Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/308537867

Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors: A Multiple Discriminant


Modeling of Determinants

Article in Strategic Change · September 2016


DOI: 10.1002/jsc.2075

CITATIONS READS

16 1,793

2 authors:

Manit Mishra Sasmita Mishra


International Management Institute, Bhubaneswar C. V. Raman Global University
45 PUBLICATIONS 398 CITATIONS 4 PUBLICATIONS 16 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Manit Mishra on 15 September 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Strat. Change 25: 485–500 (2016)
Published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/jsc.2075 RESEARCH ARTICLE

Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors:


A Multiple Discriminant Modeling of Determinants
Manit Mishra
International Management Institute, Bhubaneswar, Odisha, India
Sasmita Mishra
Department of Business Management, CV Raman College of Engineering, Bhubaneswar,
Odisha, India

The risk tolerance of individual


investors is assessed by taking
A bove‐average risk tolerance is strongly associated with greater materialism,
younger age, and male gender, while a variety of characteristics comprising
materialism, age, gender, and ratio of earnings to total family members discriminate
into consideration the personality
trait of materialism and between the risk tolerance levels of individual investors.
socioeconomic particulars.

A multidimensional evaluation of
Researchers, particularly economists, have been fascinated with the construct of
investor profile based on
risk for a long time, resulting in its profound dissection. Game theorists have
materialism, age, and gender
would aid practitioners in precise thrived on the concepts of risk and return in different rational decision‐making
targeting of prospects. situations. In fact, John von Neumann and Oskar Morgenstern invented a theory
that measured how much an individual is desirous of a return, by the size of the
Segregation of investors into
above‐average and below‐average risk she is willing to take to get it (Binmore, 2008, p. 8). Cox and Rich (1964)
risk tolerance would facilitate identified the economic cost of acquisition as the most commonly discussed
customization of a product mix to element of risk. Later, Jacoby and Kaplan (1972) broadened the horizons of the
suit the financial temperament of definition of risk and suggested five independent types of risk: financial, perfor-
prospective investors. mance, physical, psychological, and social. The present study pertains to the
financial risk tolerance of an individual as indicated by their preference for dif-
ferent investment options – from the more risky ones to the less risky ones.
Grable (2000, p. 625) defined financial risk tolerance as ‘the maximum amount
of uncertainty that someone is willing to accept when making a financial deci-
sion.’ The construct of risk tolerance, or an individual’s attitude toward accepting
risk, has implications for financial service providers as well as consumers. For
the former, a proper assessment of a prospective investor’s financial risk tolerance
enables them to design a heterogeneous but appropriate product mix of invest-
ment options (Jacobs and Levy, 1996), while for the latter, it aids in offering
customized asset composition in a portfolio such that it is in accordance with
the risk and return expectations of the individual (Droms, 1987).
The present study offers a unique perspective on financial risk tolerance, on
account of two aspects. First, it has been emphasized that risk tolerance is a

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


Strategic Change: Briefings in Entrepreneurial Finance DOI: 10.1002/jsc.2075
486 Manit Mishra and Sasmita Mishra

complicated process, going beyond demographic and Review of the literature


socioeconomic factors (Grable and Lytton, 1999b), and
researchers have seldom forayed beyond the demographic Determinants of financial risk tolerance
determinants. This study includes the individual value of Demographic parameters such as gender, age, education,
materialism to investigate risk tolerance. It is pertinent income, occupation, etc. have been of particular interest
to mention here that both the constructs ‘financial risk to researchers as independent variables shaping investors’
tolerance’ and ‘materialism’ are dimensions of an indi- financial risk tolerance (see MacCrimmon and Wehrung,
vidual’s personality and both have a bearing on the deci- 1986; Riley and Chow, 1992). An individual’s ability to
sions related to the end use of disposable income. Second, tolerate risk has been found to be contingent on charac-
financial risk tolerance has mostly been investigated as teristics such as age, time horizon, liquidity needs, income,
an individual phenomenon. However, Indians take a investor knowledge, and attitude toward price fluctua-
chronologically long‐term view of their investment deci- tions (Fredman, 1996). Despite a plethora of research on
sions, and the principal objective of investment is to do the relationship of various socioeconomic factors with the
one’s duty (‘dharma’) of providing for progeny (Jain and risk tolerance of individual investors, a brief assessment of
Joy, 1996). The family has greater traction in influencing the results indicates a clear lack of consensus on the issue.
financial risk tolerance for oriental investors than occi- Grable and Lytton (1999b) carried out an exhaustive
dental investors. Hence, the constitution of the family as investigation to assess the role of demographic, socioeco-
a unit in terms of number of earning members vs. total nomic, and attitudinal factors as determinants in shaping
number of members should play an important role in financial risk tolerance. The authors concluded that the
the risk tolerance behavior of the earning members of two most effective risk tolerance differentiating factors are
the family. Accordingly, the ratio of earning members to education and financial knowledge, with an investor’s
total number of members in the family (E/T) for each education explaining the greatest amount of variance in
respondent has been included as an independent variable the risk tolerance. Contrary to popular notions, the study
differentiating between above‐average and below‐average found that gender, age, and marital status had a lesser
risk tolerance groups. It acts as a more appropriate sub- impact on the risk tolerance of an individual. At the same
stitute for marital status/number of dependents in the time, the study challenged the belief that the age of an
Indian context. This also broadens the horizons of risk investor is negatively related to the investor’s risk toler-
tolerance research by taking it beyond the normally used ance. On the contrary, it found that the mean age of the
socioeconomic determinants of age, gender, marital individuals in the above‐average risk category was greater
status, income, and occupation. compared with the mean age in the below‐average risk
The present study intended to fulfill two objectives: category.
(a) to determine whether a set of socioeconomic param- In the Indian context, Purkayastha (2008) studied the
eters and an individual consumption value can distinguish impact of demographic factors like age, occupation,
between levels of financial risk tolerance, as individual income, and number of dependents on risk tolerance. The
variables and as a weighted variate; and (b) to empirically author concluded that younger investors and those with
investigate the extent of the contribution of the individual a high income are more risk tolerant. However, in the
value of materialism and socioeconomic determinants second stage of the research, the author attempted to
toward separation of above‐average risk tolerance inves- determine the actual investment pattern of customers
tors from those having below‐average risk tolerance, as a having a specific demographic profile and risk tolerance
variate. level. Surprisingly, a reality check revealed that customers

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors 487

prefer to invest their money in an average‐risk mutual value of the human capital endowed. Therefore, individu-
fund, irrespective of their demographic profile and risk als tend to offset this decline in the value of their human
tolerance capability. capital by reducing the risk of their financial portfolio.
While it is not expected that there would be a con- Riley and Chow (1992) took research into the rela-
formance in the findings of research carried out in diverse tionship between risk tolerance and age to an altogether
geographic regions using dissimilar research methods, new level by arguing that the relationship may not neces-
nevertheless the disparity in findings is disconcerting. It sarily be linear. They found that risk tolerance increases
brings to the fore the lack of conclusive evidence, and with age until five years prior to retirement, and then
therefore unanimity, amongst researchers on the relation- decreases with age. This contention was supported by Hal-
ship between financial risk tolerance and socioeconomic lahan et al. (2004), who found evidence of a negative (but
variables. More importantly, it presents a reservoir of non‐linear) relationship between risk tolerance and age.
research opportunities to explore the unknown. The However, the inverse relationship between age and
remaining portion of this literature review elucidates the risk tolerance is debatable, since there are conclusions to
findings related to the impact of individual socioeconomic the contrary wherein researchers have either found no
determinants on financial risk tolerance. significant relationship or a positive relationship (e.g.,
Grable, 2000; Hanna et al., 1998; Hariharan et al., 2000).
Age Wang and Hanna (1997) carried out a profound study on
Investors belonging to different age groups are known to the association between risk tolerance and age. The risk
vary significantly with regard to their choice of invest- tolerance of an individual was measured by calculating the
ment. Young investors (26–35) have been found to prefer ratio of risky assets to total wealth, wherein an asset was
mutual funds, while middle‐aged investors (36–45) have considered risky if it provided an uncertain nominal cash
shown an inclination toward debentures/bonds as invest- flow (e.g., real estate, business assets, mutual funds, cor-
ment options (Mittal and Vyas, 2007). Wallach and porate stocks, precious metals, and pension assets in the
Kogan (1961) are believed to be pioneers in studying the form of stocks, bonds, or mutual funds). The authors
relationship between risk tolerance and age. Their early concluded that risk tolerance increases with age, since the
experimental research used choice dilemmas, which indi- present financial resources of young people are limited
cated that older individuals were less risk tolerant than and future wealth cannot be used to pay for present
younger individuals. This can be explained inductively by expenses, keeping other sociodemographic variables
the fact that older individuals have less time to recover constant.
losses from riskier investments than do younger individu- The varied findings indicate that age, all by itself, may
als, and as such, risk tolerance decreases with age. Harlow not be contributing to risk tolerance. A better understand-
and Brown (1990) added another supporting justification ing of the relationship is possible through concurrent
by suggesting that biological transformations due to the consideration of a number of socioeconomic variables and
process of ageing may also be responsible for making individual values, including age.
individuals less risk tolerant as they grow old. An inverse
relationship between risk tolerance and age is substanti- Gender
ated by the works of many researchers (Dahlback, 1991; In comparison with age, the association between risk tol-
Jagannathan and Kocherlakota, 1996). The findings tend erance and gender is comparatively much less debatable.
to build on the argument that, as an individual grows old, Grable and Lytton (1997) investigated the financial atti-
her stream of future income decreases, diminishing the tudes of adults and suggested that men have a greater

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
488 Manit Mishra and Sasmita Mishra

tolerance toward risk compared with their female compa- responsibility bestowed by marital status or number of
triots. Sung and Hanna (1996) concluded that house- dependents. As argued earlier, the authors believe this
holds headed by a female member are less risk tolerant in variable to be more representative of the pluralistic Indian
comparison with households headed by a male member culture, wherein the financial responsibilities of the bread-
or a married couple. An extensive study on the role of winner extend beyond the immediate family.
gender in financial risk tolerance, with specific reference
to students, was carried out by Garrison and Gutter Occupation
(2010). Using a stratified random sample of 15,797 stu- Occupation refers to the principal activity in which an
dents drawn from 15 universities across the USA, the individual engages for pay. McClelland’s theory of person-
authors concluded that female students tend to exhibit ality states that an individual’s choice of occupation
lower financial risk tolerance from the moment they are depends on whether they are motivated by achievement,
expected to manage their own finances. Hallahan et al. power, affiliation, or security. Individuals motivated by
(2004), in a study involving multiple demographic vari- achievement choose occupations with relatively higher
ables, found that women displayed a lower level of finan- economic and political risks (e.g., entrepreneurial ven-
cial risk tolerance compared with men. Overall, there tures). Therefore, self‐employed individuals are likely to
seems to be unanimity among researchers that men tend exhibit greater risk tolerance compared with salaried indi-
to exhibit greater financial risk tolerance compared with viduals (MacCrimmon and Wehrung, 1986). Masters
women, even though independence in taking financial (1989) found that non‐professionals (e.g., clerks, farmers,
decisions plays a role. laborers) are less risk tolerant compared with professionals
(e.g., educators, doctors, lawyers, businessmen, manag-
Marital status/number of dependents ers). Mittal and Vyas (2007) suggested that salaried people
Roszkowski et al. (1993) suggested that the number of prefer to invest their money in equities and mutual funds;
dependents is inversely proportional to risk tolerance, meanwhile self‐employed people were more inclined to
since individuals with greater responsibilities act with invest their money in riskier options (e.g., debentures and
more caution. In addition, individuals having a greater real estate).
number of dependents are also likely to be affected by the
potential social risk associated with undertaking greater Income
financial risk. This proposition stands supported by Malkiel (1996, p. 401) argues that, ‘The risks you can
Sunden and Surette (1998), who assessed risk tolerance afford to take depend on your total financial situation,
by choice of pension plan and concluded that marriage, including the types and sources of your income exclusive
and therefore an enhanced number of dependents, makes of investment income.’ A higher level of income should
both men and women less risk tolerant in their choices. encourage greater risk tolerance, because greater wealth
Daly and Wilson (2001) opined that the increased respon- ensures access to more resources for investment and also
sibilities accompanying marriage and children will make serves as a cushion against the vagaries of the financial
a man less tolerant of risk. Hallahan et al. (2004) too market. Bajlelsmit and VanDerhai (1997) analyzed inves-
found a significant negative relationship between risk tol- tors’ choice of pension plan (with varying degrees of risk)
erance and the marital status of individual investors. For to find that employees with a high income were willing
the purpose of this study, the ratio of earning members to take more risk compared with employees with a low
to total number of members in the respondent’s family income. Many other researchers (e.g., Bernheim et al.,
(E/T) has been used to reflect the enhanced/diminished 2001; Hallahan et al., 2004; Lee and Hanna, 1991; Riley

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors 489

and Chow, 1992; Schooley and Worden, 1996) have Materialism, as a value, has been widely researched (see
found a significant positive relationship between income Kamineni, 2005; Wang and Wallendorf, 2006).
and risk tolerance. This study draws its strength from the definition of
The present study investigated the simultaneous materialism as a value given by Richins and Dawson
impact of the socioeconomic determinants of age, gender, (1992), wherein they categorically suggest that the influ-
occupation, income, and E/T, along with the individual ence of materialism is aimed for as an end state of exis-
consumption value of materialism. tence and extends beyond the immediate goals of
consumption. The present authors posit that materialistic
Materialism individuals, in their quest for greater consumption and
The tendency toward materialism is an inherent constitu- possession, would warrant higher earnings and therefore
ent of the human condition (O’Shaughnessy and display greater financial risk tolerance.
O’Shaughnessy, 2002). Over the last two and a half
decades, researchers have exhibited tremendous interest in Materialism and risk tolerance
the construct of materialism (e.g., Belk, 1984; Burroughs Risk tolerance has often been studied in relation to socio-
and Rindfleisch, 2002; Richins and Dawson, 1992). economic characteristics. This study broadens the hori-
Belk (1987) defined materialism as ‘the tendency to zons of risk tolerance research by assessing the impact of
believe that consumer goods and services provide the the psychological construct of materialism, in addition to
greatest source of satisfaction and dissatisfaction in life.’ the effect of socioeconomic determinants.
Pollay (1986) defined materialism as ‘the belief that con- The definition of materialism as a value has enabled
sumption is the route to happiness, meaning, and the researchers to study the ramifications of materialistic ten-
solution to most personal problems.’ The contribution dencies and comprehend its causal relationship with other
that revolutionized the way we think about materialism is constructs and variables of importance to marketers. This
that by Richins and Dawson (1992), who conceptualized study investigates the relationship between materialism
materialism as a value. Rokeach (1973) has suggested that and financial risk tolerance. Materialism, for the purpose
a value is a centrally held, enduring belief which guides of this research, has been treated as an individual value
actions and judgments across specific situations and whose impact extends to the domain of an individual
beyond immediate goals to more ultimate end states of investor’s risk tolerance.
existence. Richins and Dawson (1992) defined material- Materialists give prominence to possessions and the
ism as ‘a value that guides people’s choices and conduct acquisition of objects in life. This desire to acquire is
in a variety of situations but not limited to consumption expected to cajole individuals toward investments with
arenas.’ The construct is based on three ‘orienting values’ – greater reward, notwithstanding the risk, thereby enhanc-
acquisition centrality, acquisition as the pursuit of hap- ing their risk tolerance. Materialists believe that posses-
piness, and possession‐defined success. Materialistic sions and their consumption are essential to their
individuals, with position acquisition at the center of their satisfaction (Belk, 1984). It is this belief which not only
lives, believe that acquisition provides happiness, and influences the ‘type and quantity of goods purchased’ to
define their own success as well as the success of others attain an end state of existence, but also influences the
on the basis of their possessions. An analysis of material- ‘allocation of variety of resources, including time’ to
ism as a value leads one to conclude that it is: (1) a cen- achieve an ultimate end state of existence beyond immedi-
trally held and enduring belief; (2) aimed for as an end ate goals (Richins and Dawson, 1992). This research
state of existence; (3) influential beyond immediate goals. determines whether the desire to attain an affluent end

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
490 Manit Mishra and Sasmita Mishra

state of existence elevates the materialistic individual However, as the literature review suggests, there is a lack
investor to a position of greater financial risk tolerance. of consensus on the direction of impact for most demo-
This premise draws sustenance from the findings of graphic determinants. The justification for assessing the
several researchers who have investigated the relationship effect of socioeconomic variables on financial risk toler-
between materialism and financial decision‐making. ance is due to Samuelson (1969). He listed the four most
Richins (2011) suggested that materialism simultaneously common reasons why ‘a young businessman can take
leads to a more favorable attitude toward debt and a stron- more risk in the financial market than an old widow’ as:
ger belief that life transformations will occur as a result of (1) the businessman is more affluent than the widow;
acquisitions, and these two forces work together to increase (2) the businessman expects higher earnings in the future;
credit overuse. Watson (2003) investigated the relation- (3) the businessman can recover any current losses in the
ship between materialism and spending tendencies, saving, future; and (4) the businessman has a much longer invest-
and debt. His findings showed a negative relationship ment horizon compared with the old widow. In other
between materialism and saving, such that highly materi- words, the businessman has greater risk tolerance by virtue
alistic people are more likely to be spenders whereas people of his socioeconomic characteristics. At the same time, the
with low levels of materialism are more likely to save variables are largely intertwined in their effect on risk
through financial investments such as stocks and mutual tolerance, rather than being isolated. Morin and Suarez
funds. His findings also suggest a strong inclination on the (1983) investigated risk tolerance through an assessment
part of highly materialistic people toward borrowing of investment in risky assets such as stocks, bonds, mutual
money for non‐essential purposes. It establishes the pro- funds, real estate (other than owner‐occupied home),
clivity of materialistic individuals toward earning more equity in own business, and loans. Their findings integrate
money. More materialistic consumers have been found to the impact of age and net worth (reflecting financial sta-
be willing to carry heavier debt loads (Ponchio and Aranha, bility) on risk tolerance. They suggest that for those having
2008), and exhibit a more positive attitude toward bor- low net worth, risk tolerance decreases with age whereas
rowing (Watson, 1998). Tang et al. (2008) measured stu- for high net worth, risk tolerance increases with age.
dents’ behavioral intentions, to find that a love of money Another study that integrates the impact of socioeconomic
was significantly correlated with risk tolerance. Further, determinants is by McInish et al. (1993), who concluded
investor risk tolerance is considered to be a psychological that the relationship between net worth and risk tolerance
determinant of financial behavior (Jacobs‐Lawson and was statistically significant for individuals aged 35 and
Hershey, 2005), and therefore is likely to be influenced by above, but not for those below 35 years of age.
a hedonic value like materialism. Even as there is evidence to indicate the impact of
The present study proposes that individuals with low materialism on financial decision‐making, there is a dearth
levels of materialism would have lower financial risk toler- of research on the impact of materialistic tendencies spe-
ance, whereas highly materialistic individuals have a cifically on financial risk tolerance. An exception in this
strong disposition toward money and hence exhibit greater regard is the work of Richins (2011), who argued that a
financial risk tolerance. strong desire for wealth, goods, and life transformations
on the part of materialistic individuals has resulted in
financially risky behavior.
Research summary This study carries forward the research through
There has been plenty of research on the impact of investigation of the contentious issue of the degree and
socioeconomic determinants on financial risk tolerance. direction of the simultaneous impact of socioeconomic

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors 491

determinants and individual consumption values on analyst influence. Therefore, for this research the authors
financial risk tolerance. used a scale which could capture the multidimensional
nature of the financial risk tolerance construct, with estab-
lished reliability and validity estimations.
Methodology Grable and Lytton (1999a) operationalized financial
risk tolerance on the basis of the three dimensions of
Data investment risk, risk comfort and experience, and specula-
The data for the study was generated from employees tive risk. The scale consists of 13 items and satisfies the
working in a higher education institute of some repute in reliability and validity criteria (Grable and Lytton, 2001).
the city of Bhubaneswar, Odisha, India during the period For the purpose of the present study, the Grable and
October to December 2014. The employees included in the Lytton (1999a) scale is used to measure financial risk
sample were randomly chosen from a listing of all faculty tolerance. Materialism and five other socioeconomic vari-
and staff. One half of all employees (N = 600) received the ables were considered as independent variables for the
questionnaire, on the basis of a random selection. study. The scale developed by Richins and Dawson (1992)
The survey instrument used was a self‐administered is more acceptable and applicable to consumers in varied
questionnaire comprising 37 questions divided across cultures owing to the sheer diversity and depth of respon-
three sections. The first section sought demographic infor- dent profiles. Therefore, for the present study, the mate-
mation on the variables of age, gender, marital status, rialism of individual investors has been measured using
occupation, number of earning and total family members, the Richins and Dawson (1992) scale. Responses were also
and income. The second section required a response on sought on the five socioeconomic variables of age, gender,
risk tolerance using the 13‐item Grable and Lytton ratio of earning members to total number of members in
(1999a) scale, whereas the third section assessed the mate- the family (E/T), occupation, and family income.
rialistic tendencies of respondents through the 18‐item
Richins and Dawson (1992) scale of materialistic values. Statistical analysis
The response rate for the survey was 95.3 percent, with Financial risk tolerance cannot be treated as a continuum
286 questionnaires returned. However, six questionnaires on which individuals can be assigned values (Grable and
were unusable due to missing values. Therefore, this Lytton, 1998). This study was based on the premise that
resulted in 280 valid responses for this analysis, or a usable financial risk tolerance can be treated as high (above
response rate of 93.3 percent (n = 280). average) or low (below average). This approach has been
used by several researchers for studying financial risk toler-
Measurement ance (Grable and Lytton, 1999b; MacCrimmon and
The construct ‘risk tolerance’ is the dependent variable in Wehrung, 1986; Roszkowski et al., 1993). On the basis
the study. Roszkowski et al. (1993, p. 230) noted that of this assumption, multiple discriminant analysis was
when it comes to measurement of risk tolerance, most of used to classify respondents into risk tolerance categories
the instruments are operationalized by various financial using respondents’ materialistic values and socioeconomic
planning organizations for in‐house use. This limits their characteristics. The study demanded a multivariate analy-
applicability in a wide variety of situations. Further, Mac- sis so that multicollinearity can be taken care of while
Crimmon and Wehrung (1986, p. 65) suggest the use of measuring the substantive effect. The assessment of the
a questionnaire over any other method of measuring risk impact of materialism on risk tolerance is an important
tolerance, since it ensures a response free from decision aspect of the study, and materialism is known to be influenced

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
492 Manit Mishra and Sasmita Mishra

by socioeconomic variables – e.g., age (Belk, 1985) and


Table 1. Variable definitions
gender (Kamineni, 2005) – which are also part of the
study. Multiple discriminant analysis as the research Variable Measurement
method has an advantage over multiple regression analysis Risk tolerance 1 = less than or equal to 29
since it can be used for a non‐metric dependent variable, (median value)
2 = greater than 29
in this case financial risk tolerance, and metric indepen- Materialism Respondent’s score on MVS (Richins
dent variables. In other words, it’s a mirror image of the and Dawson, 1992)
Gender 0 = male
method of MANOVA (Hair et al., 2006, p. 300). 1 = female
­Multiple discriminant analysis has a heritage of previ- Age Respondent’s age (21–50)
Marital status 0 = single
ous use in financial risk tolerance research (Grable and 1 = married
Lytton, 1999b). Occupation 0 = staff
1 = faculty
Family income Respondent’s family income per
month in Indian currency
E/T Ratio of number of earning members
Analysis and discussion to total number of members in the
family
Demographic characteristics of the sample
The sample size exceeds the suggested minimum 5:1 ratio
of observations to independent variables and the Materialism
minimum group size of 20 observations (Hair et al., The materialism index scores ranged from a low of 21 to
2006, p. 337). The sample of 280 respondents comprised a high of 77, with a mean of 53.53, a standard deviation
100 women and 180 men. A total of 176 respondents of 10.175, and a median of 54. Respondents’ scores on a
were married and the rest (104) unmarried. The respon- materialism value scale (MVS) were used to generate the
dents who were employed as faculty numbered 172, discriminant function.
whereas 108 were engaged in administrative occupations
but not teaching. The age of respondents ranged from a Stepwise multiple discriminant analysis results
low of 21 to a high of 51. The mean age of respondents The mean and standard deviations of each independent
was 32, with a standard deviation of approximately variable were calculated for the two levels of risk tolerance:
7 years. The overall ratio of earning to total members in above average and below average. The data in Table 2
the family (E/L) was 0.505, with a standard deviation of gives the different values of each independent variable for
0.225. The average monthly family income for the the two groups, and the statistical significance of the dif-
respondents was 67,500 INR. ference. There is a significant difference in the materialism
scores of the two groups, such that respondents having
Risk tolerance greater materialistic tendencies exhibit above‐average risk
The risk tolerance score for the respondents had a low of tolerance in their financial decision‐making (p < 0.05).
17 and a high of 40. The mean value was 29, with a The difference between the above‐average and below‐
standard deviation of 4.812 and a median value of 29. average risk tolerance groups for the variables of age and
The construct risk tolerance was measured dichotomously, gender are marginally significant, at the 5 percent level.
wherein respondents obtaining a score up to the median However, for the variables of occupation, ratio of earning
value of 29 were categorized as 1 and those securing more members to total members in a family (E/T), and family
than 29 were categorized as 2 (see Table 1). income, the analysis could not find the difference to be

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors 493

Table 2. Group means and standard deviations of classifying variables

Variable Below‐average risk tolerance Above‐average risk tolerance Wilks’ lambda F Sig.

Mean SD Mean SD

Materialism 51.2750 9.72174 56.5333 10.14289 0.934 4.833 0.031


Age 33.1500 8.19490 29.8333 5.60224 0.949 3.633 0.061
Gender 0.4500 0.50383 0.2333 0.43018 0.950 3.585 0.063
Occupation 0.6750 0.47434 0.5333 0.50742 0.979 1.440 0.234
E/T 0.4862 0.18369 0.5303 0.27340 0.991 0.650 0.423
Family income 71634 41176.9 62033 40186.9 0.986 0.951 0.333

statistically significant. Gender being a dichotomous vari- ratio of earning to total members in the family (E/T), and
able (0 for males and 1 for females), the mean value family income failed to discriminate in a statistically sig-
indicates the proportion of cases with a value of 1 (i.e., nificant manner between the levels of risk tolerance. The
number of females). Therefore, while the above‐average findings are corroborated by Wilks’ lambda figures for
risk tolerance group comprises 23.33 percent women and each of the independent variables.
76.67 percent men, the below‐average risk tolerance Wilks’ lambda is defined as the ratio of the within‐
group consists of 45 percent women and 55 percent men. group sum of squares to the total sum of squares. A small
Similarly, faculty members comprise 53.33 percent of the value of lambda indicates a large difference between
above‐average risk tolerance group and 67.5 percent of group means. The smallest Wilks’ lambda value is that for
the below‐average risk tolerance group. The average age materialism, thereby suggesting its univariate ability to
for the above‐average risk tolerance group is approxi- discriminate between the two levels of risk tolerance.
mately 30 years, whereas for the below‐average risk Thus, as given in Table 2, the mean responses for mate-
tolerance group the average age is approximately
­ rialism, gender, and age were the most different for above‐
33 years. Interestingly, the average family income of the average and below‐average risk tolerance groups.
above‐average risk tolerance group (62,022 INR p.m.) is Meanwhile, the variables of occupation, E/T, and family
less than the average family income of the below‐average income – when considered in terms of their isolated
risk tolerance group (71,634 INR p.m.). The ratio of impact – failed to discriminate significantly between the
earning members to total members is higher in the above‐ two groups of risk tolerance. This analysis is not free from
average risk tolerance group (0.5303) in comparison with the impact of multicollinearity among independent vari-
the below‐average risk tolerance group (0.4862). The sta- ables. In order to determine the discriminant variate that
tistical significance of the independent variables in dif- will discriminate best between the above‐average and
ferentiating between the two levels of risk tolerance was below‐average risk tolerance groups, the appropriate
determined using one‐way ANOVA. The univariate F and linear combination of independent variables under inves-
Wilks’ lambda values represent the separate or univariate tigation needs to be identified. Toward this objective,
effect of each variable, not considering the multicollinear- multiple discriminant analysis has been used in the
ity among the independent variables. The F‐statistic present study.
results given in Table 2 indicate that only materialism, Prior to conducting discriminant analysis on the data,
gender, and age could successfully discriminate between it was tested for meeting the assumption of equal variance–
the two levels of risk tolerance. In contrast, occupation, covariance matrices across groups. Box’s M test was used

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
494 Manit Mishra and Sasmita Mishra

to assess the similarity of the dispersion matrices of the by the relative size of the absolute value of the coefficient.
independent variables among the groups. The analysis The more important variables have larger coefficients.
failed to reject the null hypothesis that the dispersion Hair et al. (2006, p. 331) suggest that variables exhibiting
matrices are homogeneous (Box’s M = 16.9, approximate an absolute loading of 0.40 or higher may be considered
F = 1.6, p = 0.106), thereby meeting the stringent condi- substantive. The variable that shared most variation with
tion for being subject to discriminant analysis. A stepwise‐ the canonical discriminant function was materialism
discriminant analysis was considered appropriate to meet (coefficient of 0.513), followed by age (coefficient of
the objectives of the research, since the purpose of this 0.445) and gender (coefficient of 0.442). Occupation,
research was to identify the variables which can success- ratio of earning members to total members in the family
fully differentiate between the above‐average and below‐ (E/T), and family income possess comparatively less dif-
average risk tolerance groups. ferentiating power between different levels of risk toler-
The second stage of analysis comprised an evaluation ance. Hence, the loadings tend to suggest that materialism
of the discriminant loadings or structure correlations has maximum power to discriminate between above‐
given in Table 3. The authors do not emphasize the dis- average and below‐average risk tolerance groups, followed
criminant weights and the resulting discriminant func- by age and gender, respectively.
tion equation, since the objective is to identify the The stepwise multiple discriminant analysis included
independent variables that have a strong relationship with the variables of materialism, age, gender, and E/T in the
group membership in the categories of the dependent discriminant function. The discriminant function is highly
variable of risk tolerance rather than building a predictive significant (p = 0.003) and displays a canonical correlation
model. The discriminant loadings measure the simple of 0.461 (p < 0.01). The canonical correlation value sug-
linear correlation between each independent variable and gests that approximately 21 percent (0.4612) of the vari-
the discriminant function, thereby reflecting the variance ance in risk tolerance is accounted for by this model,
that a particular independent variable shares with the which includes four independent variables. A high canon-
discriminant function. Thus, these loadings indicate the ical correlation indicates a function that discriminates well
discriminating power of each independent variable, between different groups. This finding stands substanti-
taking into consideration the interaction between and ated by Wilks’ lambda value. The value of Wilks’ lambda
among the independent variables. The importance of for this discriminant function was 0.787 (p < 0.01). Thus,
the variables in the discriminant function is indicated the discriminant function explained 21.3 percent of the
variance in the financial risk tolerance scores within the
sample. The stepwise discriminant analysis was able to
Table 3. Pooled within‐group correlations between independent
variables and the standardized canonical discriminant identify a set of variables – namely materialism, age,
function gender, and E/T – that can provide a rather succinct and
Variable Risk tolerance coefficient powerful distinction between the above‐average and
below‐average risk tolerance groups. The Wilks’ lambda
Materialism 0.513
Age 0.445
and univariate F‐statistic values given in Table 2 suggest
Gender 0.442 the ability of materialism, age, and gender to discriminate
Occupation* 0.294 among the groups, but only separately. However, the
E/T 0.188
Family income* 0.103 parameters depicting the overall model fit in Table 4
*
Variable not used in the final discriminant function. suggest a discriminant function comprising materialism,
age, gender, and E/T that takes into consideration

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors 495

Table 4. Overall model fit – canonical discriminant functions

Eigenvalue Canonical correlation Wilks’ lambda Chi‐square Sig.

0.270 0.461 0.787 15.778 0.003

multicollinearity among the various independent variables average risk tolerance categories. As shown in Table 5,
and simultaneously maximizes the between‐group vari- out of 120 respondents in the above‐average risk tolerance
ance relative to the within‐group variance. category, the model was able to correctly classify 84
This is reflected in the inclusion of E/T in the dis- (i.e., 70 percent). Similarly, out of 160 respondents in the
criminant function. The variable E/T did not have a sig- below‐average risk tolerance category, 116 were correctly
nificant discriminating effect when considered separately, classified (i.e., 72.5 percent). Overall, the discrimi-
but had enough unique variance to be part of the discrimi- nant function – including the independent variables of
nant function eventually. The analysis suggests that col- materialism, gender, age, and ratio of earning to total
lectively, the combination of variables materialism, age, members in the family – correctly classified 200 out of
gender, and E/T has a statistically significant strong rela- 280 respondents. This pertains to 71.4 percent accuracy
tionship with the group membership of respondents in on the part of the derived model (p < 0.05).
the above‐average and below‐average risk tolerance groups.
Thus, the discriminant variate that successfully isolates the
above‐average risk tolerance groups from the below‐ Conclusion
average risk tolerance groups comprises materialism, age, A concurrent assessment of the means for the independent
gender, and E/T. variables included in the discriminant function (given in
Table 2) and their respective discriminant loadings (given
Predicting financial risk tolerance in Table 3) was done to determine the pattern of relation-
The overall model is statistically significant and explains ships between the risk tolerance groups and the indepen-
21 percent of the variation between the above‐average and dent variables of materialism, age, gender, and E/T. The
below‐average risk tolerance groups. Therefore, an assess- variable which contributed most to explaining the differ-
ment of the predictive accuracy was made. The discrimi- ence between levels of risk tolerance is materialism. The
nant score obtained for each respondent was used to analysis suggests that respondents high on materialism
classify each respondent into above‐average or below‐ tend to exhibit above‐average risk tolerance. This is a rev-

Table 5. Classification results

Risk tolerance Actual group Predicted group

Above average Below average

Number Percentage Number Percentage

Above average 30 21 70.0 9 30.0


Below average 40 11 27.5 29 72.5

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
496 Manit Mishra and Sasmita Mishra

elation. Even though the impact of materialism on various Wilks’ lambda and canonical correlation led to the con-
financial decisions (e.g., savings and investments) has been clusion that the linear combination of variables worked
investigated in the past, the specific effect of materialism to separate the two levels of risk tolerance from each other.
on financial risk tolerance – which subsequently influences On the whole, materialism emerged as the best dis-
saving and investment decisions – has never been explored. criminating factor between above‐average and below‐
With regard to the independent variable of age, the con- average risk tolerance. The association of materialism with
ventional wisdom that risk tolerance reduces with increas- various socioeconomic (e.g., age, gender, and E/T) vari-
ing age, withstands our scrutiny. A greater number of men ables is well established. Materialism has also been studied
in the sample demonstrated above‐average risk tolerance, extensively in the consumer behavior literature in terms
whereas a greater number of women belonged to the of its impact as an independent variable on various con-
below‐average risk tolerance group. Therefore, our find- structs (e.g., innovativeness, attitude toward advertising).
ings regarding the impact of gender on financial risk toler- This study broadens the horizons of research on material-
ance tend to agree with past research, wherein men have ism by discovering its hitherto unexplored relationship
shown greater financial risk tolerance compared with with the construct of financial risk tolerance.
women. The inclusion of the independent variable of E/T In addition to establishing the relationship of materi-
was meant to cater specifically for the Indian culture. An alism with risk tolerance, another significant finding of
earning member in India not only has responsibility this study is the role of family responsibility in influencing
toward their immediate family – comprising their spouse risk tolerance levels, in collaboration with other variables.
and children – but also toward their parents and even the The ratio E/T is not able to influence financial risk toler-
extended family, comprising relatives. Therefore, this vari- ance as a separate variable. However, in association with
able is likely to have a greater impact on risk tolerance materialism, age, and gender, it determines the level of
compared with any other independent variable represent- risk tolerance as part of a variate.
ing family burden (e.g., marital status). Independently, Thus, the seminal findings of this study are: (a) the
though, E/T did not have a statistically significant contri- discriminant function comprising materialism, age, gender,
bution to risk tolerance. and ratio of earning to total members in a family (E/T) as
Overall, the analysis discovered a discriminant func- a variate discriminates significantly the above‐average level
tion comprising materialism, age, gender, and E/T, which of financial risk tolerance from the below‐average level of
collectively act as a good discriminator of risk tolerance financial risk tolerance; (b) the above‐average financial risk
levels. The discriminant function accounted for approxi- tolerance group demonstrates significantly greater material-
mately 21 percent of the variance between risk tolerance istic tendencies compared with the below‐average risk toler-
groups. In other words, financial risk tolerance, as a con- ance group; and (c) the ratio of earning to total members
struct, could best be described by a linear combination of in the family (E/T) explains enough unique variance
respondents’ materialistic tendencies, age, gender, and between risk tolerance groups so as to discriminate between
ratio of earning to total members in the family. Therefore, above‐average and below‐average financial risk tolerance
it was concluded that above‐average risk tolerance is asso- groups, in association with materialism, age, and gender.
ciated with a higher level of materialism, lower age, male
gender, and higher ratio of earning to total members in
the family. Occupation and family income explained a Implications
comparatively smaller percentage of the variation in finan- Financial service providers, financial investment planners,
cial risk tolerance. The analysis based on the values of and other practitioners have to assess individual financial

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors 497

risk tolerance. Mostly, these practitioners depend on uni- investor. The identification of materialism as a key variable
dimensional evaluations, objective heuristics, and intui- discriminating between risk tolerance levels has key impli-
tive subjective judgments to understand financial beliefs, cations for practitioners. The assessment of this personal-
pecuniary needs, and hedonic aspirations that shape ity trait at the level of an individual investor may be
financial risk tolerance. Such a piecemeal approach does difficult, due to its psychological nature compared with
not do justice to the complicated psychological construct inferring some of the socioeconomic attributes of a pro-
of financial risk tolerance. This article broadens the hori- spective investor. This, however, does not in any way
zons of assessment of risk tolerance by taking into consid- diminish the importance of the finding that materialistic
eration the personality trait of materialism as well as tendencies make individuals more risk tolerant. Societies
offering a combination of variables (including psychologi- across the world are gradually moving toward greater
cal and socioeconomic particulars) that effectively segre- materialism. Financial planners and other practitioners
gate investors into above‐average and below‐average risk can assess the materialistic values of the target market as
tolerance. An appropriate assessment would enable prac- a whole and thereafter, deduce the financial risk tolerance.
titioners to customize their product mix to suit the finan- This article makes an important contribution to the inves-
cial temperament of investors, while simultaneously tor behavior literature by taking into account both subjec-
fulfilling their financial objectives to an optimum extent. tive and objective characteristics while assessing a person’s
The variate obtained in the present study would enable a financial risk tolerance. It is an improvement on the
marketer to correctly identify the financial risk tolerance assessment of risk tolerance based on investor profiling
based on materialism, age, gender, and E/T ratio using individual socioeconomic variables separately. A
71.4 percent of the time. The high degree of accuracy linear combination of materialism, age, gender, and E/T
should provide confidence in the development of a strat- offers optimum discrimination between above‐average
egy based on these results. The study identified isolated and below‐average financial risk tolerance groups. Inves-
variables and a variate as effective risk tolerance differen- tor profiling on the basis of the variate derived would
tiating factors. The findings reinforce the notion that the provide an improved assessment of risk tolerance and,
age and gender of respondents significantly influence their thereby, contribute toward ameliorated financial well‐
financial risk tolerance perceptions. Men tend to be more being of individuals and families. Notwithstanding the
tolerant toward risk compared with women, and risk tol- statistical and practical significance of the findings in the
erance reduces with increased age. Practitioners, therefore, present study, the fact that the variate obtained explains a
can continue to rely on age and gender to classify pros- little more than 21 percent of the variance between the
pects into above‐average or below‐average risk tolerance risk tolerance levels should encourage researchers to
groups. At the same time, practitioners must note that age examine other psychological, psychographic, and demo-
and gender, when combined with materialistic tendencies graphic variables that could possibly add to the variance
and E/T ratio, influence an individual’s risk tolerance. A accounted for. A greater understanding of the financial
young materialistic male with fewer mouths to feed would risk tolerance construct and the variables shaping it would
exhibit substantially greater financial risk tolerance com- enable superior personal financial management by practi-
pared with an aged spiritual female with great fiscal tioners, researchers, and investors.
responsibility toward the family. These are two cases at
opposite ends of the spectrum, but nevertheless they aid References
in the portrayal and subsequent discrimination of a Bajlelsmit VL, VanDerhei JL. 1997. Risk aversion and
financially risk‐tolerant investor from a non‐risk‐tolerant pension investment choices. In Gordon MS, Mitchell OS,

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
498 Manit Mishra and Sasmita Mishra

Twinney MM (eds), Positioning Pensions for the Twenty‐First Grable JE, Lytton RH. 1997. Determinants of retirement
Century. University of Pennsylvania Press: Pennsylvania, PA; savings plan participation: A discriminant analysis. Personal
pp. 45–66. Finances and Worker Productivity 1(1): 184–189.
Belk RW. 1984. Three scales to measure constructs related to Grable JE, Lytton RH. 1998. Investor risk tolerance: Testing
materialism: Reliability, validity, and relationships to measure the efficacy of demographics as differentiating and classifying
of happiness. In Kinnear TF, Arbor A (eds), Advances in Con- factors. Financial Counseling and Planning 9(1): 61–74.
sumer Research. Association of Consumer Research: Duluth, MN;
Grable JE, Lytton RH. 1999a. Financial risk tolerance: Results
pp. 291–297.
of a concurrent validity analysis. Proceedings of the Association
Belk RW. 1985. Materialism: Trait aspects of living in a material for Financial Counseling and Planning Education: 65–75.
world. Journal of Consumer Research 12(Dec): 265–280.
Grable JE, Lytton RH. 1999b. Financial risk tolerance revisited:
Belk RW. 1987. Material values in the comics: A content analysis The development of a risk assessment instrument. Financial
of comic books featuring themes of wealth. Journal of Consumer Services Review 8: 163–181.
Research 14(Jun): 26–42.
Grable JE, Lytton RH. 2001. Assessing the concurrent validity
Bernheim BD, Skinner J, Weinberg S. 2001. What accounts for of the SCF risk tolerance question. Financial Counseling and
the variation in retirement wealth among U.S. households? Planning 12(2): 43–53.
American Economic Review 91: 832–857.
Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE, Tatham RL. 2006.
Binmore K. 2008. Game Theory: A very short introduction. Multivariate Data Analysis. Pearson Education: New Delhi.
Oxford University Press: Oxford.
Hallahan TA, Faff RW, McKenzie MD. 2004. An empirical
Burroughs JE, Rindfleish A. 2002. Materialism and well‐being: investigation of personal financial risk tolerance. Financial
A conflicting values perspective. Journal of Consumer Research Services Review 13: 57–78.
29(Dec): 348–370.
Hanna S, Gutter M, Fan J. 1998. A theory based measure of
Cox DF, Rich SU. 1964. Perceived risk and consumer decision‐ risk tolerance. Working Paper, Ohio State University, Depart-
making: The case of telephone shopping. Journal of Marketing ment of Consumer and Textile Sciences, Columbus, OH.
Research 1(4): 32–39.
Hariharan G, Chapman KS, Domian DL. 2000. Risk tolerance
Dahlback O. 1991. Saving and risk taking. Journal of Economic and asset allocation for investors nearing retirement. Financial
Psychology 12: 479–500. Services Review 9: 159–170.
Daly M, Wilson M. 2001. Risk‐taking, intrasexual competition, Harlow WV, Brown KC. 1990. Understanding and assessing
and homicide. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 47. financial risk tolerance: A biological perspective. Financial
University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, NE; pp. 1–36. Analysts Journal 46: 50–62.
Droms WG. 1987. Investment asset allocation for PFP clients. Jacobs BI, Levy KN. 1996. Residual risk: How much is too
Journal of Accountancy 163: 114–118. much? Journal of Portfolio Management 22: 10–16.
Fredman AJ. 1996. Wrestling with risk: A multiheaded concept Jacobs‐Lawson JM, Hershey DA. 2005. Influence of future time
with no single measure. Associates of America Individual Inves- perspective, financial knowledge, and financial risk tolerance
tors: 25–30. on retirement saving behaviors. Financial Services Review
14: 331–344.
Garrison ST, Gutter MS. 2010. Gender differences in financial
socialization and willingness to take financial risks. Journal of Jacoby J, Kaplan L. 1972. The competent of perceived risk.
Financial Counseling and Planning: 21(2). Proceedings of 3rd Annual Conference of the Association for
Consumer Research, Chicago, IL, pp. 382–393.
Grable JE. 2000. Financial risk tolerance and additional factors
that affect risk taking in everyday money matters. Journal of Jagannathan R, Kocherlakota NR. 1996. Why should older
Business and Psychology 14: 625–630. people invest less in stocks than younger people? Federal

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
Financial Risk Tolerance among Indian Investors 499

Reserve Bank of Minneapolis Quarterly Review 20(3): Richins ML. 2011. Materialism, transformation expectations,
11–23. and spending: Implications for credit use. Journal of Public
Policy & Marketing: 1–59.
Jain AK, Joy A. 1996. Does money matter? A study of savings
and investment patterns among professional South Asians Richins ML, Dawson S. 1992. A consumer values orientation
in Canada. Asia Pacific Advances in Consumer Research for materialism and its measurement: Scale development and
2: 36–37. validation. Journal of Consumer Research 19: 303–316.

Kamineni R. 2005. Influence of materialism, gender and Riley WB, Chow KV. 1992. Asset allocation and individual risk
nationality on consumer brand perceptions. Journal of Tar- aversion. Financial Analysts Journal 48: 32–37.
geting, Measurement and Analysis for Marketing 14(1): Rokeach M. 1973. The Nature of Human Values. Free Press:
25–32. New York.
Lee HK, Hanna S. 1991. Wealth and stock ownership. Proceed- Roszkowski MJ, Snelbecker GE, Leimberg SR. 1993. Risk toler-
ings of the Association for Financial Counseling and Planning ance and risk aversion. In Leimberg SR, Satinsky MJ, LeClair RT,
Education: 126–140. Doyle Jr RJ (eds), The Tools and Techniques of Financial Planning,
MacCrimmon KR, Wehrung DA. 1986. Taking Risks. Free 4th edn. National Underwriter: Cincinnati, OH; pp. 213–226.
Press: New York. Samuelson P. 1969. Lifetime portfolio selection by dynamic stochas-
Malkiel BG. 1996. A Random Walk Down Wall Street. tic programming. Review of Economics and Statistics 51: 239–246.
W.W. Norton: New York. Schooley DK, Worden DD. 1996. Risk aversion measures:
Masters R. 1989. Study examines investors’ risk‐taking propen- Comparing attitudes and asset allocation. Financial Service
sities. Journal of Financial Planning 36: 151–155. Review 5(2): 87–99.

McInish TH, Ramaswami SN, Srivastava RK. 1993. Do more Sunden AE, Surette BJ. 1998. Gender differences in the alloca-
risk‐averse investors have lower net worth and income? The tion of assets in retirement savings plans. American Economic
Financial Review 28(1): 91–106. Review 88(2): 207–211.

Mittal M, Vyas RK. 2007. Demographics and investment Sung J, Hanna S. 1996. Factors related to risk tolerance. Finan-
choice among Indian investors. The IUP Journal of Behavioral cial Counseling and Planning 7: 11–20.
Finance IV(4): 51–62. Tang TLP, Chen YJ, Sutarso T. 2008. Bad apples in bad (business)
Morin RA, Suarez AF. 1983. Risk aversion revisited. Journal of barrels: The love of money, Machiavellianism, risk tolerance, and
Finance 38: 1201–1216. unethical behavior. Management Decision 46(2): 243–263.

O’Shaughnessy J, O’Shaughnessy NJ. 2002. Marketing, the Wallach MA, Kogan N. 1961. Aspects of judgment and decision
consumer society and hedonism. European Journal of Market- making: Interrelationships and changes with age. Behavioral
ing 36(5/6): 524–547. Science 6: 23–26.
Wang H, Hanna S. 1997. Does risk tolerance decrease with age?
Pollay RW. 1986. The distorted mirror: Reflections on the unin-
Financial Counseling and Planning 8(2): 27–31.
tended consequences of advertising. Journal of Marketing
50(Apr): 18–36. Wang J, Wallendorf M. 2006. Materialism, status signaling, and
product satisfaction. Journal of the Academy of Marketing
Ponchio MC, Aranha F. 2008. Materialism as a predictor vari-
Science 34(4): 494–505.
able of low income consumer behavior when entering into
instalment plan agreements. Journal of Consumer Behavior Watson JJ. 1998. Materialism and debt: A study of current attitudes
7(Jan/Feb): 21–34. and behaviors. Advances in Consumer Research 25: 203–207.

Purkayastha S. 2008. Investor profiling and investment plan- Watson JJ. 2003. The relationship of materialism to spending
ning: An empirical study. The IUP Journal of Management tendencies, saving, and debt. Journal of Economic Psychology
Research VII(12): 17–40. 24: 723–739.

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc
500 Manit Mishra and Sasmita Mishra

BIOGRAPHICAL NOTES

Manit Mishra is Associate Professor of Marketing Sasmita Mishra is Assistant Professor of Finance
and Quantitative Techniques at the International and Accounting in the Department of Business
Management Institute, Bhubaneswar, Odisha, India. Management, CV Raman College of Engineering,
His teaching expertise includes marketing research, Bhubaneswar, Odisha, India. Her teaching expertise
business analytics, and consumer behavior. His areas includes financial accounting and financial services.
of research interest are hedonic consumption She is currently pursuing a Ph.D. in the area of
behavior, statistical modeling, marketing instrument behavioral finance at Centurion University of
validation, and multivariate consumer behavior Technology and Management, India.
analysis.

Correspondence to:
Manit Mishra
International Management Institute
IDCO Plot No. 1, Gothapatna
Bhubaneswar – 751003
Odisha, India
email: manit.mishra@imibh.edu.in;
manitmishra@rediff.com

Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Strategic Change


DOI: 10.1002/jsc

View publication stats

You might also like