Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1997, Vol. 106, No. 1, 145-153 002I-843X/97/S3.00

Perfectionism, Perceived Weight Status, and Bulimic Symptoms:


Two Studies Testing a Diathesis-Stress Model
Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. Todd F. Heatherton
University of Texas Medical Branch at Galveston Dartmouth College

M. David Rudd Norman B. Schmidt


Scott and White Clinic and Hospital and Texas A & M Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences
University Health Sciences Center

Among a total of 890 women from 2 separate nonclinical samples (1 collected in 1982 on college
undergraduates, n = 435, and 1 collected in 1992 on women who were in college in 1982, n =
455), the authors tested a diathesis-stress model of the interrelations of perfectionism, perceived
weight status, and hulimic symptoms. The authors predicted and found that perfectionism served as
a risk factor for bulimic symptoms for women who perceived themselves as overweight but did not
serve as a risk factor for those who did not perceive themselves as overweight. Perceived weight
activated perfectionism as a predictor of bulimic symptoms; actual weight did not serve the same
role. These findings are discussed in the context of recent clinical and social psychological theory
regarding development of bulimic symptoms.

Hewitt, Flett, and colleagues (e.g., Hewitt & Flett, 1991a, that among depressed and other psychiatric patients, those who
1991b, 1993), as well as Frost and associates (Frost, Heimberg, were high in both perfectionism and in daily life stress were
Holt, Mattia, & Neubauer, 1993), have demonstrated that perfec- most likely to obtain high depression scores.
tionism appears to be reliably associated with depression. Re- Perfectionism has also been indicated as a correlate of bulimic
cently, investigators have elaborated on this finding, and have symptoms. For example, Rosch, Crowther, and Graham (1991)
proposed a diathesis-stress approach. From this perspective, per- reported that clinically bulimic university women, as compared
fectionism is viewed as a risk factor for depression, but only in with general psychotherapy patients and normal controls, ob-
the presence, and not in the absence, of negative life stress. That tained elevated perfectionism scores (for similar findings, see
is, the statistical interaction of perfectionism and life stress is Ruderman, 1986; Thompson, Berg, & Shatford, 1987). Simi-
examined as a predictor of depression (e.g., Flett, Hewitt, larly, Rothenberg (1990) found that hospitalized bulimic pa-
Blankstein, & Mosher, 1991; Hewitt & Dyck, 1986; Joiner & tients differed from general psychiatric patients with respect
Schmidt, 1995). For example, Hewitt and Flett (1993) found to perfectionism, as well as related characteristics, such as
scrupulousness.
At the same time, however, the association of perfectionism
Thomas E. Joiner, Jr., Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sci- and bulimic symptoms has been questioned. For example, Hur-
ences, University of Texas Medical Branch at Galveston; Todd F. Heath-
erton, Department of Psychology, Dartmouth College; M. David Rudd, ley, Palmer, and Stretch (1990), in a study comparing eating
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, and Scott and White disordered patients with general psychiatric patients, reported
Clinic and Hospital, College of Medicine, Texas A & M University Health that perfectionism was not specific to eating-disordered patients.
Sciences Center; Norman B. Schmidt, Uniformed Services University of Blouin, Bushnik, Braaten, and Blouin (1989) found that bulimic
the Health Sciences. patients were more perfectionistic than normal controls, but
Preparation of this article was supported in part by a grant from the similarly perfectionistic relative to diabetic patients. Newton,
Henry Murray Center at Radcliffe College and by grants from the Rad- Butler, and Slade (1988) reported no differences in perfec-
cliffe College Research Fund and the Boston Obesity Nutrition Research
tionism between eating disordered patients with mild bulimic
Center (DK 42600).
The opinions or assertions contained herein are the private ones of symptoms and those with more severe symptoms.
the authors and are not to be construed as official or reflecting the views Thus, there is some question as to the very existence of a
of the Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences or the relationship between perfectionism and bulimic symptoms. Fur-
Department of Defense. thermore, should the relationship exist, the form of the relation-
We thank the Henry Murray Center of Radcliffe College for providing ship is unclear—is perfectionism in itself a risk factor for bu-
access to the data from the 1982 sample and the Alumni Office of limic symptoms; or is perfectionism one factor, which, only in
Harvard University for supplying current addresses for participants.
combination with others, heightens risk for bulimia? The goal
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Thomas E. Joiner, Jr., Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, of the present studies is to address these issues in the context
Graves Building, University of Texas Medical Branch, Galveston, Texas of a diathesis-stress framework, among large samples of women,
77555-0425. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to thomas. a subset of whom endorsed the Diagnostic and Statistical Man-
joiner@utmb.edu. ual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edition (DSM-III; American Psy-

145
146 JOINER, HEATHEKTON, RUDD, AND SCHMIDT

chiatric Association, 1980) based criteria for a clinical diagnosis women than is perceived weight status. Although both actual
of bulimia. and perceived weight serve as benchmarks by which standards
The present conceptualization views perfectionism as a risk are evaluated, it is argued that perceived weight is more powerful
factor for bulimic symptoms, but only under certain conditions. for perfectionistic women. To be sure, there is a relation between
If one strives to meet high standards (e.g., perfect body weight actual and perceived weight status. But there is also disparity.
and shape), even if such standards are excessively high, negative It is predicted that when perfectionistic women feel overweight,
outcomes should not be expected unless the standards go unmet (even if they are not; cf. quadrant D of Figure 1 ) , they will be
(cf. Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991). Perfection, if both desired at high risk for bulimic symptoms, whereas when perfectionistic
and obtained, is a positive state of affairs. Indeed, this is pre- women do not feel overweight, (regardless of whether they are;
cisely the rinding from the diathesis-stress work on perfec- (cf. quadrant B of Figure 1), they will not experience bulimic
tionism and depression; perfectionistic people, despite their per- symptoms.
fectionism, were not depressed unless things were imperfect To summarize, hypotheses for the present studies were as
(i.e., unless negative life stress occurred; Hewitt & Flett, 1993; follows;
Joiner & Schmidt, 1995). A similar result has emerged from
1. Perceived weight would display a strong main effect relation
within the literature on negative cognitive style and depression:
to bulimic symptoms (quadrants C and D would have higher
People with negative attributional styles tend not to become
bulimia scores than quadrants A and B).
depressed unless they also encounter negative life stress (e.g.,
Metalsky, Joiner, Hardin, & Abramson, 1993). 2. Perfectionism was more tentatively predicted to relate to bu-
According to the current conceptualization, perfectionistic limic symptoms, and in any event, the magnitude of the relation
women will experience bulimic symptoms when they feel that should represent a considerably weaker effect than that of per-
their standards are unmet. However, it is postulated that perfec- ceived weight on bulimic symptoms (quadrants B and D may
tionistic women, despite their perfectionism, will not experience have higher bulimia scores than quadrants A and C).
bulimic symptoms when they feel that their standards have been
satisfied. This conceptualization is depicted in Figure 1. 3. Most important to the present conceptualization, perfec-
Because there is a well-known main effect for weight percep- tionism was predicted to relate strongly to bulimic symptoms
tions on bulimic symptoms (e.g., Ruderman, 1986), women in among women who felt overweight, but less strongly among
women who did not feel overweight (quadrant D would have
quadrants C and D of Figure 1 are predicted to have higher
bulimic symptoms than women in quadrants A and B. As was the highest bulimia scores). That is, perceived weight status and
mentioned earlier, the main effect relation of perfectionism on perfectionism were hypothesized to interact to predict bulimic
symptoms.
bulimic symptoms is less clear, although there is at least some
evidence to support a weak main effect, such that women in 4. Actual weight would display a strong main effect relation to
quadrants B and D may experience higher levels of bulimic bulimic symptoms.
symptoms than women in quadrants A and C. More important
to the present conceptualization, women in quadrant D are pre- 5. However, actual weight was not postulated to interact with
dicted to experience the highest levels of bulimic symptoms, perfectionism to predict bulimic symptoms, and to the extent
because their high standards have been violated (i.e., they feel that it did, it was predicted that it did so only because it was
overweight and expect themselves not to be overweight). highly related to perceived weight.
It is important to note that the lermfeel overweight has been 6. Related to Hypothesis 5, it was hypothesized that, among
used, as opposed to are overweight. According to the present perfectionistic women, those who felt overweight but who were
view, actual weight status is less relevant to perfectionistic not overweight would experience more bulimic symptoms than
those who did not feel overweight but who were overweight.

Study 1

High Method

Participants

In the spring of 1982, researchers affiliated with Radcliffe College


distributed surveys to a randomly selected sample of 800 women and
400 men who were students at Boston University. The focus of the
present study was on the women, 435 of whom responded to the ran-
Faals Overweight? domly distributed questionnaire (response rate = 54%; response rate
for men was 50%). The women were predominantly Caucasian (88%),
and the mean age was 20.11 years (SD = 1.2).

Procedure
The questionnaire included an array of items about demographic back-
ground; height and weight; concerns about dieting, eating patterns, and
Figure I. Four groups created by crossing the perfectionism and per- body weight and shape; and eating disorder symptomatology. Of particu-
ceived weight variables. lar interest for the present study, participants completed the Perfec-
PERFECTIONISM, PERCEIVED WEIGHT, BULIMIC SYMPTOMS 147

tionism and Bulimia subscales of the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI; repeated attempts to lose weight by severely restrictive diets, self-in-
Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983), responded to questions about their duced vomiting, or the use of cathartics or diuretics; (e) indicated that
weight, and responded to questions based on DSM-I1I criteria for bu- the binges were experienced as out of control; (f ) reported often feeling
limia nervosa, as described below. extremely guilty after overeating; and (g) indicated that weight had not
Eating Disorders inventory and Perfectionism and Bulimia subscales. fallen more than 20 percent below normal since age 16.
The EDI is a frequently used 64-item self-report measure of eating- These seven criteria afford good coverage of current diagnostic criteria
related attitudes and traits. It yields eight subscales; Drive for Thinness, for bulimia nervosa in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Bulimia, Body Dissatisfaction, Ineffectiveness, Perfectionism, Interper- Disorders, 4th edition, (DSM-/V; American Psychiatric Association,
sonal Distrust, Interoceptive Awareness, and Maturity Fears. The sub- 1994), and as shown below, their relation to the EDI bulimia subscale
scales have shown adequate internal consistency coefficients and stable (r = .56) provides some confidence in them. Furthermore, the rate of
test-retest correlations and have been extensively validated (see Garner, diagnosis in the present sample (50 of 435 = 12%), although somewhat
Olmstead & Polivy, 1983). high, is similar to estimates provided by Zuckerman, Colby, Ware, and
The present study focused on the Perfectionism and Bulimia subscales. Lazerson (1986) and by Heatherton et al. (1995) on similar samples of
The Perfectionism subscale includes six items designed to measure gen- women. However, they should not be viewed as diagnoses established
eral perfectionism (e.g., "I feel that I must do things perfectly, or not through structured clinical interview. They are thus viewed cautiously
do them at all" or "Only outstanding performance is good enough in and presented as a supplement to the data on the EDI Bulimia subscale.
my family. 1 '). Alpha internal consistency coefficient in the present sam-
ple was .79, similar to the figure of .88 obtained among a sample of
undergraduate women (Joiner & Schmidt, 1995). The construct validity Results and Discussion
of the scale is supported by expected correlations with depressive
Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 1 (see
(Joiner & Schmidt, 1995) and eating disorder symptoms (e.g., Rosch
et al.. 1991). diagonal) and were consistent with previous reports (e.g.,
Joiner and Schmidt (1995) found that the EDI Perfectionism subscale Heatherton et al., 1995). Table 1 also presents the zero-order
assesses two correlated aspects of perfectionism, self-oriented perfec- correlations between all measures. Of particular interest, the
tionism (i.e., expecting self to be perfect; three items) and socially Perfectionism subscale was weakly correlated with EDI bulimia
prescribed perfectionism (i.e., perceiving that others expect perfection; subscale (r = .21, p < .001) and not correlated at all with the
three items). Despite the low number of items on each subscale, internal diagnostic variable (r = .07, p ~ ns). perfectionism and per-
consistency reliability in the present sample was adequate (.70 for self-
ceived weight status were not highly correlated (r = .14, p <
oriented; .72 for socially prescribed; Joiner & Schmidt reported .74 for
.005), which suggests that multicolinearity among predictors in
self-oriented; .76 for socially prescribed, as well as test-retest reliability
the regression analyses presented below was not an issue.
of .64 and ,68, respectively).
As was expected, the EDI Bulimia subscale correlated
The Bulimia subscale includes seven items that assess bingeing and
purging (e.g., "I stuff myself with food" or "I have thought of trying strongly with the diagnostic variable (/• = .56, p < .001), with
to vomit to lose weight."). Participants were asked to rate items on a perceived weight status (r = .49, p < .001), and with the
scale ranging from 1 to 6 where 1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, Body Mass Index (r ~ .35, p < .001). Perceived weight status
4 — frequently, 5 = usually, and 6 = alwavs. Coefficient alpha in the correlated with the Body Mass Index (r - .55, p < .001),
present study was .86. consistent with the view that there is overlap and disparity be-
Perceived weight status. Participants were asked to categorize them- tween actual and perceived weight. Perceived weight status was
selves as either overweight or not overweight (not vvenveight = 1;
also correlated with the lose/gain weight variable (r - .43, p
overweight = 2). 43% of the participants (« = 187) identified them-
< .001).
selves as overweight; the rest (n = 248) identified themselves as not
overweight. A separate question tapped a similar construct: whether
participants were trying to lose weight ( = 2 ) or not (= I ) . Three hundred Prediction of Bulimic Symptoms by the Perfectionism X
and twenty-six women (75%) indicated they were trying to lose weight,
Perceived Weight Status Interaction
and the remainder (n = 109) indicated that they were not trying to lose
weight. For both variables, these percentages are quite consistent with Consistent with the recommendations of Cohen and Cohen
those obtained by Heatherton, Nichols, Mahamedi, and Keel (1995) on
(1983), a setwise hierarchical multiple regression/correlation
similar samples of women.
(MRC) procedure was used to test predictions pertaining to
In the analyses reported below, the focus is on the perceived weight
the Perfectionism X Perceived Weight Status interaction. EDI
variable. Analyses using the lose-weight variable are offered as supple-
Bulimia subscale scores served as the dependent variable. Be-
mentary support for those involving the perceived weight variable.
Actual weight status. Participants provided their actual height and cause the distribution of EDI Bulimia scores was positively
weight. On this basis, the Body Mass Index (kg/m 2 ) was calculated. skewed (as is usually the case with a symptom measure in a
Also, participants were categorized with regard to height/weight (very population sample), EDI Bulimia scores were transformed, us-
underweight, underweight, average weight, overweight, or obese) on the ing the square-root transformation recommended by Cohen and
basis of standards from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Cohen (1983, p. 252).
Survey (see, e.g., Kuczmarski, 1992). It is important to note that Radke- EDI Perfectionism subscale scores and a dichotomous vari-
Sharpe, Whitney-Saltiel, and Rodin (1990) demonstrated that self-mea-
able indicating perceived weight status (where 1 = do not feel
surements are quite similar to measurements done by experts.
overweight and 2 = feel overweight) were entered into the
Bulimic diagnoses. Bulimic diagnoses were determined based on
regression equation simultaneously as a set, followed at Step 2
participants' responses to questions based on DSM~Ifl criteria for bu-
by entry of the Perfectionism X Perceived Weight Status interac-
limia nervosa. To meet criteria in the present study, participants must
have had (a) indicated recurrent experiences of at least one binge eating tion term.
episode per week; ( b ) indicated that binges consisted of large amounts As can be seen in Table 2, regression analysis indicated that,
of high-calorie food (not just large meals); (c) indicated that at least consistent with prediction, (a) perfectionism subscale is posi-
some binges occurred when participant was alone; (d) reported making tively and somewhat weakly related to bulimic symptoms, pr =
148 JOINER, HEATHEKTON, RUDD, AND SCHMIDT

Table 1
Descriptive Data and Intercorrelations for All Variables in Study 1 (N = 435)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. EDI perfectionism 21.98


(6.46)
2. EDI bulimia .21 13.04
(5.58)
3. Bulimic diagnosis .07 .56 —
4. Perceived weight status .14 .49 .21

5. Trying to lose weight? .09 .36 .18 .43 —
6. Body mass index .07 .35 .18 .55 .51 21.31
(2.78)
7. Weight class .07 .33 .13 .58 .46 .90 —

Note. Numbers in parentheses represent standard deviations. Bulimic diagnosis, peceived weight status,
and trying to lose weight are dichotomous variables. Correlations s.18 are significant to the .001 level;
correlations s.13 and <,18 are significant to the .005 level; correlations ^.09 are ns. EDI - Eating
Disorders Inventory (Gamer, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983).

.18, t(432) = 3.75, p < .05; (b) perceived weight status dis- highly similar. Predicting both the EDI bulimia subscale and the
played a strong main effect relation to bulimic symptoms, pr = bulimia diagnosis variable, the Perfectionism X Trying to Lose
.48, 1(432) = 11.42, p < .05; and (c) most important to the Weight interaction term was significant; respectively, pr = .11,
present conceptualization, the Perfectionism X Perceived Weight i(431) = 2.22, p < .05; pr = .10, t(431) = 2.02, p < .05.
Status interaction served as a significant predictor of EDT Bu- Thus it appears that the study's main hypothesis (that the
limia subscale scores, pr — .12, ((431) = 2.47, p < .05. Perfectionism X Perceived Weight Status interaction would be
Interestingly, a similar pattern of results emerged when the predictive of bulimic symptoms) was supported, regardless of
bulimic diagnosis variable was used instead of the EDI bulimia whether the EDI Bulimia subscale or the diagnostic variable
subscale as the dependent variable (see Cohen & Cohen, 1983, was used as the dependent variable and regardless of whether
pp. 229-230, for defense of use of MRC analysis with dichoto- perceived weight status was assessed directly, or more obliquely
mous dependent variables). These results are summarized in by way of the trying to lose weight question. It remains to be
Table 3. At the main effect level, perceived weight status contin- demonstrated, however, that the form of this interaction is as
ued to serve as a significant predictor of bulimic symptoms, pr predicted (i.e., that perfectionistic women who perceive them-
= .21, ((432) = 4.48, p < .05, but the Perfectionism subscale selves as overweight will experience the highest levels of bu-
did not, pr = .05, ((432) = 0.96, p = ns. More important, the limic symptoms; see quadrant D of Figure 1).
Perfectionism X Perceived Weight Status interaction served as To examine this issue, the relation between the Perfectionism
a significant predictor of the bulimic diagnosis variable, pr = subscale and EDI Bulimic symptoms subscale was examined
.15,r(431) = 3 . U , p < .05. among two subgroups of participants; those who did and those
It is also of interest to note that when the perceived weight who did not report feeling overweight (results using the bulimia
status variable was replaced in the two sets of analyses described diagnostic variable and the trying to lose weight variable were
above by the trying to lose weight variable, the results were highly similar and are not reported; see Joiner, Metalsky, &
Wonderlich, 1995 for a similar data-analytic procedure). In line

Table 2
Perfectionism, Perceived Weight Status, and the Perfectionism Table 3
X Perceived Weight Status Interaction Predicting Scores on Perfectionism, Perceived Weight Status, and the Perfectionism
EDI Bulimia Subscale in Study 1 X Perceived Weight Status Interaction Predicting Bulimia
Diagnosis Variable in Study 1
(for Partial
Order of entry/ Ffor within-set correlation ( for Partial
predictors in set set predictors df (PRIpr) Order of entry/ Ffor within-set correlation
predictors in set set predictors df {PRIpr)
1. Main effects 80.12 2,432 .52
Perfectionism 3.75 432 .18 1. Main effects 11.34* 2,432 .22
Perceived weight status 11.42 432 .48 Perfectionism 0.96 432 .05
2. Two-way interaciton 6.10 1,431 .12 Perceived weight status 4.48* 432 .21
Perfectionism x 2. Two-way interaction 9.67* 1,431 .15
perceived weight Perfectionism X
status 2.47 431 .12 perceived weight
status 3.11* 431 .IS
Note. EDI = Eating Disorders Inventory (Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy,
1983); PR = multiple partial correlation for a set of predictors; pr - Note. PR = multiple partial correlation for a set of predictors; pr
partial correlation for within-set predictors. All effects are significant partial correlation for within-set predictors.
at the p < .05 level. *p < .05.
PERFECTIONISM, PERCEIVED WEIGHT. BULIMIC SYMPTOMS 149

STUDY 1 STUDY 2
Perfectionism
Perfectionism
High High

Bulimia Score =10. 11 Bulimia Score = 10.76 Bulimia Score =10.45 Bulimia Score .11. 04

A B A B

Feels Feels
Overweight? Overweight'

Bull mia Score = 13.77 Bulimia Score == 16.89 Bulimia Score = 11. 73 Bulimia Score =15.03

c D C D

Compare to sample-wide Bulimia Score Mean Compare to sample-wide Bulimia Score Mean
of 1304 (§D = 5.58) of11.08(SD = 5.02)

Figure 2. Predicted scores on the Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983) Bulimia
Subscale in Study 1 (left) and Study 2 (right) for four groups created by crossing the perfectionism and
perceived weight variables.

with prediction, the Perfectionism subscale significantly pre- predicting EDI Bulimia subscale scores are reported in Table 4
dicted EDI Bulimia subscale scores among those who perceived (findings predicting the bulimia diagnosis variable were similar
themselves as overweight (r = .31, p < .05), but not among and are not reported). As can be seen there, consistent with
those who perceived themselves as not overweight (r = .09, prediction, the Body Mass Index displays a main effect relation
p = ns). to bulimic symptoms, pr = .37, /(432) = 8.44, p < .05, but
To graphically demonstrate these findings, following Cohen the Perfectionism X Body Mass Index interaction does not, pr
and Cohen (1983, pp. 323 and 419), EDI Bulimia subscale = .02, r(431) = 0.72, p = ns.
scores were computed by inserting specific values for Perfec- Second, the analyses summarized in Tables 2 and 3 were
tionism (i.e., 1 SD above and below the mean) and for perceived again repeated, now with actual weight status inserted as a
weight status (i.e., 1 = do not feel overweight and 2 = feel covariate. That is, the Body Mass Index was entered first into
overweight) into the regression equation summarized in Table the regression equation predicting EDI Bulimia subscale scores,
2(see Joiner, 1995; Joiner etal., 1995 for a similar data-analytic followed by simultaneous entry of the Perfectionism subscale
procedure). The results of this analysis are depicted in the left and perceived weight status, and at the last step, by the Perfec-
panel of Figure 2. tionism x Perceived Weight Status interaction. Notably, the co-
As can be seen in Figure 2, consistent with prediction, women variance of the Body Mass Index had very little effect on the
who were both perfectionistic and reported feeling overweight results: The main effects for perceived weight status, pr = .32,
experienced the highest bulimia scores (see quadrant D of Fig- f(431) = 7.80, p < .05, and Perfectionism, pr = .16, r(431)
ure 2). Interestingly, among women who did not feel over- = 3.90, p < .05, remained, as did the significant effect for the
weight, perfectionistic participants (quadrant B) obtained simi- interaction term, pr = .12, /(430) = 2.48, p < .05.
lar bulimia scores as nonperfectionistic participants (quadrant Third, it was suggested that when perfectionistic women feel
A). Women who felt overweight but were nonperfectionistic
obtained average bulimia scores. This pattern of findings is quite
consistent with the present view that perfectionism may lead to Table 4
bulimic symptoms for those who do (but not lead to bulimic Perfectionism, Body Mass Index, and the Perfectionism
symptoms for those who do not) perceive themselves as x Body Mass Index Interaction Predicting
overweight. EDI Bulimia Scores in Study I

t for Partial
Order of entry/ Ffor within-set correlation
Role of Perceived Weight Status Versus Actual predictors in set set predictors df (PR/pr)
Weight Status
\. Main effects 47.10* 2,432 .42
Perfectionism 4.20* 432 .20
It was predicted that feeling overweight impinged more pow-
Body mass index 8.44» 432 .38
erfully on perfectionism than being overweight. This hypothesis 2. Two-way interaction 0.52 1,431 .02
was evaluated in three ways. Perfectionism X
First, the analyses summarized in Tables 2 and 3 were re- body mass index 0.72 431 .02
peated, except that actual weight status was used as a predictor
Note. EDI = Eating Disorders Inventory (Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy,
instead of perceived weight status. That is, the Perfectionism
1983); PR = multiple partial correlation for a set of predictors; pr =
subscale, the Body Mass Index, and the interaction between partial correlation for within set predictors.
them were assessed as predictors of bulimic symptoms. Results *p < .05.
150 JOINER, HEATHEKTON, RUDD, AND SCHMIDT

overweight, even if they are not, they will be at high risk for and behavior. The earlier study occurred in 1982 at Harvard University,
bulimic symptoms, whereas when perfectionistic women do not Participants in Study 2 differed from those of Study I in four ways.
feel overweight but are, they will not experience bulimic symp- First, the response rate in Study 2 was better (82% of the original sample
participated in 1992). Second, whereas participants in Study 1 were
toms. We examined this issue among perfectionistic participants
students at Boston University in 1982, participants in Study 2 were
(i.e., those with EDI Perfectionism subscale scores above the
students at Harvard University in 1982. Third, whereas the data for
median; « = 218).
Study 1 were collected in 1982, those for Study 2 were collected in
EDI Bulimia subscale scores were examined among two 1992. And fourth, whereas the average age of participants in Study I
groups of perfectionistic women; (a) those who felt overweight was 20.11 years, the average age of participants in Study 2 was 29.93
but were not (n ~ 43), and (b) those who did not feel over- (.TO = 1.81). Study 2 included 455 women.
weight but were (» = 12).' An interesting feature of this com-
parison is to orthogonalize actual and perceived weight. Procedure
If, as is predicted, perceived weight more powerfully im-
The procedure and materials for Study 2 were identical to those for
pinges on perfectionistic attitudes than does actual weight, EDI
Study 1. As in Study 1, participants completed the perfectionism and
Bulimia subscale scores should have been significantly higher
bulimia subscales of the EDI, responded to questions about their weight,
among perfectionistic women who perceived themselves as over-
and responded to questions based on DSM-III criteria for bulimia ner-
weight but were not, than among perfectionistic women who
vosa (see description in Study I ). Regarding the D.W-based criteria,
did not perceive themselves as overweight but were. This was, 4% of participants (n = 19) met criteria (cf. Heatherton et al., 1995
in fact, the case. The mean EDI bulimia subscale score among who reported a decrease in weight- and eating-related problems from
those who perceived themselves to be overweight but were not 1982 to 1992).
overweight was 17.86; the corresponding figure among those
who did not perceive themselves to be overweight but were Results and Discussion
overweight was 12.83. The difference between these means was
statistically significant, t(53) = 2.51, p < .01. Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations are pre-
To summarize Study 1, results were consistent with all sented in Table 5 and were fairly consistent with those reported
hypotheses. Perfectionistic women reported bulimic symptoms, in Study 1. Interestingly, as is perhaps predictable, Harvard
but only when they felt that their standards were unmet (i.e., alumnae in Study 2 scored higher on the perfectionism subscale
perceived themselves as overweight; see quadrant D of Figure than did Boston University students in Study 1. Also, in Study
2). Perfectionistic women, despite their perfectionism, did not 2, the correlation between perceived weight status and the Body
experience bulimic symptoms when they perceived themselves Mass Index was high (r = .71, p < .001), which may work
as not overweight. It was argued that perceived weight impinged against our hypothesis that perceived weight impinges more
more forcefully on perfectionistic attitudes than actual weight, strongly on perfectionism than actual weight.
and results were supportive of this view.
The supportive results should be viewed in the context of Prediction of Bulimic Symptoms by the Perfectionism X
some of the study's limitations. First, the response rate to the Perceived Weight Status Interaction
questionnaire (54%) was not ideal. It is possible that the rela-
The data-analytic approach in Study 2 was identical to that
tively low response rate may have biased results. Second, al-
used in Study 1. Table 6 displays the results of a regression
though successfully used in previous work (Heatherton et al.,
equation with EDI Bulimia subscale as the dependent variable,
1995), the reliability of the perceived weight status variable is
and EDI Perfectionism subscale, perceived weight status (where
difficult to formally estimate. On the other hand, the variable's
1 = do not feel overweight and 2 = feel overweight), and the
correlation with the trying to lose weight variable is encourag-
Perfectionism X Perceived Weight Status interaction term as
ing, as is the general finding that people's self-reports of weight-
predictor variables.
related data are reliable (Radke-Sharpe et al., 1990). Third, the
As Table 6 shows, findings indicated that, consistent with
study's findings are limited to undergraduate women, and may
prediction and with the results of Study 1, (a) the Perfectionism
not apply to women in different age categories. Fourth, although
subscale was positively and somewhat weakly related to bulimic
hypotheses were supported without exception, the absolute mag-
symptoms, pr = .23, ((452) = 5.00, p < .05; (b) perceived
nitude of the interaction term's effect size (e.g., pr = .12 in
weight status displayed a fairly strong main effect relation to
Table 2) was relatively small. The relatively small effect size,
bulimic symptoms, pr = .38, /(452) = 8.63, p < .05; and (c)
together with the questions regarding reliability and replicability,
most important to the present conceptualization, the Perfec-
call for replication of the present results, preferably in a sample
tionism X Perceived Weight Status interaction served as a sig-
with a different age range and a higher response rate. Study 2
nificant predictor of EDI Bulimia subscale scores, pr - .10,
answers this call, and furthermore. Study 2 addresses a final
/ ( 4 5 1 ) = 2.07, p < .05.
concern that the data from Study 1 were somewhat dated (col-
As in Study 1, a similar pattern of results emerged when the
lected in 19K2 and data in Study 2 were collected in 1992).

Study 2 1
Because the goal of this analysis was to separate feeling from being
Method overweight, participants whose perceived weight status matched their
actual weight status were excluded. Thus, those who were overweight,
Participants and perceived themselves as such, were excluded (n = 50), as were
In the spring of 1992, Heatherton and colleagues attempted to identify participants who neither viewed themselves as overweight nor were
and follow-up all participants from an earlier study of eating patterns overweight (n = I 13).
PERFECTIONISM, PERCEIVED WEIGHT, BULIMIC SYMPTOMS 151

Table 5
Descriptive Data and Intercorrelatiom for All Variables in Study 2 (N = 455)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1 . EDI perfectionism 24.91


(5.15)
2. EDI bulimia .26 11.08
(5.02)
3. Bulimic diagnosis .23 .75 —
4. Perceived weight status .07 .41 .38

5. Trying to lose weight? .07 .19 .25 .28 —
6. Body mass index .01 .36 .35 .71 .26 22.01
(3.57)
7. Weight class -.05 .27 .22 .56 .31 .80 —

Note. Numbers in parentheses represent standard devialiens. Bulimic di agnosis, perceived weight status,
ana trying to lose weignt are aicnotomous vanaoies. \_orreiauons ^:. I? are signmcam 10 me ..001 level;
correlations ==.07 are ns. EDI = Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983).

bulimic diagnosis variable was used instead of the EDI bulimia predictor instead of perceived weight status, the Perfectionism
subscale as the dependent variable, and when the trying to lose X Actual Weight Status interaction did not predict bulimic
weight variable was used instead of the perceived weight status symptoms, pr = .03, t(451) = 0.73, p = ns. When actual weight
as a predictor (results available from Thomas Joiner upon status was inserted as a covariate in the regression analysis
request). described in Table 6, the Perfectionism X Perceived Weight
Again replicating the results of Study 1, the form of the Status interaction remained a significant predictor of bulimic
Perfectionism X Perceived Weight Status interaction was as pre- symptoms, pr = .10, r(450) = 2.05, p < .05.2
dicted, and the scores produced by the regression equation were In conclusion, the supportive results of Study 1 were fully
highly similar to those in Study 1 (see right panel of Figure replicated in Study 2, on a different, older, and more recent
2). Women who were both perfectionistic and reported feeling sample with a higher response rate. Perfectionistic women expe-
overweight experienced the highest bulimia scores. These re- rienced bulimic symptoms, but only when they perceived them-
sults, like those of Study 1, are quite consistent with the present selves as overweight. It appeared that perceived weight, not
model that perfectionism may lead to bulimic symptoms for actual weight, served to activate perfectionistic attitudes as a
those who do (but not lead to bulimic symptoms for those who predictor of bulimic symptoms.
do not) perceive themselves as overweight.
Genera] Discussion
Role of Perceived Weight Status Versus Actual
Across two studies (one in 1982 and one in 1992) including
Weight Status
a total of 890 women, our diathesis-stress model of the interrela-
The prediction that feeling overweight impinged more power- tions of perfectionism, perceived weight status, and bulimic
fully on perfectionism than being overweight was fully repli- symptoms received empirical support. Perfectionism appeared
cated in Study 2. When actual weight status was used as a to serve as a risk factor for bulimic symptoms for women who
perceived themselves as overweight, but not for those who did
not. Perceived weight activated perfectionism as a predictor of
Table 6 bulimic symptoms; actual weight did not serve the same role.
Perfectionism, Perceived Weight Status, and the Perfectionism Before elucidating the studies' implications, it is first im-
X Perceived Weight Status Interaction Predicting Scores on portant to consider some limitations. First, the study was not
EDI Bulimia Subscale in Study 2 prospective, and thus it cannot be argued that perfectionism has
been shown to be an antecedent of bulimic symptoms. Second,
(for Partial structured clinical interviews were not conducted, and results
Order of entry/ Ffor within-set correlation
on the diagnostic variable reported herein should be interpreted
predictors in set set predictors # (PR/pr)
cautiously, although it was reassuring that these results eon-
1 . Main effects 55.83 2,452 .45 verged with those using the EDI Bulimia subscale, without ex-
Perfectionism 5.00 452 .23 ception. Another potential measurement concern involves the
Perceived weight status 8.63 452 .38 EDI Perfectionism subscale, which measures perfectionism in
2. Two-way interaction 4.29 1,451 .10
Perfectionism x
perceived weight 2
Interestingly, the analysis comparing those with discrepant perceived
status 2.07 451 .10
and actual weight statuses was not possible, because too few perfection-
Note. EDI — Eating Disorders Inventory (Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, istic women in 1992 reported actual weight status in the overweight
1983); PR — multiple partial correlation for a set of predictors; pr = range. This pattern mirrors the trend, reported elsewhere (Heatherton et
partial correlation for within-set predictors. All effects are significant al., 1995), of a relative decrease from 1982 to 1992 in weight and eating
at the p < .05 level. problems.
152 JOINER, HEATHERTON, RUDD, AND SCHMIDT

general, and not in weight-specific terms. In response, it should view and in emotional distress mediate the relation between
be noted that any discrepancy between weight-specific and gen- perfectionism and bulimic symptoms represents an interesting
eral perfectionism should work against the hypotheses of the avenue for future research.
present study, and furthermore, the present model does not sug- The present results are quite consistent with self-discrepancy
gest a weight-specific altitude as a diathesis. Rather, a general theory (Higgins, 1987) and research (e.g., Strauman, Vookles,
attitude (perfectionism) is postulated to be activated by a Berenstein, Chaiken, & Higgins, 1991). Strauman and col-
weight-related variable to predict eating-related symptoms. It leagues theorized that girls are, in general, socialized to be
should also be noted that Hewitt, Flett, and colleagues (Hewitt, overcontrolled and, thus, may be more likely to suffer from
Flett, Turnbull-Donovan, & Mikail, 1991) have provided a so- overcontrolled symptoms and syndromes, such as eating disor-
phisticated measure of perfectionism, the Multidimensional Per- ders, whereas boys are socialized to be less controlled and, thus,
fectionism Scale, which would have provided a more compre- may be more likely to experience discontrolled symptoms and
hensive measure of Perfectionism. syndromes (e.g., conduct disorder). Consistent with this view,
The nomological status of perfectionism as a correlate of these researchers found that a discrepancy between ideal and
bulimic symptoms deserves consideration. As was mentioned actual views of the self was highly related to bulimotypic symp-
earlier, the studies were not prospective, and thus the data cannot toms, such as body dissatisfaction, among undergraduate
speak to whether perfectionism precedes bulimia. At the same women. The present findings were very similar; for women
time, however, the findings are not particularly consistent with whose view of actual self (i.e., perceived weight) was discrep-
the view that perfectionism is merely a correlate, associated ant from the ideal (i.e., perfectionism), bulimic symptoms were
feature, or consequence of bulimic symptoms. If it were, one more likely.
might expect a correlation between perfectionism and bulimia Strauman et al.'s (1991) theory that overcontrolled symptoms
regardless of the levels of any other variables, such as perceived are more likely in women than in men is interesting in fight of
weight status. That is, if perfectionism were an associated fea- Rothenberg's (1990) hypothesis that many eating-disordered
ture or consequence of bulimia, one might expect that perfec- symptoms are manifestations of obsessive-compulsive pathol-
tionism and bulimia would be similarly related for all women, ogy. In defense of this position, Rothenberg found that eating-
regardless of whether they perceive themselves as overweight. disordered patients, when compared with general psychiatric
The pattern of findings predicted and found in the current study patients, displayed higher levels of rumination, rituals, and ex-
suggests either a diathesis-stress view, wherein perfectionism cessive cleanliness and orderliness. It is also noteworthy that
precedes bulimia for some women but not others or, alternatively, both bulimia (Hudson, Laffer, & Pope, 1982) and obsessive-
a subset of women with bulimic symptoms (i.e., those who also compulsive disorder (Benkert, Wetzel, & Szegedi, 1993) have
perceive themselves as overweight) for whom perfectionism is been postulated to be manifestations of mood disorder. One
an associated feature or consequence of bulimia. A prospective could interpret these syndromal interrelations as supportive of
study could address this issue more definitively. Strauman et al.'s "overcontrolled" hypothesis, although one
It is interesting to note that Hewitt, Flett and colleagues (e.g., would need to grapple with the lack of gender differences in
Hewitt & Flett, 1991a) have emphasized the difference between obsessive—compulsive disorder.
socially prescribed perfectionism (perceiving that others expect Although our findings were based on nonclinical samples,
perfection from you) and self-oriented perfectionism (expecting they may have implications for the psychotherapy of bulimic
oneself to be perfect). Some "imperfections" may be expected symptomatology (both cognitive—behavioral and interpersonal
to impinge more powerfully on self-oriented versus socially therapies have received at least preliminary support as effective
prescribed perfectionism. However, perceived weight status may treatments for bulimia; Fairburn, Marcus, & Wilson, 1993;
not operate in this way, since women may expect themselves to Fairburn et al., 1995). Levels of perfectionism should be as-
be thin, and may expect others to expect them to be thin. In sessed (Hewitt et al.'s [1991] Multidimensional Perfectionism
accord with this view, current results were similar when the Scale is a well-researched device) and treatment tailored accord-
EDI Perfectionism subscale was divided into self-oriented and ingly. For example, in the context of a cognitive-behavioral
socially prescribed items (see Joiner & Schmidt, 1995 for dis- approach, high levels of perfectionism should be an early and
cussion of use of subset of EDI Perfectionism subscale as mea- constant focus, especially insofar as reanalysis of the National
sures of socially prescribed vs. self-oriented perfectionism). Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collabora-
Our findings are consistent with Heatherton and Baumeister's tive Research program suggests that perfectionism levels among
(1991) theory that binge eating may serve as an escape from depressed people remain high despite treatment and that perfec-
painful self-awareness. These theorists hypothesized that binge tionism predicts low functioning at follow-up (Blatt, Quinlan,
eaters are acutely sensitive to standards, and when they fall Pilkonis, & Shea, 1995). From an interpersonal perspective, it
short of standards, binge eaters view themselves negatively and is important to note that perfectionism has been demonstrated
assume that others do as well (cf. self-oriented vs. socially to affect quality of relationships generally (Hewitt, Flett, &
prescribed perfectionism). These painful self-perceptions, ac- Mikail, in press), as well as the quality of therapeutic alliance
companied by attendant emotional distress, cause an escape (Blatt ct al., 1995). The dimensions of socially prescribed per-
response, characterized by disinhibition and, thus, binge eating. fectionism, as well as other-oriented perfectionism (expecting
In terms of the present study, it is interesting to note that Heath- others to be perfect), appear particularly relevant to interper-
erton and Baumeister's theory proposes a mechanism (self-view sonal approaches.
and emotional distress) through which perfectionism may affect In summary, a diathesis-stress model was proposed, wherein
eating behavior among women who perceive that standards have general perfectionism was framed as a risk factor for bulimia,
gone unmet. Empirical scrutiny of whether changes in self- but only when standards were perceived to have gone unmet.
PERFECTIONISM, PERCEIVED WEIGHT, BULIMIC SYMPTOMS 153

Perceived weight status was argued to be more important than Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., & Mikail, S. F. (in press). Perfectionism and
actual weight status. Results from two large, nonclinical samples relationship maladjustment in chronic pain patients and their spouses.
Journal of Family Psychology.
of women were consistent with the model.
Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., lurnbull-Donovan, W., & Mikail, S. F. (1991).
The Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale: Reliability, validity, and
References
psychometric properties in psychiatric samples. Psychological Assess-
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical ment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, .?, 464-468.
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and af-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical fect. Psychological Review, 94, 319-340.
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Hudson, J. I., Laffer, P. S., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (1982). Bulimia related
Benkert, O., Wetzel, H., & Szegedi, A. (1993). Serotonin dysfunction to affective disorder by family history and response to the dexametha-
syndromes: A functional common denominator for classification of sone suppression test. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 685-
depression, anxiety, and obsessive-compulsive disorders. International 687.
Clinical Psychopharmacology, S(Suppl. 1), 3-14. Hurley, J. B., Palmer, R. L., & Stretch, D. (1990). The specificity of the
Blatt, S. J., Quinlan, D., Pilkonis, P., & Shea, M. T. (1995). Impact of Eating Disorders Inventory: A reappraisal. International Journal of
perfectionism and need for approval on the treatment of depression: Eating Disorders, 9, 419-424.
The National Institute of Mental Health treatment of depression col- Joiner, T. E., Jr. (1995). The price of soliciting and receiving negative
laborative research program revisited. Journal of Consulting and feedback: Self-verification theory as a vulnerability to depression the-
Clinical Psychology, 63, 125-132. ory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 364-372.
Blouin, A. G., Bushnik, T, Braaten, J., & Blouin, J. H. (1989). Bulimia Joiner, T. E. Jr., Metalsky, G. I., & Wonderlich, S. A. (1995). Bulimic
and diabetes: Distinct psychosocial profiles. International Journal of symptoms and the development of depressed symptoms: The moderat-
Eating Disorders, 8, 93-100. ing role of attributional style. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19,
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation 651-666.
analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Joiner. T. E., Jr., & Schmidt, N. B. (1995). Dimensions of perfectionism,
Fairburn, C. G., Marcus, M. D., & Wilson, G. T. (1993). Cognitive- life stress, and depressed and anxious symptoms: Prospective support
behavioral therapy for binge-eating and bulimia nervosa: A compre- for diathesis-stress but not specific vulnerability among male under-
hensive treatment manual. In C. G. Fairburn & G. T. Wilson (Eds.), graduates. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 165-183.
Binge eating: Nature, assessment, and treatment (pp. 361-404). New Kuczmarski, R. J. (1992). Prevalence of overweight and weight gain in
York: Guilford Press. the United States. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 55, 495S-
Fairbum, C. G., Norman, P. A., Welch, S. L., O'Connor, M. E., Doll, 502S.
H. A., & Peveler, R. C. (1995). A prospective study of outcome in Metalsky, G. I., Joiner, T. E., Jr., Hardin, T. S., & Abramson, L. Y.
bulimia nervosa and the long-term effects of three psychological treat- (1993). Depressive reactions to failure in a naturalistic setting: A test
ments. Archives of General Psychiatry. 52. 304-312. of the hopelessness and self-esteem theories of depression. Journal
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Blankstein, K. R., & Mosher, S. W. (1991). of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 101-109.
Perfectionism, life events, and depression: Testing a diathesis-stress Newton, T., Butler, N., & Slade, P. D. (1988). Denial of symptoms and
model. Canadian Psychology, 32, 311. self-report in eating disorders. British Review of Bulimia and Anorexia
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, PL., Blankstein, K. R., & O'Brien, S. (1991). Nervosa, 2, 55-59.
Perfectionism and learned resourcefulness in depression and self-es- Radke-Sharpe, N., Whitney-Saltiel, D., & Rodin, J. (1990). Fat distribu-
teem. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 61 -68. tion as a risk factor for weight and eating concerns. International
Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., Mania, J. I, & Neubauer, A. Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 27—36.
(1993). A comparison of two measures of perfectionism. Personality Rosch, D. S., Crowther, J. H., & Graham, J. R. (1991). MMPI-derived
and Individual Differences, 14, 119-126. personality description and personality subtypes in an undergraduate
Gainer, D. M., Olmstead, M. P., &Polivy,J. (1983). The Eating Disorder bulimic population. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 5, 15-22.
Inventory: A measure of cognitive-behavioral dimensions of anorexia Rothenberg, A. (1990). Adolescence and eating disorder: The obsessive-
nervosa and bulimia. In P. L. Darby, P. E. Garfinkel, D. M. Garner, & compulsive syndrome. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 13, 469-
D. V. Coscina (Eds.), Anorexia nervosa: Recent developments in re- 488.
search (pp. 173-184). New York: Alan R. Liss. Ruderman, A. (1986). Bulimia and irrational beliefs. Behaviour Re-
Heatherton, T. F., & flaumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape search and Therapy, 24, 193-197.
from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86-108. Strauman, T. J.. Vookles, J., Berenstein, V., Chaiken, S., & Higgins, E. T.
Heatherton, T. F., Nichols, P., Mahamedi, F., & Keel, P. (1995). Body (1991). Self-discrepancies and vulnerability to body dissatisfaction
weight, dieting, and eating disorder symptoms among college students, and disordered eating. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
1982 to 1992. American Journal of Psychiatry. 152, 1623-1629. 61, 946-956.
Hewitt, PL., &Dyck, D. G. (1986). Perfectionism, stress, and vulnera- Thompson, D. A., Berg, K. M., & Shatford, L. A. (1987). The heteroge-
bility to depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 137-142. neity of bulimic symptomatology: Cognitive and behavioral dimen-
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (199la). Dimensions of perfectionism in sions. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 6. 215-234.
unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 98-101. Zuckerman, D. M., Colby, A., Ware, N. C., & Lazerson, J. S. (1986).
Hewitt, PL., & Flett, G. L. (1991b). Perfectionism in the self and The prevalence of bulimia among college students. American Journal
social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with of Public Health, 76, 1135-1137.
psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
456-470.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1993). Dimensions of perfectionism, daily Received February 7, 1996
stress, and depression: A test of the specific vulnerability hypothesis. Revision received June 21, 1996
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 58—65. Accepted June 21, 1996 •

You might also like