TILT For Couples Helping Couples Grow Together

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Transactional Analysis Journal

ISSN: 0362-1537 (Print) 2329-5244 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rtaj20

TILT for Couples: Helping Couples Grow Together

Kalman J. Kaplan

To cite this article: Kalman J. Kaplan (1990) TILT for Couples: Helping Couples Grow Together,
Transactional Analysis Journal, 20:4, 229-244, DOI: 10.1177/036215379002000404

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1177/036215379002000404

Published online: 28 Dec 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 52

View related articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rtaj20
TILT for Couples: Helping
Couples Grow Together
Kalman J. Kaplan
Abstract discussed by Bader and Pearson) involves the
TILT (Teaching Individuals to Live synchronous replacement of irnpenneable walls
Together), a two-axis interpersonal program by articulated boundaries. Individuals on this
to differentiate healthy individual develop- axis may differ in level of maturity (an especial-
ment from clinical pathology, is applied to ly poignant problem for some couples), but
couples. Healthy couple development is should not be considered pathological. In con-
defined as mutually integrated increases in trast, the clinical axis (as defined by Gobes,
levels of intra-individual individuation and 1985) involves nonsynchronicity between
attachment. Couple pathology is defined as removing walls and developing boundaries.
incongruency between individuation and at- Fear of abandonment results in premature wall
tachment on the part of the partners. TILT permeability (before the underlying boundary
for couples is a method to "tilt" couples off has been articulated); fear of engulfment or ab-
the pathological axis onto the developmen- sorption results in prolonged wall im-
tal one. permeability (after the underlying boundary has
been articulated). TILT suggests a clinical ap-
Two recent articles in the Transactional proach designed to "tilt" individuals off of the
Analysis Journal have specifically addressed the pathological axis onto the developmental one
issues of intimacy and space in couples. The (see Kaplan, 1988). This current article extends
first (Bader & Pearson, 1983) applied Mahler's the TILT model to couple development.
concepts of infant development to the
developmental stages of couplehood. Critical A Unidimensional Model of Family Cohesion
to this ingenious application is the observation In 1979, Olson, Sprenkle, and Russell of-
by Bader and Pearson (one with which I agree) fered the following definition of marital and
that "individuals seem to replay these phases family cohesion: "The emotional bonding
of early development through their couple rela- members have with one another and the degree
tionships" (p. 29). Bader and Pearson differen- of individual autonomy a person experiences
tiate couple types with regard to the level of in the family system" (p. 5). They suggested
developmental maturity achieved by each of the that this dimension underlies the varied concep-
partners. The second article (Gobes, 1985) tual frameworks of some of the most influen-
specifically examined the pathological implica- tial marital and family theories. They anchor
tions of unresolved abandonment and engulf- the two ends of the cohesion dimension as
ment issues. follows:
This article extends the application of a two- At the extreme of high family cohesion,
axis system developed for individuals (Kaplan, enmeshment, there is an over-identifi-
1988), one that simultaneously considers level cation with the family that results in ex-
of human development and clinical pathology. treme bonding and limited individual
The system-named TILT (Teaching In- autonomy. The low extreme, disengage-
dividuals to Live Together)-differentiates ment, is characterized by low bonding
boundaries (ego strengths) from walls (ego and high autonomy from the family. It
defenses), thus leading to the emergence of two is hypothesized that a balanced degree of
axes, one developmental and the other clinical. family cohesion is the most conducive to
The developmental axis (which has been effect family functioning and to optimum

Vol. 20, No.4, October 1990 229


KALMAN J. KAPLAN

individual development. (Olson et al., that allows for separate connectedness. Buber
1979, pp. 5, 6) (1957) similarly proposed an informal model
The balanced position is usually healthy and that envisions mature individuals as having the
falls between the two pathological extremes. capacity for both relation (bonding) and
Minuchin (1974, p. 54) described boundaries distance (autonomy), Bakan (1966) stressed the
in enmeshed families as diffuse, in disengaged dual human needs for agency and communion,
families as rigid, and in healthy families as and Rank (1936) distinguished fear of death
clear. Willi (1982) took a similar position, (union) and fear of life (separation). Finally,
although he introduced the concept of ex- transactional analysis theory itself has sepa-
tradyadic boundaries as well. Bowen (1960) rated the dimensions of "I'm OK-I'm Not
also worked with a unidimensional view of OK" and "You're OK-You're Not OK"
cohesiveness, assuming pathology at both ends (Berne, 1961, 1964; Ernst, 1971; Harris,
(emotional divorce versus emotional fusion- 1969; Kaplan, 1988; Kaplan, Capace & Clyde,
undifferentiated ego mass), with health in the 1984).
middle (differentiated self). Olson et al. (1979) In spite of our awareness that bonding and
worked with the same single dimension, autonomy both represent healthy human needs,
although they broke the scale into four points we often slip into proposing theories that pre-
(disengaged, separated, connected, and sent them as polarities. Even worse, we often
enmeshed) rather than three. make the same mistake clinically, sometimes
Despite this consensus, there remains with disastrous consequences. Olson et al. 's
something problematic about this unidimen- (1979) formal definition of cohesion (as op-
sional view of cohesion. There seems to be two posed to their informal comments), which
distinct processes underlying this dimension. views bonding and autonomy as reciprocally
The first is bonding or attachment; the second antagonistic, reflects this tendency. Olson,
is autonomy or individuation. The psychologi- Russell, and Sprenkle (1983) addressed this
cal literature is replete with evidence regarding issue as well as the Beavers and Voeller
each of these processes (e.g., Bowlby, 1969, criticism by dropping the autonomy dimension
1973, on attachment and separation; Mahler, from their definition of cohesion. This,
Pine & Bergman, 1975, on symbiosis and in- however, hardly represents a solution to the
dividuation; Kegan, 1982, and Kaplan & problem.
O'Connor, 1989, on the "evolving self'). Yet The only formal model that attempts to con-
the unidimensional definition of cohesion of- ceptually distinguish bonding and autonomy is
fered by Olson et al. (1979) as an integrating that of Lewis, Beavers, Gosset, and Phillips
principle seems to assume a necessarily inverse (1976) from the Timberlawn group. They pro-
relationship between the two processes-gain posed a closeness scale which attempts to take
in one accompanied by loss in the other. One into account both closeness (bonding) and ego
end of the dimension-enmeshment-involves boundaries (autonomy). The problem with their
high bonding and low autonomy; the other approach is that they force both concepts on a
end-disengagement-involves low bonding single measurement scale.
and high autonomy. Both of these extremes are Those family systems which, in their
pathological. Instead, a healthy middle category communication, reflected vague, amor-
is called for involving, presumably, moderate phous boundaries or fusion were at one
bonding and moderate autonomy. However, extreme of the scale; in the middle por-
such a definition seems oversimplified and con- tion were those families in which the
fused given the state of our knowledge (cf. separateness of the individuals was clear,
Beavers & Voeller, 1983) and does not allow but associated with considerable inter-
for the possibility that members of a truly personal distance. Families which dem-
healthy family might possess the capacity for onstrated closeness with distinct boun-
both high bonding and high autonomy. daries were rated at the other end of the
The capacity for high bonding and high scale. (p. 85)
autonomy is not a novel idea. Indeed, Olson Although Lewis et al. reported that their
et al. (1979) echoed the thoughts of Gibran closeness scale differentiated between healthy
(1968) in defining the middle category as one and dysfunctional families, its unidimensional

230 Transactional Analysis Journal


TILT FOR COUPLES: HELPING COUPLES GROW TOGETHER

quality makes it impossible to obtain truly which have not previously been described. B
separate measures of bonding and autonomy. represents a state of low bonding and low
In contrast, this article proposes a fully autonomy, characterized by impermeable walls
bidimensional view of family cohesion. and inarticulated boundaries (IQI). D represents
a state of high bonding and high autonomy,
A Bidimensional Model of Family Cohesion characterized by permeable walls and ar-
The thrust of our bidimensional definition of ticulated boundaries (CoJ). A/C (the outside
family cohesion is to treat bonding (attachment) square at the middle intersection) represents a
and autonomy (individuation) as two concep- position split between enmeshment and
tually independent bipolar dimensions. No disengagement (!2J), indicating a conflict on
necessary reciprocal antagonism between these both the autonomy and bonding dimensions.
two processes is assumed. The bonding dimen- The arrows underneath each of these posi-
sion is anchored at one end by attachment (high tions refer to individual distancing patterns.
bonding) and at the other by detachment (low These will be discussed in detail later in this
bonding). As such, bonding-attachment article.
represents an interpersonal construct I have developed a formal measuring instru-
("distance" from the other) and can be thought ment entitled' 'The Individuation-Attachment
of as the barrier or wall between individuals Questionnaire" (IAQ) to assess these different
within the family. Detachment refers to rigidi- autonomy-bonding positions. It has been de-
ty of this wall (0); attachment to its fined in detail (Kaplan, 1988) in terms of clari-
permeability (U). The autonomy dimension is fying the inherent ambiguity in our traditional
anchored at one end by individuation (high definitions of closeness and farness. A
autonomy) and at the other by deindividuation behavioral tendency toward interpersonal
(low autonomy). Thus, autonomy-individuation "closeness" (i.e., an approach response) may
represents an intrapersonal construct be indicative of either (or both) a need for at-
("distance" from the self) referring to the con- tachment or bonding (NA) and a fear of in-
tour or boundary defining an individual within dividuation or autonomy (PI). Likewise, a
the family. Individuation refers to the articula- behavioral tendency toward interpersonal
tion of this boundary (0); deindividuation "farness" (i.e., a withdrawal response) may
refers to a diffuseness in this regard ((). be indicative of either (or both) a need for in-
Figure 1 presents this model in bidimensional dividuation or autonomy (NI) or a fear of at-
space. The ordinate refers to the bonding or at- tachment or bonding (FA). The IAQ separates
tachment dimension, and the abscissa refers to these often fine distinctions through the con-
the autonomy or individuation dimension. Peo- struction of NA, FI, NI, and FA subscales.
ple may fall into different pairings of autonomy Consider several examples. Agreement with
and bonding; Figure 1 graphically illustrates six the statement "It is important for me to take
possible positions. Three of these-A, C, and other people's needs into account" may in-
E-represent points on the cohesion dimension dicate a need for attachment. However, en-
as defined by Olson et al. (1979). Position A dorsement of the slightly different statement,
represents a state of low autonomy and high "It is important for me to meet other people's
bonding. This enmeshed state is characterized expectations of me," may indicate afear ofin-
by permeable walls and inarticulated boun- dividuation. Likewise, agreement with the
daries (IQ]). Position C represents a state of high statement, "Other people's judgment of me
autonomy and low bonding. This disengaged seldom determines how I feel about myself"
state is characterized by impermeable walls and may indicate a need for individuation. On the
articulated boundaries (1Q1). Position E (the in- other hand, agreement with the statement" A
side square within the middle intersection) person does not need involvement with others
represents an inbetween state of moderate to be fulfilled" may indicate a fear of
autonomy and moderate bonding. It is attachment.
characterized by moderately permeable walls Several important advantages derive from
and moderately articulated boundaries (M). this bidimensional approach to cohesion: (1) It
Three additional positions-B, D, and provides the basis for a deeper understanding
A/C-emerge from this bidimensional view of health versus pathology than does a

Vol. 20. No.4, October 1990 231


KALMAN J. KAPLAN

High
r 1 r 1
I
I
I
,,-,
,_/
LA __ ..J
J
I
I : 0:
LD_ _ ..J
o P o P
~ +- ~ +-
+- +- +- ~

C)
C
'0
C
o a>

---
In
C
G)
~
L-
a> I - - - - - - - - - t - r - -
""0
o
E :::2:
.c
o
-
«
co

P
{+II .....
+11.....

[J B
0
I
'-"
I

P
@] 0 P
~ ~ ~ ~
+- +- +- ~

Low Moderate High


Individuation/Autonomy

Walls Boundaries
,..--,
I
I
I
I Permeable 0 Articulated

-
L __ ..1

Moderately Permeable Moderately Articulated


,,-,
D Impermeable [

'-"
J Inarticu lated

Figure 1
A Bidimensional View of Interpersonal Distancing Patterns

232 Transactional Analysis Journal


TILT FOR COUPLES: HELPING COUPLES GROW TOGETHER

Forward Regression to Next Stage

r- --,

10'
I
__ ..J
D I

-e(1)
E
.r.
o
C'I:l
:::::
«

Level One: Position B


Level Two: Position E
Level Three: Position D

Individuation

Figure 2a
The Individual Developmental Axis
unidimensional view, (2) it allows a precise ar- ceptualized as a pathological axis, the respec-
ticulation of different couple types, and (3) it tive positions (A and C, A ..... C, and A/C)
suggestsa therapy that may "tilt" couples away representing increasing levels of pathology. It
from pathological toward developmental is important to emphasize that immaturity is not
problems. equivalent to pathology, although it may
become so if maturity is not achieved when ap-
Healthy versus Pathological Cohesion: A propriate for the organism.
Distinction between Two Axes Consider first the BED axis of Figure 2a. The
The bidimensional view of family cohesion underlying logic of this axis is that healthy
presented here proposes a distinction in terms development involves the graduated and in-
of axes rather than positions and as a matter tegrated replacement of a rigid wall (nonbond-
of degree rather than a dichotomy. More ing) with an articulated boundary (autonomy).
specifically, two axes can be differentiated in That is, the subdimensions of bonding and
Figures 2a and 2b. The first can be labeled the autonomy are not viewed as antagonistic, but
BED axis (Fig 2a), the second, the AC axis (Fig as working in concert. In fact, such coordina-
2b). The BED axis is conceptualized as a tion is postulated to be the sine qua non of
developmental axis; each position on the axis healthy development, increases in bonding
(B, E, or D) is seen as healthy, although vary- made possible by increases in autonomy.
ing in degree of maturity. The AC axis is con- According to Mahler et al. (1975) the young

Vol. 20, No.4, October /990 233


KALMAN J. KAPLAN

r -,
I , I ~.
I" )
~
I
'-" I Q)~.
L _A~", O~
Q);r
O~
r - -- t9~
~
-c:
(I)
I
I
I
0 Ale
~C9

E
.c:
o
CU
::::
«

Enmeshed: Position A
Disengaged: Position C
Oscillatory: Position A/C

Individuation

Figure 2b
The Individual Pathological Axis
infant (0 to 2 months) is in a state of normal permeable wall). This is characteristic of an E
autism with no differentiation between the in- position. Finally, at 10 months the infant begins
fant and the outside world. In other words, the the practicing phase; the baby is excited about
infant's boundary has not yet been articulated. developing his or her own abilities (i.e., ar-
During this period the mother must serve the ticulating his/her boundaries) and by the emerg-
infant as a "dual unity with one common boun- ing capacity to do things away from mother
dary," (Mahler et aI., 1975, p. 44), a "pro- (i.e., make the wall fully permeable). This is
tective shield" (Khan, 1964, p. 272), or, in characteristic of the mature D position.
terms of this article, an "impermeable wall" Such a progression over these three levels of
to protect the underlying inarticulated boun- maturity seems to occur again and again over
dary. Thus, the autistic infant can be described the life span, albeit at increasingly complex
as occupying position B. Slowly the processes stages of development. Maturity at one stage
of separation-individuation emerge. By the age of complexity may be sacrificed for temporary
of 5 to 6 months the more differentiating in- immaturity at the next. Thus, medical students
fant begins to define his or her external boun- may become less remote and defensive (less
daries by touching fingers and toes (i.e., to rigidity in walls) as they become more certain
moderately articulate his or her boundary). At of their ability to withstand the emotional and
the same time, the child begins to pull away intellectual demands of the profession (greater
from mother (i.e., to create a moderately articulateness in boundaries). However, the

234 Transactional Analysis Journal


TILT FOR COUPLES: HELPING COUPLES GROW TOGETHER

remoteness and defensiveness may temporari- personalities organized at the borderline level
ly return when they begin their own practices. (Kernberg, 1975). In more extreme cases, this
To give another example, a young couple polarization may result in a psychotic splitting
may become more capable of intimacy with one between fusion and isolation (A/C), producing
another as they become certain of themselves. irreconcilable pressures in both directions at the
However, a forward regression (i.e., regres- same time.
sion in level in the service of an advance in
stage) to a more advanced life style may make Cohesion and Couple Type: A Bidimensional
them temporarily more defensive. Each posi- Distancing Analysis
tion may be healthy at a given point in develop- Therapists who work with individuals
ment. For example, rigid walls (nonbonding) typically use different terms from those used
may be necessary to defend diffuse boundaries by marriage and family therapists. For exam-
(nonautonomy). However, the immaturity ofB ple, the former might speak of narcissistic,
is preferable to the pathology of A or C. In con- borderline, and psychotic pathology, whereas
trast, the development of articulated boundaries the latter describe marriages and families as
(autonomy) can allow the possibility of more enmeshed, disengaged, or as rejection-intrusion
permeable walls (bonding). In other words, types. The system described in this article
nonbonding is replaced in healthy development bridges this gap by using the same interpersonal
by autonomy (B-+E-+D). distancing terms to characterize individual and
The AC axis of Figure 2b is very different couple positions.
in this regard. It is, by its very nature, Individual Distancing Patterns. Individual
pathological, involving reciprocal antagonism distancing patterns are presented under the
between the subdimensions of bonding and respective positions in Figure 1. Persons with
autonomy. Relaxing one's wall prematurely different capacities for bonding and autonomy
(before one's boundary is sufficiently ar- should be expected to react differentially to in-
ticulated) produces the embeddedness variant approach and withdrawal probes on the
(nonautonomous bonding) characteristic of part of a hypothetical other. For simplicity, the
position A. Keeping the wall rigid when that "focal person" is represented as P and the
is no longer appropriate (when one's boundary "other" as O.
is no longer diffuse) produces the disengage- Let us first consider the healthy BED axis in
ment (nonbonded autonomy) of position C. Figure 1. A P in position B is immature in that
Mahler et al. (1975) provided early childhood he or she is unable to be either bonded or
examples of A and C pathologies. An example autonomous and will thus block overtures of
of A is the process of "shadowing" (p. both approach and withdrawal on the part of
133-137), where the child does not let the O. D's approaches are likely to elicit fears of
mother out of his or her sight. The C pathology attachment, P withdrawing in response. D's
is exemplified by "darting away" (p. 133-137), withdrawals, in contrast, may elicit fears of in-
where the toddler continuously runs away from dividuation, P approaching in response. Such
mother, perhaps in an attempt to induce her to a pattern (;!;!) fits the compensation or
chase the child (see Gobes, 1985, for a good "distance-equilibrium" principle described in
discussion of abandonment and engulfment the literature of social and clinical psychology
issues). (Argyle & Dean, 1965, p. 293). In position E,
Both of these positions are narcissistic in the P is moderately bonded and moderately
Kohutian sense (Kohut, 1971)-the A position autonomous and characterized as emergent. As
reflecting "idealizing narcissism ("you are such, he or she is likely to remain stationary
perfect, but I am part of you")" (p. 27) and in response to both approach and withdrawal
the C position "mirroring narcissism ("I am on the part of 0 (=:). Finally, the D position
perfect")" (p. 27). Each position in itself is describes a P who has the capacity for both high
probably organized at the neurotic level. bonding and high autonomy. He or she should
However, the imbalance of these two polarized thus respond to 0's overtures of approach
positions may produce a tendency to oscillate with his or her own approach, and D's over-
between them (A.....C). Such alternating be- tures of withdrawal with his or her own
tween fusion and isolation is characteristic of withdrawal. Such a pattern (~) is called

Vol. 20, No.4, October 1990 235


KALMAN J. KAPLAN

Partner P2
Healthy
B E D A C
...... ~ ...... ......
~ +- ~ ~

B [Q] [Q]
1.._.' I
'-'
I
[Q] f61
1.. _)
!._-_.!
Regressed- Compensatory
Reciprocal

. .
E [Q] [Q]
Emerging-
Reciprocal
~ ~ ~ ......
+- +- +- ~

Partner P
D f01 [Q]
'-'
!.._-_.!
I I f01 rOi
!.._-_.! !.._-_.!
1 Advanced-
Compensatory
Reciprocal

~ ...... ~ ~

r:=:: r:=::
A
[~~~] ,I
!..~::.!
I, II I,
!...:::.! [Q]
Enmeshed Rejection-
Intrusion
...... ...... ...... ~

C [QJ iOi I
I
!._-_.!
I
I
[QJ [Q]
Rejection-
Disengaged
Intrusion

Pathological

j---"'j
I
I
l 1
I
I Permeable Wall o Articulated Boundary

D Moderately Permeable Wall o


,-,
Moderately Articulated Boundary

D Impermeable Wall \_} Inarticulated Boundary

Figure 3
Couple Distancing Patterns

236 Transactional Analysis Journal


TILT FOR COUPLES: HELPING COUPLES GROW TOGETHER

reciprocity or the "dyadic effect" (Jourard & BB state (see Bader & Pearson, 1983, p. 29).
Friedman, 1970, p. 279) in the social and This is the "falling in love" state experienced
clinical literature. by new lovers. The present analysis diagnoses
Now consider the pathological AC axis in them as rushing together in a mutual flight from
Figure 1. In position A, P possesses the capaci- autonomy. Each may feel liberated from
ty for high bonding but low autonomy. As such, aloneness; however, neither is secure enough
he or she should match O's approach overtures, in himself or herself to tolerate genuine in-
but block those of withdrawal. This results in timacy. Each loves his or her idealization of
a noncontingent approach (enmeshed) pattern the other. Together they engage in a regressed
(;!::), P approaching regardless of the direc- (immature) reciprocal relationship, conflict
tion of O's initial distance move. In position emerging when the partners in the couple get
C, in contrast, P possesses the capacity for high
autonomy but low bonding. As such, he or she
should match O's withdrawal overtures, but at-
(=
too intimate with each other or too remote
;!). Each of the parties maintains an im-
permeable wall because his or her boundary is
tempt to block those of approach. This results not yet articulated. This is similar to the
in a noncontingent avoidance (disengaged) pat- "symbiotic-symbiotic" couple type described by
tern (;! ::), with P withdrawing regardless of Bader and Pearson (1983, p. 30).
the direction of O's initial distance move. Both Position EE represents the second stage of
of these patterns are characterized as neurotic. healthy couple development, moving past the
A P vacillating between A and C (A....C) falling-in-love state. Now each partner is semi-
should likewise show borderline vacillation be- individuated and semi-attached. Neither fears
tween noncontingent approach (enmeshment) nor needs either bonding or autonomy. Each
and noncontingent avoidance (disengagement). is in a half-way position on all fronts and may
Finally, a P in the A/C position is psychotic be characterized as emergent. Together they
and should show simultaneous tendencies to ap- engage in what appears to be a static relation-
proach and withdraw in response to both ap- ship, neither partner desiring too much in-
proach and withdrawal on the part of 0 timacy or too much space ("). This is called
(;!::II::). an emerging reciprocal or holding relationship,
Couple Distancing Patterns. (For simplici- similar to the "practicing-practicing" couple
ty, we disregard gender here, denoting one subtype described by Bader and Pearson (1983,
member of the couple as PI and the second as p. 31).
P 2 . ) Each of the partners may occupy the Position DD represents the third stage of
various cohesion positions discussed previous- healthy couple development. Finally the part-
ly. We make several admittedly oversimplified ners have come to love each other as the other
assumptions. First, marriages between a really is. This position represents two mature,
healthy and a pathological partner will not sur- highly individuated and attached partners, who
vive. Thus our discussion is limited to couples need both bonding and autonomy. As such,
with two healthy partners or two pathological they will engage in what we call an advanced
partners. Second, pathological couples will on- (mature) reciprocal relationship in which they
ly remain together if partners are at the same coordinate mutual desires for intimacy ("we"-
level of pathology (e.g., two neurotics, two ness) and space ("I"-ness) (;! =). Walls
borderlines, or two psychotics). Finally, mar- become permeable with more articulated boun-
riages of healthy couples will only survive if daries. This describes the "mutual
the two partners are at the same stage of interdependence-mutual interdependence" sub-
development (for example, BB, EE, or DD). type described by Bader and Pearson (1983, p.
We will comment later on one interesting ex- 31).
ception to this final rule, what we call a com- Positions DB and BD represent quite
pensating couple (BD or DB). anomalous situations. One partner (the D) in
Let us consider healthy couple types as each relationship is mature-at the third level
shown in BB, EE, DD, and BD or DB in Figure of development; the other (the B) is
3. One of the peculiarities of beginning a rela- immature-at the first level. D will offer bond-
tionship is that it forwardly regresses separately ing and autonomy; B will block both. The result
mature individuals into a temporarily immature is a compensatory pattern (;!;!). The less

Vol. 20, No.4, October 1990 237


·KALMAN J. KAPLAN

mature B partner will block the more mature ness) and the C continually avoids (wants an
partner's offers of either bonding or autonomy. l-ness) (-+-+).
This is similar to the "symbiotic-practicing" Position (A+-+C) (A....C) represents a cou-
couple type described by Bader and Pearson, ple consisting of two borderline individuals
(1983, p. 31). whose relationship is organized at the
All other possible healthy couples involve borderline level. They vacillate between
one partner who is at position E and the other enmeshed and disengaged distancing patterns.
at either position Bor D (either BE or DE). Finally, position (A/C) (A/C) represents a cou-
Such a pairing will invariably produce a non- ple consisting of two psychotic individuals
contingent indifferent pattern and, as such, will whose relationship will be at the psychotic
not be specifically delineated in Figure 3. level. Each partner will give mutually conflic-
Consider now the purely pathological types ting responses to the other (... 11-+ ...11-+),
in Figure 3. First there is the neurotic level of tending to produce a mutual paralysis.
pathology: couples consisting of two As, two
Cs, or one of each. In position AA, both par- TILT for Couples: A Theory and Therapy
ties are afraid of autonomy but not of bonding for Responsive Couple Growth
(i.e., they are attached but not individuated). This article offers a bidimensional definition
Gobes (1985) described these individuals as of cohesion which presents health as a separate
having abandonment issues. Minuchin (1974, axis from pathology rather than as a moderate
p. 54) calls this an enmeshed or embedded rela- position on a unidimensional axis.
tionship, conflict emerging when the parties in Several examples further illustrate this
the couple become too remote from each other distinction. Carole complains that Michael goes
(-+"'). Each party's wall is too permeable to out too much with friends on his time off from
protect his or her inarticulated underlying boun- work (the entire weekend). Michael complains
daries, and each is looking for the omnipotent that Carole wants him to spend all his spare
other to save him or her from himself or herself time with her (leaving him no time on the
(i.e., they are both idealizing narcissists weekend to go his own way). A unidimensional
[Kohut, 1971]). In position CC, both parties therapist may see both parties as pathologi-
are afraid of bonding but not of autonomy (i.e., cal-Carole as enmeshed (high bonding, low
they are individuated but not attached). Each autonomy) and Michael as disengaged (low
has engulfment issues (Gobes, 1985). As such, bonding, high autonomy). The therapist might
they will engage in what Minuchin (1974, p. hammer out a practical compromise (moderate
54) calls a disengaged or isolated relationship, bonding, moderate autonomy) allowing
conflict emerging when the parties in the cou- Michael and Carole one day on their own each
ple become too intimate with each other weekend and one day together. However, this
(... -+). Each party maintains an impermeable solution does not address the problem from a
wall around an articulated boundary, and each bidimensional perspective. In contrast, the
has an overly grandiose sense ofself (i.e., both bidimensional therapeutic goal is to restructure
are mirroring narcissists). Position AC the situation to enable both Carole and Michael
represents what Napier (1978; p. 11) labeled to express their respective identities in the com-
a "rejection-intrusion" relationship and what pany of the other.
Willi (1982, p. 76) called a "narcissistic col- In another example Dan complains that he
lusion." One partner, C, is the mirroring nar- feels exploited because he spent too much time
cissist, afraid of entrapment and engulfment (six hours) looking for a present for Judy, and
(Gobes, 1985) by the partner and looking to he resents her for it. However, on a prior oc-
become the ideal self of the partner. Partner casion he felt guilty and selfish for only spend-
A, in contrast, is the idealizing narcissist, afraid ing a half-hour looking for a present. A
of individual responsibility and abandonment unidimensional perspective might view Dan's
(Gobes, 1985) by the other partner and look- behavior on the first occasion as disengaged
ing for a substitute self in that partner. The C (Position C) and on the second occasion as
partner is so grandiose because the A partner enmeshed (Position A), with a potential solu-
is effusively adoring. In behavioral terms, the tion simply to spend a middle ground of 3-114
A partner continually approaches (desires a we- hours looking for a present for Judy. In

238 Transactional Analysis Journal


TILT FOR COUPLES: HELPING COUPLES GROW TOGETHER

Forward Regression to Next Stage

-l:
Q)
E
.s:::
o
t1J
::::
<C

-,
....
(
,_/) Level One: Position B B
B Level Two: Position E E
Level Three: Position 0 0

Individuation

Figure 4a
The Couple Developmental Axis
contrast, a bidimensional perspective would in love to loving. Cell BB is the first level (fall-
focus on the more basic question of what it ing in love) of couple development, with neither
means for Dan to feel exploited. Perhaps he party having the capacity for individuation or
could make a gift for Judy that would repre- attachment (i.e., impermeable walls around in-
sent a deep expression of himself, with the time articulated boundaries). Cell EE is the second
it took to make the gift simultaneously level (learning to love); both partners are now
representing individuation and attachment. semi-individuated and semi-attached (i.e.,
With both of these couples, a unidimensional moderate walls around moderate boundaries).
therapeutic goal might be to achieve an EE Cell DD is the third level (loving) of couple
resolution to an AC conflict. However, a development, where both parties have the
bidimensional goal ultimately involves the capacity for both individuation and attachment
potential for a DD relationship, even if the cou- (i.e., permeable walls around articulated boun-
ple must initially regress to BB. daries). Although not represented on the axis,
Problems such as these have prompted the Cell BD is a mixed-stage couple, with one
application of TILT (Teaching Individuals to member being at the third level of one stage
Live Together) to couples. The outlines of this and the partner being at the first level of the
program emerge in the multi-stage, multi-level succeeding stage. This may represent only a
approach to couple growth presented in Figures temporary imbalance until the D partner for-
4a and 4b. wardly regresses to a B at the next advanced
We begin by describing the developmental stage.
BED axis in Figure 4a. It represents the The pathological AC axis in Figure 4b is
development of a couple from a state of falling quite different. It represents oscillation between

Vol. 20. No.4. October 1990 239


KALMAN J. KAPLAN

I
I
L

-
c:
E
G)

s:
o
C'lS
::::
<C

Enmeshed: Position A A
Disengaged: Position C C
Rejection-Intrusion: Position A C

Individuation

Figure 4b
The Couple Pathological Axis
enmeshment and disengagement. Position AA again. Third, all of these moves involve coor-
represents an enmeshed couple type, CC a dination of the two partners or the couple will
disengaged couple type, and AC a rejection- disintegrate. For example, the regression of
intrusion couple type. All of these position are two A's, two C's, or one A and one C to B
neurotic. (Af-+C) (Af-+C) represents a must be coordinated, just as must be their ad-
borderline couple type and (A/C) (A/C) a vance to E and then to D and finally their reen-
psychotic type. Neither is represented on the try at Cell B at a more advanced stage.
AC axis per se. The logic underlying this approach is that
Three aspects of this approach should be em- healthy couple growth involves the coordinated
phasized. First, there is no route between any and graduated replacement of interpersonal
position on the AC axis and Cell DD. Cells walls by self-other boundaries. Taking the walls
AA, CC, and AC represent dead ends in terms down prematurely (i.e., before the boundaries
of moving ahead, as do cells (A....C), are strong enough)-as in AA-or leaving them
(A....C), and (A/C) (A/C). The only possibili- up too long (i.e., after the boundaries have
ty for growth involves regression back to Cell already formed)-as in CC-leads to the
BB and coordinated advance through Cell EE respective pathological outcomes of embedded
to Cell DD. Second, the attainment of Cell DD and isolated structures. Splitting these dysfunc-
(Level 3) at one level opens up the possibility tions according to stereotyped male and female
of forward regression to a more advanced socialization (male C and female A) leads to
stage. However, this more advanced stage must the narcissistic collusion of the AC relationship
be entered at BB (Level I)-that is, the where the idealizing wife (A) intrudes and
beginning-and the process must be undergone the mirroring husband (C) rejects. The

240 Transactional Analysis Journal


TILT FOR COUPLES: HELPING COUPLES GROW TOGETHER

unsatisfactory aspects of the more severe AC through EE to DD, (3) forward regression to
couple pathologies speak for themselves and BB at the next advanced stage, and (4) coor-
will not be discussed further here. It is clear dination between the two members of a couple
that only unsatisfactory oscillations and collu- at each of the above points.
sions are possible for couples on the AC axis (1) Regression. It is no easy task to convince
unless both partners are regressed back to the an AA, CC, or AC couple to give up the quick
BB position and TILTED forward on the BED fix of full attachment (A) or individuation (C)
axis in an integrated, coordinated, and non- to return to the seemingly hopeless BB posi-
collusive manner. tion. The allure of the AC relationship is
Some implications of this model are quite especially difficult to give up. Yet, if the TILT
disturbing. The striving of many couples on the model is correct, this is exactly what is re-
AC axis to escape their positions may simply quired. A's must temporarily put up walls, and
change or reverse the nature of the collusion, C's must temporarily eliminate their boun-
bringing neither member of the couple closer daries. Therapeutic interventions designed to
to the D position (individuatedattachment). For foster this process include, in an AA couple,
example, the enmeshed AA couple (dein- encouraging both people to disengage from
dividuated attachment) striving for individua- overconcern about their partners' problems. In
tion is likely to flip into the equally unsatisfac- a CC couple, both members are encouraged to
tory isolated CC position. Likewise, the loosen their hyperidentities. In an AC couple,
isolated CC couple (detached individuation) is the A member is encouraged to disengage from
likely to flip into the equally unsatisfactory overinvolvement with the partner, and the C
enmeshed AA position. member is encouraged to loosen his or her
Finally, in the rejection-intrusion AC cou- hyperidentity. The following examples il-
ple the liberation of a traditional enmeshed lustrate these points.
female partner (A) may flip her into the C posi- The AA couple. Mr. and Ms. A present with
tion; likewise, the liberation of the traditional mutual complaints regarding a lack of space in
isolated male partner (C) may flip him into the the relationship. They each feel smothered by
A position. This is exactly the finding of the demands of the other. We do not, at this
Neugarten and Gutmann (1958) on midlife point, advise the partners to begin to assert their
shifts in sex roles. The A and C positions are identities. If either tries, he or she will go to
simply reversed without solving the basic col- the other extreme, isolating himself or herself
lusion; individuation is achieved at the expense from the other in the name of identity (i.e.,
of attachment and vice versa. This will be true becoming a CC couple). Instead, Mr. and Ms.
for the three examples given unless the couple A are encouraged to temporarily withdraw
is TILTED off of the AC axis and reoriented from one another without asserting their respec-
ahead on the BED axis. However, this requires tive identities (i.e., regression to BB) in order
a coordinated regression from level 2 (AA, CC to provide mutual walls that allow each to avoid
or AC) to level I (BB), then a sometimes the other's demands and expectations. In our
frustratingly slow progression through level 2 judgment, neither Mr. or Ms. A is ready for
EE to level 3 (DD), and finally, after level 3 moves in such areas as career or school.
(DD) has been attained, a willingness to ad- However, they are able to disconnect from one
vance to a more complicated stage with the another, as frightening as this may seem to in-
regression to level I (BB) that such growth en- dividuals in their position.
tails. (Note that this reverses the language of The CC couple. Mr. and Ms. C present with
my earlier article [Kaplan, 1988] with regard mutual complaints regarding a lack of intimacy
to stage and level [see Kaplan & O'Connor, in the relationship. They each feel isolated. We
1989] in order to be consistent with do not, at this point, advise either to become
developmental theory.) closer to one another. If either tries, he or she
will go to the other extreme, blurring his or her
The Goals of the TILT Program sense of self in the name of intimacy (i.e.,
The TILT program has four goals: (1) becoming an AA couple). Instead, we simply
regression of a couple off of the AC collusive encourage both Mr. and Ms. C to temporarily
axis back to BB, (2) integrated progression blur their pseudo-identities without establishing

Vol. 20. No.4. October 1990 241


KALMAN J. KAPLAN

intimacy with one another (i.e., regress to BB). tience and must proceed one step at a time.
The point is to lessen the parties' own involve- What is required is the selection of interper-
ment with self, creating the isolated condition sonal behaviors that are simultaneously in-
while the protective walls are stilI maintained. dividuating and attaching and a couple environ-
Neither Mr. or Ms. C is ready for activities ment that permits and supports this. Finding
such as becoming more responsive to one such behaviors may require some creativity.
another or taking each other's needs more into For example, in the couple deciding how much
account. However, they are each able to time to spend looking for gifts for one another,
disengage somewhat from their own concerns, what is needed is a mutual redefinition of the
as frightening as that may be to people in their situation. Each, for example, can use his or her
condition. talents to create a personal present for the other.
The AC couple. Mr. C and Ms. A present The act ofcreating the present is self-enhancing
with symptoms that fit the rejection-intrusion and individuating. The offering of that self-
pattern described earlier. Mr. C feels expression to the other is the height of attach-
smothered and wants more space. Ms. A feels ment. Thus, as the relationship matures, a con-
isolated and wants more intimacy. Although the text emerges containing a repertoire of actions
temptation is great, we do not at this time ad- that are simultaneously individuating and
vise Mr. C to put himself more into the rela- attaching.
tionship and Ms. A to develop her own in- The three major problems of this stage are:
terests. If possible, such moves would simply (1) giving up quick attachments (Cell A moves)
reverse the roles between Mr. C and Ms. A and/or individuations (Cell C moves), (2) fin-
(Mr. C becoming an A and Ms. A a C) without ding a repertoire of behaviors that
establishing the desired balance in either of simultaneously individuate and attach (Cell E
them. Instead, we encourage Mr. C to tem- moves), and (3) a couple environment that per-
porarily blur his pseudo-identity without in- mits and supports this process. Such slow but
creasing his push toward intimacy and Ms. A integrated progress pushes a couple eventual-
to temporarily withdraw from Mr. C without ly to the DD position and involves giving up
asserting her own identity (i. e., we attempt to mutual walls one step at a time with graduated
regress the couple to BB). The point is to erase increases in self-other boundaries.
the oppositional set between self- and other- (3) Forward Regression. The process
involvement. Regression for each of them, described above recurs throughout the life cy-
from both the A and C positions, may provide cle for the healthy couple, at increasingly more
the initial balance to make integrated individua- complicated stages of development. This in-
tion and attachment moves possible. volves the third challenge for TILT: preparing
(2) Integrated Stage Progression. It is not a couple that reaches the third level (Cell DD)
easy to convince a couple at BB to give up the at stage X to move ahead to stage X + I even
immediate gratification of full attachment (AA) though this involves a regression back to the
or full individuation (CC). It may be even first level (Cell BB). Such moves occur often
harder to convince a BB couple to give up the in life in a natural way in reaction to en-
seeming completeness of an AC relationship. vironmental stressors. For example, a couple
The deficits of each partner are disguised by who had dated for three years may decide to
the collusion between them. Indeed, this has live together, a couple used to living together
been the implicit contract beneath many ap- may marry, and a couple may move to a new
parently successful marriages, the individual city with a faster environment. The TILT
deficits only becoming manifest when the cou- system suggests that in each of these cases the
ple disintegrates through death of one partner, couple will experience regression in level in the
divorce, or separation. process of advancing in stage. Thus, it is called
We discourage the immediate fix of full at- "forward regression." Walls become tem-
tachment (A) and individuation (C), instead en- porarily less permeable as boundaries become
couraging the partners in a BB couple to each less articulated. The therapist's job is to en-
do a little bit of both (EE), which ultimately courage couples to pay attention to the "itch"
leads to full individuation and attachment for that signals that they have outgrown their stage,
each (DD). However, this takes time and pa- even though they have reached a healthy level

242 Transactional Analysis Journal


TILT FOR COUPLES: HELPING COUPLES GROW TOGETHER

there, with impermeable walls around ar- an empirical study by Kaplan and Maldaver
ticulated boundaries. In their advance as a cou- (1989) specifically demonstrated that marital
ple they will encounter environmental stressors patterns on the AC axis are significantly more
(e.g., marriage, moving, job change) and must associated with completed adolescent suicide
expect to have to erect temporary walls once than are those on the BED axis.
again.
(4) Coordination. The moves of the two part- Kalman J. Kaplan, Ph.D., is Professor of
ners in a couple must be coordinated. For ex- Psychology, Wayne State University. Please
ample, if one partner in a BB couple moves to send reprint requests to Dr. Kaplan, Depart-
E while the other jumps at the quick fix of an ment of Psychology, Wayne State University,
A or C position, the couple may disintegrate, Detroit, MI48202, U.S.A.
psychologically if not physically. If one
member of an AC couple is willing to regress
to B while the other is not, a similar disruption REFERENCES
Argyle. M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, distance and
may occur. Finally, if one member of a DD affiliation. Sociometry, 28, 289-304.
couple is willing to forwardly regress to level Bader, E., & Pearson, P. (1983). The developmentalstages
B at a more advanced stage of development of couplehood. Transactional Analysis Journal, 13,
while the other member is not so willing, the 28-32.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: Isola-
couple may transform into an unstable and tion and communion in Western man. Boston: Beacon
compensatory relationship (DB). The therapist Press.
must simultaneously consider the needs of the Beavers, W. R., & Voeller, M. (1983). Family models
couple as well as the needs of the individual comparing and contrasting the Olson circumplex model
partners. To this end the therapist must en- with the Beavers systems model. Family Process. 22,
88-95.
courage each partner to pace his or her growth Berne, E. (1961). Transactionalanalysis in psychotherapy.
and regression moves and even his or her "tilt" New York: Grove Press
off the axis to remain synchronous with those Berne, E. (1964). Games people play. New York: Grove
of the partner. Press.
Bowen, M. (1960). The family as the unit of study and
treatment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 31.
Implications for Children 40-60.
In this article we have not yet touched on the Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
position of children in these different couple Bowlby, J. (1973). Separation: Anxiety and anger. New
types. For example, a couple organized at the York: Basic Books.
Buber, M. (1957). Distance and relation. Psychiatry, 20,
neurotic level (AC) may produce a child or 97-104.
adolescent who vacillates in borderline fashion Ernst, F. (1971). The OK corral: The grid for get-on-with.
between the two positions (A.....C) or who Transactional Analysis Journal, 1(4), 33-42.
psychotically introjects this conflict (A/C). In Gibran, K. (1968). The prophet. New York: Alfred A.
Knopf.
other words, children may represent a more Gobes, L. (1985). Abandonment and engulfment issues in
serious level of pathology than do the parents. relationshiptherapy. TransactionalAnalysis Journal. 15,
A goal of family therapy may be to transform 216-219.
this type of family to a healthy one in which Harris, T. (1969). I'm okay. you're okay. New York:
both parents are D (DD) and the child is B, with Harper and Row.
Jourard, S. M., & Friedman, R. (1970). Experimenter-
the B child avoiding the temptations of subject "distance" and self-disclosure. Journal ofPer-
premature bonding (A) or autonomy (C). This, sonaltiy and Social Psychology, 15, 278-282.
of course, may involve a temporary regression Kaplan, K. J. (1988). TILT: Teaching individuals to live
on the part of the parents to BB (AC-+BB) together. Transactional Analysis Journal, 18,220-230.
Kaplan, K. J., Capace, N. K., & Clyde, J. D. (1984). A
before they are able to move ahead bidimensional distancing approach to transactional
(BB-+EE-+DD). The therapist must shield (D) analysis: A suggested revision of the OK corral. Trans-
the child during this troubled time, allowing actional Analysis Journal, 14, 114-119.
him or her to be a child (B). In a recent book, Kaplan, K. J., & Maldaver, M. (1989, June). Parental
we compared Biblicaland Greek families in this marital pathology and completed adolescent suicide: An
empirical study. Paper presented at the meeting of the
regard (Kaplan, Schwartz, & Markus-Kaplan International Association for Suicide Prevention,
1984), drawing implications for an understand- Brussels, Belgium.
ing of child and adolescent suicide. In addition, Kaplan, K. J., & O'Connor, N. A. (1989, August). From

Vol. 20. No.4. October 1990 243


KALMAN J. KAPLAN

mistrust TO trust: Through a stage vertically. Paper Simon and Schuster.


presented at the Ninety-seventh Meeting of the American Minuchin, S. (1974). Families andfamily therapy. Boston:
Psychological Association. New Orleans, LA. Harvard University Press.
Kaplan, K. J., Schwartz, M. W., & Markus-Kaplan, M. Napier, A. Y. (1978). The rejection-intrusion pattern: A
(1984). Thefamily: Biblical and psychological founda- central family dynamic. Journal of Marital and Family
tions. New York: Human Sciences Press. Counselling, 4. 5-12.
Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self: Problem and process Neugarten, B., & Gutmann, D. (1958). Age-sex roles and
in human development. Harvard University Press: Cam- personality in middle age: A TAT study. Psychological
bridge, MA. . Monographs. 72, Whole No. 470.
Kernberg , O. (1975). Borderline conditions and Olson, D. M., Sprenkle, D. M., & Russell, C. S. (1979).
pathological narcissism. New York: Jason Aronson. Circumplex model of marital and family systems: I.
Khan, M. M. R. (1964). Ego distortion, cumulative trauma, Cohesion and adaptability dimensions, family types, and
and the role of reconstruction in the analytic situation. clinical implications. Family Process, 18. 3-28.
lnternational Journal of Psychoanalysis, 45, 272-279. Olson, D. M., Russell, C. S., & Sprenkle, D. M., (1983).
Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis ofThe self New York: In- Circumplex model of marital and family systems: VI.
ternational University Press. Theoretical update. Family Process, 22. 69-83.
Lewis, J. M., Beavers, W. R., Gosset, J. T., & Phillips, Rank, O. (1936). Will therapy. New York: Alfred A.
V. A. (1976). No single thread: Psychological health in Knopf.
family systems. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Willi, J. (1982). Couples in collusion. New York: Jason
Mahler, M., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (1975). The Aronson.
psychological birth of the human infant. New York:

244 Transactional Analysis Journal

You might also like