Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 2

Comment

https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00169-w

Establishing psychological universals


Asifa Majid Check for updates

Universals of thought and behaviour across come from economically advantaged backgrounds. They are internet
savvy, possess mobile technologies, are likely to be fluent in English and
variable cultural experiences can reveal partake of American popular culture (including films, TV and games),
uniquely human cognition. However, culturally and consume products from a monopolized flavour and fragrance indus-
try. So even when researchers test undergraduates from across the globe,
informed and theoretically motivated sampling they are not sampling a representative slice of humanity.
is needed to reveal true universals. Second, global studies are designed and conducted in English.
The use of English as both test and research language is a homogeniz-
Humans are characterized by cultural diversity. Around the planet, ing force in the study of cognition2. When researchers make efforts to
people organize social structures according to distinct beliefs and be more inclusive, they might translate their study into a well-studied
values, eat or have taboos against ingesting a variety of foods, and com- language of European descent or one spoken by a majority community
municate in 7,000 mutually unintelligible languages. This variation of Asia (such as Mandarin). This approach leads to numerically repre-
is a provocation to psychologists who strive for a unified account of sentative sampling in which easy-to-access communities from majority
the human mind and behaviour. However, theorizing is limited when cultures are included, whereas those from minority cultures are over-
observations come from a relatively homogenous group of people1 looked (Fig. 1c). English and Mandarin have over a billion speakers each;
(Fig. 1a). The group itself might not be representative of humanity, so Spanish over half a billion; French, German, and Japanese hundreds of
the generalizability of observations is called into question. Crucially, millions. By contrast, most languages of the world have 100–10,000
key human traits not evident in the observed sample — but shared across speakers. Speakers of large official languages share attributes including
other cultures — will be overlooked by scientists entirely2. literacy, formalized education and technological savviness. Further-
Cross-cultural studies are essential to establish psychological uni- more, they are members of larger, sparser, more stratified societies,
versals. If researchers find the same trait across cultures, despite varia- with more strangers and less mutually held knowledge, than are speak-
ble cultural experiences, this discovery reveals a fundamental fact about ers of smaller languages. These common factors across majority com-
the human mind and could shed light on the biological foundations munities can lead to premature conclusions of universality, even when
of human cognition. testing beyond English.
Many studies, despite aiming to collect data from a heterogenous Third, global studies often equate cultures with country. To dem-
group, overestimate the cultural diversity of their samples and there- onstrate the diversity of peoples sampled in some studies, researchers
fore cannot be used to draw conclusions about universals. Culturally simply enumerate the nations tested. Alternatively, ‘world regions’5 or
informed and theoretically motivated study designs are needed to ‘major civilizations’4 are taken to be cultural units. However, countries,
determine the extent of psychological universality and diversity. world regions, and major civilizations are questionable proxies for
meaningful cultural entities. An unstated presupposition of cultural
Limitations of current global psychology hegemony within a country or region also occurs when there is inad-
Modern technological innovations have enabled psychological testing equate reporting about who exactly is participating in studies. Every
on a grand scale. Psychologists put their studies online or gamify them participant living in a particular country cannot be assumed to be a
and recruit participants through collaboration via social networks, member of the same unitary culture. Various migrant populations
press releases and media campaigns3–5. The goal is to collect data from bring their cultures to other nations and participants can have mul-
many people in as many settings as possible with the hope of general- tiple or distinct cultural identities beyond national or mainstream
izing to the human population. Embedded in this globalized approach culture. Critically, almost half a billion indigenous people live in
is the assumption that data collected from large samples across many 90 countries worldwide. They speak two-thirds of the world’s lan-
countries can inform researchers about universals. guages and manifest unique cultures and distinct ways of life. Their
Going global through collaboration and crowdsourcing is an identities are elided in national surveys, and so genuine cultural diver-
important advance for psychology given its historical neglect of cul- sity becomes omitted from consideration. For all of these reasons,
tural diversity. These global efforts can address important questions it is misleading to report participants’ countries rather than their
about the replicability of psychological effects across contexts. How- cultural identities.
ever, it is a flawed approach for positing psychological universals for A study that focuses on undergraduates or a narrow sample of eth-
three key reasons. nolinguistic communities, or analyses geography without considering
First, global samples acquired through convenience sampling repro- cultural diversity, will only ever be a weak test of universality. A study of
duce the sampling biases of traditional lab-based psychology, in which a closely related communities or participants with shared demographic
rather homogenous group of undergraduate students is tested (Fig. 1b). backgrounds cannot unambiguously support claims of universality.
Undergraduate students are not typical of the broader communities to If similarities are uncovered, this is a minimal threshold for univer-
which they belong6. Furthermore, the internationalization of universi- sality, but such results are also entirely compatible with a cultural
ties means undergraduates everywhere — regardless of their home learning account (such as shared cultural forces across the sampled
nation — have more in common than distinguishes them. They usually communities producing a common effect).

nature reviews psychology Volume 2 | April 2023 | 199–200 | 199


a Classical non-comparative sampling b Convenience sampling Even distinct cultures can be more similar than they first seem.
For instance, the world’s 7,000 current ethnolinguistic groups are
related by common lineage and contact9. This fact must be taken into

Access to technology
consideration in analyses and interpretation of data. A claim of univer-
sality is stronger when tested against two unrelated and very different
communities for the phenomenon of interest. Even large sample sizes
of closely related communities do not mitigate against the potential
confound of common history.
Claims of universality are strongest when tested against diverse
and independent cultures. The more disparate the cultures, the more
Ease of participant recruitment
convincing the proposed universal. Therefore, genuine psychological
c Numerically representative sampling d Theory-based sampling universals can be established only by testing people from the most dis-
parate communities, where people differ most widely in their cultural
practices, belief systems, languages and habitats.
Theoretical axis 2

Establishing psychological universals


The strongest test of universality is a large-scale comparative study:
large not in terms of participant size alone, but in terms of the number
of distinct cultures sampled along theoretically relevant dimensions
(Fig. 1d). A thorny question for psychologists to tackle going forward
is exactly how many cultures ought to be sampled, an issue linguists
Culture Theoretical axis 1 and anthropologists have long grappled with. If resources are limited,
Sampling scheme a critical test of universality can nevertheless come from only two cul-
tures — if those cultures are maximally distinct for the research question
Fig. 1 | Sampling strategies for establishing psychological universals. Each blue
at hand. The generalizability of those findings can further be bolstered
dot represents a culture or ethnolinguistic group. The larger the dot, the bigger
by testing another two communities, where those cultures are again
the population. A few majority cultures are represented by millions of people, but
sampled to be maximally different along the relevant dimensions.
most minority cultures are represented by a few hundred or thousand people. Open
circles represent experimental samples within a study. a, Classical non-comparative
Converging evidence using different sampling strategies and different
sampling. A single community is sampled, historically only English-speaking cultures lends more credence to posited universals.
Americans or British people. b, Convenience sampling. An experimental sample Compelling cross-cultural research cannot be done as parachute sci-
is obtained according to ease of testing, which typically leads to sampling of a ence, in which psychologists from wealthy institutions extract data from
relatively homogenous group of undergraduates from majority communities. people without engaging with local communities. It requires research-
c, Numerically representative sampling. Sample sizes within cultures are ers to grow relationships with communities and invest in partner-
determined according to their global representation, which leads to over- ships10. Establishing psychological universals is important. But to do
representation of majority cultures and under-representation of minority cultures. it meaningfully, cultural variation must be understood and respected.
d, Theory-based sampling. The relevant theoretical parameters of cultural variation
are established and then participants are sampled from cultures accordingly. Asifa Majid 1,2
1
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford,
Oxford, UK. 2Radcliffe Institute for Advanced Study, Harvard University,
Culturally informed, theoretically motivated sampling Cambridge, MA, USA.
A strong test of universality maximizes the sampling of cultures that e-mail: asifa.majid@psy.ox.ac.uk
differ along theoretically relevant dimensions. It must also take into
consideration shared characteristics of participants and communi- Published online: 3 March 2023
ties to rule out similarities that arise from shared histories rather than
genuine psychological universals. Sampling should be aligned with References
1. Thalmayer, A. G., Toscanelli, C. & Arnett, J. J. The neglected 95% revisited: is American
theoretical considerations and designed to test diverse cultural groups. psychology becoming less American? Am. Psychol. 76, 116–129 (2021).
Tests of universals must sample cultures according to dimensions rel- 2. Blasi, D. E., Henrich, J., Adamou, E., Kemmerer, D. & Majid, A. Over-reliance on English
evant to the research question posed, motivated by theory in that domain, hinders cognitive science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 26, 1153–1170 (2022).
3. Klein, R. A. et al. Many Labs 2: Investigating variation in replicability across samples and
and considering cultural similarities and differences (Fig. 1d). For example, settings. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 1, 443–490 (2018).
a study of universal preferences in odour perception is strengthened if it 4. Volynets, S., Glerean, E., Hietanen, J. K., Hari, R. & Nummenmaa, L. Bodily maps of
minimizes common odour experiences across groups by sampling cul- emotions are culturally universal. Emotion 20, 1127–1136 (2020).
5. Jones, B. C. et al. To which world regions does the valence-dominance model of social
tures with different subsistence strategies (for example, foraging, farming perception apply? Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 159–169 (2021).
and industrial) and different smellscape ecologies (for example, urban, 6. Wild, H., Kyröläinen, A.-J. & Kuperman, V. How representative are student convenience
rainforest and coastal)7. By contrast, a study of theory-of-mind universals samples? A study of literacy and numeracy skills in 32 countries. PLoS One 17, e0271191 (2022).
7. Arshamian, A. et al. The perception of odor pleasantness is shared across cultures. Curr. Biol.
need not worry about smellscapes; it would instead sample according to 32, 2061–2066.e3 (2022).
diversity of social structures (including local norms and attitudes about 8. Barrett, H. C. et al. Early false-belief understanding in traditional non-Western societies.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 280, 20122654 (2013).
the nature of minds) and languages (for example, sampling varying mental
9. Kirby, K. R. et al. D-PLACE: A global database of cultural, linguistic and environmental
state vocabulary and relevant syntactic structures)8. diversity. PLoS One 11, e0158391 (2016).
Furthermore, claims of universality are more credible when tested 10. Urassa, M. et al. Cross-cultural research must prioritize equitable collaboration. Nat. Hum.
Behav. 5, 668–671 (2021).
against people with the most diverse experiences relative to the rel-
evant theoretical dimension. Tests of universality must go beyond Acknowledgements
undergraduate samples and include participants from varied walks The author thanks M. Frank, H. Over and C. Heyes for comments on an earlier version of this
of life across communities. Otherwise, it is unclear whether com- paper.

mon behavioural patterns are the result of uniquely human inherited Competing interests
infrastructure or shared environmental and socialization forces. The author declares no competing interests.

nature reviews psychology Volume 2 | April 2023 | 199–200 | 200

You might also like