Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 70

Functional Movement Disorder An

Interdisciplinary Case Based Approach


Current Clinical Neurology Kathrin
Lafaver (Editor)
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmeta.com/product/functional-movement-disorder-an-interdisciplinary-cas
e-based-approach-current-clinical-neurology-kathrin-lafaver-editor/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Clinical Neurology and Neuroanatomy: A Localization-


Based Approach (Second Edition) Aaron Berkowitz

https://ebookmeta.com/product/clinical-neurology-and-
neuroanatomy-a-localization-based-approach-second-edition-aaron-
berkowitz/

How to Think Like a Neurologist: A Case-Based Guide to


Clinical Reasoning in Neurology 1st Edition Ethan
Meltzer

https://ebookmeta.com/product/how-to-think-like-a-neurologist-a-
case-based-guide-to-clinical-reasoning-in-neurology-1st-edition-
ethan-meltzer/

Mistakes in Clinical Neuropsychology Learning from a


Case-based Approach 1st Edition Oliver Turnbull

https://ebookmeta.com/product/mistakes-in-clinical-
neuropsychology-learning-from-a-case-based-approach-1st-edition-
oliver-turnbull/

Clinical Obstetrics A Case based Approach 1st Edition


Pushpa Mishra Niharika Dhiman Anjali Tempe

https://ebookmeta.com/product/clinical-obstetrics-a-case-based-
approach-1st-edition-pushpa-mishra-niharika-dhiman-anjali-tempe/
Cancer Treatment - An Interdisciplinary Approach - An
Interdisciplinary Approach (Jan 1,
2024)_(3031439821)_(Springer) 1st Edition Nima Rezaei

https://ebookmeta.com/product/cancer-treatment-an-
interdisciplinary-approach-an-interdisciplinary-approach-
jan-1-2024_3031439821_springer-1st-edition-nima-rezaei/

A Functional Approach to Family Case Work Jessie Taft

https://ebookmeta.com/product/a-functional-approach-to-family-
case-work-jessie-taft/

A Case-Based Guide to Clinical Endocrinology Davies

https://ebookmeta.com/product/a-case-based-guide-to-clinical-
endocrinology-davies/

Advanced Skin Cancer: A Case-Based Approach Debjani


Sahni

https://ebookmeta.com/product/advanced-skin-cancer-a-case-based-
approach-debjani-sahni/

Netter’s Orthopaedic Clinical Examination. An Evidence-


Based Approach 3rd Edition Joshua A. Cleland

https://ebookmeta.com/product/netters-orthopaedic-clinical-
examination-an-evidence-based-approach-3rd-edition-joshua-a-
cleland/
Current Clinical Neurology
Series Editor: Daniel Tarsy

Kathrin LaFaver
Carine W. Maurer
Timothy R. Nicholson
David L. Perez Editors

Functional
Movement
Disorder
An Interdisciplinary Case-Based Approach
Current Clinical Neurology
Series Editor
Daniel Tarsy
Beth Israel Deaconness Medical Center
Department of Neurology
Boston, MA, USA
Current Clinical Neurology offers a wide range of practical resources for
clinical neurologists. Providing evidence-based titles covering the full range
of neurologic disorders commonly presented in the clinical setting, the
Current Clinical Neurology series covers such topics as multiple sclerosis,
Parkinson's Disease and nonmotor dysfunction, seizures, Alzheimer's
Disease, vascular dementia, sleep disorders, and many others. Series editor
Daniel Tarsy, MD, is professor of neurology, Vice Chairman of the Department
of Neurology, and Chief of the Movement Disorders division at Beth Israel
Deaconness Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts.

More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/7630


Kathrin LaFaver • Carine W. Maurer
Timothy R. Nicholson • David L. Perez
Editors

Functional Movement
Disorder
An Interdisciplinary Case-Based
Approach
Editors
Kathrin LaFaver Carine W. Maurer
Movement Disorder Specialist Department of Neurology
Saratoga Hospital Medical Group Renaissance School of Medicine at
Saratoga Springs, NY, USA Stony Brook University
Stony Brook, NY, USA
Timothy R. Nicholson
Neuropsychiatry Research and David L. Perez
Education Group Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry
Institute of Psychiatry Psychology & Massachusetts General Hospital
Neuroscience Harvard Medical School
King’s College London Boston, MA, USA
London, UK

ISSN 1559-0585     ISSN 2524-4043 (electronic)


Current Clinical Neurology
ISBN 978-3-030-86494-1    ISBN 978-3-030-86495-8 (eBook)
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-86495-8

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022


This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or
part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of
illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way,
and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software,
or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor
the authors or the editors give a warranty, expressed or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains
neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

This Humana imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
Preface

Functional movement disorder (FMD), a subtype of functional neurological


disorder (FND), is a prevalent, potentially disabling, and costly condition at
the intersection of neurology and psychiatry. After being of significant inter-
est to a number of early leaders across neurology, psychiatry, and psychology
(e.g., Charcot, Freud, Janet, Briquet, Babinski), the late twentieth century
was unfortunately marked by limited interest in the field of FND. Part of the
difficulties arise from the notion that FND inherently challenges the concep-
tualization of modern-day medical specialties and societal views of physical
health and mental health more broadly. Thankfully, interest in FMD and
related conditions has renewed in the last 20 years. Improvements in diagnos-
tic specificity, an expanding “toolbox” of evidence-based treatments, and an
international, multidisciplinary professional society (www.FNDsociety.org)
are all driving interest in this field among clinicians and researchers alike.
Furthermore, FMD is scientifically compelling, teaching us about a range of
cognitive-affective neuroscience principles, and when patients respond well
to treatment, effect sizes rival those seen throughout the brain sciences.
In this case-based textbook on FMD, we offer readers a practical, evidence-­
based approach to the assessment and management of FMD presentations
over 32 chapters. Chapter 1 provides a historical perspective on FMD and
FND more broadly, with Chaps. 2 and 3 outlining emerging neural mecha-
nisms and the importance of the biopsychosocial model, respectively. Chapter
4 offers an integrated clinical neuroscience approach to FMD. Thereafter,
Chaps. 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, and 13 detail case-based examples (including
videos) and practical discussion on the assessment and management of the
full range of functional movement symptoms – including but not limited to
functional limb weakness, tremor, dystonia, parkinsonism, tics, jerks, gait
difficulties, and speech/voice abnormalities. Non-motor symptoms found in
patients with FMD are outlined in Chap. 14, while Chaps. 15 and 16 offer
assessment recommendations for pediatric and elderly populations. Informed
by the latest research and expert opinions, Chaps. 17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23,
24, 25, and 26 provide practical suggestions in regard to therapeutic
approaches, including education, physical/occupational/speech and language
therapies, and psychotherapy. The role of placebo treatment and transcranial
magnetic stimulation are visited in Chaps. 27 and 28, respectively, potentially
promising interventions requiring considerably more research. Chapters 29,
30, and 31 are also on noteworthy topics, including measuring symptoms,
managing obstacles in longitudinal care, and the treatment of pediatric

v
vi Preface

FMD. Chap. 32 succinctly details the career narratives of the four co-editors,
as a way of hopefully inspiring the next generation of clinicians and research-
ers toward the field of FMD and related conditions. Throughout the text, we
advocate for a patient-centered, biopsychosocially informed clinical neuro-
science perspective. Given that the clinical landscape of FMD and related
conditions have undergone transformative changes in the last several decades,
it can be expected that portions of the content put forth here will be updated
in the coming decades.
Overall, we believe that this case-based textbook will be a valuable
resource for trainees and seasoned clinicians alike across the fields of neurol-
ogy, psychiatry, medicine, psychology, social work, allied mental health dis-
ciplines, and physical rehabilitation (including physical, occupational, and
speech-language therapists). Given the overlap between FMD, other FND
subtypes, chronic pain, and the range of functional disorders seen across
medicine, clinicians working in these spaces will likely also find this text
useful.
We would like to express our sincere and utmost gratitude to our chapter
authors, who generously provided their expertise and time to help dissemi-
nate their knowledge and skills. We are also indebted to our patients, mentors,
and current and former colleagues from whom we have learned so much.
Lastly, we thank our families for their unwavering support and
encouragement.

Saratoga Springs, NY, USA Kathrin LaFaver


Stony Brook, NY, USA Carine W. Maurer
London, UK Timothy R. Nicholson
Boston, MA, USA David L. Perez
Series Editor’s Introduction

This volume, Functional Movement Disorder: An Interdisciplinary Case


Based Approach provides a very useful and much needed comprehensive
review of functional movement disorder as a subtype of functional neurologi-
cal disorder which recently have become recognized as common and worthy
of the serious attention of both neurologists and psychiatrists. The editors of
this book, Drs. LaFaver, Maurer, Nicholson, and Perez, have collected many
careful and thoughtful contributions from their colleagues, all of whom have
taken a very serious interest in functional neurological disorder. In Part I of
this book, the historical perspective provided by Richard Kanaan in Chap. 1
sets the stage for this discussion by reminding the reader of currently out-
moded historical terms such as “conversion disorder”, “psychogenic disor-
der”, and “hysterical disorder” which have been of very limited value in
characterizing, understanding, and treating patients with functional neuro-
logical disorder. In Chap. 2, the pathophysiologic underpinnings of functional
movement disorder is imaginatively laid out by Dr. Mark Hallett, who has
emphasized the concept of loss of “self-agency” by the patient as a way of
understanding the occurrence of abnormal movements which closely resem-
ble abnormal movements that arise from physical lesions of the brain. In Part
II of the book, specific case descriptions will assist the clinician who may
believe they are encountering a patient with functional tremors, jerky move-
ments, tics, gait disorders, or some other functional movement disorder.
Finally, Part III of the book provides a variety of useful approaches to the
management of functional movement disorder by a variety of therapeutic
techniques.
Daniel Tarsy
Department of Neurology
Harvard Medical School
Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center
Boston, MA, USA

vii
Contents

Part I Framework

1 A Historical Perspective on Functional


Neurological Disorder����������������������������������������������������������������������   3
Richard A. A. Kanaan
2 Free Will, Emotions and Agency: Pathophysiology
of Functional Movement Disorder������������������������������������������������� 13
Mark Hallett
3 The Biopsychosocial Formulation for Functional
Movement Disorder ������������������������������������������������������������������������ 27
Lindsey MacGillivray and Sarah C. Lidstone
4 Integrating Neurologic and Psychiatric Perspectives in
Functional Movement Disorder������������������������������������������������������ 39
Jordan R. Anderson, David L. Perez, and Bruce H. Price

Part II Presentations

5 Functional Limb Weakness and Paralysis ������������������������������������ 53


Selma Aybek
6 Functional Tremor �������������������������������������������������������������������������� 67
Petra Schwingenschuh
7 Functional Dystonia ������������������������������������������������������������������������ 81
Francesca Morgante
8 Functional Parkinsonism���������������������������������������������������������������� 93
Marine Ambar Akkaoui, Bertrand Degos, and Béatrice Garcin
9 Functional Jerky Movements���������������������������������������������������������� 103
Yasmine E. M. Dreissen, Jeannette M. Gelauff, and
Marina A. J. Tijssen
10 Functional Facial Disorders������������������������������������������������������������ 115
Mohammad Rohani and Alfonso Fasano
11 Functional Gait Disorder���������������������������������������������������������������� 135
Benedetta Demartini

ix
x Contents

12 Functional Tics �������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 147


Tina Mainka and Christos Ganos
13 Functional Speech and Voice Disorders ���������������������������������������� 157
Carine W. Maurer and Joseph R. Duffy
14 Beyond Functional Movements: The Spectrum of
Functional Neurological and Somatic Symptoms ������������������������ 169
Caitlin Adams and David L. Perez
15 Functional Movement Disorder in Children �������������������������������� 183
Alison Wilkinson-Smith and Jeff L. Waugh
16 Functional Movement Disorder in Older Adults�������������������������� 197
Mariana Moscovich, Kathrin LaFaver, and Walter Maetzler

Part III Management

17 “Breaking the News” of a Functional Movement Disorder �������� 207


Jon Stone, Ingrid Hoeritzauer, and Alan Carson
18 Motivational Interviewing for Functional Movement
Disorder�������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 223
Benjamin Tolchin and Steve Martino
19 Communication Challenges in Functional
Movement Disorder ������������������������������������������������������������������������ 237
Gaston Baslet and Barbara A. Dworetzky
20 Developing a Treatment Plan for Functional
Movement Disorder ������������������������������������������������������������������������ 253
Mark J. Edwards
21 Psychological Treatment of Functional Movement Disorder������ 267
Joel D. Mack and W. Curt LaFrance Jr.
22 Psychiatric Comorbidities and the Role of Psychiatry in
Functional Movement Disorder������������������������������������������������������ 291
Kim Bullock and Juliana Lockman
23 Physical Therapy: Retraining Movement�������������������������������������� 311
Paula Gardiner, Julie Maggio, and Glenn Nielsen
24 Occupational Therapy: Focus on Function ���������������������������������� 329
Clare Nicholson and Kate Hayward
25 Speech Therapy: Being Understood Clearly �������������������������������� 341
Jennifer Freeburn
26 Interdisciplinary Rehabilitation Approaches in Functional
Movement Disorder ������������������������������������������������������������������������ 353
Kathrin LaFaver and Lucia Ricciardi
Contents xi

27 Placebo Effects and Functional Neurological Disorder:


Helpful or Harmful?������������������������������������������������������������������������ 367
Matthew J. Burke and Sarah C. Lidstone
28 Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) as Treatment for
Functional Movement Disorder������������������������������������������������������ 379
Daruj Aniwattanapong and Timothy R. Nicholson
29 Measuring Symptoms and Monitoring Progress in Functional
Movement Disorder ������������������������������������������������������������������������ 401
Glenn Nielsen, Susannah Pick, and Timothy R. Nicholson
30 Overcoming Treatment Obstacles in Functional Movement
Disorder�������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 415
Megan E. Jablonski and Adrianne E. Lange
31 Management Considerations for Pediatric Functional
Movement Disorder ������������������������������������������������������������������������ 433
Kasia Kozlowska
32 Choosing a Career in Functional Movement Disorder���������������� 451
Kathrin LaFaver, Carine W. Maurer, Timothy R. Nicholson,
and David L. Perez
Index���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 459
Contributors

Caitlin Adams Department of Psychiatry, North Shore Medical Center,


Salem, MA, USA
Marine Ambar Akkaoui Centre Psychiatrique d’Orientation et d’Accueil
(CPOA), GHU Paris - Psychiatry & Neurosciences, Paris, France
Psychiatric Emergency, CH Delafontaine, Etablissement Publique de Santé
Mentale deVille Evrard, Paris, France
Jordan R. Anderson Psychiatry and Neurology, Oregon Health and Science
University, Unity Center for Behavioral Health, Portland, OR, USA
Daruj Aniwattanapong Neuropsychiatry Research and Education Group,
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London,
London, UK
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Chulalongkorn University,
Bangkok, Thailand
Selma Aybek Neurology, Inselspital, Bern, Switzerland
Gaston Baslet Division of Neuropsychiatry, Brigham and Women’s
Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Kim Bullock Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford
University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA
Matthew J. Burke Neuropsychiatry Program, Department of Psychiatry,
Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON,
Canada
Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, Sunnybrook Health
Sciences Centre, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
Hurvitz Brain Sciences Research Program, Sunnybrook Research Institute,
Toronto, ON, Canada
Program in Placebo Studies, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard
Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Alan Carson Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh,
Edinburgh, UK

xiii
xiv Contributors

W. Curt LaFrance Jr. Brown University, Providence, RI, USA


Providence Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Providence, RI, USA
Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology, Rhode Island Hospital, Providence,
RI, USA
Bertrand Degos Neurology Unit, Avicenne University Hospital, Sorbonne
Paris Nord University, Bobigny, France
Dynamics and Pathophysiology of Neuronal Networks Team, Center for
Interdisciplinary Research in Biology, Collège de France, CNRS UMR7241/
INSERM U1050, Université PSL, Paris, France
Benedetta Demartini Department of Health Sciences, “Aldo Ravelli”
Research Center for Neurotechnology and Experimental Brain Therapeutics,
Università degli Studi di Milano, ASST Santi Paolo e Carlo, Milan, Italy
Yasmine E. M. Dreissen Department of Neurology, Amsterdam University
Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Joseph R. Duffy Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN,
USA
Barbara A. Dworetzky Division of Epilepsy, Brigham and Women’s
Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Mark J. Edwards St George’s University of London, London, UK
Atkinson Morley Regional Neuroscience Centre, St George’s University
Hospital, London, UK
Alfonso Fasano Edmond J. Safra Program in Parkinson’s Disease, Morton
and Gloria Shulman Movement Disorders Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital,
UHN, Toronto, ON, Canada
Division of Neurology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
Krembil Brain Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
Jennifer Freeburn Department of Speech, Language, & Swallowing
Disorders, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
Christos Ganos Department of Neurology, Charité University Medicine
Berlin, Berlin, Germany
Béatrice Garcin Neurology Unit, Avicenne University Hospital, Sorbonne
Paris Nord University, Bobigny, France
Faculty of Medicine of Sorbonne University, Brain and Spine Institute,
INSERM U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Paris, France
Paula Gardiner Royal Infirmary Edinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland, UK
Jeannette M. Gelauff Department of Neurology, Amsterdam University
Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Contributors xv

Mark Hallett Human Motor Control Section, National Institute of


Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda,
MD, USA
Kate Hayward Therapy Services Department, The National Hospital for
Neurology and Neurosurgery, London, UK
Ingrid Hoeritzauer Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of
Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
Megan E. Jablonski Department of Psychology & Neuropsychology,
Frazier Rehabilitation Institute, Louisville, KY, USA
Richard A. A. Kanaan Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne,
Austin Health, Heidelberg, VIC, Australia
Kasia Kozlowska The Children’s Hospital at Westmead, Westmead, NSW,
Australia
Discipline of Psychiatry and Discipline of Child & Adolescent Health,
University of Sydney, Medical School, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Westmead Institute for Medical Research, Westmead, NSW, Australia
Kathrin LaFaver Movement Disorder Specialist, Saratoga Hospital
Medical Group, Saratoga Springs, NY, USA
Adrianne E. Lange Private Practice, Louisville, KY, USA
Sarah C. Lidstone Integrated Movement Disorders Program, Toronto
Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
Edmond J. Safra Program in Parkinson’s Disease and the Morton and Gloria
Shulman Movement Disorders Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University
Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, University of Toronto,
Toronto, ON, Canada
Juliana Lockman Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences,
Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA
Lindsey MacGillivray Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto,
Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
Joel D. Mack Department of Psychiatry, Veterans Affairs Portland Health
Care System, Portland, OR, USA
Northwest Parkinson’s Disease Research, Education, and Clinical Center,
Department of Neurology, Veterans Affairs Portland Health Care System,
Portland, OR, USA
Departments of Psychiatry & Neurology, Oregon Health and Science
University, Portland, OR, USA
Walter Maetzler Department of Neurology, University Hospital Schleswig-­
Holstein, Campus Kiel, Kiel, Germany
xvi Contributors

Julie Maggio Department of Physical Therapy, Massachusetts General


Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
Tina Mainka Department of Neurology, Charité University Medicine
Berlin, Berlin, Germany
Berlin Institute of Health, Berlin, Germany
Steve Martino Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of
Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Psychology Service, VA Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, CT,
USA
Carine W. Maurer Department of Neurology, Renaissance School of
Medicine at Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Francesca Morgante Neurosciences Research Centre, Molecular and
Clinical Sciences Research Institute, St. George’s University of London,
London, UK
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Messina,
Messina, Italy
Mariana Moscovich Department of Neurology, University Hospital
Schleswig-Holstein, Campus Kiel, Kiel, Germany
Clare Nicholson Therapy Services Department, The National Hospital for
Neurology and Neurosurgery, London, UK
Timothy R. Nicholson Neuropsychiatry Research and Education Group,
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London,
London, UK
Glenn Nielsen Neuroscience Research Centre, Institute of Molecular and
Clinical Sciences, St Georges University of London, London, UK
David L. Perez Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Massachusetts
General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Susannah Pick Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience,
King’s College London, London, UK
Bruce H. Price Neurology, McLean Hospital and Massachusetts General
Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA, USA
Lucia Ricciardi Neurosciences Research Centre, Molecular and Clinical
Sciences Research Institute, St George’s University of London, London, UK
Mohammad Rohani Division of Neurology, Rasool Akram Hospital,
School of Medicine, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
Petra Schwingenschuh Department of Neurology, Medical University of
Graz, Graz, Austria
Jon Stone Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh,
Edinburgh, UK
Contributors xvii

Marina A. J. Tijssen Expertise Center Movement Disorders, Department of


Neurology, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
Benjamin Tolchin Comprehensive Epilepsy Center, Department of
Neurology, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Epilepsy Center of Excellence, Neurology Service, VA Connecticut
Healthcare System, West Haven, CT, USA
Jeff L. Waugh Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas Southwestern,
Dallas, TX, USA
Alison Wilkinson-Smith Department of Psychiatry, Children’s Medical
Center Dallas, Dallas, TX, USA
Part I
Framework
A Historical Perspective
on Functional Neurological
1
Disorder

Richard A. A. Kanaan

A history of one of mankind’s oldest maladies So, in addition to celebrating its birth, I will dis-
would be a monumental work, but that is not this cuss what the differences in FND are, and what
chapter. This chapter explores the history of func- difference they may make.
tional neurological disorder (FND), a malady of
much more recent birth. How recent? A search on
PubMed for “functional neurological disorder” The Way It Was
suggests its first English-language appearance was
perhaps in 2015 [1]. That year there were 2 publi- It would be hard to exaggerate how dire the situ-
cations using the term; the following year there ation of conversion disorder was at the turn of the
were 6, and a further 9 using the Americanized millennium. After a burst of popularity 100 years
“neurologic”. In 2016, mere months after this first earlier, when some of the greatest minds in medi-
appearance, the authoritative 700-page handbook cine (including leading neurologists and psychia-
of clinical neurology on the subject chose trists) shared a fascination for the disorder at the
“Functional Neurologic Disorders” as its title [2]. Salpêtrière Hospital in Paris (Fig. 1.1), it had
Strikingly, for a term that was not the official sunk into a prolonged and profound decline.
choice of DSM-5, ICD-10, or the proposed ICD-­ Psychiatry seemed to have found a way to ignore
11, it (“FND”) was incorporated into the name of it entirely, announcing its disappearance [3];
every patient group in the field. FND had very Neurologists could only wish it had disappeared,
clearly arrived, seemingly out of the blue. left grappling with a problem they did not under-
But this magical manifestation is of course an stand, and did not think was really theirs [4].
illusion, and in the following I will explore the Clinicians of all specialties would describe the
history of its arrival and its conquest. In discuss- disorder with dislike, or worse, and question
ing ‘FND’ as opposed to ‘hysteria’, or ‘conver- whether it was really all just deliberate feigning
sion disorder’, I am not indulging in mere [5, 6]. For a disorder of such prevalence and such
pedantry: as any historian will tell you, one can- morbidity, clinical research was astonishingly
not simply transplant the concepts of the present sparse, with not one single properly-powered
into the past – and, make no mistake, the concept clinical trial ever conducted during the twentieth
of FND involves profound changes from the past. century, and a medley of treatment approaches
proliferated, based on the lowest grades of evi-
R. A. A. Kanaan (*) dence [7]. The public, meanwhile, was barely
Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, aware of its existence: though ‘hysteria’ remained
Austin Health, Heidelberg, VIC, Australia a popular cultural motif, it was viewed almost
e-mail: richard.kanaan@unimelb.edu.au

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 3


K. LaFaver et al. (eds.), Functional Movement Disorder, Current Clinical Neurology,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-86495-8_1
4 R. A. A. Kanaan

Fig. 1.1 Jean-Martin Charcot’s Lectures on the Diseases Hysterics (1892) and Sigmund Freud’s Studies on
of the Nervous System (1872), and two of the critical Hysteria (1895). (Images are available in the public
works of his students, Pierre Janet’s The Mental State of domain)

exclusively as an historical artefact of medical Out With the Old…


misogyny [8] or Victorian absurdity [9]. The pub-
lic shared in clinical suspicions of feigning [10], The diagnostic criteria reflected these problems,
and patients newly diagnosed with conversion and appeared to play a critical role. ICD-10 [13]
disorder would be so outraged they would go to and DSM-IV [14] shared the view that conver-
any lengths to have the diagnosis overturned sion disorder required the exclusion of both neu-
[11]. Unsurprisingly, there were no conversion rological and factitious alternatives, and the
disorder patient advocacy groups, and public inclusion of a psychiatric explanation (see Box
demand for services in many places was non- 1.1). There were real problems with each of these
existent [12]. [15, 16].
1 A Historical Perspective on Functional Neurological Disorder 5

The exclusion of neurological alternatives was


Box 1.1 Comparison of the diagnostic not a problem in practice - despite the alarming
criteria for Conversion Disorder in the reports of large-scale neurological misdiagnosis
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM), from the 1960s [17], overall, and particularly
4th and 5th editions more recently, conversion disorder was not mis-
DSM-5 Conversion Disorder (Functional diagnosed more often than other disorders [18] -
Neurological Symptom Disorder) - 2013 but it remained a serious problem in principle.
For it meant that the neurologist would be
A. One or more symptoms of altered vol- required to exclude every alternative - known or
untary motor or sensory function. unknown - something that appears impossible in
B. Clinical findings provide evidence of principle, and a concerned patient could never be
incompatibility between the symptom sure that something had not been overlooked, or
and recognised neurological or medical had not yet been discovered. A patient’s confi-
conditions. dence in the diagnosis would depend on their
C. The symptom or deficit is not better confidence in their neurologist and second- or
explained by another medical or mental third- neurological opinions might seem only
disorder. prudent.
D. The symptom or deficit causes clini- The exclusion of feigning, or factitious disor-
cally significant distress or impairment der, was problematic both in principle and prac-
in social, occupational, or other impor- tice. In practice, it is usually impossible for a
tant areas of functioning or warrants psychiatrist, or anyone else, to exclude feigning,
medical evaluation. particularly with neurological symptoms [19]. In
principle, that this was an exclusion criterion for
DSM-IV Conversion Disorder - 1994 conversion disorder alone served to confirm all
those suspicions that conversion disorder was
A. One or more symptoms or deficits uniquely close to feigning [20]. As far as a neu-
affecting voluntary motor or sensory rologist need be concerned [21], or anyone could
function suggesting neurological or prove, they might all be feigning [22].
other general medical condition. The requirement for a psychiatric formulation
B. Psychological factors are judged to be was primarily a problem in practice. There were
associated to the symptom or deficit always patients in whom no plausible psychiatric
because conflicts or other stressors pre- explanation could be constructed, which meant
cede initiation or exacerbation of the the requirement was effectively ignored. Perhaps
symptom or deficit. this was due to these patients’ reluctance to dis-
C. The patient is not feigning or intention- close psychopathology [23], or clinician reluc-
ally producing his or her symptoms or tance to explore it [24] – or perhaps because it
deficits. was not present. But in almost every study, how-
D. The symptom or deficit cannot, after ever it was studied, a relevant trauma history
appropriate investigation, be fully could not be identified in a significant minority of
explained by a general medical condi- patients [25]. Though trauma was obviously still
tion, by the direct effects of a substance, important, this also challenged the principle: it
or as a culturally sanctioned behaviour was difficult to support a monolithic, PTSD-type
or experience. trauma model for conversion disorder when most
E. The symptom is not limited to pain or studies appeared to provide evidence against it.
to a disturbance in sexual functioning So, the diagnostic criteria brought the disor-
and is not better explained by another der into disrepute while being of little practical
mental disorder. value. They drew heavily on psychodynamic
ideas that, if not exactly disproven, were cer-
6 R. A. A. Kanaan

tainly old-­fashioned, and perhaps no longer There was a real reputational danger there, as
readily interpretable by most psychiatrists [9]. outlined above: a criterion which depended on a
There was a once-in-a-generation opportunity neurologist’s ability or zeal in finding an alterna-
for change, with both DSM-5 and ICD-11 in tive explanation risked becoming the wastebasket
preparation. for every patient the neurologist did not take seri-
ously or whose symptoms they could not quite
put together. The psychiatric aetiology require-
… in With the New ment had protected against that, in theory, since
the patient has to pass that test as well, and with-
For such ‘old-fashioned’ ideas to be so enduring out it the risk would be clearly exposed. There
suggests that replacing them would not be was a solution, however: make the neurologist
straightforward, however. They certainly had prove that the case was functional – make a ‘posi-
high-level endorsement, after all, and perhaps tive diagnosis’. If they could identify features
there were simply no better alternatives. That the specific to the diagnosis that would mean they
neurological symptoms would not fit with identi- could demonstrate what it was, not just what it
fiable pathology was established by Jean-Martin was not, and that risk would be removed.
Charcot (Fig. 1.1), arguably the greatest neurolo- Or nearly so. Unfortunately, distinguishing it
gist of all time, in the nineteenth century [26]. from feigning would be as difficult as ever for
The suspicions around feigning were part of the most features of conversion disorder [30], but if
same discussion. Neurologists at the time were excluding feigning was removed from the crite-
inclined to explain these apparently voluntary ria, and feigning no longer considered as a likely
movements as malingering [27], since no better alternative, the new criterion could still be con-
explanation was on offer [3]. The idea that these sidered ‘positive’ in regards to neurological alter-
symptoms would be instead ‘psychogenic’ dates natives. And this ‘positive diagnosis’ was to
from the same era, most famously in Sigmund become the new principal diagnostic criterion in
Freud’s seminal “Studies on Hysteria”, where he DSM-5 (Box 1.1).
essentially argues for a post-traumatic aetiol- But there were more changes in store. The
ogy – the novelty being that the traumas were name ‘conversion disorder’ still clearly harked
‘emotional’ ones (psychological conflicts), which back to Freud, and suggested a “one size fits all”
became ‘converted’ into physical symptoms [28]. psychodynamic process, even if the criteria did
This construction survived largely intact for a not. Neurologists had been using the term ‘func-
century in conversion disorder, while all around it tional’ to refer to these symptoms since at least
every other psychodynamic construction was the time of Charcot, who argued that any lesion
being torn down [9]. Why it showed such unique invisible to his microscopes must be a ‘func-
longevity is unclear, given the ethos of the times tional’ lesion of the underlying brain region [31].
was so strongly against aetiological criteria. Though the meaning of the term was opaque
Perhaps the loss of psychiatric interest allowed it [32], it appeared to be a popular alternative with
to ‘slip under the radar’. Or perhaps it just could both patients and neurologists [33]. We, and oth-
not be understood psychiatrically without it. The ers, suggested ‘functional neurological symp-
symptoms, after all, were not psychiatric: what toms’ [15], or ‘functional neurological symptom
else was there about the disorder that would jus- disorder’ [16], which latter DSM-5 finally
tify it being a psychiatric condition, if not its aeti- adopted. But in an early draft, perhaps following
ology [29]? Suggestions for improving the an earlier suggestion [34], it proposed ‘functional
criteria by simply removing the requirement for a neurological disorder’ – a small difference, but
psychiatric formulation [15] would have left it one which shifted the emphasis from the symp-
looking distinctly neurological, but a neurologi- toms being neurological to the disorder being
cal syndrome for which no neurological explana- neurological, a shift too far for most neurologists
tion could be found. [20, 32]. Yet, this is the name that emerged trium-
1 A Historical Perspective on Functional Neurological Disorder 7

phant, despite DSM-5’s choice, and has become The embrace of the disorder by patient groups
the standard bearer for the disorder: as of March represents a tremendous advance for the field. As
2020, a search on PubMed for “Functional noted in the introduction, they have universally
Neurological Disorder” yields 55 references adopted FND into the names of their groups,
since 2019, while “Functional Neurological which seems unlikely to be a coincidence. Patients
Symptom Disorder” yields only seven. have consistently expressed a preference for the
With regard to movement disorders more spe- term ‘functional’ over the more psychiatric sound-
cifically, the subject of this book, similar changes ing alternatives [33], but clearly the groups also
were underway. Long known as ‘psychogenic preferred ‘FND’ over ‘FNSD’ - the advantage,
movement disorder’, arguments were made on other than perhaps euphony, presumably being
the same grounds [35] that they should instead be that it more strongly implies the disorder is neuro-
known as ‘functional movement disorder’ (FMD) logical. The stigma of a psychiatric diagnosis is
[36]. Turning again to PubMed, ‘functional unfortunately commonplace, and has very real
movement disorder’ first appears in 2012 [37], consequences [39], but in the case of FND it
though a publication a year earlier uses the term appears bound up with the idea that the disorder is
with ‘functional’ in scare quotes [38]. But it has not real – is imaginary, or hallucinatory, or simply
already come to dominate the field: since 2019, made up [10]. Though these attributions are not
as of May 2020, “functional movement disorder” fixed [40], they are virtually absent from neuro-
yields 25 references, and “psychogenic move- logical disorders: a shift to a neurological disorder
ment disorder” only six. definitively makes the disorder seem more ‘real’.
Dropping the ‘exclusion of feigning’ criterion
should also have helped in that regard. Patient
What Difference Does It Make? groups also expressed concern with the psycho-
genic diagnostic criterion, and its implication that
Much has changed since the turn of the millen- they had suffered a trauma, when not all reported
nium. Clinical interest in the disorder has grown such experiences [29] - so dropping that criterion
considerably – the first international conference should also be welcome. Finally, a ‘positive’ dem-
(2017) open to all was hugely over-subscribed. onstration of FND could only help patient accep-
A vibrant research community has developed tance, alleviating doubts about the diagnosis
and properly-powered clinical trials are finally being an excuse for medical incompetence or a
underway. Public interest is increasing. Writing way of dismissing unwelcome patients.
from Australia, FND has featured on 3 prime- There were clear potential advantages for neu-
time current affairs TV programs in the last few rologists too. Conversion disorder was a source
years, having never featured before. And patient of great anxiety to neurologists, who found their
attitudes are changing – most patients in my patient encounters uniquely uncomfortable [4].
clinic have now actively sought an appointment Their diagnostic approach of discovering incon-
in the clinic, instead of grudgingly accepting sistency encouraged them to consider their
their neurologist’s referral, or failing to attend patients as deceptive, while also involving the
once they understand what it’s about – a posi- neurologist in a deception - of tricking their
tion colleagues tell me is true more globally. patients into revealing the truth [22]. But when it
The world of FND is a clearly different world to came to explaining their diagnosis, it involved
the world of Conversion Disorder. It’s different them in discussions of such concepts as the sub-
in its appeal, to neurologists and patients in par- conscious, on which they felt unqualified; these
ticular. We cannot be sure what caused these could be interpreted as implying feigning, or dis-
changes, and it seems unlikely that it was any missal, or incompetence, and greeted with anger
one thing, but it is easy to see the new criteria [24]. The new criteria were in these areas a major
and the new name as either causes or embodi- advance. All mention of feigning was dropped, so
ments of this difference. that the ‘positive’ demonstration could be inter-
8 R. A. A. Kanaan

preted as of FND alone. Moreover, this meant the of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) or
inconsistencies could be shown to the patient, physiotherapy most keenly may be the psychia-
removing any deception on the neurologists’ trists and psychologists who thought that ‘con-
part, and, in an inspired maneuver, turning these version disorder’ was fundamentally more valid,
‘tricks’ into ways of creating patient insight and for all its flaws. Interviews with psychiatrists who
support for the diagnosis [41]. Without the need specialize in the disorder suggests considerable
to discuss some psychogenic aetiology, neurolo- discomfort with the new approach – that they cer-
gists could stay within a zone more comfortable tainly do not all think it is valid [47]. A survey of
for them and the patient, so that their encounters psychiatrists in the UK and Australia confirmed
need no longer be hostile [42]. this, with respondents overwhelmingly reporting
they think conversion disorder is psychogenic,
and that a psychiatric formulation is not merely
So, Are the Problems All Over Now? helpful for management, but essential to the diag-
nosis [48].
The above-discussed developments represent Thirdly, the new criteria will not diagnose the
critical, and very welcome, changes. They have same patients as before. Any change in the sense
already transformed the disorder and have the of the criteria will inevitably affect their refer-
potential to solve most of the problems that con- ence – the patients whom they do or do not fit.
version disorder faced. But it would be naïve to One obvious change from an aetiological crite-
think this most intractable of medical conun- rion to a symptomatic criterion is that a larger
drums ‘finally sorted’, and these changes may yet number of seemingly straightforwardly neuro-
lead to new problems. logical patients will now be included if they have
Firstly, the thrust of the changes can be seen as any symptoms that are functional. Previously, the
rendering the disorder more neurological. The patient with a stroke who also developed a func-
‘psychogenic’ criterion is no longer essential; the tional weakness need not have been diagnosed
diagnosis can now be made by neurologists with conversion disorder if the psychiatrist did
alone; the diagnosis appears in the neurological not formulate them as such: now, a ‘positive sign’
section of ICD-11 as well [43]. The name FND, should be enough to make the diagnosis, as long
as we noted before, now appears to state that it is as it is enough to cause distress. This view, of the
so. That initial alarm about the name has qui- symptom as sufficient, is not new [17], and not
etened. A steady stream of neuroimaging studies, wrong, but it clearly broadens the number of
while not, as a class, telling us anything we did potential FND patients enormously. Conversely,
not already know [44], make a neurological view the diagnosis was ‘positive’ before, in theory, in
of the disorder seem more comfortable, and more terms of a ‘positive psychiatric formulation’, and
natural. This is no bad thing, inherently. The divi- removing this will exclude others. There will be
sion of disorders into psychiatric and neurologi- patients who do have a positive psychiatric for-
cal has always been somewhat arbitrary, and mulation but in whom no ‘positive signs’ are
many have suggested the two should be consid- found, so would no longer be diagnosed with
ered one. But it does encourage the view that the FND. Just how many of these there may be is not
psychiatric aspects are secondary, and can per- clear, as large-scale studies have not been con-
haps be ignored in management. We are currently ducted outside of specialist centres, but it could
riding a wave of optimism that a neurological be a lot. One recent study, for example, suggested
model will prevail [45], and that physical treat- that most paediatric patients diagnosed with FND
ments may be enough, at least for most patients did not meet that criterion [49]. Some of the ‘pos-
[46]. They may not, and the effect of that failure itive signs’ are of doubtful discriminatory value
on ‘FND’ as a concept is hard to predict. [50], and certainty is typically rationed in neuro-
Secondly, not everyone will be happy. Among logical diagnoses: insisting on certainty, or on
those who will be looking to the results of trials only the most valid signs, would doubtless lead to
1 A Historical Perspective on Functional Neurological Disorder 9

a more tightly defined, but inevitably smaller


group, particularly in FMD [51]. Inevitably, some Summary
patients with Conversion Disorder diagnoses are • Conversion Disorder was profoundly
going to miss out by the new criteria. Again, this neglected and stigmatized by clinicians,
is not necessarily wrong, and we cannot conclude patients and the public 20 years ago.
from this that the new criteria are more or less • The diagnostic criteria were a major
valid than the old, but one of the problems with contributor to this, based on principles
the old psychogenic criterion was that it could established in the nineteenth century.
not be fulfilled in practice, and was therefore rou- • The criteria of DSM-IV made a trauma
tinely ignored. The danger for the new criterion is history essential, which not all patients
precisely the same. And for FND, like Conversion had, and required the exclusion of neu-
disorder before it, the danger is that it may there- ropathology and of feigning, which was
fore become dependent on an idea that nobody not usually possible.
really believes any longer. • The revised criteria for DSM-5 dropped
This chapter has focused heavily on DSM-5 these requirements, instead requiring
in the preceding. It is of course only one itera- the diagnostician to make a rule-in diag-
tion of that classification system, and only one nosis, by demonstrating incompatibility
of the systems in use. ICD-11 has trodden a dif- or finding ‘positive signs’.
ferent path, in nomenclature and criteria, based • The name ‘functional neurological dis-
on the principle of dissociation espoused by the order’ is not the official name in DSM-­
great rival theorist from the Salpêtrière, Pierre 5, but was proposed in an early draft,
Janet (Fig. 1.1). If anyone needed remind- and has been adopted by the field despite
ing, Freud’s is not the only way to formu- concern that it suggests the disorder is
late a patient with ‘Dissociative Neurological solely a neurological disorder.
Symptom Disorder’, as ICD-11 would have it • The name and the new criteria have
[43], and this history is not simply of the strug- proven very popular, and the diagnosis
gle between the psychodynamic and biological is experiencing a transformation in pub-
views that is the particularly American history lic awareness and patient acceptability.
of psychiatry [9]. That struggle may obscure • As we consider the present and future of
the extent of agreement. As the later chapters FND, an appreciation of neurological
in this book will show, life events and the bio- signs and psychiatric formulation are
psychosocial model remain at the forefront of likely both to be important in the assess-
modern FMD conceptualizations. And even ment and management of this condition
DSM-5 is perhaps less definitive than its crite- at the interface of neurology and
ria may suggest: the title of the disorder retains psychiatry.
both Conversion Disorder and FNSD (indeed,
with FNSD in parentheses), and the accompa-
nying text make very clear the importance of
psychiatric formulation. Perhaps the pendulum, References
restrained for so long, swung further than any-
1. Perez DL, Dworetzky BA, Dickerson BC, Leung L,
one quite intended with this version. Perhaps Cohn R, Baslet G, et al. An integrative neurocircuit
the next iteration will more faithfully capture perspective on psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and
the diversity of the disorder, and of opinion. functional movement disorders: neural functional
unawareness. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2015;46(1):4–15.
Perhaps it will restore psychiatric formulation 2. Hallett M, Stone J, Carson A. Functional neurologic
to an equal footing with neurological signs, as disorders. Amsterdam/New York: Elsevier; 2016.
factors which contribute to, but do not define, xviii, 662 pages p
FND - and confirm FND as a core, interdisci- 3. Kanaan RA, Wessely SC. The origins of factitious
disorder. Hist Hum Sci. 2010;23(2):68–85.
plinary neuropsychiatric disorder [52].
10 R. A. A. Kanaan

4. Kanaan R, Armstrong D, Barnes P, Wessely S. In the assessment of malingering and deception. 4th ed.
psychiatrist's chair: how neurologists understand con- New York: The Guilford Press; 2018. p. 236–42.
version disorder. Brain. 2009;132(Pt 10):2889–96. 23. Stone J, Warlow C, Sharpe M. The symptom of func-
5. Ahern L, Stone J, Sharpe MC. Attitudes of neurosci- tional weakness: a controlled study of 107 patients.
ence nurses toward patients with conversion symp- Brain. 2010;133(Pt 5):1537–51.
toms. Psychosomatics. 2009;50(4):336–9. 24. Kanaan R, Armstrong D, Wessely S. Limits to truth-­
6. Edwards MJ, Stone J, Nielsen G. Physiotherapists telling: neurologists' communication in conversion
and patients with functional (psychogenic) motor disorder. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;77(2):296–301.
symptoms: a survey of attitudes and interest. J Neurol 25. Ludwig L, Pasman JA, Nicholson T, Aybek S, David
Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012;83(6):655–8. AS, Tuck S, et al. Stressful life events and maltreat-
7. Ruddy R, House A. Psychosocial interventions for ment in conversion (functional neurological) disorder:
conversion disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. systematic review and meta-analysis of case-control
2005;(4):CD005331. studies. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(4):307–20.
8. Micale MS. Approaching hysteria : disease and its 26. Charcot JM. Clinical lectures on diseases of the ner-
interpretations. Princeton/Chichester: Princeton vous system. London: New Sydenham Society; 1889.
University Press; 1995. xii, 327p 27. Freud S. Charcot. In: Freud S, Freud A, Strachey A,
9. Kanaan RA. Freud's hysteria and its legacy. Handb Strachey J, Tyson AW, editors. The standard edition of
Clin Neurol. 2016;139:37–44. the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud.
10. Stone J, Wojcik W, Durrance D, Carson A, Lewis III. London: Hogarth Press; 1953.
S, MacKenzie L, et al. What should we say to 28. Breuer J, Freud S. Studies on Hysteria. In: Freud S,
patients with symptoms unexplained by dis- Freud A, Strachey A, Strachey J, Tyson AW, editors.
ease? The "number needed to offend". Br Med J. The standard edition of the complete psychological
2002;325(7378):1449–50. works of Sigmund Freud. II. London: Hogarth Press;
11. Crimlisk HL, Bhatia KP, Cope H, David AS, Marsden 1953.
D, Ron MA. Patterns of referral in patients with medi- 29. Kanaan RAA, Craig TKJ. Conversion disor-
cally unexplained motor symptoms. J Psychosom der and the trouble with trauma. Psychol Med.
Res. 2000;49(3):217–9. 2019;49(10):1585–8.
12. Kanaan RA. Conversion disorder: who cares? 30. Stone J, Mutch J, Giannokous D, Hoeritzauer I,
Australas Psychiatry. 2018;26(4):344–6. Carson A. Hurst revisited: are symptoms and signs of
13. WHO. The ICD-10 classification of mental and functional motor and sensory disorders "dependent on
behavioural disorders : clinical descriptions and diag- idea"? J Neurol Sci. 2017;381:188–91.
nostic guidelines. WHO; 1992. 31. Trimble MR. Functional diseases. Br Med J.
14. APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis- 1982;285:1768–70.
orders : DSM-IV. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American 32. Kanaan RA, Armstrong D, Wessely SC. The func-
Psychiatric Association; 1994. xxvii, 886 p tion of 'functional': a mixed methods investiga-
15. Kanaan RA, Carson A, Wessely SC, Nicholson TR, tion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012;83(3):
Aybek S, David AS. What's so special about conver- 248–50.
sion disorder? A problem and a proposal for diagnos- 33. Ding JM, Kanaan RA. Conversion disorder: a sys-
tic classification. Br J Psychiatry. 2010;196(6):427–8. tematic review of current terminology. Gen Hosp
16. Stone J, LaFrance WC Jr, Brown R, Spiegel D, Psychiatry. 2017;45:51–5.
Levenson JL, Sharpe M. Conversion disorder: cur- 34. Stone J, LaFrance WC Jr, Levenson JL, Sharpe
rent problems and potential solutions for DSM-5. J M. Issues for DSM-5: conversion disorder. Am J
Psychosom Res. 2011;71(6):369–76. Psychiatry. 2010;167(6):626–7.
17. Slater E. Diagnosis of "Hysteria". Br Med J. 35. Stone J, Edwards MJ. How "psychogenic" are
1965;5447:1395–9. psychogenic movement disorders? Mov Disord.
18. Stone J, Smyth R, Carson A, Lewis S, Prescott R, 2011;26(10):1787–8.
Warlow C, et al. Systematic review of misdiagno- 36. Edwards MJ, Stone J, Lang AE. From psychogenic
sis of conversion symptoms and "hysteria". BMJ. movement disorder to functional movement dis-
2005;331(7523):989. order: it's time to change the name. Mov Disord.
19. Kanaan RA, Wessely SC. Factitious disorders in neu- 2014;29(7):849–52.
rology: an analysis of reported cases. Psychosomatics. 37. Czarnecki K, Thompson JM, Seime R, Geda YE,
2010;51(1):47–54. Duffy JR, Ahlskog JE. Functional movement disor-
20. Kanaan RA, Armstrong D, Wessely SC. Neurologists' ders: successful treatment with a physical therapy
understanding and management of conversion disor- rehabilitation protocol. Parkinsonism Relat Disord.
der. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2011;82(9):961–6. 2012;18(3):247–51.
21. Hallett M. Psychogenic movement disorders: a 38. Das P, Shinozaki G, McAlpine D. Post-pump chorea-
crisis for neurology. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. -choreiform movements developing after pulmo-
2006;6(4):269–71. nary thromboendarterectomy for chronic pulmonary
22. Kanaan RAA. Conversion disorder and illness decep- hypertension presenting as "functional" movement
tion. In: Rogers R, Bender SD, editors. Clinical disorder. Psychosomatics. 2011;52(5):459–62.
1 A Historical Perspective on Functional Neurological Disorder 11

39. Sykes R. Medically unexplained symptoms and 47. Kanaan RA, Armstrong D, Wessely S. The role of
the Siren “Psychogenic Inference”. Phil Psychiatr psychiatrists in diagnosing conversion disorder: a
Psychol. 2010;17(4):289–99. mixed-methods analysis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat.
40. Kanaan RAA, Ding JM. Who thinks functional 2016;12:1181–4.
neurological symptoms are feigned, and what can 48. Dent B, Stanton B, Kanaan RA. Psychiatrists’ under-
we do about it? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. standing and management of conversion disorder: a
2017;88(6):533–4. bi-national survey and comparison with neurologists.
41. Stone J, Edwards M. Trick or treat? Showing patients Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. in press
with functional (psychogenic) motor symptoms their 49. Watson C, Sivaswamy L, Agarwal R, Du W, Agarwal
physical signs. Neurology. 2012;79(3):282–4. R. Functional neurologic symptom disorder in chil-
42. Carson A, Lehn A, Ludwig L, Stone J. Explaining dren: clinical features, diagnostic investigations, and
functional disorders in the neurology clinic: a photo outcomes at a Tertiary Care Children's Hospital. J
story. Pract Neurol. 2016;16(1):56–61. Child Neurol. 2019;34(6):325–31.
43. WHO. ICD-11: international statistical classification 50. Daum C, Hubschmid M, Aybek S. The value of 'posi-
of disease and related health problems: eleventh revi- tive' clinical signs for weakness, sensory and gait
sion. World Health Organisation; 2018. disorders in conversion disorder: a systematic and
44. Kanaan RAA, McGuire PK. Conceptual challenges narrative review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry.
in the neuroimaging of psychiatric disorders. Philos 2014;85(2):180–90.
Psychiatry Psychol. 2011;18(4):323–32. 51. Morgante F, Edwards MJ, Espay AJ, Fasano A, Mir
45. Edwards MJ, Adams RA, Brown H, Parees I, P, Martino D, et al. Diagnostic agreement in patients
Friston KJ. A Bayesian account of 'hysteria'. Brain. with psychogenic movement disorders. Mov Disord.
2012;135(Pt 11):3495–512. 2012;27(4):548–52.
46. Nielsen G, Stone J, Buszewicz M, Carson A, Goldstein 52. Perez DL, Aybek S, Nicholson TR, Kozlowska K,
LH, Holt K, et al. Physio4FMD: protocol for a multi- Arciniegas DB, LaFrance WC Jr. Functional neu-
centre randomised controlled trial of specialist phys- rological (conversion) disorder: a core neuropsy-
iotherapy for functional motor disorder. BMC Neurol. chiatric disorder. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci.
2019;19(1):242. 2020;32(1):1–3.
Free Will, Emotions and Agency:
Pathophysiology of Functional
2
Movement Disorder

Mark Hallett

Introduction that patients with FMD can have another disor-


der as well, and the FMD would refer to only
In many movement disorders, the brain is those symptoms due to reversibly altered brain
hijacked by a neurodegenerative process or a function.
structural lesion and does not function normally. Abnormal movements in FMD are perceived
In some neuropsychiatric conditions, the brain is as involuntary; generally, patients will say that
so altered that it is not capable of normal func- they have no control over the symptoms. There
tion in certain domains. In a severe stroke, for are two other entities that may look somewhat
example, so many neurons are lost that full similar, with the patients saying that the symp-
recovery is impossible. In functional movement toms are involuntary, but the individuals are
disorder (FMD) the brain also is not functioning feigning, and the movements are actually volun-
normally, but, at least in some circumstances tary. These are factitious disorder, that has an
normal function is possible. Patients may pres- underlying psychiatric disorder, and malingering,
ent with weakness or involuntary movements, that does not [2]. In most medical practice set-
but these symptoms may only occur some of the tings, such patients are not commonly encoun-
time, and other instances the patient can be nor- tered. Better clinical tools and physiological tests
mal. Even during the physical examination, cer- are needed to help differentiate between these
tain maneuvers can reverse the weakness or entities. This chapter will not discuss these other
dampen the involuntary movements. Thus, the diagnoses further, and the pathophysiology of
manifestations are a product of the disordered FMD is certainly distinct from factitious disorder
central nervous system function; hence the name and malingering.
of functional movement disorder. In group anal- The underpinning of any disorder rests on the
yses comparing patients with FMD to those two pillars of etiology and pathophysiology [3].
without, some subtle structural differences have The etiology is the fundamental causes, and the
been found which might represent one end of the pathophysiology (neural mechanisms), which is
normal distribution and, in any event, do not pre- a direct result of the etiology, is what produces
vent normal function [1]. It is important to note the symptoms. This chapter will focus on the
pathophysiology, and the next chapter will focus
M. Hallett (*) on etiological factors. Suffice to say for here that
Human Motor Control Section, National Institute of the etiology is best understood to be multifacto-
Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National rial within the context of the biopsychosocial for-
Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA mulation. These factors can predispose,
e-mail: hallettm@ninds.nih.gov

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 13


K. LaFaver et al. (eds.), Functional Movement Disorder, Current Clinical Neurology,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-86495-8_2
14 M. Hallett

precipitate or perpetuate the FMD. Predisposing the reticulospinal tract. The rubrospinal tract,
factors, such as early life stress, can influence the present in monkeys, has disappeared in humans.
developing central nervous system and render a The reticulospinal tract is primitive and generally
person less resilient to stress later-on in life [4]. just deals with automatic and reflex movements.
It also receives input from the cortex. The cortex
is the main controller of voluntary movement. A
Normal Function small stroke involving just the corticospinal tract
will produce a severe hemiplegia.
In order to understand the pathophysiology, it is The motor part of the corticospinal tract origi-
necessary to understand the normal processes for nates in the primary motor cortex (M1, area 4)
making movement [5] (Fig. 2.1). Movement is with contributions from the premotor cortex
generated by muscles, and the muscles are under (PMC, lateral area 6) and the supplementary
control of the spinal cord (or for the cranial mus- motor area (SMA, medial area 6). Contributions
cles, the brainstem). The spinal cord receives to the corticospinal tract from the post-central
controlling signals in the corticospinal tract and cortex go mostly to the dorsal horn of the spinal

4 Motor nuclei of the


thalamus stimulate upper
1 Motor association motor neurons.
areas select a motor
5 Upper motor neurons
program.
execute the motor program
by sending signals to the
2 Motor association lower motor neurons in the
areas stimulate the spinal cord.
caudate nucleus
and putamen which Thalamus
are facilitated by the
Globus pallidus
substantia nigra
Caudate nucleus Basal
ganglia
Putamen
nuclei
3 The caudate
nucleus and the Substantia nigra
putamen inhibit the in midbrain
globus pallidus.
The globus pallidus Pons
stops inhibiting the
thalamus.
6 The cerebellum monitors
movement and corrects
for motor error

Cerebellum

Medulla oblongata

Fig. 2.1 Illustration of the parts and pathways of the brain that generate the movement command (see text for details).
(Redrawn with modifications from Tortora and Derrickson [6])
2 Free Will, Emotions and Agency: Pathophysiology of Functional Movement Disorder 15

cord and gate sensory information rather than anterior midline structures, the anterior/middle
adding to the motor command. What controls the cingulate, pre-SMA, SMA and then to PMC and
motor cortex, controls movement. Such influ- M1.
ences come from subcortical and cortical If you are hungry and felt rewarded recently in
connections. this situation when eating a chocolate bar, you
The two major subcortical networks involve might go toward the kitchen to find some choco-
the basal ganglia and the cerebellum. Both get late. This is top down control [7]. Seeing the
important input from the cortex and then have chocolate bar then triggers picking it up and eat-
their principal output back to the cortex via the ing it. If the chocolate bar is moldy, then this
thalamus. The loop involving the basal ganglia would discourage eating it. This is bottom up
appears to help the cortex in controlling when control. The details of skilled movements like
and what will be moved, including both whole picking up the chocolate bar are stored in parietal-­
motor programs and the individual muscles that motor pathways, perhaps because they are
contribute to the motor programs. Abnormal learned with trial and error feedback from the
function of the basal ganglia can lead to symp- periphery [8].
toms such as bradykinesia and dyskinesias. The The brain events associated with an internally
loop involving the cerebellum appears to help the triggered, top down movement, activation of the
cortex in the precise control of the spatial-­ SMA and PMC, can be identified in the EEG as
temporal features of movement, and the principal the Bereitschaftspotential (BP), also known, in
result of abnormality is ataxia. There are impor- the English translation, as the readiness potential
tant connections between these loops and the (RP) [9]. This is a slowly rising negative potential
functions may not be as distinct as was once over the anterior midline of the brain. It can be
thought. Also, relevant for aspects covered below, identified in the 1 to 2 seconds prior to the onset
both the basal ganglia and cerebellum are of movement.
involved in non-motor cognitive and affective Some brain events have correlates in con-
processes. sciousness. We do not understand how the con-
The entire cortex provides direct or indirect tents of consciousness arise, but we are all aware
input to the motor areas. Thus, the motor output of them. Each content element is a quale. Goals
at any one time reflects a complex calculation of of behavior are appreciated as intentions, and the
an enormous number of competing influences. final command signal for making a movement is
M1 receives strong input from PMC and SMA, appreciated as willing. The quale of willing often
and the latter two structures appear to synthesize has the sense that the action has been freely cho-
much of this information for M1 as well as con- sen by the person. This is the sense of free will
tribute to the corticospinal tract. As a generality, [10]. When the brain generates goals and motor
the posterior part of the brain provides informa- commands, there are feedforward signals to the
tion about the external world while the anterior inferior parietal lobule and temporoparietal junc-
part of the brain provides information about the tion (TPJ) in the posterior part of the brain. If
internal world, the body and its regulation. The these same regions get sensory input in accord
posterior part of the brain does receive all the with what was willed, this can be an indication
sensory input, visual, somatosensory and audi- that the brain caused that movement. This will be
tory. Information is then routed from there to pre- appreciated as the quale of agency, the person is
frontal and premotor areas where it can influence the agent of the movement, and this is another
motor actions. The anterior part of the brain aspect of the sense of free will. In one study of
receives information about body function, agency, the amount of control of a representation
homeostasis, emotions, and drives such as hun- of a hand on a computer screen by the subject’s
ger, thirst, sex, reward, and therefore is likely to hand was systematically varied. The right TPJ is
develop long and short-term goals. Information an important node in the multimodal integration
from the anterior part of the brain funnels to the network that identifies the match of command
16 M. Hallett

and result [11]. When there is a match, all is well, down” goal-oriented attention. The ventral atten-
and the TPJ is relatively quiet; it becomes more tion network is right lateralized and includes the
active with mismatch. Other relevant areas in the TPJ and the ventral frontal cortex and is thought
network concerned with bodily awareness are the to be involved, bottom-up, with orienting to
right anterior insula, right precuneus, and several salient stimuli. In relation to salience, there is a
regions in the frontal lobe [12]. separate, but related [14], salience network
The brain can focus on only one (or perhaps a including the anterior insula and dorsal anterior
few) thing at a time. This includes the qualia. The cingulate as well as some subcortical structures
focusing mechanism is called attention, and this including the amygdala, substantia nigra and
is a distributed system in the brain with at least periaqueductal gray. For an illustration of these
two networks that have been defined, dorsal and and other brain circuits implicated in the patho-
ventral [13]. The dorsal attention network is physiology of FMD, see Fig. 2.2.
bilateral including the intraparietal sulcus and the There is another important aspect to brain
junction of the precentral and superior frontal function that appears relevant. The brain creates
sulcus (in the region of the frontal eye fields) and reality for each person on an individual basis,
is said to be involved with voluntary or “top-­ framed as its predictive capabilities. Perception

Fig. 2.2 Display of brain circuits (and related constructs) Similarly, the amygdala is part of both the salience and
that are emerging as important in the pathophysiology of limbic networks. Prefrontal brain regions are intercon-
functional neurological disorder (FND). As depicted, nected with striatal-thalamic areas (not shown), and these
FND is a multi-network disorder involving abnormalities pathways should also be factored into the neural circuitry
within and across brain circuits implicated in self-agency, of FND. TPJ indicates temporoparietal junction, FEF
emotion processing, attention, homeostatic balance, frontal eye fields, dlPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,
interoception, multimodal integration, and cognitive/ pgACC perigenual anterior cingulate cortex, sgACC sub-
motor control among other functions. Circuits are genual anterior cingulate cortex, OFC orbitofrontal cor-
described by their related dysfunction in the pathophysiol- tex, SMA supplementary motor area, AMY amygdala,
ogy of FND. It should also be noted that several areas cut HYP hypothalamus, PAG periaqueductal gray. (From:
across multiple networks; for example, the dorsal anterior Drane DL, Fani N, Hallett M, Khalsa SS, Perez DL,
insula is most strongly interconnected with the dorsal Roberts NA, A framework for understanding the patho-
anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), while the posterior physiology of functional neurological disorder [15], pub-
insula receives afferent projections from the lamina I spi- lished with permission)
nothalamocortical pathway and somatosensory cortices.
2 Free Will, Emotions and Agency: Pathophysiology of Functional Movement Disorder 17

of external and internal stimuli depend not only Hoover sign will show normal strength when the
on the stimulus but on a person’s current belief muscle is acting as an automatic synergist. All
(expectations). Beliefs can be, but are not neces- these clinical features indicate that the motor
sarily, updated by the stimuli. Someone nervous command (M1) and its most proximal control
about catching a cold, when feeling hot may (SMA and PMC) are largely working normally.
believe they have a fever, but might change their
belief when they find out that the room they are in
has a high temperature. This can be called predic- Cortical Motor Areas
tive coding [16]; there is an a priori probability
of how to interpret a stimulus, but after the stimu- In patients with functional weakness, transcranial
lus, the probabilities might be modified. A hypo- magnetic stimulation (TMS) of M1 will produce
thetical extension of this is called active inference a normal response in the somatotopic muscle, a
where a movement can be generated by the brain motor evoked potential (MEP) with normal
in order to modify the environment to generate latency and amplitude [22]. This further confirms
sensory data that would be in accord with an a the integrity of the pathway from M1 to the mus-
priori belief [17]. How predictive coding is cle as anticipated from the clinical observations.
instantiated in the brain is not well established However, modulation of the MEP is abnormal.
but appears to include broadly distributed brain Normally, if a person thinks about moving a mus-
areas, including frontal areas and multimodal cle (without moving it), the MEP gets larger. In
integration brain areas (TPJ, dorsal anterior cin- patients with functional weakness, the MEP gets
gulate cortex, anterior insula) [18, 19]. Shifting smaller [23, 24]. Thinking about a movement
of a belief utilizes dopaminergic mechanisms should increase M1 activity, but in patients the
[19] and may involve the anterior insula [20]. activity decreases. A similar phenomenon can be
A summary of normal motor function as seen with fMRI; trying to make a movement pro-
described in this section is illustrated in a block duced a deactivation of the motor cortex [25].
diagram in Fig. 2.3. Thus, there is some top down process that is
inhibiting M1. There have been other functional
imaging studies in patients attempting to make
Pathophysiology of FMD movements that have not occurred. In these stud-
ies, dysfunctional activation is seen in the frontal
The involuntary movements in patients with lobes [26], and the frontal areas are particularly
FMD look like voluntary movements but lack the strongly connected to the “paretic” motor cortex
sense of willing and self-agency [21]. For exam- [27].
ple, they never have the quick, simple appearance The physiology of motor preparation in
of cortical myoclonus or a tremor faster than patients with functional weakness has been stud-
10 Hz. A tremor in one arm will either entrain or ied with the contingent negative variation (CNV),
stop with rhythmic tapping of the other arm as a widespread cortical negativity measured with
would happen with voluntary production of alter- EEG in between a warning stimulus (S1) and the
nating movements [21]. The EMG of the muscle go stimulus (S2). Patients with unilateral func-
activity looks like that of voluntary movement. tional weakness were compared to normal sub-
Stimulus sensitive functional myoclonus is at the jects performing normally and normal subjects
latency, and has the variability, of a voluntary feigning weakness [28]. A low amplitude CNV
reaction time movement. With functional weak- was found only for the symptomatic hand of the
ness, improved strength can be demonstrated by FMD patients. The CNV was analyzed in patients
changing the task and shifting the focus of atten- with hyperkinetic FMD and the CNV was also
tion. A patient who does not have any plantar low in that situation, even if the movement did
flexion force might be able to walk on his toes. not affect the limb being studied [29].
Additionally, exam findings such as a positive Interestingly, the CNV normalized in those
18 M. Hallett

Fig. 2.3 Block diagram


of normal brain function
for making movements. Attention
Attention is distributed
to all parts of the
system. See text for
additional details

Prediction Error

Belief
Predictive Coding

Intention
Emotion

Homeostasis
Interoception Movement
Planning

Movement
Generation

Feedforward
Signals

Multimodal
MOVEMENT Integration

Feedback

patients who improved with treatment. Although In patients with functional myoclonus, a
the interpretation of this finding is not completely hyperkinetic movement, analysis of the EEG just
clear, it could be indicative of suppressed motor prior to the movement shows a potential that
preparation in frontal midline structures such as looks like a normal BP, in timing, morphology,
the cingulate area, pre-SMA, and SMA. and amplitude [30]. This indicates that the final
2 Free Will, Emotions and Agency: Pathophysiology of Functional Movement Disorder 19

common pathway to the motor command is simi- important finding, but paradoxical since it might
lar to that of voluntary movement. Thus, not only be expected that involuntary tremor might be
M1, but SMA and PMC are functioning nor- more of a mismatch. Additionally, there were
mally, pushing the origin of the disorder even fur- some areas in the frontal lobe that were more
ther up in the hierarchy of motor control. active with the involuntary tremor. Another group
Paradoxically, the BP was absent in 59% of the of patients were studied with resting state fMRI
FMD patients for their normal voluntary move- (Fig. 2.4) [32]. Using the right TPJ as a region of
ments. A possible explanation for this is dis- interest, it was found to have decreased func-
cussed below. tional connectivity to the right sensorimotor cor-
tex, cerebellar vermis, bilateral SMA, and right
insula. The connection to the sensorimotor cortex
Loss of Self-Agency and SMA might be part of the feedforward path-
way. The connection to the insula might be part
In FMDs, the patient feels that the abnormal of the ventral attention/salience networks. In a
excessive movement (or limb weakness) is invol- study of normal movements in patients with
untary – that is, there is no perceived self-agency. FMD in the same paradigm described above
For other types of involuntary movements seen in where the amount of control of a representation
movement disorder patients, the motor command of a hand on a computer screen by the subject’s
is not produced in a normal fashion. In hemibal- hand was systematically varied, all the appropri-
lismus or Huntington chorea, the movements are ate areas were activated but the modulation by
thought to arise in the basal ganglia (although the percent control was poor [33]. Like the rest-
definitive proof is lacking). In cortical myoclo- ing state fMRI study, this result shows abnormal
nus, an epileptic-like event occurs in the motor network function even in the absence of abnor-
cortex causing a quick movement but apparently mal movements.
not generating a normal feedforward signal
despite the origin in M1. It is also the case that in
most FMD patients, sensation is normal, so there Loss of Willing
is no abnormality of the pathway from the periph-
ery to the primary sensory cortices. Therefore, An involuntary movement is characterized by a
there are two main possibilities; there is either an loss of intention or willing as well as a loss of
abnormal feedforward signal considering that it agency. Indeed, if there is no willing, there can-
does not arise solely in M1 or there is an abnor- not be any agency. No willing means no feedfor-
mality in the agency network. ward signaling. However, we already know in
As noted earlier, a particularly important node FMD, that the processes of motor cortex activa-
in the agency network is the right TPJ. The first tion in the involuntary movements are normal.
indication that this was an area of abnormal activ- Hence, there appears to be aberrant willing
ity in FMD was in a study of functional tremor. A whereby movement occurs normally, but the
group of patients were identified that could trig- feedforward signal is abnormal. This is compati-
ger their involuntary movement by moving their ble with evidence about the TPJ just discussed,
arm into a certain position, and they were also input from the sensorimotor area and SMA are
able to voluntarily mimic their involuntary tremor deficient.
[31]. The movements looked the same to external Further evidence for a failure of feedforward
observers, but they had a clear sense of when it signaling comes from studies of sensory gating.
was voluntary and when involuntary. These Sensory gating is the reduction of sensation and
patients were studied in the two conditions with somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) from a
fMRI, and the biggest difference in the two con- limb at the onset of, and during, self-generated
ditions was in the right TPJ. The TPJ was less movement. Studied in a mix of FMD patients,
active with the involuntary tremor, clearly an sensory gating was decreased in the patients [34,
20 M. Hallett

L/R SMA R precentral R postcentral


a b c

z = 62 z = 55
z = 52

d R insula e Cerebellar vermis

z = 19 z = -15

Fig. 2.4 Decreased functional connectivity (FC) between bilateral supplementary motor area (SMA) (circled), (b)
the right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ) and bilateral right precentral gyrus (circled), (c) right postcentral gyrus
sensorimotor regions in patients with functional move- (circled), (d) right insula (circled), and (e) cerebellar ver-
ment disorders (FMD). Maps demonstrate group differ- mis (circled). The threshold for display was set at p < 0.02;
ences in rTPJ resting-state FC between patients with FMD cluster size .28 voxels. (Republished with permission
and healthy controls. Images show decreased FC in from Maurer et al. [32])
patients with FMD between the rTPJ (seed) and the (a)

35]. In one study of force matching, patients did command to the sensory system, thus dampening
not overestimate the force required as the normal the sensory feedback from the movement. A pos-
controls did, indicating that they did not have sible function of gating is to reduce sensory input
normal gating [35]. In the other study of SEPs, from expected sensory events. There are two
the N20 and N30 potentials were not suppressed important implications. First, this is evidence for
as they should have been [34]. Gating must be abnormal feedforward signaling. Second, a loss
due to feedforward signaling from the motor of the gating function would mean that the move-
2 Free Will, Emotions and Agency: Pathophysiology of Functional Movement Disorder 21

ment related to the sensation would be more tem. Incidentally, it can be noted that increased
likely to be interpreted as externally generated emotional activity can affect other movement
rather than internally generated; this would foster disorders also; for example, increasing tremor in
a loss of the sense of agency. patients with Parkinson disease.
Thus, one of the main problems in FMD may In another study, patients with FMD per-
be in the premotor structures where intentions formed either an internally or externally gener-
develop. As noted earlier, there are many excit- ated 2-button action selection task in a functional
atory and inhibitory inputs to these structures, MRI study [41]. During both types of movement,
and in the next sections some of these will be patients relative to normal volunteers had higher
explored. Abnormalities of one or more of these right amygdala, left anterior insula, and bilateral
inputs could be fundamental to FMD. posterior cingulate activity and lower left SMA
activity. During internally versus externally gen-
erated action in patients, the left SMA had lower
Emotion Processing functional connectivity with bilateral dorsolateral
prefrontal cortices.
The limbic network of the brain centers on the A quantitative structural MRI study using
amygdala and its connections, particularly to the voxel-based morphometry in patients with FMD
orbitofrontal cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cor- compared with healthy controls exhibited
tex and hypothalamus (see Fig. 2.2). Additionally, increased volume of the left amygdala, left stria-
the salience network described above is also tum, left cerebellum, left fusiform gyrus, and
implicated in emotion processing. As noted ear- bilateral thalamus, and decreased volume of the
lier, emotion processing is one of the major fac- left sensorimotor cortex – abnormalities of the
tors influencing movement choice and often limbic and motor systems [42]. More work is
thought to be important in FMD [36]. Limbic needed to understand the role of laterality in
structures, such as the amygdala, can be influ- limbic-­related brain areas across the structural
enced by genetic factors and/or early life stress, and functional neuroimaging literature.
important factors in the biopsychosocial model One influence of the amygdala is on the startle
of FMD. In an fMRI study of faces showing dif- reflex. Patients with FMD show an increased
ferent affects, patients with FMD showed startle response to positive affective pictures as
increased activation of the right amygdala [37]. well as negative ones, indicating abnormal regu-
There was a pattern consistent with impaired lation of the startle response [43]. Even the sim-
amygdala habituation even when controlling for ple startle reflex is increased [44].
depressive and anxiety symptoms. Using psycho- The data are clear that there is limbic system
physiological interaction analysis, patients with hyperactivity and increased influence on the
FMD had greater functional connectivity between motor system. In resting state fMRI study using
the right amygdala and the right SMA, and graph theory-based analyses [45], increased
Granger Causality Modeling showed a direc- functional connectivity in FMD patients was
tional influence from the right amygdala to the found involving motor regions to the bilateral
right SMA. Overactivity of the amygdala was posterior insula, TPJ, middle cingulate cortex and
confirmed in other studies [38, 39], and increased putamen. From the right laterobasal amygdala,
activity in the SMA was also seen in one of these the patients showed enhanced connectivity to the
studies [38]. In another study of emotional face left anterior insula, periaqueductal grey and
processing in a group of patients with functional hypothalamus among other areas. Symptom
tremor, overactivity was seen in the cingulate/ severity correlated with increased information
paracingulate region, and not in the amygdala flow from the left anterior insula to the right ante-
[40], but this region is also part of the limbic sys- rior insula and TPJ.
22 M. Hallett

While there is strong evidence for hyperactiv- Interoceptive deficits based on use of a heartbeat
ity of the limbic and salience networks, it is curi- detection task have been described in individuals
ous that many patients with FMD have with FMD [48]. The likely locus of abnormality
alexithymia, a psychopathological trait charac- is the insula, which is well known to process such
terized by the inability to identify and describe information, particularly in its posterior seg-
emotions experienced by one’s self or others. ments. Deficits in interoception could also be
Thus, this hyperactivity, which might be trans- considered as a problem with bodily-related
lated as representing the increased influence of attention.
emotion on action, is not recognized in con-
sciousness. This might be due to a lack of
­attention to this function of the brain. There are Belief and Predictive Coding
problems with attention, which will be consid-
ered next. Other patients conversely do recognize One of the hypotheses about the genesis of FMD
that heightened arousal and negative affective is abnormal belief and predictive coding deficits
states amplify symptoms such as functional [49]. In simple terms, let’s say that a man has the
tremor, which suggests an alternative framing for belief that he is sick and that despite feedback
how to contextualize amygdala, cingulo-insular, that all is well, the belief does not change. The
and periaqueductal gray hyperactivations. prior does not update. From a clinical point of
view, this often seems the case. A patient may
feel that there is some structural brain disorder,
Attention such as a brain tumor, and refuses to drop that
belief even after normal studies and a doctor’s
From clinical assessment alone it is clear that reassurance. The patient goes on to find another
there is an important problem with attention [46]. doctor. While normal movement should update
Maintenance of functional movements or paresis the prior, abnormal movement verifies the prior
might well depend on attending to the malfunc- and helps maintain it. It becomes circular. The
tioning body part. If the patient’s attention is dis- patient believes he/she has a tremor disorder and
tracted away from the abnormal movement by, the fact that he/she has tremor confirms that
for example, by asking a patient to do a task with belief. To go one step further, if active inference
a different body part, the abnormality might dis- is an actual function, then the belief itself might
appear. A tremor in the left arm might stop when generate the tremor in order to maintain the
a patient is asked to do a task with the right arm belief. If this is occurring, the process cannot be
or solve a mathematical problem. Abnormalities conscious since the movement is felt to be invol-
in sustained and selection attention have charac- untary. If this process creates involuntary move-
terized patients with functional neurological dis- ments, while the person is also able to generate
orders, best identified in functional seizures [47]. voluntary movements, the involuntary process
would seem dissociated from the brain mecha-
nisms that are functioning normally. Indeed, for
Interoception many patients there is a sense that the FMD is
dissociated from their normal selves. There are
Interoception is the ability to appreciate the inter- two modes of operation, normal and abnormal.
nal state of body functioning. This would include Perhaps this is most apparent with paroxysmal
aspects such as hunger, thirst, or beating of the involuntary movements, such as functional sei-
heart. Interoception and emotional awareness zures, which are often called dissociative seizures
may also be partially inter-related constructs. for this reason. Additionally, the brain must focus
2 Free Will, Emotions and Agency: Pathophysiology of Functional Movement Disorder 23

attention on this belief. If attention is diverted to attention to the belief helps maintain it. The new
normal function, the abnormal function disap- predictive coding from the abnormal belief
pears. The excessive attention to the abnormal hijacks the normal motor apparatus. Movements
belief deprives other brain function of attention, (or paresis) may be generated either from the lim-
possibly including emotion and interoception. bic system or an active inference process stem-
How does an abnormal belief develop? If ming from the belief. This abnormal generation
there is a temporary abnormal condition of the does not create a quale of willing or a proper
body, for example, a heightened emotion, the feedforward signal to the multimodal network of
predictions about what the body should be like the TPJ, and no quale of agency develops either.
are incorrect. A prediction error is then fed back The original normal motor system either exists in
and the underlying probability for a new predic- parallel with the new abnormal network or tog-
tion is altered. A new model of the body might gles back and forth with it. The two systems can
then develop after several iterations. give rise to the sense of dissociation. Attention is
The last paragraphs contain many hypotheti- reduced to other brain functions such as intero-
cals, but as a model has some explanatory power ception and emotion awareness. Additionally, it
for some paradoxical phenomena discussed here is possible that across FMD patient populations,
previously. In patients with functional myoclo- distinct aspects of the neurobiology may play
nus, there is a BP before the involuntary move- more prominent roles in one patient sub-group
ments, but often not before normal voluntary over another. In the end, it is no longer only a
movements [30]. The involuntary movement gen- belief, the patient is truly sick.
erates movements via the usual pathway includ-
ing the SMA, while the voluntary movement
does not have such access. That might explain Implications for Treatment
also why patients with FMD do not improve
reaction time in response to a cue that has high If these hypotheses are correct, or even partially
validity for predicting the required movement correct, the only way of curing the patient at a
[50], and why there is not the normal beta desyn- fundamental level is to dampen down the limbic
chronization prior to movement [51]. system and adjust its functioning to be more
Additionally, it could explain why TPJ activation homeostatic. This may not be easy since its
is less for involuntary tremor than for voluntary development may have been disrupted by factors
tremor [31]; the involuntary tremor creates less such as early life stress that has left it less resil-
mismatch rather than more. ient [52, 53]. A fundamental change is not likely
to happen quickly and may require a multi-­
pronged approach. Better understanding about
Synthesis of the Pathophysiology the cellular and molecular neurobiology, and
their detailed connections, will be helpful in the
Certainly, there is more to learn, but a picture is development of individually targeted approaches
emerging (Fig. 2.5). The fundamental building to treatment of FMD.
blocks of normal movement are established, and A second approach which might help reverse
that provides a basis for understanding the patho- the disorder, but will likely still leave the patient
physiology of FMD. An overactive limbic system vulnerable, would be to correct the illness belief
disrupts brain function including a heightened and its top down influence on the motor system.
influence on the motor system. Prediction errors Develop methods to encourage the brain to
arise and gradually develop a belief that the per- function more in its normal mode. Deprive the
son is sick due to abnormal movements. Excessive “sick mode” of attention and possibly even shift
24 M. Hallett

Fig. 2.5 Pathophys­


iology model of
functional movement Attention
disorders. The normal
mechanism for making
movements is pushed
aside in favor of an
abnormal mechanism
(indicated with bolder Prediction Error
text and lines). Attention
is co-opted for the
abnormal mechanism. lllness Belief
See text for additional Belief Predictive Coding
details

Active
Inference

Intention
Emotion

Homeostasis
Interoception Movement
Planning

Slow path

Movement
Generation
Feedforward
Signals
Normal
movement Involuntary
movement

Multimodal
MOVEMENT
Integration

Feedback

it back to the normal mode by utilizing predic- appropriate fashion could support shifting. To a
tion error. There is some evidence, quoted certain extent, that is what psychologically sup-
above, that shifting of predictive coding is sup- ported physical therapy can provide by encour-
ported by dopaminergic mechanisms. Thus, aging patients to understand that they can
using reward or even dopamine itself in an function normally.
2 Free Will, Emotions and Agency: Pathophysiology of Functional Movement Disorder 25

5. Hallett M. Motor control: physiology of voluntary


Summary and involuntary movements. In: Fahn S, Jankovic J,
Hallett M, editors. Principles and practice of move-
• In functional movement disorder, ment disorders. Second ed. Philadelphia: Elsevier
instances of normal movement can be Saunders; 2011.
appreciated. 6. Tortora GJ, Derrickson BH. Principles of Anatomy
and Physiology. 15th ed. Hoboken: Wiley; 2017.
• Core constructs implicated in the patho- 7. Corradi-Dell'Acqua C, Fink GR, Weidner R. Selecting
physiology of FMD include loss of self-­ category specific visual information: top-down
agency, altered emotion processing, and bottom-up control of object based attention.
biased attentional mechanisms, altered Conscious Cogn. 2015;35:330.
8. Wheaton LA, Hallett M. Ideomotor apraxia: a review.
beliefs/predictive coding, and impaired J Neurol Sci. 2007;260:1–10.
interoception. 9. Shibasaki H, Hallett M. What is the
• Emerging evidence suggests that FMD Bereitschaftspotential? Clin Neurophysiol.
is a multi-network brain disorder with 2006;117:2341–56.
10. Hallett M. Physiology of free will. Ann Neurol.
the right temporal parietal junction-­ 2016;80:5–12.
based network implicated in distur- 11. Nahab FB, Kundu P, Gallea C, Kakareka J, Pursley R,
bances in self-agency. Pohida T, et al. The neural processes underlying self-­
• An overactive limbic system disrupts agency. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21:48–55.
12. Sperduti M, Delaveau P, Fossati P, Nadel J. Different
brain function in FMD, including a brain structures related to self- and external-agency
heightened influence on the motor attribution: a brief review and meta-analysis. Brain
system. Struct Funct. 2011;216:151–7.
• More research is needed to bridge neu- 13. Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral atten-
tion systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative
ral mechanisms and disease etiologies roles. Neuroscientist. 2014;20:150–9.
within the context of the biopsychoso- 14. Parr T, Friston KJ. Attention or salience? Curr Opin
cial model, as well as further investigat- Psychol. 2019;29:1–5.
ing the neural mechanisms of treatment 15. Drane DL, Fani N, Hallett M, Khalsa SS, Perez DL,
Roberts NA. A framework for understanding the
response. pathophysiology of functional neurological disorder.
CNS Spectr. 2021;26:555–61.
16. Siman-Tov T, Granot RY, Shany O, Singer N, Hendler
T, Gordon CR. Is there a prediction network? Meta-­
Acknowledgement Dr. Hallett is supported by the analytic evidence for a cortical-subcortical network
NINDS Intramural Program. likely subserving prediction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2019;105:262–75.
17. Brown H, Adams RA, Parees I, Edwards M, Friston
K. Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions.
References Cogn Process. 2013;14:411–27.
18. Kleckner IR, Zhang J, Touroutoglou A, Chanes L,
1. Begue I, Adams C, Stone J, Perez DL. Structural Xia C, Simmons WK, et al. Evidence for a large-scale
alterations in functional neurological disorder and brain system supporting Allostasis and Interoception
related conditions: a software and hardware problem? in humans. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1:0069.
Neuroimage Clin. 2019;22:101798. 19. Schwartenbeck P, FitzGerald THB, Dolan R. Neural
2. Galli S, Tatu L, Bogousslavsky J, Aybek signals encoding shifts in beliefs. NeuroImage.
S. Conversion, factitious disorder and malinger- 2016;125:578–86.
ing: a distinct pattern or a continuum? Front Neurol 20. Wang BA, Schlaffke L, Pleger B. Modulations of
Neurosci. 2018;42:72–80. insular projections by prior belief mediate the pre-
3. Hallett M, Stone J, Carson A. Functional neurologic cision of prediction error during tactile learning. J
disorders. In: Aminoff MJ, Boller F, Swaab DF, edi- Neurosci. 2020;40(19):JN-RM-2904-19.
tors. Handb Clin Neurol. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2016. 21. Hallett M. Physiology of psychogenic movement dis-
p. 662. orders. J Clin Neurosci. 2010;17:959–65.
4. Lecei A, van Winkel R. Hippocampal pattern sepa- 22. Liepert J, Hassa T, Tuscher O, Schmidt
ration of emotional information determining risk or R. Electrophysiological correlates of motor conver-
resilience in individuals exposed to childhood trauma: sion disorder. Mov Disord. 2008;23:2171–6.
linking exposure to neurodevelopmental alterations 23. Liepert J, Hassa T, Tuscher O, Schmidt R. Abnormal
and threat anticipation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. motor excitability in patients with psychogenic pare-
2020;108:160–70. sis. TMS study. J Neurol. 2009;256:121–6.
26 M. Hallett

24. Liepert J, Hassa T, Tuscher O, Schmidt R. Motor modulates motor control network in conversion disor-
excitability during movement imagination and move- der. Neuroimage Clin. 2017;15:143–50.
ment observation in psychogenic lower limb paresis. J 40. Espay AJ, Ries S, Maloney T, Vannest J, Neefus E,
Psychosom Res. 2011;70:59–65. Dwivedi AK, et al. Clinical and neural responses to
25. Matt E, Amini A, Aslan T, Schmidhammer R, cognitive behavioral therapy for functional tremor.
Beisteiner R. Primary motor cortex deactivation as Neurology. 2019;93:e1787–e98.
a new mechanism of motor inhibition in conversion 41. Voon V, Brezing C, Gallea C, Hallett M. Aberrant
paralysis. Mov Disord. 2019;34:148–9. supplementary motor complex and limbic activity
26. Nowak DA, Fink GR. Psychogenic movement dis- during motor preparation in motor conversion disor-
orders: Aetiology, phenomenology, neuroanatomical der. Mov Disord. 2011;26:2396–403.
correlates and therapeutic approaches. NeuroImage. 42. Maurer CW, LaFaver K, Limachia GS, Capitan G,
2009;47:1015–25. Ameli R, Sinclair S, et al. Gray matter differences
27. Cojan Y, Waber L, Carruzzo A, Vuilleumier P. Motor in patients with functional movement disorders.
inhibition in hysterical conversion paralysis. Neurology. 2018;91:e1870–e9.
NeuroImage. 2009;47:1026–37. 43. Seignourel PJ, Miller K, Kellison I, Rodriguez R,
28. Blakemore RL, Hyland BI, Hammond-Tooke GD, Fernandez HH, Bauer RM, et al. Abnormal affec-
Anson JG. Deficit in late-stage contingent negative tive startle modulation in individuals with psycho-
variation provides evidence for disrupted movement genic [corrected] movement disorder. Mov Disord.
preparation in patients with conversion paresis. Biol 2007;22:1265–71.
Psychol. 2015;109:73–85. 44. Dreissen YEM, Boeree T, Koelman J, Tijssen
29. Teodoro T, Koreki A, Meppelink AM, Little S, Nielsen MAJ. Startle responses in functional jerky movement
G, Macerollo A, et al. Contingent negative variation: a disorders are increased but have a normal pattern.
biomarker of abnormal attention in functional move- Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2017;40:27–32.
ment disorders. Eur J Neurol. 2020;27:985. 45. Diez I, Ortiz-Teran L, Williams B, Jalilianhasanpour
30. van der Salm SM, Tijssen MA, Koelman JH, van R, Ospina JP, Dickerson BC, et al. Corticolimbic
Rootselaar AF. The bereitschaftspotential in jerky fast-tracking: enhanced multimodal integration in
movement disorders. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. functional neurological disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg
2012;83:1162–7. Psychiatry. 2019;90:929–38.
31. Voon V, Gallea C, Hattori N, Bruno M, Ekanayake V, 46. Baizabal-Carvallo JF, Hallett M, Jankovic J. Pathogenesis
Hallett M. The involuntary nature of conversion dis- and pathophysiology of functional (psychogenic) move-
order. Neurology. 2010b;74:223–8. ment disorders. Neurobiol Dis. 2019;127:32–44.
32. Maurer CW, LaFaver K, Ameli R, Epstein SA, Hallett 47. Simani L, Roozbeh M, Rostami M, Pakdaman H,
M, Horovitz SG. Impaired self-agency in functional Ramezani M, Asadollahi M. Attention and inhibi-
movement disorders: a resting-state fMRI study. tory control deficits in patients with genetic general-
Neurology. 2016;87:564–70. ized epilepsy and psychogenic nonepileptic seizure.
33. Nahab FB, Kundu P, Maurer C, Shen Q, Hallett M. Epilepsy Behav. 2020;102:106672.
Impaired sense of agency in functional movement disor- 48. Ricciardi L, Demartini B, Crucianelli L, Krahe C,
ders: An fMRI study. PLoS One. 2017;12(4):e0172502. Edwards MJ, Fotopoulou A. Interoceptive awareness
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172502. in patients with functional neurological symptoms.
34. Macerollo A, Chen JC, Parees I, Kassavetis P, Kilner Biol Psychol. 2016;113:68–74.
JM, Edwards MJ. Sensory attenuation assessed by 49. Edwards MJ, Adams RA, Brown H, Parees I,
sensory evoked potentials in functional movement Friston KJ. A Bayesian account of 'hysteria'. Brain.
disorders. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0129507. 2012;135:3495–512.
35. Parees I, Brown H, Nuruki A, Adams RA, Davare 50. Parees I, Kassavetis P, Saifee TA, Sadnicka A, Davare
M, Bhatia KP, et al. Loss of sensory attenuation in M, Bhatia KP, et al. Failure of explicit movement con-
patients with functional (psychogenic) movement dis- trol in patients with functional motor symptoms. Mov
orders. Brain. 2014;137:2916–21. Disord. 2013;28:517–23.
36. Pick S, Goldstein LH, Perez DL, Nicholson 51. Teodoro T, Meppelink AM, Little S, Grant R, Nielsen
TR. Emotional processing in functional neurological G, Macerollo A, et al. Abnormal beta power is a
disorder: a review, biopsychosocial model and research hallmark of explicit movement control in functional
agenda. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2019;90:704–11. movement disorders. Neurology. 2018;90:e247–e53.
37. Voon V, Brezing C, Gallea C, Ameli R, Roelofs 52. Diez I, Larson AG, Nakhate V, Dunn EC, Fricchione
K, LaFrance WC Jr, et al. Emotional stimuli and GL, Nicholson TR, et al. Early-life trauma endophe-
motor conversion disorder. Brain. 2010a;133: notypes and brain circuit-gene expression relation-
1526–36. ships in functional neurological (conversion) disorder.
38. Aybek S, Nicholson TR, O'Daly O, Zelaya F, Kanaan Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26:3817–28.
RA, David AS. Emotion-motion interactions in 53. Spagnolo PA, Norato G, Maurer CW, Goldman D,
conversion disorder: an FMRI study. PLoS One. Hodgkinson C, Horovitz S, et al. Effects of TPH2 gene
2015;10:e0123273. variation and childhood trauma on the clinical and
39. Hassa T, Sebastian A, Liepert J, Weiller C, Schmidt R, circuit-level phenotype of functional movement dis-
Tuscher O. Symptom-specific amygdala hyperactivity orders. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2020;91:814.
The Biopsychosocial Formulation
for Functional Movement Disorder
3
Lindsey MacGillivray and Sarah C. Lidstone

Case Vignette 1 struck from behind and though he sustained only


minor musculoskeletal injuries, he never seemed
Mr. S is a pleasant 54-year-old gentleman. He to fully recover. Years later, he continues to com-
lives with his wife and 2 teenaged children and is plain of persistent low back and neck pain.
financially supported by his work as a project Routine blood work, brain and spinal MRIs and
manager for a large national firm. His medical nerve conduction studies are all normal.
history is significant for irritable bowel syndrome Two months ago, Mr. S developed sudden
but is otherwise unremarkable. He has no formal onset jerks in his truncal region. He is referred to
psychiatric history, but family and friends describe a movement disorders neurologist who, after a
him as stoic and at times “aloof.” He tells you that thorough neurological examination, diagnoses
his childhood was “normal and happy enough” him with functional myoclonus on the basis of
and denies any history of trauma, though on prob- positive examination signs including prominent
ing you discover that when he was a child his variability and distractibility. He is unable to
father often physically struck him for minor dis- self-­identify any precipitating factors but indi-
cretions. “That’s just the way it was back then,” cates that his jerks are more frequent and debili-
he says. Mr. S was unfortunately involved in a tating on days in which he is more fatigued or
motor vehicle accident in his early 50s. He was experiencing increased pain. He admits that his
abnormal movements make him feel self-con-
scious and that he has started to limit his social
interactions and is working from home whenever
L. MacGillivray (*)
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, possible. He brought disability application
Toronto Western Hospital, University Health paperwork to his appointment today.
Network, Toronto, ON, Canada
e-mail: lindsey.macgillivray@uhn.ca
S. C. Lidstone Case Vignette 2
Integrated Movement Disorders Program, Toronto
Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
Mrs. V is a 42-year-old married female and
Edmond J. Safra Program in Parkinson’s Disease and
mother to three children. She works as a human
the Morton and Gloria Shulman Movement Disorders
Clinic, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health resources manager and much of her time is
Network, Toronto, ON, Canada spent navigating disputes among employees.
Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, She is skilled at this work and reports that she
University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada finds her position rewarding, but stressful. She

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 27


K. LaFaver et al. (eds.), Functional Movement Disorder, Current Clinical Neurology,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-86495-8_3
28 L. MacGillivray and S. C. Lidstone

balances her paid work with volunteering roles any history of emotional, sexual or physical
that she has accumulated over the years— for abuse. Of note, she has been chronically high
instance, the local parent teacher association achieving. She was an academic gold medalist in
and community food bank. At home, she is the high school and is a former national-level dancer.
primary caregiver for the children and takes Her friends describe her as being “the nice one
most of the responsibility for managing the who is always willing to help” and “a bit of a
household, as her husband is CEO of a sales perfectionist.” They wonder how she manages to
agency and has to travel frequently. She wishes do it all.
her husband was able to be home more often but
is proud of his success at work and reports that
they have a mutually loving and supportive rela- Introduction to the Biopsychosocial
tionship. Her medical history is significant for Model and Formulation
dysmenorrhea and migraine headaches through-
out her 20s, but she has been generally healthy In 1977, George Engel postulated a model that
since. She has no formal psychiatric history and aimed to transform the way clinicians conceptu-
denies any past or current symptoms of anxiety, alize disease [1]. By contrast to the dominant bio-
depression or trauma-related disorders. Despite medical model that suggested disease could be
this, a possible generalized anxiety disorder is fully accounted for by measurable biological
suspected given her descriptions of being variables, Engel offered us a more holistic way of
“always on the go,” “being a worrier” and hav- understanding our patients, suggesting that we
ing difficulties “turning my brain off.” On inter- instead simultaneously consider the biological,
view, she presents as animated and anxious. She psychological and social dimensions of illness
talks quickly and you have a sense of her being (including cultural and spiritual aspects where
on ‘overdrive.’ Her body appears tense, with relevant) [1]. While in some respects this biopsy-
mild psychomotor agitation. She describes a chosocial model could inadvertently reinforce a
one-year history of shaking in her right arm, false mind-body dualism by encouraging clini-
starting after a bout of flu-like symptoms. The cians to consider biological factors as discrete
shaking comes and goes but seems to be wors- from psychological and social factors, it is impor-
ening in intensity and frequency and she also tant to remember that psychological experience
notices that she is increasingly forgetful. “It has biological underpinnings and, in turn, that
feels like my brain is in a fog.” Her grandfather biological phenomena often have psychological
suffered from Parkinson’s disease and related correlates. Nonetheless, so long as we are mind-
dementia and she is worried that she, too, is ful that brain, body and mind are parts of an inte-
developing a neurodegenerative condition. She grated whole, the biopsychosocial framework
now pays much more attentive to her body so can help guide us to a holistic understanding of
that she can be on the lookout for new symptoms our patients and to better identify a range of treat-
or other worrisome signs. She continues to push ment targets.
through at work, emphasizing that her employ- To understand and treat patients with FMD,
ees rely on her. you must endeavor to learn their story, i.e. not
Mrs. V is referred to a neurologist who diag- simply the phenomenology of their symptoms,
noses her with a functional movement disorder but the context in which those symptoms
(FMD), after observing that her arm tremor was evolved, and how those symptoms affect the
both readily distractible and entrainable (posi- person before you. You should also seek to iden-
tive signs for functional tremor). The interviewer tify factors that potentially render your patient
remembers from medical school that these disor- at risk for developing FMD and to explore their
ders are likely connected to psychological strengths; awareness of these will help clini-
trauma, and as such is surprised when she denies cians to engage with patients and to assist in
3 The Biopsychosocial Formulation for Functional Movement Disorder 29

treating their symptoms. This information is your patient and is not meant to identify a singu-
often best obtained through open-ended qualita- lar etiology for FMD (there are typically numer-
tive interviews rather than exclusive reliance on ous or many relevant factors)— one size does not
clinical scales or symptom-checklists [2, 3]. fit all. And remember, a formulation is merely a
The standard way to apply the biopsychosocial hypothesis, not conclusive fact. It should not be
model is to generate a list of predisposing, pre- delivered in a top-down fashion as a certainty, but
cipitating and perpetuating factors that align rather is best developed over time in active col-
with biological, psychological and psychosocial laboration with the patient [8].
categories. Many use a three-by-three square grid
to organize this list (see Fig. 3.1). Importantly,
any given factor can apply to one of more catego- Predisposing Vulnerabilities (Risk
ries (e.g. alexithymia may be both a predisposing Factors) for FMD
vulnerability and a precipitating factor).
FMD is a complex neuropsychiatric disorder Predisposing factors confer vulnerability to later
that is best understood as arising in the context of development of FMD. They include demographic
predisposing vulnerabilities (risk factors), acute and characterological factors, comorbid medical/
precipitants and perpetuating factors. Rarely is neurological/psychiatric illnesses and influential
there one identified cause for FMD and, by con- events occurring throughout development.
trast to early psychodynamic theories of conver- Typically, these vulnerabilities are remote from
sion, we now recognize that FMD is not exclusively the onset of the FMD and while they do not nec-
a psychiatric disorder. For some patients, promi- essarily have any direct (proximal) etiological
nent psychological stressors are not readily appar- relevance, they do foster risk.
ent or are not the most relevant factors in
understanding their illness [4–6]. Accordingly, the
most recent version of the Diagnostic and Biological
Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM-5)
has removed the diagnostic requirement for a psy- Women are more frequently affected by FMD
chological stressor to precede symptom onset [7]. than men, representing an estimated 60-75% of
In general, we suggest that clinicians de-empha- the patient population [9]. This difference may
size an upfront search for a specific cause and vary by FMD phenomenology; functional myoc-
instead focus on the rationale for the FMD diagno- lonus, for instance, appears to be equally or more
sis and the evidence-­based treatments that may common in men [10].
help the patient in their recovery journey. However, Comorbid neurological and psychiatric disor-
while an initial focus on “why” someone has ders may play a predisposing role; a broad range
developed FMD may be better left for exploration of pre-existing conditions have been reported in
in the context of engagement in rehabilitation and the FMD population, including but not limited to
psychological treatments, assessing the basic com- multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease, history of
ponents of the biopsychosocial model to formulate head injury, epilepsy, intellectual disability, anxi-
clinical cases aids the development of a patient- ety and depression [4, 11–15]. Sensory process-
centered treatment plan. ing difficulties have been reported in some
Formulation is an active process that requires patients with FMD [16]. Commonly, patients
more than collecting discrete biopsychosocial with FMD also have a history of somatic symp-
data points. To formulate, one uses information toms of unclear etiology. These, too, are broad
gleaned from a holistic interview to generate a ranging, but often include conditions such as
tentative hypothesis about how and why a patient chronic fatigue, fibromyalgia, pelvic pain and
developed symptoms at this juncture in their irritable bowel syndrome [17–19]. Whether these
lives. A formulation should be individualized to conditions are part of a larger “functional syn-
30 L. MacGillivray and S. C. Lidstone

Biological Psychological Social

Female gender Comorbid psychiatric Physical, sexual or


disorders emotional trauma;
Comorbid neglect
Predisposing neurological and Health anxiety and
Vulnerabilities psychiatric conditions somatic vigilance Low socioeconomic
(Risk Factors) status; financial
Alexithymia strain
Sensory processing
difficulties
Insecure attachment Major lossess such as
Chronic fatigue, pain style bereavement or
gastrointestinal divorce
conditions Maladaptive
personality traits Chronic interpersonal
challenges

Physical injury or Dissociation; panic Relationship


surgery attacks stressors

Preceding illness Events (e.g., losses, Significant losses–


failures) that activate death, separation or
Acute
insecure attachment divorce
Precipitants Accidents (e.g.,
patterns, distorted
motor vechicle
cognitions
accident) Interpersonal conflict

Autonomic Unprocessed guilt Job loss or


hyperarousal event and anger employment-related
(e.g., panic attack) stressors
Emotional impact of
Sleep deprivation injury

Chronic pain and/or Invalidation by the Family dysfunction


fatigue healthcare system;
stigma Interpersonal or
Chronic medical work-related
conditions stressors
Maladaptive illness
Prepetuating
beliefs; Lack of
Factors diagnostic agreement
Physical Pending litigation
deconditioning
Anxiety and Unconscious
Entrenched abnormal hypervigilance secondary gains
motor programs around symptoms
Poor communication
Avoidance patterns amongst health care
providers

Fig. 3.1 Depiction of the biopsychosocial model for Also, a given factor may cut across multiple levels, such
clinical formulation of patients with functional movement as alexithymia in a given patient being both a predispos-
disorder. Note — other themes not addressed in this ing vulnerability and a perpetuating factor
model may be relevant at the individual patient level.
3 The Biopsychosocial Formulation for Functional Movement Disorder 31

drome” that exists on a continuum is the subject a history of adverse life experiences, including
of debate, but they are nonetheless important to trauma, significant interpersonal difficulties in
screen for in your intake assessment because they one’s family or social environment, or a major
may also be relevant perpetuating factors that loss [6, 29–32]. Chronic pre-existing stressors in
should be addressed in a comprehensive treat- the form of financial strain, low socioeconomic
ment plan. Notably, associated pain, fatigue and status in general and limited psychosocial sup-
psychological symptoms are reported to account ports may also predispose individuals to later
for more disability and impaired quality of life development of FMD [30, 32].
than do the motor symptoms themselves [20, 21]. Historically, functional neurological disorder
(FND) was ascribed in entirety to a psychologi-
cal stressor or emotional conflict. Psychodynamic
Psychological theory held that emotionally-laden stress was
subconsciously repressed and “converted” to
Categorical psychiatric disorders (e.g. general- physical form [33] and previous versions of the
ized anxiety, panic, major depression, post-­ DSM included a preceding psychological
traumatic stress and personality pathology) have stressor as necessary for the diagnosis of conver-
both biological and psychological substrates and sion disorder [34]. This requirement has been
are present in some individuals that develop removed from DSM-5 given that a substantial
FMD [2, 5, 13]. Health anxiety and a pattern of proportion of patients with FND do not report
somatic vigilance are sometimes prominent [22]. having experienced traumatic events in their his-
More frequently, individuals with FMD have tory [5]. We caution clinicians against making
relational frameworks, personality traits and pat- any direct supposition to patients that their
terns of behavior that, while not necessarily symptoms are “caused” by trauma or stress,
pathological, may render them vulnerable to especially if they themselves deny any such his-
FMD. These dimensional considerations include tory. When present, however, trauma and other
factors such as alexithymia (difficulty identify- psychological factors often play important pre-
ing and labeling emotions), insecure attachment disposing and perpetuating roles in FMD and
patterns and chronic challenges with affect regu- correlate with symptom severity [5]. This is also
lation, neuroticism and/or obsessionality [23– a good example of the inter-­relatedness of vari-
25]. There is no singular personality style ous components of the biopsychsocial model;
associated with FMD, but traits we see in some childhood maltreatment has been linked to plas-
patients include excessive responsibility taking, tic changes in the brain that may help explain
chronic high achievement and perfectionism, links between life events and the later develop-
tendencies to put the needs of others before one- ment of FMD [35, 36].
self, and conflict avoidance [26–28]. Some indi-
viduals have a propensity to distance from
emotion, to dissociate in the context of stress or Acute Precipitants for FMD
to adopt a highly active “always on the go” life-
style that might serve to distract from underlying Acute precipitants are proximal factors that tem-
feelings—a manic defense, of sorts [23]. porally associate with onset of FMD. They may
or may not be identifiable. Like predisposing fac-
tors, precipitants are not “causes” of FMD. They
Social should instead be considered as triggers or
“tipping-­point” variables. It is likely that precipi-
The most frequently reported predisposing fac- tants set in motion FMD symptom onset only in
tors are psychosocial in nature. Among these are the setting of predisposing vulnerabilities.
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
telephone any discoveries you may——”
He broke off abruptly, hit with a sudden idea, and turned sharply around
to his listening confederates.
“What was the number he mentioned?” he cried. “Can you remember it?”
“Sure!” cried Blink Morgan. “Four, seven Madison!”
“Get that telephone book.” Gridley pointed to the table. “Look for the
police headquarters. See if that’s their number.”
“Rats!” growled Phelan. “He ain’t a police sleuth. He’s no plain clothsie.
I know that push.”
“Try the private agencies, then,” snapped Gridley. “Look up—stop a bit!
Begin with Nick Carter. Try him. Look up his number.”
“Holy smoke!” thought Patsy. “Here’s where the cat makes her final
jump. She’ll come clean out of the bag this time. But the rascals do not
suspect the trick I’ve put over on them. That sure is my only anchor to the
windward.”
Morgan and Turk Magill had turned pale when Nick Carter’s name was
mentioned, and their fears were completely verified.
For Phelan, suddenly starting up from the telephone book he was
hurriedly inspecting, cried excitedly:
“I’ve got it! Here’s the name and number. Four, seven Madison! It’s a
telephone in Nick Carter’s business office.”
“Last jump is right,” thought Patsy.
Gridley swung round and gazed at him with murder in his eye.
“So Nick Carter wrote this note, did he?” said he, through his teeth.
“You’re to telephone your discoveries to him, eh? What have you
discovered? What has he got on us?”
“Nothing on you that I know of,” said Patsy, unruffled. “I was not
directed to shadow you fellows.”
“What on this woman, then?”
“I don’t know for sure, and I don’t think he does,” Patsy truthfully
answered, not yet informed of Nick’s deductions and suspicions. “That’s
dead-straight goods, Gridley, on my word.”
Gridley vented an oath and shaped another course.
“Make sure that he is securely tied, Phelan,” he cried sternly. “We’ll settle
his hash a little later. Our first move must be to get the coin—and get it
mighty quick, if Carter is dipping into this business.”
“That’s right, too,” Magill declared, glaring at Patsy. “Get the coin and
bolt—that’s our only safe course.”
“We’ll take it, too, and take it on the jump,” Gridley forcibly added. “Free
that woman, Morgan, and be quick about it. She shall tell us what she
knows, or—God help her!”
CHAPTER IX.

THE LAST CALL.

Kate Crandall had not stirred from the sofa during the sensational scenes
just enacted. They told her only too plainly that she was in the hands of
knaves who would shrink from no desperate deed that would serve their
ends, and she had no thought but to escape from them by any means she
could command.
Blink Morgan hastened to liberate her, while Gridley seated himself
directly in front of her and sternly said:
“You’ve got mighty few minutes, woman, to tell us what we want to
know. We have others here who could tell us, but whose traps are tightly
closed. We have not killed them, lest we might kill our golden goose; but
understand this: We’ll end them and you, too, unless you give us the
information which——”
Kate Crandall checked him with a haughty gesture.
“One moment, Mr. Gridley, if that’s your name,” she said coldly. “I can
tell you with very few words all that I know. You will believe me, I think,
though this man refused to do so.”
She glanced at Magill, but he made no comments.
“You were seen two nights ago by him and Morgan,” said Gridley, sternly
eying her. “They had followed a girl to the home of a clergyman named
Maybrick. They saw her look through his library window and then enter his
house. They would have listened at his window to her interview with him—
but you got there first, and they could not do so without taking risks then
thought to be needless. We must know what the girl told him. It’s up to you
to tell us. You heard what she said, or you would not have remained to
listen.”
“That is true,” Kate coldly admitted. “I heard all that she said to Mr.
Maybrick.”
“Tell me,” said Gridley sternly.
“She told him that her father had recently died; that he was a criminal and
had forced her to be one, but that she now was determined to reform. She
told him that her father was one of a gang that had recently robbed a bank,
and that he had had charge of the stolen funds and had buried them,
confiding to her their hiding place while on his deathbed.”
“That’s the point,” said Gridley. “That’s the very thing we want to know
—where the plunder is hidden.”
“I cannot tell you,” said Kate.
“Not tell me! Why not?”
“Because I do not know. The girl did not inform Mr. Maybrick.”
“What did she say about it?”
“She said she would take him to the spot, and that he could then remove
the funds and restore them to the bank. She would not then reveal the hiding
place.”
“Did she give him no hint?”
“No, none,” said Kate. “She appointed a place for him to meet her last
evening, which he promised to do. That is all I can tell you.”
“Is that true?”
“God hearing me, it is true!” Kate solemnly declared. “I cannot possibly
give you the information you expected from me. I do not know——”
“Stop! I believe you,” Gridley cried curtly; then, turning to Blink
Morgan, he harshly commanded: “Bring the jade up here, Blink, and the
gospel sharp with her. I’ll find a way to force her to speak.”
Morgan seized a lamp and hastened from the room.
Patsy heard him descending the cellar stairs a moment later.
“By thunder, this is the gang that cracked that Westchester savings bank,”
he said to himself. “Gee whiz! there’s half a million at stake. If the chief was
right, Jim Nordeck must be the man who buried the plunder, and the girl in
question must be Nancy Nordeck.”
Patsy did not realize just then, however, how perfectly right Nick Carter
had sized up the entire case. This appeared a moment later, when Nancy
Nordeck and the missing rector were led into the room, both with their arms
securely bound behind them.
The Reverend Austin Maybrick was quite pale, but he carried himself
with dignity, and his fine face wore a look of scorn that told how little he
feared the threatening situation. He appeared surprised when he saw Kate
Crandall and Patsy, but he did not speak.
Gridley hardly noticed him. He turned at once to the girl who had
entered, and then leaned wearily against the nearest wall.
She was a slender, poorly clad girl, who looked ten years older than she
really was. Her dark-brown hair was in disorder, her eyes deeply ringed, but
her features were regular and she would have been quite attractive, but for a
wan and pinched look that told of dejection, suffering, and more of care and
misery than often falls upon one of her years.
She also was surprised at seeing Patsy and Kate Crandall, but it appeared
only in the sharper glint of her large, expressive eyes, which flashed from
one to another, though chiefly at Gridley, with a look of mingled
determination and defiance that evinced a fearless spirit in her frail form.
Gridley turned to her with lowering gaze, saying harshly:
“You’re surprised at seeing others here, ain’t you?”
Nancy Nordeck gave him look for look, with her thin gray lips curling
contemptuously. She drew herself up a little, replying with a sinister slang
that evinced her lack of refinement.
“Not on your life, Gridley. I wouldn’t be surprised at any scurvy trick that
you pulled off. What d’ye want, now that you’ve brought him and me from
the cellar? I’d sooner stay there than be in the same room with you.”
“Cut out that lobscouse!” commanded Gridley sternly. “I’m going to
show you where you stand, and where these persons stand whom you’ve
drawn into this mess. I’m going to force you, or them, to tell me where your
double-dealing dad hid that plunder.”
“Oh, you are!” Nancy exclaimed derisively. “You’ll get fat trying to force
that out of me. You can’t get it out of them, or any one else, for I’ve told no
one. I handed you that at first, but it seems you can’t swallow it. I’m the only
one who knows where the stuff is planted.”
“That is true, absolutely true,” said Maybrick, with habitual dignity. “I
don’t know why you have brought this other woman here, but you——”
“What you don’t know cuts no ice with us,” Gridley sharply interrupted.
“You keep quiet, or I’ll find a way to make you. There’s a bunch of sleuths
on this case who may make trouble for us at any moment, and I’m in no
mood to mince matters. This infernal jade, if she’s the only one who knows,
is going to tell me where to find that plunder.”
“Oh, is that so, Gridley?” questioned Nancy, with eyes flashing.
“You’ll find it’s so.”
“And you’ll find it isn’t,” snapped the girl defiantly. “You put that idea
out of your block. It might turn you batty.”
“See here, Gridley,” she added, with a sudden display of deeper feeling.
“I’ve been a bad egg most of my life. It come to me natural, and my old man
forced me into it. He’s dead now, and I stood by alone and saw the last
breath go out of him. I’d never seen the like before. I’d never been where
one sees the call of death—the call of death! It told me something I never
knew before—but no matter what! You wouldn’t know, if I told you—and I
couldn’t tell you if I tried.”
“See here——”
“You see here!” Nancy forcibly interrupted. “I’m going to have my say,
and it won’t take me long. I’m done with the life I’ve led, and done with you
fellows. That plunder is going back to the bank. That’s what I’m going to do
for a starter on the new road. I knew you guys would watch me. I reckoned
I’d better not take this gent to the place where the stuff is hid, not till I was
dead sure you weren’t trailing me. So I took him to a fake place first, just to
find out, and you and your push were on hand to nail us. You’ve got us, all
right; but you’ll not get the coin. I fooled you—and I’ll keep you fooled.
You’ll get nothing from me.”
She had told the whole story in those few passionate words, a story that
might have filled a volume, and the look on Gridley’s face was one to have
appalled a less fearless speaker. He turned quickly to his confederates and
fiercely cried:
“We’ll see about that, pals! We’ll find out whether she’ll speak. Pull the
boots off of this gospel sharp and shove his feet into the fire. She brought
him into this mess. Let’s see whether she’ll pull him out of it. She can do it
only by squealing. If not, we’ll burn his feet off, and——”
“Say!” cried Patsy. “Cut that, you fellows.”
“Cut nothing! You dry up, or we’ll cut out the tongue you talk with.”
Nancy Nordeck had turned as white as a sheet.
“Keep quiet, my girl, and be brave,” said Maybrick, observing her.
“Reveal nothing—no matter what these scoundrels do. That is your new
duty.”
“I’ll stick, sir, if you say it,” said Nancy, but she was trembling from head
to foot.
“Oh, you will, eh?” thundered Gridley. “We’ll see whether you will. Grab
the gospel sharp, two of you, and——”
But there was no grabbing done of that nature.
Gridley’s furious commands were drowned by the crash of a falling door,
the rending of blinds and the breaking of shuttered windows, at which the
heads of policemen and leveled revolvers instantly appeared.
Patsy Garvan guessed the truth, and a yell broke from him.
“Hurrah! Zambo! It’s all off! The chief is here!”
Patsy was right. While the words were still on his lips, Nick, Chick, and
Danny tore through the hall and rushed into the room, with weapons drawn
and blood in their eyes.
Gridley vented an oath and snatched up one of Patsy’s revolvers, still
lying on the table.
A bullet from Nick’s weapon broke the rascal’s wrist. He fell to the floor,
howling with pain.
Chick had a gun under Magill’s nose, and Morgan and Phelan had thrown
up their hands.
There was very little to it after that, in so far as opposition was
concerned. Within five minutes the crooks were in irons, their captives
liberated, and Nancy Nordeck relieved of her fears and started, indeed, on
the better road.
Through her the entire amount of stolen funds were restored to the bank,
or, more properly, through her and the Carters. She never was prosecuted for
any of her past misdemeanors. Nick Carter made sure of that—and equally
sure that Gridley and his confederates received the most severe penalty of
the law.
Nick’s deductions had been entirely correct, after the disclosures Chick
had made, and the remarkable message from Patsy had showed them the
way. Nick was right, too, in thinking that Kate Crandall, though informed of
the facts, had suppressed them only with a feeling of jealous hatred and
revenge for Maybrick and Harriet Farley, whose relief and gratitude over the
happy turn of affairs scarce need be mentioned.

THE END.
Kate Crandall very soon disappeared from her customary haunts—but
Nick Carter had not seen the last of her, as you will find by reading the story
in the next issue of this weekly, No. 122, out January 9th. The story is
entitled “The Suicide; or, Nick Carter and the Lost Hand.”
RUBY LIGHT.

By BURKE JENKINS.
(This interesting story was commenced in No. 120 of Nick Carter Stories. Back numbers can
always be obtained from your news dealer or the publishers.)
CHAPTER V.

A JOY RIDE.
I had time enough at Boston the next morning to get a good breakfast
before the Portland Express left; and this I partook of in the station dining
room. I knew within reasonable bounds when the steamers reached their
dock at Portland, and figured that I should have ample time for my plan,
which was simplicity itself. I would just be on hand when the boat docked.
And I was. Furthermore, I had stationed myself in a most advantageous
position for seeing all who quitted the boat. A pile of boxes, not five feet
from the passenger gangway, further favored me by allowing partial
concealment of myself. I would simply spot my man when he quitted the
vessel; the rest would be regulation shadowing.
A fog had delayed the boat about an hour in entering the harbor, as I
learned from a longshoreman; but she finally loomed up directly before us,
and the fog entirely cleared as the lines were cast and she was warped to the
pier.
Herded into the usual impatient mass, the passengers pressed against the
ropes until the final sliding of the gangplank.
Then, at a signal from the officer in charge, they swarmed from her. I
didn’t miss a face; and I am pretty good at this work. Besides, I counted on
seeing a face that was with me constantly now.
But I didn’t.
Little by little the crowd thinned. One or two belated ones trotted
shoreward; then no more.
I could have been sure that the man and girl hadn’t passed me; and I
found myself in a predicament.
I waited a few more minutes, but without another individual setting foot
upon the plank. Plainly any search I might further make must be done
aboard the vessel itself.
But if I did that I would have to quit the gangway, and that, naturally, I
was not pleased to do. It was the strategic point.
It was not an overbrilliant plan, or an extremely reliable one, that I
finally hit upon; but it was the best I could do under the circumstances.
I caught sight of a shaggy-browed deck hand who had stepped from the
freight hold and stood, lounging idly, waiting for the rest of the crew to
begin unloading.
I had replenished my cigar supply at Boston; so I approached the man
diplomatically.
“My friend,” said I, “I’m almost positive that some people I am very
anxious to see took this steamer; but I haven’t been able to find them.
Would you mind watching out for them while I step aboard? I certainly
don’t want to miss them.”
He accepted the proffered smoke greedily enough; but scowled at me
from under his cap visor as he grunted:
“How’n tarnation’ll I be able to know ’em? What d’ they look like?”
“A man with a sort of stoop to his shoulders, and a mighty pretty girl,” I
replied. “Anyway, you can’t fail, for if anybody shows up, it must be that
——”
I stopped, for he was scratching his pate in thick-headed brain-working.
“Hold on thar!” he finally growled. “Why, dang me, man, they ain’t
aboard no longer!”
“Ain’t aboard?” I cried. “Why, I could swear they haven’t yet crossed
that gangplank!”
“Right you are there, my lad,” he agreed; “for them there and that same
pair o’ individools must be the folks as got into a launch out yonder in the
harbor when we were stopped by the fog.”
“Into a launch!” I echoed frenziedly as a sharp memory shot through me.
Surely the couple did have a uniform method of boarding and quitting
vessels.
“Better see the purser about it,” added the fellow; “there’s his window in
there. He knows more about it as I do.”
He pointed aboard the boat where the brass grill of that officer’s office
showed up plainly enough. And immediately I strode across the plank and
up to the purser, who was figuring at his desk.
He acknowledged my nod genially, and asked what he could do for me.
“If you don’t mind,” said I hurriedly, “will you tell me all you know
about that couple that quitted the steamer for a launch back there in the
harbor?”
“Quitted the steamer for a launch?” he cried, in the utmost
bewilderment, while he looked at me as though he thought I was demented.
“Why, nobody did.”
“That deck hand there——” I started to explain as I turned, and
indicated where I had come from.
But I got no further; for, at that very moment, I saw the very same deck
hand steadying a girl across the freight gangway.
I lost an important minute in my consternation at having been thus so
easily outwitted; and I reached the wharf again just in time to see him help
her quickly into a taxicab at the pier end. But, before he sprang in himself,
he grinned back over his shoulder at me delightedly.
No, I had not exactly recognized that peculiar stoop to the shoulders of
the owner of the launch back there at Port Washington, as I had assured
myself I would be able to do.
But, if I ever hoped to retrieve myself, I hadn’t a moment to lose. I, in
turn, hot-footed it to the shore end of the pier, and fortune seemed to favor
me here a bit; for there stood another taxi, and its “vacant” flag was flying.
“Follow that cab, my man,” I said hurriedly and out of breath from my
sprint. “Don’t let ’em lose you, and there’s money in it.”
The little chauffeur grinned delightedly from under his goggles.
“Right-o!” he chortled happily, as he cranked; and next second we
jumped into the high speed.
But that first spurt was short-lived, for we came to sharply applied
brakes by the time we had but crossed the avenue.
I heard a sharp command as a man dashed from the swing door of a
shore-front saloon. Then the door beside me was yanked open, and as
quickly closed.
“After ’em now, for all that’s in you!” yelled the man who had flopped
beside me, and the car lurched forward again.
I whirled on the intruder in a rage.
“Came near losing ’em, didn’t you?” said he quietly.
“Pawlinson!” I managed to whisper in my amazement.
“I was a trifle dramatic,” he explained easily; “but, from my vantage
point of the saloon window yonder, I calculated that maybe two of us might
work together better in this case.
“And, besides,” he added meaningly, as the cab swayed us to its mad
pace, “I want to talk over some things with you.”
As I think was natural enough, I had the greatest difficulty in recovering
from complete bewilderment. But I did manage finally to blurt out:
“And I, too—I’m in the dark about a good many things.”
“Just as I thought, of course,” he replied. Then his tone changed to a
sharpness, a grittiness of command that didn’t set well with me for a whit.
“Now, I want to know,” said he, “just who you are and where you stand.”
Though I resented the manner of his query, I couldn’t but realize that he
really was entitled to the knowledge.
“My name is Tom Grey,” said I, “and I am one of Chief Garth’s men.
Last night he detailed me to——”
“Yes, yes. I know about that,” he broke in impatiently. “I saw you go in
and leave his house. But what I want explained is: How did you happen to
mix in on this thing at Port Washington?”
“The merest coincidence,” said I, rather lamely.
Then I gave him all I knew about it and the way it had come about that I
happened to be swinging my feet from the stringpiece of the dock at that
particular moment.
“Well,” he said, with a tinge of sarcasm, “it was a very unfortunate
coincidence, as you call it; for I can tell you it wasn’t any cinch to throw off
suspicion in landing that job as engineer of the launch. But I had landed it;
and if it hadn’t been for you I could have delayed the start until four of my
men arrived. They were coming on the next train from New York.”
“And so that’s why you faked that the engine was broken down?” I
blurted, somewhat idiotically.
“Rather!” was the reply; and the sarcasm was no longer veiled. “And
you can imagine my surprise when I recognized you entering Chief Garth’s
place just as I myself was leaving.”
“Now, you know well enough, Mr. Pawlinson,” said I sheepishly, “that
my interference at the launch was natural enough when you grant that I
knew nothing of the circumstances, and——”
“Enough of that!” he said. “I’m not harboring any resentment any more,
though I still have a bit of a game leg as a souvenir of the incident. But
what I do want to know—and what has made me follow you closely from
Garth’s to this very moment—is: Just where do you stand in this matter?
Are you entirely—now that you understand the thing—are you my man?”
“Why, of course,” I replied, in as convincing a tone as I could command.
But somehow it didn’t ring overtrue; for, in spite of myself, I simply
couldn’t cotton to this man.
“But now, in turn”—I changed my tone—“I should like to know——”
Here the cab swung a corner violently; then we took to the evener going
of a well-macadamized road, which seemed to lead almost indefinitely in a
dead straight line.
“We’re dogging ’em close,” said I, pointing to the rear of the cab we
were pursuing. “He’s a good man,” I added, indicating our driver.
“Good enough,” replied Pawlinson shortly. “But what do you intend to
do next? Just what is the lay, anyway?”
He certainly could make me feel like a fool; and, as a matter of cold fact,
I certainly had acted, so far, with every trait of the tyro. Indeed, I had
simply counted upon locating the man and wiring the chief of his
whereabouts. But now what was I really counting upon doing?
“It’s plain enough we’ve got to dog ’em as close as we can,” said I
finally. “I don’t see any better way now, do you?”
“No, not now,” he replied. “But I have heard of better preparedness in
my time. But come, come, Grey”—and his tone lightened perceptibly
—“we’re in this thing together, and there’s no use of us rasping against each
other any more. I really stand in need of a man, and I hope you’ll prove to
be he. This case really means a lot to——”
“That’s just it!” I broke in. “If you’ll stop to think, you’ll see that I really
don’t know a solitary thing about the affairs except that the man ahead of us
yonder is wanted. The offense is all in the dark. Don’t you think yourself
that I’d be likely to enter into the thing with more spirit if I were shown a
little more light?”
He eyed me narrowly for a moment, as though he were deciding with
himself just how far he would go in explanation. Finally he reached his
decision with a grunt.
“You know about the robberies of country places along Long Island
Sound?” said he shortly.
He really needn’t have said another word to quicken me to the most
intense interest.
“I should say I did!” I ejaculated. “Why, it was just from overzeal in one
of those cases that the chief gave me the can.”
Pawlinson smiled.
“I told Garth I didn’t want the game flushed quite then,” he explained.
“Then you were back of that, too?” I cried, trying to get through the fog.
“Yes. And the man we are after is Stroth, Carl Stroth.”
“Stroth?” I queried dubiously, for the name carried no memory. “I never
heard of him.”
“I’m prepared to believe that you never did,” agreed Pawlinson; “for the
man has proven himself wise enough, and big enough, never to appear
personally connected with the jobs.”
“But how could he——”
“Here,” put in Pawlinson. “I’ll state it in a nutshell. About the robberies
along the Sound, you know. And you know, too, as do the rest of the police,
that the tricks were invariably pulled off by water—that is, the lads worked
by boat. But what they don’t know is that this man Carl Stroth is the head,
center, and chief of a gang—a thoroughly organized and completely
equipped gang of crooks.
“And just to show you how complete is the equipment—well—quickly
stated again—there’s a hundred-foot auxiliary schooner, speedy, with a
machine gun under a tarpaulin, manned by a crew, a real crew.”
He had a sharp, decisive way of narration.
“Sounds fishy, don’t it? Fishy, but a fact,” he went on. “Now, the way a
crib was cracked was this—is cracked, I suppose I had better say, for they
haven’t been once checked yet: The schooner lies well off shore—’most
any distance, in fact. Then the launch—the very launch, by the way, that
you figured in yourself yesterday—is manned by experts at this very game.
They land, clean out the place selected, back to the launch, back to the
schooner. So it’s really very simple, as you see. And in its simplicity lies its
great effectiveness. The schooner carries regular yacht papers, and
everything is quite proper. Get it?”
“I understand,” said I slowly; “but I can scarcely believe such a thing
possible in this day and generation.”
“Why not?” he snapped. “For the life of me I can’t see why the field for
absolute and out-and-out piracy isn’t greater now than ever. Pretty nearly
every invention, though it gives pursuit improvement, likewise equips the
pursued. The world’s very much the same—— But, heavens and earth,
man, this is scarcely the time to get too wordy; and I’ll let it out now that
the only reason I had in all this explanation was to watch your face closely
while I was telling it.”
“Why, what do you mean by that?” I cried.
“Well,” he explained haltingly, “Carl Stroth’s hand reaches far.”
“How?” I hadn’t yet caught the point.
“You see, as I said before,” he continued, “I wanted to be dead certain as
to just what part you were playing.”
“I was playing?” I exclaimed, my temper beginning to rise. “Do you
mean to say you thought that I, in any way, was connected with——”
“I mean no offense, my man,” said he imperturbably; “and you must
remember that I never so much as set eyes on you till yesterday; and what
happened then didn’t predispose me in your favor, naturally. I mean simply
that I have had just about dealings enough with this man Stroth to know that
he plays a sure game. And it wouldn’t have been the first time for him to
plant one of his own men right inside the enemy’s camp.”
Now, on mature thought, there really was nothing that I should have
taken umbrage at in this suspicion; and I certainly ought to have had better
control of myself.
But I felt myself fairly rise to boiling point. Words bubbled to me at this
implication as to my being a turncoat.
Those words never came out, though, for we skidded briskly around a
corner, and were thrown sharply back to the moment and to action.
In the intense concentration of the conversation, both of us had been
more engrossed with each other than with our chase; though I did notice,
out of the tail of my eye, that the driver of our car had shot back a quick
glance over his shoulder at us inside.
Then next second, as I say, we turned a corner—a sharp, right-angle
whirl from the straight avenue both cars had been following.
Pawlinson and I both leaned forward in interest, craning our necks for a
view through the front glasses of the vehicle.
“What in time does this mean?” cried he, as another vehicle shot by us,
coming from the opposite direction.
He started pounding on the glass.
But our driver, for a moment, paid no attention. Instead, he swung the
car toward the curb and brought up short before the door of a large house.
We both sprang out to the sidewalk, and as we stood there our little
chauffeur hastened to explain.
“Them folks stopped their car and went in that there house,” said he, in a
funny, staccato voice. “And the car’s gone back.”
Instinctively Pawlinson and I started to mount the first step of the stoop,
though reason might well have prompted another course.
Then the little chauffeur enlightened us further.
“And now I guess that’ll be about all to-day, gentlemen,” he chortled
gleefully, as he slid up his goggles to his forehead.
That kind of eye shield certainly is effective on occasion. For, just as the
taxi jumped into speed, it left us there on the sidewalk in our first but most
convincing recognition.
“Stevens!” we fairly yelled, in unison.
Then we looked at each other.
CHAPTER VI.

A WATER TRAIL.

“A rather close organization, you see,” said Pawlinson finally, and with a
placidity that was galling.
“What do you mean?” I queried shortly and a bit irritably. The incident
hadn’t improved the temper of either of us.
“I mean that Stroth had everything pretty nicely arranged, didn’t he? For
your reception, that is. Even to depositing you way out here, where——”
“Hold on!” I snapped sharply. “You yourself were duped as well as I
was, and we’ve got no time for words. What do you make out of this?”
“What do you make out of it yourself? According to your own account,
you left Stevens yesterday headed off somewhere in the launch from
College Point.”
“But that certainly was he just now,” said I.
“Most positively; and his little scheme has thus landed us not only well
out of the way of the whereabouts of Stroth, but I know enough of this town
to know that we can’t get another cab inside of fifteen minutes. How do you
figure the little devil happened in Portland, anyway?”
Pawlinson again favored me with one of those searching looks which
were making me hate him more and more heartily.
Fact was, I was getting vastly sick of the entire job. But a sudden
thought did, momentarily, throw me into ejaculation.
“By Jove, I’ve got it!” I cried. “Berth number nine!”
“Berth number nine! Here, come, man, we’ve no time to waste! We’ve
got to get onto some track immediately.”
“And you must have been the one who occupied number three on the
sleeper last night,” I continued.
“You deduce well,” said he, with a grin.
“Well, when I got my own reservation, the ticket seller told me that berth
nine was taken from Stamford. So that’s it. Stevens, after running the
launch somewhere up the Sound toward Connecticut, left it there and
boarded the same train, and from there on——”
“And from there on,” grunted Pawlinson, “even I can follow. But look
here. Why didn’t you come out with this sooner?”
“I didn’t connect the thing at all,” I replied.
“No; it seems not.”
“What I can’t make out, though,” I went on, unheeding his manner, “is
this: Why in thunder all the care taken to effect the boarding of the Portland
steamer by launch from Port Washington?”
“Simply enough understood, Grey,” he said, “when you realize that I had
every dock and railway station covered. And a mighty good scheme it was
—that of handling the thing by water. I might have known——”
He broke off short. A thought had hit him which brooked not an instant’s
further delay in speculation. Much as I had come to dislike the man, I could
but admire his promptitude and vim.
He tugged me into a brisk run, and we caught a trolley car which he had
managed to whistle to a stop on the instant it was crossing the nearest
avenue’s corner.
“To the water,” said he, in indefinite explanation, when we were seated.
“The water, that’s it! The cue word in this little job, I believe.”
“What do you mean?”
“Not until to-day have I ever known Stroth to use Stevens for a thing but
water work. Fact is, where Stevens is there’s a water trick.”
“You mean that you think——”
“I mean that I’d be willing to stake a hefty sum that the schooner is lying
this very minute not two hundred yards off Orr’s Island, right here in Casco
Bay.”
“The schooner?”
“Stroth’s boat—the one I was telling you about.”
“But, heavens and earth, man,” said I, raising my voice a bit from the
whisper we had kept in the car, “open and aboveboard, broad daylight like
this?”
“Didn’t I explain that she carries yacht papers, and isn’t even suspected
by a soul?”

You might also like