Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 59

Running head: Dynamics of Shared Leadership

Shared Leadership Development and Team Performance: A New Look at the

Dynamics of Shared Leadership

Natalia M. Lorinkova
Georgetown University1
McDonough School of Business
37th & O streets
Washington, DC 20057
E-mail: nataly.lorinkova@georgetown.edu

Kathryn M. Bartol
Univers of Maryland
Management & Organization Department
Robert H. Smith School of Business
4530 Van Munching Hall
College Park, MD 20742
E-mail: kbartol@rhsmith.umd.edu

1
ESADE Business School, Barcelona, Spain as of June 2021

Author’s Note: We would like to thank Patrick McKay and two anonymous reviewers for
their thoughtful insights during the review process. We are also grateful to Sara Jansen
Perry, Brooks Holtom, Nick Lovegrove, and Mincho Minev for help with the data
collection.

Abstract

The present study offers new theoretical insights into the dynamics of shared leadership.

Integrating arguments from shared leadership and team development theory, we examine how

shared leadership changes over the course of a project team’s life cycle and how this pattern

of change relates to team performance. Guided by shared leadership theory and project team

literature, we also explore team level factors, which may alter the pattern of shared leadership

development. In particular, we propose that in project teams shared leadership develops in a

non-uniform way, approximating an inverted U-shape pattern, increasing early in the team’s

This article has been accepted for publication and undergone full peer review but has not been
through the copyediting, typesetting, pagination and proofreading process, which may lead to
differences between this version and the Version of Record. Please cite this article as doi:
10.1111/peps.12409.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


2

life cycle, peaking around the mid-point, and then decreasing in the later phase. In turn, this

development pattern relates positively to team performance. We also extend theory by

explaining how specific team characteristics influence the pattern of shared leadership

development. Using a three-study approach, we empirically examine the hypothesized

relationships and conclude with a general discussion of the theoretical and practical

implications of our findings.

Key Words: shared leadership, team development, project teams, team performance

Introduction

With the increased use of teams in organizations over the last three decades, a number

of researchers have shifted their focus from examining traditional forms of leadership to

investigating shared leadership, recognizing shared leadership as an important driver of team

effectiveness (Carson, Tesluk, & Marrone, 2007; D’Innocenco, Mathieu, & Kukenberger,

2016; Wang, Waldman, & Zhang, 2014). Burgeoning research demonstrates evidence for the

positive effects of shared leadership on a number of team outcomes (e.g., Hoch 2013;

McIntyre & Foti, 2013; Pearce & Sims, 2000; Pearce, Yoo, & Alavi, 2004;

Sivasubramaniam, Murry, Avolio, & Jung, 2002). This, coupled with evidence from three

recent meta-analyses (D’Innocenco et al., 2016; Nicolaides et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2014),

outlines an overall positive role of shared leadership in teams. Researchers, however, have

not always found evidence of the positive effects of shared leadership on final team outcomes

(e.g., Boies, Lvina, & Martens, 2010; Hmielski, Cole, & Baron, 2012; Serban & Roberts,

2016). This leaves important unanswered questions about the nature of shared leadership and

its role in teams.

Complicating matters further is a lack of in-depth theorizing about the dynamic nature

of shared leadership and its association with team outcomes, despite explicit definitions

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


3

referring to shared leadership as a “dynamic, interactive influence process among individuals

in groups” (Pearce & Conger, 2003, p. 1). Instead, the vast majority of the literature has

adopted a static approach (e.g., Carson et al., 2007; Chiu, Owens, & Tesluk, 2016; Ensley,

Hmieleski, & Pearce, 2006), conceptualizing shared leadership as a one-time, static

antecedent to different team outcomes (Contractor, DeChurch, Carson, Carter, & Keegan,

2012; Pearce & Conger, 2003. Thus, scholarly understanding of the interplay between time

and shared leadership is still limited, despite recent work cautioning that shared leadership

may take time to develop (Barnett & Weidenfeller, 2016). In fact, in their review work,

Wassenaar and Pearce (2012) pose the question “… can a group develop shared leadership

from its inception?” (p. 379) and identify the “life-cycle of the group or the project” (p. 379)

as a potential factor influencing shared leadership. Unfortunately, theoretical

conceptualizations examining shared leadership development as a function of team

development are unavailable. As a result, conceptually, we still have unknowns not only

about the nature of shared leadership development, but also about the relationship between

the dynamic changes in shared leadership and team outcomes.

Ambiguous empirical evidence also exacerbates theoretical confusion on the interplay

between shared leadership and team development. On the one hand, Nicolaides and

colleagues (2014) found that team tenure negatively impacted the relationship between

shared leadership and team performance, and suggested that team members might be unable

to “sustain the sharing of leadership over a long period of time” (p. 935). In contrast, Wang et

al., (2014) proposed that passage of time might “enhance the sharedness of leadership in a

team” (p. 192). However, all agree that time plays an important role in the development of

shared leadership and call for “additional theoretical and empirical attention” in this direction

(Nicolaides et al., 2014, p. 935).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


4

Starting the conversation on the important issue of shared leadership development

over time, Drescher, Korsgaard, Welpe, and Picot (2014) theorized and found support for

positive changes in shared leadership being associated with positive changes in team

performance. However, despite acknowledging shared leadership as a dynamic phenomenon

that “may change over time in any number of ways” (p. 771), this study made the implicit

assumption that shared leadership in teams followed a similar, progressively linear pattern of

increase over time. This linear model, however, is somewhat inconsistent with the non-

sequential team development framework (Chang, Bordia, & Duck, 2003; Gersick, 1988,

1989), and especially with the well-known punctuated equilibrium model (PEM: Gersick,

1988; 1989) that argues for a non-uniform development of the different processes in project

teams, dependent upon the focal team’s development phase. The PEM (Gersick, 1988, 1989),

however, focused mainly on progress with the task itself and left unexplored the relational

development underlying leadership, particularly shared leadership. As such, it does not

directly address shared leadership development and, indeed, may mask how leadership might

be developing in teams.

Additionally, the progressively linear model of shared leadership development

(Drescher et al., 2014) indirectly endorses the idea that team processes, including shared

leadership, unfold similarly for all teams; an assumption challenged by shared leadership

theory (Sims & Pearce, 2000; Wassenaar & Pearce, 2012). Shared leadership theory

identifies a number of team-level factors that might be expected to moderate the emergence

and the effectiveness of shared leadership. Unfortunately, this work stops short of

conceptualizing and examining how these relevant factors might influence the development

of shared leadership.

As a result, further conceptualization is needed to explain how shared leadership

develops over the course of a team’s life cycle, and to shed light on the team-level factors that

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


5

may qualify this development. This theorizing may be especially needed and relevant to self-

managed project teams (otherwise known as autonomous project teams: Bergman, Small,

Bergman, & Bowling, 2014), which, in response to fast-changing markets and dynamic

business environments, are becoming a growing phenomenon in organizations (Ilgen,

Hollenbeck, Johnson, & Jundt, 2005; Oh, 2012). Such teams face strict project deadlines and

do not have a formal, designated leader (Farh, Lee, & Farh, 2010; Knight, 2012; Langfred,

2004). Because these teams lack the guidance, role clarity, and physical and moral support

that come from a traditional vertical leader (e.g., Kozlowski, Gully, Nason, & Smith, 1999;

Lorinkova, Pearsall, & Sims, 2013), they have to develop their own mechanisms for

guidance, coordination, and, ultimately, for accomplishing the team task.

In response, we designed the current research to address the above-identified

theoretical voids. By doing so, we contribute to the shared leadership and project team

literature in several ways. First, we integrate arguments from the project team development

literature with reasoning from the shared leadership literature to conceptualize and explain

how shared leadership changes over the course of a project team’s life cycle. Recent

delineations of team development in project teams suggest two distinct phases that

characterize their life cycle (Farh et al., 2010; Ford & Sullivan, 2004; Knight, 2012), with

team needs and its members’ priorities changing accordingly. To foreshadow our theorizing,

we argue that, consistent with shared leadership theory (Pearce & Sims, 2000; Houghton,

Neck, & Manz, 2003; Wassenaar & Pearce, 2012), and in response to a team’s changing

needs and members’ priorities, shared leadership intensifies during the early phase of a

project team’s life cycle to peak at the middle, and then it diminishes in the later team

development phase. By conceptualizing a curvilinear, inverted U-shape pattern of shared

leadership development, we depart from the punctuated equilibrium model of (team)

development (Gersick, 1988, 1989), which considered task accomplishment and provided

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


6

little insight into other aspects of team development. In fact, if applied to shared leadership

development, the punctuated model suggests that shared leadership remains almost static (not

very much changes) until the midpoint, and then beyond. Thus, by delineating a non-uniform

trajectory of shared leadership development approximating an inverted U-shape, we not only

illuminate how shared leadership develops, but we also clarify when a team most needs it. In

this way, we propose alternative insights about why some researchers may have failed to find

support for a positive relationship between shared leadership and group performance. Gully

(2000) suggests that, “…to fully understand work teams, researchers must investigate how

team dynamics develop and change over time” (p. 35). To answer this call, and as a second

contribution, we link shared leadership dynamics to final team performance. In this way, we

contribute to both shared leadership literature and that of more general (project) teams’

effectiveness, arguing for the need to consider the effectiveness of shared leadership as a

function of team development. Finally, guided by the intersection of shared leadership theory

(Pearce & Sims, 2000; Wassenaar & Pearce, 2012; Zhu, Liao, Yam, & Johnson, 2018) and

the project team literature (e.g., Akgün, Keskin, Byrne, & Imamoglu, 2007; Huckman &

Staats, 2011; Huckman, Staats, & Upton, 2009; Mueller, 2012), we conceptualize how three

team level factors moderate the pattern of shared leadership development. In this way, we

contribute to shared leadership theory and the project team development literature

(Okhyusen, 2002) by answering the question of why some teams undergo different shared

leadership development patterns.

Shared Leadership and Project Team Development

Unlike traditional vertical leadership, often defined as influencing others to achieve a

specific objective (Yukl, 1989), shared leadership is defined as “an emergent and dynamic

team phenomenon whereby leadership roles and influence are distributed among team

members” (D’Innocenzo et al., 2016, p. 1968). Because of its collective nature, shared

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


7

leadership is often touted as especially suited to meet the needs of teams (Carson et al., 2007;

Carter, DeChurch, Braun, & Contractor, 2015). Researchers, however, have started to draw

attention to the inherently dynamic nature of shared leadership (e.g., Drescher et al., 2014)

and its potential implications. This, coupled with our focus on project teams and the notion

that team members’ interactions (which constitute the basis of shared leadership) tend to shift

over a team’s life cycle (Farh et al., 2010; Gerpott, Lehmann-Willenbrock, Voelpel, & van

Vugt, 2019; Marks, Mathieu, & Zaccaro, 2001), necessitates a closer look at the dynamics of

shared leadership as they relate to project team development, and ultimately performance.

Team development refers to the “the path a group takes over its life-span toward the

accomplishment of its main tasks” (Gersick, 1988, p. 9). Extending Gersick’s (non-

sequential) model of team development, more recent delineations of task progress in project

teams identify two phases, often referred to as Phase 1 (or early phase) and Phase 2 (or later

phase: Bergman et al., 2014; Farh et al., 2010; Ford & Sullivan, 2004), separated by a mid-

point (Knight, 2012). This also aligns with Marks et al., (2001) who point to transition and

action phases, and describe a team’s life cycle as action phases bifurcated by transition

phases. As Knight notes, “the literature on team pacing in the face of a deadline depicts two

phases of task progress, separated by the midpoint, that are characterized by two different

motivational frames” (2012, p. 102).

Gersick’s prominent work, however, argues further for particular scenarios associated

with the two phases and the midpoint. Accordingly, Phase 1, starting with the team’s first

meeting and continuing until the mid-point, is characterized by inertia, with initially

established behavioral and interactional patterns remaining relatively unchanged until the

team reaches a mid-point transition. During the transition, “in a concentrated burst of

activity” (Gersick, 1989, p. 278), groups revise their old patterns, adopt new perspectives and

establish their new approach to task accomplishment. Once the transition period is over,

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


8

teams enter Phase 2, which the (punctuated equilibrium) model describes as a second period

of inertia.

One limitation of Gersick’s work for our purposes, however, is its exclusive focus on

task accomplishment. As a result, applying it directly to a conceptualization of the

development of interpersonal processes – such as shared leadership – might lead to faulty

conclusions. In fact, a direct application of the punctuated equilibrium model would suggest

that shared leadership changes in a non-uniform way, approximating an S-shape pattern,

rapidly intensifying predominantly during the transitional mid-point and remaining relatively

static (inert, in Gersick’s parlance) during Phase 1 and Phase 2. Applying arguments from the

shared leadership and project team literature, however, we posit a different model within the

overall two-phase and midpoint framework. Specifically, we argue for a U-shaped pattern of

shared leadership development, which consistent with the team’s changing needs increases

during the early project team life-cycle phase, peaks around the mid-point, and then decreases

in the later phase (Farh et al., 2010; Kozlowski et al., 1999; Pearce & Sims, 2002). Figure 1

summarizes our theoretical model.

--------------------------------
Insert Figure 1 about here
---------------------------------

To begin with, Phase 1, coinciding with what other researchers refer to as the project

formulation stage (Ford & Sullivan, 2004) or premature phase (Chang et al., 2003), is

characterized by relatively abundant time perceptions and unclearly defined tasks and

members’ roles (Chang et al., 2003; Knight, 2012). During this phase, the focus is on

gathering data, idea generation, and learning about the task (Ford & Sullivan, 2004; Gersick,

1989; Farh et al., 2010). To accomplish this, team members engage in increased levels of

interpersonal exchanges, aimed at role clarification (Ford & Sullivan, 2004). In addition,

Phase 1 is characterized by articulating a number of tentative solutions, as well as searching

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


9

for useful information and engaging in strategy formulation. In traditional teams, early in

their development, vertical leadership is of primary importance, with members relying on the

leader for guidance and coordination (Chang et al., 2003; Kozlowski et al., 1999; Wheelan,

2009). In self-managed project teams, however, in the absence of a vertical leader, members

are likely to increasingly engage in exhibiting and attributing leadership influence over each

other in order to meet the team’s needs for coordination, work organization, and for

establishing the team’s social climate (Pearce & Conger, 2003). In addition, as noted by

Chang and colleagues (2003), during the early phase of team development, team members

“struggle with issues such as power, structure, and intimacy” (p. 108). In the absence of a

designated leader, members are likely to achieve higher role clarity and assert their claims for

power by more actively engaging in the social influence process of leadership. As a result, we

expect that shared leadership, exerted through reciprocal influence behaviors and attributions,

is likely to intensify as project teams progress with their development during Phase 1.

Once Phase 1 is completed, the PEM predicts that a transitional point occurs roughly

around the mid-point of the group’s allotted time (Chang et al., 2003; Gersick, 1988; 1989).

The project team literature asserts that during the transitional point, which separates Phase 1

and Phase 2, “team members’ interactions and priorities tend to shift drastically” (Farh et al.,

2010, p. 1173). With members’ focus and priorities shifting to the impending project

deadline, members need to formulate interactional strategies and establish behavioral patterns

that would enable the team to reach its goal (Gersick, 1988, 1999; Knight, 2012). The shared

leadership literature implies that, as a relational interaction structure, shared leadership is

likely to facilitate accomplishment of the team’s objective by influencing the team’s

motivation and its task accomplishment strategies (Carson et al., 2007; DeRue & Ashford,

2010). Thus, during the mid-point, consistent with feelings of time urgency and an increased

focus on the team’s task, members are likely to increase their attempts to influence team

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


10

decisions, with the team experiencing a period of intense mutual influences – i.e., a high level

of shared leadership. This high level of shared leadership likely also facilitates a needed role

renegotiation, because members exert influence consistent with their abilities and motivation

to contribute to the team’s final performance goal. Additionally, through their engagement in

leadership, team members bring more resources to the task and share more information (Katz

& Kahn, 1978), which is crucial at this point of team development when the availability of

time and resources starts to diminish (Farh et al., 2010). Thus, through high levels of shared

leadership during the mid-point transitional period, teams meet their need to move toward a

workable team role structure and viable project completion strategies. This, we propose,

positively influences final team performance. In addition, a high level of shared leadership

suggests that team members interact, communicate, and exchange information and

knowledge intensely (Nicolaides et al., 2014), which allows the team to adjust its initially

chosen strategies to ones that are more appropriate for final task execution.

As the project team enters Phase 2 of its development, focus shifts toward “task

execution and strategic implementation in order to meet deadlines” (Farh et al., 2010, p.

1174). With the need for project completion intensifying and looming closer during this

phase, we expect that, for teams to be successful, members would need to focus their efforts

on executing their own assigned tasks. In support of our concept, Chang and colleagues

(2003) argue that at this point members tend to focus on “work.” Consequentially, we expect

that exerting leadership influences at this point is likely to interfere with the directions and

patterns of interactions established and agreed upon during the midpoint transition.

Advancing a similar rationale, Mathieu, Tannenbaum, Donsbach, and Miller (2014), suggest

that this later stage of the team’s development is more likely to be dominated by task-

execution activities, with personal task-related knowledge and abilities playing more

prominent roles in the team’s effectiveness. As individuals focus on executing their own

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


11

tasks, following strategies already established during the transition point, members are likely

to decrease their engagement in and reliance on shared leadership. In addition, as power

struggles are already settled and team members’ roles have become more well-defined (Chant

et al., 2003), attempting to influence team members in these aspects is likely to distract from

the team’s focus on project completion. Thus, in a departure from what might be suggested

by the traditional punctuated model of team development, we propose that engagement in

shared leadership in project teams would decrease, rather than be sustained at a relatively

high level, during the later phase of team development.

Overall, we propose that the more shared leadership builds during the early phase of a

team’s lifecycle, the more members are able to exert leadership influence during the

transition mid-point, which then enables the team to arrive at reasonably optimal team level

structures, and to set forth successful task execution strategies. However, if members

continue to engage in leadership behaviors during the later phase (Phase 2) of team

development, this would likely divert resources from their individual task focus and the

team’s overall progress toward successful project completion. Supporting this reasoning,

Knight (2012) and Ford and Sullivan (2004) suggest that continued idea generation and novel

contributions during the later phase of a project team’s development negatively influence

team performance. Therefore, a sustained increase in shared leadership during the later team

development phase is likely to hurt the overall performance of the team, whereas an initial

increase, peak, and then decrease in shared leadership is likely to meet the team’s time-bound

needs and thus facilitate performance. Combining our arguments from above, we formally

hypothesize:

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


12

Hypothesis 1. Shared leadership will change over time following a curvilinear

pattern, increasing during Phase 1 of project team development, peaking around the

mid-point, and decreasing during Phase 2 (H1a). This curvilinear pattern,

approximating an inverted U-shape, will relate positively to team performance (H1b).

Team Characteristics as Moderators of Shared Leadership Development

So far, we have examined the development of shared leadership over the course of a

team’s life cycle. In these arguments, however, one assumes that all teams undergo a similar

pattern of shared leadership development. In their review and expansion of shared leadership

theory, Wassenaar and Pearce (2012) point to the need for further research on several team

moderators of major theoretical interest that may meaningfully influence the emergence and

effectiveness of shared leadership. In parallel, and independent of the shared leadership

literature, project team literature has also earmarked three of these same moderator areas for

special concern (e.g., Akgün et al., 2007; Huckman & Staats, 2011; Huckman et al., 2009;

Mueller, 2012). Accordingly, in line with our theory building with respect to shared

leadership and project teams, we focus on these three theoretically relevant and important

boundary conditions, which have the potential to significantly influence the configuration of

shared leadership development in project teams: team size, team social support, and team

members’ familiarity. We elaborate on the theoretical importance of each of these below.

Team Size

Theoretical and practical evidence have long shown how group size affects group

social phenomena (Kozlowski & Bell, 2003), however, its role in the context of shared

leadership remains unclear (Conger & Pearce, 2003; Cox, Pearce, & Perry, 2003; Nicolaides

et al., 2014; Seers, Keller, & Wilkerson, 2003; Wassenaar & Pearce, 2012). As noted by

Nicolaides and colleagues, team size “can be both an asset and a liability for teams” (p. 926).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


13

For instance, larger groups enjoy a number of advantages: with the increase in the number of

team members, groups have more resources and capabilities to solve group tasks (e.g., Hill,

1982; Jackson, 1992), with more members able to offer leadership influence. This view is

consistent with the notion that "at a basic level, the resources available on a team result from

how many people are on it" (Hambrick & D’Aveni, 1992, p. 1449). Research focused on

project teams also identifies group size as an important compositional characteristic, which

may influence the effectiveness of these teams (e.g., Huckman & Staats, 2011; Mehta &

Bharadwaj, 2015; Mueller, 2012). For example, Hoegl (2005) argued that smaller project

teams performed better, whereas Klug and Bagrow (2016) found that larger teams were more

successful. This mixed evidence suggests there is theoretical and empirical ambiguity

surrounding the role of team size in project teams.

A major shortcoming of both streams of literature, which conceptualize group size as

an important boundary condition, is the static treatment of shared leadership. Thus,

Wassenaar and Pearce (2012) imply that group size may “facilitate” or “enable” shared

leadership in teams, but they stop short of theorizing how group size may influence the

development of shared leadership across a team’s life cycle. Similarly, in project team

literature, Mehta and Bharadwaj (2015) provide evidence to support the notion that team size

influences members’ interactions, but the authors do not explain how the configurational

development of these interactions might be influenced by group size. By examining group

size as a moderator of shared leadership development, our study offers an alternative to

understanding the role of team size; we argue that team size influences the time-bound

changes in members’ shared leadership interactions, and not merely the absolute level of

shared leadership or its effectiveness.

In the general team literature, researchers tend to agree that coordination and

communication problems are exacerbated when team size increases (Campion, Medsker, &

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


14

Higgs, 1993; Hackman, 1987). In addition to the coordination and process loss associated

with higher team size, the so-called dilution effect perspective (Karau & Williams, 1993;

Kidwell & Bennett, 1993) argues that in larger teams, members consider their individual

contributions as less critical. Members are less motivated to contribute (Karau & Williams,

1993; Kidwell & Bennett, 1993) because individual efforts become less visible and the

monitoring of individual outputs becomes more difficult (Albanese & Van Fleet, 1985;

Alnuaimi, Robert, & Maruping, 2010).

Because by its essence shared leadership involves members’ engagement in different

leadership functions with the motivation to contribute to the team, our theorizing is consistent

with the dilution perspective. In self-managed project teams, members have limited time to

accomplish a specific purpose, and no formal guidance system (Langfred, 2004). This may

necessitate that members engage in higher levels of mutual influences to accomplish optimal

team planning and decision-making. We expect that as team size increases, members would

be less willing or less able to see the need to engage in mutual influences, and consequently

would tend to refrain from exercising leadership influence. As team size increases,

communication and collaboration within the team are also likely to deteriorate (Hambrick &

D’Aveni, 1992), which would additionally reduce the extent to which, and the intensity with

which, members take on leadership roles. As a result, and consistent with our predicted

curvilinear pattern of shared leadership development in project teams, we expect smaller

teams to reach their peaks in shared leadership (or the inflection point, at which shared

leadership starts to decrease) at a higher level than larger teams.

Hypothesis 2. Team size will moderate the curvilinear pattern of shared leadership

development in project teams, such that smaller teams will reach the inflection point

at a higher level of shared leadership.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


15

Team Social Support

A second potentially important moderator under investigation is social support

(Goodman & Leyden, 1991), or the extent to which members care for and encourage one

another (Marks et al., 2001). Both shared leadership (e.g., Carson et al., 2007; Zhu et al.,

2018) and project team literature (e.g., Demarco & Listen, 1987) target social support, or

“peopleware” (Akgün et al., 2007) as an important boundary condition to influence project

team processes and effectiveness. Practical evidence also identifies poor social relationships

as one of the barriers faced by project teams (SHRM, 2018). Research, however, has not

examined how social support may qualify the time-bound development of shared leadership,

despite having identified social relations as a potential “enabler” of shared leadership

(Wassenaar & Pearce, 2012).

Earlier in the team development, when team members do not have a clear

understanding of their roles and their power struggles are abundant, team members’ support

for one another is likely to create an environment in which members feel comfortable

claiming leadership roles for themselves and granting those roles to others. A positive,

supportive environment increases members’ commitment to their group (Chiaburu &

Harrison, 2008) and allows members the opportunity to exercise influence (e.g., offer

solutions or relevant strategies, distribute tasks among other members, influence the team’s

direction) without the fear of being rejected or embarrassed (Carmeli, 2007; May, Gilson, &

Harter, 2004; Roussin, 2008). Thus, when team members experience high levels of social

support (especially early in the team’s development), they are likely more comfortable with

engaging in leadership influences, which would enable the team as a whole to reach a higher

overall level of shared leadership. By contrast, in teams with lower levels of social support,

team members are likely cautious with their engagement in leadership, either out of fear of

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


16

repercussions or criticism, or because they are uncertain about the fruitfulness of their efforts

(May et al., 2004). In addition, when social support is low, team members are less likely to

grant leadership roles to other team members, as a way of protecting their own place on the

team. Hence, we formally hypothesize:

Hypothesis 3. Social support will moderate the curvilinear pattern of shared

leadership development, such that teams enjoying higher levels of social support will

reach the inflection point at a higher level of shared leadership.

Team members’ familiarity

Our third moderator of interest, team members’ familiarity, reflects the extent to

which members have prior interpersonal knowledge about other team members (Okhuysen,

2001). Despite the fact that, from its inception, shared leadership theory advocates that

“group member familiarity with one another should impact the display and form of shared

leadership” (Pearce & Sims, 2000, p. 129), the theoretical and empirical evidence

surrounding the role of familiarity in project teams remains ambiguous (Espinosa, Slaughter,

Kraut, & Herbsleb, 2007; Huckman et al., 2009; Gruenfeld, Mannix, Williams, & Neale,

1996; Kor, 2003; Zenger & Lawrence, 1989) . As noted by Harrison and colleagues “Perhaps

the most striking feature of the findings on familiarity is their inconsistency” (2003, p. 637).

For example, Jen and Shah (1997) found that familiarity positively influenced teams’

decision making, whereas Kim (1997) found the opposite effect. Shared leadership theory

and project teams’ research have conceptualized familiarity as a boundary condition, which

qualifies mostly the emergence or the effectiveness of static team processes (e.g., Akgün et

al., 2007; Wassenaar & Pearce, 2012) despite team development scholars explicitly arguing

that the value of familiarity might be time dependent (Guzzo & Dicksom, 1996; Harrison et

al., 2003). Therefore, additional research is needed to clarify how members’ familiarity

influences the pattern of shared leadership development in project teams.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


17

Arguments from the project team development literature suggest that members with

higher familiarity likely start (or quickly arrive) at higher levels of shared leadership earlier in

the team’s development than teams with lower familiarity. Past experiences facilitate team

coordination and communication in project teams (Akgün et al., 2007) and allow team

members to start work with lower levels of uncertainty and anxiety about social acceptance

(e.g., Hinds, Carley, Krackhardt, & Wholey, 2000). Members who are familiar with one

another tend not to waste time orienting themselves to a new project or task (Espinoza et al.,

2007; Katz, 1982). Instead, they dive right into soliciting ideas, generating solutions, and

discussing project execution strategies earlier in their team’s development. This would imply

that familiar team members engage in higher levels of shared leadership earlier in the team

development. Familiar members also do not need to establish themselves anew with the team,

and as a result, they likely more easily (in comparison to unfamiliar members) overcome the

power struggles and role clarification, which characterize Phase 1 of the project team

development. Familiar group members also rely more heavily on past experiences (Beckman,

2006). As a result, they find it easier to establish patterns of social interactions consistent

with these experiences, because, as Pearce and Sims note, they already have a “basis for

working together” (2000, p. 129), which they might be less willing to alter over time.

In contrast, unfamiliar team members are faced with the more challenging need to

familiarize themselves not only with the project requirements but also with one another’s

knowledge, abilities, and roles on the team. Thus, unfamiliar team members need more

mutual guidance and influence, which is likely accomplished by increased engagement in

shared leadership as the team progresses with its Phase 1 development. In addition, when the

team first gets together, team members’ unfamiliarity may limit the initial display of shared

leadership (Pearce & Sims, 2000); thus unfamiliar teams will start at a lower level of shared

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


18

leadership. This would make our conceptualized U-shaped pattern of shared leadership

development more pronounced in those teams.

Hypothesis 4. Team familiarity will moderate the pattern of shared leadership

development such that the curvilinear U-shaped pattern predicted in Hypothesis 1 will

be more pronounced in teams with less familiarity.

In what follows, we present the results of three studies in which we test our

hypotheses. In the first study, sampling master’s level students working in semester-long

project teams, we test Hypotheses 1, 2 and 3. In Study 2, sampling self-managed project

teams working in the IT sector, we assess Hypotheses 1 and 2. Finally, in Study 3 we test all

hypotheses.

Study 1: Methods, Results, and Discussion

Sample and Procedure

Participants in this study were students in an evening master’s-level human resource

management course in a large Midwestern University. After obtaining approval from the

affiliated Institutional Review Board (IRB #026812B3E, “Work, Leadership, and

Performance”), we collected data from four sections of the same course, taught over two

semesters by the same instructor who was blind to the study’s hypotheses. As a part of the

curriculum, participants were required to work on a team project. The project lasted 11

weeks, with participants randomly assigned to teams in week 4 of the respective 15-week-

long semesters. Each team consisted of 3 to 6 individuals, with an average of 5.35 members

per team. Our final sample included complete observations on 31 teams.

As part of the team project, participants were invited to complete online surveys four

times during the semester. Participation in the surveys was voluntary, but students who

completed all four surveys were awarded 2 % extra course credit. The first survey was

administered in week 5, shortly after students were assigned to teams, and the second survey

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


19

was administered in week 8. Survey 3 was administered in week 11; and the last, survey 4,

was administered in week 14, when team deliverables were due. 176 students completed

Survey 1, 173 Survey 2, 170 Survey 3, and 164 Survey 4. None of the 31 teams surveyed had

more than one team member who did not complete a survey at each time point.

For all four surveys, we used online data collection with a round-robin design in

which team members provided self-descriptive information and rated each other member

(Warner, Kenny, & Stoto, 1979) on the extent to which they engaged in shared leadership. To

incentivize individual team members to participate actively in the team project and the online

surveys, at the start of the project students were told that the last survey would contain a

confidential peer evaluation form, allowing students to recommend a grade reduction for

team members who failed to participate actively in the team project. As part of the last survey

(4), students were also given the opportunity to include open-ended comments about their

team experience. A few students (31 in total) shared some concerns about team members’

work quality, and 11 students recommended grade reductions for a team member.

The team project required students to develop a plan for a recruitment process (based

on a real job advertisement), role-play the recruitment and hiring process as an in-class

presentation, and prepare a written report with an analysis of the process and hiring

recommendations for the company. As a master’s level assignment, this project simulated a

realistic job recruitment. Students were expected to apply course material and concepts such

as HR planning and design, recruitment, assessment and compliance to develop appropriate

mechanisms for reaching out to potential applicants and conducting job interviews, while at

the same time complying with legal requirements for equal employment opportunity in the

work place. All team members were expected to participate in the presentation. All teams

received written instructions with a grading rubric, together with written comments on the

outline, which was due in week 10. Team performance accounted for 20 % of each member’s

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


20

final course grade. Final team performance was measured by a composite score, combining

grades on the teams’ presentations and written reports.

Measures

Shared leadership. In this study, we conceptualized shared leadership following the

social network approach (Carson et al., 2007; Mathieu, Kukenberger, D’Innocenzo, & Reilly,

2015) and measured it using social network density. Following extant work (Carson et al,

2007; Mathieu et al., 2015; Serban & Roberts, 2016) every team member rated each of

his/her individual peers (1 = not at all to 5 = to a very great extent) on the extent to which the

team relies on each of these specific individuals for leadership. Indexed in this way, shared

leadership reflects the total amount of leadership displayed by team members as perceived by

others on a team. To calculate the density shared leadership score for each team, we summed

all individual values (team members’ ratings of each other’s leadership) and divided the sum

by the total number of possible ties in a team (Carson et al., 2007; Mathieu et al., 2015).

Measured in this way, shared leadership ranges between .20 (all responses of “1”) to 1.00 (all

responses of “5”), with higher values indicating higher levels of shared leadership. Before

aggregating the leadership ratings across raters, we calculated the interrater agreement (i.e.,

intraclass correlation, ICC) for each measurement point (Bliese, 2000). For shared leadership

measured at Time 1, ICC(1) was 0.56 and ICC(2) was 0.87, for Time 2 0.52 and 0.85

respectively, for Time 3 ICC(1) was .52 and ICC(2) was .84, and for Time 4 ICC(1) was 0.54

and ICC(2) was 0.86.

Team size. Team size reflected the number of participants assigned to a team.

Social support. Social support was assessed at Time 2. Individual team members

indicated their agreement on a 4-item scale (1= “completely disagree, 5 = “completely

agree”) with three items borrowed from Carson and colleagues (2007, e.g., “The members of

my team talk enthusiastically about our team’s progress”) and one item added to the scale to

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


21

round out the construct coverage (“Members of my team know they can count on each

other”). Cronbach alpha was .87. Median rwg was estimated at .83, ICC(1) was of .11 and

ICC(2) was estimated at .39. These were viewed as acceptable levels of agreement to justify

aggregating team member scores. We did not anticipate large ICC(2) values because the

ICC(2) is a function of the number of team member scores and the ICC(1) value. Low ICC(2)

values suggest that it may be difficult to uncover emergent relationships using group means

(Bliese, 2000). Nevertheless, such circumstances should not preclude aggregation if it is

warranted by theory and substantiated by reasonably high rwg scores (Chen & Bliese, 2002;

Gelfand, Leslie, Keller, & de Dreu, 2012; Liao, Toya, Lepak, & Hong (2009). Therefore, we

proceeded with aggregating individual scores to the team level.

Team performance. Final team performance was calculated as the average of the

points teams received on their presentations and on their written reports. Team presentations

were evaluated by the instructor on an 8-point scale with points assigned for: introduction and

relevance (2 points maximum); content of the presentation (4 point maximum); and quality of

the presentation (2 points). Both the instructor and a teaching assistant (TA) independently

graded the written reports on an 8-point scale. The score for the written report was the

average of the instructor and the TA-assigned scores.

Analyses

To test our hypotheses, we utilized random coefficient growth modeling (RCM)

following the model estimation procedure of Bliese and Ployhart (2002), as also used by

Chen, Ployhart, Thomas, Anderson, and Bliese (2011) and Lorinkova, Pearsall and Sims

(2013). To test Hypotheses 2 and 4 we introduced the moderators to the specified equation.

When testing Hypothesis 1b we followed the procedure described by Chen and colleagues

(2011) -- namely, using slopes as predictors of the final outcome. In particular, based on the

findings in Hypothesis 1a, we treated the temporal change as a slope calculated across the

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


22

multiple measurement times. Then, we generated Bayesian slope estimates for each team

utilizing the random coefficient growth modeling procedure under the “nlme” package of R

3.6.1 and regressed the final team performance scores on the slope estimates in a separate

regression.

Before testing our hypotheses, we performed a multi-level confirmatory factor

analysis to establish the discriminant validity of the constructs assessed at the same time. In

particular, we included social support and shared leadership measured at T2, with social

support and shared leadership allowed to vary at Level 1 and team size introduced as an

observed variable at Level 2. Results revealed that the two-factor model fitted the data well

(χ2(4) = 4.03, p > .10; CFI = .98; TLI = .99; RMSEA = .02; SRMR = .02) and that participants

distinguished between the measured constructs. Therefore, we proceeded with hypotheses

testing.

Results

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of the variables used in Study 1.

--------------------------------
Insert Table 1 about here
--------------------------------

When testing Hypothesis 1, we started by estimating a model which included the

linear trend of time with random intercepts and fixed slopes (i.e., we regressed the respective

shared leadership scores on time, coded as 0, 1, 2, 3 for the four time periods respectively). In

this model the linear term was not significant (b = -.01, p > .1). At the next step of the model

building, we fitted a model with random intercepts and random slopes, and the result revealed

that the model with the random slopes was superior (L-ratio = 26.26, p < .01). In what

follows, we present the results from testing models with random intercepts and slopes. When

the quadratic term for time was introduced (Table 2, Model 2), it was negative and significant

(b2 =

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


23

-0.02, p < .05), while the linear term was positive (b = .05, p = .09). This provided initial

support for Hypothesis 1a. In addition, a log-likelihood comparison between the model with

the linear term only and the model containing both the linear and the quadratic term, revealed

a significant L-ratio (L = 8.05, p < .05). Figure 2 illustrates the plot for the pattern of change

of shared leadership. As seen from Figure 2, shared leadership changes over time following

an inverted U-shaped pattern, with shared leadership increasing initially and then decreasing

after a threshold (inflection) point. Visually, the inflection point appeared to be around the

mid-point of the team’s timeline. Using the formula for estimating the inflection point (where

B1 is the coefficient for the linear term of time, and B2 is the coefficient for the quadratic

term)

XInflection = -B1/ 2B2

(1) we estimated that the inflection occurred at 1.36, which corresponds to week 10.88 of the

semester, or week 6.09 of the 11-week long course. Thus, this finding supports Hypothesis

1a.

----------------------------------------------------------
Insert Table 2, Table 3, and Figure 2 about here
-----------------------------------------------------------

When testing Hypothesis 1b, we followed the procedure advocated by Chen and

colleagues (2011). Using ordinary least squares regression (OLS), we regressed final team

performance on the changes in the independent variable (represented by within-group slopes).

In addition to controlling for the average value of the independent variable, as a robustness

check (in separate models) we also controlled for the initial value of the independent variable

as well as for the value of the independent variable at Time 4. In all estimated models (see

Table 3), the quadratic change in shared leadership positively and significantly predicted final

team performance, supporting Hypothesis 1b.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


24

Results from testing the effects of the moderators (Hypotheses 2 and 3) are presented

in Table 2. When testing the moderating hypotheses, we used RCM and the recommendations

of Le et al., (2011) for estimating moderated curvilinear relationships. In particular, a

moderated polynomial model can be represented with the following equation:

Y = B0+ B1X+ B2X2+ B3Z+ B4ZX + B5ZX2 + ε, (2)

In the equation above, Y represents shared leadership, B0 is the intercept (or the value

of shared leadership at Time 1), B1 is the linear term for time, B2 is the quadratic term for

time, and Z is the respective moderator. Thus, for the pattern of change of shared leadership

to be dependent on the value of the moderator (Z), we had to find that either B4 or B5 (or

both) were statistically significant in the estimated RCM model.

At the next step of our hypotheses testing, we included both team size and social

support in the equation to assess the role of the moderators. Table 2 presents the results. We

found that team size was a significant moderator (Model 7: b5 = -.03, p < .05) and the plot of

the relationship is presented in Figure 3. A seen from Figure 3, smaller teams not only

reached a higher level of shared leadership at the inflection point, but also sustained a higher

level of shared leadership throughout their lifecycle. Thus, Hypothesis 2 was supported.

Social support was also a significant moderator (Table 2, Model 7: b4a = .31, p <.01;

b5a = -.11, p < .01), and the plot is presented in Figure 4. As seen from Figure 4, an interesting

pattern of shared leadership development emerged; teams with higher social support

experienced the hypothesized U-shaped pattern of change, reaching an inflection point at

around .74-value of shared leadership. In contrast, in teams with lower levels of social

support, shared leadership appeared to remain relatively stable (and, in comparison to teams

with higher levels of social support, relatively low) across the team’s life cycle. Additional

calculations revealed that for teams with lower levels of social support, neither the linear term

for time, nor the quadratic term were significant (b2 = -.03, p > .1, and b3 = .01, p > .10),

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


25

which suggested that in these teams shared leadership changed little over time. Therefore,

Hypothesis 3 was supported.

-------------------------------------------------
Insert Figure 3 and Figure 4 about here
-------------------------------------------------

Study 1 Discussion

In this study, we found empirical support for a curvilinear, initially positive and then

negative (approximating an inverted U-shape) pattern of change in shared leadership, with

shared leadership reaching an inflection point around the mid-point of the project team’s

allotted time. This curvilinear pattern of change was, in turn, related to team performance.

These findings contribute to the shared leadership and project team literature by highlighting

the importance of the temporal dynamics of shared leadership. In fact, Table 1 shows that the

average level of shared leadership was not significantly related to team performance. Thus,

our findings constitute a novel conceptualization: understanding this curvilinear, almost

inverted U-shape pattern of shared leadership development; and incorporating the knowledge

that this pattern of change relates positively to team performance. This helps shed light on

some of the ambiguous findings in the shared leadership literature by considering not only the

level of shared leadership (at a certain point in time) but also its pattern of change over time.

In addition, basing our choice of moderators on the intersection of the shared

leadership theory and project team literature (Carson et al., 2007; Meindl et al., 2002; Pearce

& Sims, 2000; Waseenaar & Pearce, 2012; Zhu et al., 2018), we theoretically explain and

empirically examine different patterns of shared leadership development as qualified by team

size and social support. We offer evidence that smaller teams reached higher levels of shared

leadership at the inflection point and so, as predicted, did teams with higher levels of social

support. Interestingly, teams with lower levels of social support experienced relatively low

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


26

and stable shared leadership across the team’s life cycle; we expand upon this finding in the

general discussion.

However, despite providing empirical evidence consistent with our theoretical

reasoning, Study 1 has two notable disadvantages. First, the sample size in this study is

relatively small, which can cast doubt on the reliability of our results. Second, our study

could not address the generalizability of our results to project teams in ongoing organizations.

We address these issues in our next study, utilizing a field sample of project teams.

Study 2: Methods, Results, and Discussion

Sample and Procedure

In this field study, after obtaining approval from our affiliated Institutional Review

Board (IRB #2018-1128 “Leadership in teams”), we surveyed self-managed project teams of

a mid-sized telecommunications and IT networking company in Eastern Europe.1 The

participating company offers comprehensive technology solutions to private and business

clients. It employs highly skilled engineers, who in teams of 3-6 members provide on-site

technology services to clients. Because of the nature of the business, the majority of the

employees have a high English proficiency, and 89% of the participants elected to complete

their surveys in English. The remaining 11% completed the surveys in their native language,

with surveys translated by local certified translators fluent in both languages.

Each team was responsible for a specific on-site project, with projects lasting between

3 and 17 days. Projects differed in complexity and tasks required, but they all necessitated

that team members work together and contribute to the technology solution. While working

on-site (on an assigned project) teams were responsible for the whole project from start to

finish, operating as self-managed units. Teams self-regulated all aspects of the projects,

1
The participating company operates in the territories of Greece, Bulgaria and Romania, with headquarters in
Bulgaria.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


27

including but not limited to: assessing the situation, creating a plan of action, distributing

tasks among members, executing the tasks, routine maintenance and repair of their

equipment, and delivering a final report (completion ticket) to the organization. Overall,

teams had considerable autonomy to decide how to structure their collective work efforts to

provide the best possible service to the clients.

Teams worked without a designated field supervisor, and all members had similar

qualifications. The participating organization assigned senior engineers, “coordinators,” to

some of the projects, with responsibility for 4-7 projects at a time. Coordinators worked from

headquarters, almost never going on-site. Teams, however, did have access to their

coordinator for input on technical issues or similar concerns, but the coordinators were not

involved in the team’s day-to-day on-site work. Three teams from our sample worked without

a coordinator and reported directly to upper management. According to company policy, once

a project was completed, employees were assigned to a new project, not necessarily with the

same co-workers. Thus, a newly comprised project team (operating in the field and truly self-

managed) executed each separate on-site project.

In coordination with the participating organization, we e-mailed a link to the Time 1

survey to each team member who was starting a new project at the time of data collection.

We e-mailed a second survey (Time 2) at the mid-point of the project (project lengths were

estimated by the upper management before a project commenced based on written proposals,

which included billable hours). The final survey (Time 3) was distributed on the last day of

each project. After each team submitted its completion ticket, each coordinator was asked to

complete a short survey evaluating the work of the team on this project.

Our final sample includes data from 52 project teams. The mean project length was

10.02 days (4.29 SD), and the average project team size was 4.21 members.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


28

Measures

Shared leadership. Similar to Study 1, and in order to allow for comparison between

our studies, we assessed shared leadership utilizing the same procedure and measure as in

Study 1. Team members rated each of their peers on the extent to which they relied on the

focal team member for leadership of the project team (1= “not at all”; 5 = “to a very great

extent”). Similar to Study 1, we computed the leadership density score by aggregating all of

the actual responses of the team members for each member, summing those, and then

dividing by the total possible responses (Carson et al., 2007; Chiu et al., 2016; Mathieu et al.,

2015).

Team size. The number of members assigned to an on-site project reflected team size.

Team performance. Team coordinators assessed team performance on a 10-Point

Likert-type scale. We encouraged team coordinators to communicate with the clients and get

the clients’ input before assessing team performance. Teams were rated on the following two

items: “The technical solution provided to the client on this project was …” (1 =

unacceptable; 5 = neutral, 10 = exceptional) and “The quality of the work on this project was

…“ (1 = unacceptable; 5 = neutral, 10 = exceptional). Reliability between the two items was

.94 and we averaged them to arrive at the final team performance score.

Controls. Because projects, and hence teams’ life cycles, differed in length, we used

the information obtained from the company management on project length and controlled for

it when testing our hypotheses.

Analyses and Results

Similar to Study 1, we utilized RCM under R 3.6.1 to test our hypotheses. A log-

likelihood comparison revealed that a model with random intercepts and slopes did not fit the

data significantly better than a model with random intercepts only (L-ratio = .92, p > .1).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


29

Therefore, in what follows we present results from models with random intercepts and fixed

slopes. Table 4 includes means, standard deviations, and inter-correlations for our study

variables. As seen from Table 4, project length was significantly correlated with team

performance (r = .31, p < .05). We controlled for project length in subsequent analyses.

Before testing our hypotheses, we tested for multicollinearity among shared

leadership, team size, and project length (which we used as a control variable) by calculating

the variance inflation factor (VIF). We run three separate estimates for each measure of

shared leadership (Shared leadership T1, Shared Leadership T2, and Shared Leadership T3).

The VIF ranged between 1.01 and 1.08, which is below the recommended cutoff of 5 (Cohen,

Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2002), suggesting that multicollinearity among these variables was

not likely an issue.

Table 5 presents the results for testing Hypotheses 1a and 2. As seen from Table 5,

Model 1, the linear term involving time was positive and significant (b1 = .05, p < .001).

When the quadratic term was added to the equation, it was estimated to be negative and

significant (Table 5, Model 2, b2 = -.07, p < .001), which provided initial support for our first

hypothesis. Figure 5 illustrates the plot of the relationship and as seen, consistent with our

theorizing, shared leadership initially increases and then decreases. The inflection point for

the field project self-managed teams was estimated at 1.28, which is slightly after the

estimated project mid-point (Time coded as Time 1 = 0, Time 2 = 1, and Time 3 = 2). This

finding supports Hypothesis 1a.

----------------------------------------------------------------------
Insert Table 4, Table 5, Figure 5, and Figure 6 about here
----------------------------------------------------------------------

In testing Hypothesis 1b, we followed the same procedure as in Study 1 and regressed

team performance on within-group slopes, representing the change in shared leadership. On

its own, average shared leadership was not significantly related to team performance (b = -

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


30

2.16, p >.1). However, when the change in shared leadership was added to the model, it

positively and significantly predicted team performance (b = 11.98, p < .05). In a similar

manner, estimated on its own, Time 1 shared leadership was not significantly related to team

performance (b = 3.43, p > .1), but when the quadratic change was added to the regression

equation, the coefficient for the change pattern was positive and marginally significant (b =

9.22, p = .09). Finally, the coefficient for the quadratic change, on its own, positively and

significantly (b = 10.81, p < .05) predicted team performance. We interpreted these results as

supportive of Hypothesis 1b.

Next, we tested Hypothesis 2, in which we predicted that smaller teams would reach

higher levels of shared leadership at the inflection point. As seen from Table 5, Model 3 the

interaction term involving time and team size was significant (b4 = -.06, p < .05). As seen

from Figure 6, which presents the plot of the relationships, smaller teams appear to reach

higher levels of shared leadership at the inflection point, which was estimated at .79 for

smaller teams and .72 for larger teams. Thus, Hypothesis 2 was supported.

Study 2 Discussion

Utilizing a field sample of self-managed project teams, we found support for the

predicted curvilinear pattern of change in shared leadership over time. In addition, team size

acted as a moderator, with smaller teams achieving higher levels of shared leadership.

Although our results were largely supportive of our theoretical reasoning and replicative of

the empirical findings of Study 1, the peak in shared leadership for the industry project teams

occurred slightly later than the initially expected project mid-point. We expand upon this

interesting finding in the discussion section. However, due to limitations in survey length we

were not able to further test the moderating role of team social support in Study 2.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


31

Furthermore, participants in Study 2 were well familiar with one another,2 which prevented

us from examining the role of team familiarity in this study. We address these issues in Study

3.

Study 3: Methods, Results, and Discussion

Sample and Procedure

For Study 3 we utilized a procedure similar to Study 1. Research participants were

undergraduate students organized in self-managing teams in business schools at a private

Mid-Atlantic university (1 course, 6 sections) and a private South-Central University (1

course, 2 sections). Students were assigned to teams and worked in them for 10 – 12 weeks to

complete a team project. Team projects required students to engage in a wide variety of tasks

with specific deliverables (written reports and presentations) pertaining to two courses taught

by three different instructors. All instructors were blind to the study hypotheses, and the study

was conducted with the approval of the affiliated IRB3. Teams were self-managing and had

complete discretion on how to approach and carry out their projects. In six of the

participating sections, the instructor randomly assigned students to teams. In the remaining

two sections, students were allowed to select their team members. Teams size was 3-6

members.

For our final sample, we had complete observations on 272 students in 65 teams. Data

from 53 teams were collected from a cohort of business students enrolled in a core

management class (sophomore and junior students) and students were awarded extra credit

for participation in the study. Data from the remaining 12 teams were collected from another

cohort of business students enrolled in an elective management class in a different university,

with survey completion considered part of class participation. There were no significant

2
The individual level mean for familiarity was 2.54/3 with .23 SD.
3
IRB #2018-1133 “Leadership in a Team's Life Cycle”

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


32

differences between the two cohorts of students on any variables, and the data collection

procedure and measures were identical for both cohorts. In order to increase our sample size

and hence statistical power, we combined the two samples into one dataset.

We used online data collection with three surveys administered to the students.

Survey 1 was administered approximately one week after teams were assigned, Survey 2

around the mid-point of the project length (with project length pre-determined by instructors),

and Survey 3 right after the final deliverables were submitted, but before team grades were

assigned.

Measures

Shared Leadership. We utilized the same network density procedure as in Studies 1

and 2, with participants assessing the shared leadership of their teammates with one item,

following the example of Carson and colleagues (2007), and Mathieu and colleagues (2015).

All ratings were summed and then divided by the number of total possible responses to arrive

at a final shared leadership score for the team.

Social support. To measure social support we used the three-item measure of Carson

et al. (2007). This variable was measured at Time 2 of the data collection on a 5-point Likert-

type scale (1 = “completely disagree” to 5 = “completely agree”). Before aggregating the

individual scores to the team level, we assessed whether aggregation was appropriate. The

within-team agreement statistics were estimated at .88 (rwg) and the ICC(l) of .37

demonstrated that team membership accounted for significant variance; the ICC(2) of .67

suggested reliability of the team level means. Therefore, we proceeded with aggregation to

the team level.

Team Familiarity. We collected data on students’ familiarity with one another at

Time 1 of data collection, reasoning that at this time team members’ familiarity might

influence leadership emergence (Pearce & Sims, 2000). We measured team familiarity with

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


33

one item, asking students to respond to the following question: “How familiar were you with

your team members before starting work on this project” (1 = I was not familiar with any of

my team members; 2 = I was somewhat familiar with some of my team members; 3 = I was

well familiar with my team members). We averaged team members’ responses to arrive at a

team score. However, consistent with Chan’s (1998) recommendations, we did not estimate

within-group agreement, as team familiarity represents an additive team-level construct, for

which the additive index is correctly represented by the group mean.

Team performance. Final team performance was calculated as the sum of the points

teams received on their presentations and on their written analyses. Team presentations were

evaluated on a 15-point scale with points assigned for: introduction and relevance (3 points

maximum); content of the presentation (9-point maximum); and quality of the presentation (3

points). Presentations were evaluated by all class members through an online survey

immediately after the presentation ended; each team’s final presentation score was calculated

based on the class average and the instructor score. For 53 of the teams, two independent

graders graded team written analyses (graduate-student teaching assistants) with a score

ranging between 0-5. In the cases of a higher-than-1-point discrepancy between the two

graders,4 the course instructor re-graded the written analysis and assigned a score. For the

remaining 12 teams, the instructor graded the written analysis.

Controls. We controlled for team size, because it was a significant correlate of shared

leadership in the first study. The number of students working on a team reflected team size.

Results

Table 6 presents the means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for Study

3 variables. As seen from the table, team size was significantly correlated with average

4
For 3 teams, for which there was more than a 1-point difference between the TA graders, the course instructor
regraded the assignment.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


34

shared leadership and shared leadership measured at Time 3, which confirmed our intent to

include team size in the analyses as a control variable. Similar to Study 2, we estimated the

VIF for shared leadership (measured at Time 1 (T1), Time 2 (T2), and Time 3 (T3)), social

support, and team familiarity in three separate estimates. VIF ranged between 1.01 and 1.09

when shared leadership T1, social support and team familiarity were included; between 1.11

and 1.32 with shared leadership T2 included, and between 1.12 and 1.19 with shared

leadership T3. These results are well below the recommended cut-off of 5 (Cohen et al.,

2002), and suggested multicollinearity was likely not an issue. In addition, we ran a

multilevel CFA to make sure participants distinguished between social support and shared

leadership T2 (because both were measured at T2). A two-factor model (with social support

and shared leadership allowed to vary at Level 1 and team size controlled for at Level 2)

revealed a good fit of the data: (χ2(2) = 7.43, p < .05; CFI = .97; TLI = .94; RMSEA = .075;

SRMRwithin = .04, and confirmed the discriminant validity of the constructs.

-----------------------------------------------------------
Insert Table 6, Table 7, and Figure 7 about here
------------------------------------------------------------

Hypotheses 1a, 3 and 4 were tested through RCM under R 3.6.1. We started by

estimating model fit, and we discovered that a model with random intercepts and slopes fit

the data better than a model with random intercepts only (L-ratio = 12.83, p < .01). In what

follows, we present results from models allowing both the intercepts and the slopes to vary.

Table 7 presents the results for testing Hypotheses 1a, 3 and 4. Shared leadership

change did not follow a linear trend (Model 1: b1 = .01, p > .1). When the quadratic term was

added to the equation, it was estimated to be negative and significant (Model 2: b2 = -.04, p <

.05), with a positive linear term (b1 = .10, p < .01). Figure 7 presents the plot for the pattern

of change in shared leadership, and as seen from the figure shared leadership increases

initially, only to reach a peak and then decrease later in the team development. The inflection

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


35

point was estimated at 1.12, which is consistent with the chronological mid-point of team

development (based on three observations, at Time 1, Time 2 and Time 3, coded as 0, 1, and

2 for analytical purposes), thus supporting Hypothesis 1a.5

When testing Hypothesis 1b we used the same approach as in Study 1 and Study 2. In

particular, we regressed team performance on the estimated within-group slope coefficients.

The quadratic change in shared leadership positively and significantly predicted team

performance when controlling for the value of shared leadership at T3 (b = 8.67, p < .05). In

a similar manner, controlling for the average shared leadership, the quadratic change in

shared leadership positively and significantly predicted team performance (b = 9.29, p < .05).

These results indicated support for Hypothesis 1b.

---------------------------------------------------
Insert Figure 8 and Figure 9 about here
-----------------------------------------------------

Next, we tested Hypotheses 3 and 4, estimating the role of social support and team

familiarity as moderators. Table 7 presents the results. Hypothesis 3 was supported because

the interaction term involving time and social support was significant (Model 8: b4 = .14, p <

.05) and the plot of the interaction (presented in Figure 8) reveals results consistent with our

theorizing. Additional calculations revealed that teams with higher levels of social support

reached .77 value of shared leadership at the inflection point, whereas teams with lower

levels of social support reached only .64 value of shared leadership. When testing Hypothesis

4, the interaction between time and familiarity was marginally significant (Model 8: b4a = .14,

p = .051), whereas the interaction between the quadratic term of time and familiarity reached

5
To validate Hypothesis 1 further, and to offer a more rigorous empirical test of our theorized group dynamics,
we conducted additional data collection and performed additional analyses. See the Appendix for full details. In
short, we collected data from 27 student teams, which we surveyed 5 times over the course of a condensed 5-
week long semester. Results support the theorized curvilinear pattern of shared leadership development, with
shared leadership increasing earlier in the team development and decreasing during Phase 2. We estimated that
shared leadership peaked at week 2.67 (Xinflection = 2.67), which is consistent with our theoretically outlined peak
in shared leadership.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


36

significance (b5a = -.08, p < .05). The plot, however, reveals surprising results. As seen in

Figure 9, teams in which members were relatively unfamiliar with one another experienced

almost no change in shared leadership over time, whereas in teams with higher familiarity

among members, shared leadership peaked around the mid-point at .73 value. Thus, in Study

3, we found support for Hypotheses 1 and 3, but the findings for Hypothesis 4 went in the

opposite direction. We expand upon these findings in the discussion section.

General Discussion

Based on three independent studies, we find evidence to suggest that shared

leadership changes over time in a curvilinear fashion with the pattern approximating an

inverted U-shape. In particular, over the course of a project team’s life cycle, shared

leadership increased during the early phase of team development (Phase 1), peaked around

the mid-point, and gradually decreased in the later phase (Phase 2). In turn, this pattern of

change related positively to team performance. The results also showed that team size, social

support, and team members’ familiarity, all of which were theoretically based on the shared

leadership and project team literature, acted as boundary conditions to influence the pattern of

shared leadership development. In particular, as we expected, smaller teams and teams

enjoying higher levels of social support reached higher levels of shared leadership across

their development. Somewhat surprisingly, we found that the curvilinear pattern of shared

leadership development was more pronounced in teams with higher familiarity, rather than in

teams with lower familiarity.

Theoretical Implications

Departing from previous research that has predominantly examined shared leadership

in a cross-sectional, static manner (e.g., Boies et al., 2010; Carson et al., 2007; Erkutlu, 2012;

Pearce et al., 2004), we have focused on the longitudinal development of shared leadership in

project teams. While theoretically positioning and empirically validating the pattern of shared

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


37

leadership development over time and offering insights into boundary conditions that

influence this pattern of development, we make several major contributions to relevant

literature.

First, in a departure from the traditional PEM (Chang et al., 2003; Gersick, 1988;

1989), which focuses predominantly on task execution and overlooks shared leadership

development, we describe how, in response to members’ changing needs and priorities,

shared leadership increases during Phase 1, peaks around the mid-point, and then diminishes

during Phase 2 of a project team’s life cycle. By conceptualizing this non-uniform,

curvilinear pattern of shared leadership development in project teams over time, we address

the question of how shared leadership develops and when project teams need it most. In this

way, we answer researchers’ calls for a better theoretical understanding of the role of time

(Nicolaides et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2014), and we offer an alternative explanation for

longstanding empirical confusion surrounding the role of time in shared leadership

development.

Second, our results demonstrate that teams which follow the curvilinear, inverted U-

shape shared leadership development enjoy higher levels of performance. This finding is in

line with our theorizing, extending shared leadership theory (Pearce & Sims, 2000; Pearce &

Conger, 2003), and it draws on arguments from the non-sequential team development

framework (e.g., Chang et al., 2003; Gersick, 1988, 1989) as applicable to self-managed

project teams. We argue that project teams need to develop shared leadership during the

earlier phase of team development to allow shared leadership to peak around the mid-point

(when the team needs it the most). It can then diminish during the later phase, when project

completion strategies and direction are already set. Our conceptualization of this

approximately inverted U-shape pattern of change in shared leadership also allows us to offer

a potential explanation for why some researchers (e.g., Boies et al., 2010; Hmieleski et al.,

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


38

2012; Serban & Roberts, 2016) might not have linked shared leadership to team performance.

In particular, we alert scholars to the value of paying attention not only to the level but also to

the changes in shared leadership over time, because team members’ priorities and needs

change during the course of a team’s life cycle.

Third, we identify critical boundary conditions, which help explain why some teams

experience different patterns of shared leadership development. In this way, we overcome the

limitation of assuming that all teams follow a similar pattern of shared leadership

development (e.g., Drescher et al, 2014). We also answer researchers’ calls to identify

boundary conditions that “remain to be clarified as research goes forward” (Wang et al.,

2014, p. 182). Past work has tended to treat shared leadership as similar to its counterpart –

vertical leadership – and examine it as a static input, remaining unchanged over the course of

the team’s life cycle (e.g., Chiu et al., 2016; Erkutlu, 2012), with its effects potentially

mediated through team processes (e.g., Hoch & Dulebohn, 2013). This treatment, however, is

inconsistent with shared leadership theory – which denotes a dynamic, interpersonal process

– and leaves open the question of whether and why some teams may experience different

patterns of shared leadership development. Accordingly, by grounding our inquiry in shared

leadership theory (Wassenaar & Pearce, 2012; see also Pearce & Sims, 2000, and Zhu et al.,

2018) and project team literature (e.g., Akgün et al., 2007; Huckman & Staats, 2011;

Huckman et al., 2009), we identified team size, team social support, and team members’

familiarity as important moderators of the configuration of shared leadership development in

project teams. In this way, our study advances a theoretical understanding of team-level

factors that serve as boundary conditions for shared leadership development in project teams.

For example, our finding that smaller teams reach higher levels of shared leadership during

the mid-point offers some theoretical and empirical insight into the existing dilemma

surrounding the role of team size for shared leadership development (Nicolaides et al., 2014).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


39

In a similar manner, although our findings when social support was lower were slightly

different across Study 1 and Study 3, we found evidence that teams with higher levels of

social support consistently reached higher levels of shared leadership around the critical

midpoint. Thus, our work extends the applications of the more general team literature (e.g.,

LePine, Piccolo, Jackson, Mathieu, & Saul, 2008) to shared leadership development by

highlighting the importance of social support for project teams. Somewhat surprisingly, we

found that teams in which members were less familiar with one another experienced almost

no change in shared leadership over the course of the project team’s life cycle. By contrast, in

teams with higher familiarity, the U-shape pattern of change was well evident. A potential

explanation for this finding may lie in what Richard Hackman (2002) calls “newness as a

liability”, arguing that familiarity in teams is helpful for reaching optimal interpersonal

structures and, ultimately, performance.

Managerial Implications

This study demonstrates the role of shared leadership for project teams, and offers a

number of organizational implications. First, it is important for practicing managers and

organizational leaders to understand that shared leadership could be an important predictor of

project teams’ performance. With that in mind, managers should encourage its development

in self-managed teams. However, our findings caution that shared leadership appears to be

most important around the mid-point of the team’s life cycle (which, in practitioners’ terms,

might be approximately the halfway point toward specific deadlines that project teams have

to meet), and that an increase in shared leadership earlier in a team’s development facilitates

reaching the needed high level at the mid-point. Equipped with this knowledge, we advise

organizational leaders to monitor team progression through a project, and encourage team

members to build shared leadership during the early phases of team development so that team

members are able to exert necessary and significant shared leadership influence around the

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


40

mid-point of the team’s time-line. Past this mid-point, our study advises, team members

should focus on task execution and refrain from over-exerting leadership influence.

In addition to explaining better the role of time in shared leadership development, our

study also offers insights to organizational leaders about team characteristics that can help

autonomous project teams achieve the predicted curvilinear pattern of shared leadership

development -- with mid-point inflection – and, in turn, positively influence project

execution. In particular, our study warns managers about costs associated with increasing the

number of members on a team; larger teams tend to reach lower levels of shared leadership at

the mid-point of the team’s allotted time, when shared leadership is most beneficial to the

team. A practical solution here might be to facilitate and encourage team members to engage

in shared leadership influences during Phase 1 to effectively handle the transitional mid-

point. Alternatively, one could split a larger team into smaller groups able to target different

aspects of a project. In a similar manner, our findings illuminate the importance of team

members’ familiarity and social support. For example, based on our findings, we advise

organizations and practicing managers to avoid (if possible) assigning completely unfamiliar

team members to a new project, because unfamiliarity appears to be a liability that hinders

shared leadership development. Social support as a boundary condition facilitated shared

leadership development and we advise organizational leaders to promote a climate in which

team members encourage and care for one another.

Limitations and Directions for Future Research

This study has a number of limitations, which should be noted. First, despite its three-

study, different-settings approach, the scope of our work is limited to self-managed project

teams. Thus, the generalizability of our results to different types of teams remains to be

studied. For example, long-term production teams that have a relatively stable membership

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


41

and rely on complex equipment (Sundstrom, 1999) may reach a certain level of shared

leadership and maintain it for as long as the team operates.

Second, although we conceptualize shared leadership as evolving over multiple

phases of team development, we are not able to definitively identify the beginning or end of

each phase of team development. As noted by Kozlowski and colleagues, teams develop

through a “continuous series of phases, with partial overlap at transitions” (1999, p. 248). Our

empirical findings, which identify the peak of shared leadership around the mid-point of the

teams’ allotted time, extend the theoretical application of the episodic team development

perspective (Chang et al., 2003; Gersick, 1988, 1989; Marks et al., 2001), highlighting the

importance of the transitional mid-point. Interestingly for our industry project teams, the peak

in shared leadership appeared slightly after the chronological team project mid-point. We

interpreted this to mean that for these project teams the early phase of team development

(during which team members assess the situation, exchange ideas about approaches, and start

to distribute tasks) likely takes longer than for student teams (faced with more predictable

tasks). Therefore, future research could examine the changes in shared leadership over

different types of projects or series of tasks, for which the duration of each developmental

phase might be duly coded.

In addition, although our choice of moderators was grounded in the shared leadership

and project team literature, this work did not capture all potential factors that could exert

moderating influences on shared leadership development. For example, shared leadership

theory consistently advises that task interdependence may influence the role of shared

leadership in teams, hence team needs--and as a result members’ engagement in shared

leadership--may be influenced by task interdependence. Research suggests that when tasks

require intense collaboration, team members are more likely to accept role differentiation

(Stogdill, 1959), which, in shared leadership parlance, implies that members would be more

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


42

likely to grant leadership roles. Thus under higher task interdependence we might expect to

find higher levels of shared leadership. Our field study sample, which surveyed teams with

relatively high levels of task interdependence, ameliorates this issue to some extent; however,

future research could bring more contextual clarity to the issue of shared leadership

development over time.

Moreover, a future and specific examination of the nature of shared leadership at

different time points may help bring clarity to specific types of leadership influences that

might be relevant. In this study, we focused on the overall construct of shared leadership in

order to avoid conflation with, and limiting leadership to, specific functions in which team

members engage when influencing their teams. However, traditional leadership research has

long identified task- and relationship-oriented aspects of leadership (DeRue, Nahrgang,

Wellman, & Humphrey, 2011). Although shared leadership research has traditionally not

differentiated between these two aspects of leadership (Carson et al., 2007, Chiu et al., 2016;

Drescher et al., 2014), they might meet different team needs as the team progresses through

its life cycle. Clarifying these potentially different aspects of shared leadership and their role

over time might bring clarity to the questions of what exactly is shared (Barnett &

Weidenfeller, 2016).

Finally, there may also be some ambiguity as to what mediating mechanisms help

translate the effect of shared leadership to team performance. Although our results are

consistent with Carson and colleagues (2007) and Chiu and colleagues (2016), who link

shared leadership directly to team outcomes, emerging research in the area (e.g., Drescher et

al., 2014) underscores the need to clarify the role of team processes and states, viewing them

as “translators” of the effects of shared leadership development on final outcomes. Therefore,

future research should examine the interplay and mutual influence dynamics of shared

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


43

leadership, as well as other team processes and emergent states. We encourage researchers

interested in these issues to use our work as a starting point for this important conversation.

References

Aime, F., Humphrey, S., DeRue, D. S., & Paul, J. (2014). The riddle of heterarchy:
transitions in cross-functional teams. Academy of Management Journal, 57, 327-352.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.0756

Akgün, A. E., Keskin, H., Byrne, J., & Imamoglu, S. Z. (2007). Antecedents and
consequences
of team potency in software development projects. Information & Management, 44,
646-656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.im.2007.08.001

Albanese, R., & Van Fleet, D. (1985). Rational behavior in groups: The free-riding
tendency. Academy of Management Review, 10(2), 244–255.

Alnuaimi, O. A., Robert, L. P., & Maruping, L. M. (2010). Team size, dispersion, and social
Loafing in technology-supported teams: a perspective on the theory of moral
disengagement. Journal of Management Information Systems, 27, 203–230. doi:
10.2753/MIS0742-1222270109

Barnett, R. C., & Weidenfeller, N. K. (2016). Shared leadership and team performance.
Advances in Developing Human Resources, 18, 334-351. doi:
10.1177/1523422316645885

Beckman, C. M. (2006). The influence of founding team company affiliations on firm


behavior.
Academy of Management Journal, 49, 741–758.

Bell, S. T. (2007). Deep-level composition variables as predictors of team performance: a


meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 595-615.
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.595

Bergman, S. M., Small, E. E., Bergman, J. Z., & Bowling, J. J. (2014). Leadership emergence
and group development: a longitudinal examination of project teams. Journal of
Organizational Psychology, 14, 111-126.

Bliese PD. (2000). Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: Implications


for
data aggregation and analysis. In Klein KJ, Kozlowski SWJ (Eds.), Multilevel theory,
research, and methods in organizations: Foundations, extensions, and new direction
(pp. 49–381). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Bliese, P. D., & Ployhart, R. E. (2002). Growth modeling using random coefficient models:
Model building, testing, and illustrations. Organizational Research Methods, 5, 362–
387.
doi:10.1177/109442802237116

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


44

Bligh, M. C., Pearce, C. L., & Kohles, J. C. (2006). The importance of self-and shared
leadership
in team based knowledge work: A meso-level model of leadership dynamics. Journal
of
Managerial Psychology, 21, 296–318.

Boies, K., Lvina, E., & Martens, M. L. (2010). Shared leadership and team performance in a
business strategy simulation. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 9, 195-202.
doi: 10.1027/1866-5888/a000021

Carmeli, A. (2007). Social capital, psychological safety and learning behaviours from failure
in
Organizations. Long Range Planning, 40, 30-44. doi: 10.1016/j.lrp.2006.12.002
Carson, J. B., Tesluk, P. E., & Marrone, J. A. (2007). Shared leadership in teams: An
investigation of antecedent conditions and performance. Academy of Management
Journal, 50, 1217–1234. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/20159921

Carter, D. R., DeChurch, L. A., Braun, M. T., & Contractor, N. S. (2015). Social network
approaches to leadership: An integrative conceptual review. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 100(3), 597-622. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038922

Chang, A., Bordia, P., & Duck, J. (2003). Punctuated equilibrium and linear progression:
toward
a new understanding of group development. Academy of Management Journal, 46,
106-117. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/30040680

Chen, G., & Bliese, P. D. (2002). The role of different of levels of leadership in predicting
self
and collective efficacy: Evidence for discontinuity. Journal of Applied Psychology,
87(3), 549–556. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.549

Chen, G., Ployhart, R. E., Thomas, H. C., Anderson, N., & Bliese, P. D. (2011). The power of
momentum: A new model of dynamic relationships between job satisfaction change
and
turnover intentions. Academy of Management Journal, 54, 159 –181.
doi:10.5465/AMJ.2011.59215089

Chiaburu, D. S., & Harrison, D. A. (2008). Do peers make the place? Conceptual synthesis
and
meta-analysis of coworker effects on perceptions, attitudes, OCBs, and performance.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1082-1103. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.5.1082.

Chiu, C., Owens, B. P., & Tesluk, P. E. (2016). Initiating and utilizing shared leadership in
teams: the role of leader humility, team proactive personality, and team performance
capability. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 1705-1720. doi:10.1037/apl0000159

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2002). Applied multiple
regression/correlation

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


45

analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates Inc.

Contractor, N. S., DeChurch, L. A., Carson, J. B., Carter, D. R., & Keegan, B. (2012). The
topology of collective leadership. Leadership Quarterly, 23, 994–1011.
doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.10.010

Cox, J. F., Pearce, C. L., & Perry, M. L. (2003). Toward a model of shared leadership and
distributed influence in the innovation process: How shared leadership can enhance
new product development team dynamics and effectiveness. In C. L. Pearce, & J. A.
Conger (Eds.), Shared leadership: Reframing the hows and whys of leadership (pp.
48–76). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing.

DeMarco, T & Listen, T. (1987). Peopleware: Productive Projects and Teams. Prentice-
Hall:
Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

DeRue, D. S., & Ashford, S. J. (2010). Who will lead and who will follow? A social process
of
leadership identity construction in organizations. Academy of Management Review,
35,
627-647. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2010.53503267

DeRue, D. S., Nahrgang, J. D., Wellman, N., & Humphrey, S. E. (2011). Trait and behavioral
theories of leadership: an integration and meta-analytic test of their relative validity.
Personnel Psychology, 64, 7-52.

D’Innocenzo, L., Mathieu, J. E., & Kukenberger, M. R. (2016). A meta-Analysis of different


forms of shared leadership–team performance relations. Journal of Management, 42,
1964-1991. doi: 10.1177/0149206314525205

Drescher, G., & Garbers, Y. (2016). Shared leadership and commonality: A policy-capturing
study. The Leadership Quarterly, 27(2), 200-217. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2016.02.002

Drescher, M. A., Korsgaard, A. M., Welpe, I. M. , Picot, A. & Wigand, R. T. (2014). The
dynamics of shared leadership: Building trust and enhancing performance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 99, 771-783. doi: 10.1037/a0036474

Ensley, M.D., Hmieleski, K. M., & Pearce, C. L. (2006). The importance of vertical and
shared leadership within new venture top management teams: Implications for the
performance of startups. Leadership Quarterly, 17(3), 217–231.

Erkutlu, H. (2012). The impact of organizational culture on the relationship between shared
leadership and team proactivity. Team Performance Management, 18, 102–119.
doi:10.1108/13527591211207734

Espinosa, J.A., Slaughter, S.A., Kraut, R.E., Herbsleb, J.D. (2007). Familiarity, complexity,
and team performance in geographically distributed software development.
Organization Science, 18, 613–630.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


46

Farh, J-L., Lee, C., & Farh, C. I. C. (2010). Task conflict and team creativity: a question of
how much and when. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 1173-1180. doi:
10.1037/a0020015

Ford, C., & Sullivan, D. M. (2004). A time for everything: how the timing of novel
contributions influences project team outcomes. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
25, 279-292.

Gelfand M. J., Leslie, L. M., Keller, & de Dreu C. (2012). Conflict cultures in organizations:
How leaders shape conflict cultures and their organizational-level consequences.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 1131–1147. doi: 10.1037/a0029993

Gerpott, F. H., Lehmann-Willenbrock, N., Voelpel, S. C., & van Vugt, M. (2019). It’s not
just
what is said, but when it’s said: A temporal account of verbal behaviors and emergent
leadership in self-managed teams. Academy of Management Journal, 62, 717-738.
doi:
10.5465/amj.2017.0149

Gersick, C. J. G. (1988). Time and transitions in work teams: toward a new model of group
development. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 9–41. doi: 10.5465/256496

Gersick, C. J. G. (1989). Marking time: predictable transitions in task groups. Academy of


Management Journal, 32(2), 274–309.

Goodman, P. S., & Leyden, D. P. 1991. Familiarity and group productivity. Journal of
Applied
Psychology, 76, 578–586.

Gruenfeld, D.H., Mannix, E.A., Williams, K.Y., Neale, M.A. (1996). Group composition and
decision making: how member familiarity and information distribution affect process
and
performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, 1–15

Gully, S. M. (2000). Work team research: Recent findings and future trends. In M. Beyerlein
(Ed.), Work teams: Past, present, and future, 25-44. The Netherlands: Kluwer
Academic Publishers.

Guzzo, R. A., & Dickson, M. W. (1996). Teams in organizations: Research on performance


and
effectiveness. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 307–338.

Hackman, J. R. (2002). Leading teams: Setting the stage for great performances. Boston,
MA:
Harvard Business School Press.

Hackman, J. R. (1987). The design of work teams. In J. Lorsch (Ed.), Handbook of


Organizational Behavior (pp. 315-342). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Hambrick, D. C., & D'Aveni, R. A. (1992). Top team deterioration as part of the downward

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


47

spiral of large corporate bankruptcies. Management Science, 38, 1445-1466.

Harrison, D. A., Mohammed, S., McGrath, J. E., Florey, A. T., & Vanderstoep, S. W. (2003).
Time matters in team performance: Effects of member familiarity, entrainment, and
task
discontinuity on speed and quality. Personnel Psychology, 56, 633 - 669. DOI:
10.1111/j.1744-6570.2003.tb00753.x.

Hill, G. W. (1982). Group versus individual performance: Are n + 1 heads better than one?
Psychological Bulletin, 91, 517-539.

Hinds, P., Carley, K., Krackhardt, D., & Wholey, D. (2000). Choosing work group members:
balancing similarity, competence, and familiarity. Organizational Behavior and
Human
Decision Processes. 81, 226-251. 10.1006/obhd.1999.2875.

Hmieleski, K. M., Cole, M. S., & Baron, R. A. (2012). Shared authentic leadership and new
venture performance. Journal of Management, 38(5), 1476–1499.

Hoch, J. (2013). Shared leadership and innovation: the role of vertical leadership and
employee
integrity. Journal of Business & Psychology. 28(2), 159-174. doi:10.1007/s10869-
012-9273-6

Hoch, J. E., & Dulebohn, J. H. (2013). Shared leadership in enterprise resource planning and
human resource management systems implementation. Human Resource Management
Review, 23, 114–125. DOI: 10.1016/j.hrmr.2012.06.007

Hoegl, M. (2005). Smaller teams – Better teamwork: How to keep project teams small.
Business
Horizons, 48, 209–214.

Hollenbeck, J. R., DeRue, D. S., & Guzzo, R. (2004). Bridging the gap between I/O research
and HR practice: Improving team composition, team training, and team task design.
Human Resource Management, 43, 353–366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hrm.20029

Houghton, J. D., Neck, C. P., & Manz, C. C. (2003). Self-leadership and superleadership: The
heart and art of creating shared leadership in teams. In C. L. Pearce & J. A. Conger
(Eds.), Shared leadership: Reframing the hows and whys of leadership (pp. 123-140).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Houghton, J. D., Pearce, C., Manz, C. C., Courtright, S. H., & Stewart, G. (2015). Sharing is
caring: Toward a model of proactive caring through shared leadership. Human
Resource
Management Review, 25, 313-327. 10.1016/j.hrmr.2014.12.001.

Huckman, R. S., & Staats, B. R. (2011). Fluid tasks and fluid teams: the impact of diversity
in
experience and team familiarity on team performance. Manufacturing & Service
Operations Management, 13, 310-328. https://doi.org/10.1287/msom.1100.0321

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


48

Huckman, R. S., Staats, B. R., & Upton, D. M. (2009). Team familiarity, role experience, and
performance: evidence from Indian software services. Management Science, 55, 85-
100.

Ilgen, D. R., Hollenbeck, J. R., Johnson, M., & Jundt, D. (2005). Teams in organizations:
From
input-process-output models to IMOI models. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 517–
543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070250

Jackson, S. E. (1992). Consequences of group composition for the interpersonal dynamics of


strategic issue processing. In P. Shrivastava, A. Huff, & J. Dutton (Eds.), Advances in
strategic management, vol. 8, 345-382. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Jehn, K. A., & Shah, P. P. (1997). Interpersonal relationships and task performance: An
examination of mediating processes in friendship and acquaintance groups. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 775-790.

Karau, S. J., & Williams, K.D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical
integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 681–706.

Katz, R. (1982). The effects of group longevity on project communication and performance.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 27, 81–104

Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. (1978). The social psychology of organizations (2nd ed.). New York,
NY: Wiley.

Kidwell, R. E., & Bennett, N. (1993). Employee propensity to withhold effort: A conceptual
model to intersect three avenues of research. Academy of Management Review, 18,
429–
456. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/258904

Kim, P. H. (1997). When what you know can hurt you: A study of experiential effects on
group
discussion and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process,
69, 165-177.

Klug, M., & Bagrow, J. P. (2016). Understanding the group dynamics and success of teams.
Royal Society Open Science, 3, 1-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160007

Knight, A. P. (2012). Mood at the midpoint: Affect and change in exploratory search over
time
in teams that face a deadline. Organization Science, 26, 99-118.
doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2013.0866

Kor, Y.Y. (2003). Experience-based top management team competence and sustained
growth.
Organization Science, 14, 707–719.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Bell, B. S. (2003). Work groups and teams in organizations. In W. C.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


49

Borman, D. R. Ilgen & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Industrial and


organizational psychology, 12, 333–375. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
doi:10.1002/0471264385.wei1214

Kozlowski, S. W. J., Gully, S. M., Nason, E. R. & Smith, E. M. (1999). Developing adaptive
teams : A theory of compilation and performance across levels and time. In D. R.
Ilgen &
E. D. Pulakos (Eds.), The Changing Nature of Performance: Implications for Staffing,
Personnel Actions, & Development, 240-292. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Langfred, C. W. (2004). Too much of a good thing? Negative effects of high trust and
individual
autonomy in self-managed teams. Academy of Management Journal, 47, 385-399.

Le, H., Oh, I.-S., Robbins, S. B., Ilies, R., Holland, E., & Westrick, P. (2011). Too much of a
good thing: Curvilinear relationships between personality traits and job performance.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 113-133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021016

LePine, J., Piccolo, R. F., Jackson, C. L., Mathieu, J. E., & Saul, J. R. (2008). A meta-
analysis of
teamwork processes: Tests of a multidimensional model and relationships with team
effectiveness criteria. Personnel Psychology, 61(2), 273-307.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2008.00114.x

Liao H., Toya, K., Lepak, D. P., & Hong, Y. (2009). Do they see eye to eye? Management
and employee perspectives of high-performance work systems and influence
processes on service quality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 371–391. doi:
10.1037/a0013504.

Lorinkova, N. M., Pearsall, M. J., & Sims, H. P. (2013). Examining the differential
longitudinal
performance of directive versus empowering leadership in teams. Academy of
Management Journal, 56, 573-596. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.0132

Marks, M. A., Mathieu, J. E., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2001). A temporally based framework and
taxonomy of team processes. Academy of Management Review, 26, 356–376.
doi:10.5465/amr.2001.4845785

Mathieu, J. E., Kukenberger, M. R., D’Innocenzo, L., & Reilly, G. (2015). Modeling
reciprocal
team cohesion-performance relationships, as impacted by shared leadership and
members’ competence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 713–734.
doi: 10.1037/a0038898

Mathieu, J. E., Tannenbaum, S. I., Donsbach, J. S., & Alliger, G. A. (2014). A review and
integration of team composition models: Moving toward a dynamic and temporal
framework. Journal of Management, 40, 130 –160. doi: 10.1177/0149206313503014

May, D. R., Gilson, R.L., & Harter, L. M. (2004). The psychological conditions of

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


50

meaningfulness, safety and availability and the engagement of the human spirit at
work
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 77, 11-37.

Mayo, M., Meindl, J. R., & Pastor, J. (2003). Shared leadership in work teams. In C. L.
Pearce
& J. A. Conger (Eds.), Shared leadership: Reframing the hows and whys of
leadership
(pp. 193-214). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Mehta, N., & Bharadwaj, A. (2015) Knowledge integration in software teams: the role of
sentry
and guard processes. Journal of Management Information Systems, 32, 82-115. DOI:
10.1080/07421222.2015.1029381

McIntyre, H. H., & Foti, R. J. (2013). The impact of shared leadership on teamwork mental
models and performance in self-directed teams. Group Processes & Intergroup
Relations,
16, 46-57. doi/pdf/10.1177/1368430211422923

Moreland, R. L., & Levine, J. M. (1992). The composition of small groups. In E. Lawler, B.
Markovsky, C. Ridgeway, & H. Walker (Eds.), Advances in group processes, 9, 237-
280. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Mueller, J. S. (2012) Why individuals in larger teams perform worse. Organizational


Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 117, 111-124.

Nicolaides, V. C., LaPort, K. A., Chen, T. R., Tomassetti, A. J., Weis, E. J., Zaccaro, S. J., &
Cortina, J. M. (2014). The shared leadership of teams: A meta-analysis of proximal,
distal, and moderating relationships. Leadership Quarterly, 25, 923-942.

Oh, S.-H. (2012). Leadership emergence in autonomous work teams: Who is more willing to
lead? Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, 40, 1451–1464.

Pearce, C. L., & Conger, J. A. (2003). Shared leadership: Reframing the hows and whys of
leadership. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Pearce, C. L., & Sims, H. P. Jr. (2000). Shared leadership: toward a multi-level theory of
Leadership. In Beyerlein, M. M., Johnson, D.A. & Beyerlein, S.T. (Eds), Advances in
Interdisciplinary Studies of Work Teams, 115-139. Greenwich: JAI Press.

Pearce, C. L., & Sims, H. P. (2002). The relative influence of vertical vs. shared leadership
on the longitudinal effectiveness of change management teams. Group Dynamics:
Theory,
Research, and Practice, 6(2), 172–197.

Pearce, C. L., Yoo, Y., & Alavi, M. (2004). Leadership, social work, and virtual teams: The
relative influence of vertical vs. shared leadership in the nonprofit section. In R.
Reggio

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


51

& S. Smith Orr (Eds.), Improving leadership in nonprofit organizations. San


Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.

Roussin, C. J. (2008). Increasing trust, psychological safety and team performance through
dyadic leadership discovery. Small Group Research, 39, 224-248.
doi: 10.1177/1046496408315988

Seers, A., Keller, T., & Wilkerson, J. M. (2003). Can team members share leadership:
Foundations in research and theory. In C. L. Pearce, & J. A. Conger (Eds.), Shared
leadership: Reframing the hows and whys of leadership (pp. 77–102). Thousand
Oaks,
CA: Sage.

Serban, A., & Roberts, A. J. B. (2016). Exploring antecedents and outcomes of shared
leadership
in a creative context: A mixed-methods approach. Leadership Quarterly, 27, 181–
199.

Sivasubramaniam, N., Murry, W. D., Avolio, B. J., & Jung, D. I. (2002). A longitudinal
model of
the effects of team leadership and group potency on group performance. Group &
Organization Management, 27, 66–96. doi: 10.1177/1059601102027001005

Society for Human Resource Management (SHRM, 2018). Developing and sustaining high-
performance work teams. Online publication, accessed at:
https://www.shrm.org/resourcesandtools/tools-and-
samples/toolkits/pages/developingandsustaininghigh-performanceworkteams.aspx

Stewart, G. L. (2006). A meta-analytic review of relationships between team design features


and
team performance. Journal of Management, 32, 29 –54.
doi/10.1177/0149206305277792

Stogdill, R. M. (1959). Individual behavior and group achievement: A theory; the


experimental
evidence. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Sundstrom, E. (1999). The challenges of supporting work team effectiveness. In E.


Sundstrom,
& Associates (Eds.), Supporting work team effectiveness: Best management practices
for
fostering high performance (pp. 3-23). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Wang, D., Waldman, D. A., & Zhang, Z. (2014). A meta-analysis of shared leadership and
team
Effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 181-198. doi: 10.1037/a0034531

Warner, R. M., Kenny, D. A., & Stoto, M. (1979). A new round robin analysis of variance for

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


52

social interaction data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10), 1742-
1757.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1742

Wassenaar, C. L., & Pearce, C. L. (2012). The Nature of Shared Leadership. In J. Antonakis
&
D. Day (Eds.) The Nature of Leadership. pp. 363-389. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice
Hall

Wheelan, S. A. (2009). Group size, group development, and group productivity. Small Group
Research, 40, 247-262. doi: 10.1177/1046496408328703

Yukl, G. A. (1989). Leadership in organizations. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Zenger, T. R., & Lawrence, B.S. (1989). Organizational demography: the differential effects
of
age and tenure distributions on technical communication. Academy of Management
Journal, 32, 353–376.

Zhu, J., Liao, Z., Yam, K. C., & Johnson, R. (2018). Shared leadership: A state‐ of‐ the‐ art
review
and future research agenda. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 39, 834–852.

Figure Legends

Figure 1. Proposed Theoretical Model

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


53

Figure 2. Shared Leadership Change over Time (Study 1).

Figure 3. Interaction between Shared Leadership Change and Team Size (Study 1).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


54

Figure 4. Interaction between Shared Leadership Change and Social Support (Study 1).

Figure 5. Shared Leadership Change over Time (Study 2).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


55

Figure 6. Interaction between Shared Leadership Change and Team Size (Study 2).

Figure 7. Shared Leadership Change over Time (Study 3).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


56

Figure 8. Interaction between Shared Leadership Change and Social Support (Study 3).

Figure 9. Interaction between Shared Leadership Change and Team Familiarity (Study 3).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


57

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics (Study 1)
Variables Me SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8
1. Shared Leadership T1 0.6
an 0.0 .
2. Shared Leadership T2 0.6
5 0.0
9 .21
3. Shared Leadership T3 0.6
7 0.0
8 .13
4. Shared Leadership T4 0.6
8 0.0
8 .71*
5. Average SL Leadership 0.6
9 0.0
9 .29* *.45* .83*
6. Team Size 5.3
7 0.6
7 - * .76
.55 - * *- *
.87 .86 - * -
7. Social Support Environment 4.2
5 0.2
6 .33 *.37*
*- * * * .23
.39 .66* .64* -
8. TeamPurpose
Shared Performance 6.4
7 0.9
7 -
.11 .50*
* .21 *-.01* *-
.51 .36* .04
* .04 - -
Note. N = 31; Average SL = Average6Shared
0 Leadership.
.07 * * .02 .29 .2
* **
p < .05. p <.01. 0

Table 2
Results from RCM predicting changes in Shared Leadership (Study 1)
Variable Model Model Model Model Model Model Model
Intercept .67*** .65*** .95*** .88*** .41* .81**
+ + +
Time (b1) 1-.01 2 .05 3 .05 4-.07 5.05 6-1.28** 7-1.43**
***
Time^2 (b2) -.02* -.02* .08 -.02* .45*** 1.07
.57**
Team Size (b3) -.06* -.04 -.05*
Social Support (b3a) .06 -.04 -.04
Time*Team Size (b4) .02 .03
+
Time^2*Team Size -.02 -.03*
Time*Social Support .31** .31**
(b5)
Time^2*Social -.11*** -.11**
2
(b 4a)
Pseudo R .03 .18 .15 .20 .03 .08 .25
Support
Note. (b5a) R2 is calculated as the proportional reduction of level 1 error variances resulting
Pseudo
from adding predictors (compared to a null-model with no predictors). Subscript notations are
consistent with equation 2.
+
p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Table 3
Results from OLS Predicting Team Performance (Study 1)
Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6
Intercept 6.18** 8.15*** 6.91*** 7.39*** 6.58*** 9.26***
Average Shared Leadership .37 -2.05
Shared Leadership Time 1 -.72 -1.01
Shared Leadership Time 4 -.22 -3.44
(Quadratic) Slope of SL 18.40* 16.10 *
25.04**
2
R .01 .15 .01 .14 .00 .22
ΔR2 .14* .13* .20*
Note. N = 31.
*
p < .05; **p <.01; ***p < .001.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


58

Table 4
Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables (Study 2)
Variables Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Project Length 10.02 4.29
2. Team Size 4.21 0.75 -0.07
3. Shared 0.65 0.09 0.22 0.08
Leadership T1
4. Shared 0.76 0.10 - -0.23 0.35*
Leadership T2 0.03
5. Shared 0.74 0.08 -0.01 -0.24 0.26
Leadership T3 0.59*
*

6. Average SL 0.72 0.07 0.07 -0.18


0.69* 0.85* 0.79*
* * *

7. Performance 7.86 1.45 0.32* 0.08 0.21 -0.07 - -0.09 -


0.28*
Note. N = 52; Average SL = Average Shared Leadership.
*
p < .05. **p < .01.

Table 5
Results from RCM predicting changes in Shared Leadership (Study 2)
Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Intercept .66*** .64*** .60***
Project Length .00 .00 .00
*** **
Time (b1) .05 .17 .44**
***
Time^2 (b2) -.07 -.16*
Team Size (b3) .01
Time*Team Size (b4) -.06*
Time^2*Team Size (b5) .02
2
Pseudo R .14 .22 .25
2
Note. Pseudo R is calculated as the proportional reduction of level 1 error variances resulting
from adding predictors (compared to a null-model with no predictors).
*
p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


59

Table 6
Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables (Study 3)
Variables Mea SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.
n
1. Team Size 0.7
4.18 3
2. Shared 0.1 -
Leadership T1 0.64 6 0.19
3. Shared 0.1 0.06 -
Leadership T2 0.66 8 0.12
4. Shared 0.1 -
Leadership T3 0.70 6 0.49* 0.26* 0.48*
* *

5. Average SL 0.1 -
0.67 2 0.29* 0.54* 0.68* 0.84*
* * * *
*
6. Social Support 0.4 - - 0.41
3.87 9 0.04 0.07 0.34* 0.33
*

7. Team 0.5 - - 0.02


Familiarity 1.62 0 0.21 0.07 0.24+ 0.09 0.29
*

8. Team 18.2 1.0 0.13 - 0.08 - - 0.02 0.02 -


Performance 8 2 0.25+ 0.14 0.15
Note. N = 65; Average SL = Average Shared Leadership.
+
p < .10. *p < .05. **p <.01.

Table 7
Results from RCM predicting changes in Shared Leadership (Study 3)
Variable Mode Mode Mode Mode Mode Mode Mode Mode
Intercept .85*** .59*** .96***
Time (b1) l 1.01 l 2.10* l 3.10* l 4-.60* 5*
l.10 l-.20
6 l7 l 8*
-.66
*** **** **** *** ***
Time^2 (b2) .87 .86
-.04 .57
-.04 .93.14 -.04* .92
.09+ -.04* .18
** * * * * * ** *
Team Size (b3) -.05 -.05 -.05 -.04 -.05 -.05 .10-.05 -.05*
Social Support .07* -.03 -.01
*
Time*Social Support .18 .14*
Time^2*Social -.05 .07** -.03
(b4)
Familiarity (b3a) .00 -.04 -.02 -.03
*
Support (b )
Time*Familiarity(b
5 4a .18 .14+
Time^2*Familiarity -.08* -.08*
2
)Pseudo R .04 .07 .14 .15 .06 .07 .11 .17
(b5a)
Note. Pseudo R2 is calculated as the proportional reduction of level 1 error variances resulting
from adding predictors (compared to a null-model with no predictors).
+
p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

You might also like