Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

A reflection on motor learning theory in pédiatrie occupational

therapy practice
Jill G. Zwicker • Susan R. Harris

Key words
• Motor learning • Theory, Pédiatrie practice • Occupational therapy

Mots clés
• Apprentissage moteur • Théorie • Pratique pédiatrique • Ergothérapie

Abstract
Background. Theory provides a guide to clinical practice. To date, the most prevalent theories in pédiatrie occupational therapy
practice are sensory integration and neurodevelopmental treatment. Purpose. The purpose of this paper is to present a brief
overview and reflection on motor learning theories as well as a summary of motor learning principles that can be used in
pédiatrie practice. Key Issues. Over the past two decades, motor learning theory has been applied in adult occupational therapy
practice, but it has been slow to gain popularity in pediatrics. Implications. Although therapists may be tacitly applying motor
learning principles in practice, conscious and deliberate application of these principles to a variety of pédiatrie populations is
required to determine if motor learning theory provides a viable and effective contribution to evidence-based, occupational
therapy pédiatrie practice. Further research comparing motor learning interventions to other dominant interventions in pédiatrie
oeeupational therapy is warranted.

Résumé
Description. La théorie est un guide pour la pratique elinique. A eejour, les théories les plus répandues eoneernant la pratique de
l'ergothérapie en pédiatrie sont eelles de l'intégration sensorielle et de l'approehe du développement neurologique. But Cet
artiele présente un bref aperçu des théories de l'apprentissage moteur et propose une réflexion sur ees théories, tout en résumant
les prineipes pouvant être appliqués en pratique pédiatrique. Questions clés. Depuis les vingt dernières années, les prineipes de
la théorie de l'apprentissage moteur sont appliqués dans la pratique de l'ergothérapie auprès des adultes, alors que ees mêmes
prineipes tardent à se répandre en pédiatrie. Conséquences. Bien qu'en pratique les ergothérapeutes appliquent taeitement les
prineipes de l'apprentissage moteur, il serait néeessaire d'appliquer eonseiemment et délibérément ees prineipes auprès de
différentes elientèles en pédiatrie, afin de déterminer si la théorie de l'apprentissage moteur eontribue fondamentalement et
effieaeement à la pratique de l'ergothérapie en pédiatrie fondée sur les faits seientifiques. Il serait justifié de pousser plus loin les
reeherehes en comparant des méthodes d'intervention basées sur les principes d'apprentissage moteur à d'autres méthodes
fréquemment utilisées en ergothérapie dans le domaine de la pédiatrie.

T
heory is the driving force behind occupational therapy multi-system rather than hierarchical (Shepard, 1991). This
practice. Using the Canadian Practice Process shift in thinking about the CNS led to the development of
Framework (Townsend & Polatajko, 2007), therapists contemporary theories of motor learning. While motor
select frames of reference to guide their practice. In pédiatrie learning theory has been widely used in adult occupational
occupational therapy practice, the dominant theoretical therapy practice, it has been slow to gain popularity in
approaches used in the United States, Canada, Australia, and pediatrics.
the United Kingdom are sensory integration (SI) theory and The purpose of this paper is to review the key principles
neurodevelopmental treatment (NDT) (Brown, Rodger, of motor learning theories and their application to pédiatrie
Brown, & Roever, 2005; Howard, 2002; Storch & Eskow, occupational therapy practice. Chinn and Kramer's (1995)
1996). These theoretical approaches were developed in the framework will be used to reflect on the clarity, simplicity,
1960s and 1940s respectively and are based on a hierarchical generality, accessibility, and importance of motor learning
model of the central nervous system (CNS). Since the late theories as a foundation for pédiatrie practice. We will then
1980s, the CNS has been conceptualized as multilevel and provide an example of how motor learning theories can be

VOLUME 7 6 • NUMBER 1 • CANADIAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY • FEBRUARY 2 0 0 9 29


ZWICKER & HARRIS

applied to pédiatrie practice and will conclude with future and influence motor performance of new tasks. A recall
directions for research and practice. schema initiates the GMP that closely resembles the desired
movement, and the recognition schema evaluates the
Motor learning theories occurring movement. The recall schema is then modified by
Motor learning is defined as "a set of processes associated the movement experience. A major limitation of schema
with practice or experience leading to relatively permanent theory is that it does not explain how GMP are initially
changes in the capability for movement" (Schmidt & Lee, formed. Schmidt's theory has evolved over time (Schmidt,
2005, p. 302). Motor learning has been a key concept in the 2003) and has provided important motor learning concepts
fields of physical education and sport since the 1970s. Motor of knowledge of results and variability of practice, discussed
learning theory entered the field of neurological rehabili- below.
tation during the 1980s and has been applied primarily to
adults with stroke (Carr & Shepherd, 1989; Gilmore & Dynamic systems theory
Spaulding, 2001; Krakauer, 2006; Sabari, 1991). In recent Dynamic systems theory is considered a contemporary theory
years, motor learning has formed the foundation for treating of motor learning despite its appearance prior to the previous
children with developmental coordination disorder two motor learning theories (Bernstein, 1967). Bernstein's
(DCD)(Missiuna, Mandich, Polatajko, & Malloy-Miller, work resurfaced in the 1980s with the rejection of the hierar-
2001; Niemeijer, Smits-Engelman, & Schoemaker, 2007; chical view of the CNS. Dynamic systems theory places less
Sugden & Henderson, 2007). No one theory of motor emphasis on the nervous system by viewing movement as
learning has been able to explain motor skill acquisition in its emerging from the interaction of three general systems: the
entirety, but each theory has offered an important contri- person, the task, and the environment (Kamm, Thelen, &
bution to our understanding of how motor skills are learned. Jensen, 1990; Mathiowetz & Haughen, 1995; Newell, 1986).
Three motor learning theories that have dominated the Each general system has several subsystems that interact with
literature will be highlighted, and then the key principles of one another to either support or constrain movement.
motor learning that have evolved from these theories will be Subsystems that have the potential to change are referred to
summarized. as control parameters and may be the target of therapeutic
intervention to improve motor learning. Practice and
Closed-loop theory experience alter the formation of movement patterns through
Adams (1971) was the first researcher to describe a theory of interaction with the environment and the demands of the
motor learning. The primary aspect of his theory was the task. Attractor states are efficient patterns of movement that
concept of a closed-loop process of acquiring skills. Briefly, develop with practice and experience for common tasks
Adams posited that sensory feedback is required for learning (Kugler & Turvey, 1987; Mathiowetz & Haughen).
motor skills. He proposed that movement was selected and
initiated by a memory trace, which was modified by a
Motor learning principles
perceptual trace with repeated practice. This perceptual trace Several principles of motor learning have evolved from the
is the internal reference within which to compare movement above theories and have been applied in normal and clinical
and detect error. Adams' theory assumes that motor learning populations. These principles include stages of learning,
is enhanced by repeated practice of the same movement, with types of tasks, practice, and feedback.
guidance if necessary, to minimize error.
Adams' (1971) theory has been refuted with two main Stages of learning
lines of research. First, studies with animals (Fentress, 1973; Fitts and Posner (1967) described three stages of motor
Taub, 1976) and humans (Rothwell et al., 1982) have learning: cognitive, associative, and autonomous. During the
demonstrated that motor learning is possible without sensory cognitive stage, an individual may have a general idea of the
feedback. Secondly, Adams' contention that practice needs to movement required for a task but might not be sure how to
be errorless has not been borne out by research; studies have execute that movement. Performance during this stage is
indicated that variability in practice may be superior in likely to be highly variable with a large number of errors.
promoting motor learning (Shea & Kohl, 1990,1991). Improved performance is contingent upon the individual's
conscious effort to attend to the task requirements. Often this
Schema theory is achieved through verbalization of movement strategies,
To address the weakness inherent in Adams' (1971) theory, which Adams (1971) referred to as the verbal motor stage in
Schmidt (1975) proposed an open-loop process for motor his closed-loop theory of motor learning.
learning known as schema theory. Briefly, Schmidt suggested The second, intermediate stage, of motor learning is the
that generalized motor programs (GMP) are created from past associative stage. Skills become more refined with practice,
movement patterns; these GMP are recalled from memory resulting in greater consistency of performance and fewer

30 FÉVRIER 2 0 0 9 • REVUE CANADIENNE D'ERGOTHÉRAPIE • NUMÉRO 1 • VOLUME 7 6


ZWICKER & HARRIS

errors. The therapist provides less guidance during this stage random practice, which involves varying the task demands
and allows the individual to make errors so that he or she can over practice trials (Lee, Swanson, & Hall, 1991). The effects
learn to adjust subsequent movements independently (Poole, of blocked versus random practice for children is less clear;
1991). Learning from errors is thought to promote general- some studies have found no difference between these practice
ization to similar motor tasks. schedules for children (Pollock & Lee, 1997; Wegman, 1999),
Automaticity of motor learning occurs in the third stage, whereas others have found similar results as in adults, with
the autonomous stage. At this stage, the motor skill has been random practice facilitating greater motor learning (Granda
learned and little cognitive effort is required to execute it. Vera & Montilla, 2003; Ste-Marie, Clark, Findlay, & Latimer,
Automaticity is evident when a motor skill can be performed 2004). Evidence suggests that the different results may be
while engaging in another task, such as walking and talking related to the complexity of the task and the age of the child
or playing the piano and singing. Evidence from (Jarus & Goverover, 1999; Jarus & Gutman, 2001).
neuroscience indicates that less brain activation is required A final aspect of practice is whether to practice tasks as
when automaticity of movement has been achieved (Poldrack whole tasks or in parts. While learning parts of a task may be
et al, 2005; Wu, Kansaku, & Hallett, 2004), suggesting that helpful during early stages of learning, this approach does not
fewer attentional demands are required. facilitate learning the skill in the context in which it will be
used (Peck & Detweiler, 2000). Research has shown that part
Types of tasks versus whole training results in different kinematic profiles,
Motor learning is contingent upon the type of task to be with better movement quality obtained in whole-task
learned. Schmidt and Lee (2005) classified several types of practice conditions (Ma & Trombly, 2001).
tasks that can affect how the skill is learned. Discrete tasks
have a recognizable beginning and end (e.g., throwing a ball). Feedback
Continuous tasks, on the other hand, do not have an inherent Intrinsic feedback is information provided by the sensory
start and finish as part of the task (e.g., walking); continuous systems as a result of movement (Shumway-Cook &
tasks have an arbitrary beginning and end, depending upon Woollacott, 2001) and is consistent with Gentile's (1998)
the individual Serial tasks are a collection of discrete tasks notion of implicit learning. Implicit learning is not under
that are strung together (e.g., dressing). Tasks can also be conscious control, but the therapist can facilitate it by
classified as open versus closed, depending upon predictability structuring the task and environment to support effective
in the environment. Open tasks are in an environment that is movement patterns (Gentile). Extrinsic feedback supplements
constantly changing. The individual cannot plan an entire intrinsic feedback and forms the basis for explicit learning
movement in advance but must rapidly adapt the plan in (Gentile; Shumway-Cook & Woollacott. 2001), which is
response to a changing environment (e.g., playing hockey). learning that results from clearly stated directions or
Closed tasks are in a stable environment, which offers instructions (Tabers Online, 2000-2008). Verbal feedback
predictability to the movement pattern (e.g., bowling). and demonstration are examples of how a therapist can
promote explicit learning. Feedback can be given during the
Practice movement (concurrent), right after the movement
One of the most significant tenets of motor learning is (immediate), at the completion of movement (terminal), or
practice. Practice schedules, such as massed versus after a delay (Schmidt & Lee, 2005). Feedback can also be
distributed practice and blocked versus random practice, given consistently (i.e., after every trial) or sporadically (i.e.,
have been studied extensively in motor learning literature. after some but not all trials). Contrary to what one might
Massed practice involves continuously practicing a task with expect, sporadic feedback after a delay is superior for motor
little or no rest; distributed practice entails practicing a task learning to consistent feedback given immediately after the
alternating with periods of rest. The latter is generally movement (Schmidt, 1991; Winstein & Schmidt, 1990). The
superior to massed practice in contributing to motor learning delay in feedback given over some trials allows the individual
(Donovan & Radosevich, 1999). One notable exception was a to determine what factors are inñuencing performance and
small study of children with autism in which no significant prevents reliance on external feedback to learn the skill.
differences were found between massed and distributed While sporadic feedback is superior for adult motor learning,
practice schedules on motor performance and learning (Wek recent evidence suggests that children respond differently;
& Husak, 1989). children with 100% feedback during motor skill acquisition
Blocked practice involves repetitive practice on the same performed significantly better on delayed retention than
task. While this type of practice results in improved motor children on a reduced feedback schedule (Sullivan, Kantak, &
performance in a short period of time, it does not necessarily Burtner, 2008).
promote relatively permanent motor learning (Magill & Hall, Two other points related to feedback in motor learning
1990). Greater retention and transfer are accomplished with are knowledge of results (Salmoni, Schmidt, & Walter, 1984)

VOLUME 7 6 • NUMBER 1 • CANADIAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY • FEBRUARY 2 0 0 9 31


ZWICKER & HARRIS

and knowledge of performance (Gentile, 1972), Knowledge of applied in the field of adult rehabilitation for the last two
results is terminal feedback given verbally about the outcome decades (Jarus & Ratzon, 2005, Sietsema, Nelson, Mulder,
of movement in terms of the goal. In contrast, knowledge of Mervau-Scheidel, & White, 1993; Stanton et al,, 1983) but
performance is feedback on the specific components of the only in recent years with children. Most motor learning
movement pattern, not on the achievement of the goal. research with children has focused on DCD, including the
Cognitive Orientation to daily Occupational Performance
Reflection on motor learning theories (CO-OP) approach (Missiuna et al„ 2001), Neuromotor Task
Thus far, a brief overview of the major motor learning Training (Niemeijer, Schoemaker, & Smits-Engelman, 2006;
theories and the main principles of motor learning have been Niemeijer, Smits-Engelman, Reynders, & Schoemaker, 2003;
presented. We will now apply a framework proposed by Niemeijer et al,, 2007), task-specific intervention (Revie &
Chinn and Kramer (1995) for critically evaluating motor Larkin, 1993), and ecological intervention (Sugden &
learning theories in order to reflect on how they might be Henderson, 2007), Children with cerebral palsy have also
applicable in pédiatrie occupational therapy practice. Motor benefited from therapy based on motor learning (Eliasson,
learning theory will be evaluated on five criteria: clarity, 2005; Ketelaar, Vermeer, Haart, van Petegem-van Beek, &
simplicity, generality, accessibility, and importance. Helders, 2001; Thorpe & Valvano, 2002), Principles of motor
learning have applicability to a much broader range of
Clarity children with disabilities, but this is largely undiscovered.
Clarity refers to "how well a theory can be understood and
how consistently the ideas are conceptualized" (Chinn & Accessibility
Kramer, 1995, p, 127), From the perspective of a clinician, Given the voluminous literature on motor learning, empirical
"motor learning theory" is not particularly clear; this review accessibility is a strength of motor learning theories. Concepts
highlights three motor learning theories that are contra- and relationships have been tested for several decades by
dictory in many respects. Motor learning is not one theory different disciplines, resulting in refinements to motor
but rather several interpretations and concepts related to how learning theory or development of new theories. The bulk of
motor skills are acquired. In rehabilitation literature, the term research has focused on schema theory, but dynamic systems
"motor learning" appears to refer to a theoretical approach, theory is gaining popularity (see Shumway-Cook &
with little reference to a specified theory. The use of this Woollacott, 2007; Schmidt & Lee, 2005 for overviews). Yet,
catch-all term adds to the confusion about what motor despite the application of motor learning theories for decades,
learning theory is and how it can be applied in practice. At a limited number of studies has been conducted in pédiatrie
best, we seem to apply motor learning principles with little rehabilitation (Eliasson, 2005; Missiuna et al,, 2001; Niemeijer
regard for the theory from which they evolved. Without a et al,, 2003; Niemeijer et al,, 2006; Niemeijer et al,, 2007;
clear understanding of the theoretical basis of motor Thorpe & Valvano, 2002), Deliberate application of the theory
learning, we cannot adequately apply the theory, test it is another form of accessibility, which also has been lacking in
empirically, or determine its usefulness in clinical practice. pédiatrie occupational therapy practice. We may be tacitly
using motor learning principles in our practice, but we are not
Simplicity necessarily documenting our theoretical framework or
Each of the motor learning theories presented are naturally reflecting motor learning in our clinical reasoning.
complex because they aim to explain and predict how
complex motor skills are learned. Application of motor Importance
learning principles is seemingly straightforward, but there Motor learning theories are highly compatible with models of
are many factors to consider in designing an intervention occupational therapy practice (Townsend & Polatajko, 2007;
program: practice schedule, amount of practice, type of task, Strong et al„ 1999), In pédiatrie practice in particular, a
stage of the learner, amount and type of feedback, environ- childs acquisition of motor skills is important to his or her
mental influences, and the like. The multiple factors that functioning in self-care activities, participating in school, and
need to be taken into account may hinder therapists in engaging in play. Motor learning has great clinical signif-
consistently applying motor learning theory to practice. icance to pédiatrie occupational therapists, yet it is
underutilized. In a survey of Canadian and Australian
Generality pédiatrie occupational therapists, only 30,5% and 33,0%
Motor learning concepts have broad applicability across respectively used motor learning theory in their treatment of
the lifespan in both typical (Brydges, Carnahan, Backstein, & children with neurological conditions (Brown et al„ 2005), In
Durowski, 2007; Ma, Trombly, & Robinson-Podolski, 1999) Australia, 20,4% of surveyed occupational therapists used
and chnical populations (Jarus, 1994; Poole, 1991; Sabari, motor learning theory for children with learning disabilities,
1991; Valvano, 2004), Motor learning theories have been but Canadian occupational therapists did not even identify

32 FÉVRIER 2 0 0 9 • REVUE CANADIENNE D'ERGOTHÉRAPIE • NUMÉRO 1 • VOLUME 76


ZWICKER & HARRIS

using motor learning theory for this population. therapist. Alan had left-sided spastic hemiplegia as a result of
The importance of motor learning theory in pédiatrie his ABI but was fortunately right-handed; he required the use
practice cannot be underestimated. Treatments based on of a powered wheelchair for community-based activities.
motor learning theory have show^n more promising results Both Alan and his parents were concerned that he seldom
compared to SI for children with DCD (Polatajko & Cantin, played with classmates or friends due, in part, to his limited
2005) and NDT for children with cerebral palsy (Butler & motor skills. Together with the school occupational therapist,
Darrah, 2001). they identified the functional goal of increasing Alan's
In summary, reflection on motor learning theory using playtime with age-mates in his neighborhood. Selection of a
Chinn and Kramers (1995) framework has demonstrated meaningful goal represents the first component of goal-
that motor learning theory has generality and importance but directed training (Mastos et al., 2007).
may be lacking in clarity and simplicity for application to Alan's specific goal was to learn how to bowl so that he
pédiatrie occupational therapy. Accessibility may be improved could go bowling with a group of neighborhood friends. The
by deliberately applying motor learning theory to practice second component of goal-directed training is to assess
and reflecting use of motor learning principles in our clinical baseline performance (Mastos et al, 2007). To assess baseline
reasoning. To that end, we will now share an example of how performance, the school occupational therapist analyzed
motor learning could be applied to a child with a disorder Alan's functional abilities with his right upper extremity
other than DCD or cerebral palsy. (person), while sitting in his wheelchair in the bowling alley
(environment), and performing the desired occupation
Application of motor (bowling). Because Alan had no difficulty grasping the
learning theory to pédiatrie bowling ball by inserting his fingers into the three holes but
occupational therapy practice did have trouble releasing it, as part of the baseline
Because dynamic systems theory is the most recent iteration assessment the therapist performed a task analysis (Mastos et
of motor learning, we will develop an example of its al.) of the motor skills required to release the ball. The
application to pédiatrie occupational therapy practice. We therapist also determined that there were no specific environ-
will also illustrate the three-stage model of motor learning as mental constraints caused by Alan's wheelchair or with
described by Fitts and Posner (1967). A recent set of case accessibility to the bowling alleys and lanes so she decided to
reports involving two adults with acquired brain injury (ABI) develop a motor-learning-based intervention program to
(Mastos, Miller, Eliasson, & Imms, 2007), in which dynamic assist Alan with developing the ability to release the bowling
systems theory was used as the basis for goal-directed ball in order to propel it down the lane. To accomplish this,
training, will be highlighted to develop an analogous example Alan's therapist developed the following initial therapy
for a child with ABI. In attempting to clarify and concep- objective for Alan based on his therapy goal: "While sitting in
tualize (i.e., bring clarity to) dynamic systems theory, Mastos his wheelchair in a specified 'practice lane' at the local
and colleagues stated that the underlying principles of the bowling alley, Alan will release the bowling ball onto the lane
goal-directed training approach stem from dynamic systems independently four out of five times within an eight-minute
theory, "which suggest[s] that movement patterns emerge as period with physical assistance and verbal cueing from his
a result of the interaction between the persons abilities, the occupational therapist."
environment and the goal" (p. 47). They defined goal- The intervention (third component of goal-directed
directed training simply as "an activity-based approach to training) was based on Fitts and Posner's (1967) three stages
intervention" (p. 47). In an attempt to simplify the dynamic of motor learning. In the first, or cognitive stage, the therapist
systems theory, the authors (occupational therapists and first asked Alan to try to problem solve, or think through the
physical therapists) used principles of motor learning to skills needed to release the bowling ball. She then provided
guide their intervention approach, that is, they used goal- both physical cueing and verbal instructions to facilitate
directed training. Because of similarities in the sequelae from Alan's release of the ball (Mastos et al., 2007). In the second,
ABI in adults and children, we will generalize Mastos et al.'s or associative stage, Alan practiced releasing the ball on the
goal-directed training approach to an example of a child with bowling alley without the added physical assistance from the
ABI, thus translating dynamic systems theory into pédiatrie therapist but with continued verbal cueing. He was allowed to
practice and making this theory more accessible and more make errors and to learn from those errors as he repeatedly
important to pédiatrie clinicians. (practice) attempted to release the ball onto the alley. During
In our case example, Alan is a 10-year-old boy who had the third stage of learning, the autonomous stage, Alan was
an acquired (traumatic) brain injury from a motor vehicle able to consistently release the ball onto the alley without the
accident 4 years previously. He had a medical diagnosis of need for verbal cueing from the therapist.
moderate ABI, was living at home, and received school-based The fourth and final component of goal-directed
consultation from an occupational therapist and a physical training is to evaluate the outcome of the therapy goal. The

VOLUME 7 6 • NUMBER 1 • CANADIAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY • FEBRUARY 2 0 0 9 33


ZWICKER & HARRIS

outcome of the initial specific therapy objective can be phase and report on the child's knowledge of results. During
evaluated independently by the therapist (Randall & the associative stage, we could highlight variability ofpractice
McEwen, 2000) or could be broadened into five discrete steps with a random practice schedule to facilitate motor learning.
representing different levels of success using goal attainment We could also indicate that greater emphasis is placed on
scaling (Ottenbacher & Cusick, 1993), as described by Mastos implicit feedback at this stage so the child can attend to errors
and colleagues (2007). and make adjustments for subsequent movement (and rely
less on explicit feedback). We could collect outcome data
Discussion through our clinical records and publish our findings as case
Implications for practice reports. These are critical first steps in determining the
The purpose of this paper was to provide a brief overview of viability and effectiveness of motor learning principles in the
motor learning theories and highhght motor learning various clinical populations of children with whom we work.
principles that might be applied to pédiatrie practice. Based
on this review, it was suggested that motor learning theory is Directions for future research
neither clear nor simple, but it has great potential for The amount of practice required to learn motor skills is
pédiatrie occupational therapy practice. Motor learning is largely unknown. Preliminary evidence from the CO-OP
widely applicable to the populations served by pédiatrie approach suggests that 10 sessions may be sufficient to learn
therapists, but empirical studies have not yet determined for motor skills in the context of a task-specific intervention
whom it is beneficial. Preliminary evidence suggests that based on the child's goals. Greater practice time is likely
children with cerebral palsy and those with DCD have made required for children with neuropathology, such as cerebral
functional gains with interventions based on motor learning. palsy or developmental delay. More research is needed to
Given the propensity for neuroplastic change in the nervous determine effective practice schedules for different types of
system, children with other neurological disorders, develop- tasks in a variety of pédiatrie populations.
mental delay, autism, and learning disabilities may also Using the four-step, goal-directed training process
benefit from this approach to improve motor skills and developed by Mastos and colleagues (2007), pédiatrie
functional performance. oeeupational therapists eould replicate the adult case study
Many authors have previously advocated for the use of examples by applying motor learning principles to children
motor learning theory in occupational therapy practice with ABI in their own practices. Similarly, there are published
(Baker, 1999; Goodgold-Edwards, 1984; Jarus, 1994; examples from the pédiatrie physieal therapy literature in
Lesensky & Kaplan, 2000; Poole, 1991), yet it is still not which motor learning principles have been applied in
widespread in pediatrics. This may be due, in part, to the lack interventions for children with cerebral palsy (Ketelaar et al.,
of a practice model that translates these theoretical principles 2001; Thorpe & Valvano, 2002) that could serve as useful
into a usable frame of reference for practice. The CO-OP models for occupational therapy intervention research.
approach is close to achieving this goal as it has taken many Finally, chnical trials comparing motor learning
of the principles and incorporated them into a treatment intervention to interventions based on current, dominant
approach for children with DCD (Polatajko et al., 2001). pédiatrie oeeupational therapy theory (e.g., sensory
Sugden and Henderson (2007) have also outlined guidelines integration) would determine if a shift in pédiatrie praetiee is
for using motor learning principles in interventions for warranted.
children with motor impairment.
Conclusion
Although a formal model for motor learning practice has
yet to be developed for children with developmental Motor learning theories have a rieh research history and
disabilities, we are probably applying many motor learning broad applicability to normal and clinical populations. To
principles tacitly in our practice. As clinicians, we need to be date, they have been underutilized in pédiatrie oeeupational
more conscious of and deliberate in our application of these therapy praetiee, probably beeause of the eurrent dominance
principles to determine if they are effective and to further of SI and NDT theories in practice and the lack of a cohesive
extend our understanding of motor learning theory. As a practice model based on motor learning principles. With a
starting point, we can reflect the use of the theory in our concerted effort, principles from motor learning theory can
clinical reasoning and documentation. For example, our be deliberately applied in practice to determine if motor
documentation could describe our intervention in terms of learning theory offers a contribution to evidence-based
blocked practice during the cognitive stage of learning so the pédiatrie oeeupational therapy practice.
child can understand the task. We might start practicing
parts of the tasks at this early stage, but then move to Acknowledgements
practicing the whole skill in context. We might also describe Jill Zwicker has been awarded a Ouality of Life Strategic
the type of explicit feedback provided during the cognitive Training Fellowship in Rehabilitation Research from the

34 FÉVRIER 2 0 0 9 • REVUE CANADIENNE D'ERGOTHÉRAPIE • NUMÉRO 1 • VOLUME 7 6


ZWICKER & HARRIS

Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. L (1967). Learning and skilled performance


Canadian Institutes of Health Research Musculoskeletal and
in human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Arthritis Institute and a Senior Graduate Training
Gentile, A. M. (1972). A working model of skill acquisition with
Scholarship from the Michael Smith Foundation for Health
application to teaching. Quest, 17, 2-23.
Research. The authors wish to thank Dr. Lyn Jongbloed for Gentile, A. M. (1998). Implicit and explicit processes during
reviews of an earlier draft of this paper.
acquisition of functional skills. Scandinavian Journal of
Qccupational Therapy, 5, 7-16.
Gilmore, P. E., & Spaulding, S. 1. (2001). Motor control and motor
Key messages
learning: implications for treatment of individuals post stroke.
Currently, the most prevalent theories in pédiatrie Physical and Qccupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 20, 1-15.
occupational therapy practice are sensory integration Goodgold-Edwards, S. A. (1984). Motor learning as it relates to
and neurodevelopmental treatment. development of skilled motor behavior: A review of the
Research from adult populations and from children literature. Physical and Qccupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 4(4),
wilh developmental coordination disorder suggest 5-18.
thai motor learning theory may have greater applica- Granda Vera, J., & Montilla, M. M. (2003). Practice schedule and
bility to pédiatrie occupational therapy practice than acquisition, retention, and transfer of a throwing task in 6-yr.-
the current state-of-affairs. old children. Perceptual Motor Skills, 96, 1015-1024.
Pédiatrie occupational therapists may be tacitly Howard, L. (2002). A survey of paediatric occupational therapists in
applying motor learning principles in their practice, the United Kingdom. Qccupational Therapy International, 9,
but the use of motor learning theory needs to be more 326-343.
explicit in their clinical reasoning and documentation. larus, T. (1994). Motor learning and occupational therapy: The
organization of practice. American Journal of Qccupational
Therapy 48, 810-816.
larus. T., & Goverover, Y. (1999). Effects of contextual interference
References and age on acquisition, retention, and transfer of motor skill.
Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal Perceptual Motor Skills, 88, 437-447.
of Motor Behavior, 3, 111-149. larus. T., & Gutman, T. (2001). Effects of cognitive processes and
Baker, B. (1999). Principles of motor learning for school-based task complexity on acquisition, retention, and transfer of motor
occupational therapy practitioners. School System Special skills. Canadian Journal of Qccupational Therapy, 68, 280-289.
Interest Quarterly, 6(2), 1-4. larus. T., & Ratzon, N. Z. (2005). The implementation of motor
Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movement learning principles in designing prevention programs at work.
New York: Pergamon Press. Work: A Journal of Prevention, Assessment, and Rehabilitation,
Brown, G. T., Rodger, S., Brown, A., & Roever, C. (2005). A 24, 171-182.
comparison of Canadian and Australian paediatric occupa- Kamm, K., Thelen, E., & lensen, ]. L. (1990). A dynamical systems
tional therapists. Occupational Therapy International, 12, 137- approach to motor development. Physical Therapy, 70, 763-775.
161. Ketelaar, M., Vermeer, A., Hart, H., van Petegem-van Beek, E., &
Brydges, R., Carnahan, H., Backstein, D., & Dubrowski, A. (2007). Helders, P.}. (2001). Effects of a functional therapy program on
Application of motor learning principles to complex surgical motor abilities of children with cerebral palsy. Physical Therapy,
tasks: Searching for the optimal practice schedule. Journal of 81, 1534-1545.
Motor Behavior, 39, 40-48. Krakauer, J. W. (2006). Motor learning: Its relevance to stroke
Butler, C, & Darrah, 1. (2001). Effects of neurodevelopmental recovery and neurorehabilitation. Current Qpinion in
treatment (NDT) for cerebral palsy: An AACPDM evidence Neurology 19, 84-90.
report. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 43, 778-790. Kugler, R N., & Turvey, M. T. (1987). Information, natural law, and
Carr, J. H., & Shepherd, R. B. (1989). A motor learning model for the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Hillsdale, Nl: Erlbaum.
stroke rehabilitation. Physiotherapy, 75, 372-380. Lee, T. D., Swanson, L. R., & Hall, A .L. (1991). What is repeated in
Chinn, R L., & Kramer, M. K. (1995). Theory and nursing: A a repetition? Effects of practice conditions on motor skill
systematic approach. St. Louis, MO: Mosby. acquisition. Physical Therapy, 71, 150-156.
Donovan, J. 1., & Radosevich, D. 1. (1999). A meta-analytic review of Lesensky, S., & Kaplan, L. (2000). Motor learning: Putting theory
the distribution of practice effect: Now you see it, now you into practice. QT Practice, 5(September), 13-16.
don't. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 795-805. Ma, H. 1., & Trombly, C. A. (2001). The comparison of motor
Eiiasson, A. (2005). Improving the use of hands in daily activities: performance between part and whole tasks in elderly persons.
Aspects of the treatment of children with cerebral palsy. American Journal of Qccupational Therapy, 55, 62-67.
Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 25(3), 37-60. Ma, H. 1., Trombly, C. A., & Robinson-Podolski, C. (1999). The
Eentress, ]. C. (1973). Development of grooming in mice with effect of context on skill acquisition and transfer. American
amputated forelimbs. Science, 179, 704. Journal of Qccupational Therapy, 53, 138-144.

VOLUME 7 6 • NUMBER 1 • CANADIAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY • FEBRUARY 2 0 0 9 35


ZWICKER & HARRIS

Magill, R. A., & Hall, K. G. (1990). A review of the contextual Revie, G., & Larkin, D. (1993). Task-specific intervention with
interference effect in motor skill acquisition. Human Movement children reduces movement problems. Adapted Physical
Science, 9, 241-289. Education Quarterly, 10, 29-41.
Mastos, M., Miller, K., Eliasson, A. C , & Imms, C. (2007). Goal- Rothwell, J. G., Traub, M. M., Day, B. L., Obeso, J. A., Thomas, R K.,
directed training: Linking theories of treatment to clinical & Marsden, G. D. (1982). Manual motor performance in a
practice for improved functional activities in daily life. Clinical deafferented man. Brain, 105, 515-542.
Rehabilitation, 21, 47-55. Sabari, J. S. (1991). Motor learning concepts applied to activity-
Mathiowetz, V., & Haughen, J. B. (1995). Motor behavior research: based intervention with adults with hemiplegia. American
Implications for therapeutic approaches to central nervous Journal of Occupational Therapy, 45, 523-530.
dysfunction. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 48, Salmoni, A. W., Schmidt, R. A., & Walter, G. B. (1984). Knowledge
733-745. of results and motor learning: A review and critical reappraisal.
Missiuna, C , Mandich, A. D., Polatajko, H. J., & Malloy-Miller, T. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 355-386.
(2001). Cognitive orientation to daily occupational Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill
performance (CO-OP): Part I - Theoretical foundations. learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225-260.
Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 20 (2/3), 69-81. Schmidt, R. A. (1991). Motor learning principles for physical
Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordi- therapy. In M. Lister (Ed.), Contemporary management of motor
nation. In M. G. Wade & H. T. A. Whiting (Eds.), Motor skill control problems: Proceedings of the II STEP conference (pp.49-
acquisition in children: Aspects of coordination and control. 64). Alexandria, VA: Foundation for Physical Therapy.
Amsterdam: Martinies NIJHOS. Schmidt, R. A. (2003). Motor schema theory after 27 years:
Niemeijer, A. S., Schoemaker, M. M., & Smits-Engelman, B. C. Reflections and implications for a new theory. Research
(2006). The teaching principles associated with improved Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74, 366-375.
motor performance in children with developmental coordi- Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T D. (2005). Motor control and learning: A
nation disorder? A pilot study. Physical Therapy, 86,1221-1230. behavioral emphasis (4th ed.). Ghampaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Niemeijer, A. S., Smits-Engelman, B. C , Reynders, K., & Shea, G. H., & Kohl, R. M. (1990). Specificity and variability of
Schoemaker, M. M. (2003). Verbal actions of physiotherapists practice. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 61,169-177.
to enhance motor learning in children with DCD. Human Shea, G. H., & Kohl, R. M. (1991). Gomposition of practice:
Movement Science, 22, 567-581. Influence on the retention of motor skills. Research Quarterly
Niemeijer, A. S., Smits-Engelman, B. C , & Schoemaker, M. M. for Exercise and Sport, 62, 187-195.
(2007). Neuromotor task training for children with develop- Shepard, K. (1991). Theory: Griteria, importance, and impact. In M.
mental coordination disorder: A controlled trial. Developmental Lister (Ed.), Contemporary management of motor control
Medicine and Child Neurology, 49, 406-411. problems: Proceedings of the II STEP conference (pp.5-10).
Ottenbacher, K. J., & Cusick, A. (1993). Discriminative versus Alexandria, VA: Foundation for Physical Therapy.
evaluative assessment: Some observations on goal attainment Shumway-Gook, A., & Woollacott, M. (2007). Motor control:
scaling. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 47, 349-354. Translating research into clinical practice (3rd ed.). Baltimore:
Peck, A. C , & Detweiler, M. G. (2000). Training concurrent Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins.
multistep procedural tasks. Human Factors, 42, 379-389. Sietsema, J. M., Nelson, D. L., Mulder, R. M., Mervau-Scheidel, D.,
Polatajko, H. J., & Cantin, N. (2005). Developmental coordination & White, B. E. (1993). The use of a game to promote arm reach
disorder (dyspraxia): An overview of the state of the art. in persons with traumatic brain injury. American Journal of
Seminars in Pédiatrie Neurology, 12, 250-258. Occupational Therapy, 47, 19-24.
Polatajko, H. J., Mandich, A. D., Missiuna, C , Miller, L. J., Macnab, Stanton, K. M., Pepping, M., Brockway, J. A., Bliss, L., Frankel, D., &
J. J., Malloy-Miller, T., et al. (2001). Cognitive orientation to Waggener, S. (1983). Wheelchair transfer training for right
daily occupational performance (CO-OP): Part III - the cerebral dysfunctions: An interdisciplinary approach. Archives
protocol in brief. Physical and Occupational Therapy in of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 64, 276-280.
Pediatrics, 20 (2/3), 107-123. Ste-Marie, D. M., Glark, S. E., Findlay, L. G., & Latimer, A. E. (2004).
Poldrack, R. A., Sabb, F. W., Foerde, K., Tom, S. M., Asarnow, R. R, High levels of contextual interference enhance handwriting skill
Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2005). The neural correlates of motor acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 115-126.
skill automaticity. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 5356-5364. Storch, B. A., & Eskow, K. G. (1996). Theory application by school-
Pollock, B. J., & Lee, T. D. (1997). Dissociated contextual based occupational therapists. American Journal of
interference effects in children and adults. Perceptual Motor Occupational Therapy 50, 662-668.
Skills, 84, 851-858. Strong, S., Rigby, P, Stewart, D., Law, M., Letts, L., & Gooper, B.
Poole, J. L. (1991). Applications of motor learning principles in (1999). Application of the Person-Environment-Occupation
occupational therapy. American Journal of Occupational Model: A practical tool. Canadian Journal of Occupational
Therapy, 45, 531-537. Therapy, 66, 122-133.
Randall, K. E., & McEwen, I. R. (2000). Writing patient-centered Sugden, D. A., & Henderson, S. E. (2007). Ecological intervention for
functional goals. Physical Therapy, 80, 1197-1203. children with movement difficulties. London: Harcourt

36 FÉVRIER 2 0 0 9 • REVUE CANADIENNE D'ERGOTHÉRAPIE • NUMÉRO 1 • VOLUME 76


ZWICKER & HARRIS

Assessment, acquisition and retention of fundamental motor skills.


Sullivan, K, J,, Kantak, S, S,, & Burtner, P, A, (2008), Motor learning Perceptual Motor Skills, 88, 182-187,
in children: Feedback effects on skill acquisition. Physical Wek, S, R,, & Husak, W, S, (1989), Distributed and massed practice
Therapy, 88, 720-732, effects on motor performance and learning of autistic children.
Taber's Online (2000-2008), Taber's Cyclopedic Medical Dictionary Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 107-113,
(20th ed,). Retrieved September 17, 2008: from, http:// www, Winstein, C, J,, & Schmidt, R, A, (1990), Reduced frequency of
tabers,com/tabersonline/ub/view/Tabers/74533/13/explicit_ knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. Journal of
learning Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16,
Taub, E, (1976), Movement in nonhuman primates deprived of 677-691,
somatosensory feedback. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 4, Wu, T, Kansaku, K,, & Hallett, M, (2004), How self-initiated
335-374, memorized movements become automatic: A functional fMRI
Thorpe, D, E,, & Valvano, J, (2002), The effects of knowledge of study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 1690-1698,
performance and cognitive strategies on motor skill learning in
children with cerebral palsy. Pédiatrie Physical Therapy, 14, 2- Authors
15, Jill G. Zwicker, MA, OT (C) is PhD candidate. Rehabilitation
Townsend, E, A,, & Polatajko, H, J, (2007), Enabling occupation II: Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia,
Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, T325-2211 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 2B5,
& justice through occupation. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Telephone: (604) 827-3369, E-maü: jzwicker@interchange,ubc,ca
Association of Occupational Therapists, Susan R. Harris, PhD, PT, FCAHS, is Professor Emérita,
Valvano, J, (2004), Activity-focused motor interventions for Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Medicine,
children with neurological conditions. Physical and University of British Columbia, T212-2211 Wesbrook Mall,
Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 24 (1-2), 79-107, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 1Z3, Telephone: (604) 822-7944,
Wegman, E, (1999), Contextual interference effects on the E-mail: shar(g'interchange,ubc,ca

Book Marks • Livres à la page

Occupational and Physical Therapy for copied (or downloaded from the publisher's website), inform
Children with Rheumatic Diseases: children, parents, and teachers how to manage symptoms like pain
A Clinical Handbook (2008) and fatigue, use backpacks, choose shoes, plan travel, apply
ergonomics at school, adapt sports and physical education, make
Gay Kuchta & Iris Davidson, (Eds.) decisions about alternative therapy and much more, A list of web
Radcliffe Publishing sites and suppliers of therapy equipment rounds out the resource
30 Amberwood Parkway section.
Ashland, Ohio
There is limited prose to read. Instead, information is
US, 44805
organized into charts and tables to trigger problem-solving.
344 pages, $89,00 US
Excellent, full-colour photographs and line drawings illustrate
ISBN: 9781846192333
therapeutic procedures. Tables are full of practical tips for
Occupational therapist Cay Kuchta and physiotherapist Iris assessment and intervention used across the continuum of acute,
Davidson have compiled decades of practice expertise into this out-patient, and community-based care, with a strong focus on
resource manual to guide practice with children who have arthritis self-management. Tips remind practitioners to be sensitive to
and related conditions. differing cultural expectations and child preferences, and ways to
The book is organized into 6 sections. The first three sections, build trust, especially applicable to working with teens who may
intended to be a quick reference for therapists, list considerations engage in behaviours they don't wish to disclose to parents but
for assessment and treatment of inflammatory joint disease, have an impact on their health care, such as using alcohol or sexual
connective tissue diseases, and idiopathic pain. Section 4 outlines relationships.
rehabilitation interventions in greater detail, while section 5 is As occupational therapy and physiotherapy educators in
comprised of contributions from the pédiatrie rheumatology team: rheumatology, we highly recommend this handbook for students
rheumatologists, nurses, and social worker address medical and all rehabilitation practitioners who work with children who
management, pain, physical activity, transition to adulthood, and have rheumatic diseases,
collaboration among team members to support children and
Catherine Backman and Linda Li
families. The flnal section, a collection of handouts that can be

VOLUME 7 6 • NUMBER 1 • CANADIAN JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY • FEBRUARY 2 0 0 9 37

You might also like