Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Research in Personality


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp

Conceptual ambiguities and measurement issues in sensory


processing sensitivity
Susan Hellwig a, *, Marcus Roth b
a
Department of Psychology, University of Wuppertal, Germany
b
Department of Psychology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: The aim of the article is (1) to critically discuss Sensory Processing Sensitivity (SPS) in light of personality
Sensory processing sensitivity theories and (2) to examine the validity of the only available measure of SPS, the Highly Sensitive Person Scale
Highly sensitive person (HSPS). Study 1 (N = 289) examines the relations of the HSPS with facets of the Big Five. In Study 2 (N = 226),
Personality traits
the HSPS’ relation to emotion recognition ability is estimated after controlling for the Big Five. In contrast to
Emotion recognition ability
previous studies, these relationships are estimated at the level of latent variables. Our data suggest that SPS
highly overlaps with established personality traits, and its relation with emotion recognition ability can be fully
explained by Neuroticism and Openness to Experience.

1. Conceptual ambiguities and measurement issues in sensory personality traits (Aron & Aron, 1997), whose underlying processes are
processing sensitivity still not well understood (e.g., DeYoung & Gray, 2009).
A major problem with SPS lies in the vagueness of its definition.
SPS was introduced in a Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Currently, SPS is described with heterogeneous concepts like respon­
article by Aron and Aron (1997) as a new construct which is related to siveness to the environment (Lionetti et al., 2018) or increased
but also distinguishable from the personality trait of Introversion. In emotional reactivity (Greven et al., 2019), which make it difficult to
recent years, SPS has attracted increasing attention in the scientific grasp how SPS differs from already established personality traits –
literature (see Aron et al., 2012; Lionetti et al., 2018) but has also especially from Neuroticism and Introversion (as described below). In
become a popular concept outside of the scientific literature under the addition, conceptualizations of SPS are somewhat contradictory: There
term “high sensitivity” (e.g., Aron, 1996; Falkenstein, 2019). Today, are conflicting statements as to whether SPS falls within the relatively
there are a vast number of popular scientific publications on SPS, distinct domains of ability constructs (Greven et al., 2019; Lionetti et al.,
including self-help manuals and guidebooks for parents or teachers of 2018) or personality constructs (in the sense of behavioral styles) (Aron
highly sensitive children (e.g., Aron, 2002; 2020; Rosenshein, 2013). & Aron, 1997; Greven et al., 2019). In this paper, we will argue that due
The high popularity could be due to the positive reinterpretation of to the rather vague definitions and contradictory descriptions of SPS as a
certain personality traits that are often perceived as rather socially un­ personality trait and an ability construct, current statements about SPS
desirable, as we will discuss later. In the scientific literature, SPS has are hardly falsifiable. Furthermore, the only current measure of SPS in
been suggested to play an important role in empathy (Acevedo et al., adults – the HSPS (Aron & Aron, 1997) – shows high content overlap
2014) and creativity (Bridges & Schendan, 2019) or as a predictor of with established measures of the Big Five personality domains. Thus, we
subjective well-being (Sobocko & Zelenski, 2015) or work-related stress will argue that the current measurement approach appears inappro­
(Evers et al., 2008). Recently, the HSPS (Aron & Aron, 1997), a self- priate (a) to assess a new personality construct that is distinguishable
report measure for SPS, has been adapted into several languages (e.g., from established personality factors and (b) to measure an ability
Grimen & Diseth, 2016; Konrad & Herzberg, 2019; Montoya-Pérez et al., construct.
2019; Şengül-İnal & Sümer, 2020; Slagt et al., 2018; Ueno et al., 2019). Therefore, the aim of our article is twofold: Firstly, we critically
From the perspective of personality research, SPS is a highly appealing discuss serious issues concerning the concept and measurement of SPS.
construct because it is proposed to partially underly established Particularly, we address the question of whether SPS can be

* Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Wuppertal, Gaußstraße 20, 42119 Wuppertal, Germany.
E-mail address: hellwig@uni-wuppertal.de (S. Hellwig).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104130
Received 26 February 2021; Received in revised form 1 July 2021; Accepted 5 July 2021
Available online 12 July 2021
0092-6566/© 2021 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

differentiated from established personality factors within the Big Five a central element of SPS (see Aron et al., 2012, p. 262). On the other
framework. Secondly, we evaluate validity evidence for interpretation hand, Aron et al. (2012 , p. 263) refer to the finding that individual
of test scores on the only available measure of SPS, the HSPS. Consid­ differences in shyness can be observed not only in humans but also in
ering the high conceptual overlap between SPS and factors of the Big nonhuman animals like the pumpkinseed sunfish. This finding is pro­
Five domain (particularly Neuroticism, Introversion, and Openness to vided as preliminary evidence for SPS as a meta-personality trait that
Experience), we question whether the HSPS measures an ability to exists in different species (Aron et al., 2012). If deeper processing on a
process environmental stimuli or a personality trait that is distinct from semantic level is indeed a defining characteristic of SPS, it seems ques­
the Big Five. In Study 1, we focus on the relations between the HSPS and tionable whether the observation of behavioral differences in shyness in
the Big Five personality traits on the latent variable level in terms of fish (for which few cognitive processes are known) can simultaneously
discriminant validity evidence. In Study 2, we test the claim that SPS as be viewed as evidence for SPS as a fundamental personality trait. If,
assessed by the HSPS represents an ability construct related to a more instead, SPS is proposed as a fundamental trait across different species,
accurate processing of external stimuli. inhibited behavior (or shyness) appears to be the defining characteristic
of SPS, whereas depth of processing appears to be a potential outcome in
1.1. The concept of sensory processing sensitivity humans (or species with comparable cognitive processes).
Another ambiguity relates to the question of whether SPS represents
SPS was conceptualized by Aron and Aron (1997) as a “fundamental a continuous trait dimension or a category (see Greven et al., 2019). In
individual difference” (p. 347) that affects the way “sensory information most conceptualizations of SPS, the construct is described as a contin­
is transmitted to or processed in the brain” (Aron & Aron, 1997, p. 347). uous dimension from low to high sensory sensitivity (e.g., Aron & Aron,
Aron and Aron (1997) suggested that this trait underlies dispositional 1997; Aron et al., 2012; Lionetti et al., 2018). Nevertheless, SPS is also
differences in the way persons typically react to novel stimuli: with often described as a categorical trait differentiating highly sensitive
either exploration or quiet vigilance. SPS is considered fundamental in persons from non-highly sensitive persons (Aron et al., 2012; Aron,
two ways: it is proposed (1) to underly global personality traits (like 1996). In some cases, a categorical description of SPS might be used for
Introversion) and (2) to be present not only in humans but also in other simplicity’s sake, especially when the concept is presented to a broader
animal species (Aron & Aron, 1997; Aron et al., 2012; Greven et al., audience in the popular scientific literature (see Aron, 1996). For
2019). A more recent conceptualization integrates SPS into a broader example, Aron et al. (2012) argue that SPS is probably a continuous trait
theory of environmental sensitivity (Greven et al., 2019; Pluess, 2015). but more easily described in a categorical sense as responding either
Environmental sensitivity represents the “ability to register, process, and more or less to the environment. However, some statements made about
respond to external” stimuli (Pluess, 2015, p. 138). In this framework, SPS even contradict the assumption of a continuous trait: For example, it
SPS is specified as a “temperament trait characterized by greater depth is often stated that about 15–20% of the population can be considered
of information processing, increased emotional reactivity and empathy, high on SPS (e.g. Greven et al., 2019; Konrad & Herzberg, 2019), that
greater awareness of environmental subtleties, and ease of over­ SPS can be found in roughly 20% of humans (Acevedo et al., 2014), or
stimulation” (Greven et al., 2019, p. 288). By using the term “greater that about 30% of the population score high on SPS (Lionetti et al.,
depth of information processing”, Aron et al. (2012) refer to Craik and 2019). Assuming a continuous trait dimension, those estimates would be
Lockard (1972) classical model of levels of processing in memory meaningless or at least arbitrary as the cut-off separating highly sensi­
research. In this model, stimuli are processed at different levels of tive persons from other individuals could be set at any point. Thus, es­
analysis, ranging from rather shallow sensory analyses to deeper se­ timates that a certain percentage of individuals have high levels of SPS
mantic analyses (Lockhart & Craik, 1990). Analogously, persons with imply a categorical trait. Indeed, Aron and Aron (1997) based their
high levels of SPS are expected to process environmental stimuli more initial estimate of about 20% highly sensitive persons from a series of
deeply by comparing them with past experiences of comparable stimuli studies on infants’ temperament (for an overview see Kagan, 1994) in
(Aron et al., 2012). which infants were categorized as either behaviorally inhibited or un­
inhibited. Thus, the estimate is based on a categorically defined trait
1.2. Conceptual issues with sensory processing sensitivity from the domain of infant temperament. It is questionable whether these
findings can be transferred to the new concept of SPS. And if so, how
There are some serious issues with the SPS concept. The term SPS is does SPS differ from the behavior inhibition trait proposed by Kagan (e.
still rather loosely and somewhat inconsistently defined. For example, g. 1994)?
the statement that SPS concerns how “sensory information is trans­ Moreover, some assertions about SPS seem too far-reaching given the
mitted to or processed in the brain” (Aron & Aron, 1997, p. 347) is quite actual findings. For example, Lionetti et al. (2018) provided evidence for
vague. Since many psychological constructs are related to the processing three classes of individuals using latent class analysis to explain response
of sensory stimuli to various degrees (e.g., diverse forms of cognitive patterns to the HSPS. It should be noted that these three classes differed
abilities, like visual and auditive intelligence, McGrew, 2009; person­ not only with respect to their mean HSPS scores but also in their scores
ality traits, DeYoung & Gray, 2009), how SPS uniquely influences the on the personality traits Neuroticism and Extraversion. These three
processing of sensory stimuli is an open question. As discussed in the classes have then been interpreted using the rather unconventional
previous section, several heterogenous concepts (i.e. depth of process­ metaphor of flowers (see Greven et al., 2019; Lionetti et al., 2018):
ing, emotional reactivity, empathy) have been used to further charac­ “dandelions” (i.e. low sensitivity), “tulips” (medium sensitivity), and
terize SPS (Greven et al., 2019). Lacking a more precise definition of “orchids” (i.e. high sensitivity). The flower metaphor is used to express
SPS, it remains unclear why these concepts are assumed to be connected differences between these groups in their resilience versus vulnerability
to each other and how SPS differs from them in a unique way. A mere to negative environments: the dandelion group is considered the least
description through related concepts (like emotional reactivity or vulnerable and the orchid group the most vulnerable (Ellis & Boyce,
empathy) does not seem a sufficient substitute for a clearer definition of 2008; Lionetti et al., 2018). Since Vulnerability is one facet of Neuroti­
SPS. cism (e.g., Costa et al., 1991), this interpretation is in line with the
In addition, given the rather vague definition of SPS, it is still an open finding that higher levels of Neuroticism can be found in the group of
question which of the heterogenous concepts used to describe SPS are orchids. At the same time, it is claimed that the highly sensitive group
viewed as definitional elements of the construct and which are potential does “exceptionally well in ideal conditions” (Lionetti et al., 2018, p. 2)
outcomes (e.g. empathy, emotional reactivity, or depth of processing). which cannot be directly inferred from the given findings. Thus, it is not
To give an example, on the one hand, depth of processing in the sense of clear in what way the high sensitivity group is exceptional besides their
Craik and Lockard’s model from memory research has been suggested as relative high scores on Neuroticism.

2
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

The rather vague definitions of SPS create potential issues for the demands; e.g., “Do you get rattled when you have a lot to do in a short
measurement of SPS and the concept’s theoretical integrations into a amount of time?”), Aesthetic Sensitivity (AES: awareness of aesthetics;
nomological network (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). In particular, it re­ e.g., “Do you have a rich, complex inner life?”), and Low Sensory
mains unclear whether SPS is viewed as a concept from the relatively Threshold (LST: unpleasant sensory arousal; e.g., “Are you bothered by
distinct domains of personality traits versus abilities. In the following intense stimuli, like loud noises or chaotic scenes?”). Model comparisons
section, we discuss these problems in more detail. revealed that the three-factor model fit significantly better than the one-
factor model. Booth et al. (2015), Sobocko and Zelenski (2015), and
1.3. Measurement of SPS Grimen and Diseth (2016) also proposed three-factor solutions similar to
the one by Smolewska et al. (2006) based on inductive approaches like
Aron and Aron (1997) developed the HSPS to provide a first measure exploratory factor analysis or principal component analysis.
of SPS. To the best of our knowledge, the HSPS is currently the only Nevertheless, the three-factor model is not undisputed: Evans and
measurement instrument for assessing SPS in adults. The HSPS is a 27- Rothbart (2008) suggested a two-factor model based on the fact that
item self-report instrument that includes statements about typical re­ EOE and LST were highly intercorrelated (0.73) in the study by Smo­
actions to specific environmental stimuli (e.g. “Do you become un­ lewska et al. (2006). Evans and Rothbart (2008) conceptually integrated
pleasantly aroused when a lot is going on around you?”) as well as more SPS into the model of adult temperament presented by Derryberry and
general personality-related descriptions (e.g. “Are you conscientious?”) Rothbart (1988). They noticed similarities between items of the HSPS
(Aron & Aron, 1997, p. 352). The HSPS items were developed on the (mainly EOE and LST) and “Negative Affect” factor in the adult
basis of qualitative interviews. An inductive approach was used with the temperament model (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1988). The remaining
aim of comprehensively identifying characteristics of individuals with items of the HSPS (mainly AES) appeared to be similar to the factor
high levels of SPS (Aron & Aron, 1997). Participants who described “Orienting Sensitivity”, which was found more recently by Evans and
themselves as “highly sensitive” were invited to these interviews, Rothbart (2007). Based on an exploratory factor analysis with a sample
defined more narrowly by the authors as “either highly introverted […] of N = 297 undergraduates, Evans and Rothbart (2008; see also Rinn
or easily overwhelmed by situations” (Aron & Aron, 1997, p. 359). et al., 2018) found evidence for two factors. The two-factor solution and
a three-factor model similar to the one by Smolewska et al. (2006) were
1.4. Issues concerning the measurement of SPS both superior to the one-factor model. However, differences between the
two- and three-factor model were not tested, as these two models were
The HSPS items appear to be very similar (and partly identical) to not nested due to slightly different subsets of items included (Evans &
items from standard measures of the Big Five personality traits (see e.g., Rothbart, 2008). Evans and Rothbart (2008) emphasized that their
Goldberg et al., 2006). The similarity is particularly apparent for the “reason for preferring the two-factor solution is primary conceptual,
factors Neuroticism (e.g. “Do you startle easily?”), Introversion (e.g. rather than statistical” (p. 115).
“Does your nervous system sometimes feel so frazzled that you just have In contrast, Liss et al. (2008) compared a three-factor model of the
to get off by yourself?”), Openness to Experience (e.g. “Are you deeply HSPS with a two-factor model (in which the EOE and LST items are
moved by arts or music?”), and Conscientiousness (“Are you conscien­ collapsed into one factor). Although both models had adequate fit, the
tious?”). The high content overlap with established personality factors is three-factor solution fit significantly better. Furthermore, some studies
not surprising, considering that the definition of SPS used in the item found different relations between the three factors and external vari­
generation process included terms like “introverted” or “easily over­ ables, such as the Big Five personality traits (Smolewska et al., 2006),
whelmed” (i.e. Neuroticism). Furthermore, given this rather broad Anxiety or depressive symptoms (Liss et al., 2008). EOE and LST exhibit
definition of SPS, participants may also have reported characteristics more similar patterns of relations to external variables compared to AES
that are unrelated to the construct but typical for this specific group of (Liss et al., 2008).
interviewees. To give an example, the fact that Aron and Aron (1997) Aron et al. (2012) also reported finding a three-factor solution
conducted interviews with participants recruited from a local arts as­ comparable to the one by Smolewska et al. (2006). However, the authors
sociation may have resulted in the inclusion of some items from the questioned the interpretability of previous findings on factor structure
Openness to Experience factor, since this trait is positively associated (Aron et al., 2012). In particular, they disputed the appropriateness of
with creativity (McCrae, 1987) and higher levels are typical among the factor analytic methods used given the potential non-normality of
artists (Feist, 1998). the data.
Given the similarity of the HSPS to different personality factors, we Strong evidence for a three-dimensional structure was provided by
doubt that the HSPS measures (a) a single construct that (b) underlies Konrad and Herzberg (2019) who took the problem of non-normality of
but is separable from established personality factors of the Big Five the data into account. The authors compared the one-, two-, and three-
domain, and (c) that this measure appropriately assesses the ability to factor solutions in a sample of 3,588 individuals for a German adaption
process sensory stimuli. In the following sections, we discuss these three of the HSPS. The two- and three-factor structures were derived from one
problems in more detail. half of the sample and tested in the other half using a robust estimator.
The results clearly supported a three-factor structure. We therefore use
1.5. Dimensionality of the HSPS this three-factor structure of the HSPS in all further analyses.

While Aron and Aron (1997) initially intended to design a one- 1.6. Relations between SPS and established personality factors
dimensional measure of SPS, subsequent studies of the HSPS tend to
indicate a multi-dimensional structure. Smolewska et al. (2006) were Since Aron and Aron (1997) introduced the concept of SPS, one of
the first to raise doubts about the one-dimensionality of the HSPS. From the main objectives has been to distinguish SPS from established per­
their point of view, both the rather low factor loadings of some items sonality traits, particularly Extraversion and Neuroticism. With respect
reported by Aron and Aron (1997) as well as the item content contradict to Extraversion, the authors referred to different personality models,
the assumption of a single SPS factor. In their own study, which applied including the arousal theory of Extraversion-Introversion by Eysenck
first a principal component analysis to explore the structure of the HSPS (1967), the Behavioral Inhibition and Activation System by Gray (1981)
in one part of the sample (n = 380) and a subsequent confirmatory factor as well as conceptual work on Shyness (e.g., Asendorpf, 1989). Despite a
analysis (CFA) to confirm this structure in the other part of the sample (n comprehensive discussion of these theories (Aron & Aron, 1997), inte­
= 442), Smolewska et al. (2006) found evidence for three separate gration of the new concept into a nomological network of current per­
factors, which they labelled: Ease of Excitation (EOE: feelings of sonality theories remains difficult. On the one hand, Aron and Aron

3
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

(1997) argue that previous personality models reflect a common only six to eight studies provided effect sizes for the relations of interest,
fundamental trait, namely SPS. In this vein, SPS appears to be an there are two potential limitations: first, the effect sizes summarized in
alternative explanation to previous theories. On the other hand, the these studies might be attenuated by measurement error, and second,
authors argue that SPS partly underlies Introversion. In particular, they the relations with the three SPS factors might differ on the facet level.
propose that two types of Introversion are distinguishable: introverted
behavior caused by high sensitivity and introverted behavior caused by 1.7. Relations between SPS and ability constructs
avoidant attachment styles. This theoretical assumption is weakened by
the statement there are individuals who are “sensitive, but also sociable” Although SPS was introduced by Aron and Aron (1997) as a
(p. 349) and that “many introverts are not highly sensitive” (Aron & temperament or personality trait, it has also been related to the domain
Aron, 1997, p. 362). Thus, Introversion is proposed to be sometimes of ability constructs (Greven et al., 2019; Lionetti et al., 2018). Per­
caused by SPS, yet SPS can also be high in individuals who are extra­ sonality traits and abilities can be viewed as relatively distinct domains:
verted. Given this hardly falsifiable theoretical statement, it seems cognitive abilities are nearly uncorrelated with the Big Five personality
nearly impossible to deduce predictions about the relationship between traits, with the exception of a moderate positive relation for the factor
SPS and the personality trait Extraversion. Openness to Experience (see Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997). While
A similar problem exists with the respect to the personality trait personality traits are considered to affect a person’s behavioral styles or
Neuroticism (or Emotional Instability). Although Aron and Aron (1997) typical behavior, abilities determine a person’s maximum performance
observed high correlations between SPS and Neuroticism in their data (r (for the distinction between typical and maximum performance see e.g.
= 0.65), they argue that SPS is related but not identical to Neuroticism, Cronbach, 1990). Some concepts that have been used to further char­
since the correlations were not close to 1. Again, they make a hardly acterize SPS – like better detection of subtle visual changes (Jagiellowicz
falsifiable statement about the relations between SPS and the person­ et al., 2011) or higher empathy (Greven et al., 2019) – suggest that SPS is
ality trait Neuroticism: In particular, they argue that SPS can manifest either regarded as an ability construct or is at least expected to be
itself in high negative emotionality (i.e. Neuroticism) in “some sensitive positively associated with certain abilities. Specifically, abilities that can
individuals” (Aron & Aron, 1997, p. 350), but can also result in positive be expected to have positive relationships with SPS according to the
emotionality. latter’s definition include (visual) working memory capacity (with
Another problem relates to the observability of SPS in overt respect to the detection of visual changes, e.g., Johnson et al., 2009) and
behavior. Aron et al. (2012) claimed that the Big Five Model of per­ emotion recognition ability (with respect to empathy, Schlegel et al.,
sonality (e.g. Goldberg, 1990) relies on overtly observable behavior 2019). In their initial attempt to provide a nomological network for SPS,
while neglecting inner states. The same observable behavior might be Aron and Aron (1997) themselves suggested that SPS should be related
caused by different underlying processes or inner states. At the same to “other variables and measures logically involving sensitivity” (p.
time – as already mentioned – the assessment of SPS highly overlaps 350). Aron (1996) clarified which variables and measures in particular
with typical measures of the Big Five domains. It is questionable are meant by this suggestion when she stated that highly sensitive in­
whether a trait’s underlying process can be assessed with items bearing dividuals should perform better on “tasks requiring vigilance, accuracy,
an obvious resemblance to items used to assess the trait itself. speed, and the detection of minor differences” (p. 10). At the same time,
Overall, SPS has been discussed mainly in light of biological models she restrictively notes that highly sensitive persons often cannot
of Extraversion and Neuroticism (see Aron & Aron, 1997; Aron et al., demonstrate their performance when evaluated by others or when timed
2012). Although there is currently substantial consensus regarding the (Aron, 1996). This restriction makes predictions about the relation be­
Big Five personality model (e.g. Goldberg, 1990) or the Five Factor tween SPS and ability measures difficult because it remains ambiguous
Model of personality (Costa & McCrae, 1992) as a useful taxonomy of whether SPS is viewed as an ability that enhances test performance
personality traits (e.g. John et al., 2008), the conceptual similarity of regarding processing of sensory information or rather as a personality
SPS with factors in this model has been rarely discussed (for exceptions trait (obviously similar to test anxiety) that might impede performance
see, Greven et al., 2019; Smolewska et al., 2006). The Big Five taxonomy on ability measures. Lionetti et al. (2018) make a more unambiguous
assumes a hierarchical structure of personality traits. Each of the five statement by explicitly defining SPS as an ability to perceive and process
personality dimensions (i.e. the general factor level) encompasses environmental stimuli. They also propose that individual differences in
several subordinate factors (or facets) that reflect more narrowly reactivity to environmental stimuli are a function of this underlying
described personality traits (e.g. Anxiety, Depression, Assertiveness). ability. If SPS is indeed considered to represent an ability construct, the
Thus, when introducing a new candidate personality trait, it appears current measurement approach appears questionable: On the one hand,
important to discuss the new trait in the context of such comprehensive the appropriateness of HSPS items can be doubted due to their content
models of personality. For example, besides Neuroticism and Extraver­ overlap with established personality factors; on the other hand, the
sion, the factor Openness to Experience has been related to sensory suitability of self-reports for assessing abilities is generally questionable
processes. For example, McCrae (1993) argues that the term “intellect” because self-reports and objective measures of abilities are only
is too narrow for Openness to Experience as the factor also encompasses moderately correlated (Freund & Kasten, 2012).
traits that “emphasize an interest in sensory rather than intellectual Overall, there are only few studies on the relation between SPS as
experience” (p. 84). assessed by the HSPS and ability constructs. Gerstenberg (2012) re­
The relationship between SPS and the Big Five personality traits has ported a positive relation between the HSPS factor LST and accuracy in a
been examined in a few studies. Recently, Lionetti et al. (2019) pre­ visual attention task after statistically controlling for the Big Five per­
sented results from a meta-analysis of studies assessing the associations sonality factors (no relations were found for EOE and AES). Jagiellowicz
between the three SPS factors and the Big Five personality traits. Eight et al. (2011) reported that the HSPS is associated with higher activation
articles (ten samples) were included in their meta-analysis, of which in brain regions involved in processing visual stimuli during a visual
only four articles examined all of the Big Five dimensions. EOE and LST change detection task but unrelated to a measure of accuracy of change
were found to be positively related to Neuroticism (EOE: r = 0.44, LST: r detection. It should be noted that this study was based on a small sample
= 0.27) but unrelated to the other factors (in each case: r ≤ 0.07). AES (N = 12). In a fMRI study, Acevedo et al. (2014) reported associations
was positively related to Openness to Experience (r = 0.36) and between an 11-item version of the HSPS and higher activation in brain
Neuroticism (r = 0.17) and unrelated to Extraversion, Agreeableness, regions related to empathy. Participants were shown pictures of either
and Conscientiousness (in each case: r ≤ 0.08). Based on their meta- their partner or a stranger expressing certain emotions through their
analytic results, Lionetti et al. (2019) concluded that SPS is separable face. Based on this finding, Acevedo et al. (2014) concluded that greater
from the Big Five personality traits. However, alongside the fact that awareness of others’ emotions is a central aspect of SPS. This conclusion

4
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

appears problematic, since inter-individual differences in emotional part in the study for course credit, and of otherwise recruited adults
awareness were actually controlled for by the study design. Specifically, (recruited from the social network of a student involved in the data
for each facial expression, the emotion word as well as the context of the collection process) (n = 85) who were asked to volunteer for this study.
emotion (i.e. the cause of the emotion) were presented to the partici­ Most individuals (n = 261) considered German to be their native lan­
pants as well. Thus, activation might indeed reflect participants’ guage, and all other individuals had spoken German for at least 12 years.
emotional reactions to the emotions of another person, as it was also
assumed by Acevedo et al. (2014), but is unlikely to reflect interindi­ 2.2.2. Power analyses
vidual differences in awareness of these emotion, since the tasks did not We tried to achieve a sample size that seemed sufficient for the
require participants to identify or understand others’ emotions (e.g. following analyses. Without knowing the results, we stopped data
Mayer et al., 2016; Hellwig et al., 2020). collection when the sample size reached the approximate goal of at least
200 individuals, but tried to test as many individuals as possible. (Post
1.8. Overview of the studies hoc) power analyses were conducted to evaluate the appropriateness of
the samples size in each model. Power was estimated with regard to the
The aim of the present paper is to test two central assumptions that standard χ2-test of absolute fit given an effect of RMSEA = 0.10, α = 0.05
have been made about SPS: Firstly, is SPS really a new personality trait (H0: RMSEA = 0) (see Preacher & Coffman, 2006). In all models, the
that can be distinguished from established personality factors? Sec­ achieved power was close to 1. Effect sizes of particular interest in Study
ondly, is SPS an ability to process sensory information? Since currently 1 are the factor correlations among the three factors of SPS (as
only one measurement approach of SPS in adults is available, our in­ convergent validity evidence for the test score interpretations of the
vestigations and conclusions about the construct are based on and HSPS) and the correlations with Neuroticism and Extraversion (as
limited to validity evidence for test score interpretations of this measure. discriminant validity evidence). Power analyses for the factor correla­
Based on our conceptual criticisms of SPS and the criticisms of the HSPS, tions were conducted with Monte Carlo simulation in pwrSEM (Wang &
we doubt that a new personality or ability construct is assessed. Rhemtulla, 2021). Assuming that the standardized loadings are all 0.60,
Two independent studies were conducted: In Study 1, we estimated factor correlations of 0.30 can be detected for the target relationships
the relationships between the HSPS and the Big Five personality traits at with a power of 1 – β = 0.90 to 0.94. Overall, the power analyses suggest
the latent variable level (i.e., discriminant validity evidence). In Study 2, that the sample sizes in Study 1 are adequate.
we focus on the claim that SPS can be viewed as an ability to process
sensory stimuli. In particular, we tested, whether the HSPS is related to 2.2.3. Instruments
emotion recognition ability (e.g. Schlegel et al., 2014) after controlling SPS. The German version of the HSPS was used to assess SPS (see
for the Big Five personality traits (i.e., incremental validity evidence). Konrad & Herzberg, 2019). Each item was rated on a 5-point Likert scale
ranging from “strong disagreement” (1) to “strong agreement” (5). An­
2. Study 1 alyses were based on the three-factor structure suggested by Konrad and
Herzberg (2019): EOE was assessed with 10 items, AES with 5 items, and
2.1. Rationale LST with 11 items.
Big Five. The Big Five personality factors Neuroticism and Extra­
The first study aims to test the claim that SPS assessed by the HSPS is version were assessed on the facet level with the NEO-PI-R (Ostendorf &
separable from established personality factors. Current evidence for the Angleitner, 2004). In the NEO-PI-R, each factor encompasses 6 subor­
independence of the SPS factors from the Big Five personality traits is dinate facets, and each of which is operationalized with 8 items. In this
based on correlations between manifest variables (see Lionetti et al., model, Neuroticism encompasses the facets Anxiety, Self-Consciousness,
2019). Given that the employed measures have imperfect reliability, the Depression, Impulsivity, Angry-Hostility, and Vulnerability. Facets of
correlations between manifest variables obtained are prone to under­ Extraversion are Activity, Assertiveness, Excitement-Seeking, Gregari­
estimate the true overlap between these constructs. Thus, in contrast to ousness, Warmth, and Positive Emotions.
previous studies, we estimated the linear relations between the SPS The factors Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscien­
factors and the Big Five personality traits on the latent variable level. tiousness were assessed with the NEO-FFI (Borkenau & Ostendorf,
Due to their conceptual similarities, previous studies have mainly 2008). In the NEO-FFI each factor is measured with 12 items. Items were
focused on distinguishing the SPS from Extraversion and Neuroticism rated on a 5-point Likert scale from “strong disagreement” (1) to “strong
(Aron & Aron, 1997; Lionetti et al., 2019). The studies summarized in agreement” (5).
the meta-analysis by Lionetti et al. (2019) considered the SPS’ relations
to the Big Five domain on the most global level, but not at the level of the 2.2.4. Procedure
more narrow, subordinate facets. However, it can be expected that Individuals were asked to participate in a study about personality
different facets of Neuroticism and Extraversion are differentially traits. The questionnaires were administered under the supervision of a
related to SPS factors. For example, the SPS concept appears to be more test administrator in a laboratory context at the university or in a quiet
similar to the Neuroticism facet of Vulnerability (in line with the met­ room in the participant’s home. After obtaining informed consent from
aphor of vulnerable flowers) than to the facet of depression. Identifying each participant, demographic information was assessed. Afterwards,
potential differences in the SPS factors’ relations to facets of Extraver­ the HSPS and the Big-Five items were presented in alternating order. All
sion and Neuroticism is especially important because these factors are measures were administered as paper-and-pencil questionnaires. Ethical
sometimes assessed using only a handful of items (e.g., two items were approval was gained from the Ethics Committee at the University of
used to statistically control for Introversion by Aron et al., 2005). Thus, Duisburg-Essen.
we additionally report HSPS’ factor correlations with Neuroticism and
Extraversion on the facet level. 2.2.5. Statistical analyses
CFA were conducted to estimate the latent variable correlations
2.2. Method between the three SPS factors (i.e. EOE, AES, and LST) and the Big Five
personality traits. Due to the overall high number of factors and esti­
2.2.1. Participants mates, latent variable correlations were specified in three different
A total of 289 individuals (208 female) participated in the first study. models: In the first model, we estimated the latent variable correlations
The participants’ ages ranged from 15 to 75 years (M = 27.1, SD = 10.4). between the three SPS factors and the Big Five personality factors. In the
The sample mainly consisted of university students (n = 204) who took second model, the correlations between the three SPS factors and the six

5
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

Neuroticism factors were estimated. In the third model, the latent var­ Big Five personality factors. In particular, a highly positive latent factor
iable correlations between the SPS dimensions and the Extraversion correlation (0.91) was found between EOE and Neuroticism. Addition­
facets were estimated. ally, EOE was negatively correlated with Extraversion. LST has a similar
In order to reduce the otherwise high number of estimated param­ pattern of factor correlations with the Big Five to EOE, but the effect
eters, we created item parcels by averaging subsets of items. We then sizes are somewhat smaller. The latent variable correlation between AES
used these parcel scores as factor indicators in the models. A potential and the Big Five factor Openness to Experience is near 1, and the 95%
disadvantage of parceling is that the results could depend on the confidence interval includes the value 1.
assignment of items to parcels. We therefore followed the approach by
Sterba and Rights (2016; 2017) and report pooled parameter estimates 2.3.3. Factor correlations of SPS with neuroticism and extraversion on the
and fit indices across 1,000 random item-to-parcel allocations. facet level
In Table 3, latent variable correlations between SPS and the facets of
Neuroticism (Model 1) and Extraversion (Model 2) are presented. Again,
2.3. Results the items for each factor (with the exception of AES) were randomly
assigned to three parcels and all analyses were repeated 1,000 times. Note
For re-analyses we present zero-order correlations for all measures in that all factor intercorrelations were freely estimated in both models, but
Study 1 in the online supplement. The raw data cannot be made freely only the relationships of interest are presented. Both models fit the data
available online because participants have consented to the publication well (scaled fit indices: Model 1: χ2 = 665.89, df = 341, p < .01; CFI =
of aggregated data only. 0.91; RMSEA = 0.06 [0.05, 0.06]; Model 2: χ2 = 664.74, df = 341, p <
.01; CFI = 0.88; RMSEA = 0.06 [0.05, 0.06]. The results indicate that the
2.3.1. Descriptive statistics and reliability estimates strength of relations varies not only by SPS factor but also across different
In Table 1 descriptive statistics and reliability estimates of test scores subordinate factors of Neuroticism and Extraversion. In the first model,
for all instruments are presented. The Cronbach’s alphas for the three the SPS factor EOE is highly correlated with the Neuroticism facets
SPS scales were slightly lower than the ones reported in the study by Anxiety, Self-Consciousness, and Vulnerability. The factors EOE and Self-
Konrad and Herzberg (2019). Consciousness are correlated near 1 (95% confidence interval includes the
value 1), suggesting that EOE is inseparable from this Neuroticism facet.
2.3.2. Factor correlations between SPS and the Big Five personality traits With regard to facets of Extraversion (Model 2), the factor correlations of
The items for EOE, LST and the Big Five factors were randomly EOE range from highly negative (Assertiveness) to moderately negative
assigned to three parcels per factor, while AES was indicated by 5 items. (Warmth). In both models, LST exhibits a similar pattern of relations as
The maximum likelihood method with the Satorra-Bentler χ2 test sta­ EOE, but the strengths of relations are somewhat smaller.
tistic (MLM) was used as a robust estimator for non-normality (Muthén
& Muthén, 2010). Table 2 presents the latent variable correlations be­
tween the SPS scales and the Big Five personality traits. Overall, the 2.4. Discussion
model fit the data well (scaled fit indices: χ2 = 580.59, df = 271, p < .01;
CFI = 0.91; RMSEA = 0.06 [0.06, 0.07]). Overall, the results indicate two problems with the assessment of SPS
The results regarding convergent validity evidence for test score using the HSPS: Firstly, the zero factor correlations between AES and EOE
interpretations of the three SPS factors are mixed: The LST factor was indicate weak convergent validity evidence. Secondly, discriminant validity
significantly positively correlated with the other two SPS factors. evidence with respect to two of the Big Five personality traits is weak: (1)
However, the latent variable correlations between EOE and AES were EOE is highly positively correlated with the personality factor Neuroticism
near zero. Since EOE and AES are considered indicators of a common and inseparable from the Neuroticism facet Self-Consciousness. (2) AES is
construct (namely SPS), correlations close to zero are not expected and correlated near 1 with the Big Five factor Openness to Experience, indi­
indicate low convergent validity evidence. cating a single common factor. LST is also highly positively correlated with
Furthermore, the three SPS factors were highly correlated with the Neuroticism and moderately negatively correlated with Extraversion, but

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and reliability estimates in Study 1.
Measures Items M SD Skew Kurtosis α [95% CI]
HSPS 26 2.90 0.52 0.23 0.28 0.87 [0.85, 0.89]
EOE 10 3.07 0.63 − 0.09 − 0.24 0.79 [0.75, 0.83]
AES 5 3.65 0.62 − 0.12 − 0.32 0.62 [0.56, 0.69]
LST 11 2.40 0.74 0.34 − 0.13 0.88 [0.86, 0.90]
N 48 2.89 0.55 0.28 − 0.21 0.94 [0.93, 0.95]
E 48 3.33 0.44 − 0.46 0.60 0.90 [0.88, 0.91]
O 12 3.45 0.59 0.06 − 0.48 0.76 [0.72, 0.80]
C 12 3.69 0.62 − 0.25 − 0.20 0.85 [0.83, 0.88]
A 12 3.68 0.48 − 0.52 0.36 0.72 [0.67, 0.77]
N: Anxiety 8 3.11 0.76 0.12 − 0.62 0.82 [0.79, 0.85]
N: Self-Consciousness 8 3.02 0.65 0.26 − 0.09 0.73 [0.69, 0.78]
N: Depression 8 2.72 0.86 0.59 − 0.18 0.88 [0.86, 0.90]
N: Impulsivity 8 3.19 0.59 0.01 − 0.19 0.65 [0.59, 0.71]
N: Angry-Hostility 8 2.73 0.64 0.14 − 0.29 0.75 [0.71, 0.79]
N: Vulnerability 8 2.59 0.68 0.40 0.09 0.83 [0.80, 0.86]
E: Activity 8 3.12 0.46 0.16 0.09 0.53 [0.45, 0.61]
E: Assertiveness 8 3.05 0.72 − 0.20 − 0.29 0.84 [0.81, 0.87]
E: Excitement-Seeking 8 3.08 0.66 − 0.27 − 0.26 0.63 [0.57, 0.69]
E: Gregariousness 8 3.29 0.70 − 0.55 0.21 0.78 [0.75, 0.82]
E: Warmth 8 3.79 0.53 − 0.72 1.47 0.72 [0.68, 0.77]
E: Positive Emotions 8 3.66 0.70 − 0.78 0.88 0.82 [0.79, 0.86]

Note. N = 289; HSPS = Highly Sensitive Person Scale; EOE = Ease of Excitation; AES = Aesthetic Sensitivity; LST = Low Sensory Threshold; N = Neuroticism; E =
Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; C = Conscientiousness; A = Agreeableness.

6
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

Table 2
Factor correlations between the HSPS and the Big Five.
EOE AES LST N E O A

AES 0.03 [− 0.13, 0.20]


LST 0.67 [0.57, 0.76] 0.24 [0.08, 0.39]
N 0.91 [0.86, 0.96] 0.00 [− 0.16, 0.15] 0.50 [0.39, 0.62]
E − 0.55 [− 0.67, − 0.43] 0.18 [0.03, 0.33] − 0.38 [− 0.51, − 0.25] − 0.51 [− 0.63, − 0.38]
O 0.03 [− 0.13, 0.19] 0.90 [0.78, 1.00] 0.12 [− 0.03, 0.27] 0.05 [− 0.10, 0.19] 0.02 [− 0.12, 0.17]
A − 0.08 [− 0.28, 0.12] 0.17 [− 0.01, 0.36] − 0.14 [− 0.29, 0.01] − 0.31 [− 0.47, − 0.15] 0.38 [0.22, 0.54] 0.05 [− 0.10, 0.21]
C − 0.32 [− 0.45, − 0.19] 0.04 [− 0.12, 0.21] − 0.10 [− 0.24, 0.04] − 0.35 [− 0.48, − 0.22] 0.15 [− 0.01, 0.30] − 0.11 [− 0.27, 0.04] 0.16 [0.00, 0.32]

Note. N = 289. Estimator is MLM. Summarized results over 1,000 random item-parcel allocations (Sterba & Rights, 2016; 2017). EOE = Ease of Excitation; AES =
Aesthetic Sensitivity; LST = Low Sensory Threshold; N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; C = Conscientiousness; A = Agreeableness.

Table 3
Factor correlations of the HSPS with facets of N and E.
Models Factor Correlations

EOE AES LST

Model 1
N: Anxiety 0.92 [0.85, 0.99] 0.03 [− 0.15, 0.20] 0.49 [0.37, 0.62]
N: Self-Consciousness 0.94 [0.87, 1.00] − 0.14 [− 0.33, 0.04] 0.49 [0.37, 0.62]
N: Depression 0.81 [0.74, 0.88] − 0.09 [− 0.26, 0.08] 0.47 [0.35, 0.59]
N: Impulsivity 0.37 [0.19, 0.55] 0.22 [− 0.01, 0.46] 0.16 [− 0.02, 0.33]
N: Angry-Hostility 0.72 [0.60 0.85] 0.00 [− 0.18, 0.18] 0.45 [0.31, 0.58]
N: Vulnerability 0.89 [0.83, 0.95] − 0.13 [− 0.30, 0.04] 0.47 [0.35, 0.59]

Model 2
E: Activity − 0.47 [− 0.76, − 0.19] 0.24 [− 0.02, 0.51] − 0.21 [− 0.45, 0.03]
E: Assertiveness − 0.63 [− 0.74, − 0.52] 0.20 [0.03, 0.37] − 0.32 [− 0.45, − 0.18]
E: Excitement-Seeking − 0.39 [− 0.55, − 0.24] 0.13 [− 0.07, 0.32] − 0.37 [− 0.52, − 0.21]
E: Gregariousness − 0.48 [− 0.61, − 0.34] 0.02 [− 0.16, 0.19] − 0.40 [− 0.53, − 0.26]
E: Warmth − 0.27 [− 0.42, − 0.11] 0.24 [0.07, 0.42] − 0.20 [− 0.35, − 0.04]
E: Positive Emotions − 0.36 [− 0.51, − 0.21] 0.37 [0.21, 0.54] − 0.24 [− 0.38, − 0.09]

Note. N = 289. In both models, all correlations between factors are freely estimated. MLM is the estimator in both models. Pooled results of 1,000 item-parcel allo­
cations are reported. EOE = Ease of Excitation; AES = Aesthetic Sensitivity; LST = Low Sensory Threshold; N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion.

overall the effect sizes are somewhat lower for this SPS factor. Given these near 1 correlation in Study 1). We did not expect EOE and LST to be
results, LST appears to be the only SPS factor which is potentially separable correlated with emotion recognition ability due to their strong overlap
from established personality traits from the Big Five framework although it with Neuroticism and Extraversion, Big Five personality traits that are
strongly overlaps with them. relatively independent of emotion recognition ability (e.g. Schlegel
et al., 2019).
3. Study 2 Thus, in the second study, we estimated the latent variable correla­
tions between the three SPS factors, the Big Five personality factors, and
3.1. Rationale emotion recognition ability. Since individuals with high levels of SPS are
expected to be sensitive to diverse sensory stimuli (e.g. Aron & Aron,
The second study aims to test the claim that SPS is an ability 1997), we used a measure of emotion recognition ability in which
construct regarding the perception and processing of environmental emotions are expressed through different sensory modalities (Schlegel
stimuli (see Lionetti et al., 2018). If SPS is indeed an ability construct et al., 2014). With regard to incremental validity evidence for the HSPS,
that is characterized by higher empathy (Greven et al., 2019), it is we also examined the factor correlations between the SPS factors and
reasonable to infer a positive relationship between SPS and emotion emotion recognition ability after statistically controlling for the Big Five
recognition ability (e.g., Bänziger, 2016). Emotion recognition ability is personality traits.
defined as the ability to accurately identify emotional expressions via
different sensual modalities (Bänziger et al., 2009; Bänziger, 2016) and
3.2. Method
represents a component of the broader concepts of interpersonal sensi­
tivity (Bernieri, 2001) and emotional intelligence (Mayer et al., 2016).
3.2.1. Participants
Emotion recognition ability exhibits a pattern of relationships to the
A total of 226 individuals (151 female) participated in this study. The
Big Five personality traits (Schlegel et al., 2019) that is common for
participants’ ages ranged from 15 to 56 years with an average age of
different forms of cognitive abilities (e.g. Ackerman & Heggestad,
21.1 years (SD = 5.9 years). The sample consisted mainly of students
1997): It is largely unrelated to the Big Five personality traits with the
from the University of Duisburg-Essen (n = 142) who participated either
exception of a moderate positive relation with Openness to Experience
for course credit or for a payment of 10 Euros. Other participants (n = 70
(Schlegel et al., 2019). Thus, to provide evidence for SPS as a new ability
academic high school students, n = 14 individuals recruited from the
construct, the HSPS should be positively related to emotion recognition
social network of a student involved in the data collection process) were
ability when statistically controlling for the Big Five personality traits (i.
asked to volunteer for a study about personality and emotion perception.
e. incremental validity evidence for test score interpretations of the
A total of 190 individuals regarded German as their native language. All
HSPS). In line with previous research (e.g. Schlegel et al., 2019), we
participants had spoken German for at least 5 years.
expected emotion recognition ability to be positively related to the Big
Five factor Openness to Experience in our study. We also expected AES
3.2.2. Power analyses
to be related to emotion recognition ability, but this relationship should
As in Study 1, we attempted to achieve a sufficient sample size for the
be completely explained by Openness to Experience (as indicated by the
following analyses. We ended data collection when the sample size

7
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

reached the approximate target of about 200 individuals without 3.3.2. Factor correlations between the HSPS, the Big Five personality Traits,
knowledge of the results. (Post hoc) power analyses were conducted and emotion recognition ability
with respect to the standard χ2-test of absolute fit given an effect of The items for EOE, LST, the Big Five, and emotion recognition ability
RMSEA = 0.10 and α = 0.05 (H0: RMSEA = 0) (see Preacher & Coffman, were randomly assigned to three parcel scores per factor. For AES, the 5
2006). The achieved power was close to 1 in all models. The model items were used as indicators. Again, all analyses were repeated 1,000
parameters of most interest were the latent variable correlations be­ times and pooled parameter estimates and fit indices are reported using
tween the SPS factors and emotion recognition ability. Given that the the approach suggested by Sterba and Rights (2016; 2017). MLM was
standardized loadings are all 0.60, factor correlations of 0.30 can be used as a robust estimator.
detected with a power ranging from 0.83 to 0.88. In sum, the sample In Table 5 the latent variable correlations between the three SPS
sizes appear to be adequate for the following analyses. factors, the Big Five personality factors, and emotion recognition ability
are shown. Overall, the model fit the data well (scaled fit indices: χ2 =
3.2.3. Instruments 562.86, df = 341, p < .01; CFI = 0.91; RMSEA = 0.05 [0.05, 0.06]). The
SPS. The HSPS (Konrad & Herzberg, 2019) as described in Study 1 correlations between the three SPS factors are comparable to the results
was used to assess SPS. All items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale presented in Study 1. AES and EOE were again unrelated to each other,
ranging from “strong disagreement” (1) to “strong agreement” (5). which is not expected for subordinate factors of a common construct.
Big Five. The Big Five personality factors were assessed with the The latent variable correlations between the SPS factors and the Big Five
NEO-FFI (Borkenau & Ostendorf, 2008). The items were rated on the personality traits were also comparable to Study 1. EOE was highly
same 5-point Likert scale as the HSPS. correlated with Neuroticism and AES was moderately correlated with
Emotion Recognition Ability. Emotion recognition ability was Openness to Experience. The 95% confidence interval for the latter
assessed by the Geneva Emotion Recognition Test (GERT; Schlegel et al., factor correlation includes 1. LST was again moderately positively
2014). The GERT is a computer-based performance test that encompasses associated with Neuroticism and moderately negatively associated with
83 items. Each item consists of a short audio-video clip of an actor who Extraversion.
expresses a particular emotion while uttering a pseudolinguistic sentence As expected, moderate positive relationships were found between
(i.e. fictitious language that imitates real languages). Emotions are shown AES, Openness to Experience and emotion recognition ability. The latent
simultaneously in the actor’s voice, face, and body posture. After each variable correlation between LST and emotion recognition ability was
video, 14 emotion words are presented to the participant. The participant close to zero. The positive relationship between EOE and emotion
is required to select the correct emotion, i.e. the one presented in the video recognition ability was not expected. We also did not expect a correla­
(correct response = 1, incorrect response = 0). The GERT items are timed. tion of Neuroticism with emotion recognition ability.

3.2.4. Procedure 3.3.3. Partial factor correlations between SPS and emotion recognition
All measures were administered on computers in a laboratory ability
context at the university or at school under the supervision of at least The partial factor correlations (see e.g. Preacher, 2006) between (1)
one test administrator. Informed consent was obtained from each EOE and emotion recognition ability while controlling for the Big Five
participant individually. First a demographic questionnaire and the personality factor Neuroticism and (2) AES and emotion recognition
personality items were presented. The HSPS items and the Big Five items ability while controlling for Openness to Experience were estimated in
were then presented in alternating order. Afterwards, the GERT was two models. The items for the two latent variables in each model (i.e.
administered. Due to the GERT’s visual-auditory test material, head­ EOE and emotion recognition ability/AES and emotion recognition
phones were used in this part of the study. ability) were randomly allocated to three parcels per factor and pooled
results from 1,000 runs are reported. MLM was used as an estimator in
both models. The structural parts of the models with point estimates and
3.3. Results
95% confidence intervals are presented in Fig. 1.
The two models fit the data well (Model 1: scaled χ2 = 15.66, df = 12,
Zero-order correlations for all measures in Study 2 and scatter plots
p = .35; CFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.03 [0.01, 0.08]; Model 2: scaled χ2 =
for the relationships of interest are presented in the online supplement.
44.21, df = 25, p = .35; CFI = 0.95; RMSEA = 0.06 [0.03, 0.09]. As can
be seen in Fig. 1 (Model 1), the correlation between EOE and emotion
3.3.1. Descriptive statistics and reliability estimates
recognition ability is close to zero when controlling for the personality
Descriptive statistics and reliability estimates of the test scores are
factor Neuroticism. In Model 2, the factor correlation between AES and
presented in Table 4. Cronbach’s alpha estimates for the three SPS scales
emotion recognition ability is close to zero after statistically controlling
are comparable to the ones obtained in Study 1.
for Openness to Experience.

Table 4 3.4. Discussion


Descriptive statistics and reliability estimates in Study 2.
Measures Items M SD Skew Kurtosis α [95% CI] The results of the second study confirm our expectations that any
HSPS 26 2.98 0.48 0.12 0.81 0.85 [0.82, 0.88] potential relation between the SPS factors and emotion recognition
EOE 10 3.19 0.61 − 0.13 0.20 0.76 [0.71, 0.81] ability can be fully explained by the Big Five personality traits. As ex­
AES 5 3.67 0.62 − 0.41 0.02 0.63 [0.55, 0.70] pected, AES was positively correlated with emotion recognition ability.
LST 11 2.47 0.69 0.36 0.25 0.88 [0.85, 0.90]
N 12 3.01 0.72 0.11 − 0.60 0.87 [0.84, 0.89]
After controlling for the established personality factor Openness to
E 12 3.35 0.55 − 0.17 − 0.30 0.76 [0.72, 0.81] Experience this correlation was close to zero. Thus, the results of Study 2
O 12 3.63 0.55 − 0.34 − 0.22 0.74 [0.70, 0.79] confirm that AES is inseparable from this Big Five factor. Furthermore,
C 12 3.55 0.66 − 0.36 − 0.42 0.87 [0.85, 0.90] the unexpected correlation between EOE and emotion recognition
A 12 3.66 0.52 0.48 0.05 0.76 [0.71, 0.80]

ability became close to zero after controlling for the Big Five personality
ERA 83 0.67 0.10 − 0.90 1.75 0.78 [0.74, 0.82]
factor Neuroticism. The results thus once again affirm the first study’s
Note. N = 226; HSPS = Highly Sensitive Person Scale; EOE = Ease of Excitation; conclusions that the HSPS factors EOE and AES are inseparable from
AES = Aesthetic Sensitivity; LST = Low Sensory Threshold; N = Neuroticism; E Neuroticism and Openness to Experience, respectively.
= Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; C = Conscientiousness; A = The only factor that might represent a new personality factor (or
Agreeableness; ERA = Emotion Recognition Ability.
maybe a facet of Neuroticism) is LST. However, LST was shown to be

8
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

Table 5
Factor correlations between the HSPS, the Big Five, and emotion recognition ability.
EOE AES LST N E O A C

AES 0.01 [− 0.17, 0.19]


LST 0.49 [0.34, 0.64] 0.25 [0.09, 0.41]
N 0.85 [0.76, 0.94] 0.05 [− 0.11, 0.39 [0.27, 0.52]
0.21]
E − 0.58 [− 0.72, 0.10 [− 0.09, − 0.30 [− 0.48, − 0.55 [− 0.68,
− 0.43] 0.29] − 0.13] − 0.42]
O − 0.08 [− 0.25, 0.93 [0.79, 1.00] 0.16 [0.00, 0.32] 0.03 [− 0.13, 0.19] − 0.03 [− 0.22,
0.08] 0.15]
A − 0.04 [− 0.21, − 0.01 [− 0.19, − 0.12 [− 0.30, − 0.17 [− 0.34, 0.29 [0.10, 0.47] − 0.09 [− 0.27,
0.14] 0.17] 0.05] 0.00] 0.09]
C − 0.36 [− 0.50, 0.10 [− 0.07, − 0.07 [− 0.22, − 0.31 [− 0.45, 0.18 [0.01, 0.36] − 0.02 [− 0.19, 0.11 [− 0.06,
− 0.22] 0.27] 0.08] − 0.17] 0.15] 0.27]
ERA 0.18 [0.03, 0.33] 0.24 [0.08, 0.39] 0.12 [− 0.04, 0.28] 0.20 [0.06, 0.33] − 0.09 [− 0.25, 0.35 [0.20, 0.51] 0.22 [0.05, 0.03 [− 0.13,
0.06] 0.40] 0.19]

Note. N = 226. Estimator is MLM. Summarized results of 1,000 item-parcel allocations (Sterba & Rights, 2016; 2017). EOE = Ease of Excitation; AES = Aesthetic
Sensitivity; LST = Low Sensory Threshold; N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; C = Conscientiousness; A = Agreeableness; ERA = Emotion
Recognition Ability.

Fig. 1. Partial Factor Correlations. Note.


Model 1: Structural model for the partial
factor correlation between Ease of Excitation
(EOE) and Emotion Recognition Ability
(ERA) after controlling for the personality
factor Neuroticism (N). Model 2: Structural
model for the partial factor correlation be­
tween Aesthetic Sensitivity (AES) and
Emotion Recognition Ability (ERA) after
controlling for the personality factor Open­
ness to Experience (O). The 95% confidence
intervals for standardized regression weights
and the partial factor correlations are listed
in brackets.

unrelated to emotion recognition ability. Thus, our study found no ev­ correlations, our results further suggest that AES is identical to the Big
idence that SPS can be viewed as an ability to process stimuli. A limi­ Five factor Openness to Experience and EOE completely overlaps with
tation is that only one certain ability construct (emotion recognition the Neuroticism facet Self-Consciousness. LST is the only component of
ability) was considered in this study. Nevertheless, our results and the SPS that is not correlated close to 1 with a Big-Five factor, but LST also
existing body of research on the personality traits Extraversion and highly overlaps with Neuroticism and Introversion. Thus, the results of
Neuroticism’s associations with task performance (e.g. Matthews, 2009) our studies do not support the notion that SPS is largely independent of
suggest that the Big Five personality traits are essential control variables the Big Five personality traits (see Lionetti et al., 2019). In contrast to
when considering the relations between SPS and ability constructs in previous studies, we examined the relations of the SPS factors to the Big
future studies. Five on the latent variable level. Thus, the correlations reported in our
study are not affected by imperfect reliability of the measurement in­
4. General discussion struments. In addition, our first study suggests that the relationship
between SPS and Neuroticism varies by facet, which was not considered
The aim of the present paper was to critically discuss the conceptu­ in most studies (for a rare exception, see Bröhl et al., 2020).
alization and measurement of SPS as both a new personality construct Furthermore, our results indicate that SPS assessed by the HSPS is
and as an ability to process sensory stimuli. In two studies, we examined unrelated to emotion recognition ability. For an ability construct pre­
the HSPS’s relations with the Big Five personality traits and with an sumed to be related to the processing of sensory stimuli, a positive
ability construct involving the processing of sensory stimuli. Our results relationship to other stimuli processing abilities (such as emotion
indicate that SPS assessed by the HSPS strongly overlaps with the Big recognition ability) would be an important piece of evidence (see Aron
Five personality factors, especially with Neuroticism, Openness to & Aron, 1997). The LST factor (the only factor that does not appear to be
Experience, and Introversion. In both studies, the HSPS shows weak fully explained by established personality traits) was unrelated to
convergent validity evidence since the subfactors AES and EOE were emotion recognition ability. Instead, the results of our two studies
uncorrelated. Considering the confidence intervals for the factor indicate that LST could be a facet of Neuroticism or a blend of high

9
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

Neuroticism and low Extraversion. This viewpoint is further supported and empirically from established personality theories.
when considering the content of LST items. LST items often include two
different aspects: arousal by perceived sensory stimuli (which might 5. Author Note
relate to Extraversion) and the cognitive appraisal of being over­
whelmed (a characteristic of Neuroticism). From this perspective, the We would like to thank Ralf Schulze for the adapted version of the parcel
term “sensory threshold” is somewhat misleading, because the items allocation program. We also thank Maike Pisters and Philipp Engelberg
also address at the individual’s emotional reaction to sensory stimuli. for helpful comments on an earlier version of this article.
However, without a broader assessment of Neuroticism, this remains
rather speculative. Overall, our results suggest that SPS as measured by Declaration of Competing Interest
HSPS is neither a new personality construct nor an ability construct
involving the processing of sensory stimuli. The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
4.1. Social desirability of personality traits and the flower metaphor the work reported in this paper.

Why is SPS gaining more and more attention especially but not
Acknowledgments
exclusively in popular scientific literature? Our results suggest that SPS
can be explained by already established traits (namely Neuroticism,
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
Introversion, and Openness to Experience) that receive far less attention
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
outside of science. One reason for this might lie in its positive reinter­
pretation of certain personality traits that are often viewed as socially
undesirable. The low social desirability of Neuroticism and Introversion Additional Disclosures
becomes obvious when individuals are asked to report their personality
traits under “faking good” instructions (see e.g. Furnham, 1997). In The reported studies are not pre-registered.
contrast to traits like “neurotic” or “introverted”, the term “highly sen­ The manuscript was written by the first author and reviewed and
sitive” appears to be positively connotated. In addition, the term implies supplemented by the second. The study design was developed by the
an advantage in the form of better sensory abilities. This positive view first author. Both authors made substantial contributions to data
appears to be reinforced by the flower metaphor. Although the distinc­ collection. The analyses were done by the first author.
tion between dandelions and orchids was introduced as a way express­
ing interindividual differences in vulnerability, the metaphor has also Appendix A. Supplementary material
been used to refer to the beauty of these plants (see Ellis & Boyce, 2008,
p. 184). Usually, orchids are viewed as precious and rare, while dan­ Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
delions are viewed as less desirable. Indeed, the metaphor is understood org/10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104130.
by some to mean that the group of dandelions is “average or ordinary in
outcomes” (e.g. Boyce, 2019, xi). In our view, the flower metaphor is not References
helpful for solving the problem of negative evaluations and low accep­
Acevedo, B. P., Aron, E. N., Aron, A., Sangster, M.-D., Collins, N., & Brown, L. L. (2014).
tance of certain personality profiles. Instead, the flower metaphor is
The highly sensitive brain: An fMRI study of sensory processing sensitivity and
more likely to shift the problem of low desirability from one end of the response to others’ emotions. Brain and Behavior, 4(4), 580–594. https://doi.org/
trait continuum to the other (with Emotional Stability and Extraversion 10.1002/brb3.2014.4.issue-410.1002/brb3.242
becoming less desirable). Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests:
Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121(2), 219–245. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.2.219
4.2. Limitations and future directions Aron, E. N. (1996). The highly sensitive person. New York, NY: Harmony.
Aron, E. N. (2002). The highly sensitive child: Helping our children thrive when the world
overwhelms them. London, UK: Thorsons.
The results of our study are limited to the HSPS since this is currently Aron, E. (2020). The highly sensitive parent: Be brilliant in your role, even when the world
the only available measure of SPS in adults. Thus, our conclusions about overwhelms you. London, UK: Thorsons.
the existence of SPS inevitably depend on the psychometric quality of Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1997). Sensory processing sensitivity and its relation to
introversion and emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(2),
this measurement instrument. Thus, the results need to be replicated not 345–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.345
only in other samples but also with future instruments claiming to Aron, E. N., Aron, A., & Davies, K. M. (2005). Adult shyness: The interaction of
measure SPS. Furthermore, the results of Study 2 are restricted to a temperamental sensitivity and an adverse childhood environment. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(2), 181–197. https://doi.org/10.1177/
certain kind of environmental stimuli, namely affective stimuli. 0146167204271419
Furthermore, we believe that investigating processes that might Aron, E. N., Aron, A., & Jagiellowicz, J. (2012). Sensory processing sensitivity: A review
underlie personality traits is indeed an important endeavor. In our view, in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 16(3), 262–282. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311434213
Aron and Aron (1997) provided some interesting ideas, but the empir­ Asendorpf, J. B. (1989). Shyness as a final common pathway for two different kinds of
ical basis for SPS is currently weak. This is likely due to the rather vague inhibition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 481–492. https://doi.
definitions of SPS and the fact that the measurement instrument strongly org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.481
Bänziger, T. (2016). Accuracy of judging emotions. In J. A. Hall, M. Schmid Mast, &
overlaps with previous measures of personality traits. The establishment T. V. West (Eds.), The social psychology of perceiving others accurately (pp. 23–51).
and measurement of SPS as a new personality factor would greatly Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
benefit from a more precise definition of the construct. A clear Bänziger, T., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2009). Emotion recognition from
expressions in face, voice, and body: The Multimodal Emotion Recognition Test
conceptualization of SPS is also essential for selecting an adequate
(MERT). Emotion, 9(5), 691–704. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017088
measurement approach – depending on whether SPS is viewed as an Bernieri, F. J. (2001). Toward a taxonomy of interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall, &
ability construct (Lionetti et al., 2019) which requires performance F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 3–20).
measures, or as a typical behavioral style (e.g. Aron & Aron, 1997), Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Booth, C., Standage, H., & Fox, E. (2015). Sensory-processing sensitivity moderates the
which could be assessed by self-reports. At least for the moment, it re­ association between childhood experiences and adult life satisfaction. Personality and
mains unclear whether it is even possible to separate SPS theoretically Individual Differences, 87, 24–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.020

10
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (2008). NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-Inventar nach Costa und McCrae. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(1), 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Göttingen: Hogrefe. scan/nsq001
Boyce, W. T. (2019). The orchid and the dandelion: Why some people struggle and how all John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative big
can thrive. London, UK: Bluebird. five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In O. P. John,
Bridges, D., & Schendan, H. E. (2019). The sensitive, open creator. Personality and R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd
Individual Differences, 142, 179–185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.09.016 ed., pp. 114–158). New York, NY: The Guildford Press.
Bröhl, A. S., Van Leeuwen, K., Pluess, M., De Fruyt, F., Bastin, M., Weyn, S., Goossens, L., Johnson, J. S., Spencer, J. P., Luck, S. J., & Schöner, G. (2009). A dynamic neural field
& Bijttebier, P. (2020). First look at the five-factor model personality facet model of visual working memory and change detection. Psychological Science, 20(5),
associations with sensory processing sensitivity. Current Psychology, 1–12. https:// 568–577. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02329.x
doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00998-5 Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: Konrad, S., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2019). Psychometric properties and validation of a German
The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 5–13. https://doi. High Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS-G). European Journal of Psychological Assessment,
org/10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.5 35(3), 364–378. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000411
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Dye, D. A. (1991). Facet scales for agreeableness and Lionetti, F., Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Burns, G. L., Jagiellowicz, J., & Pluess, M. (2018).
conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Personality and Dandelions, tulips and orchids: Evidence for the existence of low-sensitive, medium-
Individual Differences, 12(9), 887–898. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(91) sensitive and high-sensitive individuals. Translational Psychiatry, 8, 1–11. https://
90177-D doi.org/10.1038/s41398-017-0090-6
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockard, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory Lionetti, F., Pastore, M., Moscardino, U., Nocentini, A., Pluess, K., & Pluess, M. (2019).
research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671–684. https://doi. Sensory processing sensitivity and its association with personality traits and affect: A
org/10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 81, 138–152. https://doi.org/
Cronbach, L. J. (1990). Essentials of psychological testing. New York: Harper Collins 10.1016/j.jrp.2019.05.013
Publishers. Liss, M., Mailloux, J., & Erchull, M. J. (2008). The relationships between sensory
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. processing sensitivity, alexithymia, autism, depression, and anxiety. Personality and
Psychological Bulletin, 52(4), 281–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040957 Individual Differences, 45(3), 255–259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.009
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Arousal, affect, and attention as components of Lockhart, R. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (1990). Levels of processing: A retrospective
temperament. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(6), 958–966. https:// commentary on a framework for memory research. Canadian Journal of Psychology/
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.6.958 Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 44(1), 87–112. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084237
DeYoung, C. G., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Personality neuroscience: Explaining individual Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (2016). The ability model of emotional
differences in affect, behaviour and cognition. In P. J. Corr, & G. Matthews (Eds.), intelligence: Principles and updates. Emotion Review, 8(4), 290–300. https://doi.org/
The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 323–346). Cambridge, UK: 10.1177/1754073916639667
Cambridge University Press. Matthews, G. (2009). Personality and performance: Cognitive processes and models. In
Ellis, B. J., & Boyce, W. T. (2008). Biological sensitivity to context. Current Directions in P. J. Corr, & G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology
Psychological Science, 17(3), 183–187. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- (pp. 400–426). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
8721.2008.00571.x McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal
Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult temperament. of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1258–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Journal of Research in Personality, 41(4), 868–888. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 0022-3514.52.6.1258
jrp.2006.11.002 McCrae, R. R. (1993). Openness to experience as a basic dimension of personality.
Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2008). Temperamental sensitivity: Two constructs or Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 13(1), 39–55. https://doi.org/10.2190/H8H6-
one? Personality and Individual Differences, 44(1), 108–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/ QYKR-KEU8-GAQ0
j.paid.2007.07.016 McGrew, K. S. (2009). CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on
Evers, A., Rasche, J., & Schabracq, M. J. (2008). High sensory-processing sensitivity at the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence, 37(1),
work. International Journal of Stress Management, 15(2), 189–198. https://doi.org/ 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.004
10.1037/1072-5245.15.2.189 Montoya-Pérez, K. S., Ortega, J. I. M., Montes-Delgado, R., Padrós-Blázquez, F., de la
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Roca Chiapas, J. M., & Montoya-Pérez, R. (2019). Psychometric properties of the
Thomas. Highly Sensitive Person Scale in Mexican population. Psychology Research and
Falkenstein, T. (2019). The highly sensitive man: Finding strength in sensitivity. London, UK: Behavior Management, 12, 1081–1086. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S224808
Thorsons. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA:
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Author.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290–309. https://doi.org/10.1207/ Ostendorf, F., & Angleitner, A. (2004). Neo-Persönlichkeitsinventar nach Costa und McCrae.
s15327957pspr0204_5 Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Freund, P. A., & Kasten, N. (2012). How smart do you think you are? A meta-analysis on Pluess, M. (2015). Individual differences in environmental sensitivity. Child Development
the validity of self-estimates of cognitive ability. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), Perspectives, 9(3), 138–143. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.2015.9.issue-310.1111/
296–321. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026556 cdep.12120
Furnham, A. F. (1997). Knowing and faking one’s five-factor personality score. Journal of Preacher, K. J. (2006). Testing complex correlational hypotheses with structural
Personality Assessment, 69(1), 229–243. https://doi.org/10.1207/ equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 13(4), 520–543. https://doi.org/
s15327752jpa6901_14 10.1207/s15328007sem1304_2
Gerstenberg, F. X. R. (2012). Sensory-processing sensitivity predicts performance on a Wang, Y. A., & Rhemtulla, M. (2021). Power analysis for parameter estimation in
visual search task followed by an increase in perceived stress. Personality and structural equation modeling: A discussion and tutorial. Power analysis for parameter
Individual Differences, 53(4), 496–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.019 estimation in structural equation modeling: A discussion and tutorial Methods and
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The big-five factor Practices in Psychological Science, 4(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1177/
structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1216–1229. https://doi. 2515245920918253
org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216 Preacher, K. J., & Coffman, D. L. (2006). Computing power and minimum sample size for
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., & RMSEA [Computer software]. http://www.quantpsy.org/rmsea/rmsea.htm.
Gough, H. G. (2006). The International Personality Item Pool and the future of Rinn, A. N., Mullet, D. R., Jett, N., & Nyikos, T. (2018). Sensory processing sensitivity
public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(1), 84–96. among high-ability individuals: A psychometric evaluation of the Highly Sensitive
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007 Person Scale. Roeper Review, 40(3), 166–175. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), 02783193.2018.1466840
A model of personality (pp. 246–276). Berlin: Springer. Rosenshein, J. B. (2013). Parenting the highly sensitive child: A guide for parents & caregivers
Greven, C. U., Lionetti, F., Booth, C., Aron, E. N., Fox, E., Schendan, H. E., Pluess, M., of ADHD, Indigo and Highly Sensitive Children. Bloomington, IN: Balboa Press.
Bruining, H., Acevedo, B., Bijttebier, P., & Homberg, J. (2019). Sensory processing Schlegel, K., Fontaine, J. R. J., & Scherer, K. R. (2019). The nomological network of
sensitivity in the context of environmental sensitivity: A critical review and emotion recognition ability. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 35(3),
development of research agenda. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 98, 352–363. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000396
287–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.009 Schlegel, K., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2014). Introducing the Geneva Emotion
Grimen, H. L., & Diseth, A. (2016). Sensory processing sensitivity: Factors of the Highly Recognition Test: An example of Rasch-based test development. Psychological
Sensitive Person Scale and their relationships to personality and subjective health Assessment, 26(2), 666–672. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035246
complaints. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 123(3), 637–653. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Şengül-İnal, G., & Sümer, N. (2020). Exploring the multidimensional structure of sensory
0031512516666114 processing sensitivity in Turkish samples. Current Psychology, 39(1), 194–206.
Hellwig, S., Roberts, R. D., & Schulze, R. (2020). A new approach to assessing emotional https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9751-0
understanding. Psychological Assessment, 32(7), 649–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Slagt, M., Dubas, J. S., van Aken, M. A. G., Ellis, B. J., & Deković, M. (2018). Sensory
pas0000822 processing sensitivity as a marker of differential susceptibility to parenting.
Jagiellowicz, J., Xu, X., Aron, A., Aron, E., Cao, G., Feng, T., & Weng, X. (2011). The trait Developmental Psychology, 54(3), 543–558. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000431
of sensory processing sensitivity and neural responses to changes in visual scenes.

11
S. Hellwig and M. Roth Journal of Research in Personality 93 (2021) 104130

Smolewska, K. A., McCabe, S. B., & Woody, E. Z. (2006). A psychometric evaluation of Multivariate Behavioral Research, 51(2–3), 296–313. https://doi.org/10.1080/
the Highly Sensitive Person Scale: The components of sensory-processing sensitivity 00273171.2016.1144502
and their relation to the BIS/BAS and “Big Five”. Personality and Individual Sterba, S. K., & Rights, J. D. (2017). Effects of parceling on model selection: Parcel-
Differences, 40(6), 1269–1279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.022 allocation variability in model ranking. Psychological Methods, 22(1), 47–68. https://
Sobocko, K., & Zelenski, J. M. (2015). Trait sensory-processing sensitivity and subjective doi.org/10.1037/met0000067
well-being: Distinctive associations for different aspects of sensitivity. Personality and Ueno, Y., Takahashi, A., & Oshio, A. (2019). Relationship between sensory-processing
Individual Differences, 83, 44–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.03.045 sensitivity and age in a large cross-sectional Japanese sample. Heliyon, 5(10), 1–4.
Sterba, S. K., & Rights, J. D. (2016). Accounting for parcel-allocation variability in doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2019.e02508.
practice: Combining sources of uncertainty and choosing the number of allocations.

12

You might also like