Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Journal of Family Violence (2020) 35:603–618

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10896-019-00082-y

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Sexual Victimization and Sexual Harassment


among College Students: a Comparative Analysis
Gillian M. Pinchevsky 1 & Amy B. Magnuson 2 & Megan Bears Augustyn 3 & Callie Marie Rennison 4

Published online: 9 July 2019


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
Sexual victimization, defined as non-contact unwanted sexual experiences (e.g., sexual harassment), unwanted sexual contact, sexual
coercion, forcible rape, or alcohol- or drug-facilitated assault or rape, is nontrivial among the college study population. This study explores
how the situational contexts, which include self-reported consequences and responses to victimization, compare between incidents of non-
contact sexual harassment (SH) and contact sexual victimization (CSV) (e.g., unwanted sexual contact, sexual coercion, and rape).
Conjunctive analyses compare the commonality and variation among situational contexts of incidents of SH and incidents of CSV from
a sample of survivors from 24 different colleges. Both forms of sexual violence negatively impact college students. Respondents who
experienced SH only (i.e., did not also experience CSV) in the past year and those who experienced contact CSV only (i.e., did not
also experience non-contact SH) in the past year both reported negative outcomes associated with the victimization, but it was more
common for survivors of SH only to report being intimidated and feeling uncomfortable in their environment at the college. The
implications of this study are twofold. First, scholars should ensure that research on the negative effects of CSV does not come at the
expense of other forms of sexual victimization. Second, practitioners, campus services, and prevention programs should acknowledge both
contact and non-contact sexual victimization in programming and services in order to buffer the negative effects resulting from
victimization.

Keywords Sexual victimization . Sexual harassment . College students

“Sexual harassment is a problem with a long past but a college environment, sexual victimization experiences fall
short history” (American Psychological Association on a continuum ranging from unwanted sexual comments
1993, p.3) and invasions of space (e.g., looks, leers, or catcalls) to un-
wanted sexual contact (e.g., groping or fondling) and complet-
Social movements, such as “MeToo” and “Time’s Up,” ed rape. Fisher et al. (2010) refer to sexual victimization on
reignited the national conversation surrounding sexual victim- three dimensions reflecting “type of contact, degree of
ization. The effects of these movements are not lost on college coercion, and degree of action” (p. 90; italics added here for
communities, as sexual victimization experiences are com- emphasis). Contact ranges from no sexual contact to penetra-
mon among students enrolled in institutions of higher educa- tion; coercion ranges from psychological coercion to physical
tion (Cantor et al. 2015). Both within and external to the force; action ranges from words and threatened actions to
completed actions. Despite the acknowledgement that a con-
tinuum of sexual victimization exists (Kelly 1987), the bulk of
* Gillian M. Pinchevsky recent research on college students’ sexual victimization ex-
gillian.pinchevsky@unlv.edu periences focuses on contact (i.e., physical) forms of sexual
victimization and in particular, attempted or completed sexual
1
University of Nevada, Las Vegas, 4505 S. Maryland Parkway, Box battery and rape. While contact sexual victimization (CSV)
455009, Las Vegas, NV 89154, USA receives most attention, far less focuses on experiences of
2
Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA non-contact forms of sexual victimization only, including sex-
3
The University of Texas at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX, USA ual harassment (SH). This is somewhat surprising considering
4 that non-contact SH is commonly experienced by college
The University of Colorado Denver, Denver, CO, USA
604 J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

students (Cantor et al. 2015) and is a form of sexual miscon- individual responses (i.e., disclosure and help-seeking) asso-
duct under Title IX, which provides protections against sex- ciated with these incidents. Arguably, this approach allows for
based discrimination in educational programs. a better understanding of the current context of non-contact
The examination of the nature and extent of all forms of SH experiences among college students and speaks to the
sexual victimization is necessary because non-contact SH is a nuances, if any, in the consequences associated with the var-
form of stealth gender-based abuse (SGBA; Belknap and ious behaviors on the continuum of sexual victimization.
Sharma 2014), which is nonviolent gender-based abuse that
while common, frequently goes unnoticed by society, and is
vastly underreported and underacknowledged by victims. Sexual Harassment
SGBA is extremely damaging and traumatic to survivors.
According to Belknap and Sharma (2014, p. 182): The development of SH definitions has arisen in response to
legal challenges in educational, military and workplace set-
…many abusive tactics that qualify as SGBA are extreme- tings. The Equal Employment Opportunity Commission
ly dangerous, violating, humiliating, disempowering, and/ (EEOC) defines SH – a violation of Title VII of the Civil
or threatening…Indeed, they can be some of the most Rights Act of 1964 – as:
terrifying, intrusive, confusing, and demeaning GBA
behaviors.1 Unwelcome sexual advances, requests for sexual favors,
and other verbal or physical conduct of a sexual nature
Therefore, it is important to study not only CSV but non- constitute SH when this conduct explicitly or implicitly
contact types of sexual victimization as well. affects an individual's employment, unreasonably inter-
Although SH is anything but harmless (e.g., Till 1980; feres with an individual's work performance, or creates
Cammaert 1985) and anything but rare (see Cantor et al. an intimidating, hostile, or offensive work environment.
2015), it is often misunderstood (e.g., Castillo et al. 2011; (https://www.eeoc.gov/eeoc/publications/fs-sex.cfm).
Fitzgerald and Ormerod 1991; Gervasio and Ruckdeschel
1992; Malovich and Stake 1990; Rotundo et al. 2001). The This definition of sexual harassment includes discrim-
current study explores how the responses to and consequences ination as a result of gender identity, including transgen-
of non-contact SH (here defined by a lack of physical contact) der status, or sexual orientation in line with Title VII
compare to incidents of CSV (here defined as completed or (e.g., Oncale v. Sundowner Offshore Services, 523 U.S.
attempted rape, sexual coercion, and/or unwanted touching). 75, 1998). Within institutions of higher education set-
Exploring potential similarities and differences between non- tings, SH is identified under Title IX:
contact SH and CSV is important because most college-based
prevention and intervention programming addresses CSV, not No person in the United States shall, on the basis of sex,
non-contact SH. However, development and implementation be excluded from participation in, be denied the benefits
of prevention and intervention programming requires an un- of, or be subjected to discrimination under any educa-
derstanding of the differences between these two forms of tion program or activity receiving Federal financial as-
gender-based abuse in order to reduce the incidence and con- sistance. (https://www2.ed.gov/about/offices/list/ocr/
sequences of both non-contact SH and CSV. docs/tix_dis.html)
To address our goal, data from undergraduate students en-
rolled in 24 U.S. college campuses were examined to compare Scholars have also identified dimensions of SH (Adams et al.
the situational contexts of non-contact SH and CSV using 1983; Gruber 1992; Till 1980). Summarizing these conceptuali-
Conjunctive Analysis of Case Configurations (CACC). zations, Fitzgerald and colleagues (Fitzgerald et al. 1988; 1995)
Specifically, this strategy allows for the comparison of situa- developed the Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ) in order
tional contexts of victimization incidents among students who to identify experiences of sexual harassment. Three dimensions
experienced non-contact SH exclusively (and did not experi- of SH are generally identified: sexual coercion (e.g., subtle and
ence any CSV) to incidents among students who experienced non-subtle attempts or actions that punish someone if they do not
CSV exclusively (and did not experience non-contact SH).2 In comply with sexual demands or requests), unwanted sexual at-
particular, we focused on victim characteristics (i.e., demo- tention (e.g., unwanted discussion of sexual behavior, staring,
graphics), consequences (i.e., changes in routine activities, repeated requests for sexual attention that have been met with
emotional health and feelings of safety at the college) and rejection), and gender harassment (e.g., suggestive or offensive
comments; displaying unwelcomed, offensive materials).
1 According to Fitzgerald et al. (1995), gender harassment and
GBA refers to gender-based abuse.
2
594 individuals in initial sample reported experiencing both SH and at least unwanted sexual attention generally fall under the legal concept
one form of CSV. of a hostile environment and sexual coercion falls under the legal
J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618 605

concept of quid pro quo; in instances where the victim complies also more likely to experience harassment that involved
with the demands, the latter is considered a form of CSV. being sexually touched, cornered, or brushed up against. On the
Importantly, legal and conceptual definitions of SH do not appear other hand, males were especially more likely to experience ho-
to have fully translated into public understanding and labeling of mophobic comments (Hill and Silva 2005). Researchers have also
specific behaviors as a form SH (see Rotundo et al. 2001; examined the relationship between non-contact SH and sexual
Blumenthal 1998), particularly non-contact forms of SH. As a orientation and sexual identity and found that those in the sexual
result, individual acknowledgement and responses to this form of minority were more likely to report experiencing non-contact SH
sexual victimization are affected. compared to individuals identifying in the gender binary (e.g.,
Cantor et al. 2015) and those identifying as heterosexual (e.g.,
Sexual Harassment Experienced by College Students Cantor et al. 2015; Hill and Silva 2005; McGinley et al.
2016). Hill and Silva (2005) also found that students who iden-
Research from the 1980s and early 1990s on SH experienced by tified as lesbian, gay, bisexual, or transgender reported being
college students established it as a problem experienced primarily sexually harassed often (see also McGinley et al. 2016).
by female students (Fitzgerald et al. 1988; Lott et al. 1982; Notably, there is not a clear pattern of non-contact SH victim-
Maihoff and Forrest 1983; Metha and Nigg 1982; McCormack ization across race/ethnicity (see Kearney and Gilbert
1985; Rubin and Borgers 1990). Male students were not exempt 2012; McGinley et al. 2016; Yoon et al. 2010). Some research
from SH, though (Mazer and Percival 1989; Metha and Nigg found SH to be experienced more among non-White student
1982; Fitzgerald et al. 1988). Recent estimates of non-contact SH populations (e.g., Yoon et al. 2010), others found SH to be more
indicate that is a common experience among college students commonly experienced by White students (e.g., Hill and Silva
(Cantor et al. 2015; Coker et al. 2016; Hill and Silva 2005). 2005; Ho et al. 2012; Kearney and Gilbert 2012; McGinley
Although estimates vary, the recent Association of American et al. 2016), and others found few differences across race and
Universities Campus Climate Survey on Sexual Assault and ethnicity (e.g., Cantor et al. 2015).
Sexual Misconduct (hereafter referred to as the AAU survey)
reported that roughly 48% of students experienced non-contact Responses to and Consequences of Sexual
SH from another student, faculty or staff, or affiliated person with Harassment
the university since enrolling in college (Cantor et al. 2015).3 The
most common form of non-contact SH was someone making Similar to other forms of victimization, non-contact SH is asso-
“inappropriate or offensive comments regarding body, appear- ciated with various consequences. However, how an individual
ance or sexual activity” (Cantor et al. 2015, p. 87). Fisher and perceives the non-contact SH experience likely affects the re-
colleagues’ (Fisher et al. 2000) found that undergraduate women sponses to and consequences of SH. Differences in definitions
experienced a range of unwanted verbal victimizations, includ- of non-contact SH exist (i.e., gender harassment and unwanted
ing, but not limited to, being subjected to sexist comments sexual attention; Blumenthal 1998; Rotundo et al. 2001), partic-
(~54%), “cat calls” or similar behavior (~48%), inappropriate ularly across gender, as women are more likely to label certain
sexual questions (19%), and exposure of body parts (~5%). behaviors as SH compared to men (Marks and Nelson 1993).
Incidents of non-contact SH are not evenly distributed among McGinley et al. (2016) found that experiences of non-
college students. Although the aforementioned AAU study found contact SH undermined the health of college students. Non-
that experiences of non-contact SH were more prevalent among contact SH is associated with decreased mental health (i.e.,
female students (Cantor et al. 2015), Hill and Silva (2005) found depression, anxiety) and increased health-risk behavior such
that male and female college students experienced a similar like- as substance use as a coping mechanism, particularly among
lihood of being sexually harassed, but reported different types White females and sexual minorities (McGinley et al. 2016).
of harassment (see also Chiodo et al. 2009 for a study on non- Paludi et al. (2006) identified other research that noted the
college students). Specifically, while both male and female vic- consequences of non-contact SH including changes in physical
tims of sexual harassment reported experiencing sexually-based and mental health. Additionally, victims of non-contact SH are
"comments, jokes, gestures, or looks" (p. 18), these experiences more likely to experience future SH (Petersen and Hyde 2009).
were more commonly experienced by women. Females were After taking into consideration prior research examining re-
sponses to sexual harassment, Knapp et al. (1997) proposed that
3
Cantor et al. (2015; p. xv, 29) defined sexual harassment as experiencing any responses and coping strategies for SH fall into four categories
of the following (1) made sexual remarks or told jokes or stories that were
based on whether the focus is self- or perpetrator- in nature (e.g.,
insulting or offensive to you?; (2) made inappropriate or offensive comments
about your or someone else’s body, appearance or sexual activities?; (3) said addresses the perpetrator directly or not), and the degree of out-
crude or gross sexual things to you or tried to get you to talk about sexual side support sought: 1) advocacy seeking (both addresses the
matters when you didn’t want to?; (4) emailed, texted, tweeted, phoned, or perpetrator and seeks help), 2) social coping (does not address
instant messaged offensive sexual remarks, jokes, stories, pictures or videos to
you that you didn’t want?; (5) continued to ask you to go out, get dinner, have perperator directly but does seek support from others), 3)
drinks or have sex even though you said, “No”? avoidance/denial (neither directly addresses the perpetrator nor
606 J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

seeks help), and 4) confrontation/negotiation (addresses the per- heterosexual, female students” (Fedina et al. 2018, p. 90)
petrator directly but does not use outside support). The first two thereby limiting our understanding of sexual victimization
forms of coping involve disclosure of one’s victimization. experienced by students from other demographics.
Advocacy seeking or formal disclosure of SH victimization, par- However, we are slowly learning more about its preva-
ticularly non-contact SH victimization, tends to occur infrequent- lence across demographics. Women are more likely than
ly (e.g., Adams et al. 1983; Cammaert 1985; Cantor et al. 2015; men to be victims of CSV (see also Cantor et al. 2015;
Hill and Silva 2005). Moreover, sexually harassed victims tend to Koss 2018); moving beyond this traditional dichotomy
"engage in a trial and error approach" (Cortina and Wasti, 2005, indicates that respondents who do not identify as male or
p. 182), using a variety of individual coping responses (e.g., female and report a sexual orientation other than hetero-
avoidance/denial or confrontation/negotiation) and social coping sexual have the highest likelihood of experiencing a CSV
through informal disclosure (Cantor et al. 2015; Hill and Silva (Cantor et al. 2015). Cantor et al. (2015) also found slight
2005) rather than formal disclosure and help-seeking. The few differences in victimization across race/ethnicity, but they
who do formally disclose victimization usually only do so after did not vary dramatically, as Black or African American
the harassment has increased in frequency or individual function- and Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islanders reported
ing has greatly been compromised (Cortina and Wasti 2005). only a slightly higher rate of CSV than American Indian,
Asian and White students.

Contact Sexual Victimization Experienced Responses to and Consequences of Contact Sexual


by College Students Victimization

CSV includes unwanted sexual touching, oral sex, or pen- In comparison to SH, there is more consensus regarding the
etration. It can occur as a result of physical force or threats behaviors that constitute CSV. Nonetheless, there is still some
of physical force, or it may occur while one is incapaci- ambiguity among individuals with respect to the role of con-
tated. It may also be a result of quid pro quo behaviors, sent when identifying incidents of CSV (i.e., there is a recog-
where a victim does not want to engage in sexual acts but nition in legislation that without consent, unwanted sexual
is forced to as a result of some ultimatum or threat (i.e., behaviors are a form of assault, but half of the states in the
sexual coercion). Most estimates of the prevalence of CSV US do not define consent and there are state-level variations;
focus on one specific type of SV: rape or attempted rape. Decker and Baroni 2011), which ultimately affects the conse-
For instance, Kilpatrick and colleagues (Kilpatrick et al. quences of and responses to CSV. For instance, the ambiguity
2007) reported that 11.5% of US college women had been in perceptions of what legitimately constitutes CSV, as well as
raped, including forcible, incapacitated, and drug- or fear, shame and embarrassment (e.g., Fisher et al. 2000; 2003;
alcohol-facilitated rape (see also Krebs et al. 2007). Kilpatrick et al. 2007; Krebs et al. 2007) interact such that
Similarly, the aforementioned AAU study reported that contact forms of sexual victimization are the most under-
almost 11% of undergraduate females reported experienc- reported crimes to the police (Morgan and Kena 2017;
ing a behavior that would meet the legal criteria of rape Tjaden and Thoennes 2006).
(i.e., penetration by force or threat of force or when the Advocacy seeking (see Knapp et al. 1997) in the form
victim was unable to consent due to incapacitation) since of reporting CSV to law enforcement among college stu-
they enrolled in college (Cantor et al. 2015). dents is extremely low and varies by study (see Sabina and
Fisher and colleagues (Fisher et al. 2000) measured 12 Ho 2014). According to Fisher and colleagues’ (Fisher
distinct forms of CSV (including both attempted and com- et al. 2000), less than 5% of completed or attempted
pleted acts); notably, 10 of these victimizations were sep- rapes perpetrated against college women were reported
arate from completed or attempted rape (e.g., completion to police. For other forms of contact (attempted or com-
or attempted sexual coercion; completed or attempted sex- pleted) SV, rates of non-reporting were similarly low, if
ual contact with or without force). The prevalence rates not lower (Fisher et al. 2000, 2003). Fisher et al. (2000)
ranged from 0.18% (i.e., contact with force or threat of found that not one of the completed and attempted sexual
force) to 3% of the sample (i.e., attempted sexual contact coercions and threats of penetration without force ac-
without force). Moreover, Fedina and colleagues (Fedina knowledged by study participants were reported to law
et al. 2018) noted that among college students, unwanted enforcement. Formal reporting in the AAU study was also
sexual contact, sexual coercion, incapacitated rape, and low, ranging from 5% for incidents of sexual touching
attempted or completed attempted forcible rape are all while incapacitated to 26% of incidents with penetration
commonly experienced forms of CSV. by force (includes reporting to police as well as other
The majority of the extant literature on college student campus agencies; Cantor et al. 2015). While some victims
sexual victimization “sampled predominately White, of CSV (i.e., rape, sexual coercion, unwanted sexual
J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618 607

contact) reported the victimization to other campus author- Data and Method
ities (e.g., professor, dean, residence hall personnel) or
university-based or community-based victim services or Data
health care centers, the overwhelming majority did not
(Fisher et al. 2003; Krebs et al. 2007). Sabina and Ho’s One academic year of the multi-college Bystander Efficacy
(2014) review of the extant literature on college victims of Evaluation (mcBEE) data were used for this research.5 These
sexual assault and dating violence (with most of the re- data originate from a national study on violence prevention and
ported studies focused on sexual assault) noted “the reality intervention education funded by the Centers for Disease Control
is that few college victims actually utilize campus or other and Prevention (CDC). Data collection efforts for the study –
services” (p. 218; see also Stoner and Cramer 2017). which was developed and administered by researchers at the
Notably, though, medical and mental health services University of Kentucky – began in January of 2016 and are
(e.g., counseling; Krebs et al. 2007) appear to be more on-going (see Clear et al. 2019, Lessons learned in creating a
highly utilized than other services (Sabina and Ho 2014). college consortium, this issue, for an in-depth discussion of these
Instead of advocacy seeking through formal disclosure data and data collection procedures). Data were collected via self-
and health care assistance – both physical and mental health administered online surveys from enrolled undergraduate stu-
care – college victims of CSV are more likely to engage in dents (ages 18–24) attending at least one in-person class at 24
social coping (see Knapp et al. 1997) and disclose the vic- public colleges in the United States. All survey procedures were
timization experience to informal support systems, such as approved by each institution’s Institutional Review Board as well
friends (e.g., Cantor et al. 2015; Fisher et al. 2000, 2003; as the Institutional Review Board at the University of Kentucky.
Krebs et al. 2007). College students often employ addition- In 2017 (data collected during the 2016–2017 school year), the
al strategies to cope with the experience of CSV. These aggregate response rate was 13.4%, and response rates varied
include avoidance of the perpetrator (Krebs et al. 2007) or from 2.7% to 22.5% across institutions.
moving residences (Krebs et al. 2007, 2016). Each student responded to a series of behaviorally-specific
Related to responses to CSV are the consequences of CSV, questions (Rotundo et al. 2001; Blumenthal 1998) regarding
which are pervasive and affect physical and emotional health, whether or not the individual experienced a variety of forms
health-risk behaviors, employment performance, and interper- sexual victimization in the past year, including non-contact
sonal relationships (e.g., Jordan et al. 2014; Kilpatrick et al. SH, sexual assault, sexual coercion, and attempted or complet-
2007; Krebs et al. 2016; Leone and Carroll 2017; Thompson ed rape (see Appendix for list of specific sexual victimization
and Kingree 2010; Turchik and Hassija 2014). Moreover, behaviors and classification by physical contact status).6 This
CSV has been shown to disrupt one’s academic trajectory series of questions regarding sexual victimization was follow-
(e.g., take time off from school, withdrawal, dropping ed by a set of questions particular to the most recent incident
classes; Krebs et al. 2016). Additionally, female college stu- of sexual victimization. For the purposes of this analysis, we
dents who report CSV are at a considerably higher risk of only include data on incidents of sexual victimization from
suicidality even after controlling for psychological health- students who reported experiencing SH exclusively (i.e., stu-
related variables (Leone and Carroll 2017). dent did not experience CSV) or students who experienced
CSV exclusively (i.e., student did not experience non-contact
SH) in order to accurately speak to the similarities and differ-
Current Study ences between non-contact SH and CSV. In sum, all respon-
dents in this study experienced either non-contact SH only or
Little research has differentiated between the nature and CSV only.7
consequences of non-contact SH from CSV. Therefore this
research fills this gap and investigates differences in 5
These data were collected by a University of Kentucky based team, and
victim/survivor demographics, responses to, and conse- funds were provided by Bystander Program Adoption & Efficacy to Reduce
SV-IPV in College Community. Funding source: Department of Health and
quences of these two related forms of sexual victimization. Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center
Importantly, to remove overlapping or co-occurring expe- for Injury Prevention and Control Cooperative Agreement U01 CE002668.
riences, two mutually exclusive groups were compared: (Multi College Bystander Efficacy Evaluation). The team included Principal
Investigators Ann L. Coker, PhD, MPH and Heather M. Bush, PhD., as well as
those experiencing 1) non-contact SH alone (without
Candace J. Brancato, Emily R. Clear, and Kelsey Rutherford.
CSV) and 2) CSV alone (without non-contact SH).4 6
We only include information from incidents that occurred within the previ-
ous year and not “ever” in order to capture experiences of students while
4
Although some individuals do experience both sexual harassment and CSV, enrolled at the college.
7
due to the nature of our analytic strategy, it is not possible to disentangle the 594 students reported experiencing both sexual harassment and contact sex-
responses and consequences to each type of victimization among these indi- ual victimization. Unfortunately, we are not able to discern if the experience of
viduals. As such, we focus only on the situational contexts reported by survi- SH was related to CSV or if the perpetrator of SH also perpetrated an act of
vors of one type of sexual victimization – sexual harassment or CSV. CSV against the victim due to the structure of the interview schedule.
608 J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

Analytic Strategy Three respondent demographics were included: gender iden-


tity, sexual orientation, and race/ethnicity.8 Gender identity was
Researchers are cognizant that context matters and outcomes are measured using three categories: woman, man, and other (i.e.,
frequently associated with multiple related variables. This study transgender woman, transgender man, genderqueer/gender non-
used Conjunctive Analysis of Case Configurations (CACC) conforming, questioning, and identity “not listed”). Sexual
which is not a variable oriented approach; rather, it is an analytic orientation was measured using a binary variable: heterosexual
technique that harnesses the power of these interrelated factors and the reference group is other (i.e., gay/lesbian, bisexual, asex-
and allows one to construct, examine, and compare the situation- ual, questioning, and sexual orientation “not listed”). Finally,
al contexts – created from variable attributes – associated with race/ethnicity was included in the matrix with four categories:
particular outcomes. This exploratory technique is based on com- non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic (any race),
parative approaches used in multivariate analysis of categorical and other (i.e., other option).
data (Miethe et al. 2008). Additionally, we included two binary variables representing
Unlike variable-oriented approaches, which focus on the responses to victimization: different and detached. Different indi-
role of variables and how they are independently associated cated whether or not the student self-reported that he/she ‘felt
with an outcome, CACC identifies situational contexts asso- different’ after the incident (Yes/No). Detached referred to wheth-
ciated with outcomes. Focusing on contexts enables the iden- er or not the student self-reported that he/she felt detached from
tification of variation (or lack thereof) among contexts of SH others, activities or surroundings after the incident (Yes/No).
(in contrast to CSV). In addition, CACC allowed us to high- Traumatic consequences of sexual victimization were mea-
light common or normative contexts, as well as uncommon or sured using victim responses: interfere, limited, and intimidating.
deviant contexts (Hart and Miethe 2008). Using CACC, situ- Interfere was measured as a binary variable based on whether or
ational attributes that underlie SH and CSV were identified not the sexual victimization incident interfered with the respon-
and the contexts were rank ordered allowing for the identifi- dent’s academic or professional performance (Yes/No). The bi-
cation of contexts that occurred most (to least) often. This nary variable limited was used to indicate whether the incident
approach offers a visual presentation of case configurations affected the respondent’s ability to participate in college activities
of SH, facilitating the understanding of the nature, diversity or programs (Yes/No). Lastly, intimidating indicated whether the
and distribution of related situational contexts. We now de- sexual victimization incident created an intimidating or uncom-
scribe the variables and attributes used to construct the situa- fortable environment for the respondent (Yes/No).
tional contexts. Four binary variables are used to capture help-seeking actions
after the victimization: Bothered, talk/family, talk/counselor, and
leave. Bothered indicated whether or not the individual self-
Situational Context Measures reported that he/she was bothered by and tried hard not to think
about the incident after it occurred (Yes/No). Talk/family indicat-
The unit of analysis for this research was the violent incident ed whether or not the student self-reported talking to either
(and not the respondent/victim). We identified a sample of friends or family members about the incident (Yes/No). Talk/
3427 non-contact SH situational contexts and 397 CSV situa- counselor indicated whether or not the victim self-reported
tional contexts, yielding a total of 3824 situational contexts of talking to a counselor, therapist, or mental health professional
SH or CSV. The outcome of interest in this research is whether following the event (Yes/No). Finally, leave represents whether
a respondent experienced non-contact SH only (coded as 1) or or not the student thought about leaving college as a result of the
CSV only (coded as 0; see Appendix for individual items used incident (Yes/No).
to construct our measures of non-contact SH and CSV).
Situational contexts for the incidents in matrix form were
constructed from variables describing victim demographics, Additional Matrix Considerations
responses to sexual victimization, traumatic outcomes, and
help-seeking behaviors resulting from the victimization. The We recognize that the variables used are limited with respect to
use of the word “trauma” in this study aligns with the survey the responses and outcomes associated with students’ experi-
and data collection used in this research, which is meant to ences. Additional post-incident feelings and behaviors were in-
reflect a distressing or disturbing experience. Other disciplines cluded in preliminary analyses, but were excluded because there
may use a clinical definition that is not implied with our use of 8
The analyses originally included whether the student was full- or part-time.
the term trauma. Moreover, all variables are categorical in Given the little variation in this variable (95% were full-time), it was excluded
nature per the requirements of the analytic strategy employed from the analysis. The analyses also originally included respondent’s age and
(Hart and Miethe 2008). Nonetheless, we provide all available year in school. Age and year in school were highly correlated (r = .80), and a
series of analysis demonstrated that year in school was unrelated to the out-
response options in the data (some in parentheses) to assist the come. In the end, age was also unrelated so we opted to remove both from the
reader in interpretation. analysis to make the matrix easier to interpret.
J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618 609

was too little variation across contexts. More specifically, too few Victims were primarily female (74%), non-Hispanic
incidents included a victim that reported talking to faculty or staff White (70%), and self-identified their sexual orientation
at the college (0.9%), talked to a resident advisor (2.5%), as “heterosexual” (80%). More than 15% of victims re-
contacted a hotline or gathered data online (1.6%), or contacted ported the incident interfered with their academics or job
police (3.4%). As such, when implementing the “rule of ten” (18%) or limited their ability to participate in school
(i.e., focusing only on those situational contexts that appear in activities (16%). More than half of the victims indicated
the data ten or more times), these variables dropped out of the that the incident created an intimating or uncomfortable
analysis as no dominant situational contexts exhibited variation environment at the college (57%). About one-third (30%)
on these variables. Similarly, there was very little variation in of victims felt differently after that incidents, and 23%
academic consequences, as few incidents involved victims who reported feeling detached from others and activities.
reported missing class or work (11.0%), turning in assignments About four in ten (44%) of victims reporting that he/
late (7.7%) or getting worse grades (8.6%). As such, these vari- she was bothered by and tried hard not to think about
ables were not included in our dominant situational contexts. We the incident. Related to disclosure, 41% of victims talked
note, however, that these exclusions should not be interpreted as to friends or family members and less than 10% of vic-
meaning no incidents of non-contact SH or CSV included a tims talked to a counselor or therapist. Finally, 10% of
victim who displayed these post-incident feelings or behaviors. victims thought about leaving the college.
Rather, because of the methodology used in this study, they are When comparing the mutually exclusive non-contact
not included in our contexts as they are too uncommon to SH and CSV groups, the following differences were not-
analyze. ed (see Table 1). A significantly greater proportion of
men (35% vs 14%; p < .05) and respondents identifying
The Matrix of Situational Contexts as non-heterosexual (21% vs 14%; p < .05) were includ-
ed in incidents of non-contact SH only versus CSV. No
The situational context matrix is based on the identified differences by race/ethnicity were observed. Differences
12 variables, indicating 12,288 possible situational con- in the traumatic consequences indicated less interference
texts (combinations of the variables),9 however only 923 in academic or professional performance (17%) in the
contexts actually occurred in the data. As is standard in non-contact SH incidents relative to CSV victims
CACC, the next step was to implement the minimum (30%). A similar pattern was observed for victims' ability
frequency “rule of ten,” (Hart and Miethe 2008; Miethe to participate in college activities or programs (16% v.
and Regoeczi 2004; Ragin 1987) by focusing only on 18%; p < .05).10 Furthermore, non-contact SH incidents
those situational contexts that appear in the data ten or were associated with the victim being less likely to re-
more times. The use of the “rule of ten” minimizes con- port the range of responses to and consequences of vic-
sideration of rare situational contexts in the data, and timization, with two notable exceptions. Feeling different
focuses instead on common or dominant situational was more common among victims in incidents of non-
contexts. Following this application of the “rule of ten,” contact SH and victims were more likely to report that
there remained 68 empirically-observed dominant situa- the victimization created an intimidating or uncomfort-
tional contexts associated with SH outcomes. In total, able environment in incidents of non-contact SH com-
these 68 contexts accounted for 3824 of the actual con- pared to CSV (58% v. 51%).
texts in the data (as situational contexts occurred in the
data multiple times). Our analyses focus on these Examination of Outcome Differences in Situational
contexts. Contexts with CACC

In order to assist in interpretation of the CACC, we briefly


describe the elements of Table 2. The first column in Table 2
Results
offers identification numbers to facilitate the interpretation of
particular contexts. The next 12 columns identify the variables
Basic descriptive statistics were provided for each vari-
included to create the matrix and the associated response cat-
able included in the full sample of situational contexts
egories. The “proportion experiencing sexual harassment on-
(n = 3824) before focusing on the 68 identified dominant
ly” column indicates (when multiplied by 100) the percentage
situational contexts. Approximately nine of ten respon-
that a particular situational context is associated exclusively
dents experienced SH only, as shown in Table 1.
with non-contact SH. The “n” column identifies the number of
9
12,288 = 3 [gender identity] * 2 [sexual orientation] * 4 [race/ethnicity] * 2 times a particular context appeared in the data. The un-shaded
[interfere] * 2 [limit] * 2 [intimidate] * 2 [bothered] * 2 [detached] * 2 [talk/
10
family] * 2 [talk/counselor] * 2 [different] * 2 [leave]. Differences noted in the text are statistically significant at the p < 0.05 level.
610 J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

Table 1 Descriptives of variables

Total Percent/ Sexual Harassment Contact Sexual Victimization


Total Sample (SH) Only (CSV) Only

Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Outcome Variable
Sexual harassment only 89.6 100.0 –
Sexual violence only 10.4 – 100.0
Gender identity
Woman 73.6 72.3 84.4 *
Man 23.6 34.7 14.1 *
Other 2.9 3.0 1.5 *
Sexual orientation
Heterosexual 79.6 78.8 85.6 *
Other 20.4 21.2 14.4 *
Race/Ethnicity
White, not Hispanic 70.0 70.0 70.0
Black, not Hispanic 3.6 3.6 3.8
Hispanic, any race 12.3 12.3 12.1
Other 14.1 14.1 14.1
Consequences
Interfered with academic or professional performance 17.9 16.5 30.0 *
Limited ability to participate in activities or programs at school 15.9 15.6 18.4 *
Created an intimidating/ uncomfortable environment 56.9 57.5 51.1 *
Responses to Victimization
Felt different 30.3 31.5 20.2 *
Felt detached from others, activities or surroundings after the incident 23.4 22.1 34.3 *
Help-Seeking Behaviors
Bothered and tried hard not to think about it 43.6 41.4 63.0 *
Talked to family or friends 41.2 40.0 51.1 *
Talked to counselor, therapist, or mental health professional 9.5 8.9 14.9 *
Thought about leaving the university 9.9 9.2 15.6 *
n = 3824 n = 3427 n = 397

*
Difference between SH only and contact SV only is significant at p < 0.05
SH sexual harassment, CSV contact sexual victimization

portion of the table identifies normative situational contexts, given the greater proportion of non-contact SH situational
which are dominant contexts found within one standard devia- contexts in our sample.
tion of the mean proportion. The shaded areas represent deviant The most common situational context (#43) involved a
situational contexts,11 which are the dominant contexts that are heterosexual, non-Hispanic White woman, who indicated
more than one standard deviation from the mean proportion. The the incident resulted in no traumatic consequences (see
situational contexts found two or more standard deviations from Table 2). In this context, she did not talk to anyone about the
the mean (none are above the mean) are below the double thin incident, she did not feel detached from others as a result of the
lines and, in this instance, represent the least common situational incident, her activities remained unchanged after the incident,
contexts associated with exclusively non-contact SH. and she did not feel intimidated by her college surroundings
Because of space considerations, all 68 dominant con- after the incident occurred. However, this modal context indi-
texts are not presented in Table 2. Rather, the situational cated that she “felt different” following the incident. This par-
contexts most associated with non-contact SH (i.e., con- ticular context appears 179 times in the data, and 89% of the
texts #1-#19), contexts least associated with exclusively times it occurred, non-contact SH was the type of victimiza-
non-contact SH (#53-#68), and the modal context (#43) tion experienced.
are presented.12 The mean proportion for all contexts in In the 19 exclusively non-contact SH contexts, con-
this table is 0.90 (sd = 0.11), indicating that on average, siderable variance was noted in the victim demographics.
90% of situational contexts in the matrix are characterized These 19 contexts show that non-contact SH includes
exclusively by non-contact SH. This is not unexpected men and women as victims, heterosexual and LGBTQ
victims, and persons who identify as various races and
11
ethnicities. On the other hand, these contexts are almost
This is the term used in CACC analyses and not a term chosen by the
authors. In this case, deviant does not indicate uncommon.
universal as they were not associated with interference
12
A comprehensive matrix of all 68 dominant contexts is available upon request. with academics or work (in 18 of those 19 contexts),
Table 2 Select dominant situational contexts

Context Gender Sexual Race/ Interfere Limited Felt Felt Intimidate Bothered Told Friends/Family Told Thought to Proportion Experiencing n
J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

Identity Orientation Ethnicity Detached different Counselor Leave the University Sexual Harassment Only

1 Man Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 27
2 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Intimidated Not bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 25
3 Man Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Not bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 23
4 Woman Other NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 22
5 Woman Other NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 19
6 Woman Heterosexual Hispanic Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 18
7 Woman Heterosexual Other Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 17
8 Woman Other NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 16
9 Man Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 16
10 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 15
11 Woman Other NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 1.00 15
12 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 14
13 Man Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 14
14 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 12
15 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Limited Not detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 12
16 Woman Heterosexual Other Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Intimidated Bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 12
17 Woman Heterosexual NH White Interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Intimidated Bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 11
18 Woman Other NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 10
19 Woman Other NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Intimidated Bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 1.00 10
43 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 0.89 179
53 Woman Heterosexual NH White Interfered Limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Did not tell Did not tell Thought 0.82 11
54 Man Other NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 0.82 11
55 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Not bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 0.81 64
56 Man Heterosexual Hispanic Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 0.79 19
57 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Not intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 0.79 14
58 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 0.78 85
59 Woman Heterosexual NH White Interfered Limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 0.75 20
60 Woman Heterosexual NH White Interfered Not limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 0.75 12
61 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Felt different Not intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 0.74 46
62 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Did not tell Did not tell Did not think 0.74 42
63 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Not detached Not different Not intimidated Bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 0.74 31
64 Woman Heterosexual NH White Interfered Limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Thought to leave 0.71 14
65 Woman Heterosexual NH White Not interfered Not limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Did not tell Did not think 0.65 31
66 Woman Heterosexual NH White Interfered Limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Talked Did not think 0.64 14
67 Woman Heterosexual NH White Interfered Limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Talked Thought to leave 0.64 14
68 Woman Other NH White Interfered Limited Detached Felt different Intimidated Not bothered Told Talked Thought to leave 0.60 10
611
612 J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

and the incident did not limit the respondent’s ability to bothered by and tried hard not to think about the incident
participate in college activities or programs (in 18 of the (4 of 4 contexts), felt detached as a result of the incident
19 contexts). Moreover, the contexts of these incidents (4 of 4 contexts), talked to friends and family (4 of 4
demonstrate notable traumatic consequences to the vic- contexts), talked to a mental health professional (3 of 4
tim. Specifically, in 15 of the 19 contexts, the incident contexts), generally felt different post-incident (4 of 4
created an intimidating or uncomfortable environment for contexts), and thought about leaving college (2 of 4 con-
the victim. In 12 of the 19 contexts, the victim felt dif- texts). The situational contexts that were least associated
ferent as a result of the incident, although none thought with non-contact SH were qualitatively different in terms
of leaving college. As such, these contexts clearly dem- of universal traumatic consequences compared to the sit-
onstrate that non-contact SH is associated with traumatic uational contexts associated with non-contact SH only.
consequences regardless of other context characteristics. These situational contexts demonstrate that traumatic
Finally, it is notable that in 13 of the 19 contexts, victims consequences associated with CSV are associated with a
self-reported being bothered by the incident and tried not varied assortment of contexts.
to think about it after it occurred. In 9 of the 19 contexts, Another way to illustrate differences in contexts most
the victim talked to friends and/or family members about and least likely associated with non-contact SH is
the incident and none (0/19) talked to a counselor or shown in Table 3. We identified and compared the dif-
other mental health expert. ferences in the prevalence of variables in situational
Turning to the 13 deviant contexts (#56-#68) that are contexts most associated with non-contact SH (100%
least associated with non-contact SH, we found that of situational contexts are for exclusively non-contact
these contexts were dominated by women (in 12 of SH incidents, rows 1–19 in CACC matrix) and those
the 13 contexts), heterosexuals (in 12 of the 13 con- situational contexts most associated with CSV (rows
texts), and non-Hispanic Whites (12 of 13 contexts). 56–68 in CACC matrix) in order to facilitate compari-
In terms of traumatic consequences, the deviant contexts son. These findings show that CSV was more likely
show great variation, such that 6 of the 13 involved than non-contact SH to include women (roughly 92%
interference with academics or work, 5 of the 13 in- v. 79%), heterosexuals (roughly 92% v. 68%) and non-
volved a limited ability to participate in college activi- Hispanic Whites (roughly 92% v. 84%). In addition,
ties or programs, and 9 of the 13 were characterized by contexts least associated with exclusively non-contact
an intimidating and uncomfortable environment. An ex- SH were more likely to include traumatic consequences
amination of help-seeking behaviors and responses to in the form of interfering with activities (roughly 46%
victimization showed that 11 of the 13 deviant contexts v. 5%), limiting academic and work performance
included respondents who were bothered by and tried (roughly 39% v. 5%) and thoughts about leaving college
not to think about it, and in 8 of 13 deviant contexts, (23% v. 0%). Interestingly, though, contexts most asso-
the respondent felt detached as a result. All but 2 of the ciated with non-contact SH involved an intimidating and
13 deviant contexts involved respondents who talked uncomfortable environment after the victimization inci-
with friends and family members after the incident, dent (roughly 79% v. 69%). In sum, both non-contact
and 3 of the 13 contexts involved respondents who SH and CSV result in traumatic consequences, however,
spoke with a counselor or mental health professional. it appeared that non-contact SH was associated with
Finally, 11 of the 13 contexts involved respondents fewer traumatic consequences.
who felt different as a result of his/her victimization,
and 3 of the 13 contexts involved respondents who
thought about leaving college. Discussion
When we focused on the four contexts that were more
than two standard deviations from the mean context Recent research has largely focused on CSV experienced
(#65-#68; proportion = .60–.65), there was much less var- by college students; particular foci include rape and sex-
iation. These deviant contexts involved respondents who ual assault. As such, the consideration of non-contact SH
were women (4 of 4 contexts), heterosexual (3 of 4 con- among the recent generation of college students has
texts), and identified as non-Hispanic White (4 of 4 con- largely been ignored in comparison. This study explored
texts). These contexts also involved respondents who in- how the situational contexts of non-contact SH incidents
dicated that the victimization resulted in interference vary from the situational contexts of CSV among a sam-
with work and academics (3 of 4 contexts), limited one’s ple of college students from 24 institutions of higher
ability to engage in activities (3 of 4 contexts), and cre- education in the United States. Using conjunctive analy-
ated an intimidating/uncomfortable environment (4 of 4 ses, numerous patterns emerged, which we now discuss
contexts). Within these contexts, respondents were in more detail.
J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618 613

The top of the matrix includes contexts #1-#19, and the bottom of the matrix includes deviant contexts #56-#68. Fractions shown in the columns indicate the number of contexts that have a particular characteristic. For
First, contexts associated with non-contact SH vary

Thought to

University
Leave the
greatly. In this study, the distribution of incidents across

example, 15/19 in the gender identity - woman column indicates that 15 of the 19 contexts involved a woman. The decimal in each column is simply the value of the fraction. For example, 15/19 = 78.9%
situational contexts ranged from 100% (i.e., particular

3/13
0/19

23.1
0.0
situations that only involve non-contact SH) to a low
of 60% (i.e., a situational contexts that involves non-

Counselor
contact SH less often because they also occur among
incidents of CSV). At most, 40% of these contexts in-
Told

3/13
0/19

23.1
0.0
volved contact SV. The most common non-contact SH
Help-Seeking Behaviors

situational context involved heterosexual women who


Friends/
Family
identified as non-Hispanic White and did not report any

11/13
Bothered Told

9/19

84.6
47.4
traumatic consequences associated with the incident of
non-contact SH. The breakdown of demographics in this
study are consistent with some prior research that finds
11/13
6/19
31.6

84.6

that non-contact SH is more common among non-


Hispanic White women, (e.g., Hill and Silva 2005; Ho
Different

et al. 2012; Kearney and Gilbert 2012; McGinley et al.


12/19

11/13

2016), which also may be a function of our sample.


Felt

63.2

84.6

However, this does not necessarily mean that rates of


Victimization
Responses to

victimization among non-Hispanic White females are


Detached

higher than other racial/ethnic and gender combinations.


Race/ Ethnicity - Interfered Limited Intimidated Felt

8/13
2/19
10.5

61.5

As such, these results do not stand in contrast to extant


research on non-contact SH experiences that shows var-
iation in rates of victimization across race/ethnicity. We
15/19

9/13
78.9

69.2

simply found that situational contexts with White non-


Hispanic victims were more frequent. Furthermore, it is
important to recall that this study compared those who
5/13
1/19

38.5
5.3
Consequences

experienced exclusively non-contact SH to those who


experienced CSV exclusively; therefore, we did not ex-
plore and cannot speak to the demographics of those
A comparison of contexts most and least associated with Sexual Harassment (SH)

6/13
1/19

46.2

respondents who experienced no forms of sexual vio-


5.3

lence or those who experienced both non-contact SH


non Hispanic

and CSV.
Second, both non-contact SH and CSV are associated
with notable – albeit somewhat different – post-incident
White

12/13
16/19

92.3
84.2

feelings, traumatic consequences, and help-seeking be-


haviors. Specifically, non-contact SH contexts were
Orientation –
Heterosexual

more likely to be associated with incidents where vic-


tims reported an intimidating or uncomfortable environ-
Gender Identity – Sexual

12/13
13/19

ment at their college, while CSV was more likely to


68.4

92.3

include incidents where the victim reported interference


with academic or professional performance and limited
Demographics

ability to participate in activities or programs at the


college. Interestingly, our analyses indicated that the
Woman

12/13
15/19

most frequent or dominant situational contexts associat-


78.9

92.3

ed with non-contact SH and CSV did not include vic-


Most Associated with Contact Sexual
Violence (SV: bottom of matrix)
Harassment (SH; top of matrix)

tims who engaged in help-seeking behaviors from fac-


ulty/staff, resident advisors, police or authorities. Recall,
Most Associated with Sexual

we were unable to include these variables in our situa-


tional contexts due to a lack of variation in dominant
situational contexts (i.e., no incidents included engaging
in these help-seeking behaviors). Moreover, our domi-
Table 3

nant situational contexts did not include incidents where


victims contacted hotlines or gathered information about
614 J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

sexual victimization online, pressed charges against the provisions. Furthermore, one implication of the low prev-
perpetrator, missed class or work, did not turn in assign- alence of formal disclosure among frequent situational
ments or turned in assignments late, and received worse contexts is that reliance upon formal sources for esti-
grades. These consequences and help-seeking behaviors mates regarding the prevalence of non-contact SH (and
tend to be rare in the sample of incidents as a whole. CSV more generally) is unwise as these estimates will
However, it should be noted that a substantial percent- significantly underestimate the prevalence of SH and
age of victims of non-contact SH did indeed feel differ- CSV experiences.
ent after the incident, were bothered by the incident, A third point of discussion is that for many of the
and disclosed their experiences to friends or family, variables included in this analysis, their association with
suggesting that the incident certainly affected individual non-contact SH was highly contextual. For example,
functioning even if advocacy seeking and educational victim demographics, an incident resulting in an intim-
performance were not affected. idating environment, and an incident resulting in a vic-
Prior literature demonstrates that victims of SH, in tim feeling differently were found among situational
general, cope with their victimization in a number of contexts most and least associated with SH. This type
ways, including compromised mental health and in- of contextual variability in the association of particular
creased substance use (e.g., Knapp et al. 1997; Reilly demographic and consequence of victimization would
et al. 1986; Benson and Thomson 1982; Paludi et al. not be captured in variable-oriented analyses such as
2006). Based on this analysis, it is likely that responses regression, which does not examine the commonality
to victimization may be linked to the uncomfortable and/ of variables and the combination of variables among
or intimidating environment commonly associated with incidents.
non-contact SH (in contrast to CSV). Additionally, we Overall, this analysis provides insight for college
caution against the interpretation that the more frequently campus officials and service providers regarding the
noted disruptive consequences associated with incidents lived experiences of their students. Most importantly,
of CSV, in comparison to incidents of non-contact SH, these findings indicate that both non-contact SH and
suggest that non-contact SH does not result in any harm CSV occur among college-enrolled women and men.
to the individual. The common occurrence of an intimi- This research also confirms that both non-contact SH
dating and/or uncomfortable environment at the college and CSV have negative consequences for the victim.
is cause enough for concern, as any form of SH, includ- The belief that non-contact SH is not serious and does
ing non-contact SH, is strictly prohibited under Title IX. not have the potential to compromise individual func-
Additionally, the general lack of formal disclosure (in- tioning is erroneous and damaging. Students who feel
cluding police, counselors or other health experts) in intimidated or uncomfortable on their campus may drop
contrast to informal disclosure to family and friends out or not perform well academically (see Mengo and
may suggest that while students are significantly both- Black 2016). Moreover, stress is continually cited as an
ered by their victimization, there is likely a feeling that academic impediment for college students (ACHA
nothing can be or will be done to ameliorate the problem 2017), and incidents of non-contact SH that create an
by formal authorities and health providers. Such a toler- intimidating and uncomfortable environment certainly do
ance or acceptance of the status quo for non-contact SH nothing to reduce a students’ stress level and may argu-
is unacceptable. Furthermore, the rarity of formal disclo- ably increase student stress levels. As such, the sum of
sure may reflect the misunderstanding and mispercep- this research provides guidance for how college officials
tions about the gravity of non-contact SH and/or behav- might address and respond to non-contact SH and CSV.
iors that constitute any type of SH. Again, this misun- For one, prevention programming aimed at preventing
derstanding can be addressed and should not be a con- all forms of sexual victimization among college students
tinued reason for precluding help-seeking. Finally, it is should be required for all students and these programs
possible the revictimization through formal disclosure should include a component on non-contact SH and
(by having to relive the experience when disclosing and how this behavior is not and will not be condoned or
then experiencing substantial questioning incurred after tolerated. Moreover, information should be clearly pro-
formal disclosure) may be a concern of victims, imped- vided to all students regarding their rights under Title
ing their formal-help seeking behavior. Nonetheless, if IX, and these efforts should be reinforced with policies
college and outside victim service providers are not enforced at the institutional level in line with Title IX.
learning about experiences of non-contact SH experi- In general, colleges should do a better job of educating
enced by students (or CSV experiences for that matter) all college community members (i.e., students, staff and
because of lack of formal disclosure, it limits their ability faculty) about non-contact SH, especially, with the goal
to potentially assist student victims and address service of increasing awareness, prevention, and openness to
J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618 615

advocacy seeking. For the college community at large, Future Research


this could be accomplished through mandatory SH train-
ing addressing non-contact and contact SH for all Future research can build upon this study in multiple
employees. ways. As mentioned previously, this study did not exam-
ine and compare the contextual differences between ex-
periences of college students who experienced more than
Limitations one form of sexual victimization and those who experi-
enced only one form. Therefore, future research should
Although we believe this study extends the literature in many consider exploring these differences, as it may impact
ways, there are some limitations. First, the response rates reporting, outcomes, and consequences associated with
across the 24 public colleges – while varied – were low over- victimization experiences.
all. Therefore, we run the risk that students who did not re- Future research should also consider exploring the
spond to the survey have different experiences (and ultimately victim-offender dyad and relationship dynamics for both
incidents of victimization) than those who did. Unfortunately, non-contact SH and CSV. Our decision to exclude these
we have no way to speak to this potential sample bias. variables from this study stemmed from space limita-
Similarly, the data used by this study were only collected from tions. Nevertheless, it is possible that the victim-
undergraduate students at public, 4-year colleges. As a result, offender relationship may be differently associated with
graduate students are excluded, as are students enrolled in varying situational contexts. For example, prior research
community or technical colleges. Although research suggests on college student SH experiences heavily focused on
that undergraduate students are more likely to experience the power imbalances involving female students and male
types of victimization under study here, research also tells us faculty members (e.g., see Rubin and Borgers 1990 for
that some victimization experiences of graduate students may a review). It would be worthwhile for future research to
be different in nature (situational context; see Cantor et al. explore whether the traumatic consequences and post-
2015). Furthermore, it is possible that students in private in- incident feelings and behaviors of non-contact SH are
stitutions of higher education or those enrolled in non-four dependent upon the victim-offender relationship.
year institutions of higher education may have different expe-
riences as well (Cantor et al. 2015).
This study also only examines those students who experi-
ence exclusively non-contact SH or CSV; it excludes those Conclusion
students who experienced both types of sexual victimization
and students who did not experience either. Although our It is clear that a nontrivial number of students experience
approach in this study was purposeful to best isolate non- a range of sexual victimizations, including non-contact
contact SH contexts, it would also be fruitful to see the context SH and CSV. A comprehensive approach to student
surrounding other victimization (or lack thereof) experiences. well-being should include education and awareness that
For example, Kilpatrick et al. (2007) reported differences in any type of sexual misconduct is not tolerated and all
outcomes for college students who had and had not experi- types lead to negative outcomes. Students should be edu-
enced specific types of rape. cated on the college’s formal grievance procedures and
Lastly, this study only used data from the 2016–2017 encouraged to not accept these behaviors. This may or
academic year, and thus, only examined one cohort of may not include encouraging students to report incidents
college students. As a result, the findings presented here of sexual victimization and certainly includes providing
must be interpreted within the historical context in resources for victims to buffer the ill effects of sexual
which these victimizations occurred. For example, some victimization. Although the risk of secondary victimiza-
of these experiences occurred during the beginning of tion as a result of reporting cannot, unfortunately, be re-
the #MeToo13 and Time’s Up movements which may moved completely, persons working on campus should be
have brought greater weight and consideration to non- trained to understand the motivations for and barriers
contact SH and forms of CSV. against reporting, and respond to such situations in a sup-
portive, trauma-informed, and victim-centered approach.
As additional work in this area is conducted, a greater
13 knowledge base will be developed that can assist with
We recognize that MeToo began long before 2016/17 with the work of Tarana
Burke in 2007. Of special interest in terms of data collection is the large attention prevention and response approaches to addressing all
given to hashtag MeToo which commenced in October of 2017. forms of sexual violence including SH and CSV.
616 J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

Appendix. Sexual Victimization Items

Non-contact Sexual Harassment (1 = responded at least one time to any item; 0 = responded 0 times to all items)
1. Since Fall (YEAR) while you were a student at (SCHOOL), how many times has a student or someone employed by or otherwise associated with
(SCHOOL)
a. Made sexual remarks or told jokes or stories that were insulting or offensive (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
b. Made inappropriate or offense comments about your or somebody else’s body, appearance or sexual activities (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more
than 10 times)
c. Emailed, texted, tweeted, phoned, or instant messaged offensive sexual remarks, jokes, stories, pictures or videos that you did not want (Responses: 0,
1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
d. Harassed, insulted, threatened or intimidated you because they thought you might be gay, lesbian, bisexual or transgender (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5,
6–10, more than 10 times)
Contact sexual victimization (1 = responded at least one time to any item; 0 = responded no to all items)
1. Since Fall (YEAR) while you were a student at (SCHOOL) has someone used physical force or threats of physical force to make you do the following
things:
a. Sexual penetration (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
b. Oral sex (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
2. Since Fall (YEAR) while you were a student at (SCHOOL) has someone used physical force or threats of physical force in an unsuccessful attempt to
make you do the following
a. Sexual penetration (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
b. Oral sex (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
3. Since Fall (YEAR) while you were a student at (SCHOOL), have either of the following happened to you while you were unable to consent or stop
what was happening because you were passed out, asleep or incapacitated due to drugs or alcohol
a. Sexual penetration (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
b. Oral sex (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
4. Since Fall (YEAR) while you have been attending (SCHOOL) has someone had contact with you involving penetration or oral sex by threatening
non-physical harm or promising rewards such that you felt you must comply (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
5. Since Fall (YEAR) while you have been attending (SCHOOL) has someone had contact with you involving penetration or oral sex without your
active, ongoing voluntary consent? (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10, more than 10 times)
6. Since Fall (YEAR) while you were a student at (SCHOOL), how many times has a student or someone employed by or otherwise associated with
(SCHOOL)
a. Physically hurt you (including forced sex) because they thought you might be gay, lesbian, bisexual or transgender (Responses: 0, 1, 2, 3–5, 6–10,
more than 10 times)

Cammaert, L. P. (1985). How widespread is sexual harassment on cam-


pus. International Journal of Women's Studies, 8(4), 388–397.
References
Cantor, D., Fisher, B., Chibnall, S., Townsend, R., Lee, H., Bruce, C., &
Thomas, G. (2015). Report on the AAU campus climate survey on
Adams, J. W., Kottke, J. L., & Padgitt, J. S. (1983). Sexual harassment of sexual assault and sexual misconduct. Rockville: Westat.
university students. Journal of College Student Personnel, 24(6),
Castillo, Y., Muscarella, F., & Szuchman, L. T. (2011). Gender differ-
484–490.
ences in college students’ perceptions of same-sex sexual harass-
American College Health Association. (2017). Healthy Campus 2020. ment: The influence of physical attractiveness and attitudes toward
Retrieved October 2018. https://www.acha.org/app_themes/ lesbians and gay men. Journal of College Student Development,
HC2020/.../HC2020_Student_Objectives.doc. Accessed 3 Mar 2019 52(5), 511–522.
American Psychological Association. (1993). In the supreme court Chiodo, D., Wolfe, D. A., Crooks, C., Hughes, R., & Jaffe, P. (2009).
of the United States: Teresa Harris v. forklift systems, Inc.: Impact of sexual harassment victimization by peers on subsequent
Brief for amicus curiae American Psychological Association adolescent victimization and adjustment: A longitudinal study.
in support of neither part. Washington DC. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45(3), 246–252.
Belknap, J., & Sharma, N. (2014). The significant frequency and impact Coker, A. L., Follingstad, D. R., Bush, H. M., Fisher, B. S. (2016). Are
of stealth (nonviolent) gender-based abuse among college women. interpersonal violence rates higher among young women in college
Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 15(3), 181–190. compared with those never attending college? Journal of
Benson, D. J., & Thomson, G. E. (1982). Sexual harassment on a Interpersonal Violence, 31(8), 1413-1429.
university campus: The confluence of authority relations, sex- Cortina, L. M., & Wasti, S. A. (2005). Profiles in coping: Responses to
ual interest and gender stratification. Social Problems, 29(3), sexual harassment across persons, organizations, and cultures.
236–251. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 182–192.
Blumenthal, J. A. (1998). The reasonable woman standard: A meta- Decker, J. F., & Baroni, P. G. (2011). “No” still means “yes”: The failure
analytic review of gender differences in perceptions of sexual ha- of the “non-consent” reform movement in American rape and sexual
rassment. Law and Human Behavior, 22(1), 33–57.
J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618 617

assault law. The Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 101(4), Koss, M. P. (2018). Hidden rape: Sexual aggression and victimization in a
1081–1169. national sample of students in higher education. In Rape and society
Fedina, L., Holmes, J. L., & Backes, B. L. (2018). Campus sexual assault: (pp. 35-49). Routledge.
A systematic review of prevalence research from 2000 to 2015. Leone, J. M., & Carroll, J. M. (2017). Victimization and suicidality
Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 19(1), 76–93. among female college students. Journal of American College
Fisher, B.S., Cullen, F.T., & Turner, M.G. (2000). The Sexual Health, 64(6), 421–428.
Victimization of College Women U.S. Department of Justice, Lott, B., Reilly, M. E., & Howard, D. R. (1982). Sexual assault and
National Institute of Justice. harassment: A campus community case study. Signs: Journal of
Fisher, B. S., Daigle, L. E., Cullen, F. T., & Turner, M. G. (2003). Women in Culture and Society, 8(2), 296–319.
Reporting sexual victimization to the police and others: Results from Maihoff, N., & Forrest, L. (1983). Sexual harassment in higher education:
a national-level study of college women. Criminal Justice and An assessment study. Journal of the National Association for
Behavior, 30(1), 6–38. Women Deans, Administrators, and Counselors, 46(2), 3–8.
Fisher, B. S., Daigle, L. E., & Cullen, F. T. (2010). Unsafe in the ivory Malovich, N. J., & Stake, J. E. (1990). Sexual harassment on campus:
tower: The sexual victimization of college women. Thousand Oaks: Individual differences in attitudes and beliefs. Psychology of Women
Sage Publications. Quarterly, 14(1), 63–81.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Gelfand, M. J., & Drasgow, F. (1995). Measuring sexual Marks, M. A., & Nelson, E. S. (1993). Sexual harassment on campus:
harassment: Theoretical and psychometric advances. Basic and Effects of professor gender on perception of sexually harassing be-
Applied Social Psychology, 17(4), 425-445. haviors. Sex Roles, 28(3–4), 207–217.
Fitzgerald, L. F., & Ormerod, A. J. (1991). Perceptions of sexual harass- Mazer, D. B., & Percival, E. F. (1989). Ideology or experience? The
ment: The influence of gender and academic context. Psychology of relationships among perceptions, attitudes, and experiences of sex-
Women Quarterly, 15, 281–294. ual harassment in university students. Sex Roles, 20(3–4), 135–147.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Shullman, S. L., Bailey, N., Richards, M., Swecker, J., McCormack, A. (1985). The sexual harassment of students by teachers:
Gold, Y., Ormerod, M., & Weitzman, L. (1988). The incidence and The case of students in science. Sex Roles, 13(1–2), 21–32.
dimensions of sexual harassment in academia and the workplace. McGinley, M., Wolff, J. M., Rospenda, K. M., Liu, L., & Richman, J. A.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32, 152–175. (2016). Risk factors and outcomes of chronic sexual harassment
Gervasio, A. H., & Ruckdeschel, K. (1992). College Students' judgments during the transition to college: Examination of a two-part growth
of verbal sexual harassment 1. Journal of Applied Social mixture model. Social Science Research, 60, 297–310.
Psychology, 22(3), 190–211. Mengo, C., & Black, B. M. (2016). Violence victimization on a college
Gruber, J. E. (1992). A typology of personal and environmental sexual campus: Impact on GPA and school dropout. Journal of College
harassment: Research and policy implications for the 1990s. Sex Student Retention: Research, Theory & Practice, 18(2), 234-248.
Roles, 26(11/12), 447–464. Metha, A., & Nigg, J. (1982). Sexual harassment: Implications of a study at
Hart, T. C., & Miethe, T. D. (2008). Exploring bystander presence and Arizona State University. Women's Studies Quarterly, 10(2), 24–26.
intervention in nonfatal violence victimization: When does helping Miethe, T. D., & Regoeczi, W. C. (2004). Rethinking homicide: Exploring
really help? Violence and Victims, 23, 637–651. the structure and process underlying deadly situations. New York:
Hill, C., & Silva, E. (2005). Drawing the line: Sexual harassment on Cambridge University Press.
campus. Washington DC: American Association of University Miethe, T. D., Hart, T. C., & Regoeczi, W. C. (2008). The conjunctive
Women Educational Foundation. analysis of case configurations: An exploratory method for discrete
Ho, I. K., Dinh, K. T., Bellefontaine, S. A., & Irving, A. L. (2012). Sexual multivariate analyses of crime data. Journal of Quantitative
harassment and posttraumatic stress symptoms among Asian and Criminology, 24, 227–241.
white women. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, Morgan, R. E., & Kena, G. (2017). Criminal victimization (p. 2016).
21(1), 95–113. Washington DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics.
Jordan, C., Combs, J., & Smith, G. (2014). An exploration of sexual Paludi, M., Nydegger, R., DeSouza, E., Nydegger, L., & Dicker, K. A.
victimization and academic performance among college women. (2006). International perspectives on sexual harassment of college
Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 15(3), 191–200. students: The sounds of silence. Annals of the New York Academy of
Kelly, L. (1987). The continuum of sexual violence. In J. Hanmer & M. Sciences, 1087(1), 103–120.
Maynard (Eds.), Women, violence and social control. Explorations Petersen, J. L., & Hyde, J. S. (2009). A longitudinal investigation of peer
in sociology (British sociological association conference volume sexual harassment victimization in adolescence. Journal of
series). London: Palgrave Macmillan. Adolescence, 32(5), 1173–1188.
Kilpatrick, D.G., Resnick, H.S., Ruggiero, K.J., Conoscenti, L.M., & Ragin, C. (1987). The Comparative Method. Berkeley: The University of
McCauley, J. (2007). Drug-facilitated, incapacitated, and forcible California Press.
rape: A national study. Reilly, M. E., Lott, B., & Gallogly, S. M. (1986). Sexual harassment of
Knapp, D. E., Faley, R. H., Ekeberg, S. E., & Dubois, C. L. (1997). university students. Sex Roles, 15(7–8), 333–358.
Determinants of target responses to sexual harassment: A conceptual Rotundo, M., Nguyen, D. H., & Sackett, P. R. (2001). A meta-analytic
framework. Academy of Management Review, 22(3), 687–729. review of gender differences in perceptions of sexual harassment.
Krebs, C. P., Lindquist, C. H., Warner, T. D., Fisher, B. S., & Martin, S. L. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(5), 914–922.
(2007). The campus sexual assault (CSA) study. Washington, DC: Rubin, L. J., & Borgers, S. B. (1990). Sexual harassment in universities
National Institute of Justice. during the 1980s. Sex Roles, 23(7), 397–411.
Krebs, C. P., Lindquist, C. H., Berzofsky, M., Shook-Sa, B., & Peterson, Sabina, C., & Ho, L. Y. (2014). Campus and college victim responses to
K. (2016). Campus Climate Survey Validation Study: Final sexual assault and dating violence: Disclosure, service utilization,
Technical Report. Bureau of Justice Statistics Research and and service provision. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 15(3), 201–226.
Development Series. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics. Stoner, J. E., & Cramer, R. J. (2017). Sexual violence victimization
Kearney, L.K., & Gilbert, L.A. (2012). The role of ethnicity in Mexican among college females: A systematic review of rates, barriers, and
American and non-Hispanic White students' experience of sexual facilitators of health service utilization on campus. Trauma,
harassment. Hispanic Journal of Behavoral Sciences, 34(4), 507- Violence, & Abuse. Advanced online publication. https://doi.org/
524. 10.1177/1524838017721245.
618 J Fam Viol (2020) 35:603–618

Thompson, M. P., & Kingree, J. B. (2010). Sexual victimization, negative Yoon, E., Stiller Funk, R., & Kropf, N. P. (2010). Sexual harassment
cognitions, and adjustment in college women. American Journal of experiences and their psychological correlates among a diverse sam-
Health Behavior, 34(1), 54–59. ple of college women. Affilia, 25(1), 8–18.
Till, F. J. (1980). Sexual Harassment. A Report on the Sexual Harassment
of Students. Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (2006). Extent, nature, and consequences of jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
rape victimization: Findings from the National Violence against
Women Survey. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice.
Turchik, J. A., & Hassija, C. M. (2014). Female sexual victimization
among college students: Assault severity, health risk behaviors,
and sexual functioning. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 29(13),
2439–2457.
Reproduced with permission of copyright owner. Further reproduction
prohibited without permission.

You might also like