Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 216–221

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Personality and altruism in the dictator game: Relationship to giving to kin,


collaborators, competitors, and neutrals
Avner Ben-Ner a,*, Amit Kramer b
a
Carlson School of Management, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States
b
School of Labor and Employment Relations, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, IL 61820, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: We investigate altruism in the context of the economic dictator game experiment where subjects are pre-
Received 19 January 2010 sented with different persons who can be classified as kin, collaborator, competitor and neutral based on
Received in revised form 12 April 2010 their similarity/relationship to the subject. The classification is based on the role others play in facilitating
Accepted 28 April 2010
or impeding an individual’s access to resources needed for reproductive success. The role of the Big Five
Available online 8 June 2010
personality traits in giving to the different target persons is examined. We find that kin are treated most
generously, followed by collaborators, neutrals, and competitors. Personality has no effect on giving to
Keywords:
kin, but a significant effect on giving to collaborator, neutral and competitor. We also find non-linear rela-
Altruism
Dictator game
tionships between personality and giving.
Personality Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Identity
Kin
Reciprocal altruism

1. Introduction Altruism has been investigated mostly relative to generic


‘‘other” persons who were usually not further identified. To assess
Altruistic behavior is a sacrifice of one’s resources for the benefit the role of the attributes of the potential beneficiary of altruistic
of others, representing a tradeoff between one’s self-interest and acts the reader may conduct a mental exercise by imagining a ‘‘per-
regard for others. Resources can include time (helping an elderly son.” What describes that person? Man or woman, young or old, an
person cross the street, visiting a sick relative), money (donating acquaintance or a stranger, from your country or from abroad, be-
money to a religious organization), or flesh (donating blood, plas- liever or agnostic? Would you respond to a survey or give in a DG
ma or organs). the same way if you imagined the other person as a man, young, an
In the psychological literature, altruism is usually measured by acquaintance, from your country and your own religion, or, alter-
asking respondents how they would behave or feel towards other natively, if you thought the other person was a woman, old, a
people in various situations, e.g., whether they would donate blood stranger, from another country, and from a different religion?
or help others in need (e.g., Rushton, Chrisjohn, & Fekken, 1981), or In this paper we focus on the concept of altruism as expressed
record actual behavior (e.g., Ferguson, Farrell, & Lawrence, 2008,). by the willingness-to-give money in the DG and investigate how
In economics, altruism has been measured by the amount of this tendency varies with respect to the attribute similarity between
money an individual is willing to give to someone else, usually in the sender and the receiver. Following a recent call by Borghans,
the context of an experiment called the dictator game (DG). In this Duckworth, Heckman, and ter Weel (2008) we investigate how
experiment, each subject in the role of ‘‘sender” is granted a mon- heterogeneity in altruism, as manifested by giving in the DG, is
etary endowment and is asked to consider keeping it or sending related to personality traits.
any portion of it to another person. Experiments similar to the From an evolutionary-theoretical perspective, scholars argued
DG have also been used in psychology (e.g., Ashton, Paunonen, that there are two types of altruism: kin altruism (Hamilton,
Helmes, & Jackson, 1998; Tajfel’s 1970). The amount sent is 1964) and reciprocal altruism (Trivers, 1971). Evolutionary theory
regarded as a measure of other-regarding or altruism (Andreoni emphasizes reproduction and resources that facilitate it as well as
& Miller, 2002; Ben-Ner & Putterman, 1998). providing for oneself and one’s offspring and for relatives and their
offspring. The resources include food, safety and physical protec-
tion, all of which are, and have been in the ancestral environment,
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 612 723 5558.
E-mail addresses: benne001@umn.edu (A. Ben-Ner), kram0262@umn.edu in scarce supply. Kin altruism implies helping related persons in
(A. Kramer). order to improve their – and one’s own – reproductive success.

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.04.024
A. Ben-Ner, A. Kramer / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 216–221 217

Kin altruism is undergirded by kin selection, which consists of the and that after controlling for attachment insecurities, there is no
evolution of characteristics conducive to the reproductive success association between personality and volunteering (Erez, Mikulin-
of close relatives including willingness to sacrifice resources that cer, van Ijzendoorn, & Kroonenberg, 2008).
aid one’s own reproduction for the benefit of kin and the ability There have been only a handful of studies that investigated the
to discern kin from others (Hamilton, 1964; Daly & Wilson, relationship between personality and giving in the context of the
1988). Reciprocal altruism entails making sacrifices for unrelated DG. Ben-Ner, Kong, and Putterman (2004a) find that when the re-
others who are likely to provide at least as much help in the future ceiver is identified only as a ‘‘person”, openness is positively asso-
in a reciprocal fashion, so it entails an evolved ability to recognize ciated with giving, but when the gender of the receiver is
potential partners to transactions and the likelihood that they will identified, agreeableness is mildly and positively associated with
indeed reciprocate (Fehr & Fischbacher, 2003; Trivers, 1971). giving, whereas extraversion affects negatively males’ giving and
But kin and potential collaborators are not the only types of neuroticism affects negatively females’ giving (Ben-Ner,
persons an individual may interact with. Some persons may be Putterman, Kong, & Magan, 2004b). Ben-Ner, Kramer, and Levy
regarded as foes or competitors, and others as neither friend (2008) find that giving with actual and hypothetical money is
nor foe. On the basis of an individual’s direct or cultural experi- nearly identical on average, but there are differences associated
ences he or she may classify other persons as either (1) kin, (2) with personality: agreeableness is associated with greater giving
a collaborator or potential contributor to one’s reproductive and with actual money, whereas extraversion is associated with more
survivability resources, (3) a direct competitor for or absconder generous giving of hypothetical money. Swope, Cadigan, Schmitt,
of such resources, or (4) a neutral, someone who has no bearing and Shupp (2008) did not find associations between personality
on these resources. Individuals classify other people into these and giving in the DG.
categories on the basis of observed attributes, the expected future The research on the relationship between altruism and person-
benefits and threats from interaction with them, and what they ality thus suggests that altruism is positively associated with
learn from their own culture. Person 1 may be an individual’s agreeableness and negatively with neuroticism. In our empirical
identical twin or a possible relative. Person 2 may be a friend, a work we test for associations between these and other personality
neighbor, a coworker, or someone adhering to the same religion. traits and giving in the DG, and investigate the possibility that
Person 3 may be an active member of an enemy group that these associations are not linear. Although personality effects are
wishes to take away one’s myriad resources, a member of an usually assumed to be linear, studies have shown that this is not
opposing tribe or a fan of a rival sports team. Person 4 may be always so (e.g., Cucina & Vasilopoulos, 2005; Day & Silverman,
a member of a geographically-distant and non-competing tribe 1989; Egloff & Hock, 2001; Robins, Tracy, & Trzesniewski, 2001).
or a random person in the street. In addition, we examine whether the association between person-
The nature of altruism towards each of these persons is differ- ality and giving depends on the target of the altruism (kin, collab-
ent. Person 1 will be treated most favorably as a direct investment orator, competitor, and neutral).
in one’s reproduction; person 2 would be treated favorably to the
degree that one expects this person will reciprocate, but would
2. Methods
be treated less favorably than kin; person 3 would be undermined,
or treated least generously, depending on the nature of the interac-
2.1. Participants
tion; and person 4 will be treated less generously than kin and col-
laborators but more generously than competitors. We test this
All freshmen at the University of Minnesota were invited to
hypothesized ordering of the degree of altruism in giving to the
participate in economic-psychological experiments; nearly 10% re-
four types of persons, going beyond the standard in-group/out-
sponded, with 222 actually showing up at the experiment. The
group dichotomy and extending the investigation of the effects of
average age of the sample was 18.8 years, with female and Cauca-
identity categories (Ben-Ner, McCall, Stephane, & Wang, 2009)
sian majorities (64.0% and 71.4%, respectively).
and of social distance (Osiński, 2009).
The literature on personality and altruism is limited. Ashton
et al. (1998) examine the relationship between personality and 2.2. Materials and design
kin and reciprocal altruism. They hypothesize that kin altruism en-
tails assessment of costs and benefits to one’s kin and thus should Participants were assigned to one of two identical sessions. Sub-
be related to empathy and attachment traits, thus negatively to jects completed the following steps: (1) a timed 12-min cognitive-
emotional stability and positively to agreeableness, whereas reci- ability test, (2) a personality inventory, (3) a willingness-to-give
procal altruism entails forgiveness and retaliation, thus positively survey-experiment, (4) four more experiments that are not used
related to emotional stability and agreeableness. Their findings in this paper (discussed in Ben-Ner et al., 2009), and (5) a detailed
generally support the hypothesized relationships. Osiński (2009) personal background questionnaire. Each step was contained in a
augments the framework used by Ashton et al. (1998) by examin- separate envelope that was sealed after its completion. Subjects
ing the effect of social distance on the relationship between per- were then paid a $15 participation fee.
sonality and kin and reciprocal altruism. He finds that altruism In step 3 subjects were asked to consider seriatim and sepa-
declines with social distance and with the other’s willingness to rately 91 individuals, each characterized by one descriptor such
reciprocate, and is positively associated with agreeableness and as ‘‘is tall,” ‘‘is from Argentina,” or ‘‘is your brother in law”. The
negatively with neuroticism. Krueger, Hicks, and McGue (2001) analysis in the present paper uses the altruistic decisions subjects
investigate the relationship between personality traits and altru- made (step 3) and the data collected on subjects’ cognitive ability,
ism and antisocial behavior. They find that altruism and antisocial personality traits and background.
behavior are unrelated, and that altruism is associated with posi- Of the 222 subjects, 20 participated in a DG with just one other
tive emotionality whereas antisocial behavior is related to negative person (female) to test for possible bias associated with the inclu-
emotionality. Other studies find that charitable giving is positively sion of multiple persons in the same experiment. Tests show that
associated with openness and extraversion, whereas blood and there is no statistically significant difference between what the
organ donation are positively associated with agreeableness 202 subjects gave to a female in the multiple-person experiment
(Bekkers, 2006), that volunteerism is weakly and positively associ- and what the 20 subjects in the single-person experiment gave a
ated with agreeableness (Carlo, Okun, Knight, & de Guzman, 2005), female.
218 A. Ben-Ner, A. Kramer / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 216–221

Fig. 1. Distribution of giving in dollars (horizontal axis), by category (kin, collaborator, neutral and competitor).

2.3. Classification into different groups Table 2


Mean and standard deviation of giving, by category (in dollars).

Of the 91 ‘‘other persons” included in the experiment, we clas- Mean giving Mean giving Mean giving Mean giving
sify 74 persons into groups of kin, potential collaborators, potential to kin to collaborator to neutral to competitor

competitors and neutrals on the basis of the relationship between Meana 4.91 3.40 2.49 1.99
the information we collected about each subject and the other per- Standard 3.59 3.30 2.97 2.61
deviation
son (17 persons could not be classified). Details of the matching
Number of 790 2918 7105 3760
procedure are available from the authors. The two authors of the decisions
paper independently coded each person into one of the four a
All means are different from each other at the .01 level.
groups. For only three persons out of the 74 there was disagree-
ment; after discussion a consensus has been achieved.

2.4. Analysis giving to the four different groups. Subjects give most to kin
($4.91), followed by collaborators ($3.40), neutrals ($2.49) and
We first compare the means of amount given to the four groups. competitors ($1.99). All differences are significant at the .01 level.
Next, we analyze giving to persons in each of the four groups in Fig. 1 presents the distribution of giving to persons in each group,
relation to personality, controlling for cognitive ability, gender, which shows that in almost 50% of the observations nothing is
age, and birth order. We use random-effects GLS regression to ac- given to competitors as compared to less than 20% for giving to
count for the multiple decisions made by each subject. Because kin. In only 5% of the observations competitors are given more than
giving was constrained between zero and ten we also use Tobit $5, but in more than 30% of the observations kin are given more
analysis. To account for the skewed distribution of giving, with than $5. Table 3 provides further evidence for the hypothesized
many subjects giving zero, especially to competitors (see Fig. 1), order of giving. Controlling for the Big Five personality traits, birth
we also use Poisson regression. The results were very similar in order, age, gender and cognitive ability, subjects gave $2.40 more
all three types of analysis, hence we show only the random-effects to kin than to neutrals (column 1; p < .01), 90 cents more to collab-
GLS results. The number of subjects with valid observations is 181 orators than to neutrals (p < .01) and 49 cents less to competitors
instead of 202 because of missing values. than to neutrals (p < .01). Thus the results support our basic
hypothesis.
Concerning results for personality, consider first Model 1 in
3. Results
Table 3. In this model the personality variables are entered linearly
and without interactions, with only extraversion showing a
Table 1 provides the mean, standard deviation and pair-wise
statistically significant effect on giving. In Model 2 we allow for
correlations of the study variables. Table 2 provides the mean
quadratic effects of personality, yet without interaction with the
Table 1 type of other person. We find that subjects higher on neuroticism
Descriptive statistics of the study’s variables. give significantly more (b = .19, p < .05) but giving declines with in-
creases in neuroticism, as evidenced by the negative coefficient of
Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5
the quadratic term (b = .003, p < .10). Subjects higher on extraver-
1. Mean giving 2.74 2.12 –
sion give significantly less money (b = .46, p < .05) but this nega-
2. Neuroticism 22.09 7.64 .04 –
3. Extraversion 30.74 6.43 .19** .33** –
tive effect is reversed at higher levels of extraversion, as evident
4. Openness 29.29 6.48 .11 .05 .08 – from the quadratic term (b = .009, p < .01). We find similar results
5. Agreeableness 30.83 5.61 .01 .27** .43** .06 – for conscientiousness. Subjects higher on conscientiousness give
6. Conscientiousness 31.14 6.18 .05 .32** .14* .17* .10 significantly less money (b = .54, p < .01) but this negative effect
*
p < .05. is diminishing as evident from the quadratic term (b = .008,
**
p < .01. p < .01) and in fact becomes positive at high levels of
A. Ben-Ner, A. Kramer / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 216–221 219

Table 3 conscientiousness. For agreeableness we find an initial positive ef-


Determinants of giving to other persons, random-effects GLM regression. fect (but statistically not significant), but giving declines at higher
Model 1 Model 2a Model 3b,c levels of agreeableness. Based on these results we fail to support
Kin 2.40**
2.40**
2.38** our hypotheses about the relationship between neuroticism and
(.08) (.08) (.08) agreeableness and altruism. Surprisingly, neuroticism is positively
Collaborator .90** .90** .90** related to giving and only at high levels of neuroticism do we ob-
(.05) (.05) (.05) serve a negative effect. Agreeableness is not related to giving. Mod-
Competitor .49** .49** .49**
(.05) (.05) (.05)
el 3 includes interactions between the different personality traits
Neuroticism .04 .19* .03 and the target groups dummies, which permit us to examine the
(.03) (.10) (.03) moderating effects of personality on giving to persons belonging
Extraversion .11** .46* .10** to different categories. Because we are interested in understanding
(.03) (.22) (.03)
the moderating role of personality more clearly, we analyze giving
Openness .01 .12 .00
(.03) (.16) (.03) to persons in each of the four categories (kin, collaborator, neutral
Agreeableness .05 .37 .07* and competitor) separately in Table 4.
(.03) (.24) (.03) In Table 4, as a group, the squared terms on personality traits in
Conscientiousness .02 .54** .04 Model 2 are statistically significant for all target groups except kin.
(.03) (.18) (.03)
We therefore emphasize findings based on Model 2. As hypothe-
Neuroticism squared .003 .004
(.002) (.002) sized, personality plays a very limited role in explaining altruistic
Extraversion squared .009* .009** behavior towards kin. Only extraversion seems to have some effect
(.004) (.004) on giving to kin (negative at low levels of this trait but positive at
Openness squared .002 .002
higher levels). Personality plays a much greater role in giving to the
(.002) (.003)
Agreeableness squared .007 .007 other target groups.
(.004) (.004) Regarding collaborators, neuroticism has a positive effect on giv-
Conscientiousness squared .008** .008** ing (b = .196, p < .10). For extraversion and conscientiousness we
(.002) (.003) find a concave relationship: declining (b = .532, p < .05 and
Neuroticismkin .01
b = .579, p < .01, respectively) then increasing (b = .011, p < .01
(.01)
Extraversionkin .04** and b = .009, p < .01, respectively). We find very similar effects for
(.02) personality in giving to neutrals and competitors. When giving to
Opennesskin .01 neutrals, neuroticism and agreeableness are first positively related
(.01)
to giving (b = .196, p < .05 and b = .399, p < .10, respectively) but
Agreeablenesskin .05**
(.02)
negatively at higher levels of these scores (b = .004, p < .10 and
Conscientiousnesskin .03 b = .008, p < .10, respectively). Extraversion and conscientious-
(.01) ness are first negatively related to giving (b = .438, p < .10 and
Neuroticismcollaborator .00 b = .549, p < .01, respectively) but positively later (b = .009,
(.01)
p < .01 and b = .008, p < .01, respectively). Concerning competitors,
Extraversioncollaborator .02*
(.01) neuroticism is positively related to giving (b = .186, p < .10),
Opennesscollaborator .01 whereas extraversion and conscientiousness are both negatively
(.01) related initially (b = .461, p < .05 and b = .540, p < .01, respec-
Agreeablenesscollaborator .02
tively) but positively at higher levels of these traits (b = .009,
(.01)
Conscientiousnesscollaborator .01
p < .05 and b = .008, p < .01, respectively). In all regressions in
(.01) Table 4, the openness trait shows no association with giving.
Neuroticismcompetitor .01 Cognitive ability and the other variables (gender, age and birth
(.01) order) play no statistically significant role in explaining giving in
Extraversioncompetitor .01
our sample.
(.01)
Opennesscompetitor .02* The results of the non-linear effect of personality on giving to
(.01) persons in each of the four groups are presented in Fig. 2a–d. The
Agreeablenesscompetitor .01 relationships are quite similar across giving to persons in the four
(.01)
categories, although they are weakest for giving to kin.
Conscientiousnesscompetitor .00
(.01)
Constant 2.21 9.25 3.76*
(2.61) (5.71) (1.76)
R-squared .0997 .1404 .1420 4. Discussion

Wald v2 1523.38** 1496.82** 1543.23** Altruism has been studied by psychologists and economists in
N (groups) 13,370 13,370 13,370 (181) different ways. While psychologists generally conceptualize and
(181) (181)
measure altruism broadly, economists focus on monetary altruism.
Note on robustness of classification: we performed the same analyses using a nar- In this paper we follow the call to bring the two disciplines closer
rower classification of persons as kin, collaborators, neutrals, and competitors (Borghans et al., 2008; Frey & Stutzer, 2007) by examining individ-
(therefore fewer observations). This robustness check yielded nearly identical
ual differences in altruistic behavior in the DG in relation to per-
results to the analysis presented above. In all analyses we also control for gender,
age, first-born, youngest child and cognitive ability. None are significant at the .10 sonality and the relationship between the actor and the
level. beneficiary of the altruistic behavior. We suggest that altruistic
a
Model 2 is significantly better than Mode 1 (v2 = 17.28, p < .01). behavior is ‘‘target dependent” and hypothesize that its level is
b
In Model 3 mean centering was used for all the big five personality traits.
c
greater when the target is kin, then a collaborator, then a neutral
Model 3 is significantly better than Model 2 (v2 = 42.16, p < .01).
p < .10.
person, and last and least, a competitor for resources who may
*
p < .05. harm one’s reproductive success. Attitudes towards others are
**
p < .01. likely to be dictated by the way they are classified in the mind of
220 A. Ben-Ner, A. Kramer / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 216–221

Table 4
Determinants of giving to persons, by category, random-effects GLM regression.

Kina Collaboratorb Neutralc Competitord


Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2
Neuroticism .040 .203 .042 .196 .040 .196* .046 .186
(.034) (.132) (.029) (.112) (.026) (.098) (.026) (.098)
Extraversion .079* .473 .126** .532* .103** .438 .098** .461*
(.039) (.302) (.033) (.255) (.030) (.225) (.030) (.225)
Openness .021 .226 .009 .044 .002 .117 .020 .171
(.034) (.211) (.029) (.178) (.026) (.157) (.026) (.157)
Agreeableness .000 .069 .067 .357 .054 .399 .039 .384
(.042) (.324) (.036) (.274) (.032) (.242) (.032) (.242)
Conscientiousness .006 .237 .017 .579** .027 .549** .022 .540**
(.036) (.244) (.031) (.206) (.028) (.182) (.028) (.182)
Neuroticism2 .004 .004 .004 .003
(.003) (.002) (.002) (.002)
Extraversion2 .009 .011** .009** .009*
(.005) (.004) (.004) (.004)
Openness2 .004 .001 .002 .003
(.004) (.003) (.003) (.003)
Agreeableness2 .001 .007 .008 .007
(.005) (.005) (.004) (.004)
Conscientiousness2 .004 .009** .008** .008**
(.004) (.003) (.003) (.003)
Constant 2.065 8.469 3.274 13.092 2.611 8.881 1.255 7.794
(3.400) (7.704) (2.957) (6.507) (2.613) (5.736) (2.613) (5.741)
R-squared .0246 .0438 .0588 .1020 .0511 .0952 .0610 .1145
Wald v2 7.69 13.79 18.92* 36.00** 18.84* 37.62** 18.92* 37.49**
N (Groups) 724 724 2697 2697 6513 6513 3436 3436
(181) (181) (181) (181) (181) (181) (181) (181)

p < .10.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
a
For kin, Model 2 is not significantly better than Model 1 (v2 = 6.04, p > .05).
b
For collaborator, Model 2 is significantly better than Model 1 (v2 = 15.86, p < .01).
c
For neutral, Model 2 is significantly better than Model 1 (v2 = 17.40, p < .01).
d
For competitor, Model 2 is significantly better than Model 1 (v2 = 17.21, p < .01).

Fig. 2. The means of other variables were used in constructing these graphs. Calculations are based on Table 4, Model 2 random-effects results. (a) Giving to different target
groups by extraversion level. (b) Giving to different target groups by agreeableness level. (c) Giving to different target groups by neuroticism level. (d) Giving to different
target groups by conscientiousness level.
A. Ben-Ner, A. Kramer / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 216–221 221

an individual and therefore altruism can be elicited in the labora- ings also highlight the need to consider non-linear effects of per-
tory in anonymous one-shot situations. sonality by including quadratic terms, which in our sample and
The data from the DG experiments confirm this hypothesis. We models almost double the variance explained by the model (Table
obtained the anticipated ordering, with differences that are eco- 4, Model 2). Much more work is needed to provide a theoretical
nomically and statistically highly significant. This four-way classi- framework for understanding the role of personality in altruistic
fication constitutes a refinement of the ‘‘us vs. them” or ‘‘in-group behavior.
vs. out-group” framework that dominates the literature, and offers
insights into differing altruistic ‘‘impulses.”
References
We expected that subjects will not show any altruism towards
competitors. Indeed, in nearly 50% of giving opportunities to com- Andreoni, J., & Miller, J. (2002). Giving according to GARP: An experimental test of
petitors nothing was actually given; on the other hand, more than the consistency of preferences for altruism. Econometrica, 70, 737–753.
nothing was given in more than half of the cases, for an average of Ashton, M. C., Paunonen, S. V., Helmes, E., & Jackson, D. N. (1998). Kin altruism,
reciprocal altruism, and the big five personality factors. Evolution and Human
$1.99. This is only 40% of what is given on average to kin, but is still Behavior, 19, 243–255.
more than expected. Several factors may contribute to this. First, Bekkers, R. (2006). Traditional and health-related philanthropy: The role of
we noted that giving in DG may reflect more than just altruism. resources and personality. Social Psychology Quarterly, 69, 349–366.
Ben-Ner, A., Kong, F., & Putterman, L. (2004a). Share and share alike? Gender-
An opportunity to give may be interpreted by some subjects as pairing, personality, and cognitive ability as determinants of giving. Journal of
an expectation to give some amount of money. We would expect Economic Psychology, 25, 581–589.
this effect, if it exists, to be strongest for more agreeable subjects, Ben-Ner, A., Kramer, A., & Levy, O. (2008). Economic and hypothetical dictator game
experiments: Incentive effects at the individual level. Journal of Socio-Economics,
but we find that greater agreeableness is associated with lower
37, 1775–1784.
giving. Alternatively, subjects who are more neurotic may be more Ben-Ner, A., McCall, B., Stephane, M., & Wang, H. (2009). Identity and in-group/out-
likely to accede to such an implicit demand; we do find that for a group differentiation in work and giving behaviors: Experimental evidence.
wide range of scores, more neurotic subjects give more money. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 72, 153–170.
Ben-Ner, A., & Putterman, L. (1998). Values and institutions in economic analysis. In
Second, it is possible that some subjects are inequality averse or A. Ben-Ner & L. Putterman (Eds.), Economics, values, and organization (pp. 3–72).
fairness prone, so they want to share some of their endowment Cambridge University Press.
with others, even competitors. Finally, our categorization of other Ben-Ner, A., Putterman, L., Kong, F., & Magan, D. (2004b). Reciprocity in a two-part
dictator game. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 53, 333–352.
persons may not be shared by all subjects, and some ‘‘other per- Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & ter Weel, B. (2008). The economics
sons” whom we classified as competitors may be perceived by and psychology of personality traits. Journal of Human Resources, 43, 972–
some subjects as members of other groups. Because subjects could 1059.
Carlo, G., Okun, M. A., Knight, G. P., & de Guzman, M. R. T. (2005). The interplay of
not give less than nothing, misclassifications of persons into the traits and motives on volunteering: Agreeableness, extraversion and prosocial
competitor group can result only in greater giving to competitors. value motivation. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1293–1305.
However, a robustness check we performed with giving to persons Cucina, J. M., & Vasilopoulos, N. L. (2005). Nonlinear personality–performance
relationships and the spurious moderating effects of traitedness. Journal of
who can be more narrowly classified yielded essentially identical Personality, 73, 227–259.
results (see note following Table 3). Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Evolutionary social–psychology and family homicide.
Our results regarding the role of personality are intriguing. As Science, 242, 519–524.
Day, D. V., & Silverman, S. B. (1989). Personality and job performance: Evidence of
expected, we find that personality plays essentially no role in
incremental validity. Personnel Psychology, 42, 25–36.
explaining altruism toward kin. This result is consistent with the Egloff, B., & Hock, M. (2001). Interactive effects of state anxiety and trait anxiety on
evolutionary view that kin altruism is hard wired and strong, and emotional Stroop interference. Personality and Individual Differences, 31,
therefore few individual differences will be observed in it. How- 875–882.
Erez, A., Mikulincer, M., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. M. (2008).
ever, in interactions with non-kin where it is less clear how altru- Attachment, personality, and volunteering: Placing volunteerism in an
ism will contribute to the individual’s reproductive success, attachment-theoretical framework. Personality and Individual Differences, 44,
personality traits become significant in explaining giving to others 64–74.
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425,
in the categories of collaborator, neutral and competitor. Specifi- 785–791.
cally, we find a positive-diminishing relationship for neuroticism Ferguson, E., Farrell, K., & Lawrence, C. (2008). Blood donation is an act of
and giving, a U-shaped relationship for extraversion and conscien- benevolence rather than altruism. Health Psychology, 27, 327–336.
Frey, S. B., & Stutzer, A. (2007). Economics and psychology: A promising new cross-
tiousness and giving and a positive-diminishing effect for agree- disciplinary field. MIT Press.
ableness and giving. Our findings provide further support for the Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. I. Journal of
non-linear relationship between personality and different out- Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.
Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., & McGue, M. (2001). Altruism and antisocial behavior:
comes. We find that extraversion has a consistent U-shaped Independent tendencies, unique personality correlates, distinct etiologies.
relationship with altruism. Subjects who were highly introverted Psychological Science, 12, 397–402.
gave a little bit more money than subjects who were a little Osiński, J. (2009). Kin altruism, reciprocal altruism and social discounting.
Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 374–378.
introverted, but the relationship becomes positive and strong for
Robins, R. W., Tracy, J. L., & Trzesniewski, K. (2001). Personality correlates of self-
subjects who were at the mean or above the mean of the extraver- esteem. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 463–482.
sion scale. Rushton, J. P., Chrisjohn, R. D., & Fekken, G. C. (1981). The altruistic personality and
The highest levels of altruism in giving to all target groups are the self-report altruism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 2, 293–
302.
exhibited by individuals high on neuroticism and extraversion, Swope, K. J., Cadigan, J., Schmitt, P. M., & Shupp, R. (2008). Personality preferences in
and low on conscientiousness and agreeableness. The lowest lev- laboratory economics experiments. Journal of Socio-Economics, 37, 998–1009.
els of giving are exhibited by individuals who are emotionally sta- Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American, 223,
96–102.
ble (low on neuroticism), average-low on extraversion, high on Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology,
agreeableness and average-low on conscientiousness. These find- 46, 35–57.

You might also like