Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry **:* (2017), pp **–** doi:10.1111/jcpp.

12734

Statistical word learning in children with autism


spectrum disorder and specific language impairment
Eileen Haebig,1 Jenny R. Saffran,2 and Susan Ellis Weismer1
1
Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, WI;
2
Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, WI, USA

Background: Word learning is an important component of language development that influences child outcomes
across multiple domains. Despite the importance of word knowledge, word-learning mechanisms are poorly
understood in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD).
This study examined underlying mechanisms of word learning, specifically, statistical learning and fast-mapping, in
school-aged children with typical and atypical development. Methods: Statistical learning was assessed through a
word segmentation task and fast-mapping was examined in an object-label association task. We also examined
children’s ability to map meaning onto newly segmented words in a third task that combined exposure to an artificial
language and a fast-mapping task. Results: Children with SLI had poorer performance on the word segmentation
and fast-mapping tasks relative to the typically developing and ASD groups, who did not differ from one another.
However, when children with SLI were exposed to an artificial language with phonemes used in the subsequent fast-
mapping task, they successfully learned more words than in the isolated fast-mapping task. There was some
evidence that word segmentation abilities are associated with word learning in school-aged children with typical
development and ASD, but not SLI. Follow-up analyses also examined performance in children with ASD who did and
did not have a language impairment. Children with ASD with language impairment evidenced intact statistical
learning abilities, but subtle weaknesses in fast-mapping abilities. Conclusions: As the Procedural Deficit
Hypothesis (PDH) predicts, children with SLI have impairments in statistical learning. However, children with SLI
also have impairments in fast-mapping. Nonetheless, they are able to take advantage of additional phonological
exposure to boost subsequent word-learning performance. In contrast to the PDH, children with ASD appear to have
intact statistical learning, regardless of language status; however, fast-mapping abilities differ according to broader
language skills. Keywords: Specific language impairment; autism spectrum disorder.

(Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996). In typically devel-


Introduction
oping individuals, statistical learning serves as a
School-aged typically developing children learn as
language-learning mechanism for phonetic discrim-
many as 12 words per day (Bloom, 2000). It is well
ination (Maye, Werker, & Gerken, 2002), word
understood that a rich lexicon is crucial for language
learning (Smith & Yu, 2008), and syntactic learning
development and greatly impacts academic learning
(Gomez & Gerken, 1999). Word segmentation tasks –
(Durkin, Conti-Ramsden, & Simkin, 2012). However,
in which learners are exposed to a continuous
children with language impairments show persistent
stream of speech containing words with only statis-
deficits in word learning. Specifically, children with
tical cues to word boundaries – are commonly used
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and specific lan-
to study statistical language learning. A commonly
guage impairment (SLI) demonstrate word-learning
tracked statistical cue is transitional probability
difficulties that are noticeable early in development
(TP), which is the likelihood of stimulus Y given
and extend into the school-aged years (Kjelgaard &
stimulus X, as a function of the frequency of the co-
Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Rice & Hoffman, 2015). Given
occurrence of XY (i.e. frequency of XY| frequency of X
that language-learning difficulties are associated
[Saffran et al., 1996]). Learners appear to be sensi-
with children with ASD and SLI, it is necessary to
tive to these kinds of co-occurrence statistics, and
determine whether the nature of these language-
can use them to distinguish high-probability
learning difficulties is different in each disorder. In
sequences (words in the fluent speech stream) from
order to test hypotheses of word-learning mecha-
low-probability sequences.
nisms that result in language-learning difficulties in
Although segmenting words from fluent speech is
these populations, this study examined two word-
important, it is not sufficient for word learning. To
learning mechanisms, statistical learning and fast-
learn a spoken word, one must process phonological
mapping, in school-aged children with ASD and SLI.
information, attend to relevant linguistic and non-
Statistical learning is a domain-general implicit
linguistic contextual cues, map meaning onto the
learning mechanism whereby individuals exploit the
phonological form, relate the new meaning to previ-
statistical structure of input to facilitate learning
ous conceptual knowledge, retain the form-meaning
association, and use the word appropriately (Nation,
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. 2014). To better understand the role of statistical

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA
2 Eileen Haebig, Jenny R. Saffran, and Susan Ellis Weismer

learning in word learning, Graf Estes and colleagues chronological age-matched peers (Kan & Windsor,
examined how infants establish connections 2010). Children with SLI have deficits in encoding
between sounds and meanings (Graf Estes, Evans, phonological and semantic information about words
Alibali, & Saffran, 2007). Specifically, they asked (Alt & Plante, 2006). In addition, they have been
whether newly segmented sound sequences – dis- found to demonstrate deficits in word-learning
covered via statistical learning processes – were mechanisms like shape bias (Collisson, Grela,
more easily mapped onto a referent than sound Spaulding, Rueckl, & Magnuson, 2015). As such,
sequences that had not been previously segmented. lexical-semantic weaknesses exist in both breadth
They found that infants were able to map meanings and depth of word knowledge (Sheng & McGregor,
onto high-TP words (TP = 1.0), but not the nonwords 2010). Beyond language, children with SLI have
(TP = 0.0) or part-words (TP = 0.33), indicating that deficits in working memory (WM) and processing
the output of statistical learning can support word speed (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990; Leonard et al.,
learning in infants. There has been a call for research 2007).
to explain how the output of statistical learning Evans et al. (2009) also identified deficits in
contributes to the process of linking knowledge of statistical learning, with children with SLI requiring
sound patterns to meaning in children with ASD and additional exposure to an artificial language before
SLI (Arunachalam & Luyster, 2016; Nation, 2014). demonstrating above-chance learning of word
Given the high comorbidity of language impair- boundaries. Under the PDH, statistical learning is
ments in ASD, it is particularly important to examine crucial for learning rule-based features of language
the link between statistical learning and fast-map- such as grammar and phonology (Ullman & Pier-
ping in children with ASD and SLI. The Procedural pont, 2005; but see Hsu & Bishop, 2014 for a
Deficit Hypothesis (PDH) has been suggested to nuanced interpretation of findings). Indeed, Hede-
account for language impairments in both groups nius et al. (2011) demonstrated that procedural
(Ullman & Pierpont, 2005; Walenski, Tager-Flus- learning abilities are associated with grammatical
berg, & Ullman, 2006); however, others suggest that deficits in children with SLI. However, Evans et al.
language deficits in children with ASD and SLI stem (2009) also identified an association between seg-
from distinct or partially nonoverlapping mecha- mentation skills and vocabulary knowledge.
nisms (Boucher, 2012; Williams, Botting, & Bou- Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized
cher, 2008). Importantly, procedural learning and by core deficits in social communication and by
statistical learning both fit under the umbrella of restricted interests and repetitive behaviors (Ameri-
implicit learning. Implicit learning is the learning of can Psychiatric Association, 2013). Pragmatic defi-
information in an incidental form, without aware- cits are a defining feature of ASD; however, some
ness of the newly formed information. Although the children also have structural language deficits,
declarative/procedural model distinctly links declar- including grammatical and lexical deficits (McGregor
ative abilities with vocabulary and procedural abil- et al., 2012). Specifically, some children have
ities with grammar within the language domain, it reduced breadth of word knowledge (Kjelgaard &
has been suggested that aspects of word learning Tager-Flusberg, 2001) and many children with ASD
also may be supported by the implicit system (Evans, have limited depth of word knowledge (McGregor
Saffran, & Robe-Torres, 2009). A better understand- et al., 2012). Although children with ASD demon-
ing of the nature of procedural and declarative strate use of learning mechanisms such as cross-
learning in children with ASD and SLI is necessary. situational learning (McGregor, Rost, Arenas, Farris-
As will be outlined below, language deficits are Trimble, & Stiles, 2013) and mutual exclusivity (de
apparent in children with ASD and SLI (Durkin et al., Marchena, Eigsti, Worek, Ono, & Snedeker, 2011),
2012; Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001). Addition- they have been found to not use others, like shape
ally, both groups share risk factors spanning demo- bias (Tek, Jaffery, Fein, & Naigles, 2008). Further-
graphic, behavioral, and neural (Tager-Flusberg, more, studies have indicated that when children
2016). It is thus important to determine whether with ASD learn new words, they encode new phono-
similar learning mechanisms account for language logical forms of words, but less-readily integrate
deficits within the two populations (Obeid, Brooks, phonological information (Henderson, Powell, Gar-
Powers, Gillespie-Lynch, & Lum, 2016; Rice, 2016). eth Gaskell, & Norbury, 2014) and/or less-readily
consolidate new word knowledge (Norbury, Griffiths,
& Nation, 2010) over extended periods of time.
Specific language impairment and autism spectrum
Given previous suggestions that the PDH may also
disorders
explain deficits in children with ASD (Ruffman,
Children with SLI have a primary deficit in language Taumoepeau, & Perkins, 2012; Walenski et al.,
without hearing impairment, intellectual disability, 2006), segmentation tasks also have been used to
or neurological impairments. Although grammatical assess statistical learning in individuals with ASD.
skills are particularly poor in children with SLI, other Scott-Van Zeeland et al. (2010) found that children
abilities often are impaired. Notably, children with with ASD failed to demonstrate neural evidence of
SLI have poorer word-learning abilities relative to statistical learning. However, Mayo and Eigsti (2012)

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Statistical word learning in ASD and SLI 3

found that children with ASD evidenced intact sta- statistical learning abilities relative to the SLI group
tistical learning abilities, but that they were not due to surface-level attention biases. We also predict
strongly associated with language abilities. Attention that both language impaired subgroups would have
to phonological information and failure to integrate deficits in fast-mapping. Lastly, we expect that
semantic information to support language develop- difficulties mapping meaning to newly segmented
ment aligns with theories that suggest that children words would be most apparent in the SLI group,
with ASD may have a featural/surface-biased infor- relative to the subgroup with ALI. See Table 1 for an
mation-processing style that may enhance process- outline of the hypotheses.
ing of a single stimulus cue instead of focusing on
multiple cues (J€ arvinen-Pasley, Wallace, Ramus,
Happ" e, & Heaton, 2008). Methods
Given the deficits in consolidating linguistic infor- Seventy-four school-aged children participated in the current
mation observed in children with ASD and weak study (TD n = 26, ASD n = 25, SLI n = 23). Given the importance
of vocabulary knowledge in academic and social success in the
phonological representations or phonological WM school-aged years, we included children between the ages of 8
observed in children with SLI, it is unclear whether and 12 years. This age range also was similar to previous studies
these children are able to map meaning onto newly examining statistical learning abilities in children with ASD and
segmented words. Furthermore, the breakdown in SLI, which facilitates study comparisons. Participants lived in
mapping meaning to sound patterns may stem from the greater Madison metropolitan area (USA) and were recruited
from a larger two-year longitudinal study examining the rela-
different causes. Thus, a systematic comparison tionship between language and executive functions conducted at
between children with ASD and SLI is needed to the University of Wisconsin-Madison.
improve our understanding of the mechanisms that Standardized assessments were administered to measure
drive language impairment and to determine language and cognitive skills in the first year of the larger
whether overlapping deficits contribute to the lan- study. Nonverbal cognitive abilities were assessed using the
Perceptual Reasoning Index of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale
guage deficits seen in both populations. Although for Children-IV (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003). The Peabody Pic-
researchers have begun to assess statistical learning ture Vocabulary Test-4 (PPVT-4; Dunn & Dunn, 2007) mea-
in children with ASD and SLI separately, no study sured receptive vocabulary. The Clinical Evaluation of
has evaluated the output of statistical learning – Language Fundamentals-4 (CELF-4; Semel, Wiig, & Secord,
combining word segmentation and word learning – in 2003) was used to assess receptive and expressive lexical and
grammatical skills. Children in all groups had WISC-IV stan-
either of these populations. Therefore, in the current dard scores above 85 (with the exception of two children with
study, we asked: Do children with ASD or SLI have ASD who had scores of 79), and were monolingual American
deficits in (a) statistical learning, (b) fast-mapping, English speakers. Children with typical development (TD)
and/or (c) mapping of meaning onto newly seg- obtained standard scores that were <1 standard deviation
mented words, compared to typically developing below the mean on the CELF-4 and had no history of special
education services. Children with SLI scored at least 1.25
children? In addition, we asked whether language standard deviations below the mean on one or more of the
abilities more strongly influence statistical learning composite CELF-4 measures or demonstrated at least a 14-
and fast-mapping than diagnostic classification. point gap between one of the CELF-4 composite measures and
If atypical language development in ASD and SLI is nonverbal cognition, and had a history of or were currently
derived from underlying impairments in statistical receiving language therapy. Children in the ASD group had a
documented community diagnosis of an autism spectrum
learning as proposed by the PDH, children in both disorder. Diagnoses were confirmed by an experienced psy-
diagnostic groups should demonstrate poor statisti- chologist; children with ASD had a score of 25 or higher on the
cal learning. However, if children with ASD present Childhood Autism Rating Scale-2 (CARS-2; Schopler, Van
with a surface-biased information-processing style, Bourgondien, Wellman, & Love, 2010). To rule out ASD in
we would expect them to demonstrate intact statis- the other groups, children with TD and SLI were required to
score below the core autism cutoff score for on the Social
tical learning abilities. In addition, given word-
learning delays and difficulties with word meanings
(McGregor et al., 2012), we hypothesized that chil- Table 1 Outline of hypotheses
dren with ASD who also had a language impairment
would demonstrate poor fast-mapping abilities. We Task Hypotheses
also hypothesized that children with SLI would have Word a
TD & ASD > SLI TD = ALN;
poor fast-mapping abilities, given their known segmentation ALI > SLI; ALN = ALIa
encoding deficits. Additionally, in reference to our task
third research question, if children were able to Fast- TD & ASD > SLI TD = ALN;
mapping ALI = SLI; ALN > ALIb
segment high-TP words, we hypothesize that high-TP task
words would be more readily mapped onto referents Combination TD & ASD > SLI; TD = ALN;
than low-TP words. When comparing performance task Word > Nonword ALI > SLI; ALN = ALI
based on language impairment rather than diagnos-
tic classification, we expect children with language TD, typical development; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ALN,
ASD with normal language; ALI, ASD with language impair-
impairment to have mixed profiles of statistical ment; SLI, specific language impairment.
learning abilities. We predict that children with a
Following work by J€ arvinen-Pasley et al. (2008).
b
ASD and language impairment (ALI) will have better Following work by McGregor et al. (2012).

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


4 Eileen Haebig, Jenny R. Saffran, and Susan Ellis Weismer

Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter, Bailey, & Lord, the 2AFC test, the first stimulus played while the number 1
2003). Each child was required to complete the three exper- appeared on the left side of the computer screen and as the
imental tasks and meet inclusionary criteria listed above to be second stimulus played the number 2 appeared on the right
included in the current study. Children were matched on side of the screen. After both items had been presented, a
chronological age (p = .73) and WISC-IV standard scores question mark appeared in the center of the screen to prompt
(p = .72); see Table 2 for participant characteristics. the child to select the stimulus that sounded like the ‘Martian
language’ by pressing the button-box key with either the
number 1 or 2 above it.
Ethical considerations
Parents provided informed written consent and children pro- Fast-mapping. A fast-mapping task assessed word-learn-
vided verbal assent. The study was approved by the univer- ing abilities. Four novel words (/timo/,/bole/,/deno/,/pɑdu/)
sity’s Institutional Review Board. were paired to two-dimensional novel objects with a solid color.
During teaching, each novel object was displayed and its
corresponding auditory label was presented within a 2 s
Experimental procedures teaching trial. Each object-label pair was presented individu-
ally three times in a nonsequential pseudorandomized order.
Participants completed three experimental tasks over two In the test phase, two objects appeared at opposite sides of the
visits. Task order was counterbalanced across participants. bottom of the screen. An auditory cue directed the child’s
Half of the participants completed the segmentation and fast- attention to one of the two images (e.g. ‘Find the __.’ ‘Where’s
mapping tasks during visit 1 and the combination task during the __?’). Each object-label association was tested in four test
visit 2, and the other half of the participants completed the trials, yielding a total of 16 test trials. The task was presented
combination task during visit 1 and the segmentation and fast- using Matlab and lasted 4.5 min. A video camera recorded the
mapping tasks during visit 2. child’s face. To derive the eye-gaze data and examine learning
from the eye-gaze test phase, trained coders analyzed the
Statistical learning. A word segmentation task was used videos offline using Looking-While-Listening (LWL) coding
to assess statistical learning. Children were assigned to one of procedures (Fernald, Zangl, & Marchman, 2008). After the
two artificial languages (Graf Estes et al., 2007; Experiment 1). eye-gaze test phase completed, a second test phase ensued.
Each artificial language consisted of four disyllabic novel The examiner showed the child a piece of paper with one of the
words (Language A:/time/,/mɑno/,/dobu/,/pigɑ/; Language four object images in each corner and asked the child to point
B:/nome/,/mɑti/,/gɑbu/,/pido/) that were repeated in ran- to the object that she named (e.g. ‘Find the ___.’ ‘Where’s the
dom order without pauses or other acoustic cues to word ___?’). The examiner recorded in writing the child’s response for
boundaries. A female American English speaker recorded each each of the four labels.
syllable with all possible coarticulatory contexts. The syllables
were later concatenated to create the artificial language Mapping meaning to sounds. A combination task (Graf
containing the appropriate coarticulatory versions for each Estes et al., 2007) was designed to investigate children’s ability
syllable at a rate of approximately 200 syllables/minute. The to map meaning onto newly segmented words. The task
only reliable cue for segmentation of words was the statistical included an artificial language presentation phase, word-
structure of the artificial language (within-word TP = 1.0, learning phase, and test phase. A new artificial language –
across-word TP = 0.33). While listening, children watched a different from the one used in the segmentation task – was
silent nature slide-show for 4.75 min, presented using E-Prime created with the same statistical structure used in the
software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., 2012). Children segmentation task, using a new set of syllables (Language
were informed that they were going to listen to a ‘Martian 2A:/pɑtu/,/midɑ/,/gine/,/bodu/; Language 2B:/tune/,/
language’ and their job was to sit and listen. After the exposure pɑgi/,/mipo/,/dɑdu/). Children were randomly assigned to
phase, children completed a practice phase with commonly one of the two artificial languages. The same methods were
known disyllabic real words and disyllabic novel words used to create the artificial language for the combination task
following American English phonotactics. Then, a 32-item as the segmentation task. The labels for the word-learning
two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC; word vs. nonword) test phase of the combination task came from artificial language
phase was administered. During the 2AFC, children were that was presented in the artificial language exposure phase of
presented with two auditory stimuli – a word and nonword (a the combination task. Two of the four words in the artificial
pair of syllables that never occurred together in the speech language, with TPs of 1.0, served as the labels in the subse-
stream; TP = 0.0) from the artificial language. Items that were quent fast-mapping phase. The other labels consisted of two
words for participants who heard Language A were nonwords nonwords that were comprised of nonsequential syllables from
for participants who heard Language B, and vice versa. During the artificial language, yielding a TP of 0. Items that were words

Table 2 Participant characteristics

TD (n = 26, 13 ASD (n = 25, 3 SLI (n = 23, 12


females) females) females)

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Group comparisons

Chronological Age 10.38 1.28 10.13 1.36 10.28 1.18 TD = ASD = SLI
WISC-4a 104.50 9.06 103.65 16.63 102.35 10.88 TD = ASD = SLI
CELF-CLb 103.69 12.37 88.35 20.21 81.91 14.23 TD > ASD* = SLIc
PPVT-4d 110.96 17.02 106.27 18.90 94.22 13.77 TD = ASD > SLI*

TD, typical development; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ALN, ASD with normal language; ALI, ASD with language impairment;
SLI, specific language impairment.
a
Perceptual reasoning index standard score.
b
Core language standard score.
c
ASD versus SLI p = .101; *p < .05
d
Receptive vocabulary standard score.

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Statistical word learning in ASD and SLI 5

for participants who heard Language 2A were nonwords for


participants who heard Language 2B, and vice versa. The
stimuli in the fast-mapping task and combination task did not
differ in phonotactic probability (p = .686).
During the exposure phase, children watched a silent nature
movie while listening to the new artificial language for
4.75 min. Afterward, children were taught four object-label
associations (i.e. 2 ‘words’ and 2 ‘nonwords’ from the combi-
nation task’s artificial language;/midɑ/,/bodu/,/pɑgi/,/
tune/) individually, three times each in a pseudorandomized
order. The two-dimensional objects had novel and distinct
shapes and were different colors (relative to each other, and the
objects from the fast-mapping task; see Online Supplementary
Material: Appendix S1). In the eye-gaze test phase, children
viewed two yoked images with either the objects that corre-
sponded to words from the artificial language, or the objects
that corresponded to nonwords. Each label was tested four Figure 1 Child accuracy on the segmentation task. Gray dia-
times in a pseudorandomized order (‘Find the ___.’ ‘Where’s the monds represent group means and the dashed line represents
___?’). The combination task was presented using Matlab. After chance performance
the eye-gaze test phase completed, the examiner administered
the pointing test phase. The examiner showed the child a form
with each of the four object images in a corner and asked the
child to point to the object that she named (e.g. ‘Find the ___.’ significantly above chance (t[25] = 4.55, p < .001; t
‘Where’s the ___?’). The examiner recorded in writing the child’s [24] = 5.51, p < .001, respectively); however, the SLI
response for each of the four labels. The data from the eye-gaze group did not (t[22] = 1.52, p = .14). Given that the
test phase were coded offline by trained coders using LWL experimental tasks were administered on two sched-
procedures.
ules to control for order effects, we tested for differ-
ences in task performance according to order in
Eye-gaze data. Intercoder reliability was high (above
97.0% for frame and shift agreement, with ICCs > .890). Eye-
which the task was administered. There was no
gaze data tasks were cleaned at trial and child levels. Children effect of visit order on performance on the segmen-
were required to look toward the target or distractor image for tation task.
at least 50% of the test window and contribute at least two Growth curve analyses (GCA) were employed to
trials in each condition. Five additional children participated in analyze eye-gaze data collected during the fast-
the current study but did not pass eye-gaze cleaning criteria in
at least one of the eye-gaze tasks and were therefore excluded
mapping and combination tasks (Mirman, 2014).
from the study because they did not contribute data for all Looks to the target image during the test window
three tasks (3 TD, 1 ASD, 1 SLI). (200–1,800 ms) served as the outcome variable, and
are analogous to accuracy (proportion of looking to
the target) in traditional ANOVA analysis proce-
Results dures. Mean proportion of looks to the target were
Our first research question asked whether children transformed into empirical log odds (Elog). Linear,
with ASD and SLI have deficits in statistical learning quadratic, and other higher order polynomials were
relative to typically developing children. The mean included in the model to accurately represent the
scores on the segmentation task were 62.7% shape of the data. Linear time (i.e. slope) is similar to
(SD = 14.3%) for the TD group, 63.9% (SD = 12.6%) the traditional latency measure, reaction time, and
for the ASD group, and 53.7% (SD = 11.6%) for the indicates the rate of change in looking to the target
SLI group (see Figure 1). A mixed-effect logistic versus the distractor during the test window. As
regression model tested for group differences while before, the TD group was the reference group.
accounting for random variation at the test item Therefore, the intercept reported in the current
level. The random effect allowed childresponses to analyses represents the average overall looking to
vary, rather than fixing them at the same intercept the target image (similar to average proportion of
value. The TD group served as the reference group. looks to the target in the test window in ANOVA
Because differences between the ASD and SLI procedures) for the TD group. Lastly, degrees of
groups are also interesting, the ‘linearHypothesis’ freedom are difficult to estimate for mixed-effects
function from the ‘car’ package in R was used to test models; therefore, a z-distribution was used to
such contrasts (Fox, Friendly, & Weisberg, 2013). evaluate the significance of the t-values (t ≥ "1.96
The TD and ASD groups performed significantly was considered significant at the .05 level).
better on the segmentation task than the SLI group To address the second research question, which
(TD vs. SLI group: Estimate = !0.41; SE = .16; asked whether children with ASD and SLI have
z = !2.58; ASD vs. SLI group: Estimate = !0.45; deficits in fast-mapping, we analyzed the eye-gaze
SE = .16; z = !2.79). The ASD and TD groups did data from the fast-mapping task. Main effects
not differ significantly from each other (Esti- included linear, quadratic, and cubic orthogonal
mate = 0.04; SE = .16; z = 0.24). Additionally, each time terms, group, and group by orthogonal time
group was separately tested against chance perfor- term interactions. Random effects were included to
mance (50%). The TD and ASD groups performed allow children to vary by the orthogonal time terms.

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


6 Eileen Haebig, Jenny R. Saffran, and Susan Ellis Weismer

The random slope GCA model yielded a significant nonword trials. However, there was a three-way
effect of intercept (Estimate = 0.63; SE = .12; interaction of cubic time, condition, and SLI group
t = 5.45) and linear time (Estimate = 2.06; SE = .52; (Estimate = !1.41; SE = .64; t = !2.22), indicating
t = 3.96), indicating that children increased their that children with SLI had a different trajectory of
looks to the target during the test window and spent looking in the word condition, with looks to the target
more time looking at the target than the distractor. object, then away, and then back to the target. See
Additionally, TD and ASD groups looked to the target Appendix S2 for the full model results. As with the
image significantly more than the SLI group (Esti- eye-gaze data, the pointing data revealed that there
mate = !0.41; SE = .17; t = !2.43, Esti- were no significant group differences in pointing
mate = !0.37; SE = .17; t = !2.16, respectively). accuracy for the combination task, ps > .10, and
None of the interactions between group and time that all groups had above-chance pointing accuracy.
terms were significant. Performance did not differ Mean pointing scores on the combination task were
according to visit order. Figure 2 depicts the average 3.2 (SD = 1.4) for the TD group, 2.9 (SD = 1.5) for the
group looking behavior. ASD group, and 2.6 (SD = 1.4) for the SLI group.
Pointing data from the fast-mapping test phase When testing for visit order effects, we found that
yielded accuracy scores of 2.8 (SD = 1.4) for the TD children had higher proportion of looks to the target
group, 2.8 (SD = 1.4) for the ASD group, and 2.0 image and higher pointing accuracy when complet-
(SD = 1.0) for the SLI group, out of a possible total of ing the combination task during the second visit;
four points. The pointing data were evaluated using however, after controlling for visit order, we contin-
linear regression (which is relatively robust to viola- ued to see similar performance across the groups
tions of statistical assumptions) and nonparametric (i.e. a lack of a group effect). Figure 3 depicts group
analyses. Since results were equivalent only regres- performance.
sion findings are presented. Analysis of the pointing Our fourth research question asked whether sta-
data from the fast-mapping task revealed similar tistical learning and fast-mapping abilities were
findings to the eye-gaze data; although all groups more strongly related to child language characteris-
performed significantly above chance, the TD and tics than diagnostic group classification. As an initial
ASD groups had significantly higher pointing accu- step, we examined correlations within each group
racy relative to the SLI group (Estimate = !1.30; between performance on the experimental tasks and
SE = .57; t = !2.28, Estimate = !1.38; SE = .59; standardized language assessments (see Table 3).
t = !2.36, respectively), but the TD and ASD groups Only the ASD group exhibited significant correla-
did not differ (Estimate = 0.08; SE = .57; t = 0.89). tions between performance on the experimental
The third research question asked whether chil- measures and language assessments. Given the
dren are able to map meaning onto newly segmented heterogeneity in language abilities in children with
words. In the model of the eye-gaze data from the ASD, we also examined correlations when the TD
combination task, the intercept and linear time were and SLI groups were combined into a non-ASD
significant (Estimate = 1.68; SE = .14; t = 4.81, Esti- group, to have similar ranges in language assess-
mate = 1.86; SE = .65; t = 2.87, respectively), indi- ment scores. Despite the increased range of perfor-
cating that children increased their looks to the mance with this non-ASD group, the only significant
target image and spent more time looking at the correlation was between fast-mapping performance
target than the distractor across the test window. and receptive vocabulary on the PPVT-4 (r = .405).
There were no significant main effects of group or Nevertheless, the somewhat larger amount of vari-
condition indicating that, overall, the groups spent a ability in the ASD group may have influenced this
similar amount of time looking to the target and that pattern of correlations. It is noteworthy that nonver-
looking to the target was similar for word and bal cognition scores were not significantly correlated

Figure 2 Eye-gaze behavior on the fast-mapping task

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Statistical word learning in ASD and SLI 7

Figure 3 Eye-gaze behavior on the combination task

Table 3 Group correlations

Segmentation Fast-Mapping Combination


Taska Taskb Taskb PPVT-4c CELF-CLd

Typical development
Segmentation Task .207 .418* !.072 !.172
Fast-Mapping Task !.235 .360† .016
Combination Task .172 .219
ASD
Segmentation Task .261 .445* .469* .466*
Fast-Mapping Task .444* .566* .507*
Combination Task .541* .581*
SLI
Segmentation Task .108 .051 .170 .165
Fast-Mapping Task .249 .229 .159
Combination Task .365† .113

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; SLI, specific language impairment; Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; PPVT-4,
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-4.
a
Percent accuracy.
b
Mean proportion of looks to the target.
c
Receptive vocabulary standard score.
d
Core language standard score.

p < .1, *p < .05.

with performance on the experimental tasks for any strongly influenced statistical learning and word
group (ps > .05). learning, relative to diagnostic group classification,
To further examine our fourth research question, we compared the groups within each of the language
we classified our participants based on language classifications on the experimental measures. In
abilities. Children with ASD were subcategorized addition, we compared performance between the
according to the CELF-4 standard scores. Thirteen ALN and ALI groups, who by the nature of their
children with ASD were classified as having a language groupings differed on PPVT and CELF-4
language impairment (ALI). These children had a standard scores, ps < .001.
CELF standard score that was at least 1.25 standard First, we examined statistical learning abilities in
deviations below the mean, with the exception of two children with LI. The mean scores on the segmenta-
children who had >1 standard deviation below the tion task in the LI groups were 61.1% (SD = 9.51%) for
mean and were receiving special services for language the ALI group and 53.7% (SD = 11.6%) for the SLI
or reading. The children in the language impairment group. A mixed-effect logistic regression revealed that
(LI) classification consisted of children in the SLI the ALI group performed significantly better on the
and ALI groups; they were matched on PPVT-4 stan- segmentation task than the SLI group (Esti-
dard scores (p = .84) and CELF-4 Core Language mate = !0.31; SE = .15; z = !2.11). As before, each
standard scores (p = .54). The children classified as group was separately tested against chance perfor-
having normal language abilities consisted of children mance (50%). The ALI group performed significantly
in the TD group and the ASD group with normal above chance (t[12] = 4.19, p = .001), but the SLI
language (ALN); they also were matched on PPVT-4 group did not (t[22] = 1.52, p = .14). Next, we exam-
(p = .41) and CELF-4 (p = .82) scores. In order to ined statistical learning abilities in children with
examine whether child language characteristics more normal language abilities. The mean scores were

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


8 Eileen Haebig, Jenny R. Saffran, and Susan Ellis Weismer

66.9% (SD = 15.1%) for the ALN group and 62.7% the LI comparison model, there were significant
(SD = 14.3%) for the TD group. The mixed-effect effects of intercept (Estimate = 0.40; SE = .09;
logistic regression model revealed that performance t = 4.47), linear time (Estimate = 1.76; SE = .49;
on the segmentation task did not differ between the t = 3.62), and quadratic time (Estimate = !1.22;
ALN and TD groups (Estimate = !0.19; SE = .24; SE = .39; t = !3.10), indicating that the children
z = 0.41). Both groups performed significantly above with LI increased their looks to the target image,
chance (50%, ALN t[11] = 3.88, p = .003; TD t[25] plateaued in looks to the target image, and spent
= 4.55, p < .001). Lastly, a mixed-effect logistic more overall time looking at the target than the
regression model revealed that performance on the distractor across the test window. There were no
segmentation task did not differ between the ALN and significant main effects of group or condition indi-
ALI groups (Estimate = 0.28; SE = .23; z = 1.25). cating that, overall, the LI groups spent a similar
To determine whether fast-mapping differences amount of time looking to the target and that looking
were observed in our language groups, we analyzed to the target was similar for word and nonword trials.
eye-gaze data from the fast-mapping task. As before, However, there again was a three-way interaction of
main effects included linear, quadratic, and cubic cubic time, condition, and SLI group (Esti-
orthogonal time terms, group, and group by orthogo- mate = !2.80; SE = .73; t = !3.85), indicating that
nal time term interactions. Random effects allowed children with SLI had a different trajectory of looking
children to vary by the orthogonal time terms. The in the word condition, with looks to the target object,
random slope GCA model comparing fast-mapping then away, and then back to the target. In addition,
performance between the ALI and SLI groups yielded a pointing accuracy did not differ between the ALI and
significant effect of intercept (Estimate = 0.31; SLI groups, p = .72.
SE = .10; t = 3.15) and linear time (Estimate = 1.24; In the comparison model including the ALN and
SE = .45; t = 2.73), indicating that children with LI TD groups, there were significant effects of intercept
increased their looks to the target during the test (Estimate = 0.73; SE = .10; t = 7.15), linear time
window and spent more time looking at the target than (Estimate = 2.36; SE = .38; t = 6.22), and quadratic
the distractor. There were no significant group differ- time (Estimate = !0.65; SE = .26; t = !2.50), indi-
ences and none of the interactions between group and cating that the children with normal language abil-
time terms were significant. Comparison of the point- ities increased their looks to the target image,
ing data, however, revealed that the ALI group had plateaued in looks to the target image, and spent
significantly higher pointing accuracy than the SLI more overall time looking at the target than the
group, p < .05. The random slope GCA model compar- distractor across the test window. There were no
ing fast-mapping performance between the ALN and significant main effects of group, condition, or inter-
TD groups revealed a significant effect of intercept actions across group, condition, and time, indicating
(Estimate = 0.70; SE = .10; t = 6.93) and linear time that the ALN and TD groups spent a similar amount
(Estimate = 2.31; SE = .46; t = 5.01), indicating that of time looking to the target and that looking to the
children increased their looks to the target during the target was similar for word and nonword trials. There
test window and spent more time looking at the target also were no differences in pointing accuracy
than the distractor. The groups did not significantly between the ALN and TD groups, p = .65. Lastly,
differ and there were no interactions between group comparisons of the ALN and ALI groups on the eye-
and time terms. Furthermore, there were no signifi- gaze test phase of the combination task revealed
cant differences in pointing accuracy between the ALN significant effects of intercept (Estimate = 0.78;
and TD groups, p = .96. Lastly, the random slope GCA SE = .17; t = 4.63) and linear time (Estimate = 2.85;
model comparing fast-mapping performance between SE = .68; t = 4.17), indicating that the children with
the ALN and ALI groups revealed a significant effect of ASD increased their looks to the target image and
intercept (Estimate = 0.78; SE = .14; t = 5.57) and spent more overall time looking at the target than the
linear time (Estimate = 2.55; SE = .68; t = 3.78). distractor across the test window. Additionally, there
Although the ALN group spent slightly greater amount was a significant effect of group, with the ALN group
of time looking to the target image relative to the ALI looking more to the target image during the test
group, the groups did not significantly differ (Esti- window than the ALI group (Estimate = !0.54;
mate = !0.37; SE = .19; t = !1.92). There was a sig- SE = .23; t = !2.29). There were no significant main
nificant interaction of cubic time and group effects of condition, or interactions across condition
(Estimate = 1.09; SE = .55; t = 1.97), indicating that and time. However, there was a three-way interaction
the ALI group had a different trajectory of looking, with across cubic time, condition, and group (Esti-
looks to the target image, then away, and then back to mate = !1.73; SE = .86; t = !2.01), indicating that
the target. The ALN and ALI groups had similar the ALI group had a different trajectory of looking
pointing accuracy on the fast-mapping task, p = .98. behavior with looks to the target image in the word
Finally, we compared performance on the combi- condition, to looks away, and then looks back to the
nation task to examine whether the ability to map target toward the end of the trial. The pointing data
meaning onto newly segmented words differed revealed that the ALN group had marginally higher
among children with similar language abilities. In accuracy relative to the ALI group, p = .08.

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Statistical word learning in ASD and SLI 9

In contrast to the SLI group, the ASD group


Discussion
demonstrated intact statistical learning, even in the
This study examined word-learning mechanisms in
presence of concomitant ASD and language impair-
order to better understand the nature of language
ment. Mayo and Eigsti (2012) reported similar find-
impairments in children with ASD and SLI. We found
ings from their experiment, which used a more
that children with SLI had deficits in statistical learn-
complex artificial language. However, these findings
ing whereas children with ASD did not. Additionally,
differed from the neural data presented by Scott-Van
we found that children with SLI have deficits in fast-
Zeeland et al. (2010). Furthermore, although Mayo
mapping. Fast-mapping abilities were mixed in chil-
and Eigsti (2012) failed to find an association
dren with ASD. Children with ASD with normal (struc-
between word segmentation performance and vocab-
tural) language abilities (ALN) had intact fast-mapping
ulary abilities, Scott-Van Zeeland et al. (2010)
abilities, but children with ASD with language impair-
reported a relationship. In the current study, we
ment (ALI) demonstrated subtle weaknesses in fast-
demonstrated an association between word segmen-
mapping, evidenced by the eye-gaze data, but not the
tation abilities and PPVT-4 and CELF-4 Core Lan-
pointing data. These findings suggest that when lan-
guage standard scores in our ASD group. However,
guage impairments co-exist with ASD, the nature of the
although the ALI subgroup had slightly lower word
word-learning difficulty is different than when lan-
segmentation accuracy relative to the ALN subgroup,
guage impairment occurs without ASD.
both subgroups segmented at above-chance levels.
To address our first research question, we exam-
This finding suggests that statistical learning abili-
ined statistical learning abilities. Statistical learning,
ties may be relatively robust to language impair-
measured through a word segmentation task, was
ments in children with ASD. This may potentially be
impaired in the SLI group but unimpaired in the ASD
due to enhanced attentional biases for local process-
and TD groups. Our findings were similar to those of
ing seen in many children with ASD (Happ" e & Frith,
Evans et al. (2009) and Mayo and Eigsti (2012),
2006; J€ arvinen-Pasley et al., 2008; Mottron, Daw-
despite notable differences in the artificial languages
son, Souli# eres, Hubert, & Burack, 2006). Jeste et al.
used. In the current study, the artificial language
(2014) found that individuals with low nonverbal
consisted of four disyllabic words (Graf Estes et al.,
cognition and ASD demonstrate neural evidence of
2007). In contrast, Evans and colleagues and Mayo
visual statistical learning, though such skills were
and Eigsti used an artificial language with six tri-
rather heterogeneous in this group.
syllabic words. Given the reported deficits in phono-
Statistical learning abilities may be generally pre-
logical WM in children with SLI, a reduction in
served in individuals with ASD despite heterogeneity
syllable length in words and number of words in the
in cognitive and linguistic skills. Like Mayo and
language could have possibly lowered phonological
Eigsti (2012) suggest, statistical learning abilities
WM demands and improved segmentation perfor-
may not play a strong role in linguistic deficits seen
mance. However, most of the children with SLI in the
in children with ASD. The suggestion that implicit
current study were unable to sufficiently grasp the
learning deficits do not cause language impairments
statistical structure of the language to demonstrate
counters theories proposed in the Procedural Deficit
above-chance test performance.
Hypothesis (Ullman & Pierpont, 2005; Walenski
Poor word segmentation performance in the SLI
et al., 2006). Instead, difficulties in consolidation
group may have resulted from difficulties in implicitly
may present greater challenges to language learning
tracking the statistical regularities, due to processing
in children with ASD (Henderson et al., 2014; Norbury
constraints (Leonard et al., 2007). Alternatively, chil-
et al., 2010). Although the current study included
dren with SLI may have weak phonological represen-
children with ASD with varying language abilities, all of
tations, making it difficult to differentiate words from
our participants were able to produce sentence-level
near neighbor nonwords in the 2AFC test. Indeed,
language. Future work examining the relationship
previous research suggests that children with SLI
between statistical learning and language abilities in
have underspecified phonetic representations
children with ASD is warranted in children with more
(Edwards & Lahey, 1996). Additionally, children with
substantial linguistic limitations.
SLI have difficulties dismissing alternative candidates
Our second research question asked whether
during language processing tasks (McMurray, Mun-
children with ASD and SLI have deficits in fast-
son, & Tomblin, 2014). With our knowledge of these
mapping abilities. We measured fast-mapping using
other deficits in children with SLI, it is important to
a task in which each object-label association was
note that impaired implicit learning in children with
presented only three times. Children with SLI per-
SLI has been observed across a variety of linguistic
formed significantly worse than children in the TD
and nonlinguistic tasks (Hsu, Tomblin, & Chris-
and ALN groups. These findings are consistent with
tiansen, 2014; Obeid et al., 2016; Tomblin, Mainela-
prior evidence suggesting that children with SLI
Arnold, & Zhang, 2007). Thus, it seems likely that
typically require additional exposure in order to
reduced performance may point to a specific deficit in
learn new words (Kan & Windsor, 2010). It is
statistical learning abilities in children with SLI,
possible that in the current experiment, children
though other deficits may compound this weakness.
with SLI were unable to form stable phonological

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


10 Eileen Haebig, Jenny R. Saffran, and Susan Ellis Weismer

representations of new words during the teaching easily than nonwords in the combination task. How-
phase. It is also possible that children with SLI have ever, as indicated by the interaction across the SLI
deficits in declarative memory, contrary to the PDH group, condition, and cubic time, children with SLI
claims. Lum, Ullman, and Conti-Ramsden (2015) shifted from the target to the distractor and then back
recently suggested that declarative memory is only to the target in the word condition. Additionally,
impaired in children with SLI if children have con- despite the lack of a main effect of condition, the
comitant WM deficits. Phonological WM and nonver- correlation analyses revealed a significant association
bal WM skills were not measured in the current study; between performance on the segmentation task and
therefore, this claim cannot be directly addressed. combination task in the TD and ASD groups. The
However, future studies should examine cognitive current study was the first to examine the link between
predictors of fast-mapping in children with SLI. the output of statistical learning (i.e. identifying
Unlike our findings from the word segmentation boundaries to word forms) and subsequent mapping
task, language status may have influenced fast- of word forms to referents in children with atypical
mapping abilities. Children in the ALI subgroup and development. Contrary to our hypotheses, the lack of a
SLI group performed similarly on the fast-mapping condition effect in the combination task and the lack of
task when measured using eye gaze, but children a significant correlation between segmentation perfor-
with ALI had significantly higher pointing accuracy mance and performance on the combination task in the
than the SLI group. It has been suggested that SLI group indicate that there is no support for the idea
children with ASD have relative strengths in object- that exposure to word forms with high transitional
label associative learning (Preissler, 2008). Addition- probability in a word segmentation task facilitates
ally, children with ASD may be particularly good at subsequent word learning in school-aged children.
learning the sound structure of language, such as the The most surprising finding in the current study
phonological structure of words (J€ arvinen-Pasley was that, unlike the fast-mapping task, children with
et al., 2008; Norbury et al., 2010). The current SLI did not perform significantly worse than the other
findings support this claim for children with ASD groups in the combination task. It is possible that the
who have normal language abilities. However, the statistical structure of the words taught during the
picture is more complex with our ALI group. It is teaching phase of the combination task slightly
possible that performance on the eye-gaze test phase enhanced learning in the SLI group. Alternatively,
of the fast-mapping task may have been driven by the SLI group’s overall increase in looking to the target
attention mechanisms (Nation, Marshall, & Altmann, in word and nonword trials may be explained by the
2003; Norbury, 2005) and that simple object-label artificial language exposure phase wherein children
associative learning in a fast-mapping study may be received additional exposure to the phonemes that
mostly preserved even in children with comorbid ASD subsequently appeared in the words and nonwords
and language impairment. However, children with during the object-label associative learning phase.
ASD with comorbid language delays have been found Building on phonological explanations, exposure to
to have deficits in fast-mapping relative to mental-age the artificial language may have helped the children
expectations (McDuffie, Kover, Hagerman, & Abbe- with SLI to develop more robust phonetic representa-
duto, 2013). McDuffie et al. also found associations tions (Edwards & Lahey, 1996). The more stable
between fast-mapping and concurrent vocabulary phonetic representations may have allowed children
knowledge in children with ASD and language impair- with SLI to develop stronger representations of labels
ments. As previous studies have suggested, child during the teaching phase. Furthermore, the more
characteristics in children with ASD may influence the stable phonetic representations may have been easier
strength of associative learning in children with ASD to map to visual referents and to be differentiated from
and we will explore this in future studies. distractor images during the test phase. Dismissing
Our third research question asked whether the alternative candidates in lexical processing tasks can
output of statistical learning facilitates word learning. be difficult for children with SLI (McMurray et al.,
The results were somewhat mixed. Children with SLI 2014). Alternatively, attention during the teaching
demonstrated greater learning in the combination phase may have been primed because of the pre-
task than the fast-mapping task. As depicted in exposure of the phonemes in the artificial language.
Figures 2 and 3, the proportion of looking to the Previous language-learning studies in children with
target was higher in the combination task relative to SLI have utilized therapy strategies that provide tar-
the fast mapping task. In addition, the children in the geted input to facilitate language development. Such
ALI subgroup did not differ from the SLI group. therapeutic strategies include auditory bombardment
However, although pointing accuracy did not differ, (Alt, Meyers, & Ancharski, 2012), recast intervention
the children with ALI spent less time looking to the (Hassink & Leonard, 2010), and focused stimulation
target image relative to the ALN group in the combina- (Ellis Weismer, Venker, & Robertson, 2016). Additional
tion task, which may be explained by attention differ- work is needed to understand the types of exposure
ences in the two subgroups during the eye-gaze test that facilitate word learning. For example, is exposure
phase. The analyses also revealed that, in contrast to to the sounds/syllables of words that will be taught as
our predictions, children did not learn words more effective in enhancing word learning as exposure to the

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Statistical word learning in ASD and SLI 11

novel words in isolation before object-label associa- artificial language also may have facilitated word
tions are presented? The results from this study learning in children with SLI. Finally, this study
indicate that children with SLI have deficits in statis- underscores the importance of examining language
tical learning and fast-mapping; however, the link abilities in children with ASD and taking this child
between these deficits and their problems in word characteristic into account when setting appropriate
learning is unclear. Our data suggest that children therapeutic goals and approaches.
with SLI may require additional phonetic input to
enhance learning. In contrast, children with ASD, as a
group, did not display significant deficits in these Supporting information
learning mechanisms. Although the current study did Additional Supporting Information may be found in the
not address slow mapping or long-term word learning, online version of this article:
practitioners may need to address this component of Appendix S1. Novel objects.
the learning and consolidation process with children Appendix S2. Model summary for research question 3.
with ASD who have language deficits.

Acknowledgements
Conclusion This research was supported by a research grant from
Statistical learning and fast-mapping are impaired in the National Institutes of Health (NIH R01 DC011750),
training grants (NIH F31 DC013485, NIH T32
children with SLI, relative to their age- and cognition-
DC005359, NIH T32 DC00030), and a core grant to
matched peers with ASD and TD. In addition to the Waisman Center (NIH P30 HD03352). The current
previously suggested deficits in procedural learning project also was supported by the Emma Allen Foun-
in the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis, declarative dation Grant and the Vicki Lord Larson and James R.
learning, measured through the fast-mapping task, Larson Research Grant. The views expressed in the
also seems to be impaired in children with SLI. In current article are those of the authors and not neces-
contrast, children with ASD demonstrated intact sta- sarily those of the National Institutes of Health. E.H.
tistical learning, despite heterogeneity in language has no conflicts of interest; S.E.W. consults with the
abilities. Fast-mapping abilities varied slightly NIH on the NIDCD Advisory Council; J.R.S. consults
according to language abilities in children with ASD. with the NIH as a grant reviewer. The authors are
This finding counters suggestions that language grateful to the Language Processes Lab members, the
Language Acquisition and Bilingualism Lab members,
impairments in children with ASD stem from deficits
and the Infant Learning Lab members. We wish to
in procedural learning.
specifically thank Rita Kaushanskaya, Jan Edwards,
The current study provides an initial step in exam- Audra Sterling, Heidi Sindberg, Anna Bina, Taryn
ining whether the output of statistical learning relates Stricker, Andrea Solochek, Kaitlin Meyer, and Lizzy
to subsequent word-learning opportunities. Children Elkin. Lastly, we wish to thank the families and
with SLI demonstrated increased looking to the target children who participated in this study.
and higher pointing accuracy in the combination task
and no longer differed from the TD and ASD groups,
suggesting that the SLI group benefitted from the Correspondence
artificial language exposure before word learning in Eileen Haebig, Department of Communication Sciences
the combination task. However, improvements in & Disorders, Louisiana State University, 68 Hatcher
performance seemed to only partially be attributed Hall, Field House Drive, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA;
to the condition (words vs. nonwords), suggesting that Email: ekhaebig@gmail.com
the additional exposure to the phonemes in the
SLI processing load
/
Key points
statistical learning fast-mapping ASD
• This study examined word-learning mechanisms – statistical learning and fast-mapping – in school-aged
children with SLI andASD
ASD. We also examined whether children map meaning onto newly segmentedASD words to
clarify the relationship between these two mechanisms.
•PPTV-4
Children with SLI displayedTDpoorer statistical
ASD SLI
learning than children with ASD and TD.
• Children with SLI and, to some extent, children with ASD with language impairment evidenced poorer fast-
mapping than TD and ASD peers with normal language.
• There was some evidence that word segmentation abilities are SLIassociated
ASD with word learning in school-aged
children with TD and ASD, but not SLI.
SLI&ASD
• Clinical implications include the need to directly address language goals in some children with ASD and to
provide additional phonetic input to children with SLI.

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


12 Eileen Haebig, Jenny R. Saffran, and Susan Ellis Weismer

References Hassink, J.M., & Leonard, L.B. (2010). Within-treatment


Alt, M., Meyers, C., & Ancharski, A. (2012). Using principles of factors as predictors of outcomes following conversational
learning to inform language therapy design for children with recasting. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
specific language impairment. International Journal of Lan- 19, 213–224.
guage and Communication Disorders, 47, 487–498. Hedenius, M., Persson, J., Tremblay, A., Adi-Japha, E.,
Alt, M., & Plante, E. (2006). Factors that influence lexical and Ver"ıssimo, J., Dye, C.D., . . . & Ullman, M.T. (2011). Gram-
semantic fast mapping language impairment. Journal of mar predicts procedural learning and consolidation deficits
Speech Language and Hearing Research, 49, 941–955. in children with specific language impairment. Research in
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and sta- Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2362–2375.
tistical manual of mental disorders (5th edn). Washington Henderson, L.M., Powell, A., Gareth Gaskell, M., & Norbury,
DC: Author. C.F. (2014). Learning and consolidation of new spoken
Arunachalam, S., & Luyster, R.J. (2016). The integrity of words in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science,
lexical acquisition mechanisms in autism spectrum disor- 17, 858–871.
ders: A research review. Autism Research, 9, 810–828. Hsu, H.J., & Bishop, D.V.M. (2014). Sequence-specific proce-
https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1590. dural learning deficits in children with specific language
Bloom, P. (Ed.) (2000). Fast mapping and the course of word impairment. Developmental Science, 17, 352–365.
learning. In How children learn the meanings of words (pp. Hsu, H.J., Tomblin, J.B., & Christiansen, M.H. (2014).
25–53). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Impaired statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies
Boucher, J. (2012). Research Review: Structural language in in adolescents with specific language impairment. Frontiers
autistic spectrum disorder - characteristics and causes. The in Psychology, 5, 1–10.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 219–233. J€
arvinen-Pasley, A., Wallace, G.L., Ramus, F., Happ" e, F., &
Collisson, B.A., Grela, B., Spaulding, T., Rueckl, J.G., & Heaton, P. (2008). Enhanced perceptual processing of
Magnuson, J.S. (2015). Individual differences in the shape speech in autism. Developmental Science, 11, 109–121.
bias in preschool children with specific language impair- Jeste, S.S., Kirkham, N., Senturk, D., Hasenstab, K., Sugar,
ment and typical language development: Theoretical and C., Kupelian, C., . . . & Johnson, S.P. (2014). Electrophysi-
clinical implications. Developmental Science, 18, 373–388. ological evidence of heterogeneity in visual statistical learn-
de Marchena, A., Eigst, I.-M., Worek, A., Ono, K.E., & ing in young children with ASD. Developmental Science, 18,
Snedeker, J. (2011). Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum 90–105.
disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis. Cognition, 119, Kan, P.F., & Windsor, J. (2010). Word learning in children with
96–113. primary language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, D.M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 739–757.
test (4th edn). Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson. Kjelgaard, M.M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An Investigation
Durkin, K., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Simkin, Z. (2012). Func- of Language Impairment in Autism: Implications for Genetic
tional outcomes of adolescents with a history of specific Subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308.
language impairment (SLI) with and without autistic symp- Leonard, L.B., Ellis Weismer, S., Miller, C.A., Francis, D.J.,
tomatology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Tomblin, J.B., & Kail, R.V. (2007). Speed of processing,
42, 123–138. working memory, and language impairment in children.
Edwards, J., & Lahey, M. (1996). Auditory lexical decisions of Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50,
children with specific language impairment. Journal of 408–428.
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39, 1263–1273. Lum, J.A.G., Ullman, M.T., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2015).
Ellis Weismer, S., Venker, C.E., & Robertson, S. (2016). Verbal declarative memory impairments in specific language
Focused stimulation approach to language intervention. In impairment are related to working memory deficits. Brain
L. McCulley, M. Fey & R. Gillam (Eds.), Treatment of and Language, 76–85.
language disorders in children (2nd edn, pp. 175–202). Maye, J., Werker, J.F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimi-
Evans, J.L., Saffran, J.R., & Robe-Torres, K. (2009). Statistical nation. Cognition, 82, B101–B111.
learning in children with specific language impairment. Mayo, J., & Eigsti, I.-M. (2012). Brief Report: A comparison of
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, statistical learning in school-aged children with high func-
321–336. tioning autism and typically developing peers. Journal of
Fernald, A., Zangl, R., & Marchman, V.A. (2008). Looking while Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2476–2485.
listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language McDuffie, A., Kover, S.T., Hagerman, R., & Abbeduto, L.
comprehension in infants and young children. In H. Sek- (2013). Investigating word learning in fragile X syndrome:
erina, I.A. Fern" andez, & E.M. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmental A fast-mapping study. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Psycholonguistics: On-line methods in children’s language Disorders, 43, 1676–1691.
processing (pp. 184–218). Amsterdam: John Bejamins. McGregor, K.K., Berns, A.J., Owen, A.J., Michels, S.A., Duff,
Fox, J., Friendly, M., & Weisberg, S. (2013). Hypothesis tests D., Bahnsen, A.J., & Lloyd, M. (2012). Associations between
for multivariate linear models using the car package. The R syntax and the lexicon among children with or without ASD
Journal, 5, 39–52. and language impairment. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Gathercole, S.E., & Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Phonological mem- mental Disorders, 42, 35–47.
ory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal McGregor, K.K., Rost, G., Arenas, R., Farris-Trimble, A., &
connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. Stiles, D. (2013). Children with ASD can use gaze in support
Gomez, R.L., & Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning of word recognition and learning. Journal of Child Psychol-
by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. ogy and Psychiatry, 54, 745–753.
Cognition, 70, 109–135. McMurray, B., Munson, C., & Tomblin, J.B. (2014). Individual
Graf Estes, K., Evans, J.L., Alibali, M.W., & Saffran, J.R. differences in speech perception and word recognition:
(2007). Can infants map meaning to newly segmented Evidence from eye-movements. Journal of Speech, Lan-
words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psycho- guage, and Hearing Research, 57, 1344–1362.
logical Science, 18, 254–260. Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization
Happ" e, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: using R. New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC.
Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Souli# eres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Statistical word learning in ASD and SLI 13

update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal Schopler, E., Van Bourgondien, M., Wellman, G., & Love, S.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 27–43. (2010). Childhood autism rating scale (2nd edn). Los Angeles:
Nation, K. (2014). Lexical learning and lexical processing in Western Psychological Services.
children with developmental language impairments. Philo- Scott-Van Zeeland, A.A., McNealy, K., Wang, A.T., Sigman, M.,
sophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series Bookheimer, S.Y., & Dapretto, M. (2010). No neural evidence
B, Biological Sciences, 369, 1–10. of statistical learning during autism spectrum disorders.
Nation, K., Marshall, C.M., & Altmann, G.T.M. (2003). Investi- Biological Psychiatry, 68, 345–351.
gating individual differences in children’s real-time sentence Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W.A. (2003). Clinical evalua-
comprehension using language-mediated eye movements. tion of language fundamentals (4th edn). San Antonio, TX:
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 314–329. Harcourt Assessment.
Norbury, C.F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical Sheng, L., & McGregor, K.K. (2010). Lexical-semantic organi-
ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments zation in children with specific language impairment. Jour-
and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental nal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 146–
Child Psychology, 90, 142–171. 159.
Norbury, C.F., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Sound before Smith, L., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent
meaning: Word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsy- mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106,
chologia, 48, 4012–4019. 1558–1568.
Obeid, R., Brooks, P.J., Powers, K.L., Gillespie-Lynch, K., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2016). Risk factors associated with lan-
Lum, J.A.G. (2016). Statistical learning in specific language guage in autism spectrum disorder: Clues to underlying
impairment and autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. mechanisms. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–18. Research, 59, 143–154.
Preissler, M.A. (2008). Associative learning of pictures and Tek, S., Jaffery, G., Fein, D., & Naigles, L.R. (2008). Do
words by low-functioning children with autism. Autism: the children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias
International Journal of Research and Practice, 12, 231–248. in word learning? Autism Research, 1, 208–222.
Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 2.0]. (2016). Tomblin, J.B., Mainela-Arnold, E., & Zhang, X. (2007). Proce-
Retrieved from http://www.pstnet.com [Last accessed, 01 dural learning in adolescents with and without specific
January 2012]. language impairment. Language Learning and Development,
Rice, M.L. (2016). Specific language impairment, nonverbal IQ, 3, 269–293.
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum Ullman, M.T., & Pierpont, E.I. (2005). Specific language
disorder, cochlear implants, bilingualism, and dialectal impairment is not specific to language: The procedural
variants: Defining the boundaries, clarifying clinical condi- deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399–433.
tions, and sorting out cases. Journal of Speech, Language, Walenski, M., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Ullman, M.T. (2006).
and Hearing Research, 59, 122–132. Language in autism. In S.O. Moldin & J. L. R. Rubenstein
Rice, M.L., & Hoffman, L. (2015). Predicting vocabulary growth (Eds.), Understanding autism: From basic neuroscience to
in children with and without specific Llanguage impairment: treatment (pp. 175–203). London, UK: CRC/Taylor &
A longitudinal study from 2;6 to 21 years of age. Journal of Frances Books.
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 345–359. Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children
Ruffman, T., Taumoepeau, M., & Perkins, C. (2012). Statistical (4th edn). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
learning as a basis for social understanding in children. Williams, D., Botting, N., & Boucher, J. (2008). Language in
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 87–104. autism and specific language impairment: Where are the
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication links? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 944–963.
questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Saffran, J.R., Aslin, R.N., & Newport, E.L. (1996). Statistical Accepted for publication: 21 March 2017
learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

© 2017 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

You might also like