PDF Introduction To Early Childhood Education Pearson New International Edition Preschool Through Primary Grades Jo Ann Brewer Ebook Full Chapter

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 54

Introduction to Early Childhood

Education Pearson New International


Edition Preschool Through Primary
Grades Jo Ann Brewer
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://textbookfull.com/product/introduction-to-early-childhood-education-pearson-ne
w-international-edition-preschool-through-primary-grades-jo-ann-brewer/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Developmentally Appropriate Curriculum Pearson New


International Edition Best Practices in Early Childhood
Education Marjorie Kostelnik

https://textbookfull.com/product/developmentally-appropriate-
curriculum-pearson-new-international-edition-best-practices-in-
early-childhood-education-marjorie-kostelnik/

Introduction to Biotechnology: Pearson New


International Edition William J Thieman

https://textbookfull.com/product/introduction-to-biotechnology-
pearson-new-international-edition-william-j-thieman/

International Handbook of Early Childhood Education 1st


Edition Marilyn Fleer

https://textbookfull.com/product/international-handbook-of-early-
childhood-education-1st-edition-marilyn-fleer/

Research in education Tenth Edition, Pearson New


International Edition. Edition Kahn

https://textbookfull.com/product/research-in-education-tenth-
edition-pearson-new-international-edition-edition-kahn/
Physical Activity and Health Promotion in the Early
Years Effective Strategies for Early Childhood
Educators Hannah Brewer

https://textbookfull.com/product/physical-activity-and-health-
promotion-in-the-early-years-effective-strategies-for-early-
childhood-educators-hannah-brewer/

Child Development and Education Pearson New


International Edition Teresa M. Mcdevitt

https://textbookfull.com/product/child-development-and-education-
pearson-new-international-edition-teresa-m-mcdevitt/

Analysis with an introduction to proof. Fifth Edition,


Pearson New International Edition Steven R. Lay

https://textbookfull.com/product/analysis-with-an-introduction-
to-proof-fifth-edition-pearson-new-international-edition-steven-
r-lay/

Applied Behavior Analysis in Early Childhood Education


: An Introduction to Evidence-based Interventions and
Teaching Strategies 2nd Edition Casey

https://textbookfull.com/product/applied-behavior-analysis-in-
early-childhood-education-an-introduction-to-evidence-based-
interventions-and-teaching-strategies-2nd-edition-casey/

Migration Religion and Early Childhood Education Ednan


Aslan

https://textbookfull.com/product/migration-religion-and-early-
childhood-education-ednan-aslan/
Introduction to Early Childhood Education
Introduction to Early Childhood
Education: Preschool Through
Primary Grades

Brewer
Jo Ann Brewer Sixth Edition

Sixth Edition
ISBN 978-1-29204-201-5

9 781292 042015
Pearson New International Edition

Introduction to Early Childhood


Education: Preschool Through
Primary Grades
Jo Ann Brewer Sixth Edition
Pearson Education Limited
Edinburgh Gate
Harlow
Essex CM20 2JE
England and Associated Companies throughout the world

Visit us on the World Wide Web at: www.pearsoned.co.uk

© Pearson Education Limited 2014

All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted
in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without either the
prior written permission of the publisher or a licence permitting restricted copying in the United Kingdom
issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd, Saffron House, 6–10 Kirby Street, London EC1N 8TS.

All trademarks used herein are the property of their respective owners. The use of any trademark
in this text does not vest in the author or publisher any trademark ownership rights in such
trademarks, nor does the use of such trademarks imply any affiliation with or endorsement of this
book by such owners.

ISBN 10: 1-292-04201-X


ISBN 10: 1-269-37450-8
ISBN 13: 978-1-292-04201-5
ISBN 13: 978-1-269-37450-7

British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data


A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library

Printed in the United States of America


P E A R S O N C U S T O M L I B R A R Y

Table of Contents

Glossary
Jo Ann Brewer 1
1. Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning
Jo Ann Brewer 7
2. Designing Schools for Young Children
Jo Ann Brewer 45
3. Creating an Environment for Learning
Jo Ann Brewer 85
4. Planning Learning Activities
Jo Ann Brewer 115
5. Play: Learning at Its Best
Jo Ann Brewer 153
6. Guiding Behavior
Jo Ann Brewer 189
7. Assessment and Reporting
Jo Ann Brewer 217
8. Celebrating the Magic of Language
Jo Ann Brewer 255
9. Developing Literacy
Jo Ann Brewer 293
10. Manipulation and Discovery through Mathematics
Jo Ann Brewer 341
11. Manipulation and Discovery through Science
Jo Ann Brewer 379
12. Encouraging the Creative Arts
Jo Ann Brewer 411

I
13. Living Together: The Social Studies
Jo Ann Brewer 451
14. Promoting Wellness: Physical Education and Health, Nutrition, and Safety Education
Jo Ann Brewer 481
15. Working with Parents and Paraprofessionals
Jo Ann Brewer 511
References
Jo Ann Brewer 541
Index 561

II
Glossary
Adaptive equipment Equipment used by people with Classifying Sorting materials or objects into pre-
disabilities, such as wheelchairs, walkers, and so specified categories or into categories that one can
on. explain.
Algorithm The general rule or procedure for solving Cognitive development Development of the ability to
a specific kind of math problem; for instance, add- think and reason.
ing multidigit numbers often involves moving (or Communicating In science, sharing one’s observa-
“carrying”) the tens or hundreds value to the next tions and data collections with others through a
column. variety of means.
Anecdotal records Informal records of the teacher’s Communication In mathematics, being able to or-
observations of children’s behavior; these records ganize and express to others the solutions to prob-
are likely to reveal patterns of behavior over time. lems, how problems were solved, and so on.
Antibias curriculum A curriculum aimed at eliminat- Communicative competence The ability of a speaker
ing bias of all types by teaching children to respect to use language appropriately to achieve his needs.
people regardless of their sex, age, race/ethnicity, Comparing Examining objects and events in terms of
and other traits. their similarities and differences.
Arrival time The period just before the start of the Complex unit A play material that has several differ-
schoolday, when children are arriving at the class- ent purposes and that can be used by several chil-
room; providing activities during this period can dren at once.
help occupy children who arrive early and avoid Conflict resolution The set of skills needed to man-
behavior problems before the official start of the age difficult social situations, including recognizing
day. alternate responses, understanding others’ view-
Assessment Use of a comprehensive evaluation sys- points, and communicating one’s own feelings and
tem to determine the quality of a program or the expectations in positive ways.
progress of a child. Constructivist model A method of instruction that
Assistive technology Devices such as portable key- recognizes the importance of the learner in the
boards and speech synthesizers that allow children learning process; learners have to build their own
with disabilities to communicate. understandings, especially of math and science
Autonomy The ability to make decisions for oneself; concepts.
autonomy is essential to effective guidance because Content strands In the model of the National Council
asking children to obey without understanding why of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM), the content
does not help them learn to make good decisions strands define the subjects that will be taught:
about their behavior. Number and Operations; Geometry and Spatial
Behaviorist model A method of education based on Sense; Measurement; Patterns, Functions, and Alge-
the theory that learning is a behavior that can be bra; and Data Analysis, Statistics, and Probability.
manipulated by a system of rewards and punish- Corporal punishment Physical punishment, such as
ments or by operant conditioning. spanking, pinching, and slapping.
Behavior-management systems Approaches to man- Craft activities Activities in which the focus is on the
aging children’s behavior that are rooted in various product, suggesting that the work that results is
theories of child development and discipline. more important than the process used to create it.
Body of knowledge What is known in any field of Creativity Purposeful behaviors and ideas that ex-
study; in social studies, this would include knowing tend beyond the present and are original and
the major holidays, the names of the states, the di- imaginative.
rections on a map or globe, and so forth. Curriculum A written plan for learning experiences.
Broken home A phrase often used to describe fami- Daily class schedule A plan for daily classroom activ-
lies who are divorced; it is a negative phrase and ities that provides routine, structure, and balance
does not describe the home or the family in any yet is flexible enough to accommodate individual
useful way. and group needs.
Child care Care for children in a group, usually for Dance education Planned activities that help
the entire working day; the term day care was used children learn to express themselves through
previously. movement.

From Glossary of Introduction to Early Childhood Education: Preschool Through Primary Grades, Sixth Edition.
Jo Ann Brewer. Copyright © 2007 by Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. 1
Decision making A focus in social studies educa- in time; children’s early literacy behaviors lay the
tion that involves helping children solve prob- foundation for their development.
lems and make decisions related to the classroom Emerging curriculum An approach to curriculum
environment or their own behaviors and development in which the teacher follows the lead
choices. of the children.
Deficit model A point of view that regards parents English language learners The most common term
in terms of what they do not know or do and that for describing children who do not speak English
ignores the strengths of the family. as their first language; in some school systems,
Departure time The end of the schoolday, when ma- these children are described as being limited
terials are being distributed, coats are being put English proficiency (LEP) or as ESL (English-as-a-
on, and children are getting ready to leave; having second-language) students.
a routine for these tasks can help avoid behavior Environmental print Print that occurs in the every-
problems at this time of day. day environment, such as stop signs and product
Developmentally appropriate practice (DAP) labels.
Teaching that is age and individually appropriate Expanding curriculum A social studies curriculum
for each child in a program. that begins with a focus on the individual in kin-
Dialect A systematic variation of the common lan- dergarten or first grade and moves on in subse-
guage that is spoken by a particular group; the quent grades to address the family, the community,
variation can be in word order, meaning, or pro- the state, the nation, and the world.
nunciation; by definition, a dialect is rule governed Experimenting Controlling one or more variables
and consistent. and manipulating the conditions; the purposes are
Direct instruction Instruction in which the teacher to determine if the results obtained match those
presents information directly to the children. that were expected and if those results can be
Directed play Play in which the teacher instructs the repeated.
children in how to accomplish a specific task. Exploratory play Play in which children have no
Discipline Guidance aimed at helping children gain objective other than exploration.
self-control of their behavior. Food pyramid A model that specifies the amounts
Dispositional learning Learning to be more empa- and kinds of food needed for a healthy diet; the
thetic, to try different techniques, and to avoid current model was created by the U.S. Department
negative behaviors (such as sulking, whining, and of Agriculture and is called My Pyramid.
having tantrums) in discipline situations; modeling Formative assessment Assessment that is conducted
these skills is an effective way of helping children on a regular basis as children work through a
learn them. project; this feedback allows instruction to be
Drama Experiences in which children play, pretend, adapted if children are not making progress.
role-play, and create characters and ideas. Free play Play in which children have many choices
Drawing realism The fourth stage in children’s art of materials and how to use them.
development, which is marked by creating smaller Functions The patterns created when certain actions
drawings, adding more detail, and being less will- are performed on objects or numbers.
ing to share their art. Fundamental motor skills Basic motor skills, such
Early childhood The period from birth to age eight. as running, jumping, hopping, throwing, and
Eclectic approach An approach to social studies in- so on.
struction that combines parts of several different Games of construction Games in which children
approaches—for example, teaching children the build mechanical constructions or solve problems
names of the states as well as what underlies the by using materials symbolically.
concept of a state or a country such as the United Games with rules Games in which children play ac-
States. cording to rules they have created or rules that are
Ecological systems theory A theoretical point of generally agreed on for the given game.
view that explains a child’s development through Guidance An approach to behavior management
a dynamic system of relationships, including the that helps children gain self-esteem, learn to
family and the culture of the child. be cooperative, and gradually learn the skills
Egocentric thought patterns Thought patterns in necessary for taking responsibility for their own
which the child fails to consider the viewpoints of behavior.
others. Guided listening An instructional strategy in which
Emergent literacy The concept that children start the teacher selects a piece of music for a specific
to learn about printed language in infancy and de- group of children and then guides them through
velop their skills gradually, rather than at one point their initial hearing of it.

Glossary
2
Guided play Play in which the teacher has selected Language A system of communication used by hu-
materials from which the children may choose in mans; it is either produced orally or by sign and
order to discover specific concepts. can be written.
Guided reading An instructional strategy in which Latchkey children A term used to describe children
the teacher selects a text for a specific group of who are home alone after school until a parent or
readers and then guides them through their initial caregiver returns from work.
reading of it. Learning areas Places designated in the classroom
Head Start A federal program initiated in 1965 that where certain activities take place and related ma-
provides educational, social, medical, dental, nu- terials are stored; also called learning centers.
trition, and mental health services to low-income Learning environment A space, indoors or out, that
preschool children. is planned to facilitate instruction.
Health education Activities integrated throughout Learning plan A comprehensive plan of learning ex-
the schoolday that help children learn good health periences for children.
habits, such as daily personal routines. Least restrictive environment (LRI) The educa-
High-stakes testing Testing for which the results tional setting that is least restrictive for an indi-
are used to make major decisions about children vidual with given characteristics; the setting that
and their futures (e.g., to pass to another grade, to provides the best educational opportunity for a
graduate from high school, and so on). child who is disabled.
Holiday celebrations In social studies education, Linguistic model An approach to language develop-
choosing and celebrating holidays that are rele- ment that suggests that language is inherent in the
vant to children’s lives and the goals of the child at birth and needs only to be triggered by so-
curriculum. cial contact with speakers to emerge.
Home visits Planned visits to children’s homes by a Listening Being engaged in hearing and comprehend-
school staff member such as a teacher, counselor, ing oral language and other sounds.
or principal. Literacy The ability to read and write well enough to
Illicit play Play that is discouraged and perhaps for- function in day-to-day life.
bidden by the teacher, such as gun play, sex play, Logico-mathematical learning Learning that re-
and “superhero” play. quires children to create and reflect on relation-
Incidental learning Learning opportunities that ships between and among objects; unlike social
present themselves in the course of a day through learning (such as language), logico-mathematical
the child’s play or interaction with people and learning involves replacing previously learned
materials. concepts.
Instructional materials The materials provided to Mainstreaming A placement approach in which
help children develop specific skills and learn spe- children with disabling conditions are included in
cific concepts. regular classrooms; now more frequently referred
Instrumental play Play that the teacher plans and to as inclusion.
encourages to teach specific concepts or ideas to Mandated goals Goals set by the school director or
children. written into the school curriculum that both the
Integrated curriculum A curriculum in which the teacher and children are expected to meet.
content is integrated across subject areas; learning Manipulatives Materials such as cubes, blocks,
goals in the subject areas are met by organizing the beads, and the like that children can physically
curriculum around a topic or theme. handle while learning mathematical concepts.
Journal writing An instructional strategy in which Mastery learning The concept that children can mas-
children record messages and ideas in personal ter the tasks presented to them at school but that
notebooks through drawing and writing. some of them will need more time and more activi-
Kidwatching A term coined by Goodman (1985) ties or guidance to do so.
to describe how teachers should observe chil- Mathematics The science of numbers and space con-
dren’s language, thinking, and responses; do- figurations; for children, it provides a way of view-
ing so is very important in assessing language ing the world and their experiences in it.
development. Maturationist theory A theoretical point of view sug-
Kindergarten A class or school for children who are gesting that when given optimal conditions, a child
usually from four to six years old; kindergarten pre- will develop according to his own schedule.
cedes first grade. Measuring Collecting and quantifying information; it
Language delay A delay in a child’s language develop- is not restricted to using standard measures.
ment that results in her skills not matching what is Medical procedures The common procedures that
typical of age peers. children undergo when visiting a doctor, such as

Glossary
3
getting a shot, wearing a splint on a limb, being more positive attitudes and behaviors, a higher
weighed and measured, and the like. graduation rate, and an increased likelihood of en-
Metaknowledge Knowledge of what one knows. rolling in higher education.
Montessori model A model of education that focuses Parent place A special area of the classroom or a
on the individuality of each child and that em- room in the school that is set aside for parents for
phasizes that children are different from adults in the purpose of making them feel important and
terms of how they develop and think. needed.
Morpheme The smallest unit of sound that carries Patterns Discernible combinations or arrangements
meaning; some words are morphemes (e.g., tiger) of numbers, sounds, words, color, and so on.
and some are combinations of morphemes (e.g., Phonemic awareness The ability to understand lan-
tigers = tiger + s). guage separate from meaning and manipulate its
Morphology The system of language that involves the component sounds; it involves a cluster of skills
meanings of sounds; included are the prefixes and that develop over time.
suffixes that change the meanings of words (for in- Phonics A method of instruction that involves teach-
stance, adding an s to form a plural). ing readers how to decode the print symbols used
Movement experiences Planned activities in which to represent language sounds.
children respond to music through various types of Phonology The system of language that involves the
movement, such as clapping or tapping, walking or sounds of the language; included are rules for com-
skipping to a beat, moving slowly or quickly, and bining sounds and how to adjust stress and intona-
so on. tion to communicate meaning.
Multicultural education An approach to education Physical development The process of ongoing physi-
that encourages children to understand and respect cal growth and maturation; the acquisition of mo-
all people and cultures. tor skills is generally sequential and age related.
Multiple intelligences theory A theoretical point Physical education A content area that involves
of view that suggests learners have particular studying movement, increasing skills in move-
strengths in one or more areas, such as verbal skill, ment, and learning to play games; some teachers
physical skill, or musical skill. have special certification for teaching physical
Music Vocal, instrumental, and mechanical sounds education.
that are characterized by rhythm, melody, and Physical fitness For young children, physical fitness
harmony. is defined by cardiorespiratory function, relative
No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act Legislation leanness, abdominal endurance, lower-back flex-
passed in 2001 that requires states to administer ibility, and upper-body strength and endurance.
content-area tests to students at various grade lev- Physical knowledge Knowledge gained by exploring
els and to use those test scores as the basis for de- objects and identifying their properties (such as
termining effectiveness of individual schools. hard, soft, square, able to roll, and so on).
Nonjudgmental attitude An attitude of acceptance; Play An active behavior that is personally motivated,
a refusal on the part of the teacher to criticize is often nonliteral, has no extrinsic goals or rules,
parents. and for which the individual supplies the meaning.
Nutrition education Planned activities about various Portfolio A collection of artifacts and anecdotes that
kinds of food and how to make nutritious choices document a student’s efforts and achievements; the
to ensure good health. student is involved in selecting and evaluating these
Observing Examining objects in an attempt to notice materials.
specific qualities or behaviors; it involves all the Practice play Play in which children explore the
senses, not just sight. nature of objects or materials with no other play
Open-ended materials Play materials that allow mul- goals.
tiple outcomes and unique uses in each situation, Pragmatics The system of language that determines
such as blocks, sand, and water. the use of language in social interactions; speak-
Operant conditioning Rewards that are offered to a ers must learn acceptable ways to adapt their lan-
subject for moving closer to a desired behavior. guage to achieve communication goals in different
Oral language Communicating by expressing infor- situations.
mation through speech. Preschematic stage The second stage in children’s art
Paraprofessionals Paid assistants in the classroom, development, which is marked by attempts to rep-
who are sometimes referred to as teacher aides. resent familiar objects in the environment.
Parent involvement Parents’ interest and participa- Print-rich environment An environment in which
tion in their children’s schooling; it has been shown functional print is prevalent, such that learners no-
to result in higher grades, improved attendance, tice it, use it, and add to it.

Glossary
4
Problem solving The basic feature of mathemat- School handbook A booklet prepared by the school
ics instruction, given that the purpose of learning for the parents that describes the school’s philosophy
math is to solve problems; this term should not be and goals and provides practical information such
confused with the term word problem. as a calendar, phone numbers to call, and so on.
Process strands In the model of the National Coun- Science The process of observing, thinking, and re-
cil of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM), the pro- flecting on actions and events; for children, science
cess strands represent how mathematical content involves constructing a framework of relationships
knowledge is used: Problem Solving; Reasoning within which factual information can be organized
and Proof; Communication; Connections; and into meaningful and useful concepts.
Representation. Scientific process The cycle or steps of investigat-
Progressive education movement A model that ing various properties and reactions: forming hy-
rejects methods involving memorization and potheses, collecting data; confirming or rejecting
recitation and provides more active and engaging hypotheses; making generalizations; and repeating
experiences for learners. the cycle.
Pullout programs Special lessons and activities Screening tests Tests designed to determine whether
that involve removing children from the class- children are eligible for particular programs or
room at certain times of day—for instance, speech need special attention.
therapy. Scribbling In the visual arts, the beginning stage in
Punishment Removing children’s privileges or physi- development; also the first stage in the develop-
cally reprimanding them with the intent of chang- ment of writing.
ing their behavior. Semantics The system of language that governs the
Read-aloud A critical reading instructional strat- meanings of words; context is very important in de-
egy in which the teacher reads aloud a carefully termining meaning.
selected text to a whole class, small group, or Shared reading An instructional strategy in which
individual. the teacher takes primary responsibility for the
Readiness tests Tests designed to predict children’s reading but actively involves the children in the
preparedness for specific instructional programs. process.
Reading Gaining information from print. Sight-word vocabulary The set of words that a given
Reflective abstraction Learning a concept by ma- reader can recognize on sight and without having
nipulating objects and examining the results. to do any other decoding.
Reliability The degree to which the scores gener- Simple abstraction Learning a concept from re-
ated by a test are consistent, repeatable, and thus peated experience with objects.
dependable. Simple unit A play material that has one obvious use
Representation Related to communication in mathe- and that only one child can use at a time.
matics, representation is the skill of finding ways to Social development The process of acquiring social
record one’s information so that it can be retrieved, skills and understanding the nature of social inter-
discussed, used again, and so on. actions and relationships.
Room arrangement The arrangement of furniture Social knowledge The ability to put incidents into
and materials to meet the needs of the children and perspective and to gain a basic understanding of
the teacher. the concept of justice.
Rubric A scoring plan that indicates the tasks or qual- Social learning theory A theoretical point of view
ities that underlie successful completion of a given that describes the importance of social learning
activity (say, writing a story) as well as the weight and the use of modeling as an important method
or value of each task or quality. for assisting learning.
Rule-governed play Play in which children have ob- Social play Play in which children are involved to
jectives, such as finding the solution to a problem various degrees with other people.
or determining cause and effect. Social skills The set of behavioral skills that a child
Safety education Planned activities that help chil- needs to interact with others in positive ways while
dren learn how to protect themselves and what to being assertive about meeting his own needs.
do in an emergency; topics include fire safety, traf- Social skills The skills needed to interact effectively
fic safety, water safety, poison safety, and personal with others, especially in a group—for instance,
safety. taking turns when talking, abiding by group deci-
Schematic stage The third stage in children’s art sions, asserting preferences gracefully, and recog-
development, which is marked by using specific nizing individual differences.
forms and shapes to represent elements of the Social studies The integrated study of the social
environment. sciences and humanities; for young children, the

Glossary
5
purpose is to help them learn how to make deci- calling and voice mail systems, answering ma-
sions as informed citizens in a diverse, democratic chines, and websites; its use in school makes com-
society. munication with parents quick and easy.
Sociodramatic play Play in which children assume Testing The administration of various kinds of
roles and act out episodes, such as putting a baby tests (such as standardized, readiness, screen-
to bed. ing, achievement, and so on) to measure certain
Sociolinguistics The study of language in a social outcomes.
context; this study is closely related to pragmatics. Theme A topic of study that can be addressed across
Specialists Those teachers who are certified to teach the content areas; it should be worthy of study and
art, music, or physical education to children. relevant and of interest to learners.
Standardized tests Tests for which individuals’ “Time out” Removing a child from the group and
scores are interpreted against the scores of a norm isolating her, perhaps by sitting on a chair; the
group that has taken the test and that is considered intent is to provide an opportunity for the child to
representative of the population. gain control of her behavior and then return to the
State standards Standards implemented at the state group when ready.
level that specify, in more or less detail, what is to Traffic patterns The paths where children and teach-
be taught at each grade level in each subject. ers walk in the classroom, which are often deter-
Stimulus-response theory The theory that when a mined by the arrangement of furniture.
stimulus is offered and a reward or punishment Transitions The periods of time needed between
is given in response, a certain behavior will be re- activities, which may involve movement from one
peated or extinguished over time; this is one of the area to another; these are the most likely times for
key mechanisms for learning from the behaviorist behavior problems and other disruptions.
point of view. Validity The degree to which a test measures what it
Strand Model A way of conceptualizing the impor- is intended to measure.
tant elements of mathematics instruction using Verbal skills The set of language skills that a child
content strands (actual math topics) and process needs to solve problem situations, such as being
strands (ways of using math knowledge). able to negotiate with others and express one’s own
Summative assessment Assessment that is con- needs without being rude or sullen.
ducted at the end of a project or unit; the final as- Vertical space Wall space and space on the sides
sessment, after which instruction can no longer be of file cabinets, bookshelves, and so on; this
adjusted to provide a different outcome. space is often overlooked and thus underused in
Super unit A play material that has three or more classrooms.
purposes and that can be used by several children Visual arts The graphic arts, such as drawing, paint-
at once. ing, and printing as well as producing sculpture
Symbolic play Play in which children use items to and collage.
represent other items—say, using blocks to build a Volunteers Parents and community members who
corral and dominoes to represent the animals in it. help in the classroom without being paid.
Syntax The system of language that provides the Withitness The teacher’s ability to know what is going
rules for combining words into phrases and sen- on in the classroom and on the playground at all
tences and for transforming sentences into other times.
sentences. Writing Communicating by recording information in
Teaching strategies Methods of presenting in- print.
struction, such as demonstrations, lectures, and Zone of proximal development (ZPD) The gap be-
simulations. tween what a child can do independently and what
Telecommunication Communication that is facili- he cannot do even with assistance.
tated by electronic means, such as computerized

Glossary
6
Young Children
Growing, Thinking,
and Learning

From Chapter 1 of Introduction to Early Childhood Education: Preschool Through Primary Grades, Sixth Edition.
Jo Ann Brewer. Copyright © 2007 by Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. 7
KEY TERMS you’ll find in this chapter:
■ Cognitive ■ Maturationist theory ■ Stimulus-response
development ■ Metaknowledge theory
■ Developmentally ■ Multiple intelligences ■ Zone of proximal
appropriate practice theory development (ZPD)
(DAP)
■ Operant conditioning
■ Early childhood
■ Social learning
■ Ecological systems theory
theory

8
Young Children
Growing, Thinking,
FOCUS
and Learning QUESTIONS
● What is developmentally appro-
priate practice (DAP)?
● What are the five major theo-
When you enrolled in the program to become a teacher of ries that describe child growth
and development?
young children, you knew that you had much to learn about
● What patterns make up the
planning activities for children, keeping records of children’s physical, social, emotional,
progress, and handling all the other responsibilities of a and intellectual development
of young children?
teacher. You were pleased when your first observation was to
● How can knowledge about
be focused on children’s growth and development because that child development be used to
seemed to you to be basic information that every teacher would identify children with special
needs?
need to know.
● What is the relationship be-
You selected a second-grade classroom in a school where tween child development and
the population was diverse and the school was in a low socio- diversity?
economic area of the city. After your observation, when you
looked over your notes, you found many indicators of devel-
opment in all areas. The children in this classroom exhibited
their physical development through skillful ball throwing and
catching, jumping rope, hanging by their knees from bars,
and other physical skills. They also had the fine motor skills to
make writing, drawing, and painting easy for them to manage.
You noted evidence of social growth as children settled a dis-
agreement about who was responsible for cleaning up the art
center by talking about their points of view and listening to one
another’s perception of the problem. You noted that indications
of frustration or anger were rare in this classroom. You also
found that these children could attend to a task for an extended
period of time. You watched in amazement as they worked on
their research projects for an hour.

9
When you talked with the teacher, she explained that for chil-
dren ages seven and eight, she plans many activities that give the
children a choice about what they will do to achieve a goal, she in-
vites children to share their ideas and discoveries with one another,
she plans regular sessions in which classroom problems are solved,
and she is sensitive to the cultural expectations of the parents. She
knows that second-graders still need to move around and talk to
one another. ■

Developmentally Appropriate Practice


This chapter is meant to be a review of the principles of child development,
not a comprehensive treatment of the topic. A basic knowledge of child de-
velopment is critical to appropriate planning, teaching, and evaluating in
programs for young children. Just as we would never plan a meal or vacation
without knowing something about the ages and tastes of the participants,
we cannot plan curricula without knowing what children of a given age will
likely be able to do and be interested in doing.
The National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC)
defines early childhood as the period from birth to age eight (Bredekamp
1987). Some child-care programs serve infants and toddlers as well as pre-
schoolers (three- and four-year-olds) and primary children (five-, six-, seven-,
and eight-year-olds). Most of this chapter will discuss normal growth and
development. Keep in mind that to determine the average age when a child
is able to perform a task or behave in a certain way, some children must
perform that task earlier than the average and some later than the average.
You will meet many children in your teaching career who have special
needs, ranging from severe mental or physical challenges to slight delays
in the development of language. You also will meet children who are gifted
and talented in a variety of ways, who have attention deficits, and who are
wise beyond their years in relating to people. Children come in all sizes,
colors, and places on the developmental continuum. Our job as teachers is
. to recognize each child’s abilities and growth and plan for the next steps as
ww she grows physically, intellectually, socially, and emotionally.
w

www.naeyc.org The term developmentally appropriate practice (DAP) means prac-


Several statements about tice that is age and individually appropriate for each child in a program.
developmentally appropri- Planning a developmentally appropriate curriculum means that teachers
ate practice along with have to know each child—where he is developmentally and what his indi-
resources for teachers vidual talents and interests are. DAP also requires that teachers think about
children’s basic needs for play and rest, that they focus on children’s develop-
ment in all areas, that they plan an inclusive program that honors the cul-
tural differences each child brings to school, and that they work to support
parents and families (Bredekamp 1987, 1996).
Programs for young children can vary widely and still be considered
DAP programs. However, some practices are not appropriate for young chil-
dren. These practices include timed tests, a focus on academic skill devel-

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


10
A variety of developmentally
appropriate toys should be
provided in the preschool
classroom.

opment through worksheets or workbooks, and a schedule that ignores a


young child’s need to move physically and expects her to sit for extended
periods of time.
Please remember that although some basic guidelines help define DAP,
in classrooms across the United States, DAP looks quite different. DAP re-
quires that teachers and children work continuously to determine the best
practice for a group of children at a given time. DAP is dynamic, not static.
It cannot be the same for all children or all groups of children.
The purpose of this chapter is to review briefly some theoretical lenses
through which child development is seen and to outline typical patterns of
development. This knowledge is most useful when it can be consciously ap-
plied to selecting and organizing curriculum experiences. Often, teachers
choose activities and organize those activities in ways that can be traced
to the different theories of child growth and development, but they are
unaware of these connections. As a teacher, you need to recognize why
you choose one activity over another or one organizational scheme over
another.

Theories of Development
Children’s growth and development can be de-
scribed from several theoretical points of view.
Each of these points of view has its supporters Parents and Development
among practitioners in early childhood educa- ● Each month, find an article on some aspect of child
tion, and each offers explanations of human development related to the age of children you teach.
growth and development. These theoretical Make the article available to parents by summarizing it
positions include behaviorism, maturationism, in the class newsletter or letting those who are inter-
constructivism, multiple intelligences theory, ested check it out.

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


11
social learning theory, and ecological systems theory. Throughout your ca-
reer, as you read and think about children and about planning programs that
will be beneficial to them, reflect on the points of view expressed by authors
of articles and reports, and notice how their suggestions differ depending on
what they believe about children’s development.

.
ww Behaviorist Theory
w

www.sil.org/lingualinks/ Behaviorist theory suggests that behavior can be shaped by the response
literacy/implement that follows any particular action. John B. Watson (1878–1958), Edward
ALiteracy/Behaviorist
TheoriesofLearning.htm Thorndike (1874–1949), and B. F. Skinner (1904–1990) are well-known
Information and links to re- behaviorists.
sources on behaviorism and its Skinner is the best known of the modern behaviorists. He wrote exten-
important theorists sively about learning as controlled by a system of rewards and punishments.
His name is synonymous with the terms stimulus-response theory and
.
ww operant conditioning (W. F. Hill 1977). The basic elements of stimulus-
w

http://web.cocc.edu/cbuell/ response theory include reinforcements, punishments, operant condition-


theories/behaviorism.htm ing, and extinguishing behavior.
More information about In classical conditioning, a learner is presented with a stimulus and a
behaviorism reward and learns to expect the reward whenever the stimulus is presented.
This type of conditioning is exemplified by the classic Pavlovian experiment
P R AX I S of ringing a bell and immediately presenting a dog with food until the dog
Be able to describe various salivates at the sound of the bell. Operant conditioning differs from classical
theories of child development conditioning in that the behavior precedes the reinforcement. For example,
a pigeon may learn to push a lever to get a food pellet. Presenting a reward
after a response provides positive reinforcement of the behavior. Negative
reinforcement can also be used to elicit behavior. If the floor of the pigeon’s
cage were electrified and pushing the lever stopped the shock, the pigeon
would learn to press the lever to avoid an unpleasant stimulus; that is nega-
tive reinforcement. Behavior can also be punished rather than reinforced.
A pigeon might be punished by an electric shock for failing to push a lever
on a set schedule.
In the classroom, reinforcements are often used to shape behaviors.
Reinforcements in the classroom can be positive or negative. A positive re-
inforcement is something the learner views as desirable. If a child completes
a task or exhibits a behavior that is desirable, the teacher may positively
reinforce that behavior by verbal praise, a token such as a sticker, or some
other treat. Negative reinforcement involves allowing a child to avoid or es-
cape from an undesirable consequence or situation if certain behaviors are
exhibited. For example, the teacher might give a child the option of leaving
the “time-out” chair more quickly if she does not talk at all for five min-
utes. Finally, punishment in the classroom need not necessarily be corporal
(physical) punishment but can be time-outs, exclusion from the group, with-
drawal of privileges, and so on.
In a classroom, operant conditioning might be used to shape children’s
performance on an academic task. For example, suppose a class of second-
graders has been assigned the task of completing twenty arithmetic prob-
lems. Those children who complete the task promptly and correctly get
tokens, which can be redeemed for objects in the class store on Friday. With
those children who do not complete the task either promptly or correctly,
the teacher may take one of several approaches. If he knows that three of the
children have never completed twenty problems, he may choose to reward

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


12
their performance if they complete more than they have completed before.
He may choose to provide neither positive nor negative reinforcement to
children who complete the same number of problems they have completed
in the past. If he believes that some children deliberately did not attempt
the task for some reason, he might punish them by reducing their recess
time.
Operant conditioning can be used to shape behavior by providing rein-
forcements when the learner’s behavior moves closer to the target behavior.
Shaping behavior involves the following components (Pellegrini 1987):

1. targeting the desired behavior


2. fixing a behavioral baseline
3. selecting reinforcers
4. analyzing the task and sequencing the segments
5. systematically applying the reinforcers (p. 113)

In the classroom example just discussed, by rewarding a child who


completes more problems than in previous assignments, the teacher is re-
warding closer approximations to the target behavior of completing twenty
problems. Teachers often use such techniques to help children gain control
of their classroom behaviors. If a child is hitting other children, for example,
the teacher will first collect baseline data through observations to determine
how often the child is hitting and then provide rewards as the hitting behav-
ior decreases.
Behaviors can be extinguished by failing to provide reinforcements for
them. If a child is behaving in ways the teacher deems inappropriate, then
the teacher can ignore the less desirable behaviors while consistently re-
warding the more desirable behaviors.
The goal of employing behavioristic techniques is for the desired be-
havior to become rewarding to the child so that the teacher or parent need
not continue to provide extrinsic rewards. Most teachers, even those who
believe strongly in other theories of development employ some behavioristic
strategies as they ignore some behaviors and praise others. Most teachers
have had experience with children who prefer negative attention—that is,
punishment—to no attention; the usual strategy for dealing with such chil-
dren is to make every effort to ignore their misbehavior and to reward their
appropriate behavior.
Behaviorist theory does not say much about physical development
because most authorities agree that physical development is genetically
determined and thus does not affect a child’s behavior, at least in an opti-
mal environment that includes good nutrition, lack of disease, and safety.
Behaviorists are more concerned with how children develop socially, emo-
tionally, and intellectually.

.
Maturationist Theory ww
w

The roots of the maturationist theory are found in the work of Jean-Jacques www.ncrel/org/sdrs/areas/
Rousseau (1712–1778), who believed that children should be allowed to “un- issues/students/earlycld/
ea7lk18.htm
fold.” A child is like a seed that contains all the elements to produce a won-
derful apple if given the proper amounts of nutrients from the soil and water Explanations of maturationist
theory
along with sunshine and an ideal climate. The modern maturationist point
of view is most often associated with the work of the Gesell Institute (Ilg and

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


13
Ames 1955). The Gesellian literature describes growth and development in
terms of children’s maturity.
Experience plays a much less important role in development in the
maturationist point of view than it does in the behaviorist point of view.
Maturationists believe that each child’s physical, social, emotional, and in-
tellectual development follows an individual schedule that is basically pre-
determined. They believe that a child will develop to his potential when
placed in an optimal environment and that his development will be slowed
or retarded if the environment is not optimal.
Maturationists believe that a child’s developmental level is the most
important determiner of social and intellectual success, especially in school
settings. They suggest that the child will have difficulty in school if he is
“overplaced,” that is, placed in settings where the requirements do not
match his developmental level. Maturationists emphasize the child’s own
schedule of maturation rather than rewards and punishments, experiences,
or interactions with the environment. Experience, in the maturationist point
of view, is always filtered by the child’s maturation level.
Even though this theoretical position has been criticized, many schools
continue to screen children for entry into programs on the basis of develop-
mental tests developed by the Gesell Institute (Meisels 1987).

.
ww Constructivist Theory
w

www.nvcc.edu/home/Kludlow/ The constructivist, or developmentalist, theory is founded on the work of Jean


theories/default.htm Piaget (1896–1980) and Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934). Modern constructivists—
www.ncrel/org/sdrs/areas/ such as Jerome Bruner, George Forman, and others—continue to refine the
issues/content/cntareas/
science/sc5model.htm theories of Piaget and to clarify concepts about children’s development.
Both offer explanations of
constructivist theory and its Jean Piaget
major theorists Piaget believed that children create knowledge through interactions with
.
ww the environment. Children are not passive receivers of knowledge; rather,
w

they actively work at organizing their experience into more and more com-
http://chiron.valdosta.edu/
whuitt.col.cogsys/piaget.html plex mental structures. Piaget and Inhelder’s (1969) descriptions of chil-
A biography of Piaget and an dren’s thinking include the concepts of assimilation, accommodation, and
explanation of his theories equilibrium.
Assimilation is fitting information into existing schemas or categories.
If a child has developed a schema for “dog” and is presented with a new ex-
ample of a dog, such as a St. Bernard, the new example can be assimilated,
or included, in the existing schema. If presented with a cat, the child may
create a new schema for “furry pets that are not dogs.” Creating a new cat-
egory is the process of accommodation. Through a series of repeated assimi-
lations and accommodations, the child eventually creates a mental structure
that will account for all animals.
Equilibrium is the balance achieved whenever information or experi-
ence is fitted into a schema or a new schema is created for it. This balance
is very short lived, as the child constantly encounters new experiences and
information. Disequilibrium describes the mental state in which there is an
imbalance between assimilation and accommodation. Disequilibrium is
motivating in that it drives the learner to achieve equilibrium.
Equilibration is the process of moving from disequilibrium to equi-
librium. Equilibration allows the learner to employ assimilation and ac-
commodation as tools for achieving equilibrium (Piaget 1985). In the “dog”

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


14
example, if the child is presented with a basenji, a dog that rarely barks,
disequilibrium may result as the child is confronted with a dog that does not
exhibit one of the usual dog behaviors. The child must then assimilate this
dog into her schema of dogs. Some theorists agree that equilibration is one
process that contributes to cognitive growth but question the idea that one
process can account for all cognitive development.
Piagetians categorize knowledge as physical, social, or logico-math-
ematical. Forman and Kuschner (1983) postulate a fourth kind of knowl-
edge: knowing what one knows. The term used in the literature to describe
this category of knowledge is metaknowledge. If a child knows about
the number system, for instance, then his knowing that he knows the sys-
tem is another type of knowledge that is not social, physical, or logico-
mathematical.
Wadsworth (1989) describes the definition of learning in Piagetian
terms as follows:

Two usages or meanings of the word learning are differentiated by Piagetians.


The first usage can be called learning in the broad sense, and it is synonymous
with development. It is appropriate to talk about the development of, or the
learning of, physical knowledge, logical-mathematical knowledge, and social
knowledge. The second usage of learning is narrower. It refers to the acquisi-
tion of specific information from the environment, learning that is assimilated
into an existing schema. Most content learning is the second kind. Both forms
of learning imply comprehension.
Rote memory, or memorization, is not considered learning because it does
not involve assimilation and comprehension. Some theories, such as behav-
iorism, consider rote memory as a form of learning and do not differentiate
between it and the two kinds of learning defined here. For Piagetians, learning
always involves construction and comprehension. (p. 156)

Wadsworth goes on to explain that even though rote memory is not


considered by Piagetians to be a kind of learning, it is valuable. Rote mem-
ory is certainly useful for knowing one’s address or telephone number, for
example, but comprehension is not necessarily a component of memoriza-
tion. A child who comprehends place value is intellectually different from
one who has simply memorized the algorithm for solving addition problems
that require regrouping.

.
Lev Vygotsky ww
w

Lev Vygotsky was a contemporary of Jean Piaget, and the two men agreed www.indiana.edu/~intell/
on many points about human growth and development. Whereas Vygotsky vygotsky.shtml
died as a young man, Piaget lived into his early eighties and thus produced A brief biographical sketch of
a greater volume of work. Moreover, Vygotsky’s work remained relatively Vygotsky and an explanation
unknown for many years, as U.S. scholars rarely read the work of Soviet of his most important ideas
.
ww
scholars during the cold war era. In more recent years, Vygotsky’s work has
been translated and studied, such that his sociocultural theory is now influ-
w

ential in our understanding of children. www.psy.pdx/PsiCafe/


Keytheorists/Vygotsky.htm
Vygotsky (1978) described learning as the construction of knowledge
Information about Vygotsky
within a social context. He believed that development could not be separated
and links to other sites about
from its social context and that learning could lead development. Vygotsky
related topics
viewed the learner as an active participant in constructing his own learning
within the context of interactions with caregivers, a family or community,
and a society. For example, a child in a society that depends on technology

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


15
will learn to think differently than a child in a society that depends on
agriculture.
Vygotsky also believed that language plays a central role in cognitive
development, as learning language influences the way a person thinks about
the world. For example, a child who learns the scientific language of clas-
sification will think differently about classification tasks than one who has
not learned such language. Vygotsky agreed with Piaget that learning is de-
pendent on the child’s development, but he believed that learning new strat-
egies (when presented at the appropriate level for the child) could lead to
increased development.
Bodrova and Leong (1996) describe a three-year-old child who is learn-
ing to classify objects but cannot keep the categories straight. When the
teacher helps her by providing two boxes, one labeled “big” and one labeled
“little,” the child is able to keep the categories straight. Learning to classify
in terms of big and little will aid the child’s development of categorical think-
ing. As summarized by Bodrova and Leong (1996):

The highest level of development is associated with the ability to perform and
self-regulate complex cognitive operations. Children cannot reach the level
through maturation or the accumulation of experiences with objects alone.
The emergence of this higher level of cognitive development depends on the
appropriation of tools through formal and informal instruction. (p. 19)

For early childhood educators, one of the most significant elements of


Vygotsky’s theory is the zone of proximal development (ZPD). The ZPD is
the gap between what the child can do independently and what he cannot do
even with the assistance of someone (such as an adult or peer) who is more
skilled than he is. For example, if a child can independently solve addition
problems involving regrouping, then he does not need instruction in solv-
ing these types of problems but could learn to solve subtraction problems
that require regrouping. However, he probably would not be able to solve
problems that involve long division, even with assistance. The skill range
in which the child can work successfully with some assistance is the ZPD.
Clearly, it is a waste of time to teach children what they already know as well
as what they cannot do, even with assistance.
NAEYC CRITERIA Piaget and Vygotsky agreed that learning involved “major, qualita-
3.61 tive transformations in thinking” (Berk and Winsler 1995, p. 111). And
even though Piaget and Vygotsky did not agree on other points, both of
their theories are valuable to early childhood educators. Namely, both can
help us understand that teachers must recognize the development of in-
dividual children, provide activities and experiences that will enhance
children’s thinking, and remember that all learning takes place in a social
context.

Multiple Intelligences Theory


.
ww Howard Gardner
w

www.thomasarmstrong.com/ The theories of Howard Gardner (1943– ) in explaining the growth of hu-
multiple_intelligences.htm man intelligence are very important to the work of early childhood teachers.
Information about multiple in- In his multiple intelligences theory, Gardner has identified nine intelli-
telligences and links to other gences that meet the strict criteria for being labeled as such. Gardner (1997a)
sites with related topics describes these multiple intelligences as follows:

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


16
.
1. Linguistic intelligence is the individual’s capacity to use language— ww

w
her native language and perhaps other languages—to express herself and to www.infed.org/thinkers/
understand other people. A poet really specializes in linguistic intelligence, gardner.htm
but any kind of writer, orator, speaker, lawyer, or person for whom language Information about Gardner and
is important to her trade demonstrates a high level of linguistic intelligence. his theories
2. Logical-mathematical intelligence is highly developed in someone
who understands the underlying principles of some kind of causal system—
the way a scientist or a logician does—or who can manipulate numbers,
quantities, and operations—the way a mathematician does.
3. Spatial intelligence refers to a person’s ability to represent the spa-
tial world internally in her mind—the way a sailor or airplane pilot navigates
the large spatial world or the way a chess player or sculptor represents a
more circumscribed spatial world. Spatial intelligence can be used in the
arts or in the sciences. If someone is spatially intelligent and oriented toward
the arts, she will more likely become a painter, sculptor, or architect than,
say, a musician or writer. Similarly, certain sciences, such as anatomy and
topology, require spatial intelligence skills.
4. Bodily-kinesthetic intelligence is the capacity to use the whole body
or parts of it—hands, fingers, arms—to solve a problem, make something,
or put on some kind of production. The most evident examples of people
with well-developed bodily-kinesthetic intelligence are those
in athletics or the performing arts, particularly dance and
drama. If we understand the mind
5. Musical intelligence is the capacity to think in mu- of the five-year-old we will
sical terms—to be able to hear patterns, recognize them, re- understand human nature.
member them, and perhaps manipulate them. People who
have a strong musical intelligence do not just remember —Howard Gardner
music easily—they cannot get it out of their minds, it is so
omnipresent. Some critics argue that musical ability is a talent, not an intel-
ligence. However, the genius of individuals such as Mozart and Beethoven
would seem to demonstrate the existence of musical intelligence.
6. Interpersonal intelligence is understanding other people. It is an
ability everyone needs, but it is vital to success for anyone who is a teacher,
clinician, salesperson, or politician. Anybody who deals with other people
has to be skilled in the interpersonal sphere.
7. Intrapersonal intelligence refers to a person’s understanding him-
self—knowing who he is, what he can do, what he wants to do, how he reacts
to things, which things to avoid, and which things to embrace. People are
drawn to individuals who have a good understanding of themselves because
they tend to be confident and successful. They know what they can and can-
not do, and they know where to go if they need help.
8. Naturalist intelligence is the human ability to discriminate among
living things (such as plants, animals) and to be sensitive to other features of
the natural world (such as clouds, rock formations). This ability was clearly
of value in humans’ evolutionary past as hunters, gatherers, and farmers;
it continues to be central in such roles as botanist and chef. Much of con-
sumer society also seems to exploit naturalist intelligence in discriminating
among goods such as cars, sneakers, kinds of makeup, and the like. The kind
of pattern recognition valued in certain of the sciences may also draw on
naturalist intelligence.

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


17
9. Existentialist intelligence is the most recent type of intelligence
proposed by Gardner. He describes this intelligence as the “proclivity to ask
fundamental questions about life: Who are we? Where do we come from?
Why do we die?” (1999). Questions such as these underlie much of religion,
art, science, and philosophy. Children are often intrigued by these questions,
as well.

In a classroom for young children in which the teacher believes that the
theory of multiple intelligences is a meaningful guide to making curriculum
decisions, the program would include various ways of learning material, but
lessons would not be repeated to address every type of intelligence. At least
once during each week, however, information would be presented through
each of the intelligences so that every child could learn through her stron-
gest skills some of the time. For example, if the children were investigat-
ing simple machines, they would take apart old appliances looking for the
simple machine parts, work in small groups to invent their own appliances
using simple machines, write descriptions of their appliances, and present
their appliances to the class and try to persuade others to buy them. On an-
other day, the children would learn about the life cycle of the frog through
watching a video, put together a puzzle depicting the life cycle, listen to a
recording of the sounds of various kinds of frogs, and examine the back legs
of a frog on a computer simulation that compares the frog’s leg muscles
to those of a human being, measuring the distance each can jump from a
standing start, and so on.
The significance of multiple intelligences theory for early childhood
teachers is that it gives them the opportunity to look for learning strengths
in all children and to justify providing a wide variety of learning experi-
ences so that each child will have the chance to learn through areas of
strength. Gardner explains that all learners need all the intelligences and
opportunities to grow intellectually in all areas. However, initial learning or
entry-point learning is most easily achieved through employing individuals’
personal areas of strength. In sum, by understanding and applying multiple
intelligences theory, teachers affirm that every child has abilities that can be
recognized and honored.

Social Learning Theory


.
ww Albert Bandura
w

www.psy.pdx.edu/PsiCafe/ Albert Bandura (1925– ) is one of the major theorists who describes
KeyTheorists/Bandura.htm social learning theory (Bandura 1977), which emphasizes imitation,
www.ship.edu/~cgboeree/ or social learning, in children’s development. This theory suggests that chil-
bandura.html
dren observe their caretakers performing certain actions and then imitate
Both have information about
those actions themselves. In more recent years, social learning theory has
Bandura and social learning
theory emphasized cognition, suggesting that children begin to think about what
they are imitating and become more selective about the elements in their en-
vironments that they choose to imitate (Bandura 2001). Although Bandura
comes from a behaviorist tradition, he believes that children take an active
role in their development.
The most important element of social learning theory for teachers is
the idea of modeling desired behaviors so that children will have an example
to follow. Such modeling is most prominent in the area of social develop-
ment. Teachers commonly model kindness, turn taking, and other behaviors

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


18
that they wish children to emulate. Even in the area of intellectual develop-
ment, teachers often try to model curiosity and interest so that children will
follow their example.

Ecological Systems Theory


.
Urie Bronfenbrenner ww

w
Urie Bronfenbrenner’s (1917–2005) ecological systems theory explains www.psy.pdx.edu/PsiCafe/
that a child develops within a complex system of relationships and that all KeyTheorists/Bronfenbrenner.
htm
these relationships are themselves influenced by multiple levels of the sur-
rounding environment (Bronfenbrenner and Evans 2000). For example, a Information about Bronfen-
brenner’s theories and links
child has a relationship with a parent, and that relationship is set within a
to other useful sites
family structure of some kind. In some families, the child and a single par-
ent make up the family. In other cases, the child and parent are set within a
family that consists of grandparents, aunts, uncles, cousins, and so on—an
extended family model.
However the family is structured, it is also set within various cultural
environments. The cultural milieu may expect that the parent has complete
responsibility for the child or that all the family or even the “village” is re-
sponsible for childrearing. The cultural group itself may be set in another
layer of environments. For instance, it might be the dominant culture in a
given area or a minority culture, such that its cultural beliefs are very differ-
ent from those of the dominate culture or even in direct conflict with them.
This layer also may be complicated by various laws (e.g., laws about what
constitutes child abuse), customs (e.g., circumcision), and values (e.g., at-
tending formal school or learning skills at home).
Further complicating the nature of ecological systems is the fact that
they are not static but dynamic, or always changing (Spencer and Schöner
2003; Thelen and Smith 1998). As children get older, they have varying de-
grees of opportunity to create their own settings and experiences. The dy-
namic systems view is that a child’s mind and body and physical and social
worlds form an integrated whole that guides the development of new skills
and knowledge. If a change occurs in one part of the system, then the system
must be reorganized so that the components work together again but in a
more complex manner. For example, a child of about eight may discover that
her parents do not know everything. This knowledge will change the way she
and her parents interact. They must learn to interact in new ways that reflect
the new knowledge.

Theoretical Influences on Teaching


P R AX I S
The theoretical stance a teacher takes will influence the way she plans and
implements instruction. In the following sections, we will examine how a Be able to plan instruction from
teacher might teach one-to-one correspondence from each theoretical point various theoretical points of view
of view.
One-to-one correspondence means that a child knows that each item
he is counting has only one number tag and that if two sets are equivalent
numerically, then the objects in one set can be matched to the objects in
the other set. An example that indicates that the concept is understood is
for the teacher to ask a child to pass out one piece of paper to each child in
the class. If the child can count the number of children and then count the
number of pieces of paper without having to physically match each piece of

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


19
paper to each child, then that child has mastered the concept of one-to-one
correspondence.
How will this concept be taught according to the various theories we
have discussed?

Behaviorist The teacher will first determine whether the child has a grasp
of the concept. If needed, the teacher will provide direct instruction by ask-
ing the child to connect one set of objects to another and determine that
the sets are equivalent. The teacher might supply a worksheet with a set of
dogs and a set of bones and ask the child to draw lines from the dogs to the
bones; the teacher would then tell the child that these sets have the same
number of items because there is one bone for each dog. In various ways,
the teacher will ask the child to practice matching sets until he can say that
sets are equivalent if there is one item in one set for each item in another set.
Rewards such as “happy faces” and tokens will be given each time the child
succeeds on the worksheet.

Maturationist The teacher will determine if the child is mature enough to


learn this concept by observing his physical development for the ability to
sit still long enough for instruction and his interest in counting and numbers
by observing how often he plays with magnetic numerals and other math
materials at choice time. Once the teacher decides that the child is ready for
this concept, she will place objects on a table that can be matched one to one
and ask the child to match them. Then she will explain that these sets are the
same numerically if there is one ball for each bat. This might be repeated
several times until the child can state that the sets are equivalent.

Constructivist Knowing that the child will have to develop his own under-
standing of one-to-one correspondence, the teacher will provide many dif-
ferent materials that the child can match into equivalent sets (e.g., dolls and
dollclothes, paintbrushes and paint containers, unit blocks and half units,
lids and jars, and so on). As the child plays with the materials, the teacher
will question him about what he is doing and ask him to explain what it
means if the sets are the same numerically.

Multiple Intelligences Knowing that some children would find mathemat-


ics more meaningful than others, the teacher would determine who under-
stood the concept of one-to-one correspondence and then set up situations
that would help children understand the concept. For example, a child who
learns best through visual means could be encouraged to create matching
sets in paintings; another might be encouraged to create matching move-
ments such as taking a step for each bounce of a ball; another might be en-
couraged to create two strings of musical notes that match in number; and
so on. However the child creates the sets, the teacher would help the child
make the connection from one set to the matching set. Such experiences
would be repeated until the child grasped the concept.

Social Learning Knowing that children learn by observing in a social con-


text, the teacher would model the concept of one-to-one correspondence
many times. This modeling would happen in many daily interactions. For
example, when passing out paper to each child the teacher would comment
that each child had one piece of paper so that the number of sheets of paper

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


20
was equal to the number of children. At snack time, similar modeling could
be done as each child got one serving of juice, one plate, one set of silver-
ware, and so on. After modeling many times, the teacher would expect the
child to begin noting the comparisons between sets when gathering materi-
als or creating sets that were equivalent in their cardinal numbers.

Ecological Systems The teacher will arrange an environment in which the


child can be taught the concept of one-to-one correspondence. The teacher
might demonstrate the concept several times and then ask the child to dem-
onstrate his understanding with a different set of materials. The teacher will
point out such correspondences in the school environment, ask the parents
to call attention to the concept at home, and demonstrate social applica-
tions, such as one voter, one vote in the classroom. Once the child can dem-
onstrate understanding of the concept, then the teacher will help him apply
the skill to counting and other mathematical experiences.

Theoretical Influences on Observations


The different theories of development provide points of view through which
to interpret observations of children as they grow and develop. Depending
on our theoretical orientation, we will look at examples of children’s devel-
opment differently. If a child is observed throwing a ball at a target, a be- NAEYC CRITERIA
haviorist might point out the reinforcements that make the child seek closer 4.12
approximations to the most effective throwing techniques. The maturation-
ist observing the same child might focus on the child’s physical maturity,
as indicated by her ability to grasp and release the ball appropriately. The
constructivist will view repeated attempts to hit the target as evidence that
the child is actively seeking information about velocity and angle of release
in order to hit the target (although the child will not be expected to verbalize
these concepts).
A child develops as a whole. Development in one area certainly influ-
ences development in other areas. For example, when a child becomes mo-
bile, he opens up many more possibilities for exploration and learning about
the environment. Children who feel that they are learning successfully or
who feel confident about their physical abilities develop more positive self-
esteem. Children who learn to control their impulsive behaviors may be able
to sustain interactions with people and materials longer than children who
do not, which affects their intellectual development. A child’s social, physi-
cal, emotional, and intellectual development are always interrelated.
.
ww
w

Children’s Development www.pbs.org/wholechild/abc/


physical.html
This section provides brief overviews of patterns in children’s physical, so- Milestones in physical devel-
cial, emotional, and intellectual development. Table 1 summarizes and gives opment for the typically devel-
oping child
examples of these patterns.
.
ww
w

Physical Development www.childdevelopmentinfo.


com/health_safety/
Patterns of Development physical_development.shtml
Physical development is orderly, not random. Infant development is marked Information about the physical
by the change from undifferentiated mass activity to controlled activity. It development of children

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


21
TABLE 1 Overview of Patterns in Children’s Development
BIRTH TO THREE THREE TO FOUR FIVE TO SIX SEVEN TO EIGHT
YEARS OLD YEARS OLD YEARS OLD YEARS OLD
Physical Physical skills Physical skills increase Skips on alternate feet Physical skills become im-
Development develop rapidly portant in self-concept
Rides a tricycle Rides two-wheel bike
Sits and crawls Energy level is high
Walks up and down Skates
Walks and begins stairs, alternating feet Rate of growth slows
to run Throws fairly accurately
Runs Fine muscle control is
Fine motor skills Catches ball with hands good; can form letters
develop; can Jumps with both feet well
Turns somersaults
stack and pick up Walks on balance beam
small objects Participates in games Permanent teeth appear
Climbs on playground requiring physical skills Body proportions, facial
Manages spoon equipment
or fork for Small muscle development structure change
feeding Undresses and dresses increases; eye–hand More mature throwing and
self coordination develops catching pattern
Grasps and
releases objects Catches ball with Fine muscle control in- Increases accuracy in
arms extended creases; can use hammer, throwing and kicking
pencil, scissors, etc.
Walks backward and
on tiptoe Copies geometric figures
Holds crayon Cuts on lines
with fingers
Prints some letters
Pastes and glues
Begins to lose teeth
Handedness is well
established

Social Responds to Becomes more aware Expresses rigid ideas Is more competitive
Development others of self about sex roles with peers
Enjoys company Develops more altruistic Has best friends but for Depends on parents for
of other children feelings short periods of time expansion of interests,
activities
Can maintain Becomes aware of Quarrels often but anger
involvement with racial/ethnic and is short lived Is influenced by peer
another for a sexual differences opinions
very short period Is able to share and take
Is able to take direc- turns Plays with opposite sex
Is unable to tion, follow some rules less often
share without Is eager to participate
coaxing Has strong feelings to- in school experiences Needs teacher approval
ward home and family
Shows very Considers teacher very Is able to share
little ability Shows a growing sense important
of self-reliance Wants to please
to postpone Wants to be first
gratification Parallel play is common; Is more independent at
cooperative play begins Becomes possessive work and play
Imitates actions
of others Imaginary playmates are Forms more enduring
fairly common friendships
Begins to engage
in parallel play Peer groups begin to form

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


22
TABLE 1 Continued
BIRTH TO THREE THREE TO FOUR FIVE TO SIX SEVEN TO EIGHT
YEARS OLD YEARS OLD YEARS OLD YEARS OLD
Emotional Cannot tolerate Can tolerate some Expresses and labels Expresses reactions to
Development frustration frustration feelings others
Cries easily Develops some Controls aggression Is sensitive to ridicule
self-control better and criticism
Is often un-
able to control Appreciates surprises Expresses less concern Expresses more worries:
impulses and novel events when separated from war, loss of parents
parents
Begins to express Begins to show sense Shows more persistence
affection of humor Expresses sense of hu-
mor in jokes, nonsense Expresses more empathy;
Needs routines Needs overt expres- words is able to see others’
and security sions of affection viewpoints
Learns right from wrong
Begins to per- Fears the dark, being
ceive emotions abandoned, strange Develops a conscience
of others situations
Expresses self,
sometimes
emphatically

Cognitive Sensorimotor Follows instructions Shows a growing atten- Differences in reading


Development investigation of of two commands tion span and language abilities
environment is widen
predominant Makes impulsive judg- Is able to seriate
ments and frequent objects Transition to concrete
Development of mistakes operational thinking
concepts is rapid Is able to group objects begins
Develops vocabulary
Develops a rapidly Is more deliberate, less Talking and discussion
sense of object impulsive in judgments are important
permanence Uses numbers without
understanding Differentiates between Is able to plan
Develops fantasy and reality
language Has difficulty differ- Can sustain interest over
entiating fantasy and Uses language aggres- long periods of time
May use some reality sively, in categorization
number and color Begins to understand
Begins to classify, Is aware that words and cause and effect
words but may pictures represent real
not understand especially by function
objects Develops a growing
them Begins to use some understanding of time,
functional abstract Becomes interested in money
words numbers and letters
Uses slang and profanity
“Why?” questions are Knows names of colors
constant Understands and uses
Does not spontane- more abstract terms
Thinking is very ously use rehearsal in
egocentric memory tasks Expresses more aware-
ness of community,
Follows three unrelated world
commands
May begin to conserve
number, length

Sources: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development 1975; Berk 2006; Tudor 1981.

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


23
Gross motor development is
aided by providing play activi-
ties that involve climbing, jump-
ing, and so on.

is easy to observe undifferentiated mass activity in an infant. If she is ex-


cited, her whole body moves and her arms and legs flail. Gradually, the baby
becomes more capable of differentiated movements, such as deliberately
reaching for and grasping a rattle.
Movements that become controlled and deliberate also become orga-
nized into patterns, such as pulling oneself to a standing position, releasing
the hands, and moving the legs and feet to walk. The patterns then become
available to the child as possible responses to different situations. If the
child wants a toy that is across the room, at first the only option for getting
there is scooting and rocking. As development progresses, crawling and fi-
nally walking and running become patterns of physical movement available
to the child.
The physical development of children in the infant stage proceeds rap-
idly. The child learns to control head position, to grasp objects, and perhaps
to stand and walk in the first year. As children grow, the development of
their motor skills is not quite as rapid as it is in infancy, but it continues
throughout childhood.
Observations of physical development reveal that growth is cephalo-
caudal (it proceeds from head to tail) and proximodistal (it proceeds from
the center of the body outward) and that gross motor movements are devel-
oped before fine motor movements. Control of the head and arm muscles
is achieved before control of the leg muscles. Similarly, children are able to
control the muscles of their arms before they can control the fine muscles
in their hands that are needed for tasks such as writing and cutting with
scissors.
The rate of children’s physical development is variable and related to
environmental features such as nutrition and the freedom to practice move-
ments. Some behaviors, such as walking, tend to emerge at about the same
time in children, even if they have been confined as infants; others, such as
throwing, seem to depend more on practice opportunities. Most children
are encouraged to practice their developing skills through interactions in
daily life. The parent may encourage the child to progress from letting go of

Young Children Growing, Thinking, and Learning


24
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
every man has his own. The word is said to be derived from Sanscrit
and to be etymologically identical with Avatar, the Dyaks regularly
substituting p or b for v. See Rev. J. Perham, op. cit. pp. 133 sqq.; H.
Ling Roth’s Natives of Sarawak and British North Borneo, i. 168 sqq.
48.1 H. Ling Roth, “Low’s Natives of Borneo,” Journal of the
Anthropological Institute, xxi. (1892) pp. 113 sq., 133; compare id.,
ibid. xxii. (1893) p. 24.
48.2 Spenser St. John, Life in the Forests of the Far East, Second
Edition (London, 1863), i. 63 sq.
49.1 Hugh Low, Sarawak (London, 1848), pp. 300 sq.
50.1 Charles Hose and William McDougall, The Pagan Tribes of
Borneo (London, 1912), ii. 196-199.
50.2 Charles Brooke, Ten Years in Sarawak (London, 1866), i. 69
sq.
51.1 A. W. Nieuwenhuis, Quer durch Borneo (Leyden, 1904-1907),
i. 367.
51.2 M. T. H. Perelaer, Ethnographische Beschrijving der Dajaks
(Zalt-Bommel, 1870), pp. 59 sq.
51.3 A. W. Nieuwenhuis, Quer durch Borneo, ii. 99; id., In Centraal
Borneo (Leyden, 1900), ii. 278.
51.4 A. H. F. J. Nusselein, “Beschrijving van het landschap Pasir,”
Bijdragen tot de Taal- Land- en Volkenkunde van Nederlandsch-
Indië, lviii. (1905) p. 538.
51.5 A. Bastian, Indonesien, i. (Berlin, 1884) p. 144.
52.1 G. A. Wilken, Verspreide Geschriften (The Hague, 1912), ii.
335 (“Huwelijken tusschen bloedverwanten,” p. 26).
52.2 B. F. Matthes, “Over de âdá’s of gewoonten der Makassaren
en Boegineezen,” Verslagen en Mededeelingen der Koninklijke
Akademie van Wetenschappen, Afdeeling Letterkunde, Derde
Reeks, ii. (Amsterdam, 1885) p. 182.
52.3 Digest, xlviii. 9.9, “Poena parricidii more majorum haec
instituta est, ut parricida virgis sanguineis verberatus deinde culleo
insuatur cum cane, gallo gallinaceo et vipera et simia: deinde in
mare profundum culleus jactatur.” Compare Valerius Maximus, i. 1.
13; Professor J. E. B. Mayor’s note on Juvenal, viii. 214. If the view
suggested above is correct, the scourging of the criminal to the
effusion of blood (virgis sanguineis verberatus) must have been a
later addition to the original penalty, unless indeed some provision
were made for catching the blood before it fell on the ground.
53.1 A. C. Kruijt, “Eenige aanteekeningen omtrent de Toboengkoe
en de Tomori,” Mededeelingen van wege het Nederlandsche
Zendelinggenootschap, xliv. (1900) p. 235.
53.2 A. C. Kruijt, “Van Posso naar Mori,” Mededeelingen van wege

het Nederlandsche Zendelinggenootschap, xliv. (1900) p. 162.


53.3 N. Adriani en Alb. C. Kruijt, De Bare’e-sprekende Toradja’s van

Midden-Celebes, i. (Batavia, 1912) p. 187.


54.1 Hissink, “Nota van toelichting, betreffende de zelbesturende

landschappen Paloe, Dolo, Sigi, en Beromaroe,” Tijdschrift voor


Indische Taal- Land- en Volkenkunde, liv. (1912), p. 115.
54.2 M. J. van Baarda, “Fabelen, Verhalen en Overleveringen der
Galelareezen,” Bijdragen tot de Taal- Land- en Volkenkunde van
Nederlandsch-Indië, xlv. (1895) p. 514. In a letter to me of 14th
March 1909 Sir John Rhŷs compares a Welsh expression, “Rain
through sunshine, the devil going on his wife.” He adds: “I do not
think I ever heard it except when it was actually raining during
sunshine. I can now see that instead of ar i wraig the original must
have been ar i fam ‘on his mother.’ In fact I am not at all sure but that
I have heard it so.”
54.3 F. S. A. de Clerq, Bijdragen tot de kennis der Residentie
Ternate (Leyden, 1890), p. 132.
55.1 O. Dapper, Description de l’Afrique (Amsterdam, 1686), p.
326; R. E. Dennett, At the Back of the Black Man’s Mind (London,
1906), pp. 53, 67-71.
56.1 R. E. Dennett, op. cit. p. 52.
56.2 A. C. Hollis, The Nandi, their Language and Folk-lore (Oxford,
1909), p. 76.
56.3 Rev. E. Casalis, The Basutos (London, 1861), p. 252.
56.4 Rev. E. Casalis, The Basutos, p. 267. The writer tells us (pp.

255 sq.) that “death with all that immediately precedes or follows it, is
in the eyes of these people the greatest of all defilements. Thus the
sick, persons who have touched or buried a corpse, or who have dug
the grave, individuals who inadvertently walk over or sit upon a
grave, the near relatives of a person deceased, murderers, warriors
who have killed their enemies in battle, are all considered impure.”
No doubt all such persons would also be prohibited from handling
the corn.
57.1 Edward Westermarck, Ceremonies and Beliefs connected with
Agriculture, Certain Dates of the Solar Year, and the Weather in
Morocco (Helsingfors, 1913), p. 46.
57.2 E. Westermarck, op. cit. p. 54; compare pp. 17, 23, 47.
57.3 C. G. Seligmann, s.v. “Dinka,” in Dr. J. Hastings’s
Encyclopaedia of Religion and Ethics, iv. (Edinburgh, 1911) p. 709.
57.4 Henri A. Junod, “Les conceptions physiologiques des Bantou
Sud-Africains et leurs tabous,” Revue d’ Ethnographie et de
Sociologie, i. (1910) p. 146 note 2.
59.1 Henri A. Junod, The Life of a South African Tribe (Neuchatel,

1912-1913), ii. 60-62.


59.2 A. van Gennep, Tabou et Totémisme à Madagascar (Paris,

1904), pp. 342 sq., quoting the evidence of M. Gabriel Ferrand.


Similar testimony was given to me verbally by M. Ferrand at Paris,
19th April, 1910. Compare Gabriel Ferrand, Les Musulmans à
Madagascar et aux Iles Comores, Deuxième Partie (Paris, 1893),
pp. 20 sq.
60.1 In Fiji the rite of circumcision used to be followed by sexual
orgies in which brothers and sisters appear to have been
intentionally coupled. See Rev. Lorimer Fison, “The Nanga, or
Sacred Stone Enclosure of Wainimala, Fiji,” Journal of the
Anthropological Institute, xiv. (1885) pp. 27-30, with the note of Sir
Edward B. Tylor on pp. 28 sq.; Totemism and Exogamy, ii. 145-148.
Such periods of general licence accorded to the whole community
are perhaps best explained as temporary revivals of an old custom
of sexual communism. But this explanation seems scarcely
applicable to cases like those cited in the text, where the licence is
not granted to the whole people but enjoined on a few individuals
only in special circumstances. As to other apparent cases of
reversion to primitive sexual communism, see Totemism and
Exogamy, i. 311 sqq.
60.2 Job xxxi. 11 sq. (Revised Version).
60.3 ‫ְּת כּןָא ה‬. See Hebrew and English Lexicon, by F. Brown, S. R.

Driver, and Ch. A. Briggs (Oxford, 1906), p. 100.


61.1 Genesis xii. 10-20, xx. 1-18.
61.2 Leviticus xviii. 24 sq.
61.3 Sophocles, Oedipus Tyrannus, 22 sqq., 95 sqq.
61.4 Tacitus, Annals, xii. 4 and 8.
62.1 See The Magic Art and the Evolution of Kings, i. 12, 14 sqq.
62.2 G. Keating, History of Ireland, translated by J. O’Mahony (New
York, 1857), pp. 337 sq.; P. W. Joyce, Social History of Ancient
Ireland (London, 1903), ii. 512 sq.
62.3 “Corc means croppy or cropped: in this instance the name
refers to the bearer’s ears, and the verb used as to the action of his
brother maiming him is ro-chorc.”
63.1 (Sir) John Rhŷs, Celtic Heathendom (London and Edinburgh,

1888), pp. 308 sq., referring to the Book of the Dun, 54a.
64.1 Laws of Manu, viii. 371 sq., translated by G. Bühler, pp. 318
sq. (Sacred Books of the East, vol. xxv.). Compare Gautama, xxiii.
14 sq., translated by G. Bühler, p. 285 (Sacred Books of the East,
vol. ii.).
64.2 Code of Hammurabi, §§ 129, 157, C. H. W. Johns, Babylonian
and Assyrian Laws, Contracts and Letters (Edinburgh, 1904), pp. 54,
56; Robert W. Rogers, Cuneiform Parallels to the Old Testament
(Oxford, preface dated 1911), pp. 427, 434.
64.3 Deuteronomy xxii. 22.
64.4 Deuteronomy xxii. 20 sq.
64.5 Leviticus xxi. 9.
64.6 Leviticus xx. 14.
65.1 Rev. J. Roscoe, The Baganda (London, 1911), pp. 261 sq.
65.2 Rev. J. Roscoe, op. cit. p. 262. As to the totemic clans, see id.
pp. 133 sqq. One clan (the Lung-fish clan) was excepted from the
rule.
65.3 Sir Harry Johnston, The Uganda Protectorate (London, 1904),
ii. 719.
66.1 Sir Harry Johnston, op. cit. ii. 746 sq.
66.2 A. C. Hollis, The Nandi (Oxford, 1909), p. 76.
66.3 Werner Munzinger, Ostafrikanische Studien (Schaffhausen,

1864), p. 243.
66.4 W. Munzinger, op. cit. p. 322. However, the child of an

unmarried slave woman is brought up; the father pays for its nurture.
66.5 H. S. Stannus, “Notes on some Tribes of British Central

Africa,” Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, xl. (1910) p.


290.
67.1 Cullen Gouldsbury and Hubert Sheane, The Great Plateau of
Northern Rhodesia (London, 1911), p. 57.
67.2 Peter Kolben, The Present State of the Cape of Good Hope,
Second Edition (London, 1738), i. 157. For more examples of the
death penalty inflicted for breaches of sexual morality in Africa, see
A. H. Post, Afrikanische Jurisprudenz (Olbenburg and Leipsic, 1887),
ii. 69 sqq.
68.1 G. J. van Dongen, “De Koeboes,” Bijdragen tot de Taal- Land-
en Volkenkunde van Nederlandsch-Indië, lxiii. (1910) p. 293.
68.2 R. van Eck, “Schetsen van het eiland Bali,” Tijdschrift voor
Nederlandsch Indië, Nieuwe Serie, viii. (1879) pp. 370 sq.; Julius
Jacobs, Eenigen Tijd onder de Baliërs (Batavia, 1883), p. 126.
68.3 See above, pp. 52 sq.
68.4 Hoorweg, “Nota bevattende eenige gegevens betreffende het
landschap Mamoedjoe,” Tijdschrift voor Indische Taal- Land- en
Volkenkunde, lxiii. (1911) p. 95.
68.5 G. A. Wilken, Verspreide Geschriften (The Hague, 1912), ii.
481.
69.1 J. S. G. Gramberg, “Schets der Kesam, Semendo, Makakauw
en Blalauw,” Tijdschrift voor Indische Taal- Land- en Volkenkunde,
xv. (1866) pp. 456-458. Compare G. G. Batten, Glimpses of the
Eastern Archipelago (Singapore, 1894), pp. 105 sq.
69.2 G. A. Wilken, Verspreide Geschriften, ii. 481 sq.
69.3 Franz Junghuhn, Die Battaländer auf Sumatra (Berlin, 1847),
ii. 147, 156 sq.
70.1 A. R. Wallace, The Malay Archipelago, Sixth Edition (London,
1877), pp. 173 sq.
71.1 See above, pp. 46-54.
72.1 James Dawson, Australian Aborigines (Melbourne, Sydney,
and Adelaide, 1881), p. 28.
73.1 A. W. Howitt, Native Tribes of South-East Australia (London,
1904), pp. 222-224.
74.1 Walter E. Roth, Ethnological Studies among the North-West-
Central Queensland Aborigines (Brisbane and London, 1897), p.
181.
74.2 A. W. Howitt, Native Tribes of South-East Australia, pp. 264,

266.
74.3 A. W. Howitt, Native Tribes of South-East Australia, pp. 246
sq.
74.4 Mrs. Daisy M. Bates, “The Marriage Laws and some Customs
of the West Australian Aborigines,” Victorian Geographical Journal,
xxiii.-xxiv. (1905-1906) p. 42. The statement in the text was made by
a settler who had lived in the Tableland district, inland from
Roeburne, for twenty years.
75.1 A. W. Howitt, Native Tribes of South-East Australia, p. 208.
Similarly among tribes on the Hunter River “a man is not permitted to
speak to his wife’s mother, but can do so through a third party. In
former days it was death to speak to her, but now a man doing so is
only severely reprimanded and has to leave the camp for a certain
time” (A. W. Howitt, op. cit. p. 267).
75.2 See for example (Sir) E. B. Tylor, “On a method of

investigating the Development of Institutions,” Journal of the


Anthropological Institute, xviii. (1889) pp. 246-248; Salomon
Reinach, “Le Gendre et la Belle-Mère,” L’Anthropologie, xxii. (1911)
pp. 649-662; id., Cultes, Mythes et Religions, iv. (Paris, 1912) pp.
130-147.
75.3 In Totemism and Exogamy (Index, s.vv. “Avoidance” and
“Mother-in-law”) will be found a collection of examples. In what
follows I abstain for the most part from citing instances which have
been adduced by me before.
76.1 Rev. John H. Weeks, Among Congo Cannibals (London,
1913), pp. 133 sq. Compare id., “Anthropological Notes on the
Bangala of the Upper Congo,” Journal of the Royal Anthropological
Institute, xl. (1910) pp. 367 sq.
77.1 Father M. A. Condon, “Contribution to the Ethnography of the

Basoga-Batamba, Uganda Protectorate,” Anthropos, vi. (1911) pp.


377 sq.
78.1 C. W. Hobley, Ethnology of A-Kamba and other East African
Tribes (Cambridge, 1910), pp. 103, 104.
78.2 Father Eugene Hurel, “Religion et vie domestique des
Bakerewe,” Anthropos, vi. (1911) p. 287.
79.1 Father Picarda, “Autour du Mandera, Notes sur l’Ouzigoua,
l’Oukwéré et l’Oudoé (Zanquebar),” Les Missions Catholiques, xviii.
(1886) p. 286.
79.2 H. S. Stannus, “Notes on Some Tribes of British Central
Africa,” Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, xl. (1910) p.
307.
79.3 H. S. Stannus, op. cit. p. 309.
79.4 Cullen Gouldsbury and Hubert Sheane, The Great Plateau of
Northern Rhodesia (London, 1911), p. 259.
79.5 “The Angoni-Zulus,” British Central Africa Gazette, No. 86,
April 30th, 1898, p. 2.
80.1 Henri A. Junod, Les Ba-Ronga (Neuchâtel, 1898), pp. 79 sq.;
id., The Life of a South African Tribe (Neuchatel, 1912-1913), i. 230-
232.
80.2 Henri A. Junod, The Life of a South African Tribe, i. 239.
81.1 Hermann Tönjes, Ovamboland, Land, Leute, Mission (Berlin,
1911), p. 133.
81.2 A. C. Hollis, “A Note on the Masai System of Relationship and
other Matters connected therewith,” Journal of the Royal
Anthropological Institute, xl. (1910) p. 481.
81.3 Werner Munzinger, Sitten und Recht der Bogos (Winterthur,
1859), p. 63.
81.4 G. Casati, Ten Years in Equatoria (London and New York,
1891), i. 69.
81.5 Travels of an Arab Merchant [Mohammed Ibn Omar El-Tounsy]
in Soudan, abridged from the French by Bayle St. John (London,
1854), pp. 97 sq.
82.1 J. Kreemer, “De Loeboes in Mandailing,” Bijdragen tot de Taal-

Land- en Volkenkunde van Nederlandsch-Indië, lxvi. (1912) p. 324.


82.2 Stefan Lehner, “Bukaua,” in R. Neuhauss’s Deutsch Neu-

Guinea (Berlin, 1911), iii. 426 sq.


83.1 J. Baegert, “An Account of the Aboriginal Inhabitants of the

Californian Peninsula,” Annual Report of the Board of Regents of the


Smithsonian Institution for the year 1863, p. 368. This and the
following American cases have already been cited by me in
Totemism and Exogamy, iv. 314 sq.
83.2 Alvar Nuñez Cabeça de Vaca, Relation et Naufrages (Paris,
1837), pp. 109 sq. (in Ternaux-Compans’ Voyages, Relations, et
Mémoires originaux pour servir à l’Histoire de la Découverte de
l’Amérique). The original of this work was published in Spanish at
Valladolid in 1555.
83.3 Brasseur de Bourbourg, Histoire des Nations civilisées du
Mexique et de l’Amérique-Centrale (Paris, 1857-1859), ii. 52 sq.
83.4 G. Klemm, Allgemeine Culturgeschichte der Menschheit
(Leipsic, 1843-1852), ii. 77.
83.5 J. B. du Tertre, Histoire generale des Isles de S. Christophe,
de la Guadeloupe, de la Martinique et autres dans l’Amerique (Paris,
1654), p. 419. A similar, but rather briefer, account of the custom is
given by De la Borde, who may have borrowed from Du Tertre. See
De la Borde, “Relation de l’origine, mœurs, coustumes, réligion,
guerres et voyages des Caraibes, sauvages des Isles Antilles de
l’Amerique,” p. 56 (in Recueil de divers Voyages faits en Afrique et
en l’Amerique qui n’ont pas esté encore publiez, Paris, 1684).
84.1 Edmond Reuel Smith, The Araucanians (London, 1855), p.
217.
84.2 We have met with a custom of avoidance between father and
daughter among the Akamba (above, p. 78). For more examples see
Totemism and Exogamy, Index, s.v. “Avoidance,” vol. iv. p. 326.
85.1 Among those who incline more or less definitely to accept this
view are the late Dr. A. W. Howitt (“Notes on some Australian Class
Systems,” Journal of the Anthropological Institute, xii. (1883) pp. 502
sq.), Dr. R. H. Codrington (see below, p. 86), M. Joustra (see below,
p. 85), and the Rev. J. H. Weeks (see above, p. 76). Three of these
writers are experienced missionaries who are only concerned to
record the facts, and have no theories to maintain.
85.2 Totemism and Exogamy, ii. 188 sq. The authority for these
statements is M. Joustra, “Het leven, de zeden en gewoonten der
Bataks,” Mededeelingen van wege het Nederlandsche
Zendelinggenootschap, xlvi. (1902) pp. 391 sq.
86.1 R. H. Codrington, D.D., The Melanesians (Oxford, 1891), p.
232.
87.1 R. H. Codrington, op. cit. p. 43.
88.1 Max Girschner, “Die Karolineninsel Námōluk und ihre
Bewohner,” Baessler-Archiv, ii. (1912) p. 164.
90.1 P. G. Peckel, “Die Verwandtschaftsnamen des mittleren
Neumecklenburg,” Anthropos, iii. (1908) pp. 467, 470 sq.
90.2 P. G. Peckel, op. cit. pp. 463, 467.
90.3 Rev. J. Roscoe, The Baganda (London, 1911), pp. 128 sq.,
131; Sir Harry Johnston, The Uganda Protectorate (London, 1904),
ii. 695. The latter writer says generally: “Cousins cannot enter the
same house, and must not eat out of the same dish. A man cannot
marry his cousin.” But from Mr. Roscoe’s researches it appears that
a man has only to avoid certain cousins, called kizibwewe, that is,
the daughters either of his father’s sisters or of his mother’s brothers.
91.1 Rev. J. Roscoe, op. cit. p. 129. Among the women with whom

man was forbidden to have sexual relations under pain of death


were (besides his cousins mentioned above) his father’s sister, his
daughter, and his wife’s sister’s daughter. See J. Roscoe, op. cit. pp.
131, 132. The reason alleged for avoiding a mother-in-law, namely,
because a man has seen her daughter’s nakedness (compare
above, p. 76) is probably a later misinterpretation of the custom.
91.2 G. McCall Theal, Records of South-Eastern Africa, vii. (1901)
pp. 431, 432. The writer adds: “Among the tribes within the Cape
Colony at the present time the differences are as follows:—
“Xosas, Tembus, and Pondos: marry no relative by blood, however
distant, on either father’s or mother’s side.
“Hlubis and others commonly called Fingos: may marry the
daughter of mother’s brother and other relatives on that side, but not
on father’s side.
“Basuto, Batlaro, Batlapin, and Barolong: very frequently marry
cousins on father’s side, and know of no restrictions beyond actual
sisters.”
92.1 Henri A. Junod, The Life of a South African Tribe (Neuchatel,
1912-1913), i. 243-245. As to the rules concerning the marriage of
cousins in this tribe, see id. i. 241 sq.
92.2 Heinrich Claus, Die Wagogo (Leipsic and Berlin, 1911), p. 58.
93.1 C. W. Hobley, “Kikuyu Customs and Beliefs,” Journal of the
Royal Anthropological Institute, xl. (1910) p. 438.
94.1 See above, pp. 78 sq., 81.
94.2 See above, pp. 80, 81, 84.
94.3 See above, p. 81.
94.4 See above, pp. 44 sqq.
94.5 See below, pp. 102 sqq.
95.1 On the question of the effect of inbreeding see Totemism and
Exogamy, iv. 160 sqq.
95.2 A. H. Huth, The Marriage of Near Kin considered with respect
to the Laws of Nations, the Results of Experience, and the
Teachings of Biology, Second Edition (London, 1887).
96.1 J. Arthur Thomson, article “Consanguinity,” in Dr. James
Hastings’s Encyclopaedia of Religion and Ethics, iv. (Edinburgh,
1911) p. 30.
96.2 André Thevet, La Cosmographie Universelle (Paris, 1575), ii.
933 [967].
97.1 Father P. Schumacher, “Das Eherecht in Ruanda,” Anthropos,
vii. (1912) p. 4.
97.2 H. H. Milman, History of Latin Christianity, New Impression
(London, 1903), ii. 54.
97.3 These particulars as to the Slavonic peoples of the Balkan
peninsula I take from a letter with which Miss M. Edith Durham, one
of our best authorities on these races, was so good as to favour me.
Her letter is dated 116a King Henry’s Road, London, N.W., October
16th, 1909. The stoning of the betrothed couple near Cattaro is
recorded, so Miss Durham tells me, in a Servian book, Narodne
Pripovjetke i Presude, by Vuk Vrcević. For many more examples of
the death penalty and other severe punishments inflicted for sexual
offences, see E. Westermarck, The Origin and Development of Moral
Ideas (London, 1906-1908), ii. 366 sqq., 425 sqq.
98.1 F. S. Krauss, Sitte und Brauch der Südslaven (Vienna, 1885),
pp. 209, 216, 217. Compare F. Demelić, Le Droit Coutumier des
Slaves Méridionaux (Paris, 1876), p. 76.
98.2 F. S. Krauss, op. cit. pp. 208-212, citing as his authority Vuk
Vrčević, Niz srpskih pripovijedaka, pp. 129-137.
98.3 F. S. Krauss, Sitte und Brauch der Südslaven, p. 204.
99.1 For examples of the attempt to multiply edible plants in this
fashion, see The Magic Art and the Evolution of Kings, ii. 97 sqq.
The reported examples of similar attempts to assist the multiplication
of animals seem to be rarer. For some instances see George Catlin,
O-Kee-Pa, a Religious Ceremony and other Customs of the
Mandans (London, 1867), Folium Reservatum, pp. i.-iii.
(multiplication of buffaloes); History of the Expedition under the
Command of Captains Lewis and Clark to the Sources of the
Missouri (London, 1905), i. 209 sq. (multiplication or attraction of
buffaloes); Maximilian Prinz zu Wied, Reise in das innere Nord-
America (Coblentz, 1839-1841), ii. 181, 263-267 (multiplication or
attraction of buffaloes); Reports of the Cambridge Anthropological
Expedition to Torres Straits, v. (1904) p. 271 (multiplication of
turtles); J. Roscoe, “Further Notes on the Manners and Customs of
the Baganda,” Journal of the Anthropological Institute, xxxii. (1902)
p. 53; id., The Baganda (London, 1911), p. 144 (multiplication of
edible green locusts); S. Gason, in Journal of the Anthropological
Institute, xxiv. (1895) p. 174 (multiplication of edible rats); id., “The
Dieyerie Tribe,” in Native Tribes of South Australia (Adelaide, 1879),
p. 280 (multiplication of dogs and snakes).
100.1 I have given my reasons for thinking so elsewhere (The
Magic Art and the Evolution of Kings, i. 220 sqq.).
103.1 Rev. J. Roscoe, The Baganda (London, 1911), p. 262.
103.2 Rev. J. Roscoe, op. cit. p. 55. Compare id., “Further Notes on
the Manners and Customs of the Baganda,” Journal of the
Anthropological Institute, xxxii. (1902) p. 39.
103.3 Rev. J. Roscoe, The Baganda, p. 262.
103.4 Rev. J. Roscoe, op. cit. pp. 72, 102.
104.1 Cullen Gouldsbury and Hubert Sheane, The Great Plateau of

Northern Rhodesia (London, 1911), pp. 57, 178.


104.2 Henri A. Junod, “Les Conceptions Physiologiques des Bantou

Sud-Africains et leurs Tabous,” Revue d’Ethnographie et de


Sociologie, i. (1910) p. 150; id., The Life of a South African Tribe
(Neuchatel, 1912-1913), i. 38 sq.
105.1 Henri A. Junod, “Les Conceptions Physiologiques des

Bantous Sud-Africains et leurs Tabous,” Revue d’Ethnographie et de


Sociologie, i. (1910) p. 150; id., The Life of a South African Tribe, i.
194 sq.
105.2 C. W. Hobley, “Kikuyu Customs and Beliefs,” Journal of the
Royal Anthropological Institute, xl. (1910) p. 433. A similar state of
ceremonial pollution (thahu) is supposed by the Akikuyu to arise on
many other occasions, which are enumerated by Mr. Hobley (op. cit.
pp. 428-440). See further below, p. 115, note 5.
105.3 H. S. Stannus, “Notes on some Tribes of British Central

Africa,” Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, xl. (1910) p.


305. Compare R. C. F. Maugham, Zambezia (London, 1910), p. 326.
105.4 Max Weiss, Die Völkerstämme im Norden Deutsch-Ostafrikas
(Berlin, 1910), p. 385.
105.5 C. W. Hobley, Ethnology of A-Kamba and other East African
Tribes (Cambridge, 1910), p. 61.
106.1 C. W. Hobley, op. cit. p. 103.
106.2 A. Karasek, “Beiträge zur Kenntniss der Waschambaa,”
Baessler-Archiv, i. (1911) p. 186.
106.3 P. Reichard, Deutsch Ostafrika (Leipsic, 1892), p. 427; H.

Cole, “Notes on the Wagogo of German East Africa,” Journal of the


Anthropological Institute, xxxii. (1902) pp. 318 sq.; A. D’Orbigny,
Voyage dans l’Amérique méridionale, iii. Part i. (Paris and Strasburg,
1844) p. 226; Ivan Petroff, Report on the Population, Industries, and
Resources of Alaska, p. 155.
106.4 C. Lumholtz, Unknown Mexico (London, 1903), ii. 128 sq.
106.5 De Flacourt, Histoire de la Grande Isle Madagascar (Paris,
1658), pp. 97 sq. Compare John Struys, Voiages and Travels
(London, 1684), p. 22; Abbé Rochon, Voyage to Madagascar and
the East Indies, translated from the French (London, 1792), pp. 46
sq.
107.1 Rev. J. Roscoe, The Baganda (London, 1911), pp. 352, 362,
363, sq.
107.2 Rev. John H. Weeks, “Anthropological Notes on the Bangala

of the Upper Congo River,” Journal of the Royal Anthropological


Institute, xl. (1910) p. 413; id., Among Congo Cannibals (London,
1913), p. 224.
107.3 J. R. Swanton, “Contributions to the Ethnology of the Haida,”
p. 56 (The Jesup North Pacific Expedition, Memoir of the American
Museum of Natural History, vol. v. Part i., Leyden and New York,
1905).
107.4 2 Samuel xi.
108.1 “Mr. Farewell’s Account of Chaka, the King of Natal,”
Appendix to W. F. W. Owen’s Narrative of Voyages to explore the
Shores of Africa, Arabia, and Madagascar (London, 1833), ii. 395.
108.2 L. Alberti, De Kaffers (Amsterdam, 1810), p. 171.
108.3 C. Wunenberger, “La Mission et le Royaume de Humbé, sur
les bords du Cunène,” Les Missions Catholiques, xx. (1888), p. 262.
108.4 J. B. Labat, Relation historique de l’Éthiopie occidentale
(Paris, 1732), i. 259 sq.
109.1 Proyart, “History of Loango, Kakongo, and other Kingdoms in
Africa,” in J. Pinkerton’s Voyages and Travels (London, 1808-1814),
xvi. 569.
109.2 J. Kreemer, “De Loeboes in Mandailing,” Bijdragen tot de
Taal- Land- en Volkenkunde van Nederlandsch-Indië, lxvi. (1912) p.
323.
109.3 P. Rascher, M.S.C., “Die Sulka, ein Beitrag zur Ethnographie
Neu-Pommern,” Archiv für Anthropologie, xxix. (1904) p. 211; R.
Parkinson, Dreissig Jahre in der Südsee (Stuttgart, 1907), pp. 179
sq. In the East Indian island of Buru a man’s death is sometimes
supposed to be due to the adultery of his wife; but apparently the
notion is that the death is brought about rather by the evil magic of
the adulterer than by the act of adultery itself. See J. H. W. van der
Miesen, “Een en ander over Boeroe, inzonderheit wat betreft het
distrikt Waisama, gelegen aan de Z.O. Kust,” Mededeelingen van
wege het Nederlandsche Zendelinggenootschap, xlvi. (1902) pp.
451-454.
110.1 P. A. Talbot, “The Buduma of Lake Chad,” Journal of the

Royal Anthropological Institute, xli. (1911) p. 247.


Chapter V Notes
112.1 Humboldt, Voyage aux Régions Equinoxiales, viii. 273.
113.1 Alcide d’Orbigny, Voyage dans l’Amérique Méridionale, ii.
(Paris and Strasburg, 1839-1843) pp. 99 sq. As to the thieving
propensities of the Patagonians, the author tells us that “they do not
steal among themselves, it is true; but their parents, from their tender
infancy, teach them to consider theft from the enemy as the base of
their education, as an accomplishment indispensable for every one
who would succeed in life, as a thing ordained by the Evil Spirit, so
much so that when they are reproached for a theft, they always say
that Achekenat-Kanet commanded them so to do” (op. cit. p. 104).
Achekenat-Kanet is the supernatural being who, under various
names, is revered or dreaded by all the Indian tribes of Patagonia.
Sometimes he appears as a good and sometimes as a bad spirit.
See A. d’Orbigny, op. cit. ii. 87.
114.1 Plato, Laws, ix. 8, pp. 865 d-866 a; Demosthenes, xxiii. pp.
643 sq.; Hesychius, s.v. ἀπενιαυτισμός.
114.2 Aeschylus, Choëphor. 1021 sqq., Eumenides, 85 sqq.;
Euripides, Iphig. in Taur. 940 sqq.; Pausanias, ii. 31. 8, viii. 34. 1-4.
114.3 Demosthenes, xxiii. pp. 643 sq.
114.4 Demosthenes, xxiii. pp. 645 sq.; Aristotle, Constitution of
Athens, 57; Pausanias, i. 28. 11; Pollux, viii. 120; Helladius, quoted
by Photius, Bibliotheca, p. 535 a, lines 28 sqq. ed. I. Bekker (Berlin,
1824).
115.1 Plato, Laws, ix. 8, p. 866 C D.
115.2 Polybius, iv. 17-21.
115.3 Plutarch, Praecept. ger. reipub. xvii. 9.
115.4 Pausanias, ii. 31. 8.
115.5 C. W. Hobley, “Kikuyu Customs and Beliefs,” Journal of the

Royal Anthropological Institute, xl. (1910) p. 431. The nature of the


ceremonial pollution (thahu) thus incurred is explained by Mr. Hobley
(op. cit. p. 428) as follows: “Thahu, sometimes called ngahu, is the
word used for a condition into which a person is believed to fall if he
or she accidentally becomes the victim of certain circumstances or
intentionally performs certain acts which carry with them a kind of ill
luck or curse. A person who is thahu becomes emaciated and ill or
breaks out into eruptions or boils, and if the thahu is not removed will
probably die. In many cases this undoubtedly happens by the
process of auto-suggestion, as it never occurs to the Kikuyu mind to
be sceptical on a matter of this kind. It is said that the thahu
condition is caused by the ngoma or spirits of departed ancestors,
but the process does not seem to have been analysed any further.”
See also above, pp. 93, 105.
116.1 Aeschylus, Eumenides, 280 sqq., 448 sqq.; id., quoted by
Eustathius on Homer, Iliad, xix. 254, p. 1183, ἐπιτήδειος ἐδόκει πρὸς
καθαρμὸν ὁ σῦς, ὡς δηλοῖ Αἰσχύλος ἐν τῷ, πρὶν ἂν παλαγμοῖς
αἵματος χοιροκτόνου αὐτός σε χρᾶναι Ζεὺς καταστάξας χεροῖν;
Apollonius Rhodius, Argonaut. iv. 703-717, with the notes of the
scholiast. Purifications of this sort are represented in Greek art. See
my note on Pausanias ii. 31. 8 (vol. iii. pp. 276 sqq.).
116.2 Lieutenant Thomas Shaw, “The Inhabitants of the Hills near
Rajamahall,” Asiatic Researches, Fourth Edition, iv. (London, 1807)
p. 78, compare p. 77.
116.3 See above, pp. 44 sqq.
116.4 Missionary Autenrieth, “Zur Religion der Kamerun-Neger,”

Mitteilungen der geographischen Gesellschaft zu Jena, xii. (1893)


pp. 93 sq.
117.1 V. Solomon, “Extracts from Diaries kept in Car Nicobar,”
Journal of the Anthropological Institute, xxxii. (1902) p. 227.
117.2 See my note on Pausanias, ii. 31. 8 (vol. iii. pp. 276 sqq.).
117.3 This was the view of C. Meiners (Geschichte der Religionen,

Hanover, 1806-1807, ii. 137 sq.), and of E. Rohde (Psyche3,


Tübingen and Leipsic, 1903, ii. 77 sq.).
117.4 καθαίρονται δ᾽ ἄλλως αἵματι μιανόμενοι οἶον εἴ τις εἰς πηλὸν
ἐμβὰς πηλῷ ἀπονίζοιτο, Heraclitus, in H. Diels’s Die Fragmente der
Vorsokratiker, Zweite Auflage, i. (Berlin, 1906) p. 62.
117.5 Pausanias, viii. 34. 3.
118.1 Rev. J. H. Bernau, Missionary Labours in British Guiana
(London, 1847), pp. 57 sq.; R. Schomburgk, Reisen in Britisch-
Guiana (Leipsic, 1847-1848), ii. 497.
118.2 J. Dumont D’Urville, Voyage autour du monde et à la
recherche de la Pérouse (Paris, 1832-1833), iii. 305.
118.3 John Bradbury, Travels in the Interior of America (Liverpool,
1817), p. 160.
118.4 Pomponius Mela, Chorogr. ii. 12, p. 35, ed. G. Parthey
(Berlin, 1867).
118.5 A. C. Hollis, The Nandi (Oxford, 1909), p. 27.
119.1 Major A. G. Leonard, The Lower Niger and its Tribes (London,
1906), pp. 180, 181 sq.
119.2 Mrs. Leslie Milne, Shans at Home (London, 1910), p. 192.
Among the Shans “in a case of capital punishment more than one
executioner assisted, and each tried to avoid giving the fatal blow, so
that the sin of killing the culprit should fall upon several, each bearing
a part. The unfortunate man was killed by reason of repeated sword
cuts, no one of which was sufficient to kill him, and died rather from
loss of blood than from one fatal blow” (Mrs. Leslie Milne, op. cit. pp.
191 sq.). Perhaps each executioner feared to be haunted by his
victim’s ghost if he actually despatched him.
119.3 Vincenzo Dorsa, La Tradizione greco-latina negli usi e nelle
credenze popolari della Calabria Citeriore (Cosenza, 1884), p. 138.
119.4 J. Liorel, Kabylie du Jurjura (Paris, N.D.), p. 441.
120.1 Lieut.-Colonel J. Shakespear, “The Kuki-Lushai clans,”
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, xxxix. (1909) p. 380;
id., The Lushei Kuki Clans (London, 1912), pp. 78 sq.
120.2 J. H. West Sheane, “Wemba Warpaths,” Journal of the
African Society, No. 41 (October, 1911), pp. 31 sq.
120.3 Taboo and the Perils of the Soul, pp. 165 sqq.
120.4 Rev. E. Casalis, The Basutos (London, 1861), p. 258.
121.1 Father Porte, “Les Réminiscences d’un missionnaire du
Basutoland,” Les Missions catholiques, xxviii. (1896) p. 371.
122.1 Psanyi is half-digested grass found in the stomachs of
sacrificed goats (H. A. Junod, The Life of a South African Tribe, ii.
569).
122.2 Henri A. Junod, The Life of a South African Tribe (Neuchâtel,
1912-1913), i. 453-455. I have omitted some of the Thonga words
which Mr. Junod inserts in the text.
123.1 N. Adriani en Alb. C. Kruijt, De Bare’e-sprekende Toradja’s
van Midden-Celebes, i. (Batavia, 1912) p. 239.
123.2 Sir H. Johnston, The Uganda Protectorate (London, 1902), ii.
743 sq.; C. W. Hobley, Eastern Uganda (London, 1902), p. 20.
123.3 Extract from a type-written account of the tribes of Mount
Elgon, by the Hon. Kenneth R. Dundas, which the author kindly sent
to me.
123.4 Sir H. Johnston, op. cit. ii. 794; C. W. Hobley, op. cit. p. 31.
123.5 Pausanias, viii. 34. 3; compare Strabo, xii. 2. 3, p. 535.
124.1 E. Torday and T. A. Joyce, “Notes on the Ethnography of the
Ba-Yaka,” Journal of the Anthropological Institute, xxxvi. (1906) pp.
50 sq.
124.2 J. G. Frazer, “Folk-lore in the Old Testament,” in
Anthropological Essays presented to E. B. Tylor (Oxford, 1907), p.
108.
124.3 “Relation des Natchez,” Recueil de Voyages au Nord, ix. 24
(Amsterdam, 1737); Lettres édifiantes et curieuses, Nouvelle Édition,
vii. (Paris, 1781) p. 26; Charlevoix, Histoire de la Nouvelle France
(Paris, 1744), vi. 186 sq.
125.1 Ch. Keysser, “Aus dem Leben der Kaileute,” in R. Neuhauss’s

Deutsch Neu-Guinea (Berlin, 1911), iii. 147 sq.


125.2 Ch. Keysser, op. cit. p. 132.
126.1 R. E. Guise, “On the Tribes inhabiting the mouth of the
Wanigela River, New Guinea,” Journal of the Anthropological
Institute, xxviii. (1899) pp. 213 sq.
126.2 Rev. J. Owen Dorsey, “Omaha Sociology,” Third Annual
Report of the Bureau of Ethnology (Washington, 1884), p. 369.
127.1 Franz Boas, Chinook Texts (Washington, 1894), p. 258.
128.1 K. Vetter, “Über papuanische Rechtsverhältnisse, wie solche
namentlich bei den Jabim beobachtet wurden,” Nachrichten über
Kaiser Wilhelms-Land und den Bismarck-Archipel, 1897, p. 99; B.
Hagen, Unter den Papuas (Wiesbaden, 1899), p. 254.
128.2 Rev. J. H. Weeks, Among Congo Cannibals (London, 1913),
p. 268; compare id., “Anthropological Notes on the Bangala of the
Upper Congo River,” Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute,
xl. (1910) p. 373.
129.1 C. W. Hobley, “Kikuyu Customs and Beliefs,” Journal of the

Royal Anthropological Intitute, xl. (1910) pp. 438 sq. As to the


sanctity of the fig-tree (mugumu) among the Akikuyu, see Mervyn W.
H. Beech, “The sacred fig-tree of the A-kikuyu of East Africa,” Man,
xiii. (1913) pp. 4-6. Mr. Beech traces the reverence for the tree to the
white milky sap which exudes from it when an incision is made in the
bark. This appears to have suggested to the savages the idea that
the tree is a great source of fertility to men and women, to cattle,
sheep, and goats.
129.2 N. Adriani en Alb. C. Kruijt, De Bare’e-sprekende Toradja’s

van Midden-Celebes, i. (Batavia, 1912) pp. 285, 290 sq. In recent


years the wars between the tribes have been suppressed by the
Dutch Government.
130.1 Compare The Belief in Immortality and the Worship of the
Dead, i. (London, 1913) pp. 136 sq., 278 sq., 468 sq.
130.2 Rev. E. B. Cross, “On the Karens,” Journal of the American

Oriental Society, iv. No. 2 (New York, 1854), pp. 312 sq.
130.3 Bringaud, “Les Karins de la Birmanie,” Les Missions

catholiques, xx. (1888) p. 208.


131.1 W. H. Keating, Narrative of an Expedition to the Sources of

St. Peter’s River (London, 1825), i. 109, quoting Mr. Barron.


131.2 Charlevoix, Histoire de la Nouvelle France (Paris, 1744), vi.

77, 122 sq.; J. F. Lafitau, Mœurs des sauvages amériquains (Paris,


1724), ii. 279.
131.3 H. von Rosenberg, Der malayische Archipel (Leipsic, 1878),

p. 461. Compare J. L. van Hasselt, “Die Papuastämme an der


Geelvinkbai (Neuguinea),” Mitteilungen der geographischen
Gesellschaft zu Jena, ix. (1891) p. 101.
131.4 K. Vetter, “Über papuanische Rechtsverhältnisse,” in
Nachrichten über Kaiser Wilhelms-Land und den Bismarck-Archipel
(1897), p. 94; B. Hagen, Unter den Papuas (Wiesbaden, 1899), p.
266.
131.5 Stefan Lehner, “Bukaua,” in R. Neuhauss’s Deutsch Neu-

Guinea (Berlin, 1911), iii. 444.


131.6 George Brown, D.D., Melanesians and Polynesians (London,

1910), pp. 142, 145.


132.1 John Jackson, in J. E. Erskine’s Journal of a Cruise among

the Islands of the Western Pacific (London, 1853), p. 477.


132.2 C. Wiese, “Beiträge zur Geschichte der Zulu im Norden des
Zambesi,” Zeitschrift für Ethnologie, xxxii. (1900) pp. 197 sq.
132.3 Rev. Samuel Mateer, The Land of Charity, a Descriptive
Account of Travancore and its People (London, 1871), pp. 203 sq.
132.4 E. W. Nelson, “The Eskimo about Bering Strait,” Eighteenth
Annual Report of the Bureau of American Ethnology, Part i.
(Washington, 1899) p. 423.
133.1 Rev. J. Owen Dorsey, “A Study of Siouan Cults,” Eleventh
Annual Report of the Bureau of Ethnology (Washington, 1894), p.
420.
133.2 Dr. P. H. Brincker, “Character, Sitten, und Gebräuche speciell

der Bantu Deutsch-Südwestafrikas,” Mitteilungen des Seminars für


orientalischen Sprachen zu Berlin, iii. dritte Abteilung (1900), pp. 89
sq.
133.3 Rev. R. H. Nassau, Fetichism in West Africa (London, 1904),

p. 220; M. Abeghian, Der armenische Volksglaube (Leipsic, 1899), p.


11.
133.4 H. A. Rose, “Hindu Birth Observances in the Punjab,” Journal

of the Royal Anthropological Institute, xxxvii. (1907) pp. 225 sq.


133.5 G. F. D’ Penha, “Superstitions and Customs in Salsette,” The

Indian Antiquary, xxviii. (1899) p. 115.


133.6 Census of India, 1911, vol. xiv. Punjab, Part I. (Lahore, 1912)

p. 303. As to these perturbed and perturbing spirits in India, see


further W. Crooke, Popular Religion and Folk-lore of Northern India
(Westminster, 1896), i. 269-274. They are called churel.
134.1 E. M. Gordon, Indian Folk Tales (London, 1908), p. 47.

You might also like