories and hypotheses are absolutely vital to science, otherwise ‘we shall all be
washed out to sea in an immense tide of unrelated information’ (Watt, 1971). With
them, ‘the enormous ballast of factual information, so far from being about to sink
us, is used to reveal patterns and processes so that we need no longer to record the
fall of every apple’ (Dixon, 2000).
The word ‘hypothesis’ sounds rather formal and, indeed, in some cases
hypotheses may be set out in a tightly constructed, formal way. In more general
usage, however, its meaning is a good deal looser. Verma & Beard (1981), for
example, define it as simply:

a tentative proposition which is subject to verification through subsequent


investigation. In many cases hypotheses are hunches that the researcher has
about the existence of relationships between variables.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 9 19/10/2016 14:54


10 Chapter 1 Doing Science

A hypothesis, then, can be little more than an intuitive feeling about how
s­ omething works, or how changes in one factor will relate to changes in another,
or about any aspect of the material of interest. However vague it may be, though,
it is formative in the purpose and design of investigations because these set out
to test it. If at the end of the day the results of the investigation are at odds with
the hypothesis, the hypothesis may be rejected and a new one put in its place. As
we shall see later, hypotheses are never proven, merely rejected if data from tests so
dictate, or retained for the time being for possible rejection after further tests.

1.3.2 How is a hypothesis tested?


If a hypothesis is correct, certain things will follow. Thus if our hypothesis is that
a particular visual display by a male chaffinch is sexual in motivation, we might
expect the male to be more likely to perform the display when a female is present.
Hypotheses thus generate predictions, the testing of which increases or decreases
our faith in them. If our male chaffinch turned out to display mainly when other
males were around and almost never with females, we might want to think again
about our sexual motivation hypothesis. However, we should be wrong to dismiss
it solely on these grounds. It could be that such displays are important in defending
a good-quality breeding territory that eventually will attract a female. The context
of the display could thus still be sexual, but in a less direct sense than we had
first considered. In this way, hypotheses can produce tiers of more and more
refined predictions before they are rejected or tentatively accepted. Making such
predictions is a skilled business because each must be phrased so that testing it
allows the investigator to discriminate in favour of or against the hypothesis. While
it is best to phrase predictions as just that (thus: males will perform more of display
y in the presence of females), they sometimes take the form of questions (do males
perform more of display y when females are present?). The danger with the question
format, however, is that it can easily become too woolly and vague to provide a
rigorous test of the hypothesis (e.g. do males behave differently when females are
present?). Having to phrase a precise prediction helps counteract the temptation
to drift into vagueness.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 10 19/10/2016 14:54


1.4 Where Do Questions Come From? 11

Hypotheses, too, can be so broad or imprecise that they are difficult to reject.
In general the more specific, mutually exclusive hypotheses that can be formulated
to account for an observation the better. In our chaffinch example, the first
hypothesis was that the display was sexual. Another might be that it reflected
aggressive defence of food. Yet another that it was an anti-predator display. These
three hypotheses give rise to very different predictions about the behaviour and it
is thus, in principle, easy to distinguish between them. As we have already seen,
however, distinguishing between the ‘sexual’ and ‘aggressive’ hypotheses may need
more careful consideration than we first expect. Straw man hypotheses are another
common problem. Unless some effort has gone into understanding the material,
there is a risk of setting up hypotheses that are completely inappropriate. Thus,
suggesting that our displaying chaffinch was demonstrating its freedom from avian
malaria would make little sense in an area where malaria was not endemic. We shall
look at the development of hypotheses and their predictions in more detail later on.

1.4 Where do questions come from?

As we have already intimated, questions do not spring out of a vacuum. They are
triggered by something. They may arise from a number of sources.

1.4.1 Curiosity
Questions arise naturally when thinking about any kind of problem. Simple
curiosity about how something works or why one group of organisms differs in
some way from another group can give rise to useful questions from which testable
hypotheses and their predictions can be derived. There is nothing wrong with
‘armchair theorising’ and ‘thought experiments’ as long as, where possible, they
are put to the test. Sitting in the bath and wondering about how migratory birds
manage to navigate, for example, could suggest roles for various environmental cues
like the sun, stars and topographical features. This in turn could lead to hypotheses
about how they are used and predictions about the effects of removing or altering
them. By the time the water was cold, some useful preliminary experiments might
even have been devised.

1.4.2 Casual observation


Instead of dreaming in the bath, you might be watching a tank full of fish, or
sifting through some histological preparations under a microscope. Various things
might strike you. Some fish in the tank might seem very aggressive, especially
towards others of their own species, but this aggressiveness might occur only
around certain objects in the tank, perhaps an overturned flowerpot or a clump of
weed. Similarly, certain cells in the histological preparations may show unexpected

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 11 19/10/2016 14:54


12 Chapter 1 Doing Science

differences in staining or structure. Even though these aspects of fish behaviour


and cell appearance were not the original reason for watching the fish or looking
at the slides, they might suggest interesting hypotheses for testing later. A plausible
hypothesis to account for the behaviour of the fish, for instance, is that the localised
aggression reflects territorial defence. Two predictions might then be: (a) on average,
territory defenders will be bigger than intruders (because bigger fish are more likely
to win in disputes and thus obtain a territory in the first place) and (b) removing
defendable resources like upturned flowerpots will lead to a reduction in aggressive
interactions. Similarly, a hypothesis for differences in cell staining and structure is
that they are due to differences in the age and development of the cells in question.
A prediction might then be: younger tissue will contain a greater proportion of (what
are conjectured to be the) immature cell types.

1.4.3 Exploratory observations


It may be that you already have a hypothesis in mind, say that a particular species
of fish will be territorial when placed in an appropriate aquarium environment.
What is needed is to decide what an appropriate aquarium environment might be
so that suitable predictions can be made to test the hypothesis. Obvious things to
do would be to play around with the size and number of shelters, the position and
quality of feeding sites, the number and sex ratio of fish introduced into the tank,
and so on. While the effects of these and other factors on territorial aggressiveness
among the fish might not have been guessed at beforehand, such manipulations
are likely to suggest relationships with aggressiveness that can then be used to
predict the outcome of further, independent investigations. Thus if exploratory
results suggested aggressiveness among defending fish was greater when there were
ten fish in the tank compared with when there were five, it would be reasonable
to predict that aggressiveness would increase as the number of fish increased, all
other things being equal. An experiment could then be designed in which shelters
and feeding sites were kept constant but different numbers of fish, say 2, 4, 6, 8, 10
or 15, were placed in the tank. Measuring the amount of aggression by a defender
with each number of fish would provide a test of the prediction.

1.4.4 Previous studies


One of the richest sources of questions is, of course, past and ongoing research.
This might be encountered either as published literature (see Box 1.1) or ‘live’ as
research talks at conferences or seminars. A careful reading of most published
papers, articles or books will turn up ideas for further work, whether at the level
of alternative hypotheses to explain the problem in hand or at the level of further
or more discriminating predictions to test the current hypothesis. Indeed, this is
the way most of the scientific literature develops. Some papers, often in the form
of mathematical models or speculative reviews, are specifically intended to generate
hypotheses and predictions and may make no attempt to test them themselves.
At times, certain research areas can become overburdened with hypotheses and
predictions, generating more than people are able or have the inclination to test.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 12 19/10/2016 14:54


1.4 Where Do Questions Come From? 13

If this happens, it can have a paralysing effect on the development of research.


It is thus important that hypotheses, predictions and tests proceed as nearly as is
feasible hand in hand.
Reading scientific literature, especially journal articles (‘papers’), is a bit of a
misnomer. Rather than reading from beginning to end as you would a novel or
text book chapter, reading a paper involves dipping into, probably skim-reading,
particular sections (Introduction, Methods, Results, Discussion – see section 4.3.1)
of the paper and deciding after each dip whether it is sufficiently relevant to the
information you are seeking to continue to the next step or whether you will move
on to another paper. Perhaps you will end up reading the whole of a particularly
relevant paper, but never in the order in which it is presented. The scientific
literature is so vast that you will need to acquire the skill of getting information
from a paper quickly – there is a real risk of being overwhelmed by material that
turns out not to be pertinent to your question. A series of approaches that we, and
our students, have found to be useful are detailed below.

Getting key information from papers quickly


We will illustrate the process of quickly getting key information from a paper by
referring to the example report in Box 4.2 which follows the format of a paper in a
journal such as Animal Behaviour. Imagine that this paper is one that seems relevant
to your question; perhaps it was turned up by a search (see Box 1.1), or perhaps it
has been cited in another paper.

First step: information – general or detailed


Decide whether you need general information on a topic or whether you need
information that is more detailed. (It is also worth keeping a note on the information
you require to hand, to cross check with information you come across.)
• General information. If you have just started researching a topic in the literature,
then you are most likely to require general information. For example, if the topic
of interest is aggressive behaviour, you could be seeking general information on
fighting behaviour including definitions, characteristics, experimental approaches
and why fighting is particularly interesting to the broader field of animal
behaviour.
• Detailed information. If you have acquired enough general information to be
planning a series of experiments to investigate fighting between males, then you
require detailed information. For example, you may be looking for information
on suitable study species for laboratory use (crickets?); or, having decided on
crickets, you need information on how to distinguish individual males (e.g. mark
them with dot of paint).

Second step: is the paper likely to contain the information needed?


If it is not clear from the title whether the paper will contain the information you
need, read the ‘Keywords’ (common in journal papers but not present in our example
paper) and/or skim-read the Abstract. This will allow you to decide whether to
continue or move on to the next paper. For example, if the Abstract makes it clear

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 13 19/10/2016 14:54


14 Chapter 1 Doing Science

that the paper is a review, then it should be a good source of general information,
but it is unlikely to contain detailed information on experimental details.

Third step: assess the likely quality and relevance of information.


This usually means skim-reading parts of the Methods and/or Results to establish
two key aspects of the paper:

1. Do the variables measured relate to the title and topic addressed?


Skim-read the Methods to find details of the variables measured. This step is
necessary because in biology it is often difficult to measure directly the feature
of interest. Instead we have to measure a variable we think is related to the real
variable of interest; the less closely linked the measured variable is to the real
variable of interest, the more likely that other factors will contribute to the
reported results.
The title of our example paper, ‘The effect of body size on the escalation of
aggressive encounters between male field crickets (Gryllus bimaculatus)’, includes
one variable of interest (body size) and hints at another (fight success). This makes
the topic of study clear and it is easy to decide that there is a close link between the
topic and the measured variables. If the paper had been called something catchier
but less specific, perhaps the poster title (p.188) ‘Know thine enemy: assessment
and fighting in male crickets’, we would have to dip into the Methods and Results
to discover the variables measured and to decide how closely linked they are to
assessment.

2. Do the results bear out the title and detail in the Abstract?
Skim-read the Results, particularly looking at the figures or tables because only
the main results are shown in these formats; look for p-values and details of the
statistical tests that produced them.
In our example paper, the three figures show the key results (large males
more likely to win, and to initiate fights; escalated encounters more likely when
opponents are of similar size) and the figure captions include p-values and details
of the statistical tests that produced them.
If the paper has passed the two assessments of quality/relevance in Step 3 (as
our example paper does), the next step depends on whether you are looking for
general or detailed information.

General info step: work through the paper.


This step involves reading and skim-reading sections of the paper. The more papers
you read, the more you will skim-read successive papers as most will have a lot of
general information in common and you will quickly recognise that you already
have the information. The best order to tackle the sections of a paper reporting
research (rather than a review) is generally as follows:
1. Introduction – to get the background to the study, the specific hypotheses/
questions being addressed and citations to related information.
2. Discussion – the first paragraph often summarises the key results and the rest
interprets the results and often includes suggestions for further study.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 14 19/10/2016 14:54


1.4 Where Do Questions Come From? 15

3. Results – to get information on the details of the results, pay particular


attention to the figures, tables and information on the statistical tests used.
4. Methods – for the details that underpin the relationships reported in the
Results.

Detailed info step 1: identify section of paper.


Detailed information is usually restricted to one section of the paper. Some
common examples are listed in the table below. Go to that section. (If you want
information on several of the items listed below, you are probably looking for
general information – follow the General info step.)

Specific information Section of paper


Detail of protocol and techniques Methods
Experimental design
Study site
Background on topic Introduction
Specific hypotheses/questions addressed
Related literature
Interpretation and context of results Discussion
Suggestions for further study
Key results in brief Discussion (first paragraph)
Detail of the results of the study Results (especially Figures & Tables)

Detailed info step 2: skim-read section.


Skim-read to identify where the relevant information is within the section. For
example, the information on marking male crickets is in the final sentence of the
first paragraph of the Methods of the example paper (p.174).
As you read more papers, each with some relevant information, the faster and
more extensive your skim-reading becomes as you will quickly recognise that you
already have that information. For example, you may have read several papers on
laboratory studies of male–male aggression in crickets, so you will have information
on housing conditions from all of these papers. However, if you are looking for
very detailed information, perhaps information on how experimenters manipulated
male confidence before encounters, this will only be found in a few papers that used
this experimental approach (e.g. Barnard & Burk, 1979).

Final step for general or detailed info: make notes!


Make a note of the information you found and its location – not just the citation
(we recommend using reference manager software for this), but the location
of the key sentence or paragraph in the paper. Some people write notes, some
prefer to highlight hard copies or pdfs. However you choose to do it, making a
note means you don’t have to repeat the process when you come to writing up
your study.

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 15 19/10/2016 14:54


16 Chapter 1 Doing Science

1.5 What this book is about

We’ve said a little about how science works and how the kind of question-asking on
which it is based can arise. We now need to look at each part of the process in detail,
because while each may seem straightforward in principle, some knotty problems
can arise when science is put into practice. In what follows, we shall see how to:
• frame hypotheses and predictions from preliminary source material;
• design experiments and observations to test predictions;
• analyse the results of tests to see whether they are consistent with our original
hypothesis; and
• present the results and conclusions of tests so that they are clear and informative.

The discussion deals with these aspects in order so that the book can be read
straight through or dipped into for particular points. A summary at the end of each
chapter highlights the important take-home messages, and the self-test questions at
the end show what you should be able to tackle after reading the book.
Remember, the book is about asking and answering questions in biology – it is
not a biology textbook or a statistics manual, and none of the points it makes are
restricted to the examples that illustrate them. At every stage you should be asking
yourself how what it says might apply in other biological contexts, especially if you
have an interest in investigating them!
We suggest you get hold of a statistical package in order to follow the analyses.
The one we use in detail in the book is R®. You can download the installation file
of R® free from www.r-project.org, and follow the simple instructions to install it
on your computer. This book will enable you to run analyses in R®; a simple and
very clear guide to learning more about R® is available in Zuur et al. (2009).

M01_BARN5999_05_SE_C01.indd 16 19/10/2016 14:54


Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
themselves are often protected with earth during winter.
In the south of Persia, and also in the north, the very slightest
cultivation is employed, yet in good years the harvests of every kind
are very heavy. In all cases it is the abundance, or the reverse, of the
supply of water that regulates the amount of harvest. Given plenty of
water, the harvest must be large.
The melon has been spoken of in another place. The water-
melons and the white-fleshed melons of Gourgab, near Ispahan, are
the finest in the world. I have seen melons weighing twenty-eight
pounds of the former variety; these were, however, phenomenal.
In the orchards of Ispahan clover is sown under the trees to break
the fall of the fruit, which is only plucked when a choice dish of it is
required as a present. The usual way is simply to pick up the
droppings. The Persian never thins the fruit. I have often
recommended their doing so, but on two occasions when I tried the
experiment and marked the trees, it did not succeed: the trees where
the fruit had been thinned merely giving fewer fruit, of the same size
as the unthinned trees under the same conditions and of the same
variety.
Nothing can be more delightful than these cool and silent gardens
in the summer in Ispahan. The thick foliage keeps out the sun, and
the deep green of the short clover refreshes the eye. Tea in a
garden, with pipes and fruit, is a pleasant way of spending a warm
afternoon. Unfortunately the Persian or Armenian usually looks on a
fruit garden merely as a good place to get drunk in, and the frequent
sounds of music and singing show the passer-by that this idea is
being carried out.
Garden parties are, however, often given by Persians who never
touch liquor, and they are as enjoyable a form of entertainment as
may be. An invitation to one is generally given without any
preparation, as during the paying of a call; it is accepted, and
forthwith an immediate start is made. A few carpets and pillows are
rolled up and placed on a mule, with the samovar or Russian urn in
its leather case, and the tea equipage in its travelling box. The cook,
on his pony, takes his whole batterie de cuisine, and hurries to the
garden indicated by his master, probably buying a lamb and a couple
of fowls, as he passes through the bazaar. The entertainer, his wife
and children too, if we are very intimate, the former on his horse, the
latter astride on white donkeys, proceed at a leisurely pace in the
direction of the garden; while the servants, all smiles, for they enjoy
the outing as much as the family, accompany them on foot or
horseback, carrying water-pipes, umbrellas, and odds and ends. On
reaching the garden, fruit is eaten; then the whole party roam
unrestricted among the shady paths while tea is prepared. This is
partaken of, and then a musician, or a singer, or perhaps a story-
teller, makes his appearance and diverts us all. Or some servant,
who has a good voice, sings or plays on the flute to us.
Often a grave and reverend merchant will produce a “tarr,” a
species of lute, or a “santoor” (the dulcimer), a kind of harmonica,
and astonish us by really good Eastern music. Few will consent to
sing; it is infra dig. The nightingales sing merrily, and dull care is
effectually banished. In these thoroughly family parties, wine or
spirits are never introduced. Chess or backgammon (Takht-i-Nadir,
the camp of Nadir Shah) are constantly played for a nominal stake of
a lamb or fat pullet.
The party is collected on a raised daïs in the open air, and sit on
carpets or lean on huge pillows. Candles are lighted in the lallahs or
Russian candle-lamps; these are convenient, as they are not
extinguished by wind. At about nine dinner is brought, after
innumerable kalians or water-pipes have been smoked: this is eaten
in comparative silence; host, guest, wives, and children, all sitting
round the leathern sheet which represents a table, and dipping their
hand in the platters. At about ten all retire, the bedding of each is
spread in a separate nook, on one’s own carpet—all of course being
in the open air—and at dawn one smokes a pipe, drinks a little cup
of black coffee, and takes one’s leave.
Our host and his guests go about their several businesses, while
the women and children and servants generally breakfast in the
garden and return home together in the cool of the evening, bringing
back fruit and huge bouquets of the moss-rose with them.
These impromptu entertainments are most enjoyable: there is no
sense of restraint, and their absolute suddenness, absence of
formality, and true hospitality, form a remarkable contrast to the more
formal pleasures of European life and the regularity of
entertainments which hang over one, till their very thought becomes
insupportable. Of course, such entertainments are only possible in a
country where the gardens are freely thrown open to everybody.
CHAPTER XXIX.
PERSIAN CHARACTER, COSTUMES, AND
MANNERS.

Character of the Persians—Exaggeration—Mercifulness—Anecdote—Costumes of


men—Hair—Beards—Arms—Costumes of women—Jewellery—Glass bangles—
Nose-rings—Painting of the face—Tattooing—Hair—Outdoor costume—Dress of
children—Their manners—Strange custom—Love of mothers—The uncle—
Cousins—Slaves—Servants—Slavery.

The character of the Persian, as it appears to me, is that of an easy-


going man with a wish to make things pleasant generally. He is
hospitable and obliging, as honest as the general run of mankind, and
is specially well disposed to the foreigner. He is very kind and indulgent
to his children, and as a son his respect for both parents is excessive,
developed in a greater degree to his father, in whose presence he will
rarely sit, and whom he is in the habit of addressing and speaking of as
“master;” the full stream of his love and reverence is reserved for his
mother; and an undutiful son or daughter is hardly known in the country.
Home virtues among the Persians are many.
No act of serious import is ever undertaken without the advice of the
mother; no man would think, for instance, of marrying contrary to his
mother’s advice; and by the very poorest the support of their parents
would never be looked on as a burden. Respect for the aged is
universal; “this grey beard” is a common term of respect; and an aged
man or woman will frequently give an opinion unsolicited, and such
advice is often requested, and always listened to as valuable.
The peculiar honesty of the Persian servant towards his master in
respect to his goods and chattels has been previously remarked; and in
commercial morality, I fancy that a Persian merchant will compare not
unfavourably with that of the European generally, if he does not always
attain the high standard theoretically adopted by the Englishman.
To the poor, Persians are unostentatiously generous; most of the rich
have regular pensioners, old servants, or poor relations who live on
their bounty, and though there are no workhouses, there are in ordinary
times no deaths from starvation; and charity, though not organised, is
general.
The Persian is, I regret to say, a liar, but Oriental exaggeration and a
tendency “to run into poetry,” as Mr. Wegg said, perhaps accounts for
much of this. After a time one learns to mentally discount the
statements made by the natives, and habit generally enables one to do
this correctly. All ranks of society exaggerate and draw the long bow; a
curious instance of this occurred in Shiraz. I was conversing on the
subject of hunting with the king’s son, and a large circle of courtiers and
priests filled the room.
The prince narrated his exploits in hunting the antelope the previous
day, and gravely stated that while pursuing a pair of “ahū,” when riding
a very restive horse that he had, his head-stall broke.
“What should you have done, doctor?”
“I should have tried to stick on as long as the ground was good, and,
expecting an accident, have awaited it.”
“Ah, that was because you were not a prince,” he said. “I leant
forward, and unclasping my belt, placed it in the horse’s mouth as a
bridle, and thus directing him, pursued my game and killed both
antelope.”
All the circle applauded (as of course they were bound to do). I was
silent.
“You don’t mean to say you don’t believe that?” said the prince.
I smiled.
“Speak out if you don’t; I shan’t be offended in the least.”
“Well, your Highness, I don’t believe it.”
“Quite right, darogh bood” (it was a lie), unblushingly replied his
Royal Highness, and burst into a fit of laughter quite unabashed; the
circle of courtiers, of course, were convulsed.
The giving of the lie is no insult in Persia; among the natives a
common expression is, “You are lying,” and the general reply is merely
to asseverate the statement by an oath, no indignation whatever being
shown at the charge.
Procrastination is the attribute of all Persians, “please God, to-
morrow,” being ever the answer to any proposition, and the to-morrow
means indefinite delay. A great dislike is shown generally to a written
contract binding the parties to a fixed date; and, as a rule, on breaking it
the Persian always appeals for and expects delay and indefinite days of
grace.
Only the upper classes and the natives of towns, among the military
and servant class, are in the habit of indulging in intoxicants, and
unchastity is confined to the females of these classes; this vice among
young women prior to marriage is very unusual; and the Persian
woman compares favourably with her European sister in this respect.
Persians are clean in their persons, washing themselves and their
garments frequently, differing in this habit from the Armenians, who
never wash more frequently than once a month, and consider it
unhealthy to do so; these people have great fear of taking cold, and
dread water like cats.
I will not trust myself to give my opinion on the character of the
Armenian. Of course I have known brilliant exceptions; but when I say
that I endorse all that Morier, Malcom, Lady Shiel, and the standard
writers on Persia have said of these people, I need not add that my
impression is unfavourable in the extreme. They possess one good
quality, however,—thrift.
The Persian always makes the best of his appearance; he is very
neat in his dress, and is particular as to the sit of his hat and the cut of
his coat They are all fond of animals, and do not treat them badly when
their own property. Of course hired horses and mules are often over-
ridden, and a good deal of cruelty from ignorance, in the way of riding
animals with sore backs, is seen; but as travellers must proceed, and
are frequently unable to give their horses or mules a rest, because they
must keep with their caravan, this is not to be wondered at. The
Persian, however, generally saves his animal as much as possible, and
frequently dismounts and walks, leading his mule or horse. Much of the
frequency of sore backs must be put down to the badly-made saddles
and pack-saddles, the latter of which are merely stuffed with “kah” (cut
straw).
MIDDLE CLASS PERSIANS.
PERSIAN BOY.

Cruelty is not a Persian vice; torture and punishments of an unusual


and painful nature being part of their judicial system. There are no
vindictive punishments, such as solitary confinement, penal servitude
for long terms of years, etc. Seldom, indeed, is a man imprisoned more
than twelve months, the rule being that there is a general jail delivery at
the New Year. Royal clemency is frequently shown, often, perhaps, with
want of judgment; still, it is very frequent. A cook I had, was years ago
one of the Baabi rebels, and was seized and conducted to Teheran.
(His guilt was undoubted; he himself acknowledged it; and these men
had made an attempt, nearly successful, on the Shah’s life, actually
wounding him.) This cook, “Mehdi,” was chained by the neck, with
eleven others, and led out in the Shah’s presence for execution. The
eleven men had their throats cut. “Enough,” said the king, “let that poor
rascal go!”
He was taken back to prison and his life spared; but though the Shah
had meant that he should be released, there being no formal order, he
remained in prison for several years, making a good living by selling
savoury messes to his fellow-prisoners.
The costume of the Persians may be shortly described as fitted to
their active habits. The men invariably wear an unstarched shirt of
cotton. This is sewn with white silk, cotton as a sewing material being
unknown; it is often, particularly in the south of Persia, elaborately
embroidered about the neck. It fastens in front by a flap, having two
small buttons or knots at the left shoulder. It seldom comes below the
hips. There is no collar, and the sleeves are quite loose, and are not
confined at the wrist. The lower orders often have it dyed blue,
particularly the villagers; but the servant and upper classes invariably
wear a white shirt. Silk shirts used to be worn, but are now seldom seen
on men. Among the very religious, during the mourning month
(“Mohurrim”), the shirt is at times dyed black.
The “zerejumah,” or trousers, are of cloth among the higher classes,
particularly those of the military order, who affect a garment of a
tightness approaching that worn by Europeans. But the ordinary
“zerejumah” is of cotton, white or dyed blue, or at times red, cut very
loose, and exactly similar to the “pyjamas” worn by Europeans in India.
They are held up by a thin cord of red or green silk or cotton round the
waist, and the labouring classes, when engaged in heavy or dirty work,
or when running, generally tuck the end of these garments in under the
cord, which leaves their leg bare and free to the middle of the thigh.
The amplitude of these loose garments enables the Persian to sit
without discomfort on his heels, his usual mode of sitting, for chairs are
only used by the rich, great, or Europeanised; and it is a common thing
for a visitor, if on familiar terms, to ask to be allowed to sit on his heels,
as the unaccustomed chair tires him.
Over the shirt and “zerejumah” comes the “alkalūk,” generally of
quilted chintz or print. This is a closely-fitting garment, collarless, with
tight sleeves to the elbow, whence to the wrist are a number of little
metal buttons; these are fastened in winter, and left open in summer.
Above this is the coat, or “kemmercheen,” a tunic of coloured calico,
silk, satin, moiré, cloth, Cashmere, or Kerman shawl, gold embroidered
silk, satin, or velvet, according to the time of year and the purse or
position of the wearer. This, like the alkalūk, is open in front, and shows
the shirt. It has a small standing collar at times, and is double-breasted.
It has a pocket-hole on either side, giving access to the pockets, which
are always in the alkalūk, in which garment is the breast-pocket, where
watch, money, jewels, and seals are kept. The length of the
“kemmercheen” denotes the class of the wearer. The military and
official classes and the various “noker,” or servants, from the king’s
valet, who may be also prime minister, wear them short; that is to say,
to the knee, while fops and lutis (sharpers) wear them shorter even
than this. The priests, and merchants, and the villagers, especially
about Shiraz, and the townsmen and shopkeepers, with doctors and
lawyers, wear them very long, often nearly to their heels.
Over the kemmercheen is worn the kolajah, or coat. This is as a rule
cast off in summer, save on formal occasions, as when the wearer is
performing his functions or making a call, and is often borne by a
servant or carried over the shoulder by the owner himself. They are of
cloth, shawl, or camel-hair cloth, and are invariably lined throughout
with either silk or cloth, flannel, or even fur. They are like the Turkish
frock-coat, made with a very loose sleeve, and with many plaits behind.
They have lappels, as with us, and are trimmed with gold lace
(derbeeri), shawl, or fur, or at times worn quite plain; they have a roll
collar and false pockets.
Besides these garments there are others, as the long juba, or cloth
cloak. This ample and majestic garment is affected by mirzas
(secretaries), Government employés of high rank, as ministers, farmers
of taxes, courtiers, physicians, priests. The wearers carry a staff as a
rule. The jubas are made of the finest cloth, very amply cut. They have
a standing collar and long sleeves. These sleeves are from one to two
feet longer than the arm, and are often allowed to hang down empty
when the garment is worn out of doors; but when in the actual presence
of guests or a grandee, they are used to keep the hands hidden (a
token of respect to those present), and the many wrinkles formed by
the excessive length of these sleeves are supposed to be their beauty.
The abba, or camel-hair cloak of the Arab, is often worn by travellers,
priests, and horsemen. The priests particularly affect it; it is a very
picturesque garment, warm, and waterproof, also very light. Some of
these abbas are very expensive, though plain; while others, much
embroidered in gold, are given as dresses of honour to the middle
classes and priests, and are used at weddings, etc.
Among outer garments worn by travellers and the aged are the well-
known poosseen, or Afghan skin-cloak. These are full length, only used
by travellers and the sick or aged; and the “neemtan,” or common
sheepskin jacket, with short sleeves, used by shopkeepers and the
lower class of servants, grooms, etc., in winter. They are mostly seen at
Ispahan. The Afghan poosseen is a wonderful garment for travellers, as
it is so very warm, and forms bed and bedding, but it has to be kept dry.
The skins are dyed yellow; the fur is generally a natural brown. An
ample cloak is made with very long sleeves, which act as gloves, the
hands not being protruded. They are often elaborately embroidered
with yellow silk, and are worth in Teheran, where they are very
common, from four pounds (ten tomans) to sixteen pounds (forty
tomans).
Besides these “balapoosh,” or overcoats, is the “yapunjah,” or
woollen Kūrdish cloak. This is a kind of felt, having a shaggy side, of
immense thickness. It looks like a bear-skin, and is of great weight. It is
a half-circle in shape; a strap at the neck holds it on. The wearer,
generally a shepherd, uses it as great-coat, bed and bedding. It is quite
waterproof, and very warm. The thing is worn slung, closed side to the
wind, and is used as a shield against the wind or snow.
There is also the felt coat of the villager, before described, a very
warm and inexpensive garment, which wears well. It is from half an inch
to one inch thick, and enables the villager to defy the severest weather.
The cost is from five to fifteen kerans.
The kemmerbund, or belt, is also characteristic of the class. It is
made of muslin, shawl, or cotton cloth among the priests, merchants,
traders, and bazaar people; shawls and muslin are also affected by the
secretary class and the more aged or old-fashioned among the great
Government employés.
In it is carried by the literati and merchants the pencase and a roll of
paper, and its voluminous folds are used as pockets: and by the bazaar
people and villagers, porters, and merchants’ servants a small sheath
knife is stuck in it; while by “farrashes,” the carpet-spreader class, a
large canjar, or curved dagger, with a heavy ivory handle, is carried;
less for use than as a badge of office.
The headgear, too, is very distinctive. The turban is worn by the
priests. These use generally a white one, consisting of many yards of
muslin, unless they be “Syuds,” or descendants of the prophet, when a
green one is worn. This at times is a very deep colour, nearly black; at
others a grass-green.
These Syuds, too, usually wear a kemmerbund, or girdle of green
muslin, shawl, or cotton cloth. Merchants also affect the turban, usually
of muslin, embroidered in colours; or of a yellow pattern on straw-
coloured muslin, or of calico, or at times of shawl.
The waist of the Persian is generally small, and he is very proud of
his fine figure and broad shoulders.
The distinctive mark of the courtier, military, and upper servant class
is the belt, generally of varnished leather, black in colour, with a brass
clasp, usually of Russian manufacture. The princes and courtiers often
replace the brass clasp by a huge round ornament of cut stones, the
favourite one of his Royal Highness Zil-es-Sultan being of diamonds, of
large size, a huge emerald being in the centre.
The “kola,” or hat, is of cloth or sheepskin, on a frame of pasteboard.
The most expensive are made of the black skin of the fœtal lamb.
Strange to say, these skins usually go to Europe to be dyed—I believe
to Leipzig. The commoner people wear coloured lambskin hats, as
grey, or even sheepskin, with the wool long. The fashions in hats
change yearly; they are generally affected by the military and noker
(servant) class, by courtiers and beaux, and are usually worn with a
knowing cock. The Ispahani merchant, and the Armenian, at times wear
very tall ones.
The hair is generally shaved at the crown, or the entire head is
shaved, a karkool, or long thin lock, being at times left, often two feet
long from the middle of the crown. This is kept knotted up and hidden.
Its use is to enable the prophet Mahommed to draw up the believer into
paradise. The lower orders generally have the hair over the temporal
bone long, and this is brought in two long locks, turning backwards
behind the ear; they are termed “Zūlf;” the beaux and youths are
constantly twisting and combing them. The rest of the head is shaven.
Long hair, however, is going out of fashion in Persia, and the more
civilised affect the cropped hair worn by Europeans, and even have a
parting in it.
The chin is never shaved, save by “beauty men,” or “Kashangs,”
though often clipped, while the moustache is usually left long. At forty, a
man generally lets his beard grow its full length, and cherishes it much;
part of a Persian’s religious exercises is the combing of his beard.
Socks, knitted principally at Ispahan, are worn: they are only about
two inches long in the leg. The rich, however, affect a longer sock: white
cotton ones are worn in summer, and coloured worsted in winter. The
patterns of these worsted socks are often very pretty and effective. The
villagers only wear a sock on state occasions, as at a wedding, the New
Year, etc.
Shoes are of many patterns; the Orūssi or Russian shoe, similar to
our children’s shoe without the strap, is the most common. Next, the
Kafsh, or slippers of various kinds. The heel is folded down, and
remains so. The priests affect a peculiar heavy shoe, with an ivory or
wooden lining at the heel. Green shoes of shagreen are very common
at Ispahan. Blacking is unknown to Persians generally, but a
European’s servant may be always recognised by his polished shoes.
Boots are only used by horsemen, and are then worn much too large,
for ease. Those worn by couriers often come up the thigh, and are
similar to those used by our sewer-men.
With boots are worn shulwar, or baggy riding breeches. These are
very loose, and tied at the ankle by a string; a sort of kilt is worn by
couriers. Pocket-handkerchiefs are never used, save by the rich or the
Teheranis.
Most Persians affect a “shub kola,” or night hat, for wear in their
homes. This is a loose, baggy cap, of shawl or quilted material: it is
often embroidered by the ladies, and presents of “night hats” are as
frequent with them as our ones of embroidered slippers.
As to arms, these are usually carried only by the tribesmen, who
bristle with weapons. The natives of the south of Persia and servants—
these latter generally, particularly in Shiraz—carry a kammer, or dirk,
which is, however, seldom used as an offensive weapon, save in
drunken rows. The soldiery, on or off duty, always carry one of these
“kammers” or their side-arms, sometimes both. They hack, but never
thrust with them. Of course on the road the carrying of weapons is the
rule, and it is needed, as there is no police, save the ephemeral
phenomena introduced by Count Monteforte at Teheran. These men,
who are really efficient, are too good to last.
The costume of the women has undergone considerable change in
the last century; it is now, when carried to the extreme of the fashion,
highly indecent, and must be very uncomfortable.
The garment doing duty as a chemise is called a perhān; it is, with
the lower orders, of calico, white or blue, and comes down to the middle
of the thigh, leaving the leg nude. Among the upper classes it is
frequently of silk. At Shiraz it is often of fine cotton, and elaborately
ornamented with black embroidery: among the rich it is frequently of
gauze, and much embroidered with gold thread, pearls, etc. With them
it often reaches only to the navel.
The head is usually covered with a chargāt, or large square of silk or
cotton, embroidered. These chargāts are folded, as were shawls
amongst us some years ago, thus displaying the corners, two in front
and two behind; it is fastened under the chin by a brooch. It is often of
considerable value, being of Cashmere shawl, embroidered gauze, etc.
A jika, a jewelled, feather-like ornament, is often worn at the side of
the head, while the front hair, cut to a level with the mouth, is brought
up in love-locks on either cheek. Beneath the chargāt is generally a
small kerchief of dark material, worn to set off the complexion, and
preserve the chargāt; only the edge of this is visible. The ends of the
chargāt cover the shoulders, but the gauze perhān, quite transparent,
leaves nothing to the imagination. The breasts and chest are very
visible, and the abdomen is quite bare.
On state occasions, or with women who aim at beauty, the face is
always painted more or less, and a profusion of jewellery worn. This is
of the most solid description, the gold some twenty-three carats fine,
and quite flexible: no hollow jewellery is worn, intrinsic value being what
is aimed at.
Silver is only worn by the very poor: coral only by negresses.
Necklaces and bracelets are much worn, and numerous chains with
scent-caskets attached to them; while the arms are covered with
clanking glass bangles, called “Alangū,” some twenty even of these
hoops being worn on an arm.
Jewelled “Bazūbund,” containing talismans, are often worn on the
upper arm, while among the lower orders and South Persian or Arab
women nose-rings are not uncommon, and at times bangles, or anklets
of beads, on the ankles.
The face on all important occasions—as at entertainments,
weddings, etc.—is usually much painted, save by young ladies in the
heyday of beauty. The colour is very freely applied, the cheeks being
reddled, as are a clown’s, and the neck smeared with white, while the
eyelashes are marked round with kohl (black antimony). This is
supposed to be beneficial to the eyes, and almost every woman uses it
—very needlessly, as the large languishing eye of the Persian belle
needs no adventitious aid. The eyebrows are widened and painted till
they appear to meet, while sham moles or stars are painted on the chin
and cheek—various in their way, as the patches of the eighteenth-
century belles: even spangles are stuck at times on the chin or
forehead. Tattooing is common among the poor and villagers, and is
seen among the upper classes.
The hair, though generally hidden by the chargāt, is at times exposed
and plaited into innumerable little tails of great length, while a
coquettish little skull-cap of embroidery or shawl or coloured silks is
worn. False hair is common. The Persian ladies’ hair is very luxuriant,
and never cut; it is nearly always dyed red with henna, or black with
indigo to a blue-black tinge; it is naturally a glossy black. Fair hair is not
esteemed, and I have been asked to condole with ladies in their grief in
being the possessors of fair locks. At Ispahan so universal is this
feeling that a young half-caste lady having beautiful golden hair, dyed it
on her marriage to a pre-Raphaelite auburn, to please her Baghdadi
husband.
Blue eyes are not uncommon, but brown ones, like those of the full-
blooded Jewess, are the rule: a full-moon face is much admired, and
the possession of a dark complexion termed “nummak” (salt) is the
highest native idea of beauty.
Most Persian women are small, with tiny feet and hands. The figure,
however, is always lost after maternity, and they wear no support of any
kind.
A very short jacket of gay colour, quite open in front, and not covering
the bosom, with tight sleeves with many metal buttons, is usually worn
in summer: a lined outer coat in cold weather.
In winter a pair of very short white cotton socks are used, and tiny
slippers with a high heel; in summer in the house ladies go often
barefoot.
The rest of the costume is composed of the “tūmbūn,” or “shulwar;”
these are simply short skirts of great width, held by a running string; the
outer one usually of silk, velvet, or Cashmere shawl, often trimmed with
gold lace, according to the purse of the wearer; or among the poor, of
loud-patterned chintz or print. Beneath these are innumerable other
garments of the same shape, and varying in texture from silk and satin
to print.
The whole is very short indeed; among the women of fashion merely
extending to the thigh, and as the number of these garments is
amazing, and they are much bouffée, the effect of a lady sitting down
astonishes the beholder, and would scandalise the Lord Chamberlain.
As the ladies are supposed, however, to be only seen by their lords in
these indoor dresses, there is perhaps no harm done.
Indecency, too, is very much an idea, for a Persian lady, who will thus
expose her extremities and the greater part of her trunk, will carefully
veil her face, showing nothing but the eyes. The ladies of rank,
however, have no shame of any kind, and display very redundant
charms. The indoor costume of the Persian lady is in fact exactly that of
the corps de ballet, but shorter: while in winter, an over-mantle like the
“kolajah” or coat of the man, and with short sleeves, lined and trimmed
with furs, is worn; this gives the costume a peculiarly graceful
appearance.

OUTDOOR DRESS OF PERSIAN WOMEN.

(From a Native Drawing.)

Leg-coverings are now being introduced, and the last princess of the
blood royal I saw added to her comfort, though she destroyed the
poetry of her appearance, by a tightly-fitting pair of black cloth “pants”
with a gold stripe! This garment will doubtless soon become general.
In ancient days the Persian ladies always wore them, as may be
seen by the pictures in the South Kensington Museum. In those times
the two embroidered legs, now so fashionable as Persian embroideries
(“naksh”), occupied a girl from childhood to marriage in their making;
they are all sewing in elaborate patterns of great beauty, worked on
muslin, in silk.
The outdoor costume of the Persian women is quite another thing;
enveloped in a huge blue sheet, with a yard of linen as a veil,
perforated for two inches square with minute holes, the feet thrust into
two huge bags of coloured stuff, a wife is perfectly unrecognisable,
even by her husband, when out of doors. The dress of all is the same;
save in quality or costliness, the effect is similar. And yet with such a
hideous disguise, a Persian coquette will manage to let the curious
know if she have a good face and eye, by lifting her veil in a sly and
half-timid way. The only thing I know exceeding in folly the chimney-pot
hat, is the outdoor dress of the Persian woman. Expensive, ugly,
uncomfortable, hot in summer, cold in winter, words fail to express its
numerous disadvantages; it has one positive quality—as a disguise it is
perfect, and its use favours the intrigues rife in the country.
As for the children, they are always when infants swaddled: when
they can walk they are dressed as little men and women, and with the
dress they often, nay generally, ape the manners; a Persian child of the
upper class being a master of etiquette, an adept at flattery, and a
mirror of politeness. It is a strange custom with the Persian ladies to
dress little girls as boys, and little boys as girls, till they reach seven or
eight years; this is often done for fun, or on account of some vow,
oftener to avert the evil eye.
Persian women are very fond of their children, and pet them greatly.
The love of the Persian for his mother is very great; he never leaves her
to starve, and her wishes are laws to him, even when he is an old man,
and she an aged crone. The mother is always the most important
member of the household, and the grandmother is treated with
veneration. Mothers-in-law are not laughed at or looked down on in
Persia; their presence is coveted by their sons-in-law, who look on them
as the guardians of the virtue of their wives. The uncle, too, is a much
nearer tie than with us, that is to say, the paternal uncle: while men look
on their first cousins on the father’s side as their most natural wives.
Possibly this is because their female cousins are the only women they
have any opportunity of knowing anything of personally. Black slaves
and men-nurses, or “lallahs,” are much respected and generally
retained in a household, while the “dyah,” or wet nurse, is looked on as
a second mother, and usually provided for for life.
Persians are very kind to their servants, and try to make their people
look on them as second fathers; a master will be often addressed by a
servant as his father, and the servant will protect his master’s property
as he would his own, or even more jealously.
A servant is invariably spoken to as “butcha” (child). The servants
expect that their master will always take their part, and never allow
them to be wronged; if he does not do so, he cannot obtain a good
class domestic, while if he sticks to the man, he never leaves him.
The slaves in Persia have what Americans call “a good time;” well
fed, well clothed, treated as spoiled children, given the lightest work,
and often given in marriage to a favourite son, or taken as a “segah,” or
concubine, by the master himself (and respectable Persians only take a
“segah” for ninety-nine years, which is equivalent to a permanent
marriage), slaves have the certainty of comfort and a well-cared-for old
age. They are always looked on as confidential servants, are entrusted
with large sums of money, and the conduct of the most important
affairs; and seldom abuse their trust.
The greatest punishment to an untrustworthy slave is to give him his
liberty and let him earn his living. They vary in colour and value: the
“Habashi” or Abyssinian is the most valued; the Souhāli or Somāli, next
in blackness, is next in price; the Bombassi, or coal-black negro of the
interior, being of much less price, and usually only used as a cook. The
prices of slaves in Shiraz are, a good Habashi girl of twelve to fourteen,
forty pounds; a good Somāli same age, half as much; while a Bombassi
is to be got for fourteen pounds, being chosen merely for physical
strength. They are never sold, save on importation, though at times
they are given away. Strange as it may appear, to the mind of any one
who has lived in Persia, slavery in that country to the African is an
unmixed good. Of course the getting to Persia, and the being caught, is
another thing. But I have never seen a Persian unkind to his own horse

You might also like