ÉT000420

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Emotion Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association

2006, Vol. 6, No. 2, 330 –334 1528-3542/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.330

Gender, Socioeconomic Status, Age, and Jealousy: Emotional Responses to


Infidelity in a National Sample

Melanie C. Green John Sabini


University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill University of Pennsylvania

The authors used a representative national sample (N ⫽ 777) to test the evolutionary hypothesis that men
would be more bothered by sexual infidelity and women by emotional infidelity, the Jealousy as a
Specific Innate Module (JSIM) effect. Our alternative conceptualization of jealousy suggests that there
are distinct emotional components of jealousy that did not evolve differently by gender. The authors
looked for effects of age, socioeconomic status (SES), and type of measure (continuous or dichotomous)
on jealousy. The authors did not find age or SES effects. Forced-choice items provided support for our
alternative view; both genders showed more anger and blame over sexual infidelity but more hurt feelings
over emotional infidelity. Continuous measures indicated more emotional response to sexual than
emotional infidelity among both genders.

Keywords: jealousy, emotion, evolution, sex differences

Evolutionary psychologists have devoted significant attention to different reproductive fitness challenges. Because of paternity
the question of gender differences in emotional responses to be- uncertainty, males should become more jealous over their mates’
trayal in romantic relationships (e.g., Buss, Larsen, Westen, & sexual infidelity than emotional infidelity; sexual infidelity raises
Semmelroth, 1992; Buss, 2000; DeSteno & Salovey, 1996; Harris the possibility of the evolutionary cost of devoting resources to the
& Christenfeld, 1996; Sabini & Green, 2004; Sagarin, 2005). A offspring of another male. Females, in contrast, are certain about
striking amount of data using forced-choice measures show that their genetic link to their offspring, but face the threat that their
women are more distressed over emotional infidelity (a partner mates will withdraw resources from their offspring. Mates’ attach-
falling in love with another), whereas men are more upset over ment to another female is likely to lead to diversion of resources to
sexual infidelity (a partner engaging in intercourse with another); that female and her offspring. Thus, the theory suggests that
indeed, this pattern has been replicated in at least 22 samples (see females are more likely to become upset by signs of resource
Harris, 2003, and Sabini & Green, 2004 for a list), although not in withdrawal (presaged by emotional infidelity) by their mates than
every sample. However, more recent evidence suggests that this by signs of sexual infidelity. As a result of the replications men-
effect may not be robust: specifically, it does not appear to repli- tioned here, this theory is the currently accepted wisdom about
cate in nonstudent samples or with continuous dependent variables evolved responses to infidelity.
(see DeSteno, Bartlett, Braverman, & Salovey, 2002; Harris, 2003,
2005; Sabini & Green, 2004). The failure to find effects in non-
student samples suggests that the effect might be a) a life-stage Limitations on Jealousy As a Specific
effect or b) an educational effect. In this study, we used a national Innate Module Findings
probabilistic sample to explore these questions.
The jealousy as a specific innate module (JSIM) view is not
without its critics, however. Two major problems have been iden-
Jealousy as a Specific Innate Module
tified: reliance on undergraduate samples and lack of robustness
Buss (e.g., Buss et al., 1992; Buss, 2000) sparked interest in the across measures.
evolutionary basis of jealousy by suggesting that genders face Sears (1986), among others, has highlighted problems with
relying on undergraduate samples. For jealousy, one obvious pos-
sibility is that life stage or experience might play a role in re-
Melanie C. Green, Department of Psychology, University of North sponses to infidelity. For example, Harris (2000) found that within
Carolina at Chapel Hill; John Sabini, Department of Psychology, Univer- her undergraduate sample, the results depended, at least for
sity of Pennsylvania. women, on whether participants had been involved in a serious
This research was made possible through Time-Sharing Experiments in relationship (see also Buss et al., 1992; although this effect was
the Social Sciences. We are grateful to the National Science Foundation not replicated in Sabini & Green, 2004). Undergraduates are less
and to Diana Mutz and Arthur Lupia, Principal Investigators for TESS. likely to have been involved in such relationships than are older
This article is dedicated to the memory of John Sabini— collaborator,
adults.
mentor, and friend—who died from a sudden illness shortly after this
article was submitted.
Sabini and Green’s (2004) data suggested that the JSIM effect
Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Melanie was replicable but not robust; we replicated the effect but only
Green, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel with undergraduates. On the other hand, adults recruited in public
Hill, CB#3270 Davie Hall, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3270. E-mail: places did not replicate the gender difference in responses to
mcgreen@email.unc.edu emotional or sexual infidelity.
330
BRIEF REPORTS 331

DeSteno and Salovey (1992) and DeSteno et al. (2002) found ary history) about to procreate or in the midst of procreating. For
that the JSIM effect appears only with dichotomous choice ques- this reason, Buss et al. (1992) predicted that “male sexual jealousy
tions. Harris (2000) also noted the dependence of the result on will diminish as the age of the male’s mate increases because her
forced-choice methods (although see Geary, Rumsey, Bow- reproductive value decreases” (p. 254).1 It is reasonable to imagine
Thomas, & Hoard, 1995; Pietrzak, Laird, Stevens, & Thompson, that for our ancestors, the ages of 18 –22, the ages of undergrad-
2002, who found convergent results with Likert measures). Buss, uates, is just the period when concerns connected to reproduction
Shackelford, Kirkpatrick, et al. (1999) argue that continuous Likert should be most acute.
scale items obscure the relevant differences because of ceiling A second possibility is that the JSIM effect is not innate but is
effects. They claim that men and women find both sorts of infi- learned, presumably from one’s culture (see DeSteno et al., 2002,
delity so distressing that the measures lose sensitivity; instead,
for evidence that forced-choice JSIM results are likely the result of
people must explicitly compare infidelity types.
effortful reasoning rather than “gut reactions”). Markus and
Sabini and Green’s (2004) data on nonstudent adults using
Kitayama (2004) have highlighted differences in socioeconomic
continuous measures found that both genders found sexual infi-
status (SES), particularly the divide between high school- versus
delity worse than emotional. The failure of the predicted pattern to
emerge was not attributable to a ceiling effect because females college-educated individuals, as an important cultural variable.
(like males) rated the emotional infidelity as less provocative than Although they focused on differences in models of agency, these
the sexual infidelity (consistent with DeSteno et al., 2002). differences may extend to emotional expression, gender role ex-
Given this controversy, in the current study, we examine both pectations, or related factors that influence responses to infidelity.2
forced-choice and continuous measures. Continuous measures College-educated individuals may have been differentially ex-
were counterbalanced to examine possible effects of comparing posed to cultural models or norms that emphasize the importance
sexual and emotional infidelity. We expected that the findings may of sex for men and emotion for women.
emerge more strongly on the dichotomous measures. Sabini and Green (2004) did not find age effects, but our
samples were not large enough to provide a strong test of age-
An Alternative to Jealousy As a Specific Innate Module: related differences. Our samples were also insufficiently diverse to
Componential View of Jealousy look for SES effects. The current study used much larger samples
and allowed us to raise these issues in a statistically serious way.
Following Harris (2003), Sabini and Green (2004) proposed as
an alternative to the JSIM view of jealousy a componential view:
that jealousy consists of a set of emotions that happen to serve the The Current Study
reproductive role that Buss has proposed for jealousy but that did
not evolve for that purpose. We proposed specifically that both The current sample’s age distribution is matched to the U.S.
men and women would be angrier and blame their partner more for (adult) age distribution, allowing us to look seriously at age as a
sexual infidelity than over emotional infidelity but would be more marker of life stage effects. (The study also allowed examination
hurt by emotional infidelity. Anger emerges when people believe of the related life-stage markers of marital and parental status.) The
another person is responsible for some unacceptable behavior, national sample also enabled us to test SES differences.
including sexual conduct (see Averill, 1982; Baumeister, Stillwell, We used Time-Sharing Experiments in the Social Sciences
& Wotman, 1990; Sabini & Silver, 1982; Weiner, 1995). Sexual (TESS) to examine the breadth of the JSIM findings as well as our
behavior is likely to be viewed as deliberate and under one’s alternative approach on a representative sample of American
control. Hurt feelings, on the other hand, are triggered by “rela- adults. TESS is a National Science Foundation-supported project
tionship devaluation,” and a relationship may be more profoundly that uses large-scale, cooperative data-collection instruments. We
devalued by emotional betrayal (e.g., Leary & Springer, 2001). posed several research questions: a) Is the JSIM effect age-
Falling in love may be seen as less controllable (and thus less dependent using the standard forced-choice measure? b) Does the
blameworthy). We know of no evidence to suggest that these JSIM effect differ between higher and lower SES? c) Does the
responses are sexually dimorphic, and, therefore, we would not componential finding vary with age and SES in this sample? d) Is
expect jealousy to be sexually dimorphic. This is what we found the componential finding sexually dimorphic? e) Does the JSIM
both in college samples and in adult convenience samples. These
effect occur with continuous measures?
results leave open the possibility that these links between emotion,
sex, and the constituents of jealousy are not variable across age and
education and are not sexually dimorphic. The current sample
1
allows us to test the robustness of these effects as well as of the We did not measure mate age (which would provide the strongest test
JSIM phenomenon. of this idea), but partner ages are generally strongly correlated (above .90).
2
Another potentially relevant cultural variable is culture of honor
(CoH). Nisbett and Cohen have characterized the South and West of the
Why Are College Students Different From Nonstudent United States as CoH, which have an extreme concern with reputation as
Adults? well as a focus on male autonomy and purity of women (e.g., Nisbett &
Cohen, 1996; Vandello & Cohen, 2003). This framework suggests that
Why might the JSIM effect appear in undergraduates but not in CoH men would be even more upset and angry over sexual infidelity than
the adult sample? At least two hypotheses arise. First, consistent other men. We compared participants who lived in the South and West
with the evolutionary view, the JSIM module is innate but may be (CoH) with those who lived in the Northeast and Midwest (non-CoH). The
sensitive to situational factors. It may be active only at certain CoH hypothesis was not supported for either continuous or categorical
stages in one’s life, perhaps when one is (or was in our evolution- measures.
332 BRIEF REPORTS

Table 1
Percentage of Participants Reporting That Emotional Betrayal Is Worse Than Sexual by Gender
and Type of Effect

Males Females Sex difference

Variable % ␹2 p % ␹2 p ␹2 p

Upset 54.6 1.56 .21 67.9 25.00 .0005 7.06 .008


Hurt 57.4 4.17 .04 66.5 21.11 .0001 3.32 .07
Angry 27.0 40.04 .0001 41.9 5.03 .02 9.34 .002
Blame 38.1 10.71 .001 42.6 4.12 .04 0.81 .37
Partner leave 76.6 53.19 .0001 83.3 85.33 .0001 2.70 .10
You leave 52.4 .44 .51 60.5 8.42 .004 2.50 .11

Method one, the ␹2 measuring the association between sex versus emotion
and the dependent variables and the ␹2 for the gender difference.
Participants A significant gender difference emerged on “upset” in the
Participants were part of a representative national sample recruited direction that JSIM would predict (a greater percentage of women
through TESS, administered by Knowledge Networks. (For sampling pro- were more upset over emotional infidelity). However, contrary to
cedures, see www.experimentcentral.org.) the JSIM prediction, a majority of men also selected emotional
A total of 777 people (378 male; 399 female) participated online. Of infidelity as more bothersome (although not significantly so).
these, 74.6% were white, 10.9% were black, 9.3% were Hispanic, and We conducted logistic regressions predicting sexual/emotional
5.1% had “other” racial/ethnic background. The average age was 48 choice by gender, education, marital status, age, race (coded as
(standard deviation ⫽ 16). Approximately 15% had less than a high school white/nonwhite), household income, whether the individual lived
education, 30.9% had completed high school, 29.5% had some college, and
in a dual-income household, and whether the individual had chil-
24.8% had a bachelor’s degree or higher. The majority of the sample was
dren. Demographics other than gender, specifically age and edu-
married (64%) and the remainder was single (18%), divorced (11.2%),
widowed (5.1%), or separated (1.7%). Almost all participants reported cation (our marker of SES), were not significantly related to
having been in a serious relationship (including marriage); only 5.8% had responses.4
not, and 1.8% did not answer. Approximately one-fourth of the sample We divided the sample into those with a high school diploma or
(23.9%) had no children.3 less and those with some college or college/advanced degrees.
Again, SES did not affect selection of emotional versus sexual
Design infidelity (Tarone’s ␹2[1] ⫽ .04, p ⬎ .1). There were no effects of
SES for men (␹2[1: N ⫽ 185] ⫽ 1.26, p ⬎ .1) or women (␹2[1:
We used the same instructions as Buss, et al. (1992): participants were N ⫽ 196] ⫽ 1.89, p ⬎ .1).
asked to “think of a serious or committed relationship that you have had in
the past, that you currently have, or that you would like to have. Imagine
that you discover that the person with whom you’ve been seriously in- Alternative Model
volved became interested in someone else.” Hurt, anger, and blame. Replicating our prior studies with
For half of the sample, each respondent saw both intercourse (“now
nonstudent adults (Sabini & Green, 2004), we found that a major-
imagine your partner having sexual intercourse with that person”) and
emotional attachment (“now imagine that your partner has fallen in love
ity of both genders said they would be more hurt by an emotional
and formed a deep emotional attachment to that person”) versions. infidelity but would be more angry and would blame their partner
They rated how much the infidelity would cause them to be upset, hurt, more for a sexual infidelity. There was a significant gender dif-
and angry on 1–7 scales anchored by “not at all” and “very.” They also ference for anger such that men were even more likely than women
reported how much they would blame their partner, how likely it was that to be angered by sexual infidelity. A trend toward a gender
their partner would leave them in that circumstance, and how likely they difference on hurt also emerged; women were more likely to report
would be to leave their partner. Half of these participants received the feeling hurt by emotional infidelity.
emotion questions first and half received the intercourse questions first. Effect on relationship. Again replicating our previous result,
Taken together, these sets constitute a within-participant design, but the both genders were more likely to believe their partners were going
first half of the data from each participant constitutes a between-
to leave them as a result of an emotional rather than a sexual
participants design.
The other half of the sample answered in forced-choice format; they
infidelity. Both men and women were also more likely to say they
reported which of the infidelity types made them more upset, angry, and so would leave their partners over an emotional infidelity (but this
on. The design is 2 (gender) ⫻ 2 (sexual versus emotional infidelity) ⫻ 2 effect was not significant for males).
(dichotomous versus continuous measures).
3
TESS limits the number of “participant/questions” available to each
Results researcher; additional questions reduce the sample size. Therefore, we did
Forced-Choice Measures not ask about sexual orientation, partner age, or other less central variables.
4
The only effect to achieve conventional significance levels was that
Table 1 presents the percentage of participants of each gender whites were more likely to blame their partner for sexual infidelity
who reported that an emotional infidelity was worse than a sexual (Wald ⫽ 4.21, p ⬍ .05).
BRIEF REPORTS 333

Table 2
Mean and (Standard Deviations) of Ratings by Gender of Participants, Whether They Are Rating
a Sexual or Emotional Affair, and Whether They Rated Sexual or Emotional Infidelity First

Males Females

Sex Emotion Sex Emotion

EmoF SexF EmoF SexF EmoF SexF EmoF SexF

Upset 6.41 6.21 6.14 5.85 6.71 6.46 6.41 6.33


(1.35) (1.50) (1.46) (1.42) (.98) (1.34) (1.26) (1.41)
Hurt 6.41 6.01 6.09 5.97 6.68 6.53 6.40 6.38
(1.32) (1.65) (1.54) (1.42) (1.01) (1.15) (1.33) (1.43)
Angry 6.21 5.78 5.59 5.15 6.58 6.21 5.97 6.11
(1.51) (1.65) (1.76) (1.75) (1.13) (1.44) (1.55) (1.60)
Blame 5.94 5.87 5.20 5.45 6.53 6.24 5.84 6.10
(1.72) (1.52) (1.80) (1.88) (1.13) (1.40) (1.46) (1.45)
You leave 5.63 5.23 4.60 5.61 5.45 5.29 5.08 5.91
(1.99) (1.94) (2.27) (1.76) (2.07) (1.95) (2.16) (1.63)
Partner leaves 5.30 4.66 4.59 5.38 4.54 4.44 4.45 5.51
(1.81) (1.95) (2.01) (1.83) (2.10) (2.09) (2.17) (1.68)

Socioeconomic status. There were no significant effects of Discussion


SES on any variable.
Our results provided strong support for the similarity of emo-
tional responses to infidelity, across gender, age, and SES; these
Continuous Measures responses did not support the widely reported JSIM hypothesis.
We now reprise our major questions subsequently.
Table 2 presents the mean ratings for the emotions by gender, Did the JSIM effect appear in a national sample with forced-
infidelity type, and whether participants responded to sex or emo- choice measures? The JSIM effect did not emerge strongly; a
tion first. direct replication of whether individuals would be more upset by a
Comparing the first set of questions answered by each partici- sexual or an emotional infidelity revealed that a majority of both
pant provides a between-participants design. Multivariate analysis genders were more upset by an emotional infidelity. However, this
of variance (MANOVA) showed a significant effect of infidelity effect was larger among women, which would be consistent with
type (F[6, 359] ⫽ 2.78, p ⬍ .05) and of gender (F[6, 359] ⫽ 2.79, a JSIM prediction; similarly, men were angrier than women over
p ⬍ .05) but no interaction (F[6, 359] ⬍ 1, p ⬎ .50). We then sexual infidelity. Nonetheless, it appears that a strong form of
conducted 2 (gender) ⫻ 2 (infidelity type) analyses of variance on JSIM effect (in which males should be more upset by sexual
each dependent variable. Men and women differed in their re- infidelity) is not robust in nonstudent adults.
sponses, but contrary to the JSIM hypothesis, none of the interac- Were there age or SES effects on the JSIM phenomena? We
tions with sexual versus emotional infidelity were significant (all found no evidence of age or SES effects on men’s or women’s
Fs ⬍ 1.4, ps ⬎ .20). In terms of sex versus emotion differences reports of whether sexual or emotional infidelity would upset them
more. It is important to note that although negative results are
collapsing across gender, although the means were in the expected
always perilous to interpret, these negative results are less perilous
direction (individuals are more upset, hurt, and think their partner
than most because they used a) a probabilistic national sample, b)
is more likely to leave over emotion, but show more anger, blame,
well-established procedures, and c) a sizable number of partici-
and are more likely to leave their partner over sex), the only
pants. The negative result is not easily attributed to inappropriate
significant differences were for blame (F[1, 364] ⫽ 11.57, p ⬍
procedures or a lack of power.
.01) and leave partner (F[1, 364] ⫽ 4.02, p ⬍ .05).5
The fact that life-stage results do not emerge here provides a
Socioeconomic status. A 2 (gender) ⫻ 2 (infidelity type) ⫻ 2
theoretical advance about the nature of these effects; whatever
(SES) MANOVA revealed significant main effects of gender and
infidelity type as well as an SES ⫻ infidelity type interaction (F[6,
369] ⫽ 2.75, p ⬍ .05). On individual variables, these interactions 5
One could also examine these data within-participants by calculating
were significant for hurt (F[1, 374] ⫽ 5.34, p ⬍ .05) and angry the difference on each variable between participants’ rating of sex and
(F[1, 374] ⫽ 3.74, p ⬍ .06). Low SES respondents were slightly emotion (sex ⫺ emotion); as a result of space constraints, we only sum-
more hurt over sex than emotion (Memotion ⫽ 5.84; Msex ⫽ 6.17), marize these analyses here. Using difference scores, individuals showed
whereas high SES individuals were more hurt by emotion stronger emotional responses to sexual versus emotional infidelity on all
measures, replicating DeSteno, Bartlett, Braverman, and Salovey (2002)
(Memotion ⫽ 6.57; Msex ⫽ 6.07). Low SES individuals were angrier
and Sabini and Green (2004), study 3. In regression analyses including all
over sex than emotion (Memotion ⫽ 5.40; Msex ⫽ 5.89), and high demographics, there was no effect of education. The only age effect was
SES were slightly angrier over emotion (Memotion ⫽ 5.96; Msex ⫽ that older men were more likely to believe that a partner would leave over
5.71). SES did not interact with gender and the three-way inter- sexual infidelity (b ⫽ .19, p ⬍ .05), and older men were marginally more
actions were not significant. likely to leave over emotional infidelity (b ⫽ .14, p ⬍ .09).
334 BRIEF REPORTS

mechanisms account for emotional responses to infidelity appear ences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology. Psychologi-
to be active throughout the lifespan. Jealousy responses were also cal Science, 3, 251–255.
not moderated by whether individuals already had children, their Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion: Why jealousy is as necessary
income, or other factors that might show differences if these as love and sex. New York: Free Press.
Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Kirkpatrick, L. A., Choe, J. C., Lim,
mechanisms were sensitive to situational factors. These results are
H. K., Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T., & Bennett, K. (1999). Jealousy and
consistent with the more parsimonious account that jealousy is
beliefs about infidelity: Tests of competing hypotheses in the United
comprised of lower-level emotional states (e.g., anger and hurt), States, Korea, and Japan. Personal Relationships, 6, 125–150.
which may be triggered by a variety of events. DeSteno, D., Bartlett, M. Y., Braverman, J., & Salovey, P. (2002). Sex
The lack of SES effects also speaks to the generality of jealousy differences in jealousy: Evolutionary mechanisms or artifact of measure-
mechanisms. Similarly, differences in social class were an obvious ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1103–1116.
potential explanation for differences between college samples and DeSteno, D. A., & Salovey, P. (1992). Evolutionary origins of sex differ-
other samples. ences in jealousy? Questioning the ‘fitness’ of the model. Psychological
Was a more differentiated conception of jealousy supported by Science, 7, 367–372.
dichotomous data? Individuals of both genders were angrier and Geary, D. C., Rumsey, M., Bow-Thomas, C. C., & Hoard, M. K. (1995).
Sexual jealousy as a facultative trait: Evidence from the pattern of sex
blamed their partners more for sexual infidelities but were more
differences in adults from China and the United States. Ethology and
hurt by emotional ones, thus supporting a view of jealousy as a
Sociobiology, 16, 355–383.
complex of emotions and replicating Sabini and Green (2004). It is Harris, C. R. (2000). Psychophysiological responses to imagined infidelity:
clear that using a more diverse set of emotion measures, beyond The specific innate modular view of jealousy reconsidered. Journal of
“upset or distressed,” provides useful information. Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1082–1091.
Did the JSIM effect appear with continuous measures? The Harris, C. R. (2003). A review of sex differences in sexual jealousy,
continuous measures did not support the JSIM effect. In a including self-report data, psychophysiological responses, interpersonal
between-subjects analysis, there were no significant interactions violence, and morbid jealousy. Personality and Social Psychology Re-
between gender and infidelity type on upset or the other dependent view, 7, 102–128.
variables. The JSIM effect does not appear robust over continuous Harris, C. R. (2005). Male and female jealousy, still more similar than
different: Reply to Sagarin (2005). Personality and Social Psychology
measures.
Review, 9, 76 – 86.
The current study thus provides further confirmation of the
Harris, C. R., & Christenfeld, N. (1996). Gender, jealousy, and reason.
methodological limitations of the sex difference. To explain this Psychological Science, 7, 364 –366.
discrepancy, DeSteno, Bartlett, Braverman, and Salovey (2002) Leary, M. R., & Springer, C. A. (2001). Hurt feelings: The neglected
suggested that a forced-choice format induces individuals to use a emotion. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.), Behaving badly. Washington, DC:
more effortful decision strategy. They found that under cognitive APA Press.
load, the sex difference on forced-choice measures disappears, Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2004). Models of agency: Sociocultural
which suggests that responses to infidelity are not modular and diversity in the construction of action. In V. Murphy-Berman & J.
automatic, as JSIM predicts. Berman (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 49. Cross-
Of course, some mystery remains as to why the JSIM effect is cultural differences in perspectives on self. Lincoln: University of Ne-
braska Press.
robust in college samples but not in adult samples. Education alone
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of
does not account for these differences. Perhaps unmeasured vari-
violence in the South. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
ables such as sensitivity to social norms, stereotypes about male Pietrzak, R. H., Laird, J. D., Stevens, D., & Thompson, N. S. (2002). Sex
versus female sexuality, or some aspect of relationship experience differences in human jealousy: A coordinated study of forced-choice,
will explain this discrepancy. continuous rating scale, and physiological responses on the same sub-
jects. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 83–94.
Sabini, J., & Green, M. C. (2004). Emotional responses to sexual and
Conclusions emotional infidelity: Constants and differences across genders, samples,
and methods. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1375–
In summary, these results weigh against the view that jealousy 1388.
evolved as a special module. Rather, our findings provide addi- Sabini, J., & Silver, M. (1982). Moralities of everyday life (pp. 163–182).
tional support for the view of jealousy as comprised of distinct New York: Oxford University Press.
emotions that are triggered by different events or aspects of events. Sagarin, B. J. (2005). Reconsidering evolved sex differences in jealousy:
These triggers are not substantially different for men and women, Comment on Harris (2003). Personality and Social Psychology Review,
and they do not appear to be moderated by differences in individ- 9, 62–75.
uals’ life stage or life circumstances. Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a
narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530.
References Vandello, J. A., & Cohen, D. (2003). Male honor and female fidelity:
Implicit cultural scripts that perpetuate domestic violence. Journal of
Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 997–1010.
York: Springer-Verlag. Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A., & Wotman, S. R. (1990). Victim and of social conduct. New York: Guilford Press.
perpetrator accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narra-
tives about anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, Received July 8, 2005
994 –1005. Revision received October 3, 2005
Buss, D., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex differ- Accepted October 28, 2005 䡲

You might also like