Peer Group Homogeneity in Adolescents' School Adjustment Varies According To Peer Group Type and Gender

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 65

International Journal of Behavioral Development © 2009 The International Society for the
2009, 33 (1), 65–76 Study of Behavioural Development
http://www.sagepublications.com DOI: 10.1177/0165025408098014

Peer group homogeneity in adolescents’ school adjustment varies


according to peer group type and gender

Noona Kiuru, Jari-Erik Nurmi, Kaisa Aunola, and Katariina Salmela-Aro


University of Jyväskylä, Finland

This study investigated whether the members of adolescents’ peer groups are similar in terms of their
school adjustment and whether this homogeneity varies according to peer group type and gender. A
total of 1262 peer group members who had recently moved to post-comprehensive education filled
in questionnaires measuring their academic achievement, satisfaction with their educational track,
school engagement, and school burnout. They also gave positive peer nominations on the basis of
which 360 peer groups were identified and categorized as cliques, loose groups, and isolate dyads.
The results showed that the members of adolescents’ peer groups particularly resembled each other
in terms of academic achievement. Moreover, the members of girls’ cliques showed greater similarity
to each other in terms of their satisfaction with educational track and school engagement than did
the members of girls’ loose groups. Girls’ isolate dyads were, in particular, at risk for low adjustment
at school.

Keywords: academic achievement; gender; multilevel modeling; peer groups; peer group type; school
engagement

Peer groups are among the most significant social contexts in what is taught at school, and also receive high grades and test
adolescence (Magnusson & Stattin, 1998; Rubin, Bukowski & scores (Berndt & Keefe, 1996). School adjustment can be
Parker, 1998). Research in academic settings has shown, for assumed to reflect adolescents’ overall resources for school
example, that the members of adolescents’ peer groups are work. In the present study three aspects of school adjustment
similar with respect to many school adjustment variables, such were examined: academic achievement; satisfaction with
as academic achievement (Chen, Chang, & He, 2003; Ryan, educational track; and school engagement.
2001), school engagement (Kindermann, 2007; Kindermann, Although a substantial amount of research has been carried
McCollam, & Gibson, 1996), and intrinsic learning motiva- out on school adjustment and well-being at school (Deci,
tion (Ryan, 2001). Moreover, adolescents belonging to the Vallerand, Pelletier, & Ryan, 1991; Konu & Lintonen, 2006;
same peer group have been found to resemble each other with Roeser, 1998; Wigfield & Eccles, 1994), less research has been
regards to school-related burnout (Kiuru, Aunola, Nurmi, carried out on school maladjustment. In the present study,
Leskinen, & Salmela-Aro, 2008). However, although many school maladjustment was conceptualized by using the concept
studies have examined peer group homogeneity in school of school burnout (Salmela-Aro & Näätänen, 2005). Although
adjustment, no previous studies have examined whether type burnout was originally regarded as a work-related disorder
of peer group, that is being a member of clique, loose group, (Demerouti, Bakker, Nachreiner, & Schaufeli, 2001; Leiter &
or isolate dyad, or gender contributes to group homogeneity. Schaufeli, 1996; Maslach, 1982; Maslach, Schaufeli, & Leiter,
The aim of the present study was to investigate both of these 2001), it has also been found to be a useful concept in school
factors. (Kiuru et al., 2008; Salmela-Aro, Kiuru, Leskinen, & Nurmi, in
press-a; Salmela-Aro, Kiuru, Leskinen, & Nurmi, in press-b)
and university (Schaufeli, Martínez, Pinto, Salanova, & Bakker,
Adolescents’ adjustment at school 2002) contexts as well. Even though students are not paid
School adjustment is a broad construct which consists of many employees, their core activities at school can be considered as
different aspects, such as academic achievement (Berndt & “work” from a psychological perspective: they attend classes and
Keefe, 1995; Wentzel, Barry, & Caldwell, 2004), overall school do assignments in order to pass exams and acquire a degree.
satisfaction (e.g., Epstein & McPartland, 1976), school Burnout in the school context can be defined as exhaustion due
engagement (e.g., Berndt & Miller, 1990; Esptein & McPart- to school demands, cynical and detached attitude towards one’s
land, 1976; Wentzel, 1993), and prosocial behavior at school school, and sense of insufficiency as a student (Salmela-Aro &
(Wentzel et al., 2004). Well-adjusted students usually value Näätänen, 2005). It can be assumed to originate from a lack of
what they are learning, are positively involved in classroom fit between the student’s internal resources for school work and
activities, are rarely disruptive (Berndt & Miller, 1990; Dubow, his or her own demands, or those of other people, such as
Tisak, Causey, Hryshko, & Reid, 1991; Wentzel, 1993), learn teachers, peers, and parents, for academic success.

Correspondence should be sent to Noona Kiuru, Department of (FinEdu) project, and was funded by grants from the Finnish
Psychology, University of Jyväskylä/Agora, P.O. Box 35, 40014 Graduate School of Psychology and the Finnish Academy (720 7421,
Jyväskylä, Finland; e-mail: noona.kiuru@psyka.jyu.fi 121 0319, #213486). We would like to thank Esko Leskinen and Asko
This study is a part of the ongoing Finnish Educational Transitions Tolvanen for statistical consultation.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 66

66 KIURU ET AL. / PEER GROUP HOMOGENEITY IN ADOLESCENTS’ SCHOOL ADJUSTMENT

Previous research has also shown many gender differences cliques, loose groups, and isolate dyads (Kiuru, Aunola, Vuori,
in school adjustment. For example, girls typically attribute & Nurmi, 2007; Ryan, 2001). Because there is little research
more importance to academic achievement (e.g., Berndt & on whether peer group characteristics vary according to peer
Miller, 1990), have higher levels of intrinsic academic motiva- group type, one major aim of the present study was to compare
tion (Ryan, 2001), and have higher academic performance cliques, loose groups, and isolate dyads in terms of peer group
than boys (e.g., Frome & Eccles, 1998; Fuligni, Eccles, Barber, homogeneity and mean level adjustment.
& Clements, 2001). However, girls also worry more about their Gender differences in the membership of different types of
academic success (e.g., Murberg & Bru, 2004) and experience peer groups have been little studied. There is some evidence to
more school burnout compared to boys (Kiuru et al., 2008; suggest, however, that girls more often than boys belong to
Salmela-Aro et al., in press-b). peer cliques whose members have close and intensive ties,
School adjustment is an important indicator of how well whereas boys more often than girls belong to less cohesive
adolescents have been able to cope with the challenges and loose groups (Kiuru et al., 2007). Girls are also more
expectations presented by school. Classes, work assignments, connected to their peer network than boys (Benenson, 1990;
homework and exams, on the one hand, and making new Urberg, Değirmencioğlu, Tolson, & Halliday-Scher, 1995)
friends and being a member of a peer group, on the other, are and more often retain the same peer network role over time
typically ranked as the most important challenges at school than boys (Değirmencioğlu et al., 1998). Girls are also more
(Newman, Lohman, Newman, Myers, & Smith, 2000). Such interpersonally oriented (Leadbeater, Kuperminc, Blatt, &
challenges can be particularly demanding after the educational Hertzog, 1999; Maccoby, 1995), attribute more importance to
transition to a new school environment, such as senior high peer groups (Crockett, Losoff, & Petersen, 1984; Kerr, Stattin,
school. The participants of the present study had moved half & Kiesner, 2007; Maccoby, 1995), and report higher friend-
a year previously to post-comprehensive schooling, that is, to ship quality and peer support than boys (Berndt & Keefe,
senior high school or vocational school (for details in the 1995; Brendgen, Markiewicz, Doyle, & Bukowski, 2001;
Finnish context see last part of this section). Academic Bukowski, Hoza, & Boivin, 1994; Furman & Buhrmester,
subjects and pressure for high grades are given greater 1992). In the present study gender and peer group type inter-
emphasis on the academic track, whereas the vocational track actions were also examined in relation to peer group homo-
focuses more on hands-on and practical activities. geneity and mean level adjustment.

Peer groups and group type Peer groups and adjustment at school
The developmental significance of peers increases as Previous research in academic settings has shown that the
adolescents start to spend more time in peer groups (Brown, members of adolescents’ peer groups are similar with respect
1990, 2004; Rubin et al., 1998). Through interactions with to school adjustment, that is, academic achievement (Chen et
their peers, adolescents acquire a wide range of skills, attitudes al., 2003; Ryan, 2001), school engagement (Kindermann,
and experiences (Bukowski, Newcomb, & Hartup, 1996; 2007; Kindermann et al., 1996), and intrinsic learning
Rubin et al., 1998). It has been suggested that peer interactions motivation (Ryan, 2001). Adolescents belonging to the same
take place on multiple levels (e.g., Brown, 1990; Hinde, 1987; peer group have also been found to resemble each other with
Rubin et al., 1998). Brown (1989), for example, described peer regards to school-related burnout (Kiuru et al., 2008).
interactions as operating on three levels: dyads, cliques, and However, no previous studies have examined the extent to
crowds. The dyadic level consists of reciprocal dyadic peer which peer group homogeneity in school adjustment varies
relations, such as best friendships or romantic relations. The according to the specific type of peer group. Some research
clique level, in turn, consists of peer groups of a small number has, however, examined the role of peer group cohesion (i.e.,
of adolescents who “hang around” together and develop close the level of reciprocity in the peer group) in peer group simi-
relationships. The crowd level consists of reputation-based larity. These studies show that high peer group cohesion
peer groups or larger collectives of similarly stereotyped indi- predicts high peer group similarity in external problem behav-
viduals. In this study a peer group refers to what Brown defines iors, such as aggression and delinquency (e.g., Cairns &
as a “clique.” Cairns, 1994; Haynie, 2001; Kiesner, Cadinu, Poulin, &
Recently, different subtypes of peer groups (or peer cliques Bucci, 2002). The present study examined whether peer group
according to Brown) have been identified. The term “clique” homogeneity in school adjustment varies according to peer
has been used to refer to highly cohesive peer groups with group type, and whether different kinds of peer groups differ
intensive ties among the members, whereas the term “loose in their mean level of school adjustment.
group” has been used to refer to distinguishable peer groups However, gender may also play a role in peer group homo-
with less intensive ties among the group members (e.g., geneity. There is some evidence to show that the members of
Değirmencioğlu, Urberg, Tolson, & Richard, 1998). “Isolate girls’ peer groups show more similarity with each other than do
dyads”, in turn, refer to groups of two individuals who share the members of boys’ groups (e.g., Cairns & Cairns, 1994;
a reciprocal tie but do not belong to any larger peer group (e.g., Cairns, Xie, & Leung, 1998; Kiuru et al., 2007). Girls also show
Espelage, Holt, & Henkel, 2003; Ryan, 2001). Previous studies a greater tendency than boys to conform to their peer groups
have been inconsistent in terms of which types of peer group (e.g., Benenson & Schinazi, 2004; Davies & Kandel, 1981;
they have considered to be peer groups. Some studies have Laird, Pettit, Dodge, & Bates, 1999). Not all studies, however,
defined peer groups in terms of cliques and isolate dyads have been able to show such differences (e.g., Chen et al., 2003;
(Espelage et al., 2003); some studies have identified cliques Urberg et al., 1997). Consequently, the present study examined
and loose groups as major types of peer groups (Değirmen- whether gender plays a role in peer group homogeneity with
cioğlu et al., 1998; Urberg, Değirmencioğlu, & Pilgrim, 1997); respect to school adjustment and whether girls’ and boys’ peer
and still other studies have defined peer groups in terms of groups show different levels of school adjustment.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 67

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (1), 65–76 67

Aims of the study year ago prior to the data collection had faced the transition to
post-comprehensive schooling (six senior high schools, seven
The following research questions were examined:
vocational schools, two voluntary tenth grades). A total of 1494
• Do the members of adolescents’ peer groups resemble each (755 girls, 739 boys) adolescents filled in a questionnaire
other in their academic achievement, satisfaction with concerning their peer relations and adjustment in the school
educational track, school engagement, and school burnout? context. They were first contacted and asked if they would be
We expected that adolescents belonging to the same peer willing to participate in the study. Those who consented were
group would resemble each other in different aspects of then group-administered questionnaires during a regular school
school adjustment (Hypothesis 1). hour. The participation rate as a proportion of the total number
• Does peer group similarity in academic achievement, satis- of students was 84%, ranging between 65% and 100%.
faction with educational track, school engagement, and The final sample of the present study consisted of 1,262
school burnout vary according to group type and gender? (659 girls, 603 boys) adolescents, all of whom were members
We expected to find greater similarity among adolescents of a peer group. Adolescents who had consented to participate
belonging to highly cohesive peer groups, that is, clique but did not belong to any peer group (N = 232) were excluded
members, than among the members of loose groups in from the present study. Analyses were carried out to examine
school adjustment (Hypothesis 2a, Kiesner et al., 2002). the selection effect due to the study design; that is, adolescents
Also the interaction between group type and gender was who were members of a peer group (N = 1262) were compared
examined. We expected that the effect of peer group with those who did not belong to any peer group (N = 232)
cohesion would be particularly salient among girls (Hypoth- with regards to the variables of interest in the present study.
esis 2b, Benenson, 1990; Cairns & Cairns, 1994; Urberg et The results showed that peer group members showed more
al., 1995). satisfaction with their educational track (M = 4.06, SD = 0.78)
• Do the mean levels of academic achievement, satisfaction with than did the adolescents who had no peer group affiliation
educational track, school engagement, and school burnout (M = 3.88, SD = 0.86, t(1372) = 3.06, p < .01). No selection
differ according to group type and gender? We expected that effects between the adolescents who belonged and those who
girls’ peer groups would show higher academic achievement did not belong to any peer group were found in academic
as well as higher levels of school engagement and school achievement, school satisfaction, school engagement, or school
burnout than those of boys (Hypothesis 3a). Moreover, we burnout.
expected that isolate dyads would experience less school The mean age of the participants was 16 (M = 16.47, SD =
adjustment and more school burnout than the adolescents 1.73). The majority of the participants (99%) were Finnish-
belonging to cliques or loose groups (Hypothesis 3b). speaking, 1% of them having some other mother tongue. This
agrees well with the figures for ethnic minorities at the national
level. A total of 27% of the fathers and 20% of the mothers of
The Finnish school system the participants worked in higher white-collar occupations
(e.g., physicians, teachers, lawyers, managers); 17% of the
Finnish children start their education at kindergarten during
fathers and 49% of the mothers were in lower white-collar
the year of their sixth birthday. One year later, at age 7, they
occupations (e.g., secretaries, salespersons, nurses); 36% of
move to compulsory comprehensive school where they
fathers and 17% of mothers were in blue-collar occupations
continue for the next nine years. Up to age 16, all Finnish
(e.g., welders, construction workers, mechanics); 11% of
adolescents have a similar basic education. After comprehen-
fathers and 4% of mothers were private entrepreneurs; 1% and
sive school adolescents’ educational trajectories begin to
2% were students; 3% and 2% were retired; and 5% and 6%
diverge. A total of 55% of the adolescents enter senior high
had other status (e.g., unemployed).
schools and 37% vocational schools, 2% stay on for a volun-
tary tenth grade, and 6% exit formal education (School Statis-
tics, Central Statistical Office of Finland, 2003). High
academic achievement in the ninth grade is required for admis- Measures
sion to senior high school. Senior high school education, in
turn, is a prerequisite for university education, whereas Identification of peer groups
education in vocational schools leads directly to a lower level
Participants’ peer groups were identified by using the socio-
occupational qualification. Educational choices at the end of
metric procedure developed by Coie, Dodge, and Coppotelli
comprehensive school channel Finnish young people into
(1982). The participants were asked to nominate up to three
either an academic or a vocational track (e.g., Kosonen, 1983;
schoolmates from the same grade level in their school with
Malmberg, 1996; Savolainen, 2001). Finnish girls graduate
whom they most liked to spend time and three classmates with
from senior high schools and enter universities more often than
whom they least liked to spend time (positive and negative
boys (Education in Finland, 1999; Nevala, 2000). Education in
nominations). Cross-gender nominations were allowed in
Finland is state-provided and tuition is free.
order to gain a realistic picture of the adolescents’ peer
relations in their classrooms (Terry & Coie, 1991).
To identify peer groups, only positive peer nominations were
Method used. Approximately 87% of positive nominations were of
same-sex peers. The number of positive peer nominations
Participants ranged from zero to three with an average of 2.4 nominations.
The study reported here is part of the Finnish Educational A total of 12.6% of the participants gave no nominations. Girls
Transitions (FinEdu) study (Salmela-Aro, Niemivirta, & gave more nominations (M = 2.61, SD = 0.84) than boys (M =
Nurmi, 2003). The participants were adolescents who half a 2.20, SD = 1.20; t(1433) = 7.49, p < .001). A total of 55.3%

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 68

68 KIURU ET AL. / PEER GROUP HOMOGENEITY IN ADOLESCENTS’ SCHOOL ADJUSTMENT

of nominations were for peers in the same class, 29.4% for School engagement. School engagement was measured by the
peers in other classes in the same school, 7.4% for peers in abbreviated student version (Salmela-Aro, 2004) of the
other schools and 7.9% for unknown peers. Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (UWES-S) developed origi-
Sociograms based on the positive nominations were drawn nally by Schaufeli et al. (2002) on the basis of the Utrecht
for all 15 schools. Peer group membership was defined on the Work Engagement Scale (UWES-9, Schaufeli, Bakker, &
basis of reciprocal, unilateral and indirect links. A reciprocal Salanova, 2006). The scale consisted of nine unidimensional
link is a reciprocal positive nomination between two indi- items measuring vigor (e.g., When I study, I feel that I am
viduals. A unilateral link is a unilateral positive nomination bursting with energy), dedication (e.g., I am enthusiastic about my
from one adolescent to another. An indirect link (i.e., common studies), and absorption (e.g., Time flies when I’m studying) in
friendship) is a link between two individuals via a third person. relation to school work and rated on a 7-point scale (0 = never;
The following criteria were used to determine group 6 = every day). For the purpose of this study, a mean sum score
membership. To be categorized as a member of a particular was calculated from all nine items to indicate the level of
group: (1) at least 50% of a person’s reciprocal and unilateral adolescents’ school engagement (see also Hakanen, Bakker, &
links had to be within the peer group; and (2) either a recipro- Demerouti, 2005). The Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was .94.
cal, a unilateral or an indirect link had to exist from each Academic achievement correlated .15 (p < .001) with
member to every other member of the peer group. Participants satisfaction with educational track, and .16 (p < .001) with
were assigned to groups if both the group membership criteria school engagement. The correlation between satisfaction
were met. If participants had links to multiple peer groups, with educational track and school engagement was .55
they were assigned to the peer group in which they had the (p < .001).
largest number of peer links. The criteria resembled those used
in other studies using social network analysis to identify peer School burnout. School burnout was examined with the
groups (e.g., Ennett & Bauman, 1994; Espelage et al., 2003; School Burnout Scale (BBI-10) developed by Salmela-Aro and
Ryan, 2001; Urberg et al., 1997). Näätänen (2005). The scale is based on the Burnout Inventory
for Working Life (BBI-15) (Näätänen, Aro, Matthiesen, &
Salmela-Aro, 2003; Salmela-Aro, Näätänen, & Nurmi, 2004).
Peer group type The scale consists of 10 unidimensional items measuring
The peer groups were categorized as cliques, loose groups, or school burnout (e.g., I feel overwhelmed by schoolwork; I have
isolate dyads. The criteria for identifying these types of groups become less interested in schoolwork and I often think of dropping
were based on procedures used in previous studies (e.g., out of school; I expected to do better academically than I have done),
Ennett & Bauman, 1994; Espelage et al., 2003; Ryan, 2001; which were rated on a 6-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 6 =
Urberg et al., 1997) but modified for the purpose of the strongly agree). For the purpose of this study, a mean sum score
present study. Cliques were highly cohesive peer groups was formed on the basis of the 10 items to indicate the level
(consisting of three or more members) among whom at least of adolescents’ school burnout. The Cronbach’s alpha
85% of the all possible direct nominations were reciprocal. reliability for the scale was .90. School burnout correlated –.47
Loose groups consisted of adolescents who met the criteria for (p < .001) with satisfaction with school track and –.37 with
group membership, but among whom less than 85% of the ties school engagement.
were reciprocal. Isolate dyads consisted of only two members
who shared a reciprocal tie and who did not belong to any
larger peer group.
Educational track
Educational track after comprehensive school was assessed by
asking the participants the following questions: (1) Are you in
School adjustment education at the moment? (1 = yes, 0 = no); (2) If you are in
Academic achievement. Academic achievement was measured education, what is the name of your school? Participants’ answers
by asking the participants to report their Grade Point Average to these questions were coded in the following way: 1 = senior
(GPA) in their final comprehensive school report. In Finland, high school (academic track; 47.5%); 2 = vocational school
GPA ranges from 4 (lowest) to 10 (highest). Self-reported (vocational track; 47.4%); 3 = voluntary tenth grade of
grade point average has shown to have a correlation of .96 with comprehensive school (3.8%); and 4 = outside formal
actual grade point average among Finnish ninth-graders education (1.3%). In the present study, an educational track
(Holopainen & Savolainen, 2005). variable was created by contrasting academic track with
vocational track. Adolescents in senior high school (N = 710)
Satisfaction with educational track. Adolescents’ satisfaction were coded 1 and adolescents in vocational school (N = 708)
with their educational track after the transition to post- were coded 0. Adolescents in 10th grade comprehensive or
comprehensive schooling was measured by Satisfaction with outside education (N = 76) were coded as missing data.
Educational Track scale (Nurmi, Niemivirta, & Salmela-Aro,
2003). The scale consisted of four unidimensional questions:
(1 = To what extent are you satisfied with your educational track?;
Gender
2 = To what extent are you interested in the subjects you are being Gender was coded by asking the adolescents to circle the
taught in your education?; 3 = Do you enjoy going to school?; 4 = correct alternative (1 = girl, 2 = boy).
Do you feel that your choice of education was successful?) which
were rated on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all; 5 = very much). A
mean sum score was calculated from all four items to measure
Analysis strategy
adolescents’ satisfaction with their educational track. The The first aim of the present study was to examine whether
Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was .90. adolescents belonging to the same peer group would resemble

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 69

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (1), 65–76 69

each other in their academic achievement, satisfaction with cliques (43 girls’ cliques, 26 boys’ cliques, 4 mixed cliques),
educational track, school engagement, and school burnout. 204 loose groups (78 girls’ loose groups, 91 boys’ loose groups,
The Multilevel Modeling technique (Duncan et al., 1997; 35 mixed loose groups), and 83 isolate dyads (48 girls’ isolate
Muthén, 1997) is an excellent tool for the purposes of answer- dyads, 33 boys’ isolate dyads, 2 mixed isolate dyads). Girls
ing this research question, as it enables the variance in the were more likely to belong to peer cliques (Adjusted Standard-
observed variables to be differentiated into two components: ized Residual (ASR) = 3.7) and isolate dyads (ASR = 2.0) than
variation that is due to similarity among the adolescents in the boys, whereas boys were more likely to belong to loose groups
same peer group (between-peer group variation) and variation (ASR = 4.6) than girls (χ2, N = 1071) = 21.87, p < .001). No
that is due to individual differences within the peer groups gender differences were found with regards to peer group size.
(within-peer group variation). One statistical index used in Cliques and loose groups were by definition larger than isolate
multilevel modeling is the intraclass correlation (ICC), which dyads. Moreover, loose groups (M = 4.15, SD = 1.48) typi-
provides an estimate of what proportion of the total variance cally contained of more members than cliques (M = 3.41, SD
in an observed variable is due to between-group differences = 0.62, t(275) = –5.85, p < .001). A total of 167 peer groups
(Heck, 2001; Muthén, 1991). In other words, the ICC is (148 gender-homogeneous groups; 19 mixed gender groups)
calculated by dividing between-group variance by the total were on an academic track, 169 peer groups (149 gender-
variance (total variance = between-group variance + within- homogeneous groups; 20 mixed groups) were on a vocational
group variance). In the present study, ICCs were calculated track, 12 peer groups (all gender-homogeneous) were in the
and their statistical significance tested for each adjustment tenth grade, and 12 peer groups (10 gender-homogeneous
variable separately by using the peer grouping as a clustering groups; 2 mixed groups) contained adolescents on different
variable. tracks. Only the peer groups that were homogeneous in gender
The second aim of the present study was to examine and whose members belonged either to an academic or to a
whether peer group type and gender would contribute to peer vocational track (N = 297; 42 girls’ cliques, 25 boys’ cliques,
group similarity in academic achievement, satisfaction with 71 girls’ loose groups, 82 boys’ loose groups, 46 girls’ isolate
educational track, school engagement, and school burnout dyads, 31 boys’ isolate dyads) were included in the analyses in
when controlled for the effect of educational track. This which differences in peer group homogeneity and differences
research question was analyzed by conducting multiple group at the mean level in school adjustment were examined accord-
analyses in which intraclass correlations were compared ing to gender and peer group type when controlled for
between different combinations of peer group type and gender educational track. Of these 297 peer groups, 148 were peer
(i.e., girls’ cliques, boys’ cliques, girls’ loose groups, boys’ groups on an academic track (15 girls’ cliques, 9 boys’ cliques,
loose groups, girls’ isolate dyads, boys’ isolate dyads). 41 girls’ loose groups, 39 boys’ loose groups, 27 girls’ isolate
The third aim of the present study was to examine whether dyads, 17 boys’ isolate dyads) and 149 were peer groups on a
the mean levels of academic achievement, satisfaction with vocational track (27 girls’ cliques, 16 boys’ cliques, 30 girls’
educational track, school engagement, and school burnout loose groups, 43 boys’ loose groups, 19 girls’ isolate dyads, 14
would differ according to gender and peer group type when boys’ isolate dyads).
controlled for the effect of educational track. This research
question was analyzed by deploying multiple group analyses of
the mean differences between the different peer group type and
Peer group homogeneity in school adjustment
gender combinations (i.e., girls’ cliques, boys’ cliques, girls’ The first research question was to examine whether
loose groups, boys’ loose groups, girls’ isolate dyads, and boys’ adolescents in the same peer group would resemble each
isolate dyads). other in their academic achievement, satisfaction with
All the statistical analyses were performed using the Mplus educational track, school engagement, and school burnout.
statistical package (Version 4.1; Muthén & Muthén, Intraclass correlations and their statistical significance are
1998–2006) with the missing data method, that is, the presented separately for each adjustment variable in Table 1.
standard MAR approach (missing at random) to missingness A total of 68% of the variation in academic achievement,
(Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2006). This missing data method 19% in satisfaction with educational track, 20% in school
uses all the data that are available in order to estimate the engagement, and 6% in school burnout were due to differ-
model without imputing data. Because the variables were ences between peer groups: adolescents in the same peer
skewed, the parameters of the models were estimated using the group resembled each other, particularly in academic achieve-
MLR estimator (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2006). ment, but also in satisfaction with educational track, school
engagement, and school burnout. To ensure that the results
were not due to educational track, the effect of educational
track on peer group similarity was also examined (Table 1).
Results The results for academic achievement, satisfaction with
educational track, and school engagement showed no differ-
Peer group composition ences between the two tracks in peer group homogeneity
A total of 360 peer groups with total of 1262 members were (nominal = .05, two-tailed test). The results for school
identified in the sample. The majority (N = 319; 89%) of the burnout showed, in turn, that significant peer group homo-
peer groups were composed of adolescents of the same sex. geneity existed only among the peer groups on an academic
The number of girls’ peer groups was 169 and the number of track, whereas no peer group homogeneity was found among
boys’ peer groups was 150. A total of 41 peer groups contained the peer groups on a vocational track.
members of both sexes. The size of the peer groups ranged
from two to nine (M = 3.52; SD = 1.45). Peer groups were
classified according to type. The 360 groups comprised 73

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 70

70 KIURU ET AL. / PEER GROUP HOMOGENEITY IN ADOLESCENTS’ SCHOOL ADJUSTMENT

Table 1
Intraclass correlations of observed variables among the whole sample (Nwithin =
1117–1262; Nbetween = 355–360) and separately for adolescents on an academic track
(Nwithin = 469–472; Nbetween = 148) and on a vocational track (Nwithin = 420–460;
Nbetween = 145–149)

Variables Whole sample Academic track Vocational track

Academic achievement .68*** .53*** .42***


School satisfaction .19*** .22*** .17***
School engagement .20*** .12*** .27***
School burnouta .06*** .05*** .01***

Notes. ***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05 (two-tailed test).
a In the case of school burnout, intraclass correlation was significant only at the whole

sample level and among peer groups on an academic track.

The role of peer group type and gender in peer group The results for academic achievement in the overall test for
homogeneity of school adjustment differences in intraclass correlations showed that the hypothe-
sis that all the intraclass correlations would be equal was
The second aim was to examine whether peer group type and rejected (χ2diff(5) = 14.48; p = .01 < .05). The results of paired
gender would contribute to peer group similarity in academic comparisons (see Table 2) showed further that the members of
achievement, satisfaction with educational track, school girls’ isolate dyads resembled each other more in terms of
engagement, and school burnout. The results for the positive academic achievement than did the members of girls’ cliques
indicators of school adjustment (i.e., academic achievement, and girls’ loose groups. Similarly, the members of boys’ isolate
satisfaction with educational track, school engagement) are dyads resembled each other more in terms of academic
presented first, followed by the results for school burnout. achievement than did the members of girls’ cliques and girls’
Because the results for academic achievement, satisfaction loose groups. The results in the overall test for differences in
with educational track, and school engagement showed no the intraclass correlations for both satisfaction with
differences between the two educational tracks, the intraclass educational track (χ2diff(4) = 12.61; p = .02 < .05) and school
correlations of these variables were calculated separately for engagement (χ2diff(4) = 9.75; p = .04 < .05) showed that the
the different peer group type and gender combinations (i.e., hypothesis that all the intraclass correlations would be equal
girls’ cliques, boys’ cliques, girls’ loose groups, boys’ loose was rejected. The results for the paired comparisons (see Table
groups, girls’ isolate dyads, and boys’ isolate dyads) combin- 2) showed further that the members of girls’ cliques resembled
ing both educational track groups (Table 2). Moreover, each other more than did the members of girls’ loose groups,
multiple group analyses were conducted in which differences boys’ loose groups and girls’ isolate dyads in terms of satis-
in the intraclass correlations among the different peer group faction with educational track. In school engagement the
type and gender combinations were examined. The compari- members of girls’ cliques resembled each other more than did
sons between the intraclass correlations for each adjustment the members of girls’ loose groups and boys’ loose groups. No
variable were carried out only for those groups among whom other statistically significant group differences were found,
the intraclass correlation was significant or equal/larger than except that boys’ isolate dyads showed no group homogeneity
.10 (see also Muthén & Muthén, 2001). The comparisons were either in satisfaction with educational track or school satis-
carried out in two steps for each variable measuring school faction. Because the intraclass correlation for school burnout
adjustment: (1) overall test for differences in intraclass was significant only among peer groups on an academic track
correlations (Satorra-Bentler scaled χ2-test for difference, (Table 1), the role of gender and peer group type was
Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2006) and (2) paired comparisons examined only for peer groups on this particular track.
of differences in intraclass correlations (nominal = .05, two- However, the results showed that none of the peer group type
tailed test). and gender combinations showed significant peer group

Table 2
Intraclass correlations in school satisfaction, school engagement, and school burnout across different combinations of peer group type and
gender

Girls’ Boys’ Girls’ loose Boys’ loose Girls’ isolate Boys’ isolate
cliques cliques groups groups dyads dyads
Number of groups (Nbetween = 42) (Nbetween = 25) (Nbetween = 71) (Nbetween = 82) (Nbetween = 46) (Nbetween = 31)

Academic achievement .56***/a .66***/ab .60***/a .69***/ab .81***/b .79***/b


School satisfaction .48**/a .19/ab .17**/bc .10/bc .13/bc .03#
School engagement .42***/a .39**/ab .11*/bc .14*/bc .18/abc .00#

Notes. ***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05 (two-tailed test).
Intraclass correlations with different letters in superscript differ by a significance level of at least .05 (two-tailed test).
# Not part of comparison, because of non-existing intraclass correlation.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 71

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (1), 65–76 71

homogeneity in school burnout. Consequently, no further girls’ loose groups, boys’ loose groups, girls’ isolate dyads, and
comparisons were carried out. boys’ isolate dyads). The means and standard deviations of the
adjustment variables among these groups are presented in
Table 4. Multiple group analyses for mean differences were
Mean differences according to peer group type and carried out in the following steps for each variable measuring
gender in school adjustment school adjustment: (1) overall test for differences in means
The third aim of the present study was to examine whether the (Satorra-Bentler scaled χ2-test for difference, Muthén, &
mean levels of academic achievement, satisfaction with Muthén, 1998–2006); and (2) paired comparisons of differ-
educational track, school engagement, and school burnout ences in means (nominal = .05, two-tailed test). Paired
would differ according to different combinations of gender and comparisons between the means in different groups (nominal =
peer group type when controlled for educational track. The .05, two-tailed test) were carried out only if the overall test
means and standard deviations for the peer groups on the level showed that the means between all the groups were not equal.
of the whole sample, and separately for peer groups on an Because no mean differences between the peer groups on an
academic track and on a vocational track, are presented in academic track and those on a vocational track were found in
Table 3. Multiple group analyses of the mean level using the satisfaction with educational track and school engagement, the
Satorra-Bentler scaled χ2-test for difference (Muthén & whole sample was used in the analyses of these variables. The
Muthén, 1998–2006) showed no mean differences between results of the overall test for school engagement showed mean
peer groups on an academic track and peer groups on a differences among the groups; that is, the hypothesis that all
vocational track in satisfaction with educational track and the means would be equal was rejected (χ2diff(5) = 15.46; p =
school engagement. However, with regards to academic .01 < .05). Consequently, paired mean comparisons were
achievement and school burnout, mean differences were found carried out for mean differences in school engagement (see
for the two tracks (see Table 3): the peer groups on an Table 4). The results showed that girls’ cliques showed more
academic track showed higher levels of academic achievement engagement than boys’ loose groups, girls’ isolate dyads, or
than those on a vocational track. Moreover, the peer groups on boys’ isolate dyads. Moreover, girls’ loose groups showed more
an academic track also showed higher levels of school burnout school engagement than boys’ loose groups, girls’ isolate
than those on a vocational track. dyads, or boys’ isolate dyads. The results of the overall test for
Next, the role of peer group type and gender in the mean satisfaction with educational track showed no mean differences
level of the school adjustment variables was examined by between the groups.
comparing the means among the different combinations of Next, because in the case of academic achievement and
peer group type and gender (i.e., girls’ cliques, boys’ cliques, school burnout, school track showed a statistically significant

Table 3
Means and standard deviations of observed variables for the peer groups among the whole sample (Nwithin =
1117–1262; Nbetween = 355–360) and separately for peer groups on an academic track (Nwithin = 469–472;
Nbetween = 148) and on a vocational track (Nwithin = 420–460; Nbetween = 145–149)

The whole sample Academic track Vocational track

Variables M SD M SD M SD Range of scale

Academic achievement 8.01 0.68 8.52a 0.42 7.46b 0.45 4–10


School satisfaction 4.08 0.33 4.04a 0.35 4.08b 0.35 1–5
School engagement 3.75 0.57 3.73a 0.41 3.76b 0.67 0–6
School burnout 2.37 0.22 2.47a 0.20 2.26b 0.10 1–6

Note. Means with different superscript differ by a significance level of at least .05 (two-tailed test).

Table 4
Means and standard deviations of school satisfaction and school engagement across different combinations of peer group type and gender

Girls’ Boys’ Girls’ loose Boys’ loose Girls’ isolate Boys’ isolate
cliques cliques groups groups dyads dyads
(Nbetween = 42) (Nbetween = 25) (Nbetween = 71) (Nbetween = 82) (Nbetween = 46) (Nbetween = 31)

Number of groups M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Academic achievement 8.07a 0.53 7.78b 0.63 8.34a 0.57 7.71b 0.69 8.15a 0.70 7.76b 0.76
School satisfaction 4.13 0.60 4.21 0.33 4.04 0.33 4.06 0.22 3.93 0.28 4.04 0.10
School engagement 4.01a 0.75 3.70ab 0.92 3.93a 0.40 3.59b 0.47 3.50b 0.56 3.63b 0.10
School Burnouta 2.36a 0.82 2.20a 0.85 2.50a 0.89 2.25a 0.91 2.59b 0.82 2.16a 0.69

Notes. a When examining mean differences between the different combinations of peer group type and gender general modeling was used instead
of multilevel modeling because peer group level variance was lacking.
Means with different superscript differ by a significance level of at least .05 (two-tailed test).

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 72

72 KIURU ET AL. / PEER GROUP HOMOGENEITY IN ADOLESCENTS’ SCHOOL ADJUSTMENT

effect, this was controlled for in the multiple group analyses of The results showed further that peer group homogeneity in
the mean level for these variables. The results of the overall test school adjustment was not dependent on school track
for academic achievement showed mean differences among the (academic track vs. vocational track). The only exception was
groups; that is, the hypothesis that all the means would be equal school burnout, in which peer group homogeneity was signifi-
was rejected (χ2diff(5) = 43.32; p = < .001). The results of cant only in the case of groups whose members were on an
paired mean comparisons (see Table 4) showed further a strong academic track. This result may be due to the fact that
gender effect: girls’ cliques had higher academic achievement academic pressures are higher on an academic track than on a
than boys’ cliques, girls’ loose groups had higher academic vocational track, as on the latter there is a greater emphasis on
achievement than boys’ loose groups, and girls’ isolate dyads manual skills. The results also showed that adolescents on an
showed higher academic achievement than boys’ isolate dyads. academic track experienced a higher level of school burnout
The results for the overall test for mean differences in school than those on a vocational track (see also Salmela-Aro et al.,
burnout (χ2diff(5) = 15.55; p = .01 < .05) showed that the means in press-b).
were not equal across the different peer group type and gender
combinations; that is, the hypothesis that all the means would
be equal was rejected. The results for the paired mean compari-
The role of peer group type in group homogeneity
sons (see Table 4) for school burnout showed further that girls The main aim of the present study was to examine whether
belonging to isolate dyads differed from adolescents belonging peer group similarity in school adjustment would vary accord-
to any other peer group type: they experienced more school ing to group type. The results were different for girls and boys,
burnout than girls’ cliques, boys’ cliques, girls’ loose groups, suggesting a group type ⫻ gender interaction effect. Peer group
boys’ loose groups, or boys’ isolate dyads. cohesion was found to play a particularly important role
among girls (Hypotheses 2a and 2b): girls’ cliques showed
more peer group homogeneity in terms of both satisfaction
Discussion with educational track and school engagement than girls’ loose
groups. There are at least two explanations for this result. First,
Previous research has shown that the members of adolescents’ girls’ may be particularly motivated to conform to their peer
peer groups are similar with respect to many school adjustment group when the group members share intensive ties with each
variables (e.g., Chen et al., 2003; Kindermann, 2007; Kiuru other. Girls who belong to highly cohesive peer cliques may
et al., 2008; Ryan, 2001). However, no studies have been also experience higher levels of trust and social support in their
conducted to examine how type of peer group (i.e., clique, group (e.g., Berndt, 1989, 2002; Hallinan & Williams, 1990)
loose group, isolate dyad) contributes to peer group homo- and higher sense of belonging (e.g., Brown, 1989) than girls
geneity. This was the main purpose of the present study. As belonging to less cohesive peer groups. This may lead to higher
found previously, the results showed that the members of peer group conformity in girls’ cliques compared to loose
adolescents’ peer groups resembled each other in terms of groups. Second, the members of highly cohesive peer groups
school adjustment. However, this peer group homogeneity was may interact on a more regular basis (cf. flow of norms and
found to vary according to group type. The members of girls’ information, Moody & White, 2003) than less tightly
cliques resembled each other more in terms of satisfaction with connected peer groups. This provides more opportunities for
their educational track and school engagement than did the shared talk and disclosure than is the case in less connected
members of girls’ loose groups. Similar effect was not found peer groups.
among boys. Girls’ isolate dyads, in particular, were at risk for In contrast, among boys peer group cohesion did not play
low school adjustment. such a significant role in peer group homogeneity (Hypothesis
2b). There are at least two explanations for this result. One
explanation is that girls and boys show intimacy in different
Peer group homogeneity in school adjustment ways in their peer relations. Boys typically show intimacy
The first aim of the present study was to examine whether the through shared activities, whereas girls often express intimacy
members of adolescents’ peer groups resemble each other in through talk and self-disclosure (McNelles & Connolly, 1999).
their school adjustment a half a year after the transition to The results of the present study showed that peer group
post-comprehensive schooling. The results were consistent cohesion played a particularly important role among girls in
with Hypothesis 1, suggesting that peer groups were homoge- satisfaction with educational track and school engagement,
neous, in particular, in terms of academic achievement, but variables that might be particularly affected by shared
also in satisfaction with educational track, school engagement, discussions and the sharing of opinions in peer groups. In
and school burnout. These results are consistent with previous contrast, because boys spend time together mainly in shared
studies showing that the members of peer groups are similar activities and doing things together, even boys belonging to
with respect to many school adjustment variables (e.g., Chen tightly connected peer cliques may spend less time in group
et al., 2003; Kindermann et al., 1996; Ryan, 2001). Overall, talk than girls in similar groups and are therefore less likely to
peer group homogeneity was higher in the positive indicators show more peer group homogeneity in satisfaction with their
than the negative indicators of school adjustment. One educational track and school engagement than the members of
possible explanation for this result is that the adolescents were loose groups. The second possible explanation concerns
examined only half a year after moving to a new learning statistical power in the case of school engagement. Girls were
environment (a starting point in the construction of peer more likely to belong to peer cliques than boys and thus the
groups). In the initial selection into new peer groups, academic overall number of peer cliques was somewhat larger among
resources, such as academic engagement and achievement, girls than boys. This may have affected the results on peer
may play a more important role than subjective feelings of group cohesion among girls, as compared to that among
academic overload, such as school burnout. boys.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 73

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (1), 65–76 73

The results showed further that all the types of peer groups Caldwell, 1997; Wentzel et al., 2004) which may then be
were strikingly similar in terms of academic achievement (see reflected on how they think and feel about school. This was
also Chen et al., 2003). This finding is in accordance with the found, however, only for girls. This may be for several reasons.
notion that peer group homogeneity tends to be larger in First, the tendencies of girls to attribute more importance to
visible behaviors and shared activities than in attitudes and peer group membership (e.g., Crocket et al., 1984; Kerr et al.,
motivation (Kandel, 1978; Werner & Parmelee, 1979). 2007) and be more vulnerable to interpersonal stress (e.g.,
Finally, except for academic achievement, the members of Leadbeater, Blatt, & Quinlan, 1995; Rudolph & Hammen,
adolescents’ isolate dyads showed no or very low similarity to 1999; Towbes, Cohen, & Glyshaw, 1989; Wagner & Compas,
each other in terms of school adjustment. This may be due to 1990) compared to boys may cause girls to suffer more than
the fact that the members of isolate dyads have been compelled boys from being somewhat rejected or neglected in the larger
to spend time with each other, as they have not been accepted peer network and to be more vulnerable than boys in isolate
in other groups. Consequently, they may not have chosen each dyads where peer relationship quality is likely to be low. The
other on the basis of similarity and they may also be less moti- reason why relationship quality can be expected to be lower in
vated to conform to each other than the members of other isolate dyads is that peer selection may be more limited for
types of groups. these individuals as they are less accepted among their peers
(e.g., Salmivalli, Huttunen, & Lagerspetz, 1997; Urberg, Luo,
Pilgrim, & Değirmencioğlu, 2003). Another possible expla-
Mean differences in school adjustment between boys’ nation for the results is gender differences in peer group
and girls’ peer groups composition. Previous research has shown that girls’ peer
The third aim of the present study was to examine whether the groups are more exclusive towards group-outsiders compared
means of academic achievement, satisfaction with educational to boys’ peer groups, which tend to be more interconnected
track, school engagement, and school burnout differ accord- (e.g., Urberg et al., 1995). Consequently, it may be that boys’
ing to gender and to peer group type. As expected (Hypothe- isolate dyads have more contacts in the peer network than
sis 3a), the results showed clear gender differences in academic girls’ isolate dyads and thus feel less rejected than girls’ isolate
achievement: girls’ peer groups showed higher academic dyads.
achievement than those of boys. However, peer group types did
not differ from each other in this respect.
Similarly, as expected (Hypothesis 3a), gender effects were
Limitations of the study
found in school engagement and to some extent in school At least five limitations have to be taken into account in any
burnout: girls’ peer groups tended to show not only more effort to generalize the results of the present study. First, the
school engagement but also more school burnout than those sample sizes in some of the peer group type and gender combi-
of boys. These results are in accordance with previous research nations (boys’ cliques, in particular) were relatively small in
showing that girls typically show higher academic achievement relation to others (i.e., girls’ and boys’ loose groups). This lack
(e.g., Frome & Eccles, 1998; Fuligni et al., 2001), higher of power may have influenced some of our results. Second,
school engagement (e.g., Berndt & Miller, 1990; Ryan, 2001), peer groups were studied only within schools. It has been
as well as more concerns about their academic success (e.g., suggested, however, that the sole use of school-based data may
Murberg & Bru, 2004), and more school burnout (Kiuru et underestimate the extent of peer relations, as adolescents are
al., 2008; Salmela-Aro et al., in press-b). One explanation for likely to have friends and peer groups outside as well as in
the finding that girls not only show higher achievement but also school (e.g., Kerr et al., 2007; Kiesner, Poulin, & Nicotra,
higher levels of school-related stress and school burnout 2003). It can be assumed, however, that in-school peers will
compared to boys is that girls also tend to have lower academic be the most influential for school-related adjustment, because
self-perceptions than boys (e.g., Cole, Martin, Peeke, they share similar school experiences. Third, the participants
Seroczynski, & Fier, 1999; Frey & Ruble, 1987; Pomerantz, of the present study were restricted to only one peer group
Altermatt, & Saxon, 2002). Low academic self-perceptions each (i.e., primary peer groups). There is, however, evidence
combined with placing high importance on achievement may that adolescents can belong to several peer groups simul-
make girls more prone to school-related stress and burnout taneously (e.g., Brown, 2004) and that many peer groups may
compared to boys, even though they are more academically be influential in adolescents’ adjustment. As new techniques
successful than boys. Some evidence has also been found to have been developed recently which provide a possibility to
show that girls are more vulnerable than boys to the negative examine the membership in multiple peer groups simul-
effects of difficulties at school and low academic achievement taneously, such as the MLWin program (Rasbash, Steele, &
(e.g., Bryant, Schulenberg, O’Malley, Bachman, & Johnston, Browne, 2005), such efforts would provide an important
2003; Frome & Eccles, 1998; Pomerantz et al., 2002). extension the existing research. Some new tools for examining
Unexpectedly, no gender differences were found between network autocorrelation and peer influence and selection have
girls’ and boys’ isolate dyads in school engagement and school also been introduced recently, such as the SIENA program
burnout. However, girls’ isolate dyads showed less school (Burk, Steglich, & Snijders, 2007; Snijders, Steglich, &
engagement and more school burnout than other types of girls’ Schweinberger, 2007; Snijders, Steglich, Schweinberger, &
peer groups, that is, girls’ cliques and girls’ loose groups. These Huisman, 2006). As the SIENA is based on simulations of
results suggest that our hypothesis (3b) concerning the peer relationships and peer characteristics, it is not limited to
tendency of isolate dyads to show less school adjustment than the analyses of a particular peer group variable at a particular
adolescents belonging to cliques or loose groups was true only time. Fourth, consistent with previous research (e.g.,
among girls. One possible explanation for these results is that Bukowski, Gauze, Hoza, & Newcomb, 1993; Bukowski,
isolate dyads show a lower level of overall adjustment similar Sippola, & Hoza, 1999; Furman & Buhrmester, 1992), only a
to that of isolates (e.g., Ennett et al., 2006; Wentzel & few peer groups with members of both sexes were identified in

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 74

74 KIURU ET AL. / PEER GROUP HOMOGENEITY IN ADOLESCENTS’ SCHOOL ADJUSTMENT

the sample (11%). Consequently, our low sample size did not Burk, W.J., Steglich, C.E.G., & Snijders, T.A.B. (2007). Beyond dyadic interpe-
allow examination of the role of peer group type among mixed- dendence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 397–404.
Cairns, R.B., & Cairns, D.B. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of youth in our
gender peer groups. This task remains for the future. Finally,
time. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
all the measures included in the present study were based on Cairns, R.B., Xie, H., & Leung, M.-C. (1998). The popularity of friendship and
self-report measures. It is well-known that self-reports are not the neglect of social networks: Toward a new balance. In W.M. Bukowski &
always the most valid and reliable method of data collection A.H. Cillessen (Eds.), Sociometry then and now: Building on six decades of
(Shaffer, 2002). However, in the case of school adjustment it measuring children’s experiences with the peer group (pp. 25–53). San Francisco,
CA: Jossey-Bass.
is likely that subjective experiences are particularly important. Chen, X., Chang, L., & He, Y. (2003). The peer group as a context: Mediating
and moderating effects on relations between academic achievement and social
functioning in Chinese children. Child Development, 74, 710–727.
Conclusions Coie, J.D., Dodge, K.A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and types
of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18,
The results of the present study showed, first, that the
557–570.
members of adolescents’ peer groups resembled each other, in Cole, D.A., Martin, J.M., Peeke, L.A., Seroczynski, A.D., & Fier, J. (1999).
particular, in terms school adjustment, and less so in terms of Children’s over- and underestimation of academic competence: A longitudi-
school burnout. Moreover, peer group type was found to nal study of gender differences, depression, and anxiety. Child Development,
contribute to peer group homogeneity, but was moderated by 70, 459–473.
Crockett, L., Losoff, M., & Petersen, A.C. (1984). Perceptions of the peer
gender: the members of girls’ cliques resembled each other group and friendship in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 4,
more in satisfaction with their educational track and school 155–181.
engagement than girls’ loose groups, whereas the same was not Davies, M., & Kandel, D.B. (1981). Parental and peer influences on adolescents’
true among boys. Girls’ isolate dyads, in particular, were at risk educational plans: Some further evidence. American Journal of Sociology, 87,
363–387.
for low adjustment.
Deci, E.L., Vallerand, R.J., Pelletier, L.G., & Ryan, R.M. (1991). Motivation
and education: The self-determination perspective. Educational Psychologist,
26, 325–346.
Değirmencioğlu, S.M., Urberg, K.A., Tolson, J.M., & Richard, P. (1998).
References Adolescent friendship networks: Continuity and change over the school year.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 313–337.
Benenson, J.F. (1990). Gender differences in social networks. Journal of Early Demerouti, E., Bakker, A.B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2001). The job
Adolescence, 10, 472–495. demands-resources model of burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86,
Benenson, J.F., & Schinazi, J. (2004). Sex differences in reactions to outperform- 499–512.
ing same-sex friends. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 317–333. Dubow, E.F., Tisak, J., Causey, D., Hryshko, A., & Reid, G. (1991). A two-year
Berndt, T.J. (1989). Obtaining support from friends in childhood and adoles- longitudinal study of stressful life events, social support, and social problem-
cence. In D. Belle (Ed.), Children’s social networks and social supports solving skills: Contributions to children’s behavioural and academic adjust-
(pp. 308–331). New York: Wiley. ment. Child Development, 62, 583–599.
Berndt, T.J. (2002). Friendship quality in social development. Current Directions Duncan, T.E., Duncan, S.C., Alpert, A., Hops, H., Stoolmiller, M., & Muthén,
in Psychological Science, 11, 7–10. B. (1997). Latent variable modeling of longitudinal and multilevel substance
Berndt T.J., & Keefe, K. (1995). Friends’ influence on adolescents’ adjustment use data. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32, 275–318.
to school. Child Development, 66, 1312–1329. Education in Finland (1999). Statistics and indicators. Helsinki, Finland: Statistics
Berndt, T.J., & Keefe, K. (1996). Friends’ influence on school adjustment: a Finland.
motivational analysis. In J. Juvonen & K.R. Wentzel (Eds.), Social motivation: Ennett, S.T., & Bauman, K.E. (1994). The contribution of influence and selec-
Understanding children’s school adjustment (pp. 248–278). New York: tion to adolescent peer group homogeneity: The case of adolescent cigarette
Cambridge University Press. smoking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 653–663.
Berndt, T.J., & Miller, K.E. (1990). Expectancies, values, and achievement in Ennett, S.T., Bauman, K.E., Hussong, A., Faris, R., Foshee, V.A., & Cai, L.
junior high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 319–326. (2006). The peer context of adolescent substance use: Findings from social
Brendgen, M., Markiewicz, D., Doyle, A.B., & Bukowski, W.M. (2001). The networks analysis. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16, 159–186.
relations between friendship quality, ranked-friendship preference, and Epstein, J.L., & McPartland, J.M. (1976). The concept and measurement of the
adolescents’ behavior with their friends. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 395–415. quality of school life. American Educational Research Journal, 13, 15–30.
Brown, B.B. (1989). The role of peer groups in adolescents’ adjustment to Espelage, D.L., Holt, M.K., & Henkel, R.R. (2003). Examination of peer-group
secondary school. In T.J. Berndt & G.W. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relationships in child contextual effects on aggression during early adolescence. Child Development,
development (pp. 188–215). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. 74, 205–220.
Brown, B.B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In S.S. Feldman & Frey, K.S., & Ruble, D.N. (1987). What children say about classroom perform-
G.R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 171–196). ance: Sex and grade differences in perceived competence. Child Development,
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 58, 1066–1078.
Brown, B.B. (2004). Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In R.M. Lerner & Frome, P.M., & Eccles, J.S. (1998). Parents’ influence on children’s achieve-
L.D. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., ment-related perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
pp. 363–394). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 435–452.
Bryant, A.L., Schulenberg, J.E., O’Malley, P.M., Bachman, J.G., & Johnston, Fuligni, A.J., Eccles, J.S., Barber, B.L., & Clements, P. (2001). Early adolescent
L.D. (2003). How academic achievement, attitudes, and behaviours relate to peer orientation and adjustment during high school. Developmental Psychol-
the course of substance use during adolescence: A 6-year, multiwave national ogy, 37, 28–36.
longitudinal study. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 361–397. Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in perceptions
Bukowski, W.M., Gauze, C., Hoza, B., & Newcomb, A.F. (1993). Differences of networks of personal relationships. Child Development, 63, 103–115.
and consistency between same-sex and other-sex peer relationships during Hakanen, J.J., Bakker, A.B., & Demerouti, E. (2005). How dentists cope with
early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 29, 255–263. their job demands and stay engaged: The moderating role of job resources.
Bukowski, W.M., Hoza, B., & Boivin, M. (1994). Measuring friendship quality European Journal of Oral Science, 113, 479–487.
during pre- and early adolescence: The development and psychometric prop- Hallinan, M.T., & Williams, R.A. (1990). Students’ characteristics and the peer-
erties of the friendships qualities scale. Journal of Social and Personal Relation- influence process. Sociology of Education, 63, 122–132.
ships, 11, 471–484. Haynie, D.L. (2001). Delinquent peers revisited: Does network structure matter?
Bukowski, W.M., Newcomb, A.F., & Hartup, W.W. (Eds.). (1996). The company American Journal of Sociology, 106, 1013–1038.
they keep – Friendship in childhood and adolescence. Cambridge: Cambridge Heck, R.H. (2001). Multilevel modeling with SEM. In G.A. Marcoulides &
University Press. R.E. Schumacker (Eds.), New developments and techniques in structural equation
Bukowski, W.M., Sippola, L.K., & Hoza, B. (1999). Same and other: Inter- modelling (pp. 89–127). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
dependency between participation in same- and other-sex friendships. Journal Hinde, R.A. (1987). Individuals, relationships and culture: Links between ethology
of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 439–459. and the social sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 75

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (1), 65–76 75

Holopainen, L., & Savolainen, H. (2005). Unpublished raw data. University of Näätänen, P., Aro, A., Matthiesen, S., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2003). Bergen Burnout
Joensuu and University of Jyväskylä, Finland. Indicator 15. Helsinki, Finland: Edita.
Kandel, D.B. (1978). Homophily, selection, and socialization in adolescent Nevala, A. (2000). Opintietä onneen? Korkeakoulutukseen valikoituminen ja
friendships. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 427–436. työmarkkinoille sijoittuminen Suomessa toisen maailmansodan jälkeen
Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Kiesner, J. (2007). Peers and problem behavior: Have [Education and a better life? Selection for further education and placement
we missed something? In R. Engels, M. Kerr, & H. Stattin (Eds.), Friends, in the labor market]. In R. Raivola (Ed.), Vaikuttavuutta koulutukseen [The
lovers, and groups:Who is important in adolescence and why? London: Wiley. influence on education]. Helsinki, Finland: Edita.
Kiesner, J., Cadinu, M., Poulin, F., & Bucci, M. (2002). Group identification in Newman, B.M., Lohman, B.J., Newman, P.R., Myers, M.C., & Smith, V.L.
early adolescence: Its relations with peer adjustment and its moderator effect (2000). Experiences of urban youth navigating the transition to ninth grade.
on peer influence. Child Development, 73, 196–208. Youth & Society, 31, 387–416.
Kiesner, J., Poulin, F., & Nicotra, E. (2003). Peer relations across contexts: Nurmi, J.E., Niemivirta, M., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2003). Satisfaction with
Individual-network homophily and network inclusion in and after school. educational track-scale. University of Jyväskylä and University of Helsinki,
Child Development, 74, 1328–1343. Finland.
Kindermann, T.A. (2007). Effects of naturally existing peer groups on changes Pomerantz, E.M., Altermatt, E.R., & Saxon, J.L. (2002). Making the grade but
in academic engagement in a cohort of sixth graders. Child Development, 78, feeling distressed: Gender differences in academic performance and internal
1186–2103. distress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 396–404.
Kindermann, T.A., McCollam, T.L., & Gibson, E., Jr. (1996). Peer networks Rasbash, J., Steele, F., & Browne, W.J. (2005). A user’s guide to MLwiN version
and students’ classroom engagement during childhood and adolescence. In J. 2.0. University of Bristol.
Juvonen & K.R. Wentzel (Eds.), Social motivation: Understanding children’s Roeser, R.W. (1998). On schooling and mental health: Introduction to the
school adjustment (pp. 279–312). New York: Cambridge University Press. special issue. Educational Psychologist, 33, 129–133.
Kiuru, N., Aunola, K., Nurmi, J.E., Leskinen, E., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2008). Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J.G. (1998). Peer interactions, relation-
Peer group influence and selection in adolescents’ school burnout: A longi- ships, and groups. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook
tudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54, 23–55. of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development
Kiuru, N., Aunola, K., Vuori, J., & Nurmi, J.E. (2007). The role of peer groups (pp. 619–700). New York: Wiley.
in adolescents’ educational expectations and school adjustment. Journal of Rudolph, K.D., & Hammen, C. (1999). Age and gender as determinants of
Youth and Adolescence, 36, 995–1009. stress exposure, generation, and reactions in youngsters: A transactional
Konu, A., & Lintonen, T. (2006). Theory-based survey analysis of well-being in perspective. Child Developoment, 70, 660–677.
secondary schools in Finland. Health Promotion International, 21, 27–36. Ryan, A.M. (2001). The peer group as a context for the development of young
Kosonen, P. (1983). Uranvalinnan valmiuksia peruskoulun päättöluokalla adolescent motivation and achievement. Child Development, 72, 1135–1150.
[Career choice readiness at the end of comprehensive school]. Työvoima- Salmela-Aro, K. (2004). Revised school engagement scale. Jyväskylä: University of
poliittisia tutkimuksia [Studies on labor policy], 39. Helsinki: Ministry of Labor. Jyväskylä.
Laird, R.D., Pettit, G.S., Dodge, K.A., & Bates, J.E. (1999). Best friendships, Salmela-Aro, K., Kiuru, N., Leskinen, E., & Nurmi, J.-E. (in press-a). School-
group relationships and antisocial behavior in early adolescence. Journal of burnout inventory (SBI): Reliability and validity. European Journal of Psycho-
Early Adolescence, 19, 413–437. logical Assessment.
Leadbeater, B.J., Blatt, S.J., & Quinlan, D.M. (1995). Gender-linked vulnerabil- Salmela-Aro, K., Kiuru, N., & Nurmi, J.-E. (in press-b). The role of educational
ities to depressive symptoms, stress, and problem behaviours in adolescents. track in adolescents’ school burnout: A longitudinal study. British Journal of
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 5, 1–29. Educational Psychology.
Leadbeater, B.J., Kuperminc, G.P., Blatt, S.J., & Hertzog, C. (1999). A multi- Salmela-Aro, K., & Näätänen, P. (2005). BBI-10. Nuorten koulu-uupumus-
variate model of gender differences in adolescents’ internalizing and external- menetelmä [Method of assessing adolescents’ school burnout]. Edita:
izing problems. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1268–1282. Helsinki, Finland.
Leiter, M.P., & Schaufeli, W.B. (1996). Consistency of the burnout construct Salmela-Aro, K., Näätänen, P., & Nurmi, J.E. (2004). The role of work-related
across occupations. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 9, 229–243. personal projects during two burnout interventions: A longitudinal study.
Maccoby, E.E. (1995). The two sexes and their social systems. In P. Moen, Work & Stress, 18, 208–230.
G.H. Elder, Jr, & K. Lüscher (Eds.), Examining lives in context: Perspectives on Salmela-Aro, K., Niemivirta, M., & Nurmi, J-E. (2003). Finnish Educational
the ecology of human development (pp. 347–364). Washington, DC: American Transitions (FinEdu) study (ongoing). Helsinki: University of Helsinki, and
Psychological Association. Jyväskylä: University of Jyväskylä, Finland.
Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (1998). Person–context interaction theories. In Salmivalli, C., Huttunen, A., & Lagerspetz, K.M.J. (1997). Peer networks and
R. Lerner (Ed.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. bullying in schools. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 305–312.
Theoretical models of human development (pp. 685–740). New York: Wiley. Savolainen, H. (2001). Explaining mechanisms of educational career choice. A follow-
Malmberg, L.E. (1996). How do Finnish students prepare for their future in up study of the educational career choices of a group of youths that finished compul-
three school types? The relation between content of plans, information sory education in 1990. University of Joensuu, Finland: Publications in
gathering, and self-evaluations. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, education.
457–469. Schaufeli, W.B., Bakker, A.B., & Salanova, M. (2006). The measurement of work
Maslach, C. (1982). Understanding burnout: Definitional issues in analyzing a engagement with a short questionnaire: A cross-national study. Educational
complex phenomenon. In W.S. Paine (Ed.), Job stress and burnout (pp. 29–40). and Psychological Measurement, 66, 701–716.
Beverly Hills, CA: SAGE. Schaufeli, W.B., Martínez, I.M., Pinto, A.M., Salanova, M., & Bakker, A.B.
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W.B., & Leiter, M.P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review (2002). Burnout and engagement in university students: A cross-national
of Psychology, 52, 397–422. study. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 33, 464–481.
McNelles, L.R., & Connolly, J.A. (1999). Intimacy between adolescent friends: School Statistics (2003). http://www.stat.fi/til/khak/2005/khak_2005_2006-12-21_
Age and gender differences in intimate affect and intimate behaviors. Journal tie_001.html Helsinki: Central Statistical Office of Finland.
of Research on Adolescence, 9, 143–159. Shaffer, D.R. (2002). Developmental psychology: Childhood and adolescence (6th
Moody J., & White, D.R. (2003). Structural cohesion and embeddedness: A ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.
hierarchical concept of social groups. American Sociological Review, 68, Snijders, T.A.B., Steglich, C., & Schweinberger, M. (2007). Modeling the co-
103–127. evolution of networks and behavior. In K. Van Montfort, H. Oud, &
Murberg, T.A., & Bru, E. (2004). School-related stress and psychosomatic A. Satorra (Eds.), Longtitudinal models in the behavioral and related sciences
symptoms among Norwegian adolescents. School Psychology International, 25, (pp. 41–71). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
317–332. Snijders, T.A.B., Steglich, C.E.G., Schweinberger, M., & Huisman, M. (2006).
Muthén, B. (1991). Multilevel factor analysis of class and student achievement Manual for SIENA, version 3. Groningen: University of Groningen.
components. Journal of Educational Measurement, 28, 338–354. Terry, R., & Coie, J.D. (1991). A comparison of methods for defining sociomet-
Muthén, B. (1997). Latent variable modeling of longitudinal and multilevel data. ric status among children. Developmental Psychology, 27, 867–880.
In A. Raftery (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 453–480). Cambridge, MA: Towbes, L.C., Cohen, L.H., & Glyshaw, K. (1989). Instrumentality as a life-
Blackwell. stress moderator for early versus middle adolescents. Journal of Personality and
Muthén, L., & Muthén, B.O. (1998–2006). Mplus Version 4.1. http://www. Social Psychology, 57, 109–119.
statmodel.com/index2.html Urberg, K.A., Değirmencioğlu, S.M., & Pilgrim,C. (1997). Close friend and
Muthén, L., & Muthén, B.O. (2001). Mplus short courses: Special topic in latent group influence on adolescent cigarette smoking and alcohol use. Develop-
variable modeling using Mplus. Course material. Santa Monica, CA, November mental Psychology, 33, 834–844.
5–8, 2001. Urberg, K.A., Değirmencioğlu, S.M., Tolson, J.M., & Halliday-Scher, K.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016


065-076 098014 Kiuru (Q8D):210 x 280mm 18/12/2008 14:17 Page 76

76 KIURU ET AL. / PEER GROUP HOMOGENEITY IN ADOLESCENTS’ SCHOOL ADJUSTMENT

(1995). The structure of adolescent peer networks. Developmental Psychology, Wentzel, K.R., Barry, C.M., & Caldwell, K.A. (2004). Friendships in middle
31, 540–547. school: Influences on motivation and school adjustment. Journal of
Urberg, K.A., Luo, Q., Pilgrim, C., & Değirmencioğlu, S.M. (2003). A two- Educational Psychology, 96, 195–203.
stage model of peer influence in adolescent substance use: Individual and Wentzel, K.R., & Caldwell, K. (1997). Friendships, peer acceptance, and group
relationship-specific differences in susceptibility to influence. Addictive membership: Relations to academic achievement in middle school. Child
Behavior, 28, 1243–1256. Development, 68, 1198–1209.
Wagner, B.M., & Compas, B.E. (1990). Gender, instrumentality, and expressiv- Werner, C., & Parmelee, P. (1979). Similarity of activity preferences among
ity: Moderators of the relation between stress and psychological symptoms friends: Those who play together stay together. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42,
during adolescence. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 383–406. 62–66.
Wentzel, K.R. (1993). Does being good make the grade? Social behavior and Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J.S. (1994). Children’s competence beliefs, achievement
academic competence in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, values, and general self- esteem: Change across elementary and middle
357–364. school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 107–138.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 18, 2016

You might also like