Testing The Beneficial Acclimation Hypo

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

66 Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.

2 February 2002

Testing the beneficial in which those changes were induced. A daptive


arguments were often formulated after identifying
the functional role of the phenotypic modification and

acclimation usually involved logical arguments that showed how


the phenotype enhanced reproductive success, growth
or survival. T his long-held assumption, now referred

hypothesis to as the beneficial acclimation hypothesis (B A H) [8],


has recently received a significant amount of
experimental interrogation.
T he B A H h as been tested predomin a ntly by
Robbie S. Wilson and Craig E. Franklin exa mining the acclim a tory responses of ectotherms to
temper a ture. T he hypothesis predicts th a t a nim als
acclim a ted to a pa rticula r temper a ture h ave
Recent developments in evolutionary physiology have seen many of the enh a nced perform a nce or fitness a t th a t temper a ture
long-held assumptions within comparative physiology receive rigorous in compa rison with a nim als acclim a ted to other
experimental analysis. Studies of the adaptive significance of physiological temper a tures. However, to the surprise of m a ny
acclimation exemplify this new evolutionary approach. The beneficial compa r a tive physiologists, all empirical
acclimation hypothesis (BAH) was proposed to describe the assumption that exa min a tions of the B A H so fa r h ave rejected its
all acclimation changes enhance the physiological performance or fitness of gener ality [8,10–12]. T hese studies h ave
an individual organism. To the surprise of most physiologists, all empirical demonstr a ted th a t the phenotypic ch a nges
examinations of the BAH have rejected its generality. However, we suggest (P H E N O T Y P I C P L A S T I C I T Y ) th a t occur in orga nisms during
that these examinations are neither direct nor complete tests of the functional development in different therm al environments do
benefit of acclimation. We consider them to be elegant analyses of the adaptive not alw ays lead to a n increased fitness in th a t
significance of developmental plasticity, a type of phenotypic plasticity that environment when compa red with the fitness of
is very different from the traditional concept of acclimation that is used by orga nisms r aised a t other temper a tures.
comparative physiologists. I n the first test of the B A H , L eroi et a l. [8] exposed
genetically identical lines of the bacteria Escherichi a
To bring t r adition al compa r a tive physiology in to coli to either 32°C or 41.5°C for 24 h (~6.7 cell
line wit h con tempor a ry evolu tion a ry biology, gener a tions d−1 a t 37°C) a nd then competed the two
physiologists over t he past decade or so h ave been groups a t both exposure temper a tures ( F ig. 1). T he
using a more t heoretical a nd hypot hesis-driven B A H w as used to predict th a t the 32°C group would
approach to evolu tion a ry questions in physiological outcompete the 41.5°C group a t 32°C, a nd vice versa
resea rch. H istorically, m a ny st udies in compa r a tive a t 41.5°C. However, bacteria grown up a t 32°C
physiology proposed post-hoc adaptive stories to outcompeted the 41.5°C-group a t both temper a tures,
explain t he fu nction al significa nce of a physiological a nd so the B A H w as rejected ( F ig. 1). I n a more
t r ait after elucida ting its mech a nistic basis. extensive test of the B A H , Bennett a nd L ensk i [9]
H owever, m a ny critics of t he adapt a tionist r aised E . coli a t 22, 27, 32, 37 or 40°C a nd then
progr a m me h ave highligh ted t h a t t here a re m a ny competed the different acclim a tion groups against
alter n a tives to adaptive scen a rios [1,2], including each other a t each temper a ture. As in the previous
genetic drift, past selection, genetic cor rela tions a nd study, m a ny groups were outcompeted a t their
historical a t t ribu tes [3]. T he st rengt h a nd success ‘acclim a tion’ temper a ture by bacteria r aised a t other
of t his new evolu tion a ry approach to compa r a tive temper a tures (benefit for acclim a tion w as found in
physiology is reflected by t he diversity of st udies only seven out of the 12 compa risons). Again, these
t h a t a re producing a deeper u nderst a nding of t he results were used to reject the gener ality of the B A H .
evolu tion of physiological systems (e.g. Refs [4–6], G ibert a nd co-wor kers [14] recently outlined
reviewed in Ref. [7]). a nother experiment al test of the B A H . T hey r aised
D rosophil a mel a nogaster from two different
The beneficial acclimation hypothesis popula tions a t 18, 25 or 29°C a nd then tested the
O ne of the best examples of this new approach to w al k ing speed of each development al group a t each
Robbie S. Wilson* physiological research has been the experimental temper a ture. T he B A H w as used to predict th a t flies
Dept of Biology, analysis of the adaptive significance of physiological would w al k faster a t their actu al development al
University of Antwerp,
A C C L I M A T I O N [8–12] (see G lossary). Traditionally, temper a ture th a n would flies developed a t other
Universiteitsplein 1,
B-2610 Wilrijk, Belgium. acclimation has been defined as the adjustment of temper a tures. However, in contr ast with their
*e-mail: physiological traits in response to changes in a single predictions, flies rea red a t 25°C w al ked faster a t all
rwilson @ uia.ua.ac.be environmental variable in the lab [13], whereas other temper a tures th a n did those r aised a t 18 or
Craig E. Franklin A C C L I M A T IZ A T I O N refers to physiological responses to 29°C, a nd the B A H w as again rejected.
Physiological Ecology environmental variables in the field [13]. Physiologists
Laboratory, Dept of
often assumed that all acclimation changes to the Acclimation or developmental plasticity?
Zoology, The University
of Queensland, St Lucia, phenotype enhanced the physiological performance or We suggest th a t the empirical studies discussed here
QLD 4072, Australia. fitness of an individual organism in the environment a re neither direct nor complete tests of the function al

http://tree.trends.com 0169-5347/02/$ – see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0169-5347(01)02384-9
Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002 67

benefit of therm al acclim a tion, as defined from


tr adition al physiological studies of acclim a tion. Exposure of 32oC 41.5oC
R a ther, we suggest these studies a re elega nt a n alyses bacteria for~7
generations
of the A D A P T I V E SI G N I F I C A N C E of D E V E L O P M E N T A L
P L A S T I C I T Y . Acclim a tion responses studied by
tr adition al compa r a tive physiologists differ
subst a ntially to the development al plasticity Compete both 32oC-group 41.5oC-group
exa mined by L eroi et a l. [8], Bennett a nd L ensk i [9] groups against versus versus
a nd G ibert et a l. [14]. H istorically, compa r a tive each other 41.5oC-group 32oC-group
physiologists considered acclim a tion as a reversible
response by a n orga nism to ch a nges (often season al)
32oC-group 32oC-group
in a single environment al va riable [13]. B y contr ast, Results outcompeted outcompeted
development al plasticity deals with the entire suite 41.5oC-group 41.5oC-group
of phenotypic ch a nges th a t occur as a result of at 32oC at 41.5oC
differences in the development al environment, not TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution
just the facult a tive physiological responses of a n
orga nism (acclim a tion). Fig. 1. Experimental investigation of the Beneficial Acclimation
Beca use of the highly sensitive n a ture of Hypothesis (BAH) by Leroi et al. [8]. The BAH was rejected in this case.

orga nisms during the ea rly st ages of development,


sm all va ria tions in the development al environment st ressful temper a t u res (acclim a tion response) or
ca n lead to a cascade of phenotypic ch a nges [15–17]. were associa ted wit h direct phenotypic effects
Besides acclim a tion responses, development al a rising from da m age or developmen t al const r ain ts
plasticity ca n a rise from the direct biophysical effects ca used by t he high temper a t u res.
of the environment, a nd ca n be detriment al, neutr al To test this idea, the a uthors exa mined whether
or beneficial. E nvironment al factors th a t lead to these the increased resist a nce to high temper a tures in
un avoidable, a nd often nonreversible phenotypic T. nr. br assicae could occur without a ny of the
ch a nges include temper a ture [18], oxygen tension observed decreases in fitness [18]. Pupae of
[19–21], nutrition [22,23] a nd density of conspecifics T. nr. br assicae were exposed to 33°C for 2, 3 or 4 h d−1
[24]. F or exa mple, M a tsch a k et a l. [19] found th a t for four days [18]. F or each trea tment group, there
temper a ture-induced ch a nges in muscle cellula rity w as a n increase in adult resist a nce to higher
during embryonic development of the A tla ntic salmon temper a tures. However, fitness decreased only in
S a lm a sa l a r were pa rtly due to restricted oxygen the groups exposed to 33°C for 3 or 4 h d−1 . T hus,
availability a t higher temper a tures r a ther th a n to decreases in fitness following exposure to high
facult a tive responses to temper a ture. T he egg capsule temper a tures were clea rly not ca used by the
of embryonic salmon ca n act as a n oxygen ba rrier, increased resist a nce to stressful temper a ture
pa rticula rly a t higher temper a tures when there is a n (acclim a tion) but r a ther to either gener al da m age to
increased oxygen dem a nd. I rreversible ch a nges in the phenotype or development al constr aints imposed
the size a nd number of muscle fibres occur a t high by the high temper a tures. I ncreased resist a nce to
development al temper a ture as a direct consequence temper a ture without fitness costs h as also been
of a constr aint in oxygen availability. T hese high- observed in the egg pa r asitoid T. ca rver ae in both
temper a ture-induced development al ch a nges in labor a tory a nd field experiments [25].
muscle cellula rity a re clea rly not facult a tive E xposing orga nisms to stressful conditions
acclim a tion responses. confounds a ny a n alysis of the B A H (Box 1). Besides
O bliga tory developmen t al ch a nges a re acclim a tion responses possibly aimed a t minimizing
pa r ticula rly prevalen t following exposu re to st ressful the stress of the environment, pa thological da m age
conditions, bu t t heir effects a re often subtle. to the phenotype also occurs. I n spite of these
H offm a n n a nd H ew a- K apuge [18] distinguished t he confounding effects, sever al a n alyses of the B A H
rela tive con t ribu tions of differen t types of phenotypic h ave incorpor a ted stressful conditions [8,9,26,27].
ch a nge following exposu re to high temper a t u res in Met abolic costs a nd gener al phenotypic da m age
t he pa r asitic w asp Tr ichogr a m m a n r. br assicae . could overwhelm a ny positive acclim a tion responses
I mpor t a n tly, t hey fou nd t h a t some bu t not all in a stressful environment a nd the B A H might be
phenotypic ch a nges du ring developmen t were incorrectly rejected. R a ther th a n compa risons a mong
t he result of facult a tive acclim a tion responses. orga nisms r aised under stressful conditions for
H offm a n n a nd H ew a- K apuge [18] initially observed a n alyses of the B A H , Woods a nd H a rrison [27]
t h a t adults of T. n r. br assicae exhibited a n increased advoca te exa mining the costs a nd benefits of specific
resist a nce to st ressful temper a t u res following acclim a tion responses.
exposu re to 33°C as pupae, bu t t h a t t hese ch a nges N ot a ll developmen t a lly i nevi t able ch a nges to t he
were accompa nied by deleterious fit ness effects. phenot ype a re ca used by st ressfu l condi t ions. I n t he
T hey suggested t h a t t hese fit ness decreases eit her specific case of temper a t u re, i t is doubtfu l t h a t ever y
reflected a gener al cost of increasing resist a nce to ph ysiologica l process t h a t is affected by temper a t u re

http://tree.trends.com
68 Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002

Box 1. Experimental analysis of the BAH under stressful conditions

Woods and Harrison [a] addressed whether the acclimation of • Conclusions


Manduca sexta caterpillars to water stress was beneficial. The authors concluded that, under current usage, the
beneficial acclimation hypothesis (BAH) had to be rejected in
• Treatments this case. However, we suggest that phenotypic damage owing
Larvae were raised from the first instar on low- (69%) or high-water to hydric stress confounds any analysis of the benefits of
(80%) artificial diets. After reaching the fifth instar, growth rates for acclimation.
both groups were measured on the same or opposite diet for 36 h.
Several determinants of larval water budget were also recorded. Do the lower growth rates of larvae raised on a low-water
diet (when tested on both diets) reflect pathological
• Results phenotypic changes owing to stress OR a lack of beneficial
Caterpillars raised on high-water diet grew faster than did those acclimation? We suggest that the pathological effects of chronic
raised on low-water diet when tested on both diets. Thus, no stress might overwhelm any possible beneficial acclimation
benefit for previous exposure to low-water diet was observed. responses.
However, larvae responded to short-term hydric stress (low-water
Reference
diet) by minimizing water excretion by increasing rectal water
a Woods, H . A. a nd H a rrison, J. F. (2001) T he beneficial acclim a tion hypothesis
absorption, and to long-term hydric stress by significantly versus acclim a tion of specific tr aits: physiological ch a nges in w a ter-stressed
reducing faecal water excretion. M a nduca sexta ca terpilla rs. Physiol. Zool. 74, 32–44

ca n ‘accli m a te’ to differen t temper a t u res. T h is Exploring the BAH using competing hypotheses
w ill i nevi t ably lead to phenot ypic differences I n t wo addi t ion a l st udies explor i ng t he B A H , bot h
bet ween orga n isms from differen t developmen t a l H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12]
temper a t u res t h a t a re si mply due to t he di rect advoca ted a st rong i nference approach to
effects of temper a t u re on t hese developmen t a l ex a m i n i ng quest ions rel a t i ng to t he t her m a l
pa t h w a ys [17,19–21]. T hese phenot ypic ch a nges accli m a t ion of ectot her ms. T hei r approach i n volved
a re cer t a i n ly not t hose t h a t were t r adi t ion a lly test i ng a mong compet i ng h ypot heses t h a t m a k e
descr ibed as accli m a t ion responses by compa r a t ive differen t predict ions as to how developmen t a l
ph ysiologists, bu t a re u ndoubtedly i ncl uded i n t he temper a t u re i nfl uences t he t her m a l sensi t ivi t y of
st udies of L eroi et a l. [8] a nd B en net t a nd L ensk i [9]. perfor m a nce (Box 2).
T h us, previous a n a lyses of t he B A H usi ng H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12]
developmen t a l pl ast ici t y a re confou nded by t hen used t he da t asets of sever a l previous st udies,
i ncl udi ng sever a l t ypes of phenot ypic pl ast ici t y. such as t h a t by Z w a a n et a l. [28], to compa re t he
A more compelli ng exper i men t a l a n a lysis of t he h ypot heses. Z w a a n et a l. [28] a n a lysed t he effect
benefi t of accli m a t ion wou ld be based a rou nd t he of developmen t a l temper a t u re on adu l t longevi t y
concept of accli m a t ion t h a t t r adi t ion a l compa r a t ive i n D . mel a nogaster a nd fou nd t h a t flies r a ised a t
ph ysiologists were cr i t icized for assu m i ng w as i n ter medi a te temper a t u res su r vived longer as
a l w a ys benefici a l. adu l ts t h a n did t hose flies r a ised a t cool or h igh

Box 2. Set of competing hypotheses

This set of competing hypotheses is as suggested by Huey and sufficiently advantageous to outweigh any benefits of
Berrigan [a], and Huey et al. [b]. acclimation [b].
Beneficial Acclimation Hypothesis (BAH): organisms acclimated Warmer is Better Hypothesis (WBH): organisms raised at high
to a particular environment have enhanced performance or temperatures have higher relative fitness across all temperatures
fitness in that environment relative to organisms acclimated to than do those raised at intermediate or cool temperatures. The
other environments [c]. WBH is the reciprocal of the CBH of Huey et al. [b].
Optimal developmental Temperature Hypothesis (OTH): References
organisms raised at intermediate temperatures have higher a H uey, R. B. a nd Berriga n, D . A. (1996) Testing evolution a ry hypotheses of
relative fitness across all temperatures than do organisms raised acclim a tion. I n A nim a ls a nd Temper ature: Phenotypic a nd E volution a ry
at high or low temperatures. The OTH was suggested as an A d aptation. Society for E xperimenta l B iology Semin a r Series (Johnston, I. A.
a nd Bennett, A. F., eds), pp. 205–237, C a mbridge U niversity Press
alternative to the BAH by Zamudio et al. [d], Huey and Berrigan [a] b H uey, R. B. et a l. (1999) Testing the adaptive significa nce of acclim a tion: a
and Huey et al. [b]. strong inference approach. A m. Zool. 39, 323–336
Cooler is Better Hypothesis (CBH): organisms raised at cool c L eroi, A. M. et a l. (1994) Temper a ture acclim a tion a nd competitive fitness: a n
temperatures have higher relative fitness across all experiment al test of the beneficial acclim a tion assumption. Proc. N atl. Aca d.
Sci. U . S. A. 91, 1917–1921
temperatures than do organisms raised at intermediate or
d Z a mudio, K .R. et a l. (1995) B igger isn’t alw ays better: body size, temper a ture
high temperatures. The CBH is based on the assumption a nd m ale territorial success in D rosophil a mel a nogaster . A nim. Beh av.
that the larger size of cool-developed organisms is 49, 671–677

http://tree.trends.com
Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002 69

Conclusions and future directions


Glossary
Previous empirical tests of the B A H h ave elega ntly
Acclimation: any facultative modification in a physiological trait in response to changes in demonstr a ted the evolution a ry significa nce of
an environmental variable in the lab. Changes can be in response to the developmental therm ally induced development al plasticity [8,9],
environment or long-term environmental shifts during the later stages of the life history of an
especially with the advent of a rigorous experiment al
organism (more traditionally studied). Responses can be beneficial, neutral or negative.
Acclimatization: facultative modifications in a physiological trait in response to changes in design testing sever al competing hypotheses [11,12].
one or more environmental variables in the field. I mport a ntly, these studies of the B A H h ave forcefully
Adaptive significance: (in context of plasticity) fitness advantages and disadvantages associated m ade the point th a t acclim a tion ch a nges ca nnot just
with the expression of phenotypic plasticity across a range of environments. Fitness
consequences can be positive, negative or neutral.
be assumed to be beneficial, but this is a hypothesis
Developmental plasticity: phenotypic changes induced by variation in the developmental th a t must be rigorously tested. However, we believe
environment. Changes can include facultative responses by the organism (e.g. acclimation) th a t a det ailed empirical exa min a tion of the adaptive
or unavoidable biophysical consequences of the environment (obligatory and/or
significa nce of more tr adition al measures of
pathological).
Phenotypic plasticity: environmentally induced variation in morphology, physiology and/or physiological acclim a tion is now required to test the
behaviour of an organism. B A H . We ch allenge compa r a tive physiologists to
develop new inventive experiment al designs to
explore the benefits a nd costs of the more tr adition al
temper a t u res, rega rdless of t he temper a t u re a t acclim a tion responses. However, this will not be
w h ich t he adu l t flies were k ept. T hese da t a were easy a nd using a nything less th a t a close correla te
used by H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12] of fitness to test the hypothesis, such as survival,
to aga i n reject t he gener a li t y of t he B A H i n fa vou r reproductive success or competitive ability, would be
of t he opt i m a l developmen t a l temper a t u re less th a n desir able. Previous empirical tests of the
h ypot hesis (O T H ). B A H h ave cert ainly set a benchm a r k for exa mining
However, as with the previous a n alyses of the the adaptive significa nce of phenotypic plasticity,
B A H , we suggest this experiment al design is also rega rdless of the source of phenotypic va ria tion.
confounded by sever al different types of plasticity O ne entertaining possibility for future tests of the
underlying the phenotypic ch a nges, not just B A H would be to examine the thermal acclimation
acclim a tion responses. We consider th a t, in this of reproductive performance, especially in a system
context, the O T H , cooler is better hypothesis (C B H) where females discriminate between displaying
a nd w a rmer is better hypothesis (W B H) all deal males. For example, in a species where females are
specifically with the adaptive consequences of the choosy about their mates, females could be given the
development al environment, rega rdless of the source opportunity to discriminate between cool- and warm-
of phenotypic ch a nges. However, the B A H refers only acclimated males at various temperatures. T he ability
to the facult a tive physiological responses of the to attract and procure a female might depend on
orga nisms a nd is thus only one specific type of characters such as swimming performance, aerobic
phenotypic plasticity. We suggest th a t the O T H , C B H capabilities and general activity, all of which have
Acknowledgements a nd W B H a re not mutu ally exclusive to the B A H . been shown to acclimate to temperature in a variety of
We thank Ian A. Johnston, F or exa mple, it is possible th a t the development al taxa [13]. I n this specific case, the B A H would predict
Helga Guderley, constr aints imposed on the phenotype by some that, at high temperatures, females would find the
Andy Clarke, Craig Moritz
and Raoul Van Damme for
temper a tures a re so grea t th a t the over all warm-acclimated males more attractive than they
stimulating discussions perform a nce is domin a ted not by the acclim a tion would the cool-acclimated males and vice versa at cool
and/or reading the article. responses (if they occur), but by these phenotypic temperatures. E ven more compelling would be the
This article was improved
inevit abilities. I n other words, there might be a n inclusion of males that had been raised at different
substantially by
comments from several optim al temper a ture for development th a t is temperatures, so the relative merits of developmental
anonymous referees. This determined solely by the un avoidable ch a nges to plasticity could be compared with the more reversible-
work was supported by a the phenotype th a t occur in the different therm al type acclimation responses. We suggest that future
small ARC grant awarded
environments. T his, of course, says nothing about tests of the B A H should investigate traditional types
to C.E.F. and R.S.W. and a
ARC Large Grant to C.E.F. the rela tive merit of the ‘acclim a tion’ ch a nges in of acclimation using the protocols developed for
and Craig Moritz. each environment. analysing developmental plasticity.
References 4 K ingsolver, J. G . a nd H uey, R. B. (1998) the beneficial acclim a tion assumption. Proc. N atl.
1 F eder, M. E . (1987) T he a n alysis of physiological E volution a ry a n alyses of morphological a nd Aca d. Sci. U . S. A. 91, 1917–1921
diversity: the prospects for pa ttern physiological plasticity in therm ally va riable 9 Bennett, A. F. a nd L ensk i, R. E . (1997)
document a tion a nd gener al questions in environments. A m. Zool. 38, 545–560 E volution a ry adapt a tion to temper a ture:
ecological physiology. I n N ew D irections in 5 Bennett, A. F. a nd L ensk i, R. E . (1999) V I. Phenotypic acclim a tion a nd its evolution in
E cologica l Physiology ( F eder, M. E . et a l., eds), E xperiment al evolution a nd its role in Escherichi a coli. E volution 51, 36–44
pp. 38–75, C a mbridge U niversity Press evolution a ry physiology. A m. Zool. 39, 346–362 10 Hoffm a nn, A. A. (1995) Acclim a tion: increasing
2 G arland, T., Jr and C arter, P.A. (1994) E volutionary 6 B r adley, T.J. et a l. (1999) Physiological responses survival a t a cost. Trends E col. E vol. 10, 1–2
physiology. Annu. Rev. E col. Syst. 56, 579–621 to selection for desicca tion resist a nce in 11 H uey, R.B. and Berrigan, D.A. (1996) Testing
3 Gould, S.J. a nd L ewontin, R. C. (1979) T he D rosophil a mel a nogaster . A m. Zool. 39, 337–345 evolutionary hypotheses of acclimation. In A nimals
spa ndrels of Sa n M a rco a nd the P a nglossia n 7 F eder, M. E . et a l. (2000) E volution a ry physiology. and Temperature: Phenotypic and E volutionary
P a r adigm: a critique of the adapt a tionist A nnu. Rev. E col. Syst. 31, 315–341 A daptation. Society for E xperimental B iology
progr a mme. Proc. R. Soc. London B B iol. Sci. 8 L eroi, A. M. et a l. (1994) Temper a ture acclim a tion Seminar Series (Johnston, I.A. and Bennett, A. F.,
205, 581–598 a nd competitive fitness: a n experiment al test of eds), pp. 205–237, C ambridge U niversity Press

http://tree.trends.com
70 Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002

12 H uey, R. B. et a l. (1999) Testing the adaptive 18 Hoffm a nn, A. A. a nd H ew a-K apuge, S. (2000) outcome in ad libitum -fed and chronically food-
significa nce of acclim a tion: a strong inference Acclim a tion for hea t resist a nce in Trichogr a mm a restricted rats. J. N utr. 117, 1959–1966
approach. A m. Zool. 39, 323–336 nr. br assicae: ca n it occur without costs? F unct. 24 N ewm a n, R. A. (1998) E cological constr aints on
13 Prosser, C. L . (1991) E nvironmenta l a nd Metabolic E col. 14, 55–60 a mphibia n met a morphosis: inter actions of
A nim a l Physiology: Compa r ative A nim a l 19 M atscha k, T.W. et al. (1995) Is physiological hypoxia temper a ture a nd la rval density with responses to
Physiology , 4th edn, Wiley– L iss the driving force behind temperature effects on ch a nging food level. Oecologi a 115, 9–16
14 G ibert, P. et a l. (2001) Locomotor perform a nce of muscle development in embryonic A tlantic salmon 25 T homson, L .J. et a l. (2001) F ield a nd labor a tory
D rosophil a mel a nogaster : inter actions a mong (Salmo salar)? D ifferentiation 59, 71–77 evidence for acclim a tion without costs in a n egg
development al a nd adult temper a tures, age, a nd 20 M a tsch a k, T.W. et a l. (1998) Met abolic enzyme pa r asitoid. F unct. E col. 15, 217–221
geogr aphy. E volution 55, 205–209 activities in A tla ntic salmon ( S a lmo sa l a r L .) 26 G ibbs, A. G . et a l. (1998) E ffects of cuticula r lipids
15 U sher, M. L . et a l. (1994) M uscle development in embryos respond more to chronic ch a nges in a nd w a ter bala nce in a desert D rosophil a : is
A tla ntic salmon ( S a lmo sa l a r ) embryos a nd the oxygen availability th a n to environment al therm al acclim a tion beneficial? J. E xp. B iol.
effect of temper a ture on muscle cellula rity. J. F ish temper a ture. F ish Physiol. B iochem. 18, 115–123 201, 71–80
B iol. 44, 953–964 21 M a tsch a k, T.W. et a l. (1998) Temper a ture a nd 27 Woods, H . A. a nd H a rrison, J. F. (2001) T he
16 Vieir a, V. L . A. a nd Johnston, I. A. (1992) I nfluence oxygen tension influence the development of beneficial acclim a tion hypothesis versus
of temper a ture on muscle-fibre development in muscle cellula rity in embryonic r ainbow trout. acclim a tion of specific tr aits: physiological
la rvae of the herring C lupea h a rengus. M a r. B iol. J. F ish B iol. 53, 581–590 ch a nges in w a ter-stressed M a nduca sexta
112, 333–341 22 Shine, R. a nd Downes, S.J. (1999) C a n pregn a nt ca terpilla rs. Physiol. Zool. 74, 32–44
17 Johnston, I. A. et a l. (1998) E mbryonic liza rds adjust their offspring phenotypes to 28 Zw a a n, B. et a l. (1995) A rtificial selection for
temper a ture modula tes muscle growth environment al conditions? Oecologi a 119, 1–8 development al time in D rosophil a mel a nogaster
ch a r acteristics in la rval a nd juvenile herring. 23 Rasmussen, K .M. and F ischbeck, K .L. (1987) E ffect in rela tion to the evolution of aging: direct a nd
J. E xp. B iol. 201, 623–646 of repeated reproductive cycles on pregnancy correla ted responses. E volution 49, 635–648

Putting predators in general [4–8]. Wor k on behavioral predator–prey


interactions therefore provides an important bridge
between the studies of behavior and ecology.

back into behavioral I n spite of t hese m a ny adva nces, ou r


u nderst a nding of beh avior al preda tor–prey
in ter actions is limited by a simple oversigh t: we

predator–prey vir t u ally forgot abou t t he beh avior of preda tors!


H istorically, we h ave been so focused on prey
beh avior t h a t we (myself included) beca me

interactions comfor t able wit h t rea ting preda tors as u n responsive


‘black boxes’ r a t her t h a n pa r ticipa n ts in a beh avior al
in ter action. T his oversigh t h as not only led to a n
incomplete view of beh avior al in ter actions bet ween
Steven L. Lima preda tors a nd prey, bu t h as also obscu red a n en tire
class of such in ter actions t h a t occu rs a t la rge spa tial
scales. M y goal is to explore some of t he insigh ts
In the study of behavioral predator–prey interactions, predators have been gained from pu t ting preda tors back in to beh avior al
treated as abstract sources of risk to which prey respond, rather than preda tor–prey in ter actions.
participants in a larger behavioral interaction. When predators are put back
into the picture by allowing them to respond strategically to prey behavior, How were predators removed from the interaction?
expectations about prey behavior can change. Something as simple as allowing T he removal of preda tors from the beh avior al
predators to move in response to prey movements might not only (radically) preda tor–prey inter action is appa rent in the
alter standard expectations of prey behavior, but might also reveal new classes ubiquitous ‘fixed-risk’ assumptions of const a nt a tt ack
of behavioral phenomena that occur at large spatial scales. Similar revelations r a tes over time a nd pa tch-specific risks of preda tion
undoubtedly await many well-studied aspects of the behavioral interaction (e.g. Ref. [9]); such assumptions imply th a t preda tors
between predator and prey. Most examples studied to date, both theoretical and a re not influenced by prey beh avior. As few would
empirical, require attention from this ‘predatory’ perspective. Putting predators a rgue for the strict validity of this assumption,
back into the picture will be challenging, but doing so might change the way in why were preda tors relega ted to the st a tus of
which biologists think about predator–prey interactions in general. unresponsive entities? I n m a ny w ays, the fixed-risk
approach (i.e. the assumption of unresponsive
O ver the past 20 years, the study of behavioral preda tors) w as a sensible st a rting point.
interactions between predator and prey has shed much C h a r acterizing preda tion risk as a n environment al
light on prey behavior, and it is now clear that almost const a nt seemed reason able given th a t preda tors ca n
Steven L. Lima
any aspect of prey decision-ma king (from foraging stri ke opportunistically a nd could be a nywhere a t a
Dept of Life Sciences, behavior to mate choice) can be influenced by the risk given time. M a them a tical convenience might h ave
Indiana State University, of predation [1–3]. A growing literature also suggests also played a role: models of a ntipreda tor decision-
Terre Haute, IN 47809,
that nonlethal interactions between predator and prey m a k ing a re much simpler under a n assumption of
USA.
e-mail: (those driven by prey avoidance of predation) might be fixed risk th a n they a re when both preda tor a nd prey
S-Lima @ indstate.edu an important component of predator–prey interactions a re allowed to respond to one a nother. F urthermore,

http://tree.trends.com 0169-5347/02/$ – see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0169-5347(01)02393-X

You might also like