Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/13629560

Personality and social change: Individual differences, life path, and


importance attributed to the women's movement

Article in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology · July 1998


DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1545 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
40 208

2 authors, including:

Lauren E Duncan
Smith College
44 PUBLICATIONS 1,194 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Psychobiography of Gloria Steinem View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Lauren E Duncan on 04 January 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1998, Vol. 74, No. 6, 1545-1555 0022-3514/98/S3.00

Personality and Social Change: Individual Differences, Life Path,


and Importance Attributed to the Women's Movement
Gail S. Agronick Lauren E. Duncan
University of California, Berkeley Harvey Mudd College

This article identifies antecedent characteristics of individuals who found the women's movement
important and then shows how finding it important was associated with personality change. Eighty-
six women provided personality and life data as college seniors in 1958 or 1960, prior to the onset
of the women's movement, and in 1981, after the movement gained momentum. A combination of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

openness, ambition, and dissatisfaction, as assessed by the California Psychological Inventory (CPI;
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

H. Gough, 1957/1996) in college, and subsequent life path from ages 28 to 43 significantly predicted
importance attributed to the women's movement (IWM). On CPI scales, IWM was associated
with significant increases on scales including Dominance, Self-Acceptance, Empathy, Psychological
Mindedness, and Achievement via Independence. Correlates of IWM with self-reported feelings at
ages 33 and 43 and observer-based personality ratings at age 43 supplemented analyses of personality
change. Findings support the utility of examining the impact of social change on personality.

The women's movement that emerged in the mid-1960s sig- effects on behavior and identity formation that vary systemati-
nificantly shaped the socioeconomic, political, and psychologi- cally with life stage, \bung adults in the process of forming an
cal terrain of the United States (Buechler, 1990; Chafe, 1991; identity, for example, are more receptive to change than more
Evans, 1979; Fitzgerald, 1973). Although such social move- established adults, whose commitments to young children or
ments span a number of years and create climates with wide- building a career reduce the degree to which they are influenced
spread effects, some individuals are more affected by cultural (Stewart & Healy, 1989). Stewart and Healy's model has found
change than others. Which individuals are most affected, and empirical support (Duncan & Agronick, 1995; Stewart & Gold-
what is the nature of the influence? Steinberg, 1990).
Age, or more precisely, cohort membership, is one factor In this article, we investigated influences within a cohort
that affects which individuals are most influenced. By common that affected responsiveness to a specific social movement, the
definition, a cohort is a group of individuals of about the same women's movement. There are many factors that might lead
biological age who experience the same social events within the members of the same cohort to "experience and derive personal
same social context (Mannheim, 1928/1952; Rosow, 1978). meanings from the same social experiences differently" (Stew-
The same event often influences different cohorts differently. art, 1994, p. 248). We are particularly interested in two such
Elder (1979, 1981) found, for example, that the Great Depres- factors: individual differences in personality and role configura-
sion affected children and adolescents in divergent ways. tion, or lifestyle.
Building in part on Elder's (1979, 1981) work, Mannheim's Personality characteristics such as openness or flexibility
(1928/1952) writings on cohorts, and Erikson's (1968, 1975) would be expected to make an individual more receptive to
theory of development, Stewart and Healy (1989) proposed a new ideological possibilities engendered by social movements
conceptual framework to study the links between social change (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Role configuration might be expected
and individual lives. They posited that the same social event has to contribute to receptivity if the values of the movement sup-
ported the lifestyle or to make a person less receptive if the
values seemed threatening or irrelevant (Stewart & Healy,
Gail S. Agronick, Institute of Personality and Social Research, Univer- 1989). Jennings and Niemi (1981) found that women with
sity of California, Berkeley; Lauren E. Duncan, Department of Humani- young children were the least receptive to cultural changes of
ties and Social Sciences, Harvey Mudd College. the 1960s and 1970s.
This research was supported by Grant MH-43948 from the National In addition to the question of which individuals were most
Institute of Mental Health. Portions of this article were presented at the and least receptive to social change, we were interested in the
103rd Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, nature of a movement's influence on personality over time. We
New York, New York, August 1995. assumed that personality change associated with social move-
We thank Ravenna Helson for the use of her data set and invaluable
ments would be greater for those persons who consider a move-
advice; Abigail Stewart, Jill Morawski, Christina Maslach, and Kenneth
ment personally important and that the general nature of this
Craik for comments on the article; and Brent Roberts, Eva Klohnen, and
Oliver John for statistical consultation. influence is to move the individual in the direction of the move-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gail ment's values. In making this assumption, we kept in mind that
S. Agronick, Institute of Personality and Social Research, University of such change would be shaped by the personality characteristics
California, Berkeley, California 94720-5050. Electronic mail may be and life experiences that an individual brings to movement
sent to agronick@uclink4.berkeley.edu. involvement (Magnusson, 1990; Scarr & McCartney, 1983).
1545
1546 AGRONICK AND DUNCAN

Does personality change beyond young adulthood? Major di- and participation in the movement varied. Some women were
mensions of adult personality show considerable rank-order con- active as early as the mid-1960s; others became involved later.
sistency over time (McCrae & Costa, 1990). However, this con- Still others did not participate in organized activities or protests
sistency is not absolute nor does it imply the absence of mean- per se, but supported movement ideology, acknowledging the
level change. Although the measurement of change presents role it played in their life choices and sense of self. Then there
many problems (Cronbach & Furby, 1970), much evidence indi- were women who recognized the movement's influence on them
cates that personality does change through the adult years (e.g., and their life course but did not embrace the label feminist. In
Haan, Millsap, & Hartka, 1986; Helson & Stewart, 1994; How- assessing the personal importance of the women's movement,
ard & Bray, 1988) and that social roles and cultural shifts are we were interested in this full range of responses.
among the factors implicated (Helson & Picano, 1990; How-
ard & Bray, 1988; Roberts, 1997; Roberts & Helson, 1997;
Stewart & Vandewater, 1993). Characteristics of Supporters of the
Women's Movement
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

We explored the interplay among lifestyle, personality, and


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

social change within one cohort of women, members of the A number of articles, primarily cross-sectional studies pub-
Mills Longitudinal Study (Helson, 1967; Helson, Mitchell, & lished in the 1970s, delineated characteristics of supporters or
Moane, 1984; Helson & Wink, 1992). This sample was particu- advocates of the women's movement. Advocates in these studies
larly useful for our purposes because of its place in social his- were usually members of women's movement groups affiliated
tory. Graduating from college in 1958 or 1960, participants with universities, but most studies left unclear the nature of
entered the adult world prior to the profound societal changes these organizations (e.g., whether participants were engaged
of the 1960s and 1970s. Plans for the future were initiated within primarily in political activism, consciousness raising, or both).
a social context that shifted radically within a very short period Questionnaires were usually used to assess attitudes toward the
of time. Some women adhered to traditional values, whereas women's movement in undergraduate student samples, though
others saw the opportunity to develop in new ways. In our earlier Cherniss (1972) conducted unstructured interviews with gradu-
work, we found a wide range of responses to the women's ate students, working women, and full-time mothers, and Usher
movement in this cohort—one third of the sample found the and Fels (1985) sampled married women between the ages of
movement not at all personally important, another third found 40 and 50. This body of research informed our hypotheses about
it somewhat personally important, and the remaining third found characteristics to be expected in women who found the women's
the women's movement very personally important (Duncan & movement personally important.
Agronick, 1995). Support for the women's movement has been associated with
Women who graduated from college just a few years after high levels of ambition, autonomy, and assertiveness (Cherniss,
the Mills sample launched their adult lives within a world with 1972; Goldschmidt, Gergen, Quigley, & Gergen, 1974). Re-
dramatically different views about women's roles and were more ported to have high self-esteem, middle-aged women involved
likely to find the movement personally important. Within one in the movement were found by Usher and Fels (1985) to be
such cohort of women, less than one tenth of the sample found comfortable with themselves but unhappy with socioeconomic
very little personal importance in the movement, whereas over and personal opportunities open to them. Many women's move-
two thirds of respondents found it very personally important ment participants believed that if discrimination were elimi-
(Duncan & Agronick, 1995). With the wider range of reactions nated, women would be able to achieve their full potential (Sto-
represented in the Mills sample, we were able to examine indi- loff, 1973).
vidual differences in the receptivity to and the impact of this Eager to succeed professionally, supporters may have been
cultural change on individual lives. actively searching for a life path different from that traditionally
Tested initially during their senior year of college, the Mills pursued by their female contemporaries (Goldschmidt et al.,
women were contacted several times subsequently. Participants 1974). In fact, involvement with the women's movement was
were asked about the personal importance of the women's move- reported to follow a period of questioning of the conventional
ment in 1981, when they were, on average, 43 years old. Person- modes of female development (Cherniss, 1972; Usher & Fels,
ality and life path data are consequently available both prior to 1985). Not surprisingly, supporters of the movement favored
and well after the movement gained momentum in mainstream changes in traditional sex roles (Mahoney, 1975).
American society. Behavior advocated by the women's movement presented a
Two main issues were addressed in this research. First, do risk to which some may have had trouble accommodating:
college-age personality and early adult life path distinguish Women opposed to the movement, feminism, or feminists have
women who found the movement personally important from been found to score higher on measures of authoritarianism,
those who did not? Second, is there psychological change associ- anxiety, and harm avoidance than those who supported it (Dun-
ated with having found the women's movement personally can, Peterson, & Winter, 1997; Haddock & Zanna, 1994; Sarup,
important? 1976; Worell & Worell, 1977). Consistent with this picture,
In using the term personally important, we acknowledged, women's movement advocates were found to value being broad-
and tried to capture, the complexity of assessing the influence minded and imaginative, to have higher levels of ego develop-
of social change. The women's movement, like other social ment, and to be more self-actualizing than women who did not
movements, may be personally important in a variety of ways. support the movement (Mahoney, 1975; Rozsnafszky & Hendel,
Some women were never receptive to the women's movement, 1977).
but for those who were receptive, expression of feelings about These findings are somewhat hard to reduce, but some are
IMPORTANCE ATTRIBUTED TO THE WOMEN'S MOVEMENT 1547

more applicable to our study than others. We selected openness in the women's movement, perhaps because they were unlikely
to change (lack of rigidity), dissatisfaction, and ambition as a to find their life commitments validated. In addition, those in-
core of characteristics that would have been likely antecedents volved in raising young children may have found that the issues
to interest in the women's movement and to finding it personally raised by the women's movement (e.g., sexual freedom and
important. We did not make hypotheses about some traits men- equal pay) were removed from their day-to-day concerns. In
tioned in the literature review because we lacked measures of contrast, we expected that those with a focus on maintaining a
them at age 21 (e.g., ego development). Though measures of career between ages 28 and 43 would be receptive, in part
dominance and autonomy were available, previous research in because movement activists advocated increased employment
the Mills sample showed that individual differences in these opportunities for women. Women working in upwardly mobile
characteristics (in relation to nontraditional life paths) were careers (largely dominated by men) would have found support
minor in college and only became important after our first as- and validation for their career choices and work experiences in
sessment between young adulthood and midlife (Helson & Pi- the women's movement. Finally, we expected that women whose
cano, 1990). focus shifted from family to building an upwardly mobile career
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

in the 1970s, when the influence of the women's movement was


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

quite strong, would be the group who found the movement most
Life Path and Receptivity to Social Change personally important of all. Whether finding personal impor-
Although personality characteristics may contribute to indi- tance in the movement induced a shift in life priorities or
vidual differences in finding the women's movement personally whether a change in life focus prompted finding the movement
important, divergent life priorities and commitments within the important, women with newly established priorities in the paid
sample may have affected receptivity as well. People whose labor force would have found much meaning in a movement that
interests and lifestyle are reflected in the ideology of a social fostered questioning of traditional gender norms and advocated
movement are more inclined to advocate or take part in the women's increased presence outside the home.
movement than those who do not see their ideas or life commit-
ments supported (Jennings & Niemi, 1981; Stewart & Healy,
Effects of Participation in the Women's
1989). In addition, individuals considering a shift in commit-
Movement on Personality
ments, such as a homemaker reentering the workforce, may be
particularly receptive to social change: Although there has been little longitudinal study of the effects
of engagement with the women's movement on personality,
For example, women [slightly older than the Mills participants] cross-sectional studies using personality measures and retro-
who raised children at home during the 1950s and early 1960s were spective reports suggest that those who found the women's
sometimes moved by the women's movement of the later 1960s
movement meaningful changed psychologically through activi-
and 1970s to reconsider the structure of their lives—to go back to
school, abandon a marriage, or find a career. These women some-
ties such as consciousness-raising groups. The movement em-
times reconstructed their worldviews, and their personal identities, powered its advocates and provided opportunities for personal
in radical ways. (Stewart & Healy, 1989, p. 33) growth and self-actualization (Cherniss, 1972; Mahoney, 1975;
Rozsnafszky & Hendel, 1977). Personal insights and reforma-
Previous researchers classified the Mills participants into life- tion of self were attributed to critical evaluation of traditional
path groups on the basis of cohort-specific patterns in the timing gender role expectations, attitudes, and behavior (Cherniss,
and focus of commitments to family or career (Helson, Mitch- 1972; Stoloff, 1973). Activists reported feeling an increased
ell, & Moane, 1984; Helson & Moane, 1987). Two main life solidarity with, and a greater understanding of, other women;
paths were labeled the feminine social clock (FSC) and the they also reported increased self-esteem and reduced self-blame
masculine occupational clock (MOC). The FSC adherent's pri- as a result of movement activity (Cherniss, 1972). In addition,
mary focus was on the family. The MOC adherent's focus was supporters may have changed through taking advantage of the
on maintaining an upwardly mobile career (though most women increased opportunities to develop high-status careers outside
had families as well). Within these two main life paths, distinc- the home during the 1960s and 1970s. Such work is known to
tions were made on the basis of time of initiation (early or late) be associated with increased gender consciousness (Gurin,
and maintenance of adherence to a respective life path. Among 1985) and successful functioning (Helson, Elliott, & Leigh,
patterns that these distinctions identify are those of women who 1990; Kohn & Schooler, 1978; Roberts, 1997), including skills
maintained a primary focus on family from ages 28 to 43, those necessary to achieve in work environments requiring initiative
who maintained a primary commitment to building an upwardly and autonomy.
mobile career from ages 28 to 43, and those who began with a Why did involvement in the movement spur personality
primary focus on family but shifted after age 28 to an increased change? One potent factor may be the group processes inherent
emphasis on work, establishing an upwardly mobile career tra- in much movement activity (Cole & Stewart, 1996). The goal
jectory and orientation by age 43. of consciousness-raising groups, for example, was to shift the
These life-path variables enabled us to test the receptivity to cognitive focus from passive acceptance, self-blame, and feel-
the women's movement of adherents to the homemaker and ings of isolation from other women to self-development, social
career patterns, as well as of those who shifted from a predomi- support, and group action (Brodsky, 1973; Downing & Roush,
nantly homemaker lifestyle to a careerist trajectory during the 1985; Peskilis, 1970). Interpersonal trust was developed on the
1970s. We expected that those maintaining a commitment pri- basis of the view that a woman's struggles in society were not
marily to family would be least likely to find personal meaning primarily the result of her individual intrapsychic difficulties but
1548 AGRONICK AND DUNCAN

rather a result of the challenges she faced in a limiting social The two times of testing included in this study are labeled ages 21
structure. Sharing experiences with others and reading feminist and 43, respectively. The sample consisted of 86 women who completed
writings empowered women to develop a more differentiated personality inventories at both ages 21 and 43 and extensive question-
view of the factors that motivated others to maintain the status naire material regarding their work, relationships, attitudes, and feelings
quo, to persevere and show initiative in the face of discrimina- at age 43.
tion, and to resist social norms and expectations. In conjunction Analyses have shown that attrition effects tend to be small in the
Mills sample. Women who completed the CPI at both ages 21 and 43
with this perspective, women were encouraged to develop a
differed at age 21 from those who would drop out of the study after
sense of self beyond the social roles of mother and wife (Brod-
this first assessment on only 1 of the 20 scales: They had higher scores
sky, 1973). We expected, then, that women who found much on the Communality scale, f(137) = 2.04, p < .05. Dropouts thus felt
personal meaning in the women's movement would have in- more marginal or alienated than women who stayed in the study. These
creased in social poise and assurance, resistance to social norms, two groups did not differ at age 21 in work or family plans for the
and complexity of outlook, which includes a greater understand- future, though the women who dropped out had lower SAT scores and
ing of other women and gender roles. lower grade point averages.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

We also compared women who, at age 43, completed both the CPI
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and the section of materials that included the women's movement ques-
Overview tion with those who completed the CPI but did not answer this specific
part of the questionnaire (which came late in the battery and was some-
This study investigated two questions. The first asked what
what optional). The 86 women who provided responses to the women's
antecedent personality characteristics and life-path factors were movement question differed on only 1 of the 20 CPI scales from those
associated with finding the women's movement personally im- who did not: They scored higher on the Socialization scale, ((105) =
portant at midlife. On the basis of the literature on supporters 2.77, p < .01. Presumably, women who completed all of the materials,
of the movement, Mills participants who were ambitious and were more conscientious than those who did not complete the entire
capable, open to new ideas, and dissatisfied with the opportuni- assessment packet. No differences were found in marital status, number
ties available to them upon college graduation were expected of children, total amount of time spent in the labor force, or status level
to have been most affected by the women's movement. The life of paid employment among women who did and did not answer the
path a woman followed is also expected to have influenced the section that included the question about the women's movement.
degree to which she found personal meaning in the women's
movement—women who focused on family should show the
least influence, those who maintained a career path between
Measures
ages 28 and 43 should show more influence, and those who Importance attributed to the women's movement (IWM). As part
switched from commitments primarily in the home to a career in of the age-43 questionnaire, the women were asked to respond to the
the workforce during the 1970s should show the most influence. following question: "Have you taken part in the women's movement?
The second question addressed whether finding the women's If so, in what ways? If not, are there ways in which you think that you
movement personally important was associated with personality have been affected by it?" Responses were coded on a 3-point scale.
change. We expected finding the women's movement personally A reply was scored as a 1 (no personal importance) if there was evi-
dence that the women's movement had no impact on the person's life.
important to be associated with personality change in the direc-
For example, one woman wrote, " [ I ] have not taken part in it. I think
tion of increased confidence, initiative, self-esteem, and com-
that all the ballyhoo is silly." A response was coded as 2 (some personal
plex understanding of others. We also expected it to be associ- importance) if there was evidence that the women's movement had
ated with an increased questioning of social norms, and for some, but not extensive, impact on the person's life. If the woman
those in upwardly mobile careers, increased high-level achieve- considered the movement significant, but was not actively involved, she
ment skills, such as the desire and ability to work independently. received a score of 2. For example, a participant replied, " I have not
To complement our change analyses, we studied feelings taken part, but the results [of the women's movement] have given me
about life experienced by participants in their early 30s and 40s, more self confidence." To receive a score of 3 (much personal impor-
tance), a response had to state that the woman was quite affected by
especially feelings regarding gender roles. We also presented
the movement or to mention political activity related to women's rights.
observer-based personality descriptions of the women as they
For example, one individual wrote, " I have been active in establishing
were perceived at midlife, expecting individuals who found the a Women's History Week. . . . My relationships with women have
women's movement personally important to have been viewed changed—we have a support group. I have also changed my ideas on
by others as open, ambitious, and dissatisfied as they were at rearing children as a result of the women's movement. I want my daugh-
age 21 and as confident, connected, and engaged by the ideas ter to have the choices I felt I didn't!"
and the possibilities of the movement as they became over time. As reported by Duncan and Agronick (1995), 30 women (34.9%)
received a score of 1; 28 women (32.5%) received a score of 2; and 28
(32.5%) received a score of 3. The mean rating for the sample was
Method
1.97. Interrater reliability for the two coders was .93.
Participants and Overall Design of Study The California Psychological Inventory (CPI). The CPI (Gough,
1957/1996) is a widely used inventory that includes 20 scales with
In 1958, and again in 1960, a representative two thirds of the senior themes of social poise and assurance, norm adherence, and achievement
class (N = 141) at Mills College participated in a study of personality potential. Among these are several that were particularly suitable to test
characteristics and plans for the future among college women (Helson, our hypotheses. First, we expected that women who found the women's
1967). They were contacted again, primarily by mail, at approximately movement important would have been open to new ways of thinking,
ages 27, 43, and 52 (Helson et al., 1984; Helson & Wink, 1992). The alert to new opportunities for success, and dissatisfied at age 21. The
women were predominantly White, Protestant, and middle-class. CPI Flexibility scale is an appropriate measure of desire for change,
IMPORTANCE ATTRIBUTED TO THE WOMEN'S MOVEMENT 1549

variety, and new ways of thinking (which should include an openness adult development, including those of Gould (1972), Levinson (1978),
to alternative roles and modes of behavior). The Capacity for Status and Neugarten (1968), and some were intended to reflect attitudes asso-
scale assesses ambition, breadth of outlook, and skills leading to increase ciated with the women's movement.
in status. The Well-Being scale, which assesses an individual's current Previous work (Helson & Moane, 1987) has shown that the Mills
life satisfaction and future outlook, as well as her sense of emotional sample as a whole reported feeling more confident and effective (e.g.,
and physical health, was used to test dissatisfaction. Those who score "feeling more confident," "influence in my community or area of inter-
moderately low on this scale are described as " . . . not entirely comfort- est") and less vulnerable (e.g., "feeling weaker and less competent than
able with themselves or their social supports" (McAllister, 1986, p. 14). others"; "feeling I will never get myself together") in their early 40s
For the second question in our research, we constructed expectations than 10 years previously. Though this perceived change in social and
about how IWM was related to personality change from ages 21 to 43. personal empowerment was relevant to the present study, the 3-point
It is best to use caution when hypothesizing that particular CPI scales scale imposed a ceiling effect, which makes it difficult to discriminate
will show change and others will not—much depends on the initial the influence of the women's movement from that due to broader pro-
scores of the women and their intervening experiences and environments, cesses of normative development.
both those common to most of the sample and those affecting subgroups. Some items in the set of feelings about life concerned gender roles
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

specifically, and more variation in response to these items was repre-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

In general, we expected increases on measures of social poise and assur-


ance, decreases in some aspects of norm adherence, and increases on sented within the sample. Here, our general expectations were that IWM
measures of complexity of understanding others and high-level achieve- would be related to negative attitudes toward traditional gender roles
ment skills. Among measures of social poise and assurance, the most ("feeling angry at men and masculinity"), increases between the early
obvious choices were Dominance, which assesses initiative, confidence, 30s and early 40s in feeling a connection with women ("feeling women
assertiveness, and sense of direction; Self-Acceptance, which measures are more important to me than they used to b e " ) , and experiencing an
expanded consciousness (e.g., "discovering new parts of myself").
self-approbation; and Empathy, which is not a measure of sympathetic
attitudes, but of sophistication and active involvement in understanding CAQ personality descriptions based on archival data. The Califor-
others. nia Q-sort (CAQ; Block, 1978) consists of 100 items that provide a
Among measures of norm adherence, some scales assess deeply in- comprehensive description of personality functioning. The items are
grained characteristics, whereas others measure more fluid orientations sorted by raters, using a forced normal distribution, to describe an
individual. In the Mills study, judges based their ratings on extensive
that are more likely to change. A decrease in Good Impression (concern
questionnaire data collected at age 43. Reliability of the Q-sorts was
with presenting oneself favorably) from 1958/1960 to 1981 would re-
satisfactory (Wink, 1991). The raters saw no inventory scores, but their
flect a resistance to norms about women's self-presentation as agreeable
material did include responses to the question about the importance of
and accommodating. To assess complexity and objectivity in understand-
the women's movement. Because there were about 35 pages of informa-
ing others, we chose Psychological Mindedness, and to appraise high-
tion on the participants' lives since college, including responses about
achievement skills, such as the ability and desire to work independently,
partners, divorces, children, parents, work, and health, the influence of
we chose Achievement via Independence.
a paragraph or two about the women's movement on the raters' place-
Life path variables. Previous work by Helson and her collaborators ment of Q-sort items was quite minor.
described a set of life-path trajectories (social clock projects; see Helson
We expected that the raters' descriptions of the women in their early
et al., 1984; Helson & Moane, 1987) in the Mills sample. Two main
40s would be consistent with a picture of women who were open,
projects were labeled the Feminine Social Clock (FSC) and the mascu-
ambitious, and dissatisfied as college seniors, subsequently stimulated
line Occupational Clock (MOC). Adherents to the FSC had partners
by the ideas and the possibilities of the women's movement, and able
and children by age 28, intact marriages at age 43, and if these partici-
to use its influence to raise their consciousness and gain a sense of
pants worked, low-status or temporary jobs rather than upwardly mobile
empowerment.
careers. Followers of the MOC were women who had an upwardly
mobile career at both ages 28 and 43 (though most had families as
well). Another pattern consisted of FSC-MOC shifters, participants
Analyses
who started out in traditional roles (adhering to the FSC) but initiated
a career after age 28 and were maintaining an MOC project at age 43. To study personality and lifestyle predictors of importance attributed
Of 86 women, 22 were coded as FSCs (primary focus on family), 18 to the women's movement (IWM), ratings of IWM were first correlated
as MOCs (central commitment to career), and 8 as FSC-MOC shifters. with CPI Capacity for Status, Weil-Being, and Flexibility scale scores
The other 38 participants started a family later than the sample norm, at age 21 and with each of the social clock project patterns. A hierarchical
initiated a family or career project but did not maintain it, or never multiple regression analysis was then performed to show the combined
established either of these projects. Clear predictions about IWM could effects of antecedent personality and life path on reported importance
not be made for these remaining participants. For example, women who of the women's movement at age 43. Capacity for Status, Well-Being,
began families late did so for divergent reasons (such as waiting for and Flexibility scores were entered together in Step 1, and three dummy
their partner to finish professional school, having problems with fertility, variables used to code for life path were entered together in Step 2 of
or delaying children in the interest of their own career), which had this analysis. These dummy variables were used to code for the three
different implications for their vested interest in the women's movement. life paths for which we had hypotheses in comparison with a fourth
Status level in work. To assess career success, we used ratings of group of trajectories for which clear predictions could not be made. The
status level in work (Helson, Elliott, & Leigh, 1989). Ratings of the three variables were computed as follows: traditional lifestyle (FSC =
amount of autonomy, responsibility, training, and talent required in the 1; otherwise = 0 ) ; career commitment between ages 28 and 43 (MOC
woman's work were made on a 7-point scale. Interrater reliability for = 1; otherwise = 0 ) ; and shift from traditional lifestyle to career com-
the two coders was .96. mitment (FSC at age 28 to MOC by age 43 = 1; otherwise = 0 ) .
Feelings about life. At age 43, the women rated each of 40 character- To study personality change associated with IWM we again used
istic "feelings about life" on a 3-point scale (3 = very descriptive, 1 hierarchical multiple regression to determine whether reported impor-
= not at all descriptive). In addition, they rated these same feelings tance of the women's movement was associated with personality change
retrospectively, as they had experienced them about 10 years previously from ages 21 to 43. Scores at age 43 on the six CPI scales considered
(Helson & Moane, 1987). The items were selected from theories of most likely to show change were the criterion variables. To assess person-
1550 AGRONICK AND DUNCAN

ality change associated with IWM controlling for the influence of age- Table 2
21 personality, we entered scores on the respective CPI scales at age 21 Change in Personality From Ages 21 to 43 Associated With
in Step 1; we then entered IWM in Step 2. In secondary analyses, we
Importance Attributed to the Women's Movement
controlled for both age-21 personality and status level of work before
entering IWM in a third step. 0*
IWM was correlated with two sets of the women's self-report of
feelings about life; the first set was descriptive of themselves in their Age-21 Women's R2
early 40s, and the second recalled at that time as having been descriptive Age-43 CPI scale CPI scale movement R change
10 years earlier. Finally, to show distinctive features of personality asso-
ciated with IWM as the women were viewed by others, IWM was Social poise and assurance
Dominance .52 .27 .61 .07**
correlated with the items of the Q-sort descriptions made by file raters.
Self-Acceptance .26 .34 .45 .11**
Because hypotheses related to feelings about life and Q-sort descriptions
Empathy .43 .38 .61 .14**
were stated somewhat abstractly, we used the .01 level of significance Norm adherence
in reporting results. Good Impression .45 -.14 .48 .02
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Other scales
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Achievement via
Results Independence .34 .25 .43 .06*
Psychological Mindedness .35 .25 .42 .06*
Who Found Personal Meaning in the Women's
Movement? Note. R2 change indicates the additional percent of variance explained
by entering importance of the women's movement in Step 2 of the
IWM was significantly correlated with CPI Flexibility at age analyses. N = 84. CPI = California Psychological Inventory.
21 (r = .22,p < .05, N = 85) and showed correlations signifi- • Beta weights from the final equations are reported.
cant at the trend level (p < .10) with Capacity for Status and *p < .05. **p < .01.
Weil-Being (rs = .21 and - . 1 8 , respectively). In addition, IWM
showed correlations of -.24 (p < .05, N = 86) with having
initiated and maintained a traditional lifestyle and of .25 (p < The addition of the social clock project variables, introduced
.05) with having shifted from a traditional lifestyle to a career at Step 2, significantly increased the multiple regression coeffi-
commitment. Correlations with other social clock project pat- cient from .38 to .47. These results indicate that antecedent
terns, including a continuous career commitment between ages personality and subsequent life path each significantly predicted
28 and 43 (r = .15) were not significant. who reported the women's movement to be personally important
Table 1 reports the regression analysis used to predict IWM and that the combination of these two variables was a stronger
from age-21 personality and the life-path variables. Scores from predictor of reported importance of the movement than either
the Capacity for Status, Well-Being, and Flexibility scales, en- variable alone.
tered together in Step 1, produced a significant multiple regres-
sion coefficient of .38. These results provide support for the Was Finding the Women's Movement Important Related
hypothesis that women who found the movement personally to Personality Change?
important would have been open to new ideas, ambitious, and
dissatisfied at age 21. The combination of these three character- Change on the CPI from ages 21 to 43. Table 2 reports the
istics predicted reported importance of the women's movement results from hierarchical regression analyses to predict changes
much more strongly than any one characteristic considered in personality associated with reported importance of the wom-
separately. en's movement. The six scales on which change was predicted
are shown in Table 2. Age-21 personality, entered in Step 1 of
each analysis, was a significant predictor of age-43 personality
Table 1 on all scales, reflecting the stability of personality between these
Antecedent Personality and Life Path as Predictors of two age periods.
Importance Attributed to the Women's Movement IWM was entered in Step 2 of each analysis. The R2 change
column in Table 2 shows the percent change in the total amount
Variable r ? R R2 change of variance explained by the addition of IWM. Significant
change in personality attributable to IWM (after controlling for
Step 1 .38 .14**
the influence of age-21 personality) was shown by increases on
Flexibility .22* .15
Well-Being -.24 Dominance, Self-Acceptance, and Empathy, the scales we had
-,18t
Capacity for Status •21t .18 selected from the social poise and assurance category. Psycho-
Step 2 .47 .08* logical Mindedness and Achievement via Independence also
Traditional lifestyle (FSC) -.24* -.12 showed significant increases after IWM was entered in Step 2.
Career commitment (MOC) .15 .12
Shift to career from traditional
However, the hypothesis that decrease on the Good Impression
lifestyle .25* .22 scale would show a significant association with IWM was not
supported.
Note. Scale scores are from the California Psychological Inventory The relative contribution of antecedent personality and IWM
(CPI, Gough, 1957/1996) obtained from the sample at age 21. N = 85.
FSC = feminine social clock; MOC = masculine occupational clock. to age-43 personality for each of the CPI scales is reflected in
a
Beta weights in final equation. their respective beta weights, as reported in the final regression
t p < .10 (marginally significant). * p < .05. ** p < .01. equations. Antecedent personality and reported importance of
IMPORTANCE ATTRIBUTED TO THE WOMEN'S MOVEMENT 1551

the women's movement each significantly contributed to age- used the movement's influence to raise their consciousness (e.g.,
43 personality on five of the six scales.1 "has insight into own motives and behavior"). They had a
We speculated that the relation between IWM and decrease sense of empowerment (e.g., "values own independence and
in norm adherence was not significant because the demands of autonomy") at midlife. Finally, raters saw those who found
challenging careers were leading many women to increase rather much personal importance in the movement as expressive, non-
than decrease on such measures (Roberts, 1997). (Status level conformists (e.g., negative correlation with "is emotionally
of work shows a correlation of .30, p < .01, with IWM in the bland, has flattened affect"; positive correlation with "tends to
Mills sample.) Additional regression analyses in which both be rebellious and nonconforming'').
age-21 personality and status level in work were controlled
produced results that supported this hypothesis: With the influ- Discussion
ence of status level of paid employment removed, quite similar
results were obtained for Dominance (R2 change = .04, p < We asked two questions in this research. First, who found
.05), Self-Acceptance (R2 change = . 10, p < .01), and Empathy the women's movement personally important? All Mills study
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(R2 change = .11, p < .001), but there was now a significant participants, regardless of subsequent life path, entered the adult
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

association with amount of change on Good Impression (R2 world a few years before the movement began. Yet all members
change = .04, p < .05). However, the relations between IWM of the study were not influenced by it to the same degree.
and increases on Achievement via Independence no longer Personality characteristics at age 21, a combination of ambition,
reached significance, and Psychological Mindedness was only dissatisfaction, and openness, significantly predicted reported
significant at the trend level {R2 change = .03, p < .10). Thus, IWM at age 43.
the pattern of personality change that was associated with IWM The life path a woman followed also affected the degree to
depended, in part, on the work experiences of subgroups within which she reported the women's movement to have been im-
the sample. portant. We found that women who were primarily homemakers
Feelings about life in one's early 30s and early 40s. Correla- between ages 28 and 43 regarded the women's movement as
tions between IWM and feelings about life are presented in least important and that those who initiated a career after age
Table 3. As hypothesized, women for whom the movement was 28, during the peak years of the movement, regarded it as most
most important recalled feeling angry toward men and masculin- important. The combination of antecedent personality and life
ity in their early 30s. In their early 40s, they reported feeling path was a much stronger predictor of reported movement im-
powerful, connected to other women, still dissatisfied with men portance than either of these two factors alone.
and masculinity, and engaged in processes of self-discovery and Our second question examined how finding the movement
expanded horizons. personally important was related to personality change between
Q-sort descriptions of the women at age 43. Correlations ages 21 and 43. We assessed consistency and change using a
between IWM and CAQ items are presented in Table 4. As standardized inventory, first prior to the onset of the women's
expected, descriptions by raters were consistent with a picture of movement and again well after the movement gained momentum
women who were open (e.g., negative correlation with "favors in mainstream American society. Antecedent scores on CPI
conservative values in a variety of areas"), ambitious ("has scales predicted personality at mid-life, reflecting the expected
high aspiration level for s e l f ' ) , and dissatisfied (negative corre- rank-order consistency of these characteristics. There was also
lation with "is subjectively unaware of self-concern; feels satis- the expected evidence of personality change. Consistent with
fied with self"), all characteristics associated with IWM at age cross-sectional analyses of retrospective data reported in the
21. High scorers on IWM showed evidence of an ability to have literature, finding the movement personally important was asso-
ciated with increases in social poise and assurance, including
increased confidence, initiative, and self-esteem, as well as with
increases sophistication and active involvement in understanding
Table 3
of others. These results indicate that importance of the women's
Correlations of Importance Attributed to the Women's
movement was associated with increased empowerment from
Movement With Feelings in Participants' Early 30s
college graduation to midlife.
(Retrospective) and Early 40s (Concurrent)
IWM was also associated with increases in the desire and
Women's ability to work independently and in the ability to understand
Feelings about life movement self and others from a complex, objective vantage point—skills
particularly useful in high-status careers. The expected relation
Retrospective
Feeling angry at men and masculinity .48
Concurrent: Empowerment and gender issues
Feeling powerful .41 ' Besides the scales shown in Table 2, IWM was associated with
Feeling women more important to me than they significant change on all other measures of social poise and assurance:
used to be .40 Capacity for Status (R 2 change = .04, p < .05), Sociability (R2 change
Feeling angry at men and masculinity .33 = .05, p < .05), Social Presence (R2 change = .09, p < .01), and
Concurrent: Self-discovery and change
Independence (R2 change = .05, p < .05), but not with change on any
Discovering new parts of myself .32
Anxiety that I won't live up to opportunities .31 other CPI scale (six measuring aspects of norm adherence and four
Interest in things beyond my own family .29 measuring aspects of achievement potential). These results indicate that
IWM was associated with a broad-band increase in what can be called
Note. All correlations significant at p < .01 are shown. iV = 83. social empowerment.
1552 AGRONICK AND DUNCAN

Table 4
Correlations of Importance Attributed to the Women's Movement
with Q-Sort Items at Age 43

Women's
Q-Sort item movement

Openness
Favors conservative values in a variety of areas -.48
Handles anxiety and conflicts by, in effect, refusing to recognize their presence;
repressive or dissociative tendencies —.47
Is uncomfortable with uncertainty and complexities -.35
Is concerned with philosophical problems; e.g., religions, values, the meaning
of life, etc. .31
Is moralistic -.30
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Ambition
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Has high aspiration level for self .38


Dissatisfaction
Is subjectively unaware of self-concern; feels satisfied with self -.30
Raised consciousness
Has insight into own motives and behavior .52
Able to see to the heart of important problems .41
Is introspective and concerned with self as an object .40
Evaluates the motivation of others in interpreting situations .30
Empowerment
Genuinely submissive; accepts domination comfortably -.35
Values own independence and autonomy .34
Gives up and withdraws where possible in the face of frustration and adversity -.31
Expressiveness, nonconformity
Is an interesting, arresting person .42
Is emotionally bland; has flattened affect -.42
Tends to be rebellious and nonconforming .37
Judges self and others in conventional terms like popularity, "the correct thing
to do," social pressures, etc. -.37
Thinks and associates to ideas in unusual ways; has unconventional thought
processes .33
Is verbally fluent; can express ideas well .29

Note. All correlations significant at p < .01 are shown. N = 85

between importance of the women's movement and increased seniors and able to use the influence of the women's movement
resistance to social norms over time was not supported. How- to raise their consciousness and gain a sense of empowerment
ever, in secondary analyses which controlled for the influence by midlife.
of status level of work on personality change, importance of the One would not necessarily expect the strength of the results
women's movement was significantly associated with a de- that we have reported. One potent contributor to our findings
creased desire to present oneself favorably, and skills particu- may be the interpersonal processes central to much movement
larly associated with career success were no longer significant. activity (Cole & Stewart, 1996). Informal discussions and orga-
These analyses suggest that the pattern of personality change nized activities, such as consciousness-raising groups, promoted
associated with finding the movement personally important de- a cognitive shift from passive acceptance of women's traditional
pended, in part, on the environmental presses experienced by roles, self-blame for structural problems, and feelings of isola-
subgroups within the sample. Demands of challenging careers tion, to resistance to sexism, self-development, and connection
were leading many women to increase, rather than decrease, in with other women (Brodsky, 1973; Downing & Roush, 1985;
norm adherence over time (Roberts, 1997). Peskilis, 1970). The personality changes, feelings, and Q-sort
Self-reports of feelings at age 43 (1981) and retrospectively findings all reflect this psychosocial reorientation.
at age 33 (1971) add to the above mentioned personality change The magnitude of our findings may also rest on the fact
findings. Women who found the movement most important de- that the women's movement challenged the worldview of some
scribed themselves as having been frustrated with women's role members of the sample, leaving others unaffected. This cohort,
in society in the early 1970s, a time at which the movement women born in the late 1930s and early 1940s, entered adulthood
was quite strong. In 1981, at age 43, these women reported with clear ideas about the future that were based on the goals
feeling powerful, connected to other women, still dissatisfied and values of the 1950s. ' 'Marriage and family were the corner-
with traditional gender roles, and engaged in a process of self- stones on which nearly every women's life plan was erected"
discovery and change. In addition, observers' descriptions of (Helson, 1993, p. 193). Most women expected to work after
the women in their early 40s were consistent with a picture of high school or college, but usually for a limited time, until they
women who were open, ambitious, and dissatisfied as college married or had children. Yet the lives of three fourths of the
IMPORTANCE ATTRIBUTED TO THE WOMEN'S MOVEMENT 1553

Mills women turned out differently from their college-age ex- ple as a whole did not increase significantly in these two re-
pectations (Helson, 1993). For women who entered the adult spects. The largest increases in measures of self-assurance for
world on the cusp of the values of the 1950s and 1960s, it was the sample as a whole were on Dominance (confidence, initia-
possible to either continue to embrace the ideology of the 1950s, tive, and sense of direction) and Independence (self-reliance).
or to question it. How general are the implications of our findings? It may
For cohorts younger than that of the Mills sample, it became usually be open, dissatisfied, and ambitious people who are
increasingly more normative to question, and possibly eschew, influenced by new ideas and prospects. In addition, advantaged
the 1950s model of women's priorities. Because the Mills sam- individuals, such as our college-educated, primarily White, mid-
ple engaged in the tasks of young adulthood during the transi- dle-class sample, may be more likely to have the socioeconomic
tional years between two radically different views of women's resources necessary to be agents and beneficiaries of social
roles, individual differences in antecedent personality and life change. Some of the studies of the student protest movements
path became potent predictors of IWM. Women who were open of the 1960s reported psychological and demographic findings
to new ideas and alternative lifestyles, dissatisfied with norma- similar to ours (Flacks, 1967; Keniston, 1967). On the other
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

hand, different social movements may attract different support-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tive gender roles, and ambitious to succeed questioned women's


traditional roles and found the women's movement most im- ers. For example, although Black and White women participated
portant; others, who adhered to the FSC, fulfilling the expecta- in both the civil rights and women's movements, significantly
tions of the 1950s, found it least important. more White than Black women participated in the women's
Yet it is also important to recognize some limitations of this movement, and significantly more Black than White women
study. The women's movement was one factor in an admittedly took part in the fight for civil rights (Cole & Stewart, 1996).
complex world. Other social movements, such as the fight for In addition, social movements and cultural climates differ in
civil rights and protests against the Vietnam War; secular trends, the particular aspects of personality they affect. Whereas the
such as the increase in individualism; economic changes; and women's movement empowered its advocates and increased
demographic shifts also shaped the country's sociopolitical their identification with other women, the culture of individual-
landscape. We, of course, cannot account for the whole network ism increased the self-focus (narcissism) of those it influenced
of influences in this research. (Roberts & Helson, 1997). The essential point is that individual
In addition, the longitudinal design of our study does not differences do influence receptivity to social movements, and
allow us to draw causal conclusions from our results. One could social movements do influence personality change.
argue that it is possible that women who were flexible, ambi- Recently, personality and social psychologists have been pay-
tious, and dissatisfied at age 21 would have shown the reported ing increased attention to the influence of broader societal
personality changes by age 43, regardless of exposure to the change on psychological processes. Some have asserted that it is
women's movement. Although the women's movement was not necessary to link social change to individual lives to understand
the sole factor contributing to personality change within the personality development more adequately (Stewart & Healy,
Mills sample, similar patterns of empowerment are not found 1989). In such analyses, attention is paid to the intertwined
in all cohorts. For example, Helson, Stewart, and Ostrove (1995) nature of historical period, cohort membership, and psychoso-
found that integrated searchers, individuals characterized by cial life tasks (Duncan & Agronick, 1995; Elder, 1979, 1981;
high aspirations for self, independence, and flexibility in out- Giele, 1993; Helson et al., 1984; Helson & Picano, 1990; Helson
look, did not show the same degree of realization across three et al., 1995). Yet consideration of such issues runs "counter to
cohorts of women. Integrated searchers who were in their 40s the stability bias implicit in much social psychological research,
in the early 1950s (the oldest cohort) lacked the challenge and where it is frequently assumed that particular effects are applica-
outside validation from careers that became progressively more ble across age groups and historical periods" (Ryff, 1987, p.
accessible to those in the younger two cohorts. 1200).
Whereas evidence supports the general accuracy of the Mills We hope to contribute to a revitalization of a societal psychol-
women's retrospections (see Harker & Solomon, 1996; Hel- ogy that explicitly explores the relationship between sociohistor-
son & Wink, 1992), the ratings of the feelings that they attrib- ical context and psychological processes (Himmelweit, 1990).
uted to themselves in their early 30s may show some retrospec- To our knowledge, ours is the first longitudinal study to describe
tive bias (McFarland, Ross, & Giltrow, 1992; Woodruff & Bir- antecedent personality characteristics and lifestyle associated
ren, 1972). Even if they contain bias, these self-recollections with personal importance attributed to the women's movement
would seem to constitute an important part of the women's life and to explore psychological outcomes of investment in it. We
narratives. Because participants were in their early 30s during hope that these findings underscore the feasibility and value of
the early 1970s, a time when the women's movement was quite including an analysis of the broader social context (political
strong, these retrospective accounts help to assess its emotive climates) in investigations of personality consistency and
importance. change.
It should be noted that the influence of the women's move-
ment was not simply to accentuate in its adherents the increase
in social poise and assurance often associated with adult devel- References
opment from young adulthood to midlife (Haan et al., 1986; Block, J. (1978). The Q-sort method in personality assessment and
Helson & Moane, 1987). The empowerment associated with psychiatric research (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psycholo-
IWM emphasized feeling good about oneself (Self-Acceptance) gists Press.
and actively connecting with others (Empathy). The Mills sam- Brodsky, A. M. (1973). The consciousness-raising group as a model for
1554 AGRONICK AND DUNCAN

therapy with women. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, of creative college women. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 76,
10, 24-29. 205-256.
Buechler, S. M. (1990). Women's movements in the United States: Helson, R. (1993). The Mills classes of 1958 and 1960: College in the
Woman suffrage, equal rights, and beyond. New Brunswick, NJ: Rut- fifties, young adulthood in the sixties. In K. D. Hulbert & D. T. Schus-
gers University Press. ter (Eds.), Women's lives through time: Educated American women
Chafe, W. H. (1991). The paradox of change: American women in the of the twentieth century (pp. 190-210). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
twentieth century. New 'Vbrk: Oxford University Press. Helson, R., Elliott, T, & Leigh, J. (1989). Adolescent personality and
Cherniss, C. (1972). Personality and ideology: A personological study women's work patterns. In D. Stern & D. Eichorn (Eds.), Adolescence
of women's liberation. Psychiatry, 35, 109-125. and work: Influences of social structure, labor markets, and culture
Cole, E. R., & Stewart, A. J. (1996). Meanings of political participation (pp. 259-289). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
among black and white women: Political identity and social responsi- Helson, R., Elliott, T, & Leigh, J. (1990). Number and quality of roles:
bility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 130-140. A longitudinal personality view. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 14,
Costa, P. T, & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO-PI-R. The revised NEO 83-101.
personality inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Helson, R., Mitchell, V., & Moane, G. (1984). Personality and patterns
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Resources. of adherence and nonadherence to the social clock. Journal of Person-


Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure ality and Social Psychology, 46, 1079-1096.
"change": Or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68-80. Helson, R., & Moane, G. (1987). Personality change in women from
Downing, N. E., & Roush, K. L. (1985). From passive acceptance to college to midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
active commitment: A model of feminist identity development for 176-186.
women. Special issue: Cross-cultural counseling. Counseling Psychol- Helson, R., & Picano, J. (1990). Is the traditional role bad for women?
ogist, 13, 695-709. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 311-320.
Duncan, L. E., & Agronick, G. S. (1995). The intersection of life stage Helson, R., & Stewart, A. (1994). Personality change in adulthood.
and social events: Personality and life outcomes. Journal of Personal- In T. F. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality
ity and Social Psychology, 69, 558-568. change? (pp. 201-225). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Duncan, L. E., Peterson, B. E., & Winter, D. G. (1997). Authoritarianism Association.
and gender roles: Toward a psychological analysis of hegemonic rela- Helson, R., Stewart, A., & Ostrove, J. (1995). Identity in three cohorts
tionships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 41-49. of midlife women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
Elder, G. H. (1979). Historical change in life patterns and personality. 544-557.
In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and Helson, R., & Wink, P. (1992). Personality change in women from the
behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 117-159). New York: Academic Press. early 40s to the early 50s. Psychology and Aging, 7, 46-55.
Elder, G. H. (1981). History and the life course. In D. Bertraux (Ed.),
Himmelweit, H. T. (1990). Societal psychology: Implications and scope.
Biography and society (pp. 77-115). New York: Sage.
In H. T. Himmelweit & G. Gaskell (Eds.), Societal psychology (pp.
Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.
17-45). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Erikson, E. (1975). Life history and the historical moment. New %rk:
Howard, A., & Bray, D. (1988). Managerial lives in transition: Advanc-
Norton.
ing age and changing times. New 'fork: Guilford Press.
Evans, S. (1979). Personal politics: The roots of women's liberation in
Jennings, M. K., & Niemi, R. G. (1981). Generations and politics: A
the civil rights movement and the new left. New 'fork: Knopf.
panel study of young adults and their parents. Princeton, NJ:
Fitzgerald, L. E. (1973). Women's changing expectations: New insights,
Princeton University Press.
new demands. Counseling Psychologist, 4, 90-95.
Keniston, K. (1967). The sources of student dissent. Journal of Social
Flacks, R. (1967). The liberated generation: An exploration of the roots
Issues, 23, 108-137.
of student protest. Journal of Social Issues, 23, 52-75.
Kohn, M., & Schooler, C. (1978). The reciprocal effects of the substan-
Giele, J. Z. (1993). Women's role change and adaptation, 1920-1990.
tive complexity of work and intellectual flexibility: A longitudinal
In K. D. Hulbert & D. T. Schuster (Eds.), Women's lives through time:
assessment. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 24—52.
Educated American women of the twentieth century (pp. 32-60).
Levinson, D. J. (1978). The seasons of a man's life. New York: Knopf.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Magnusson, D. (1990). Personality development from an interactional
Goldschmidt, J., Gergen, M., Quigley, K., & Gergen, K. J. (1974).
perspective. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory
The women's liberation movement: Attitudes and action. Journal of
and research (pp. 193-222). New York: Guilford Press.
Personality, 42, 601-617.
Mahoney, J. (1975). An analysis of the axiological structures of tradi-
Gough, H. (1996). CPI manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psycholo-
tional and proliberation men and women. Journal of Psychology, 90,
gists Press. (Original work published 1957).
31-39.
Gould, R. (1972). The phases of adult life: A study in developmental
psychology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 129, 521-531. Mannheim, K. (1952). The problem of generations. In P. Kecskemeti
Gurin, P. (1985). Women's gender consciousness. Public Opinion Quar- (Ed.), Essays on the sociology of knowledge (pp. 276-322). London:
terly, 49, 143-163. Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1928).
Haan, N., Millsap, R., & Hartka, E. (1986). As time goes by: Change McAllister, L. W. (1986). A practical guide to CPI interpretation. Palo
and stability in personality over fifty years. Psychology and Aging, 1, Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
220-232. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T, Jr. (1990). Personality in adulthood.
Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1994). Preferring "housewives" to New York: Guilford Press.
"feminists": Categorization and the favorability of attitudes toward McFarland, C , Ross, M., & Giltrow, M. (1992). Biased recollections
women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 25-52. in older adults: The role of implicit theories of aging. Journal of
Harker, L., & Solomon, M. (1996). Changes in goals and values of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 837-850.
men and women from early to mature adulthood. Journal of Adult Neugarten, B. L. (1968). Middle age and aging: A reader in social
Development, 3, 133-143. psychology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Helson, R. (1967). Personality characteristics and developmental history Peskilis, I. (1970). Resistances to consciousness. In R. Morgan (Ed.),
IMPORTANCE ATTRIBUTED TO THE WOMEN'S MOVEMENT 1555

Sisterhood is powerful: An anthology of writings from the women's Stewart, A. J., & Gold-Steinberg, S. (1990). Midlife women's political
liberation movement (pp. 337-339). New \brk: Vintage Books. consciousness: Case studies of psychosocial development and political
Roberts, B. W. (1997). Plaster or plasticity: Are adult work experiences commitment. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 14, 543-566.
associated with personality change in women? Journal of Personality, Stewart, A. J., & Healy, J. M. (1989). Linking individual development
65, 205-232. and social changes. American Psychologist, 44, 30—42.
Roberts, B. W., & Helson, R. (1997). Changes in culture, changes in Stewart, A. J., & Vandewater, E. A. (1993). The Radcliffe class of 1964:
personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 6 4 1 - Career and family social clock projects in a transitional cohort. In
651. K. D. Hulbert & D. T. Schuster (Eds.), Women's lives through time:
Rosow, I. (1978). What is a cohort and why? Human Development, 21, Educated American women of the twentieth century (pp. 235-258).
65-75. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Rozsnafszky, J., & Hendel, D. D. (1977). Relationship between ego Stoloff, C. (1973). Who joins women's liberation? Psychiatry, 36, 3 2 5 -
development and attitudes toward women. Psychological Reports, 41, 340.
161-162. Usher, S., & Fels, M. (1985). The challenge of feminism and career for
the middle-aged women. International Journal of Women's Studies,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Ryff, C D . (1987). The place of personality and social structure re-


8, 47-57.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

search in social psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-


Wink, P. (1991). Self- and object-directedness in adult women. Journal
chology, 53, 1192-1202.
of Personality, 59, 769-791.
Samp, G. (1976). Gender, authoritarianism, and attitudes toward femi-
Woodruff, D. S., & Birren, J. E. (1972). Age changes and cohort differ-
nism. Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 57—64.
ence in personality. Developmental Psychology, 6, 252-259.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environ-
Worell, J., & Worell, L. (1977). Support and opposition to the Women's
ments: A theory of genotype environment correlations. Child Develop-
Liberation Movement: Some personality and parental correlates. Jour-
ment, 54, 424-435. nal of Research in Personality, 11, 10-20.
Stewart, A. J. (1994). The women's movement and women's lives: Link-
ing individual development and social events. In A. Lieblich & R. Received January 24, 1997
Josselson (Eds.), Exploring identity and gender. The narrative study Revision received May 20, 1997
of lives (Vol. 2, pp. 230-250). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Accepted June 3, 1997 •

View publication stats

You might also like