Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

EFFECTS ON CARE-GIVERS OF SPECIAL

DAY CARE PROGRAMMES FOR


DEMENTIA SUFFERERS
Y .D. Wells, A.F. Jorm. F. Jordan and R. Lefroy

Two groups of care-givers to dementia sufferers were interviewed to assess


their psychological symptoms and the behaviour problems of the dementia
sufferers. The first group were using special dementia day care while the
second group were about to begin using it. The second group were re-inter-
viewed three months later. Therefore two comparisons could be made; a
with/without day care comparison and a before/after admission to day care
comparison. Care-givers had a high level of psychological symptoms which
were not significantly reduced by using day care. Full-time institutional care
did reduce distress. Day centre clients continued to deteriorate as would be
expected with a degenerative condition. While special dementia day care
centres appear to play a useful role for many care-givers, they may provide too
few hours relief per week to markedly reduce the care-givers’ psychological
symptoms. Family care-givers currently take the major role in caring for
dementia sufferers in the community and the burden of care needs to be shared
more equitably.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 1990;24:82-90

Dementia sufferers are major users of institutional the Australian population is expected to produce a
care facilities such as nursing homes. It has been substantial increase in the prevalence of dementia
estimated that, in Australia, around half of moderate- which will bring about a corresponding rise in demand
to-severe cases live in institutions [ 11. The ageing of for dementia services [ 2 ] .If the level of nursing home
bed provision were allowed to rise in step with the
prevalence of dementia, there would be an enormous
Moorfields Community for Adult Care financial burden on governments for dementia care.
Y.D. Wells, BA However, there has been a shift in policy towards
NH&MRC Social Psychiatry Research Unit, The Australian emphasizing community care services and cheaper
National University forms of residential care (hostels) at the expense of
A.F. Jorm, PhD nursing homes [3]. While the financial sense of this
Department of Social Work, Princess Alexandra Hospital policy change cannot be doubted, little is known about
F. Jordan, Dip. Social Work the possible benefits or disadvantages to dementia
Human Ageing Research Unit, The University of Western sufferers and their families.
Australia
In seeking to reduce the demand for institutional
R. Lefroy, FRACP
care, we must ask why dementia sufferers are so often
Correspond with DrJorm,NH&MRC SocialPsychiatry Research
Unit. The Australian National University, GPO Box 4, Canberra found in such care. Because dementia involves major
ACT 260 I cognitive impairment, sufferers are usually unable to

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015


Y .D. WELLS, A.F. JORM, F. JORDAN, R. LEFROY 83

live in the community without constant support from Scores on psychological symptom inventories often
others. This support is generally provided by a close go down on re-testing, even in general population
relative who acts as the primary care-giver. Primary surveys where no average change should take place
care-givers often experience great distress as a result [8]. Furthermore, there may be demand characteristics
of their care-giving role, and several studies have in a before/after evaluation of services, such that care-
shown that the state of the care-giver is a more impor- givers feel that their psychological symptoms should
tant determinant of institutionalization than charac- improve. Although the degree of improvement seen in
teristics of the dementing person [4]. Furthermore, Gilleard’s care-givers was probably too great to be
institutional care appears to be an effective treatment explained entirely in terms of such artefacts, efforts
for care-giver distress. In a longitudinal study of care- need to be made to estimate effects of this sort.
givers in Britain, Levin et al. [5] found that their T h e best way of o v e r c o m i n g the a b o v e
mental health tended to improve when the dementia methodological limitations is to use a control group,
sufferer went into institutional care, while the mental but in practice this can be difficult to do in studies
health of care-givers who continued on tended to get evaluating services. In the present study of day care,
worse. More direct evidence of the potential benefits we attempted to circumvent these problems by carry-
of institutional care to care-givers comes from a con- ing out two complementary comparisons. The first
trolled evaluation of an Australian special nursing was a simple comparison of care-givers before entry
home unit for dementia sufferers [6]. Immediate entry to day care and three months after entry. We call this
to this Unit produced a large reduction in the the “before/after comparison”. This is essentially the
psychological symptoms of care-givers, whereas design used by Gilleard [7]. The second involved
symptoms remained high for care-givers of dementia comparing clients already receiving day care with
sufferers placed on a waiting list for entry to the Unit. those about to receive it. We call this the “with/without
If community care programmes are to be an effec- comparison”. If day care is effective in relieving care-
tive substitute for institutional care, they should pro- giver distress, then care-givers of dementia sufferers
vide equivalent benefits to care-givers. Several types already receiving it should have fewer psychological
of community care programmes have been targetted symptoms than care-givers of those about to receive
specifically at dementia sufferers, including carer sup- it. In this comparison, all care-givers were being as-
port groups, community options programmes, respite sessed for the first time, so re-test artefacts were not
care and day care. There is a need to establish whether an issue and demand characteristics may have been
each of these aid the psychological health of care- reduced. This comparison was not perfect, however,
givers and, if they do not, to reconsider or redesign and assumed that the type of clients entering day care
these programmes accordingly. The present paper did not change over time.
reports on the benefits to care-givers of one of these
community care programmes: special dementia day
care. In such programmes, dementia sufferers come to
day centres one or more days a week and are provided Procedure
with activities which are suited to their cognitive
limitations. Special day care programmes of this sort The staff of 16 special dementia day centres agreed
have proliferated in Australia in the past few years. to assist in the study. Ten of these day centres were in
Although the effect of day care on care-givers has Melbourne, four in Brisbane, one on the Gold Coast
not previously been evaluated, there is some evidence and one in Perth. Each of these day centres provided
on day hospitals. Gilleard [7], in Britain, found that the contacts with care-givers whose relative was already
psychological distress of many care-givers was attending the centre, and also with care-givers who
reduced when the dementia sufferer attended a were about to make use of the service. This second
psychogeriatric day hospital. However, for others group was approached for another interview three
whose distress was not alleviated, subsequent admis- months later. Each care-giver gave verbal or written
sion to institutional care did produce a reduction. consent to be interviewed. The care-givers of those
Unfortunately, the simple before/after comparison of about to attend constituted a consecutive series, apart
care-givers used by Gilleard may have exaggerated the from a small number who refused to participate.
degree of benefit received from day hospital care. Altogether, 219 care-givers were interviewed; 155

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015


84 EFFECTS ON CARE-GIVERS OF SPECIAL DAY CARE PROGRAMMES

were currently using special day care, and 64 were Assessment instruments
about to make use of it. Of the latter, 52 could be
re-interviewed three months later and 37 were still Problem checklist
using day care. Sample sizes for some analyses
reported below are, however, slightly smaller due to A 27-item problem checklist [9] was used. The
missing data. wording of some items was altered because these had
Thirty-nine percent of the care-givers were wives of proved difficult to administer in a previous study (61.
the day centre clients, 20% were husbands, 29% were The “Unable” items (to wash/to dresdto manage
daughters, 4% were daughters-in-law, 3% were sons stairs/to walk outside unaided) were changed to
and 5% were other relatives or friends. Through over- “Needs help” (to wash/to dress, with stairs/to walk
sight, the age of care-givers was not recorded. How- outside). “Vulgar habits” was changed to “Bad habits,
ever, research on the Australian general population has e.g. spits, poor table manners”, “Creates personality
shown that age explains very little of the variance in clashes” was changed to “Starts arguments”, “Unable
psychological symptoms [8]. to hold a sensible conversation” was changed to
Some of the care-givers received no services apart “Loses track of conversation”, and “No concern for
from the day centre: 20% of the Victorian, 25% of the personal hygiene” was changed to “Resists bathing”.
Queensland sample and 43% of the Western In this way the problem caused by answering a nega-
Australian sample. Of the total sample, 9% used other tively worded question with a negative was avoided.
day placement, 28%used respite care, 8% used a sitter The more dependent and/or difficult to manage
service, 11% used meals-on-wheels, 26% used home scored highest.
help, 37% used district nurses, and 22% used a support
group. Three percent of the care-receivers were actual- General Health Questionnaire
ly in long-term residential care.
All of the day centre clients had some degree of The 30-item version [lo] was used to assess
cognitive impairment, and in most cases this was due psychological problems in the care-givers. In addition
to dementia. The average age of the clients was 76 to the conventional scoring system, the modified scor-
years, with a range from 35 to 96 years. Males com- ing system was used to take account of chronic
prised 44.5% of the client group. problems [ 111. With both scoring systems high scores
The day centres varied in their hours of opening, indicate poor functioning. However, the chronic GHQ
from 5 to 8.5 hours per day, and in the number of days scores were found to be highly correlated with scores
on which they operated, from one to five days per obtained in the conventional way (r=.86) and gave the
week. Most had one or more paid staff on duty at any same pattern of results, so only the conventional GHQ
one time, but one centre had no paid staff, and one had scores are presented here.
five paid staff. Most of the centres used volunteers.
The number of attenders ranged from 5 to 15. Some DSSI/sAD
programmes were very structured, others were very
casual and unstructured. Some provided transport to This instrument [ 121 assessed symptoms of anxiety
and from the centre, others relied on the care-givers. and depression in the care-givers. High scores indicate
Most centres operated in ordinary houses, but three poor functioning.
used halls and two were purpose-built.
The average number of day centre hours per week Quality of life questionnaire
allotted to care-givers was 11.9 (SD 7.1). The averages
for individual day centres ranged from 5.0 hours per This 20-item scale [6] assessed the care-givers’
week for three day centres up to 22.5 hours per week ability to engage in social and recreational pursuits.
for the Perth day centre. Existing clients had been Higher scores indicate a better quality of life. Because
attending for an average of 10 months. this questionnaire was found to have a complex fac-
torial structure, the items were analysed individually
to find out in which specific ways the groups of care-
givers differed.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015


Y.D. WELLS, A.F. JORM, F. JORDAN, R. LEFROY 85

Table 1 . Prevalence of psychological disturbances in care-givers


compared to a general population sample

Group N GHQscore DSSl DSSl


6+ anxiety depresslon
score 4 + score 4 +

Care-givers of those about to attend day care 63 69% 52% 29%


Care-givers of those about to attend day care (excluding sub-
sequent drop-outs) 37 64% 46% 27%
Care-givers of those currently attending day care 154 59% 45% 26%
General population - males 19% 16% 7%
General population - females 20% 16% 13%

Table 2 . Mean scores (and standard deviations) of dementia sufferers and their care-givers
in about-to-attend and currently-attending groups

About to About to attend Currently


attend day (excluding subsequent attending day
care dropouts) care
Dementia sufferers
(n=64) (n=37) (n=155)

Age 74.8 (10.7) 72.8 (11.9) 76.3 (8.0)


Behaviour problems 20.0 (8.1) 18.0 (7.2) 21.0 (8.2)

Caregivers

General Health Questionnaire 9.4 (6.5) 8.1 (6.0) 8.3 (7.1)


DSSl anxiety 4.2 (3.6) 3.5 (2.6) 4.1 (3.8)
DSSI depression 2.6 (3.8) 2.4 (3.6) 2.6 (3.8)
Guilt scale 4.4 (3.2) 3.9 (2.9) 5.0 (4.3)
Grief scale 10.2 (5.8) 9.6 (6.2) 10.5 (6.2)

Guilt Scale tress. It is also an expansion of an earlier scale [6].The


items are listed in the Appendix. High total scores
This nine-item scale attempted to measure the guilt indicate greater grief. Coefficient alpha for the scale
felt by care-givers specific to that role. It is an expan- was .86. Again, no data on validity are available.
sion of a scale used by Wells and Jorm [6].The items
are listed in the Appendix. High total scores indicate
greater guilt. Coefficient alpha for the scale was .79.
Apart from the data reported by Wells & Jorm [6]no The analyses reported here involved evaluating the
information on its validity is available. significance of many effects. To avoid an excessive
rate of Type I errors, a significance level of p<O.Ol was
Grief scale used. More conservative procedures, such as Bonfer-
roni, were not used because of their lack of power.
This ten-item scale attempted to measure the grief
suffered by care-givers as distinct from general dis-

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015


86 EFFECTS ON CARE-GIVERS OF SPECIAL DAY CARE PROGRAMMES

general population, but day care attendance appeared


Table 3. Prevalence of quality of life to make no difference to the high rate.
indicators in care-givers of about-to-attend Similar results emerged when mean scores on the
and currently-attending groups various outcome measures were examined. As can be
seen in Table 2, means did not differ greatly as a
function of day care attendance. The only differences
Quality of life About to About to Currently were found on the Quality of Life Questionnaire. As
indicator * attend attend attending shown in Table 3, day care attenders more often
day care (excluding day care
reported having time to themselves and shopping
subsequeni
dropouts) alone. While these differences were significant at the
p<O.Ol level when the complete about-to-attend group
was used for comparison (x2 of 12.12 and 11.91
Friends visited 63% 65% 51yo
16% 19%
respectively), they failed to reach this level of statisti-
Rows with family 18%
Visited relatives 39% 38% 37% cal significance when subsequent drop-outs were ex-
Weekend outing 22% 24% 18% cluded (x2
of 4.16 and 5.05 respectively).
Cinema or show 17% 19% 11%
Restaurant 19% 24% 19%
Paid work 25% 32% 16%
Before/after comparison
Voluntary work 17% 22% 15%
Broken sleep 77% 76% 70% Of the 52 care-givers who were assessed both before
Too little money 11% 16% 17% and after, 36 attended for at least three months. Of the
Shopping alone 55% 59% 79%
Visited doctor 42%
other 16,6 moved into residential care (nursing home
43% 51yo
Read book 48% 54% 58% or hostel) and 10 were not attending for other reasons.
Read magazine 61'lo 68% 67% Table 4 shows the mean changes from before to after
Visited relatives 45% 43% 52% for these three outcome groups. Although behaviour
Relatives visited 72% 68% 75%
Telephone chat 92% 97%
problems tended to get worse, this trend was not
91%
Worked at hobby 56% 59% 67% statistically significant. Amongst care-givers, those
Time to self 39% 46% 66% who subsequently dropped out tended to have higher
Church meeting 38% 43% 47% initial scores than those who continued, but this trend
was also not statistically significant. Overall, care-
Abbreviations of actual items
givers improved from pre-test to post-test on the
General Health Questionnaire, F( 1,47)=20.58, and on
Withhnrithout comparison DSSI Anxiety, F( 1,49)= 10.96. However, the greatest
improvement occurred in the care-givers of those who
Before comparing these two groups on psychologi- went into residential care. In the case of DSSI Anxiety,
cal symptoms, a check was made to see if they were the Groups X Testing Occasion interaction effect was
matched in terms of otherfactors. The two groups were statistically significant, F(2,49)=5.49, while this inter-
found not to differ in age or sex of the dementing action barely missed reaching the p<O.Ol level for the
person or in sex or relationship of carer. General Health Questionnaire, F(2,47)=4.82.
Consistent with much previous research, the care- With the Quality of Life Questionnaire, continued
givers in the present study had a high prevalence of attendance only improved frequency of time to self
psychological disorders, as defined by high scores on (from 46% to 73%), but this failed to reach the p<O.Ol
screening questionnaires. Table 1 shows the percent- level with a McNemar test (Binomial p=.0129).
ages of the currently-attending and about-to-attend
groups with scores of 6+ on the General Health Ques- Correlates of caregiver distress
tionnaire and 4+ on the DSSI Anxiety or Depression
scales. These cutoffs were used to allow comparison Within the group of current day care attenders,
with general population data reported by Henderson et analyses were carried out to find correlates of care-
al. [8,13]. Not only did the care-givers have a high giver distress. Table 5 shows that characteristics of the
prevalence of psychological disorders compared to the dementia sufferer, amount of day care received and
number of other services received had very small

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015


Y.D. WELLS, A.F. JORM, F. JORDAN, R.LEFROY 87

Table 4 . Mean scores (and standard deviations) of dementia sufferers and their care-givers before start-
ing day care and three months after

N Pre-test Post-test at 3
Behaviour problems of dementia sufferer months

Attending at 3 months 36 17.7 (7.1) 20.3 (7.3)


In residential care at 3 months 3 24.3 (13.5) 25.3 (4.5)
Not attending for other reasons 9 21.7 (8.7) 25.0 (10.8)

General Health Questionnaire in care-giver

Attending at 3 months 35 8.3 (6.0) 5.6 (5.8)


In residential care at 3 months 5 11.2 (10.0) 0.6 (1.3)
Not attending for 10 13.6 (6.4) 12.1 (6.4)
DSSl anxiety in care-giver other reasons

Attending at 3 months 36 3.5 (2.6) 3.3 (3.0)


In residential care at 3 months 6 6.2 (6.4) 1.0 (1.5)
Not attending for other reasons 10 6.3 (4.2) 5.6 (3.1)

DSSl depression in care-giver

Attending at 3 months 36 2.4 (3.6) 1.8 (2.5)


In residential care at 3 months 6 2.5 (4.8) 0.0 (0.0)
Not attending for other reasons 10 4.5 (4.6) 4.3 (3.9)

Guilt in caregiver

Attending at 3 months 36 3.9 (3.0) 3.3 (2.8)


In residentialcare at 3 months 6 5.0 (3.3) 2.7 (2.0)
Not attending for other reasons 10 3.7 (2.2) 4.1 (3.1)

Grief in care-giver

Attending at 3 months 35 9.8 (6.2) 9.5 (5.6)


In residential care at 3 months 6 10.2 (6.3) 6.2 (3.7)
Not attending for other reasons 10 12.0 (3.5) 12.9 (4.0)

correlations with measures of distress. The only amined for the three biggest groups of care-givers:
relationship worthy of note was the tendency for wives, husbands and daughters. These results are
younger age in the dementia sufferer to be associated shown in Table 6. The only statistically significant
with more care-giver grief. However, even this cor- effect was with the Guilt scale, F(2,133)=5.42.
relation was small in magnitude. The relationship of Daughters reported more guilt than wives (significant
the care-giver to the dementia sufferer was also ex- using modified LSD procedure). Daughters also cared

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015


88 EFFECTS ON CARE-GIVERS OF SPECIAL DAY CARE PROGRAMMES

Table 5 . Correlates of care-giver distress in group receiving day care

General Health DSSl DSSl Guilt Grief


Questionnaire anxlety depression

Characteristics of dementia sufferer

Age -0.09 -0.10 -0.13 -0.15 -0.29


Sex (O=femaie,1 =male) -0.03 0.05 0.07 -0.14 0.11
Behaviour problems 0.19 0.16 0.16 0.15 0.07

Services received

Hours/week of day care -0.02 -0.07 -0.08 -0.06 -0.09


Months of day care -0.12 -0.12 -0.11 -0.10 0.02
Number of other services -0.06 -0.04 0.01 -0.07 -0.09

Table 6. Means (and standard deviations) of care-givers on measures of distress as a function of


relationship to dementia sufferer

Relationship General Health DSSl DSSl Guilt Grief


Questionnaire anxiety depression

Wife 8.33(7.06) 4.51 (4.02) 3.08(4.46) 4.03(3.42) 11.31 (6.46)

Husband 7.52 (6.43) 3.10(3.26) 1.97(2.61) 4.67(3.98) 10.43(6.41)

Daughter 9.69(7.84) 4.71 (3.93) 2.87(3.60) 6.71 (5.20) 10.31 (5.57)

for an older group of dementia sufferers (mean age of provide some relief to the most distressed care-givers.
78.5 years) than either the husbands (72.2 years) or However, the initial psychological symptom scores of
wives (75.2 years). those who had dropped out by 3 months tended to be
higher than the scores of those still attending. In other
Discussion words, a high level of care-giver distress was, if any-
thing, predictive of dropping out rather than successful
This study, like earlier work by Gilleard et a1 [ 141 attendance at day care. Although day care did not
and Levin et al. [ 5 ] , has shown that care-givers of relieve the considerable psychological distress of care-
dementia sufferers have high levels of psychological givers, there were some positive benefits. Care-givers
symptoms that are associated with the care-giving reported having more time to themselves and more
role. Severe psychological problems were found to be often going shopping on their own. Anecdotal reports
three times more common in care-givers than in the from the interviewers in the present study indicated
general population, and those already receiving day that care-givers were very pleased with day care and
care seemed little better off than those about to receive grateful for its provision. It is, of course, possible that
it. day care has quite a specific effect on particular carers
When care-givers of new attenders were followed looking after particular kinds of dementing patients at
over 3 months, there was some reported reduction in particular stages of their dementia. However, the
distress, but this seems to be due to the re-test artefact. present study was not designed to assess specific ef-
It might be hoped that, if anything, day care would fects of this sort.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015


Y.D. WELLS, A.F. JORM, F. JORDAN, R. LEFROY 89

In view of the poor psychological health of many compared to those who relinquish the major respon-
care-givers, it is notable that receipt of other com- sibility to an institution. Perhaps the major factor
munity services was not uncommon. These carers influencing the care-giver’s psychological health is
were receiving what could be regarded, by Australian the degree to which caring is shared with others and,
standards, as a high level of community services. in this respect, residential care easily wins over com-
The relationships between the level of care-giver munity care.
distress and other variables are quite small. This study The small number of care-givers in our study who
replicates the findings of others [4]in demonstrating relinquished that role before the 3-month follow-up
a small positive correlation between the level of de- showed the most dramatic improvement in
pendent and inappropriate behaviours of the clients psychological health. This result is consistent with an
and the level of distress of their care-givers. Care-giver earlier study of the effects of residential care on care-
distress was not related to the number of day centre givers [6].
hours allotted to them or to the number of other ser- The present conclusion about the ineffectiveness of
vices which they received. This does not mean that day care in relieving care-giver distress may not apply
receiving more support does not help care-givers to to an expanded community care programme in which
cope. It may be that those care-givers who are more the burden of caring is more equitably shared. If day
distressed in the first place are offered more help by care is to have any hope of impact on the carer’s
community agencies. psychological health, it may need to be minimally
Different categories of care-givers appeared to dif- expanded to an 8.30-5.30 Monday-Friday service so
fer little in the level of distress which they felt. How- that the carer has some chance of leading an inde-
ever, daughters did seem to feel more guilty. This may pendent life. Although such an expansion may be seen
be because they had the demands of several genera- as expensive by the government, it is still cheap com-
tions of family members to cope with. They have been pared to full-time residential care and would only be
called “the sandwich generation”, under pressure on matching the type of service commonly provided for
all fronts. As might be expected, daughters looked child care in Australia. There is a need to trial and
after an older group than the other categories of care- evaluate a considerably extended community care ser-
givers. vice for dementia sufferers and their families, with the
The effect of day care on care-givers’ psychological aim of reducing care-giver distress and reducing the
state was disappointingly small. A number of reasons demand for residential care.
can be advanced for this. Some of the distress may Finally, it is interesting to note who is shouldering
have been a reaction to the diagnosis of dementia in a the burden of community care for dementia sufferers.
relative and all that this implies. Day care could not be In our day care study, 73% of the carers were women.
expected to relieve distress of this sort. Another major Wives made up 39%, daughters and daughters-in-law
source of distress is the responsibility and time 33% and husbands 20%. Interestingly, sons made up
demands of providing care. Day care would be ex- only 3% of carers. Most of the spouse carers would be
pected to help in this area by taking a share of the elderly, but this would not be so for the daughters and
burden of caring. However, the amount of relief, in daughters-in-law. As participation in the paid
terms of time, which day care typically provided was workforce becomes increasingly the norm for women,
rather small. In our study, the average attendance was the pool of younger women available for unpaid care
1 1.9 hours a week. Viewed in the context of the 168 may decrease and the pressure for an increased level
hour week which caring for a dementia sufferer invol- of community support may increase. This has been the
ves, the care-giver still takes responsibility for 93% of trend with child care in Australia. In many ways, the
the week. Even allowing for other community ser- term “community care” is a misnomer. The sociologist
vices, like respite and district nursing, the care-giver Wilkin [IS] has pointed this out clearly with com-
still takes the lion’s share of the load. If the dementia munity care for the intellectually handicapped:
sufferer were to move into residential care, the care- “...community care does not mean care by the com-
giver’s share would of course shrink dramatically. In munity, nor does it mean care by the family, it means
other words, there is presently a considerable im- maternal care with varying but generally low levels of
balance in the degree of help provided to care-givers support from others”. If we substituted “care by wives
who look after a dementia sufferer in the community and daughters” for “maternal care”, this quotation

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015


90 EFFECTS ON CARE-GIVERS OF SPECIAL DAY CARE PROGRAMMES

could well summarize the current situation with com- 8. Henderson S, Byme DG, Duncan-Jones P. Neurosis and the So-
cial Environment. Academic Press, Sydney 198l.
munity care of dementia sufferers in Australia. 9. Gilleard CJ, Gilleard E, Gledhill K, Whittick J. Caring for the
elderly mentally infirm at home: a survey of the supporters. Jour-
Acknowledgements nal of Epidemiology and Community Health 1984;38:3 19-325.
10. Goldberg DP. Manual for the General Health Questionnaire. Nel-
We wish to thank the following for their assistance: the participat- son Publishing Company, Windsor 1978.
ing care-givers; Ann Don, Joan Cume, Mathilde Bachouse and Jan 11, Goodchild ME, Duncan-Jones, P. Chronicity and the General
Elliott, research assistants; Brian Moss, Director, Moorfields Com- Health Questionnaire. British Journal of Psychiatry 1985:14655-
munity for Adult Care; A.S. Henderson, Director, NH&MRC Social 61.
Psychiatry Research Unit; Marie Morgan and Penny Evans, for 12. Bedford A, Foulds GA, Sheffield BF. A new personal distur-
secretarial assistance; Angelo Carlini, social worker, Sunshine bance scale (DSSI/sAD). British Journal of Social and Clinical
Regional Geriatric Assessment Team; Sr. Rene Thomas, “Gwen- Psychology 1976; 151387-394.
nap”Day Centre; Bill Best and Sr. Rosemary Houlihan, “Strathdon” 13. Henderson S, Duncan-Jones P, Byrne DG, Scott R, Adcock S.
Community; Yvonne Hargrave, Doncaster and Templestowe Day Psychiatric disorder in Canberra: A standardised study of
Centre: Olive Graham, “The Haven” We Care Centre: Sr. Ann prevalence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 1979;60:355-374.
Stannus and Jackie McCormack, “Hurlingham” Day Centre: Ber- 14. Gilleard CJ, Belford H, Gilleard E, Whittick JE, Gledhill K.
nadette Byrne, M.E.C.W.A. Day Centre; Sr. Eunice Barter, Emotional distress amongst the supporters of the elderly mentally
“Gatehouse” Day Centre; Bert Fine, “Maranoa House”, Lisa infirm. British Journal of Psychiatry 1984;145:172-177.
Drayton and Trish Maggs, “Caladenia” Day Centre; Sandra Kep- 15. Wilkin D. Caring for the Mentally Handicapped Child. Biddles
pich-Arnold, “Oswald Street Group”; Anglican Homes Incor- Ltd., Guildford Surrey 1979.
porated, Perth; Joan Burn, “Bull Creek Club”, Perth; Heather
Hockings, ADARDA Centre, Gold Coast; Margaret Cooke, ADAR-
DA Centre, Gordon Park; Mark McDonald, West End Day Centre;
Andrea Taylor, “Bethesda” Day Centre; Karen Grinlaubs, Coopers
Plains Day Centre; Blue Nursing Service, Queensland Wesley Guilt Scale
Central Mission, Brisbane.
This study was supported by a grant from the Can- 1. I feel guilty regarding my decision to seek help for my relative
2. I keep thinking I should be doing more for my relative
berra Office, Commonwealth Department of Com-
3. I worry about whether my relative is cared for well enough
munity Services and Health. 4. I feel I may have contributed in some way to my relative’s ill-
ness
References 5. I am failing to live up to my own expectations as a caregiver
6. I feel bad about my lack of patience with my relative
7. It seems to me that more should be done for my relative
I. Preston GAN. Dementia in elderly adults: Prevalence and in-
8. I feel bad because my relative has the illness instead of me
stitutionalisation. Journal of Gerontology 1986:4 1:261-267.
9. I sometimes feel guilty because I can enjoy myself
2. Jorm AF, Korten AE. A method for calculating projected in-
creases in the number of dementia sufferers. Australian and New
Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 1988;22: 183-189. Grief Scale
3. Department of Community Services. Nursing Homes and Hostels
Review. Australian Government Publishing Service, Canberra 1. I miss not being able to talk to my relative
1986. 2. I often daydream about how my relative used to be
4. Moms RG, Moms LW, Britton PG. Factors affecting the emo- 3. I don’t like to think about how my relative is more than I have
tional well-being of the care-givers of dementia sufferers. British to
Journal of Psychiatry 1988;153:147-156. 4. I feel angry when I think about my relative
5. Levin E, Sinclair I, Gorbach P. The Supporters of Confused Elder- 5.1 frequently feel like crying about my relative
ly Persons at Home. National Institute for Social Work Research 6. I ask myself, “Why has this happened to my relative?”
Unit, London 1983. 7. I can’t help hoping that my relative will recover
6. Wells Y, Jorm AF. Evaluation of a special nursing home unit for 8. I feel helpless in the face of my relative’s illness
dementia sufferers: A randomised controlled comparison with 9. I feel that grief has aged me
community care. Australian and New Zealand Journal of 10. Funerals upset me
Psychiatry 1987;21:524-53 1.
7. Gilleard CJ. Influence of emotional distress among supporters on Items from both scales are rated as: Not at all, A little, A lot or Al-
the outcome of psychogeriatric day care. British Journal of most unbearably
Psychiatry 1987;150:219-223.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE DE MONTREAL on July 7, 2015

You might also like