Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 35

Psychological Review

© 2022 American Psychological Association 2023, Vol. 130, No. 3, 569–603


ISSN: 0033-295X https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000398

Seeking Connection, Autonomy, and Emotional Feedback:


A Self-Determination Theory of Self-Regulation in
Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
Rebecca E. Champ, Marios Adamou, and Barry Tolchard
School of Human and Health Sciences, University of Huddersfield

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most prevalent and highly debated diagnosis
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

for mental disorder in practice today. Two decades of research have substantially contributed to evolving
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

conceptualizations and understanding of the condition. However, this evolution has not been extended to
theoretical research. Current cognitive behavioral-based theories aim to identify the etiology of ADHD and
experience challenges in accommodating the full spectrum of both neurobiological and behavioral research
evidence. Characterizations historically associated with mental illness have generated public stigma,
influencing low self-esteem, negative self-concept, and identity development in ADHD individuals.
Neurodiversity research and activism recognize a diversity of nonnormative development and highlight
the need for alternatives to deficit models of functioning. Recent research in psychology recommends
developing approaches beyond symptom control and seeking to develop positive psychological factors and
well-being. We propose that the perspective presented by self-determination theory (SDT) on human
motivation, self-regulation, and self-determination offers a new understanding of ADHD research evidence
and symptomology. According to this theory, humans have a natural tendency toward growth and self-
actualization. We propose a framework grounded in SDT that provides an alternative understanding of
ADHD neural processing, motivation and engagement, self-regulation, and a potential foundation for
treatment approaches with self-determination and positive identity outcomes.

Keywords: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, motivation, neurodiversity

Scientific objectivity is a critical element when interpreting clini- a serious increased risk of adverse outcomes such as substance use,
cally relevant data. Although it is debatable whether pure objectivity psychosis and conduct disorders (Erskine et al., 2016; Groenman et
is attainable, it is a helpful heuristic for fostering scientific integrity, al., 2017; Kessler et al., 2006), educational and occupational failure
particularly scientific experimentation, inference, and theory choice (Nigg, Sibley, et al., 2020), and late-onset psychiatric problems
(Reiss & Sprenger, 2017). In understanding and working with including self-harm and suicide (Forte et al., 2020; Ljung et al.,
neurodevelopmental disorders, the combination of psychological 2014). If not addressed, ADHD can also have a significant economic
theory and interpretation of research are critical influencers in clinical impact beyond the individual, in terms of increased healthcare costs,
treatment development and design. Highlighted is the essential nature decreased productivity, and risk of offending (Champ et al., 2021;
of identifying assumptions, beliefs, theories, and goals to guide Kooij et al., 2019). Subthreshold cases of ADHD in childhood may
decision-making in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; meet full diagnostic criteria in adolescents (Kooij et al., 2019).
Nigg, Karalunas, et al., 2020; Toplak et al., 2008). However, due to the decline of hyperactive and impulsive symptoms
with age, some individuals may not have symptoms severe enough to
Negative Self-Concept in ADHD be diagnosed until adulthood, when executive function and atten-
tional difficulties become more salient. Many adults never receive a
Initially characterized as a childhood disorder of behavioral diagnosis; instead, they have symptoms attributed to motivational or
control, ADHD is now identified as a neurodevelopmental disorder intellectual deficits (Goodman, 2007). Studies examining healthy
of self-regulation with significant impact in multiple dimensions individuals’ perceptions of ADHD show symptoms being childish
across the lifespan (Barkley, 2002; Bokor & Anderson, 2014; and inappropriate; presentation of behaviors increased tendencies of
Solanto et al., 2008). Undiagnosed and untreated ADHD confers peer rejection and feelings of hostility; and debates on antisocial
tendencies, prejudices around symptom etiology, and immediate and
long-term effects of the medication may contribute to stigmatization
(Mueller et al., 2012). The literature on the stigma associated with
This article was published Online First December 22, 2022. mental illness emphasizes that the labeling of behavior as symptoms
Rebecca E. Champ https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3771-5213
of a mental disorder can create public stigma, leading to a reduced
Barry Tolchard https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4137-9013
The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
ability to use cognitive resources to perform well (Forbes &
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rebecca Schmader, 2010). Public stigma frequently results in self-stigma:
E. Champ, School of Human and Health Sciences, University of an internalization of a “new degraded identity” which negatively
Huddersfield, 7 Berristead Close, Wilburton, Ely CB6 3RS, United affects self-esteem, self-efficacy, and impacts behavioral goals
Kingdom. Email: rebecca.champ@hud.ac.uk (Corrigan et al., 2006; Fabrega, 1990; Kooij et al., 2019).

569
570 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

In addition, repetition and frequency of negative feedback on commitment therapy, and do not recognize any universal strengths
ADHD behaviors from authority figures often generate a negative associated with ADHD (Barkley et al., 2008, 2010; Brooks, 2001;
self-concept at an early age (Young & Bramham, 2012; Young et al., Champ et al., 2021; Demontis et al., 2019; Dipeolu, 2011; Ramsay &
2008). This association results in low self-esteem and a lack of trust Rostain, 2008a; Ramsay et al., 2016; Table 1).
in making decisions or taking action without external validation of An emerging body of research on neurodiversity recognizes
the appropriateness of behavior or choice, which creates an uncon- diverse neurological presentations and promotes the perspective
scious reliance on external structures or individuals for support. that nonnormative patterns of neurodevelopment are not psycho-
Psychology defines this as contingent self-esteem (Blom, 2011; logically unhealthy (Armstrong, 2010; Rosqvist et al., 2020a).
Crocker & Wolfe, 2001; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Defined as the infinite variation in neurocognitive functioning
within the human species, neurodiversity seeks recognition for
any significant embodied divergence from dominant cultural norms
Theoretical Etiology of ADHD
of neurocognitive functioning (Walker, 2021). Neurodiverse activ-
A substantial amount of research has been conducted over the ism criticizes the discourse of patterns of cognitive “normality” and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

past 20 years, with significant advancements in neuroimaging, promotes a recognition of conditions that impact the identity of an
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

genetics, pharmacology, psychology, and the social sciences, dra- individual alongside differences in perceiving and responding to the
matically changing the etiology and understanding of the symptoms world (Fenton & Krahn, 2007; Rosqvist et al., 2020a; Waltz, 2020).
and impact of ADHD. Despite these advancements, etiological Recent research in psychology also suggests that treatment ap-
theories are unable to account for all evidence entirely. Although proaches focusing on deficits and residual symptoms may not be
there are recognized neurobiological differences in ADHD, the role the best approach to improve mental health, and it may be necessary
of executive function (EF) is debated (Barkley & Brown, 2008; to develop positive psychological factors and emotions that culti-
Posner et al., 2020; Surman et al., 2015); heterogeneity remains vate health, well-being, and support psychological growth (Dell
unresolved (Cordova et al., 2020; Nigg, Karalunas, et al., 2020; et al., 2021; Jacob, 2015; Liberman, 2008; Slade, 2010). Self-
Posner et al., 2020); and context variability, or interest-based main- determination theory (SDT) is an organismic-dialectical approach
tenance of task engagement, is representative of ADHD challenges to human motivation and development (Ryan & Deci, 2017). SDT
with self-regulation (Brown, 2014; Hirvikoski et al., 2011; Roberts et views self-regulation as an evolving process of internalization and
al., 2014). It is noticeable that there has been little reexamination of integration of intrinsically motivated self-determined actions lead-
the theoretical underpinnings of ADHD etiology in light of these ing to self-organization (Ryan et al., 1999). In contrast to the
advancements. A recent scoping review highlighted that a single cognitive–behavioral theory, the roots of SDT lie in the core concept
etiological perspective based on cognitive–behavioral theory that living things have an organizational nature with inherent growth
forms the foundation for cross-disciplinary research in guidance tendencies. From this perspective, the SDT definition of well-being,
and treatment approaches for ADHD (Champ et al., 2021). or flourishing, consists of more than just the experience of hedonic
Cognitive–behavioral theoretical perspectives conceptualize ADHD positive emotions. It originates from pursuing a life of activity that is
as having weak intrinsic motivation due to neurobiological impair- subjectively satisfying developed from and expressing the most
ments with emotional self-regulation leading to challenges initiating reflectively valued and well-integrated human potentialities, a eu-
and persisting with goal-directed behavior (Barkley, 2014a). daimonic view of wellness (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2017;
This ADHD deficit-focused perspective is guided by cognitive– Ryan et al., 2013). According to this theory, this inherent human
behavioral models of self-regulation developed from mechanistic tendency to act volitionally and engage with the environment to
frameworks which prioritize goal achievement supported by cogni- achieve integration requires social and environmental support to
tive control of emotional response and behaviors (Carver & Scheier, satisfy basic psychological needs, and developing an individual
2016; Gross, 2013, 2015). These models are grounded in psycho- awareness of these needs is central to healthy self-regulation
logical (or motivational) hedonism, which claims that all human (Ryan & Deci, 2017). SDTs alternative theoretical perspective on
action and behavior aim at only increasing pleasure and avoiding human motivation offers the opportunity to reevaluate the potential
pain (Gosling, 1998; Moore, 2019). In these models, self-regulation strengths of ADHD, reinterpret ADHD research, and develop new
is synonymous with self-control, and a reduction in resources for treatment approaches with outcomes that include integration and
self-control is identified as ego depletion or deficits. Focus on flourishing. To understand the development of the etiology of
counteracting these deficits, or self-control strengthening, is a pri- ADHD, we begin with a short history of theoretical development.
mary outcome of research and frameworks in these approaches An overview of the current main theories of ADHD is followed by a
(Maranges & Baumeister, 2016). In ADHD, attributable behavioral critical review based on current research. Finally, we discuss SDT as
motivations are seen as approach and avoidance dynamics (Elliot, a theory and present a potential framework for an SDT-based
2006) presented as deficits in attention, response inhibition, and ADHD etiology and treatment approach.
arousal resulting in low motivation, stimulus seeking, and task
avoidance, among others, ultimately informing treatment design, Theoretical Development: Disorder to Syndrome
aims, and outcomes. Although treatment approaches include some
Disorder: /dɪsˈɔːdə/noun An illness or condition that disrupts normal
techniques for identification and development of strengths alongside physical or mental functions. (Oxford University Press, n.d.-a)
symptom reduction and control, they only support clarification of
prevalence for diagnostic purposes, fewer impairments (Brites et al., The clinical view of approaching, understanding, and working with
2015; Crosbie et al., 2013; Greven et al., 2016, 2018; Smalley et al., ADHD has a complicated history, heavily derived from social-
2007) or identification of individual supportive strengths, as dem- cognitive and cognitive–behavioral theory and practice. Initially,
onstrated in cognitive–behavioral therapy and acceptance and identification of this collection of behaviors arose from a frame of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Table 1
Comparison of ADHD Etiological Theories

Theoretical models of ADHD


Key differences Barkley Brown Sonuga-Barke Sonuga-Barke Sagvolden Nigg et al. Champ et al.

Definition of Challenges in self- Challenges with Challenges with Challenges with Challenges with altered Challenges with Challenges with
impairment regulating arousal engagement in inhibitory control inhibitory control reinforcement of accurate predictions acquisition and
concerning goal- successful self- delayed gratification delayed gratification novel behavior and resulting in difficulty
application of
directed behavior and regulating behavior generating generating deficient extinction of adjusting behavior tocognitively dominant
difficulties using attention and motivational deficits motivational deficits reinforced behavior what is presented skill sets,
maintaining attention memory to guide resulting in resulting in when something susceptibility to
for tasks with little to action preference for preference for unexpected happens salient and affective
no reinforcement or immediacy immediacy stimuli, and
immediate reward maintenance of
motivation and task
engagement in
autonomy depriving
and thwarting
contexts and
environments
Neurobiological Deficits in hierarchical Developmental Dysregulation of action Dysregulation of action Dysregulation of tonic/ Developmental Affect dominant and
origin maturational impairment of and thought resulting and thought resulting phasic dopamine impairments in inefficient cognitive
development of EF unconscious from poor inhibitory from poor inhibitory control causes stunted reinforcement, self-regulatory
and control. Caused automatic cognitive control associated control associated dopamine phasic reward, and effortful processes,
by abnormalities in network guidance with the meso-cortical with the meso-cortical responses despite regulation reliant on overinclusive
structure, function, systems of dynamic branch of the branch of the low-tonic levels. affect-related neural processing style, and
and bio-chemical EF clusters. Caused dopamine system dopamine system Affects the systems that detect altered temporal
operation of fronto- by differences in projecting in the projecting in the functioning of the events’ emotional processing. Caused
parietal and fronto- parieto-temporal cortical control cortical control anterior cingulate, significance. Caused by reduced volumes
striatal neural activation and centers (EF). Altered centers (EF). Altered dorsolateral prefrontal by deficits in in cognitive control
networks fronto-striatal and delay-of-reward delay-of-reward and motor circuits, maturation of top- networks and
fronto-cerebellar gradient linked to the gradient linked to the and subsequently a down cortical prolonged
connectivity, meso-limbic meso-limbic variety of behaviors projections from PFC development and
impairments in dopamine branch dopamine branch impairing hypofunctioning
default-mode associated with the associated with the differentiation of dopaminergic systems
deactivation, reward circuits (e.g., reward circuits (e.g., cognitive and neural impacting time
neurodevelopmental NAccs) NAccs) systems awareness and
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY

delay in maturation, resulting in


and inadequate hyperconnectivity to
release and control of SN and
dopamine and semisuppression of
norepinephrine DMN in cognitive
task engagement
Foundation of Neurobiopsychosocial Clinical interview Laboratory testing of Laboratory testing of Behavioral research Developmental Organismic-dialectical
model theory of EF as research of individuals with individuals with with animal models behavioral theory of human
psychological individuals with ADHD ADHD temperament theory motivation and
construct separate ADHD and their of personality traits development
from neurological families
functions
Impact of Disruption to EF Impairments to EF in Negative feedback on Negative feedback on Individual variations in Breakdowns or Variable interest-
impairment extended phenotype unconscious impulsive behaviors impulsive behaviors dopamine functioning disruption in linking dependant experience
levels adversely automaticity create and poor performance and poor performance on learning processes the known correct of need frustration or
571

impacting behavioral disruption in over time generates over time generates and behavior result in behavior with the amotivation in task
control resulting in activation delay aversion or delay aversion or developmental delays current context leads engagement.
(table continues)
572 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

with use of cognitive-


mental illness. In North America in the 1960s, the first shift in the

estimation interfering
embodied time leads

organizational skills.

motivational coping
dopaminergic stress
to over- or under-

Note. ADHD = attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; EF = executive function; NAccs = nucleus accumbens; PFC = prefrontal cortex; SN = salience network; DMN = default-mode network.
Champ et al. perspective around these behaviors appeared: the concept of hyper-

dependence on
Distortions in activity changed from an extreme state of excessive activity to a

response as a
Overreliance or
unconscious
behavioral syndrome of more significant than moderate activity

dominant

strategy
and an extreme but common disturbance in childhood. This concept
diverged significantly from Europe, particularly Great Britain, which
continued to view hyperactivity as uncommon, extreme, and usually
associated with brain damage. This shift would substantially impact
thoughts and actions
resulting in less top-

differing views of prevalence, diagnostic criteria, and treatment for

regulation of affect
recruited, affecting

the next 20 years (Prendergast et al., 1988; see Figure 1).


down cognitive

maintenance of
Nigg et al.

over time, and


control being
expectations,
to inaccurate

Dissatisfaction with a lack of objective measures for these


planning,

behaviors led to more detailed research. The work of Virginia


Douglas (Douglas, 1972, 1980a, 1980b, 1983; Douglas & Peters,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1979) effectively presented the first model of attention deficit by


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

introducing several key concepts of deficits. Barkley (2014b, p. 13)


instructional demands

behaviors and lack of


and behavior, leading

later summarized these as follows:


situationally relevant
connections between

development of self-
directed speech for
guiding behavior

1. “the investment, organization, and maintenance of


Sagvolden
in identifying

situational or

attention and effort;


to problems
anticipating
appropriate

2. inability to inhibit impulsive responding;

3. inability to modulate arousal levels to meet situational


Theoretical models of ADHD

demands; and
functional expressions

protection, and use of

delay aversion related


of motivational delay
avoidance or escape.

secondary effects of
negative emotional

4. unusually strong inclinations to seek immediate


to patterns of task
response to delay.

Cognitive deficits
Sonuga-Barke

with provision,

reinforcement.”
Behaviors are

time arise as

engagement

The impact of this article and the subsequent research that


followed resulted in a change in the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III) of the name “hyperkinetic
impulse disorder” to “attention-deficit disorder” and ADHD in the
functional expressions

delay aversion related


protection, and use of

DSM-IV (APA, 1980, 1987). Douglas’ original research formed the


of motivational delay
avoidance or escape.

secondary effects of
negative emotional

foundation for investigations into an underlying deficit in self-


to patterns of task
response to delay.

Cognitive deficits
Sonuga-Barke

with provision,

regulation (Barkley, 1997a) and further research by Douglas pointed


Behaviors are

time arise as

engagement

out the high degree of relationship between these deficits and


response to stimulant medication across all four domains
(Douglas, 2005).
Syndrome: /,ˈsɪndrəʊm/noun A group of symptoms which consistently
occur together, or a condition characterised by a set of associated
effort, and processing

(managing frustration

accessing recall), and


monitoring and self-
regulation), memory
alertness, sustaining

symptoms. (Oxford University Press, n.d.-b)


shifting attention),

(utilizing working
activating action),

effort (regulating

regulating action
focus (focusing,

speed), emotion
estimating, and

sustaining, and

Although there was general agreement that ADHD must be


Brown

memory and
(organizing,
prioritizing,

and general

associated with cognitive deficits, Douglas’ model identified a


pattern of behaviors but could not explain their origin. Continued
empirical research began to cast doubt on the central focus on
problems with attention in ADHD due to the situational variability
of symptoms and the lack of substantial evidence for deficits
gratification, impaired

involving perception, filtering, and processing of information.


impaired sense of

impaired intrinsic
verbal problem-
self, inability to
contemplate the

Also critical was the separation of ADHD, conduct disorder, and


future or delay

reasoning, and
Barkley

oppositional defiant disorder (Wender, 1995). Early studies of


solving and

motivation

“hyperactivity” failed to distinguish these disorders as separate


and characterized ADHD with these behaviors as standard. There-
fore, this literature is more relevant to childhood “externalising
Table 1 (continued)

behavior problems: for example, hyperactivity and aggression or


Key differences

hyperactivity and conduct disorders” (Loney & Milich, 1982,


p. 143) rather than ADHD alone. Investigators were increasingly
examining motivation and sustained efforts to conceptualize pro-
blems experienced by those with ADHD (Barkley, 2014b; Douglas,
1989; Slusarek et al., 2001).
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 573

Figure 1
Timeline of Changes in Etiology of ADHD
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. ADHD = attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; DSM-II = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Second Edition; DSM-III =
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition; DSM-IV = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition;
DSM-V = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition; ICD-8 = International Classification of Diseases, Eighth Revision; ICD-9 =
International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision; ICD-10 =International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Revision; PET = positron emition
tomography; MRI = magnetic resonance imaging. See the online article for the color version of this figure.

ADHD Evidence: Genetic and Neurobiological Research delay; however, the effect sizes were small with the exception of the
amygdala (Hoogman, Bralten, et al., 2017). Cortical thickness also
In the late 20th and early 21st century, research on ADHD appeared reduced in adults with ADHD (Almeida et al., 2010;
continued to be cognitively focused and advances in imaging and Duerden et al., 2012; Proal et al., 2011) as well as volume and
neurobiological studies influenced how ADHD was perceived. surface area in young people, although developmental patterns were
There was a removal of the association between ADHD and brain similar to controls (Ambrosino et al., 2017; Silk et al., 2016).
damage symptoms due to a lack of neurological abnormality evi- Evidence for structural deficits involving interconnections between
dence (Barkley, 2014b; Ramsay, 2010). Single-photon emission large-scale brain networks from diffusion tensor imaging (Cortese et
computed tomography (SPECT) indicated decreased blood flow in al., 2013; Konrad et al., 2010; Liston et al., 2011; Van Ewijk et al.,
the prefrontal regions and connecting pathways to the limbic system 2014) was attributed to microstructural abnormalities in white matter
via the striatum, specifically the anterior caudate and cerebellum. tracts in the cingulum, isthmus, fronto-occipital, frontostriatal, tem-
SPECT imagery of dopamine transporter indicated lower dopamine poral, and temporo-occipital fasciculi with atypical hemispheric
transporter levels in medication-naive patients (Fusar-Poli et al., connection most commonly identifed in the corpus callosum (Aoki
2012; Volkow et al., 2009). Positron emission tomography demon- et al., 2018; Cortese et al., 2013; Dramsdahl et al., 2012; Konrad et al.,
strated that methylphenidate blocks dopamine and norepinephrine 2012; Onnink et al., 2015; Shaw et al., 2015). There were also
transporters, increasing the dopamine in the synapse and amplifying indications of more significant inattention symptomology associated
the saliency value of stimuli (Volkow et al., 2005). Structural brain with lower microstructural integrity in the left uncinate and inferior
imaging showed reduced total/whole brain and gray matter volume fronto-occipital fasciculi than controls (Shaw et al., 2015).
(Albajara Sáenz et al., 2019), with abnormal volumes in gray matter Functional MRI (fMRI) meta-analysis identifies dysfunctions in
in several areas, including the right frontal and prefrontal areas several areas of the motor inhibition network (Lei et al., 2015).
(Depue et al., 2010; Pironti et al., 2014), anterior cingulate (Amico Studies indicated that hypoactivation is described as “deficient
et al., 2011; Frodl & Skokauskas, 2012; Makris et al., 2010), basal fronto-striatal activation” when compared to controls in the
ganglia and cerebellum (Almeida Montes et al., 2010; Makris et al., fronto-parietal executive control network, putamen, and ventral
2015; Proal et al., 2011; Seidman et al., 2011), and visual cortex attention network (Cortese et al., 2012, p. 1051). Hyperactivations
(Ahrendts et al., 2011). Several studies have identified subcortical in regions of the default-mode and optical networks and consistent
brain volume differences with the most consistent findings showing underactivation compared to controls in fronto-cerebellar networks
reduced volumes in areas of the basal ganglia, specifically involving for timing functions (Hart et al., 2012) as well as abnormally
the right lentiform nucleus (putamen and globus pallidus) and the enhanced activation in default-mode regions (Hart et al., 2012),
caudate nucleus (Ellison-Wright et al., 2008; Hutchinson et al., inferior fronto-striatal networks during cognitive tasks (Hart et al.,
2008; Norman et al., 2016; Valera et al., 2007). Significantly smaller 2013), and dorsolateral fronto-striato-parietal networks during atten-
volumes were identified in the accumbens, amygdala, caudate, tion tasks (Cai et al., 2021; Hart et al., 2013) were perceived as faulty
hippocampus, and putamen in children confirming brain maturation regulation of relationships between default-mode and task-positive
574 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

networks (McCarthy et al., 2014; Sonuga-Barke & Castellanos, Goldman-Rakic, and Damasio. Bronowski’s (1977) theory of the
2007). Differential task activations during inhibition, attention, or uniqueness of human language describes four unique human mental
working memory tasks appeared in regions overlapping the intrinsic abilities attributed to the prefrontal cortex (PFC): prolongation,
fronto-parietal, dorsal attentional, visual, motor, and default-mode separation of affect, internalization, and reconstitution. Fuster’s
networks (DMNs) in ADHD compared to controls (Castellanos & (1997) theory of prefrontal lobe functions emphasizes EFs’ temporal
Proal, 2012; Lei et al., 2015). Abnormally enhanced functional integration and orientation toward the future. Goldman-Rakic
connectivity between limbic and orbitofrontal regions during emo- (1995) argues that activation and use of visual representations of
tional processing, both positive and negative, indicates emotional the world and the storage of those representations for behavior
hyper-responsivity (Rubia, 2018), and local efficiency and cluster- regulation are significant functions of the PFC. Damasio (1994,
ing of the right insula are positively associated with emotional 1995) postulates that a critical component of decision-making in the
dysregulation, particularly with the hyperactive presentation of prefrontal lobes is a cost–benefit analysis supported by emotional
ADHD (Viering et al., 2021). and motivational information provided by somatic markers. Bark-
Genetic studies also indicated that ADHD has high heritability ley’s theory uses constructs from behavioral and social-cognitive
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Franke et al., 2012; Kooij et al., 2019), a co-occurrence or overlap theories of personality and functioning, goal theory, socioeconomic
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

with autistic spectrum disorder (Ghirardi et al., 2018; Miller et al., theory, and neuropsychological models of executive functioning
2019), intellectual disability (Faraone et al., 2017), communication and evolutionary psychology.
and learning disorders (Thapar et al., 2017), and heritable phenotypes Barkley’s foundation of self relies on Skinnerian radical operant
including anxiety and depression and health risk behaviors such as conditioning (Skinner, 1953). In this theory of personality, the
smoking, obesity (Demontis et al., 2019), and alcoholism (Derks et concept of self, or everything an organism does in response to a
al., 2014). A strong correlation between ADHD diagnosis and trait stimulus, is defined as behavior (Skinner, 1971). Barkley cites Von
scores in the general population demonstrated that ADHD represents Mises (1990) economic model and objectivism (Piekoff, 1993) as
the extreme of a continuously distributed trait (Demontis et al., 2019; sources for definitions of means or action toward goals or values.
Faraone & Larsson, 2019; Middeldorp et al., 2016). Literature Goals are states resulting in decreased dissatisfaction or unease
published during and after this time show a shift in how professionals relative to the present state and future states that we seek to attain,
and researchers describe ADHD based on this ongoing research: fulfill, and retain. Barkley defines wants and desires as these future
states that we value because they increase satisfaction, happiness,
The genetic explanation of ADHD has an important implication that can
and welfare (Piekoff, 1993). Energization and direction of behavior
easily go overlooked: ADHD may simply represent an extreme form of
a normal human trait and not a pathological condition in most cases.
are motivated by movement to change the present state to a future
(Barkley, 2005, pp. 75–76) improved state, toward, or away from pleasure or pain—a hedonic
view of wellness which is a common theme in social-cognitive
Consequently, rather than being a disorder of the 21st Century, ADHD theories of functioning (Carver & Scheier, 2016; Mischel & Shoda,
is an ageless syndrome, likely having been present ever since there has 1995). According to Von Mises (1990), the utility or use value of a
been a human brain. (Ramsay, 2010, p. 11) goal is subjective or objective, with subjective being individually
evaluated. The degree to which the individual perceives a goal to
Current Theories of ADHD alleviate dissatisfaction will determine its value and drive goal-
directed actions and behavior. The course of action pursued to attain
Douglas (1972) model set the stage for self-regulation as a core that goal is defined as the means to that end. Barkley equates this
deficit in ADHD, but it constituted a psychological model of individual evaluation for choices and actions with self-regulation or
behavior rather than a theory of ADHD. Barkley (1998) postulated self-control. EF and self-regulation in this model are therefore
the first unifying theory of ADHD, which places a core deficit of directly linked to forward projections of effect in linear time: an
behavioral inhibition at the source of ADHD behaviors. Strongly executive act is any action an organism takes to modify behavior
based on Skinnerian principles, Barkley reframed issues with predicting it will change future outcomes (Barkley, 2015).
attention and impulsivity as stimulus control problems in a relation-
ship between a stimulus and behavior (Barkley, 1989; Mash &
The Six EFs
Barkley, 2006). Barkley’s executive dysfunction theory suggests
deficits in four key areas: working memory, emotional regulation, For Barkley, the ability to evaluate subjective goals correlates to
internalization of language, and reconstitution, or creation of novel, EFs, which he defines as “those self-directed actions needed to
complex goal-directed behaviors (Barkley, 1997b, 1998; Willcut, choose goals and to create, enact and sustain actions toward those
2014). As an extended phenotype theory, Barkley’s model is built goals” (Barkley, 2012, p. 60), a view echoed in neuropsychology
upon observation of perceived chronic difficulties in behavior, (Eslinger, 1996; Wagner & Heatherton, 2011). Barkley describes six
measured as “excessive or inappropriate for their age or develop- forms of EF that are interactive and serve the common purpose of
ment level” (Roberts et al., 2014, p. 63). These symptoms present in internalizing self-directed behavior to rehearse actions to test prob-
two dimensions: hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention. able consequences mentally. In particular, it allows for assessing
emotional responses, defined by Barkley as somatic markers
(Damasio, 1994, 1995), to determine social acceptability.
EF Theory Perspective on Self-Regulation
Muscular–skeletal manifestations of behavior initially observable
Barkley’s (1997b, 2012) theory defines EF as a psychological in childhood are inhibited by EFs through maturation to form private
construct separate from the traditionally presented neurological behaviors in adulthood to align with social–cultural norms. Man-
functions. At its core are four critical theorists: Bronowski, Fuster, agement of social conduct by EF, specifically the PFC, is
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 575

highlighted as the evolutionary purpose of self-regulation (Barkley, EF, to which Barkley again adds Vygotsky’s model of
2012; Eslinger, 1996). Therefore, these abilities facilitate adaptive internalization of speech which forms the ability of self-
functioning by anticipating and preparing for future action through control via language through covert self-description self-
an internal trial-and-error process (Barkley, 2015). instruction, self-questioning, and problem-solving. This
The six forms of EF or self-directed actions are hierarchically loop invents rules and metarules for oneself (Diaz & Berk,
arranged as follows: 1992), but Barkley also suggests that it is responsible for
maintaining moral conduct or internalizing socially pre-
1. Self-directed attention (self-awareness). Self-directed scribed rules. This “cool EF” (Castellanos et al., 2006)
attention is the first to arise and forms the conscious represents the internalization in Bronowski’s (1977) the-
sense of “self.” Barkley places self-awareness as the ory and activates the same prefrontal regions used for
“central executive” seat responsible for the coordination audible speech (Ryding et al., 1996).
of cognitive processes, similar to Stuss and Alexander
(2000) and in contrast to other models of EF. This EF 5. Self-directed appraisal (emotion/motivation to the self).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

provides the awareness of values and wants and the Arising from the first three EFs, this EF represents affec-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

capacity to generate and consider options available to the tive and motivational valences, or hedonic tone, associated
individual over time, making it the “seat of human free with visual and verbal information. These valences are
will” (Barkley, 2012, p. 29). Evidence for self-awareness Damasio’s (1994, 1995) “somatic markers.” They indicate
as a brain function primarily centered in the PFC and the intrinsic “goodness” or “badness” of an event, object,
connected networks is drawn from three sources: or situation related to homeostasis, generating the motiva-
diminished self-awareness following brain injuries, tion to move toward satisfaction and away from dissatis-
specifically to the PFC (Stuss & Alexander, 2000), faction. Similar to the previous EFs, Barkley defines these
Damasio’s neurobiological triumvirate model (Damasio, valences as emotional displays that eventually move from
2000), and neuroimaging research (Herwig, 2010). visible to covert through maturation, transforming from
reactions to an internalized motivationally guiding force,
2. Self-restraint (executive inhibition). Executive inhibition or the “wellspring of intrinsic motivation” (Barkley, 2015,
provides the delay or temporal gap required for self-directed p. 415). With the support of the previous three EFs,
actions, or goal-directed behavior, to arise. Separation of emotions are responses to environmental stimuli and
the event from the sensorimotor responses of future activity states created as needed as self-motivation or “hot EF”
exemplifies the decoupling of stimulus–response behavior (Castellanos et al., 2006). These states initiate and sustain
required for attention to be shifted away from the present action toward future goals, attributed to bidirectional net-
moment environment and directed toward the self and the works linking the DLPFC, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior
goal, or contemplated future (Barkley, 2015). This EF cingulated cortex, and amygdala (Damasio, 1994, 1995;
represents the separation of affect in Bronowski’s (1977) Etkin et al., 2006; K. N. Ochsner et al., 2009; K. N.
theory, and Barkley locates this at the automatic level of Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Rushworth et al., 2007).
brain functioning, incorporating the frontal–striatal cir-
cuitry and basal ganglia (Saint-Cyr, 2003). Combined with the previous three EFs, mainly nonverbal work-
ing memory, this EF provides the persistence (sustained attention)
3. Sensing the self (nonverbal working memory). This EF is and willpower to support the attainment of future goals. Barkley
conceptualized as a self-directed action to use visual defines this capacity to inhibit immediate behavior favoring future
imagery or imagination. Often compared to Baddeley’s outcomes, or delay gratification, as a conscious executive appraisal.
(1986; Baddeley & Hitch, 1994) visual–spatial sketch pad, From this perspective, the ability to contemplate “later” versus
Barkley combines this concept with Vygotsky’s (1978, “now” makes the action of choice a biological function to support
1987) theory of the development of private speech to goal-directed behavior. This action includes emotional self-
suggest a visual form of self-guidance and direction, or regulation in choosing to inhibit strong emotional displays to
“cool EF” (Castellanos et al., 2006) that becomes progres- appropriately respond to a social context, meet social demands,
sively internalized with maturation. This EF has two roles: and maintain relationships.
the frontal lobe enables access to past sensory experiences
and links them to covert prospective preparatory motor 6. Self-play (reconstitution). The final EF completes Bark-
elements to mentally rehearse options for future responses ley’s model basis on Bronowski (1977). This EF conveys
(Fuster, 1997; Goldman-Rakic, 1995) and experiences can the ability to take apart (analysis) and recombine
then be self-elicited in the absence of the primary stimulus (synthesis) information to form novel combinations of
for behavioral reenactment or mimesis. Barkley considers behaviors, defined in Barkley’s model as problem-
the ability of this EF to bridge cross-temporal elements solving. Behavioral innovation, flexibility, generativity,
crucial for self-control. The foundation of this EF is a and planning for goal-directed action from a present state
prolongation, from Bronowski’s (1977) theory, linked to the desired state are all attributed to this EF, and
with the right dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) and posterior Barkley describes this as a process of action fluency (Piatt
visual association areas (D’Esposito et al., 1995, 1997). et al., 1999) or the ability to retrieve verbs impaired in
frontal lobe injury. Barkley locates this alongside verbal
4. Speech to the self (verbal working memory). Baddeley’s and nonverbal working memory in separate DLPFC
(1986) construct of a phonological loop is the basis for this regions (Lee et al., 1997; Stuss & Alexander, 2000).
576 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Extended Phenotype Model of EF temporal “now” and in the immediate environment outside of
self-awareness or self-consciousness. Primary emotions occur at
As self-regulatory processes, Barkley suggests each EF contri-
this level that may heighten attention and redirect behavior to
butes to adaptive functioning in the species’ phenotype (Dawkins,
immediate well-being situations. However, Barkley perceives this
1982), which critically impacts social activity and individual sur-
as a primitive, unthinking, unreasoning animal-like level of exis-
vival and welfare. Figure 2 represents the effect of the outward
tence, and does not consider these responses forms of self-regulation
extension of the EF phenotype into personal, social, community, and
and therefore not EF (Barkley, 2012).
cultural activities (Barkley, 2012).
Instrumental-Self-Directed Level. The six self-directed ac-
tions of EF are at the instrumental-self-directed level of the model.
Self-direction and internalization are taken from Vygotsky’s model
Preexecutive and Executive Levels of Functioning
(Diaz & Berk, 1992; Vygotsky, 1978, 1987), causing this level to
The extended phenotype model is hierarchical and includes six arise from the preexecutive through human development musculo-
functioning levels, dependent on attaining the previous level for skeletal movements’ expression and action to become private and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

emergence, identified as means of self-control, or classes of action “cognitive.” This representation of the executive level exerts effort-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

self-directed to make choices and improve the future. ful control to cause the “top-down” regulation of automatic actions
Preexecutive. The preexecutive level of the model contains the through generation and sustainment of mental representations of the
“automatic” or largely unconscious level of human activity seen in future (Carver & Scheier, 2016; Eisenburg et al., 2011). In current
feedback loop theory (Carver & Scheier, 2016), Gross’s emotional cognitive theory, “top-down” self-regulation is influenced by cog-
self-regulation model (Gross, 2007, 2013; K. N. Ochsner & Gross, nitive regulation skills managed by EFs demonstrated as intentional
2005), attentional control models (Rueda et al., 2011), and the cognitive control (Carver & Scheier, 2016). Emotional regulation is
concept of effortful control (Eisenburg et al., 2011). It represents considered as a less intentional and more reactive “bottom-up”
neuropsychological functions responsible for the operant condition- influence which can be helpful and unhelpful. Metacognition, or
ing behavior with stimulus structure—response—consequence self-monitoring, is considered essential to self-regulation for self-
(Skinner, 1953), moment-to-moment actions directed to the evaluation, task difficulty, performance, and strategy acquisition

Figure 2
Barkley’s Extended Phenotype Model of EF

Note. EF = executive function. Adapted from “Executive Functions: What They Are, How They
Work, and Why They Evolved,” by R. A. Barkey, 2012. Guildford Press. Copyright 2012 by
Guilford Press. Adapted with permission. See the online article for the color version of this figure.
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 577

(Fernandez-Duque et al., 2000). The integration of cognitive regu- & Fitzsimons, 2011; Fitzsimons & Finkel, 2011; McCullough &
lation and emotional regulation to regulate behavior and actions are Carter, 2011).
the central work of self-regulation, and these domains influence one Strategic-Cooperative Level. Barkley separates this level
another in a bidirectional relationship (Blair & Ursache, 2011). from the previous level as he equates cooperation with a conscious
Barkley cites Gross’s (2007) modal model and Carver and understanding of the division of labor and increased sacrifice of self-
Scheier’s (2016) feedback loop theory as two equivalent self- interest to obtain greater personal benefit leading to acceptance of
regulation models. In Gross’s modal model, Barkley compares interdependence. Conscience, sympathy, empathy, guilt, remorse,
the executive level pathways of emotional control as methods the and a sense of belonging are defined as psychological capacities
EFs use to influence automatic behaviors via five vectors: situation arising from and facilitating social cooperation (Barkley, 2012).
selection, situation modification, attentional redirection, event reap- Specialization in skills based on the talent through foresight and
praisal, and response modification/suppression (Gross, 2007). The reasoning arises at this level and trust, investment, and behavior
first two stages are proactive, decreasing the likelihood that an based on laws and ethics. Social mutualism, or placing others’ self-
unwanted primary action/emotion will occur. The remaining three interest over or ahead of one’s own, characterizes this level.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

are reactive and potentially less effective as a primary action/ Extended Utilitarian Level. Barkley’s final level acts as a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

emotion has been elicited, and there is a limited resource pool of feedback mechanism for EF successful use or self-regulation from
willpower which can become depleted in the process (Bauer & the previous levels. Evidence for successful individual use of EF is
Baumeister, 2011). This process can be used both to self-regulate demonstrated in the extension of life expectancy, economic and
strong negative emotions and enhance or prolong positive emotions social success, and long-term welfare of offspring. Conscientious
through methods such as mental contrasting—visual images of the behavior, or considering the consequences to self and others over
desired state/goal contrasted with the present state—thus creating a impulsiveness and selfishness, prevents premature death, promotes
form of emotional self-control (Gollwitzer & Oettingen, 2011). In positive judgment and feedback from others, ensures professional
Carver and Scheier’s multisystem feedback loop model, Barkley advancement, improves parenting abilities, and ensures economic
compares EFs self-directed action with the discrepancy reduction resources to support future generations. Barkley supports this with
monitoring between the present and desired state/goals. As emotions research on the negative impact of ADHD on daily life activities
in Barkley’s model provide motivational indicators of progress generally (Barkley, 2011), and specifically including marriage and
toward goals, Carver and Scheier’s (2016) model uses emotional relationships (Barkley, 2008; Ninowski et al., 2007) and parenting
markers to measure variation in rates and velocity of progress (Banks et al., 2008; Barkley, 2006; Murray & Johnston, 2006).
toward goals. Barkley also maps EFs components to the two-level
emotional feedback loop model as the executive level of effortful,
conscious, deliberative, and rational goal-directed action that alters EF Theory Perspective of ADHD
or overrides the automatic, experiential, unconscious, and intuitive Barkley’s theory prioritizes two key elements: the core of EF or
level if one’s actions become inappropriate to the situation or goal. self-regulation: the self-direction of self-regulation of actions across
Barkly has a particular economic view of self-directed behaviors, time for goal attainment and internalization of those self-directed
differentiating between leisure and labor (Von Mises, 1990). Self- actions (Barkley, 2012). From this perspective, Barkley believes
directed actions that are pleasurable or reduce dissatisfaction can be that ADHD demonstrates apparent deficits. There are observable
ends in themselves but are not EF if there is no modification to goal- challenges in self-regulating arousal concerning goal-directed
directed behavior or alter delayed consequences. Barkley classes behavior and difficulties maintaining attention for tasks with little
these as potentially maladaptive because they may interfere with to no reinforcement or immediate reward (Barkley, 1997b, p. 68).
long-term goals and welfare, such as mind wandering/daydreaming According to Barkley, deficits in the hierarchical maturational
interfering with completing a task on time (Barkley, 2012). development of EF in ADHD are predicted to disrupt this extended
Methodical-Self-Reliant Level. This model’s level extends the phenotype’s various levels, adversely impacting the sources of
analysis and synthesis into recombination from the self-directed control over behavior. In other words, ADHD will interfere with
level to sequences of actions within the physical environment, such the biological action of choice and, therefore, a loss of freedom and
as routines. Barkley (2012, 2014a) argues that this is the basis for self-determination (Barkley, 2012). This interference results in an
human productivity and innovation to effectively attain goals and
increase one’s quality of life and long-term welfare through envi- • Impaired sense of self, including self-awareness and self-
ronmental reorganization. The ability to self-organize and act monitoring.
beyond internal executive cognitions is the hallmark of this level.
• Inability to contemplate the future, resulting in reduced
Tactical-Reciprocal Level. This level represents social reci-
hindsight, forethought, and the creation of anticipatory
procity and the organization of the social environment for goal
action toward future events. This inability impacts the
achievement. Effective moral and ethical rules of social conduct
ability to organize and execute actions to achieve goals
require inhibition of self-favoring others and long-term welfare for
concerning time and engage in imitation and vicarious
circumstances where goals cannot be achieved alone. The ability to
learning.
engage in behaviors that support effective social exchange for
mutual benefit over the longer term such as sharing, taking turns, • Inability to delay gratification, leading to impulsivity, or
and accepting social etiquette, which Barkley (2012) states is inability to subordinate self-interest to others or long-term
challenging for individuals with PFC injuries (Harlow, 1848, personal goals; and distractibility, or failure to inhibit
1868; Luria, 1966) and affects individual self-regulation (Finkel responses to a task or goal-irrelevant events.
578 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

• Reduced behavioral self-control because of greater public This self-management system of the brain provides the mecha-
and less private self-speech and reflection before acting, nism for self-regulation, defined as the ability to change oneself and
impairing verbal problem-solving and reasoning. Barkley exert control over one’s inner processes specifically by organizing
implies that this includes difficulty following rules and actions toward goals, managing emotional distress, obeying laws,
instructions and other ethical guides for conduct. and internalizing social standards of both moral and competent
behavior (Vohs & Baumeister, 2004). Brown’s model views the
• Impulsive, emotional expression and reactivity, interfering coordinated efforts of EFs as a networked guidance system, much
with top-down emotional regulation processes in the ser- like a computer (Carver & Scheier, 2016) or the conductor of an
vice of long-term goals, including situation selection and orchestra as opposed to a separate concept of conscious sense of self,
modification, attention deployment, cognitive reappraisal, a difference that Barkly challenges as a passive view of the organism
and response suppression or modification (Gross, 2007). in existing models of EF (Barkley, 2012). Brown sets this manage-
Somatic marker-based decision-making would also be ment system firmly in the unconscious or System 1 of Kahneman’s
impaired (Damasio, 1994, 1995). (2011) behavioral “thinking fast and slow” theory via “automatic-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

• Impaired intrinsic motivation or diminished internal ability ity,” defined as rapid activation of attitudes, emotions, or behaviors
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to induce drive or motivational states toward goals, result- that emerge in a specific context without directed conscious thought
ing in a dependence on the external environment for (Bargh, 1994; Brown, 2014). Brown (2014) cites LeDoux (1996)
motivation. Without external motivators, sustained atten- and Dodge (1991), who highlight the primacy of unconscious
tion and persistence become erratic. emotional processing as the energetic, motivational force, or “hot
EF” (Castellanos et al., 2006) that drives cognitive activity and
• The decline in problem-solving and innovation ability shapes experience and action. Circuits of the limbic system, specifi-
leading to overreliance on automatic or inadequate strate- cally the amygdala, are designated as instantaneous processers that
gies or complete abandonment of goal pursuits. assess emotional reactions and appraise the level of reward or threat
concerning instinct and memories of past experiences to designate
how much attention should be allotted (Damasio, 1999). Brown
Alternative Neuropsychological Origins of ADHD differentiates his model from Barkley’s by broadening his emotional
Since the executive dysfunction model’s publication, several modulation scope from inhibition of negative emotions to include
theoretical models attribute additional and alternative cognitive value for positive emotions, such as interest, attraction, and desire.
and behavioral sources for developing ADHD symptoms. Presented Dopamine, a neurotransmitter particularly sensitive to pleasure and
are four theories reviewed by Johnson et al. (2009). Included is reward, is highlighted as a signaller of important stimuli to the PFC,
Brown’s EF model, often cited in treatment literature. which selects action plans, including storing in working memory
(Brown, 2005). Dopamine increases triggers interest and indicates
reward, and therefore motivation that creates a feedback loop to
Brown’s EF Cluster Model sustain engagement—a hedonic view of well-being. This system
Similar to Barkley, Brown’s (2005, 2013, 2014) model also effortlessly draws upon personal memory of past experiences and
places EF impairments and their relationship with self-regulation learned skills to automatically produce complex responses in a
at the core of ADHD (Brown, 2006). However, the definition of EFs, variety of contexts, reducing the need to make conscious deliberate
the origin of impairments, and their impact on individuals are decisions. The ability to use emotion-based stored working memory
considerably different. Instead of Barkley’s heavily theoretical to hold things in mind and utilize that information to direct current
approach, Brown’s cognitive–behavioral model builds from action, or make choices, via “cool EF” (Castellanos et al., 2006) is
research based on intensive clinical interviews with individuals crucial for the guidance of EFs and self-regulation (Brown, 2005).
diagnosed with ADHD and their families. Brown identifies In this model, ADHD persistence in impairments is attributed to
ADHD as a complex syndrome of developmental impairments of EFs’ location in unconscious automaticity (Brown, 2013), making
EFs (Brown, 2013). Defined as capacities enabling engagement in medication treatment essential. The situational variability of im-
successful independent self-serving behavior (Lezak et al., 2004), pairments and adaptive problems in day-to-day activities that char-
EFs in this model are not hierarchical but clusters of cognitive acterize ADHD is presented as the evidence of impairments in
functions that interact dynamically and continuously for self- cognitive management networks, not independent cognitive func-
management in the following ways: tions (Brown, 2006). Neurobiologically, four areas are highlighted
as interfering with effective self-regulation. Limitation to the inter-
• Activation: organizing, prioritizing, estimating time, and active communication networks due to white matter differences is
activating action to work. suggested as the origins of working memory deficits (Castellanos &
Proal, 2012; Cortese et al., 2012, 2013). This deficit is evidenced by
• Focus: focusing, sustaining, and shifting attention to tasks.
a single fMRI study in children where performance differences in
• Effort: regulating alertness, sustaining effort, and proces- parieto-temporal activation and fronto-striatal and fronto-cerebellar
sing speed. connectivity during vigilant attention were normalized compared to
controls with methylphenidate (K. Rubia et al., 2009). Differences
• Emotion: managing frustration and regulating emotions. in increasing deactivation of the DMN during active attention tasks
• Memory: utilizing working memory and accessing recall. (Fassbender et al., 2009) are identified as impairments in the
coordination of dynamic shifts in rate and rhythms of bran cell
• Action: monitoring and self-regulating action. oscillations (Brown, 2014). This impairment is evidenced by a
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 579

single fMRI study in children of increased default-mode suppression mechanisms, energetic activation and effort mechanisms, and man-
with methylphenidate (Peterson et al., 2009). Neurodevelopmental agement, or EF, system deficits. The influence of effort highlights
delay in maturation (P. Shaw et al., 2007), or less maturity in Sergeant’s relationship between effort and the activation pool over
emotional management than peers, is highlighted as damaging to response choice, motor preparation, or motivation. Research has
learning, relationships, and self-esteem in years when education and demonstrated that individuals will delay motor preparation until
preparation for adult life are key. Inadequate release and control of they gain maximum payoff for effort with minimum energy alloca-
dopamine and norepinephrine and its impact on the delay of tion (Hackley & Miller, 1995). Children with ADHD have exhibited
reinforcement (Swanson et al., 2011) are suggested as origins in sensitivity to response contingencies, rewards, and deficient
deficits in motivation and sustained effort. Studies indicating response organizations in laboratory testing, particularly when
improvements in utilization of working memory and functional stimuli are presented slowly, which is demonstrated as a rapid
connectivity using methylphenidate are presented as beneficial decline in task efficiency concerning the event rate (Sergeant
(Prince & Wilens, 2009), although Brown admits few studies et al., 1999). Because the event rate influences motor activation
address the impact of medication treatment on emotion regulation in the cognitive-energetic model, Sergeant defines ADHD as an
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Brown, 2014). energetical state dysfunction with insufficient effort allocation.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Allocation of energetic resources is probably strongly associated


with the availability of particular neurotransmitters (Sergeant et al.,
Sergeant’s State Regulation Hypothesis
2003). Thus, symptoms will increase or decrease depending on the
Similar to Barkley, Sergeant (2000, 2005) identifies inhibition as task parameters and the state of the individual with ADHD, and for
a central deficit in ADHD. However, disinhibition is not unique to Sergeant, this could explain ADHD heterogeneity.
ADHD, and therefore the State Regulation Hypothesis of ADHD
(Sergeant et al., 1999) focuses on the activation of inhibition and
Sonuga-Barke’s Delay Aversion/Dual Pathway Theory
highlights the energetic state as an origin for performance deficits.
Based on human vigilance and performance research, the cognitive- In contrast to the EF deficit models, Sonuga-Barke’s delay
energetic model (Sanders, 1983) is used to act as a bridge between aversion/dual-pathway theory (Sonuga-Barke, 1994, 2003, 2004;
“top-down” and “bottom-up” theories of functioning and addresses Sonuga-Barke et al., 1992) began as a simple single deficit model
the heterogeneity found in ADHD (Sergeant, 2005; Sergeant et al., based on motivational factors. Based on the laboratory evidence,
2003). Sanders (1983) identifies three levels of information proces- this theory initially identified ADHD impulsivity as the expression
sing: management, energetic, and cognitive processing, divided into of a natural preference for immediacy in children with ADHD,
two functional categories: process or computational mechanisms of particularly when a choice is an option. Because this preference is
attention and state or energy for activation. Cognitive information expressed even when rewards are offered following delay, it is
processing operates in four stages: encoding, search, decision, and interpreted as a biologically based shortened delayed reward gradi-
motor organization. State processes are categorized into three ent (Sonuga-Barke, 2003). It is suggested that children with ADHD
energetic pools: effort, arousal, and activation. have automatic “bottom-up” hypervigilance to emotionally or
The effort is the energy necessary to meet task demands and is motivationally relevant environmental cues indicating opportunities
required when the organism experiences stress, an intervening to escape from delay (Sonuga-Barke et al., 2004). The ability to
variable response when the organism’s current state does not delay gratification has high social–cultural importance in terms of
meet the required state for task performance. Emotions, defined socialization, and this model theorizes that parental intolerance of
as physiological feedback, are indicators of stress. The effort is “impulsiveness” creates an association over time of delay with
given state primacy in this model as it excites and inhibits the arousal negative responses. Consequently, children with ADHD develop
pool and the activation pool, highlighting its role in motivation and a negative emotional response to delay or delay aversion (Sonuga-
response contingencies (e.g., threat/reward)—a hedonic view of Barke, 2003).
well-being. The effort is associated with the hippocampus, and pupil Inattention and hyperactivity are also expressions of this prefer-
dilation measures cognitive load and effort engagement (Sergeant ence, presented as a “top-down” process designed to modify the
et al., 1999). subjective experience of time passing and reduce the experience of
Arousal is a phasic, or rapid, adaptation to stimuli that indicates a delay. The aim is to effectively “speed up time” in situations without
rate of change, described as “time-locked” (Pribram & McGuinness, choice by moving attention to non-task-related aspects in the
1975; Sergeant et al., 1999). Arousal is associated with the mesen- environment (Sonuga-Barke, 2003; Sonuga-Barke et al., 1996).
cephalic, reticular formation, and amygdala regions of the brain. Neurobiologically, delay aversion is placed within a motivational
Activation is a tonic or slow physiological readiness to respond to or affective reward circuit to include the ventral striatum (nucleus
stimuli associated with the basal ganglia and corpus striatum. The accumbens [NAccs]), frontal regions (notably the anterior cingu-
process by which stimuli may be maintained and manipulated in a late), and orbitofrontal cortex. The circuit is completed by connec-
short-term buffer is called working memory (Baddeley, 1996). tions in the ventral palladium, structures in the thalamus, and the
Planning, monitoring, and error detection and correction are the amygdala. The meso-limbic neurotransmitter dopamine, which we
management mechanism’s role, or EF, associated with the PFC. have seen is involved in ADHD, is a key modulator for reward
These levels’ interaction demonstrates how the cognitive-energetic signaling in this circuit (Sonuga-Barke, 2004).
model encompasses both “top-down” and “bottom-up” self- In light of evidence that children with ADHD also demonstrate
regulation processes. EF deficits, Sonuga–Barke incorporated Barkley’s (1997b) response
Sergeant (2005) highlights that deficits in ADHD have been inhibition hypothesis rather than current EF theories. Defined as
identified at all three levels of the model: cognitive response “higher-order, top-down, cognitive processes that allow appropriate
580 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

task set, maintenance and shift, and that facilitate the flexible pursuit this model, combined with a smaller striatal volume, could also
of future goals” (Sonuga-Barke, 2004, p. 1232), EFs generally and account for deficits in working memory (Castellanos, 2001).
response inhibition specifically are regarded in this model as critical The DDT indicates dopamine hypofunctioning in ADHD impacts
for self-control, emotional regulation, and cognitive flexibility. It is three dopaminergic networks responsible for self-regulation: the
suggested that motivational and cognitive development could be meso-limbic, the meso-cortical, and the nigrostriatal. Impact on the
impacted by the experience of challenges in effective engagement meso-limbic network could result in altered reinforcement and
with tasks and environments that require inhibition-based executive extinction behavioral processes, interfering with establishing behav-
processes. As with delay, the negative social–cultural responses to ioral stimulus control and acquisition of verbal instructions to set
task failure may lead to “executive-task aversion,” impacting intrin- rule-governed behavior. This behavior produces a shorter delay-of-
sic motivation and reducing interaction with these tasks, therefore reinforcement gradient, resulting in the delay aversion described by
limiting opportunities to develop executive skills. Neurobiologi- Sonuga-Barke (2004). Because reinforcement and extinction behav-
cally, EFs are placed within an executive circuit to include the ior acquisition processes require tonic and phasic dopamine to be
PFC, the dorsal neostriatum (caudate nucleus), the basal ganglia, successful, naturally occurring lower tonic dopamine in ADHD will
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and the dorsomedial thalamus excitatory glutaminergic cells. The require increased phasic dopamine release to achieve reinforcement.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

meso-cortical neurotransmitter dopamine is a key circuit modulator Similarly, normal tonic but reduced phasic dopamine release asso-
(Sonuga-Barke, 2004). ciated with a reinforcer will result in less efficient reinforcement.
This model argues that neither the executive circuit nor the reward This model claims this describes the “motivation” issues seen in
circuit could fully account for the behavioral self-regulation chal- ADHD, and therefore reinforcers must be stronger and more salient
lenges evidenced in ADHD. However, a head-to-head study of the to control behavior. Extinction deficits, or inability to stop ongoing
two models indicates that both are distinct, separate processes, and behavior despite lack of reinforcement, accounts for Barkley’s
can be identified in ADHD (Solanto et al., 2001). Sonuga-Barke (1997b) response inhibition in this model. Meso-cortical network
suggests that a dual-pathway model recognizes the heterogeneity disturbances result in attentional deficiencies that increase behav-
that comes with ADHD and may indicate additional ADHD sub- ioral control and direct action toward long-term goals. Deficiencies
types. More recently, a third pathway has also been suggested in the nigrostriatal network are associated with impaired motor
involving temporal processing deficits (Sonuga-Barke et al., 2010). control and habit formation through nondeclarative memory, which
may be partly responsible for Barkley’s (1997b) response inhibition.
In other words, reinforcement of behavior is only successful if the
Sagvolden’s Dynamic Developmental Theory
consequences are perceived within a limited time window. Hypo-
Similar to Barkley (1997b) and Sonuga-Barke (2004), Sagvolden functioning dopamine systems may impact this time window length
et al. (2005) also postulated that ADHD is grounded in issues with in ADHD, leading to limited stimulus control around attention,
response inhibition and delay aversion. However, this model’s preferences for short sequences of behavior leading to motor
origin is represented as hypofunctioning dopamine systems that impulsiveness, and deficient maladaptive behavioral extinction
may account for all the symptoms in the hyperactive/impulsive and leading to excessive behavior or hyperactivity. In this model,
combined ADHD subtypes. The dynamic developmental theory EFs are “untangled” into motor impulsiveness and cognitive impul-
(DDT) is based on the work with animal models in behaviorism siveness due to the slower acquisition of longer sequences of
(Skinner, 1953, 1971) and defines ADHD as arising from two faulty behavior and difficulty with the extinction of previously reinforced
behavioral processes: altered reinforcement of novel behavior and behaviors (Sagvolden et al., 2005).
deficient extinction of reinforced behavior (Sagvolden et al., 2005). The DDT hypothesizes that the interplay between behavior and
Based on the research with methylphenidate (Grace, 2001), the environment at different times of life will generate ADHD hetero-
DDT argues that issues with ADHD arise from naturally occurring geneity. For the person with ADHD, the impact of individual
abnormally low levels of tonic dopamine transmission or slow variations in dopamine functioning on learning processes and
stimulus adaptation, leading to naturally elevated phasic dopamine behavior will result in developmental delays in identifying connec-
transmission or rapid stimulus adaptation. It is suggested that phasic tions between situational or instructional demands and behavior
dopamine has two subcomponents: a fast component signaling a leading to problems anticipating appropriate situationally relevant
primary response for engagement with the expectation of reward behaviors and lack of development of self-directed speech for
before confirmation, defined as erroneous “reward prediction,” and guiding behavior. The DDT highlights that ADHD impacts individ-
an intermediate subcomponent involved with behaviors such as ual and family relationships, particularly when the parents may also
movement, reward, punishment, stress, and sex (Schultz, 2002). have ADHD. Although a non-ADHD parent may assist with
Phasic dopamine is released as a pulse or spike response. Tonic developing stimulus control in their child by providing frequent
dopamine is located in the extrasynaptic space but is low concen- and immediate reinforcers, it is unlikely to be achieved by an ADHD
tration and tightly regulated. It is too low in concentration to parent (Sagvolden et al., 2005). Cultural and social value for
stimulate receptors, and it is strong enough to exert a continuous behavioral self-control, learning to use time efficiently, and foresee
downregulation and modulation of phasic response spikes. (Grace, consequences of the behavioral impact on socializing, gaining an
2001) Tonic dopamine is thought to be regulated by frontal cortical education or gaining employment generate challenging expectations
sensory neurons to the NAccs via the DLPFC, an area attributed to for those with ADHD. In Western cultures, permissible unwanted
being responsible for working memory. The DDT suggests that behaviors in childhood are expected to extinguish by adulthood.
ADHD arises from dysregulation of tonic/phasic dopamine control, According to this model, an adult with ADHD will have acquired
causing stunted dopamine phasic responses despite low-tonic levels many maladaptive behaviors that will be difficult to change. Inter-
(Sagvolden et al., 2005). Lower levels of existing tonic dopamine in estingly, the DDT also recognizes a successful side of this
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 581

neurobiology as a positive, creative individual who directs their from an expected level of parental support or correction and
energy toward work, defined as a type A personality (Sagvolden therefore integration and self-regulation (Figures 3–6).
et al., 2005). In highlighting the reciprocal connection between emotions and
the recruitment of cognitive control, the core focus is on updating
goal states by continual contextual processing of frequency, timing,
Nigg et al.’s Multiple Pathway Model and emotional valance of information which disrupts goal-directed
behavior in ADHD. This model associates the influence of positive
Unlike other ADHD theories, the focus of the multiple pathway
emotional valance, an expectation of reward, and negative emo-
model (Nigg, 2017; Nigg, Karalunas, et al., 2020; Nigg & Casey,
tional valance, or nonexpectation of reward, with excessive
2005; Nigg et al., 2004) is less on the etiology of ADHD and more
approach motivation and withdrawal/nonapproach motivation con-
on clarifying the heterogeneity and different developmental trajec-
textual behaviors. This behavior is defined as temperament or
tories represented by comorbidities found in ADHD populations
categories of traits involving affective reactivity and regulation.
(Nigg et al., 2004). To accommodate these differences, this model
Thus, inattention–disorganization can be mapped to effortful con-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

highlights the integration of cognitive and affective neuroscience


trol, hyperactivity to activity level, and impulsivity to negative/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

through considering components of cognitive control, affect regu-


positive approach (Nigg et al., 2004).
lation, and their mutual influence on one another in self-regulation
This model suggests that there are advantages to temperament
and in development (Nigg & Casey, 2005). Maturation leads to
parallels with ADHD behaviors: temperament coding would be
neural organization and increasing hierarchical specialization and
advantageous in the early stages of development to identify pre-
differentiation in cognitive neural systems. Prediction, or learned
cursors of ADHD and illuminate risk factors or as an outcome
expectations of temporal and contextual cues, is based on the
moderator. Maturational changes in temperamental traits combined
attentional responses from infancy (Saffran et al., 1996). Cognitive
with experience also give rise to personality traits in adulthood
control is defined as behavioral regulation, or the ability to adjust
similar to maturational changes in ADHD symptoms (Nigg et al.,
behavior based on these cues and suppress inappropriate behaviors.
2004). Therefore, this model argues that temperamental tendencies
This ability arises out of four basic circuits identified in neurode-
may lead to ADHD, and ADHD may be identified as a common
velopment (Amaral, 1986):
phenotype clinical outcome of several developmental pathways
• reflexive, or “where” orienting, guided by a parietal– (Nigg & Casey, 2005; see Figure 7a). Alternatively, ADHD has
thalamic circuit; multiple identifiable pathways with slightly different phenotypic
outcomes (see Figure 7b): prediction breakdowns leading to top-
• alerting and vigilance, or “when” orienting, guided by down weakness, arousal regulation, and cognitive control, described
noradrenergic activation of right-lateralized circuitry, as “executive functioning” difficulties (Barkley, 1997b); extreme
including the cerebellar–cortical circuits; positive approach tendencies leading to problems in reinforcement
learning (Sagvolden et al., 2005) with minimal to no EF deficits;
• reinforcement learning and reward response, guided by
breakdowns in response inhibition, or cognitive control, and affec-
fronto-limbic circuitry; and
tive responding, or a poorly regulated approach system (Sonuga-
• effortful regulation and voluntary planning of behavior and Barke, 2003, 2004); and also faulty avoidance learning leading to
cognition, which involves frontostriatal circuitry (Nigg & impulsive and antisocial behavior and psychopathy (Nigg & Casey,
Casey, 2005). 2005; Nigg et al., 2004). Further work on this model has extended to
the broader field of psychopathology using network modeling
This model links reinforcement and reward and effortful regula- (Borsboom & Cramer, 2013; McNally, 2016) which assumes that
tion into a third “what” orientation based on the mutual reliance on the accumulation or “clusters” of symptoms generates a syndrome
fronto-striatal loops and dopaminergic modulation, forming affect- rather than a latent disorder or trait (Nigg, Karalunas, et al., 2020;
related neural systems that detect events’ emotional significance. Nigg, Sibley, et al., 2020).
These circuits facilitate the adjustment of action when predictions of
what is being presented are violated. It is the “when” and “what”
orientations requiring voluntary or effortful control that this model Critical Review of Theories
highlights as challenging to activate efficiently in ADHD, seen as a
failure to form accurate predictions resulting in difficulty adjusting
Issues in ADHD Neurobiology
behavior to what is presented when something unexpected happens. The cognitive–behavioral theory-based conceptualization of
The actions from these inaccurate predictions account for ADHD ADHD is prominent in advances in neuroscientific research. In
behaviors that are continued inappropriately for the context where ADHD, differences in structure and function of the brain are
the outcome/consequences are misjudged, or when action has its presented as evidence of deficits leading to hyperactivity, impulsiv-
effect mispredicted. In other words, breakdowns or disruption in ity, and inattention. These include size differences in the pallidum,
linking the known correct behavior with the current context lead to thalamus, caudate nucleus, putamen, NAccs, amygdala, and hippo-
inaccurate expectations, resulting in less top-down cognitive control campus (Hoogman, Buitelaar, et al., 2017); abnormalities within
being recruited, affecting planning, maintenance of thoughts and dopaminergic networks, specifically the meso-limbic (Posner et al.,
actions over time, and regulation of affect (Nigg & Casey, 2005). 2013; Volkow et al., 2011); differences in cortical maturation rate
Here, the model agrees with Sagvolden et al. (2005) that disruptions (Shaw et al., 2006, 2007); as well as altered activity in the PFC,
in these systems may also lead to inefficiencies in developmental blood flow to limbic areas via the striatum, and activation of
learning and socialization, interfering with the benefits received hippocampus, insula, and anterior cingulate (Barkley, 2014a).
582 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Figure 3
Brown’s EF Model
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. EF = executive function. Adapted from Brown, T. E. (2005). Attention Deficit Disorder:
The Unfocused Mind in Children and Adults. Yale University Press. Copyright 2005 by Yale
University Press. Adapted with permission. See the online article for the color version of
this figure.

Despite these findings, neuroimaging data remain correlational adults with ADHD are limited in scope due to their focus on single
(Kooij et al., 2019), and recent meta-analysis indicates no significant variable isolation. They recommend assessing the ability to perform
difference in brain structure between adults with ADHD and con- complex everyday self-managed tasks as a more appropriate way to
trols (Hoogman, Buitelaar, et al., 2017; Wolfers et al., 2019). In measure individual executive functioning. Consequently, Barkley’s
addition, there are several persistent concerns and considerations (1997b, 2012) EF model is still promoted as clinically useful for case
regarding ADHD neurobiology (Figures 8–10). formulation, treatment planning, and intervention design (Ramsay,
2016, 2020).
Issues With ADHD and EF
Altered Brain Activity in ADHD
Recent research has shown that no single EF deficit is high
enough to support executive dysfunction as the cause of all cases Although altered brain activity in ADHD has been noted in
(Boonstra et al., 2005; Castellanos et al., 2006; Nigg, Stavro, et al., neuroimaging research, it has received limited positive attention.
2005; Nigg, Willcutt, et al., 2005; Posner et al., 2020; Willcutt et al., In several studies, brain activity in ADHD engaged differently or
2005) and neuropsychologically identified EF is not associated with oppositely to controls. Selective attention tasks showed robust
deficient emotional self-regulation in ADHD (Surman et al., 2015). bilateral activity in the ventrolateral PFC (BA 45) and insular cortex
Criticism of EF weaknesses highlights that the complexity of compared with controls and unilateral activation of caudate, puta-
neurocognitive processes in executive tasks makes isolating the men, thalamus, and pulvinar, indicating potential recruitment of a
exact locus of dysfunction difficult using current EF task testing different response pathway (Bush et al., 1999). In switch tasks,
designs (Willcutt et al., 2005). However, Barkley (2012, 2014a) and adults with ADHD more strongly engaged the right middle temporal
Brown (Barkley & Brown, 2008; Brown, 2005, 2006, 2009) argue gyrus, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), precuneus, lingual
that laboratory-based neuropsychological tests of EF impairments in gyrus, and the left precentral gyrus (supplementary motor area) and
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 583

Figure 4
Sergeant’s Cognitive-Energetic Model
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. Adapted from “The Cognitive-Energetic Model: An Empirical Approach-Deficit


Hyperactivity Disorder”, by J. Sergeant, 2000, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,
24(1), pp. 7–12 with permission from Elsevier (http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(99)
00060-3). See the online article for the color version of this figure.

insula, demonstrating that those with ADHD display different inferior parietal lobe, and right caudate instead of bilateral frontal
fronto-striatal and parietal activation than controls during the per- areas of the PFC suggesting activation differences may reflect
formance of an executive control task (Dibbets et al., 2010). In differences in strategies for task performance (Fassbender &
working memory tasks, a diffuse network of regions was activated Schweitzer, 2006; K. Rubia et al., 2000). When progressive
rather than right PFC regions, including the parietal, precuneus, and time-on-task activation was examined in a working memory task,
occipital lobe, indicating that individuals used more visual strategies increases in activation in the right lenticulate, left parahippocampal
in this aurally presented task suggesting a system more reliant on gyrus, and cerebellum was shown over time, possibly indicating a
visual strategies and response to visual stimuli (Fassbender & reliance on motor regions (Schweitzer et al., 2004). Complex
Schweitzer, 2006). In research on an inhibitory paradigm and delay cognitive function tasks showed activation of the right side of
task, activations were seen in right pre and postcentral gyrii, right the basal ganglia rather than left, leading to the speculation of

Figure 5
Sonuga–Barke’s Delay Aversion/Dual Pathway Model

Note. Adapted from “The Dual Pathway Model of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (AD/HD): An Elaboration of Neuro-Developmental
Characteristics”, by E. J. S. Sonuga-Barke, 2003, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(7), pp. 593–604 with permission from Elsevier
(http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.005). See the online article for the color version of this figure.
584 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Figure 6
Sagvolden’s DDT Model
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. DDT = dynamic developmental theory. Adapted from “A Dynamic Developmental Theory of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
Predominantly Hyperactive/Impulsive and Combined Subtypes”, by T. Sagvolden, H. Aase, E. B. Johansen, & V. A. Russell, 2005, Behavioural Brain
Research, 28, pp. 397–468 (http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000075). Copyright 2005 by Cambridge University Press. Adapted with permission.
See the online article for the color version of this figure.

differences in how this region is used to perform tasks (Durston et to a broad attentional focus (Kasof, 1997; Mendelsohn, 1976;
al., 2003; Shafritz et al., 2004). Zedelius & Schooler, 2015).
It has long been recognized that the dopaminergic reward path- Investigations of stop-signal tasks and stop-signal reaction time
way is altered in ADHD. However, findings also showed a strong indicate that inattentive and hyperactive symptoms represent an
positive correlation with the motivational measure of Positive entirely different integration of distributed processing in ADHD that
Emotionality and D2/D3 receptor availability in the midbrain in recruits resources used in task-directed performance by typically
adults, indicating the importance of intrinsically interesting activi- developing (TD) individuals. It is hypothesized that this style of
ties in improving motivation (Volkow et al., 2011). processing is not amenable to the reinforcement learning models that
In resting-state connectivity analysis, connections between dorsal appropriately describe neural function in TD (Chevrier & Schachar,
(DAN) and ventral (VAN) attentional networks were less segregated 2020). Cordova et al.’s (2020) findings from the comparison of
functionally in ADHD than controls, with hyper connections with subgroups showed that biological differences exist and are not
the VAN. Increased coupling with the VAN and the salience entirely attributable to a global theme of over- or under-connected
network (SN) produces an altered saliency attribution mechanism functional networks but display a fundamentally different organi-
in ADHD. Interestingly, the connection between the DMN and the zation attributable to more complex mechanistic interactions across
SN was intact and unaffected (Sidlauskaite et al., 2016). The SN has networks.
been identified as crucial in identifying biologically and cognitively
relevant endogenous and external stimuli to adaptively guide behav- Heterogeneity
ior between large-scale brain networks, with the anterior insula Heterogeneity remains an unresolved problem, leading research-
particularly key in salient event detection signaling reward, moti- ers to infer that causes of ADHD may be distinct and also shared
vation, and affective saliency (Menon, 2015). More recently, with other conditions as well as TD (Cordova et al., 2020; Feczko
research in altered interaction between large-scale networks showed et al., 2019; Nigg, Karalunas, et al., 2020; Posner et al., 2020).
hyperactivation with the DMN during cognitive paradigms leading Heterogeneity in ADHD is demonstrated both etiologically and
to excessive spontaneous “mind wandering” in ADHD (Bozhilova phenotypically in multiple dimensions including neurobiological,
et al., 2018; Posner et al., 2015). Although excessive mind wander- developmental, psychosocial, and emotional/cognitive self-
ing can be impairing, mind wandering has been positively correlated regulation (Luo et al., 2019; Nigg, Sibley, et al., 2020). The
with creativity tasks such as the generation of novel and useful ideas polygenic liability of ADHD adds to its complexity, considering
(Carson et al., 2003; Fink et al., 2012) and better creative output due the overlap or co-occurrence with other phenotypes and psychiatric
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 585

Figure 7
The Multiple Pathway Model
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. ADHD = attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; PFC = prefrontal cortex. Part labels and images adapted from “Temperament and Attention Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder: The Development of a Multiple Pathway Model,” by J. T. Nigg, H. H. Goldsmith, & J. Sachek, 2004, Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), pp. 42–53 (http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_5). Copyright 2004 by Cambridge University Press; and “An
Integrative Theory of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Based on the Cognitive and Affective Neurosciences,” by J. T. Nigg, & B. J. Casey, 2005,
Development and Psychopathology, 17(3), pp. 785–806 (http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050376). Copyright 2005 by Cambridge University Press.
Adapted with permission. See the online article for the color version of this figure.

disorders (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics (Bjerrum et al., 2017). However, these positive aspects tend to
Consortium, 2013; The Brainstorm Consortium, 2018) and a recent be perceived as “individual strengths” which are encouraged in
Genome-Wide Associaton Study that has identified 12 independent treatment, rather than unifying positive features of ADHD (Champ
significant associated genetic loci (Demontis et al., 2019). However, et al., 2021).
affect sizes are small for each individual gene despite multiple gene
involvement (Faraone & Larsson, 2019; Luo et al., 2019; Nigg, The ADHD Paradox or Context Variability
Sibley, et al., 2020). Although heterogeneity is acknowledged in the Research has also identified that while there is a persistence of
DSM-5 by specifying three presentations: mostly inattentive, mostly core symptoms in ADHD, there is significant variability in context
hyperactive, and combined (APA, 2013), some theories account for (Brown, 1995, 2014; Hirvikoski et al., 2011). Individuals with
all these presentations (Brown, 2013; Nigg, Karalunas, et al., 2020; ADHD demonstrate consistently good, sustained attention on tasks
Sergeant, 2005; Sonuga-Barke, 2003), whereas others characterize which are engaging or “interesting” but symptoms increase in
them as separate conditions (Barkley, 2014c; Sagvolden et al., situations synonymous with low stimulation or boring activities,
2005). Conceptualizations of ADHD currently recognize the often resulting in delay aversion and inattention (Antrop et al., 2000;
dimensionality of the disorder, yet there is very little research on Boonstra et al., 2005; Hoza et al., 2001; Johansen et al., 2002;
positive attributes such as hyperfocus, creativity, divergent thinking, Roberts et al., 2014; Wender, 1995; Zentall et al., 1985). However,
curiosity, courage, transcendence, and resilience (Boot, 2017; Boot characterizations of ADHD often define this behavior as “stimulus
et al., 2020; Redshaw & McCormack, 2022; Sedgwick et al., 2018; seeking” and tend to focus on the inability to engage with non-
H. A. White & Shah, 2006, 2011, 2016). Phenomenological quali- stimulating tasks. Research also shows a correlation in ADHD with
tative studies of the lived experience of ADHD indicate that interest a need for autonomy, defined in this context as a freedom of choice
is associated with meaning and a sense of “flow” (Csikszentmihalyi, (Dimic & Orlov, 2014; Houghton, 2006; Partridge & Williams,
1975), mainly when working with or assisting others (Ek & 2008; Young, 2000). Studies show that those with ADHD per-
Isaksson, 2013); that aspects such as persistence and perfectionism formed better in working environments where autonomy was
can be identified as undervalued resources, and that those with supported, sometimes highlighted as entrepreneurship (Boot,
ADHD strive to find a healthy balance and develop strategies 2017; Toner et al., 2006). Young and Bramham (2012) specifically
586 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Figure 8
The Taxonomy of SDT Regulatory Styles
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. SDT = self-determination theory. Adapted from “Approaching and Avoiding Self-Determination: Comparing Cybernetic and Organismic Paradigms of
Motivation,” by R. M. Ryan, & E. L. Deci, in R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Perspectives on Behavioural Self-Regulation (pp. 193–215), 1999, Routledge. Copyright
1999 by Taylor & Frances Group. Adapted with permission. See the online article for the color version of this figure.

stress that for those with ADHD to succeed, they must define their requires an additional cognitive cost, as described in “effortful
structures and boundaries. There is little research into the motiva- coping” (K. Ochsner et al., 2002), implying a high demand of
tional influences of interest and autonomy in ADHD other than as a performance for those with ADHD considering effort avoidance
reward. (Roberts et al., 2014) and an inability to allocate sufficient cognitive
effort are both considered core characterizations of ADHD
(Sergeant, 2000). Reappraisal also is shown to be less accessible
Theoretical Influence on Current Treatment Approaches as a resource when emotional intensity is high (Gross, 2013),
Traditionally, the primary goal of research on self-regulation in and intensity of emotional dysregulation is identified as a core
ADHD is the management and reduction of the experience of component of impairment in ADHD (Barkley, 2014a; Corbisiero
negative emotions, which are presented as problematic, destabiliz- et al., 2013).
ing, and leading to dysregulation and overwhelm. Therefore, the
focus of the nonpharmacological interventions has been to improve
SDT: A New Perspective
self-regulation by the cognitive reframing of maladaptive schema,
habituation of behavioral management skills via continuous rein- Extensive research in multiple areas of human functioning has led
forcement, and development of cognitive regulation skills in atten- to the understanding that ADHD presents motivation, engagement,
tion shifting and positive reappraisal (Philipsen et al., 2010; and self-regulation issues. Understanding interest and its role as a
Ramsay, 2020; Ramsay & Rostain, 2015; Safren et al., 2005; motivational factor in ADHD is key to gaining a new perspective on
Solanto, 2010; Tuckman, 2009; Young & Bramham, 2012; ADHD behavior. However, motivational research has highlighted
Zylowska et al., 2008). The reappraisal goal is to identify negative interest as a central affective marker of intrinsic motivation (Ryan &
responses early in the emotional experience and consciously alter Deci, 2017). SDT presents intrinsic motivation as the spontaneous
them to positive perspectives (Gross, 2013). Although this skill is tendency “to seek out novelty and challenges, extend and exercise
helpful in some contexts, such as managing an immediate crisis or one’s capacity, explore, and learn” (Ryan & Deci, 2000, p. 70). SDT
when another’s safety or emotional stability is of primary concern, is an empirical organismic approach to personality development that
such as a child, the process of reappraisal involves avoidance of the investigates people’s inherent growth tendencies as a basis for self-
experience of emotion in preference to minimizing negative affect motivation and personality (Ryan & Deci, 2000). At the core of SDT
(Wolgast et al., 2013). However, being cognitively led, reappraisal is the assumption that human development includes an innate, active
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 587

Figure 9
ADHD Altered Neural Processing Model
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. ADHD = attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; EF = executive function; DMN = default-mode network; SN = salience network. See
the online article for the color version of this figure.

tendency toward integration of structures, functions, and experi- and structurally to organize integration and regulate actions to
ences resulting in greater effectiveness, organization, and unity in integrate new functions, values, narratives, preferences, and regula-
functioning (Ryan, 1993; Ryan & Deci, 2017). Defined as organis- tions (Ryan & Deci, 2017). Thus, self-regulation in SDT is synon-
mic integration, SDT describes organization as both a fundamental ymous with autonomy and “disturbances in autonomy” are equated
biological principle and a perspective reflected in multiple psycho- to self-regulation disturbances (Ryan et al., 1997).
logical traditions such as Piaget’s (1971, 1981) ideas of organization Traditionally, the exploration of issues with healthy self-
expressed as propensities toward assimilation; Freud’s (1923) con- regulation focuses on the control and management of behavior.
cept of synthetic function of the ego; White’s (1959, 1963) descrip- Instead, SDTs organismic approach orients questions about moti-
tion of inherent independent ego energy manifesting in intrinsically vation and behavior toward what supports human functioning and
motivated activities; and Rogers (1963) primacy for the tendency flourishing. According to the SDT, the quality of an individual’s
toward actualization as a basic motivational tendency underlying motivation and vitality depends on what resources they have been
behaviors. able to draw from their exchanges with their environment to
maintain, support, and enhance their existence or satisfy their needs.
While it is clear that organisms have biological deficit needs (Hull,
SDT and Self-Regulation
1943) that, when satisfied, will return an organism to equilibrium,
SDT defines the self as a synthetic function of the psyche, SDT postulates that there are also basic psychological needs that do
reflecting this inherent tendency toward organization and integration not require deficiencies to motivate action. When satisfied within
as an active process. Unlike social-cognitive approaches which cultural, interpersonal, and developmental contexts, basic psycho-
define self as an object perceived by oneself or another (self- logical needs facilitate growth, integrity, and well-being and, when
concept), supported by mechanisms for governing action (self- frustrated, generate psychological harms (Ryan & Deci, 2017). SDT
schemas), SDT cites McAdams (1990, 1996, 2013) view of the identifies three innate basic psychological needs—autonomy, com-
“self-as-subject” phenomenally experienced both as a center of petence and connection, or relatedness—which, when satisfied,
experience and the initiator and regulator of volitional behavior allow optimal function and growth. As previously described, auton-
(Ryan & Deci, 2017). This “self-as-process,” or autonomous func- omy is the feeling of willingness and volition with respect to
tioning, is defined in SDT as having the freedom to choose actions behaviors. Competence is defined as feeling effective with interac-
that align with the interests, values, and a sense of meaning tion in social environments or, more precisely, experiencing support
originating in the internal self-concept. Freedom, in this instance, and opportunities for the exercise, expansion, and expression of
is not defined as independence from external influences, but a higher capacities and talents. Relatedness is defined as feeling connected
order coordination of component systems into a relative unity and and having a sense of belonging, referring to both experiencing
acting from that organized vantage point (Ryan et al., 1997). others as responsive, and sensitive (Ryan & Deci, 2017). SDT
Autonomy, meaning “self-governing,” is considered central to suggests that people can become self-determined, make choices, and
healthy development as it functions both phenomenologically manage their lives independently when their needs for competence,
588 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Figure 10
ADHD Self-Regulation and Task Engagement
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. ADHD = attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. See the online article for the color version of this figure.

relatedness, and autonomy are fulfilled (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Acting sense of gaining competence, or “flow” state (Csikszentmihalyi,
volitionally to achieve goals and satisfy needs experienced as 1975), is seen as a process of continually seeking and reducing
intentional and thus personally caused is the basis of self-determi- information gaps in knowledge or problem-solving (Di Domenico &
nation. SDT defines self-determination from the phenomenological Ryan, 2017). Motivation to engage and regulate activity is height-
tradition as a willed action caused “as an initial act of the ego-center ened in some domains, forestalled, or conflicted in others, and some
itself” (Pfander, 1967, p. 20) differentiated from acts caused by opportunities will amplify or diminish interest, meaning SDT
external agents. However, this autonomous action could occur even recognizes both social contextual and within-person variations in
when experiencing external pressures if one agrees to act in that way regulatory functioning (Ryan & Deci, 2017).
(Ricoeur, 1966). In light of these potential variations and in contrast to other
As an organismic approach, SDT is concerned with both goal models of self-regulation, SDT recognizes three regulatory styles
achievement and the nature and motives of goal adoption. SDT that are characteristic of human functioning: relatively autonomous
highlights that there are process differences for goals that have and integrated modes, relatively controlled and introjected (nonau-
different origins. The satisfaction of the basic psychological needs tonomous) modes, and absence of regulation (and motivation) (Deci
will be consistent with the achievement of some goals but not others, & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2017). Integrative regulation repre-
and thus “all goals are not created equal” (Ryan et al., 1996). sents natural human development to actively internalize and assimi-
Critically, SDT cites Heider’s (1958) and de Charms’ (1968) work late social norms and regulations for healthy functioning.
on the psychological construct of perceived locus of causality and its Independent reproduction of socially and culturally accepted assim-
impact on motivation: the degree to which autonomy is experienced ilated behaviors, attitudes, and values reflect the process in which
as a salient sense of personal causation determines the intrinsic extrinsic behaviors become a part of an aspect of an individual’s
nature of the action. Individuals who are intrinsically motivated mind and motives. Research indicates that three facilitating factors
engage with activities because they are interested in and find them lead to internalization: a meaningful rationale, so individuals find
inherently satisfying. Exploratory “seeking” behaviors, experienced value in the activity; acknowledging potential negative feelings; and
as curiosity, interest, sensation seeking, and search for meaning, highlighting choice. A key conclusion from this research was the
produce a sense of immediate positive feedback on progress when a importance of the relationship between the subject’s behavior and
challenging task at an achievable or optimum level is mastered. This feelings: the more positive the relationship, the more integrated the
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 589

regulation (Deci et al., 1994). SDT views healthy emotion regulation satisfaction is what makes the experience of engagement with a task
as including rich access to positive and negative emotions as enjoyable or interesting. The anterior insula generates these intrinsic
informational inputs that assist in both the choice and self-guidance satisfactions, and the extent to which an activity is considered
of actions. Taking an active interest in emotional responses, ex- interesting is represented by activity in the anterior insular cortex,
pressing them, and using them as a built-in feedback system to making it the key structure in intrinsic motivation (Reeve & Lee,
provide physiological, cognitive, and motivational signals that 2019b). Engagement with an environmental activity that allows a
inform behaviors and goals to satisfy basic psychological needs person to feel volition, effectance, and connection also produces
is a key part of integrated emotional regulation (Roth et al., 2019). need-satisfying spontaneous satisfactions, showing that basic psy-
This system supports an individual to exercise potential, find chological need satisfaction is indicated by anterior insular and
meaning, connect with others, and express vitality—a eudaimonic striatum activations (Lee & Reeve, 2017; Reeve & Lee, 2019a).
view of wellness (Ryan et al., 2006). Studies in dopaminergic function show that task-related feedback is
In addition, SDT outlines that rewards, contingencies, and pres- registered even in the absence of external rewards (Tricomi &
sures attached to specific outcomes can be experienced as control- DePasque, 2016) and that salience-related dopaminergic activity
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ling rather than based on the individual’s autonomous choice. The drives curiosity and desire for information (DeYoung, 2013).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

opposite of autonomy in SDT is not dependence but heteronomy, Therefore, SDT proposes that intrinsic motivation is associated
defined as controlled by outside forces (Ryan & Deci, 2017). with activity in the dopaminergic value system (Di Domenico &
Behaviors that are introjected or externally motivated to establish Ryan, 2017). The SN, involves the anterior insula, dACC, and
internal control over the self are simply responses to the environ- subcortical nodes in the amygdala, NAccs, substantia nigra, and
ment rather than internalized and establish an expectancy (implicit ventral tegmental area (Menon, 2015). The SN is believed to receive
or explicit) that the contingency is in effect. When introjected, motivationally significant dopaminergic information from several of
behaviors are experienced as an internal, controlling force—a sense its main structures and subcortical nodes, which is integrated with
that one “should” or “must” do the behavior. Therefore, the beha- sensory input from the environment and viscera for “bottom-up”
viors will not be sustained over time because they are dependent on detection of contextually relevant events in self-regulation. It is
the maintenance of these external or internal pressures and contin- suggested that a function of the SN is to act as a dynamic hub to
gencies for reliable occurrence. Controlling regulation results in modulate activity between two other large-scale brain networks that
unstable self-esteem (Kernis & Paradise, 2002; Ryan & Brown, support cognitive functions. The first is the DMN, which has major
2013) and suppression of negative emotions impairing the ability to nodes in the medial PFC and posterior cingulate cortex and is active
disclose, listen to, or deal with them effectively (Roth et al., 2014). during passive resting states and involved in internally focused self-
Absence of regulation, or amotivation, is defined as a state where referential cognition, spontaneous cognition, and mind wandering
one is not motivated to behave or behaves in a way not mediated by (Buckner et al., 2008). The DMN is also associated with introspec-
intentionality. Amotivation potentially results from two sources: tive attentional orientation related to mentalizing and emotional
lack of perceived competence, or believing that acting will either not processing (Gusnard et al., 2001) and the maintenance of a sense of
achieve the desired outcome or that the behavior cannot be per- self (Gusnard, 2005; Sonuga-Barke & Castellanos, 2007). The
formed, or perceiving a lack of value or interest in the behavior. The second is the central executive network (CEN) which includes
first is recognized in social–cognitive theory (Bandura, 1996), but the DLPFC and the posterior parietal cortex and is active during
the second is also recognized in SDT as an absence of interest and cognitively demanding, externally focused tasks and involves work-
utility, leading to a lack of motivation to act (Ryan & Deci, 2017). ing memory and EFs. Neuroimaging studies support the SDT theory
Emotionally, amotivation can be experienced as dysregulation, that activities involving intrinsic motivation recruit the salience and
where individuals’ daily functioning is impacted by their inability CENs, while suppressing the DMN, specifically activity within the
to manage negative emotions and consequently feel overwhelmed midbrain, anterior cingulate cortex, and bilateral insula in response
and therefore engagement with others and the environment is to free-choice (autonomy) cues (Di Domenico & Ryan, 2017).
impaired (Roth et al., 2014). SDT highlights that intrinsic motiva-
tion only occurs when individuals experience self-determined
action, perceived competence, and relational responsiveness and SDT Etiology of ADHD
security, or relatedness. Therefore, autonomy is “fragile,” and must
SDT and ADHD
be supported for optimal development and expression (Ryan et al.,
1997). Not all goals will result in positive consequences for the Applying the organismic approach of SDT to our current under-
individual, and individuals may discontinue practice once pressure standing of ADHD clinical presentation, neurobiological research,
or controlling elements are removed (Ryan & Deci, 2008). There- and treatment design and outcomes has the potential to offer a new
fore, autonomy-supportive environments are critical to integrated conceptualization and approach. Recognizing that those with
self-development (Ryan et al., 2016). ADHD actively seek to improve engagement with their environment
to achieve integration and self-actualization fundamentally alters the
ADHD discourse. This perspective also highlights the importance of
SDT and Neuroscience
understanding the task or activity targeted for engagement and its
Recent research in motivational neuroscience has identified brain relationship with both ADHD neurocognitive abilities and the
structures and neural pathways, establishing a neural basis for individual’s regulatory state. Seen from an SDT perspective,
motivational states. Neuroscience literature highlights that intrinsic ADHD etiology could be described as neurobiologically altered
motivation arises from spontaneous satisfaction one experiences approaches to processing and task engagement, supported by struc-
while engaged in a task or intrinsic rewards, and this sense of task tures that have been primarily associated with salient event detection
590 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

and affective functioning. The following presents a positive etiology potential positive outcome of ADHD particularly in the dimension
of ADHD within a practical framework for treatment design and of idea generation (Cardello & George, 2021; Girard-Joyal &
outcomes. Gauthier, 2021; Steele et al., 2021), although findings are inconsis-
tent and more research is needed (Boot, 2017; Hoogman et al., 2020;
Sedgwick et al., 2018; Taylor et al., 2018; White, 2018; H. A. White
Altered Neural Processing
& Shah, 2006, 2016). ADHD individuals would also be strongly
Research has demonstrated that there are neurobiological differ- intrinsically motivated by an interest in seeking environmental
ences in development, function, and information processing in engagement and positive feedback from problem-solving (Di
ADHD. A prolonged developmental trajectory, enhanced connec- Domenico & Ryan, 2017; Sagvolden et al., 2005). Task approaches
tivity, and clustering of regions associated with emotional proces- that are inherently interesting and therefore increase dopaminergic
sing and smaller volumes in areas related to cognitive control activity will generate positive affective feedback—they will feel
networks can lead to less efficient “top-down” management and easy or more engaging, and positive experiences of competence can
dominant cognitive self-regulation. However, recent research sup- lead to a sense of “flow” or hyperfocus (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Di
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ports an entirely different integration of distributed processing with Domenico & Ryan, 2017; Ek & Isaksson, 2013; Sedgwick et al.,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

different neural representations and subjective experience of task- 2018). However, task approaches requiring dopaminergically sup-
related stimuli in ADHD (Chevrier & Schachar, 2020). ported suppression of the DMN and efficient activation of the CEN
Altered connectivity between brain networks has been indicated will be more challenging for those with ADHD. From an SDT
in ADHD, specifically altered connectivity among the CEN, SN, perspective, an individual with ADHD will feel less competent at
and DMN (Castellanos & Proal, 2012; Cortese et al., 2012; Gao et engaging with these task approaches, as they will require more effort
al., 2019). Research indicates that hypofunctioning dopaminergic to engage and potentially maintain engagement.
systems resulting in low-tonic/high-phasic dopamine transmission As we have seen, curiosity and interest are concurrent with
in ADHD may increase exploratory-seeking behaviors (Di dopaminergic release and are key factors in intrinsic motivation
Domenico & Ryan, 2017; Sagvolden et al., 2005). The altered (DeYoung, 2013; Gruber et al., 2014; Ryan & Deci, 2017).
dopaminergic activity also results in semisuppression of the DMN Therefore, if the challenge is too difficult to feel competent, lacks
during cognitive tasks (Fassbender et al., 2009; Tomasi et al., 2009) clarity, or the interest is too low, a natural reaction would be to
and hyperconnectivity between the SN and the DMN (Hart et al., not engage with these challenging tasks but to initiate seeking
2012, 2013; Sidlauskaite et al., 2016; Viering et al., 2021). Increased behaviors to facilitate engagement with tasks or activities that
connectivity with the SN demonstrates a susceptibility to higher feel more achievable or interesting. In controlled environments
salient stimuli (Götting et al., 2017; Sidlauskaite et al., 2016) and where an individual is unable to autonomously pursue activities
reduced connectivity between the orbitofrontal cortex and ventral of interest, this engagement-seeking behavior could appear as
striatum results in less cognitive regulatory control (Posner et al., the hallmark ADHD indicators of hyperactivity, impulsivity,
2013). A high level of sensitivity to affective stimuli and weaker and inattention.
cognitive functionality would indicate that self-regulation is more
affect dominant than cognitive dominant within the bidirectional
Psychopathology
relationship. This sensitivity may also influence the differences in
temporal processing associated with ADHD, as research into models SDT states that psychopathology arises from autonomy depriving
of time processing or the “internal clock” highlight the dependence and thwarting environments (Ryan et al., 2016). Evidence indicates
of time awareness on attentional executive control processes (Droit- that adults with ADHD have experienced a lifelong history of need
Volet et al., 2018) and the premotor cortex (Droit-Volet, Monceau, frustration (Oram et al., 2020). They find low autonomy environments
et al., 2020), the influence of dopaminergic activation of cortico- that are highly structured or controlled challenging, and will experi-
striatal circuits including attention and working memory (Toplak ence boredom, are made redundant or change jobs frequently (Boot et
et al., 2006) and the effect of emotion and arousal on the magnitude al., 2020; Dimic & Orlov, 2014; Houghton, 2006). Inconsistencies in
of time distortions (Droit-Volet, 2018; Droit-Volet, El-Azhari, personal management, organizational skills, and underachievement
et al., 2020; Droit-Volet et al., 2018; Vasile, 2015). Research provide negative evidence for competence or efficacy in achieving
also shows that individuals with ADHD experience cognitive goals for personal growth (Bokor & Anderson, 2014; Goodman,
domain-dependent neuro-functional impairments in different neural 2007; Mitchell et al., 2013; Newark & Stieglitz, 2010). Rejection,
networks depending on the cognitive context (Dibbets et al., 2010; lack of acceptance, or understanding of differences in behavior creates
Hart et al., 2012). a significant impact on interpersonal skills and social connections or
relatedness (Houghton, 2006; Toner et al., 2006).
Developmentally, and depending on the social–cultural context,
Motivation and Engagement
an individual with ADHD will encounter more and more task
This neurobiological foundation impacts the effectiveness of the approaches that require DMN suppression and CEN activation,
task approach and engagement in multiple dimensions. Distortions in other words, those that require independent management, cogni-
in estimating external time longitudinally combined with suscepti- tive control, and behavioral inhibition leading to an increase in
bility to higher salient stimuli would lead to a wider and more symptomatic behaviors and coping mechanisms (Fleming &
immediate attentional awareness resulting in an overinclusive pro- McMahon, 2012; Fortes et al., 2014; Knouse & Fleming, 2016;
cessing style (Acar & Runco, 2012; Eysenck, 1993, 1994; White, Nigg, Sibley, et al., 2020; Ramsay & Rostain, 2008b; Wender,
2018). This style would suggest a tendency for chaotic cognitions 1995). Lack of understanding of the altered processing in ADHD
and divergent thinking, an aspect of creative cognition shown to be a results in inefficient learning (Chevrier & Schachar, 2020) and
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 591

dominance of standardized reinforcement models to develop skills, significantly affect treatment design and outcomes. Treatment
habits, and routine behaviors are experienced as controlling or design would include assessing an individual’s regulatory style,
amotivating. Therefore, behaviors often become introjected or contextualizing task-based need frustration, and clarifying where
remain extrinsically motivated by social or environmental domains supportive behaviors need to become internalized. Identification of
and will cease once circumstances alter or change (Roth et al., 2019; variations in regulatory functioning could highlight keys for
Ryan & Deci, 2008). individual challenges and context-specific treatment application.
Occasional successful strategy design and application is often Therapist characterization of ADHD as an alternative or neurodi-
followed by turmoil (Toner et al., 2006) as lack of awareness of verse processing style requiring an unconventional skill set could
neurocognitive and psychological needs leads to an incoherent support the individual development of self-acceptance and self-
understanding of the building blocks for success. Many individuals esteem by recognizing biological and individual strengths and
will describe experiences of constant stress or crisis, high anxiety, or resources.
a desire to procrastinate or be “under pressure” to complete tasks Psychoeducation on the importance of the relationship among
(Nadeau, 2005; Ramsay et al., 2016; Safren et al., 2004; Wolf et al., natural-seeking tendencies, interest and motivation, and the impact
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2009). This framework suggests that through salient affect-based of engagement of the stress cycle as a motivational tool would be
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

responses to either the task or the approach or both depending on key to increasing self-awareness. Developing skills to interpret
context (Posner et al., 2020), the ADHD individual is aware of a lack information from emotional responses as feedback to inform beha-
of interest or challenges with engagement. The internal conflict viors would facilitate awareness of the intention and need satisfac-
between natural-seeking behaviors, the need to complete the task tion behind these behaviors concerning the context, increasing a
and lack of skills or understanding to support achievement generates sense of choice and development of autonomous self-regulation.
a stress response. Exposure to psychosocial stress is associated with Acknowledging the reduced sensitivity to longer time durations
dopaminergic output (Bloomfield et al., 2019; Payer et al., 2017), would generate exploration of tools and methods for supportive time
providing the additional dopaminergic “push” needed for the management. Understanding the tendency toward natural-seeking
ADHD individual to suppress the DMN and activate the CEN to behaviors and the importance of positive feedback from problem-
utilize the reinforcement model-based skills to engage with the task. solving would influence goal selection, strategy development, and
With a lack of access to alternative strategies, and because stress outcome measures for achievement.
generates a dopaminergic response leading to intermittent success,
over time, the individual with ADHD becomes unconsciously
dependent on anxiety (“flight”) or frustration and aggression Summary and Future Work
(“fight”) as a motivational coping strategy. Chronic exposure
also leads to compensatory downregulation (Bloomfield et al., Theoretical perspectives strongly influence the etiology of symp-
2019; Payer et al., 2017), therefore active use of the stress cycle toms, research focus, and treatment guidance in ADHD. This
as a motivational strategy generates long-term comorbid anxiety and influence extends beyond academia and mental health decision-
depression, which is often the first presentation clinicians will see in making and impacts public perception and the development of the
undiagnosed ADHD (Bolea-Alamañac et al., 2014; Kooij et al., individual’s self-concept and identity. The dominant theoretical
2019; Lackschewitz et al., 2008; Nigg, Sibley, et al., 2020; Solanto paradigm of ADHD provided by cognitive–behavioral theory pre-
et al., 2008). sents an etiology limited to a deficit-focused, motivationally
The impacts of the altered neural processing on embodied time hedonistic perspective that generates treatment outcomes focused
(Droit-Volet, Monceau, et al., 2020) also have a developmentally on symptom reduction and emotional and behavioral control.
negative impact on perceived competence. More affect-dominant Alternative theoretical approaches are needed that incorporate
self-regulation combined with an altered perception of embodied conceptualizations with outcomes aiming to strengthen positive
time may result in reduced sensitivity to longer durations but a psychological factors and support growth and well-being.
higher response to shorter durations, as the experience of time We have introduced a theoretical framework for ADHD etiology
fluctuates according to arousal and affect (Droit-Volet et al., grounded in SDT to provide an alternative view of the research data
2013). Many socially presented skills, habits, and planning pro- and a positive approach to well-being and treatment outcomes. The
cesses are predicated on spontaneous time awareness that evaluates main propositions from this framework are as follows:
long durations, which rely on efficient cognitive processing and
working memory (Droit-Volet, 2018; Droit-Volet et al., 2013). 1. That all humans have an inherent growth tendency and
Inefficient cognitive processing in ADHD makes this resource thus have a natural inclination to seek engagement with
unreliable, leading to over- or under-estimation of embodied time their environment to satisfy basic psychological needs
and therefore interfere with cognitive-dominant strategies and skill for autonomy, a sense of competence, and belonging or
use, impacting time management, planning, prioritization, and relatedness.
estimation (Barkley, 2006; Brown, 2013; Roberts et al., 2014). 2. ADHD neurocognitive differences in hypofunctioning
dopaminergic systems, efficiency of cognitive processes,
Treatment Design and Outcomes and susceptibility to higher salient and affective stimuli
may result in:
Research has shown that reinforcement models may be inap-
propriate for learning in ADHD (Chevrier & Schachar, 2020). a. seeking behaviors designed to facilitate environmental
Therefore, recognizing the impact of altered neural processing and engagement and interest, interpreted in some contexts
using an SDT approach to the etiology of ADHD would as hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention,
592 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

b. self-regulation processes that are more affect domi- attempts to satisfy basic psychological needs, act via intrinsic
nant than cognitive dominant in the bidirectional motivation, and engage in self-regulation requires further
relationship, and research.

c. less efficient cognitive control generating under- or • Shifting the focus away from deficit-based models of
over-estimations in embodied time, creating distortions functioning alters the perspective on heterogeneity, provid-
in longitudinal time awareness and a higher sensitivity ing opportunities to research ADHD etiology and behaviors
to short durations, and therefore a wider and more in relation to other presentations within a neurodiversity
immediate attention (or overinclusive) processing style. paradigm and investigate the potential benefits of ADHD-
altered neuro-processing.
3. These differences are observable in task engagement and
motivation as: • If goal design, progress, and achievement utilize basic
psychological need satisfaction as outcomes, then pro-
a. a tendency toward chaotic and also creative cogni- cesses for goal achievement, particularly long-term goals,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tions, including the potential for divergent thinking


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

can accommodate ADHD neurobiological time distortion.


and idea generation,
Using this SDT-based framework as a foundation, our following
b. active participation in task approaches that are ori- projects focus on understanding potential positive aspects of
ented toward problem-solving, are intrinsically inter- ADHD, and a pilot of a treatment approach. Recent research high-
esting, and provide positive and more immediate lights the critical nature of lived experience accounts, particularly
feedback, and in neurodivergent populations (Grant & Kara, 2021; Milton &
Bracher, 2013; Rosqvist et al., 2020b; Stenning & Rosqvist,
c. negative outcomes if a task approach requires more
2021). Examining ADHD lived experience through the lens of
cognitive effort than feels achievable and autonomous
SDT may provide new insights into the positives offered by
approach design is unsupported, generating feelings
ADHD. Following analysis of this research, a treatment approach
of lack of competence.
is designed to include psychoeducation in ADHD-altered neuro-
4. Psychopathology arising from a lifetime of need frustra- processing, understanding the relationship between ADHD neuro-
tion, experiences of social rejection and lack of under- biology and environmental engagement, and skills development in
standing due to stigma, and environmental and social mindful self-awareness, task identification, and goal design and
demands to prioritize task approach processes based on development. Treatment outcomes would include symptom reduc-
the reinforcement models of learning. Attempts to engage tion and quality of life and critically also include measures for
a consistently high level of cognitive effort generate a autonomy and self-determination.
dependency on stress responses (“fight or flight”) based on Working with a trained therapist in an autonomy-supportive
internal conflict to provide a dopaminergic “push” to manner, we suggest that individuals with ADHD could better
activate this cognitively dominant skillset, often with understand how they function, differentiate between biological
inconsistent results further reinforcing a negative self- needs and individual needs, and develop an alternative skillset to
concept and identity. improve self-regulation, integrate experiences, and act volitionally
in ways that feel more self-determined resulting in an integrated
We propose that viewing ADHD behaviors through an SDT lens positive ADHD identity and self-concept.
provides an opportunity to shift the focus in research, diagnosis, and
treatment outcomes from deficit identification and symptom control References
to neurodivergent difference identification and integration. This
theoretical shift addresses the challenges in ADHD neurobiological Acar, S., & Runco, M. A. (2012). Psychoticism and creativity: A meta-
research, heterogeneity, and context variability in the following ways: analytic review. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4),
341–350. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027497
• If the motivational drive behind ADHD symptomatic Ahrendts, J., Rüsch, N., Wilke, M., Philipsen, A., Eickhoff, S. B., Glauche,
behaviors is an organismic need for engagement and V., Perlov, E., Ebert, D., Hennig, J., & Tebartz Van Elst, L. (2011). Visual
growth, then an awareness and understanding of the cortex abnormalities in adults with ADHD: A structural MRI study. World
Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 12(4), 260–270. https://doi.org/10.3109/
origin, or “why,” of the behavior is critical to identifying
15622975.2010.518624
the need, and challenges with context variability are Albajara Sáenz, A., Villemonteix, T., & Massat, I. (2019). Structural and
reframed as issues with engagement. functional neuroimaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Devel-
opmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 61(4), 399–405. https://doi.org/
• If self-regulation is more affect dominant than cognitive
10.1111/dmcn.14050
dominant in ADHD, then developing skills to increase Almeida, L. G., Ricardo-Garcell, J., Prado, H., Barajas, L., Fernández-
awareness and understanding of emotional responses as Bouzas, A., Ávila, D., & Martínez, R. B. (2010). Reduced right frontal
informational in relation to behavior is key to ADHD self- cortical thickness in children, adolescents and adults with ADHD and its
awareness, self-monitoring, and self-regulation. correlation to clinical variables: A cross-sectional study. Journal of
Psychiatric Research, 44(16), 1214–1223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
• If psychopathology arises not from neurobiological deficits, .jpsychires.2010.04.026
but altered neuro-processing combined with need frustration, Almeida Montes, L. G., Ricardo-Garcell, J., De la Torre, L. B., Prado
then clarity of ADHD-altered neuro-functioning process Alcántara, H., Martínez García, R. B., Fernández-Bouzas, A., & Ávila
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 593

Acosta, D. (2010). Clinical correlations of grey matter reductions in the emerging trends in attentional and behavioural disorders in childhood (pp.
caudate nucleus of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. 203–234). Pergamon Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-036508-4
Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 35(4), 238–246. https://doi.org/10 .50017-5
.1503/jpn.090099 Barkley, R. A. (1997a). ADHD and the nature of self control.
Amaral, D. G. (1986). Amygdalohippocampal and amygdalocortical projec- Guildford Press.
tions in the primate brain. In R. Schwarcz & Y. Ben-Ari (Eds.), Advances Barkley, R. A. (1997b). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and
in experimental medicine and biology (203rd ed., pp. 3–17). Springer. executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. American
Ambrosino, S., De Zeeuw, P., Wierenga, L. M., Van Dijk, S., & Durston, S. Psychological Association, 121(1), 65–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
(2017). What can cortical development in attention-deficit/hyperactivity 2909.121.1.65
disorder teach us about the early developmental mechanisms involved? Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Scientific
Cerebral Cortex, 27(9), 4624–4634. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx182 American, 279, 66–71. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0998-66
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). The diagnostic and statistical Barkley, R. A. (2002). Major life activity and health outcomes associated
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The Journal of Clinical
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). The diagnostic and statistical Psychiatry, 63(12), 10–15. https://www.psychiatrist.com/jcp/neurodeve
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). lopmental/adhd/major-life-activity-health-outcomes-associated-attention/


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). The diagnostic and statistical Barkley, R. A. (2005). Taking charge of ADHD. Guilford Press.
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Barkley, R. A. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook
Amico, F., Stauber, J., Koutsouleris, N., & Frodl, T. (2011). Anterior for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.). Guilford Press.
cingulate cortex gray matter abnormalities in adults with attention deficit Barkley, R. A. (2008). ADHD in adults: What the science says.
hyperactivity disorder: A voxel-based morphometry study. Psychiatry Guilford Press.
Research: Neuroimaging, 191(1), 31–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j Barkley, R. A. (2011). Barkley deficits in executive functioning scale.
.pscychresns.2010.08.011 Guilford Press.
Antrop, I., Roeyers, H., Van Oost, P., & Buysse, A. (2000). Stimulant Barkley, R. A. (2012). Executive functions: What they are, how they work,
seeking and hyperactivity in children with ADHD. Journal of Child and why they evolved. Guildford Press.
Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 41(2), 225–231. Barkley, R. A. (2014a). Emotional dysregulation is a core component of
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021963099005302 ADHD. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A
Anttila, V., Bulik-Sullivan, B., Finucane, H. K., Walters, R. K., Bras, J., handbook for diagnosis and treatment (4th ed., pp. 81–115). Guilford Press.
Duncan, L., Escott-Price, V., Falcone, G. J., Gormley, P., Malik, R., Barkley, R. A. (2014b). Executive functioning and self regulation viewed
Patsopoulos, N. A., Ripke, S., Wei, Z., Yu, D., Lee, P. H., Turley, P., as an extended phenotype: Implications of the theory of ADHD and
Grenier-Boley, B., Chouraki, V., Kamatani, Y., … The Brainstorm its treatment. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention deficit hyperactivity
Consortium. (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 405–434).
of the brain. Science, 360(6395), Article eaap8757. https://doi.org/10 Guilford Press.
.1126/science.aap8757 Barkley, R. A. (2014c). History of ADHD. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention
Aoki, Y., Cortese, S., & Castellanos, F. X. (2018). Research review: deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment
Diffusion tensor imaging studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor- (4th ed., pp. 3–51). Guilford Press.
der: Meta-analyses and reflections on head motion. Journal of Child Barkley, R. A. (2015). Executive function and self-regulation viewed as an
Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 59(3), 193–202. extended phenotype: Implications of the theory for ADHD and its
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12778 treatment. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:
Armstrong, T. (2010). The power of neurodiversity: Unleashing the ad- A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (4th ed., pp. 405–434). The
vantages of your differently wired brain. Da Capo Press. Guilford Press.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Clarendon Press. Barkley, R. A., & Brown, T. E. (2008). Unrecognized attention-deficit/
Baddeley, A. (1996). The fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of hyperactivity disorder in adults presenting with other psychiatric disor-
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(24), ders. CNS Spectrums, 13(11), 977–984. https://doi.org/10.1017/S109285
13468–13472. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.24.13468 2900014036
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K., & Fischer, M. (2010). ADHD in adults: What the
memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485–493. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894- science says. Guildford Press.
4105.8.4.485 Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., & Fischer, M. (2008). Adult ADHD: What the
Bandura, A. (1996). Regulation of cognitive processes through percieved science says. Guilford Press.
self-efficacy. In G. H. Jennings & D. Belanger (Eds.), Passages beyond the Bauer, I., & Baumeister, R. (2011). Self-regulatory strength. In K. Vohs & R.
gate: A Jungian approach to understanding the nature of American Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory and
psychology at the dawn of the new millennium (pp. 96–107). Simon & applications (2nd ed., pp. 64–82). The Guilford Press.
Schuster. Bjerrum, M. B., Pedersen, P. U., & Larsen, P. (2017). Living with symptoms
Banks, T., Ninowski, J. E., Mash, E. J., & Semple, D. L. (2008). Parenting of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adulthood: A systematic
behavior and cognitions in a community sample of mothers with and review of qualitative evidence. JBI Database of Systematic Reviews
without symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of and Implementation Reports, 15(4), 1080–1153. https://doi.org/10
Child and Family Studies, 17(1), 28–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826- .11124/JBISRIR-2017-003357
007-9139-0 Blair, C., & Ursache, A. (2011). A bidirectional model of executive functions
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, inten- and self-regulation. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook
tion, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In P. Robbins & M. of self-regulation: Research, theory and applications (pp. 300–320).
Aydede (Eds.), Handbook of Situated Cognition (pp. 1–40). Cambridge Guildford Press.
University Press. Blom, V. (2011). Striving for self-esteem: Conceptualizations and role in
Barkley, R. A. (1989). The problem of stimulus control and rule-governed burnout. Department of Psychology, Stockholm University.
behaviour in attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. In L. Blooming- Bloomfield, M. A., McCutcheon, R. A., Kempton, M., Freeman, T. P., &
dale & J. Swanson (Eds.), Attention deficit disorder: Current concepts and Howes, O. (2019). The effects of psychosocial stress on dopaminergic
594 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

function and the acute stress response. eLife, 8, 1–22. https://doi.org/10 Cai, W., Griffiths, K., Korgaonkar, M. S., Williams, L. M., & Menon, V.
.7554/eLife.46797 (2021). Inhibition-related modulation of salience and frontoparietal net-
Bokor, G., & Anderson, P. D. (2014). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor- works predicts cognitive control ability and inattention symptoms in
der. Journal of Pharmacy Practice, 27(4), 336–349. https://doi.org/10 children with ADHD. Molecular Psychiatry, 26(8), 4016–4025. https://
.1177/0897190014543628 doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0564-4
Bolea-Alamañac, B., Nutt, D. J., Adamou, M., Asherson, P., Bazire, S., Cardello, A., & George, T. (2021). Effects of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity
Coghill, D., Heal, D., Müller, U., Nash, J., Santosh, P., Sayal, K., Sonuga- Disorder (ADHD) on generative and elaborative aspects of creativity.
Barke, E., & Young, S. J. (2014). Evidence-based guidelines for the Union College. https://digitalworks.union.edu/steinmetzsymposium/ste
pharmacological management of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: inmetz_2021/posters/193/
Update on recommendations from the British Association for Psycho- Carson, S. H., Higgins, D. M., & Peterson, J. B. (2003). Decreased latent
pharmacology. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 28(3), 179–203. https:// inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-
doi.org/10.1177/0269881113519509 functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Boonstra, A. M., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2005). 85(3), 499–506. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499
Executive functioning in adult ADHD: A meta-analytic review. Psycho- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2016). Self-regulation of action and affect. In
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

logical Medicine, 35(8), 1097–1108. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329 K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

170500499X Research, theory, and applications (3rd ed., pp. 3–23). The Guilford Press.
Boot, N. (2017). The creative brain: Subclinical symptoms of attentionde- Castellanos, F. X. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of ADHD. In M. V.
ficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are associated with specific creative Solanto, A. F. T. Arnsten, & F. X. Castellanos (Eds.), Stimulant drugs
processes. In The creative brain (pp. 71–98). UvA-DARE (Digital Q and ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience (pp. 243–258). Oxford
Academic Repository). University Press.
Boot, N., Nevicka, B., & Baas, M. (2020). Creativity in ADHD: Goal- Castellanos, F. X., & Proal, E. (2012). Large-scale brain systems in ADHD:
directed motivation and domain specificity. Journal of Attention Disor- Beyond the prefrontal—Striatal model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
ders, 24(13), 1857–1866. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054717727352 16(1), 17–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.007
Borsboom, D., & Cramer, A. O. J. (2013). Network analysis: An integra- Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Milham, M. P., & Tannock, R.
tive approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annual Review of (2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunc-
Clinical Psychology, 9(1), 91–121. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev- tion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(3), 117–123. https://doi.org/10
clinpsy-050212-185608 .1016/j.tics.2006.01.011
Bozhilova, N., Michelini, G., Kuntsi, J., & Asherson, P. (2018). Mind Champ, R. E., Adamou, M., & Tolchard, B. (2021). The impact of
wandering perspective on ADHD. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Re- psychological theory on the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity
views, 92, 464–476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.07.010 disorder (ADHD) in adults: A scoping review. PLOS ONE, 16(12), Article
Brites, C., Salgado-Azoni, C. A., Ferreira, T. L., Lima, R. F., & Ciasca, S. M. e0261247. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261247
(2015). Development and applications of the SWAN rating scale for Chevrier, A., & Schachar, R. J. (2020). BOLD differences normally attrib-
assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A literature review. uted to inhibitory control predict symptoms, not task-directed inhibitory
Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 48(11), 965–972. control in ADHD. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 12(1),
https://doi.org/10.1590/1414-431x20154528 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-020-09311-8
Bronowski, J. (1977). A sense of the future. MIT Press. Corbisiero, S., Stieglitz, R.-D., Retz, W., & Rösler, M. (2013). Is emotional
Brooks, R. B. (2001). Nurturing islands of competence: Is there really room dysregulation part of the psychopathology of ADHD in adults? Attention
for a strength-based model in the treatment of ADHD? The ADHD Report, Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 5(2), 83–92. https://doi.org/10.1007/
9(2), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1521/adhd.9.2.1.19075 s12402-012-0097-z
Brown, T. E. (1995). Differential diagnosis of ADD versus ADHD in adults. Cordova, M., Shada, K., Demeter, D. V., Doyle, O., Miranda-Dominguez,
In K. G. Nadeau (Ed.), A comprehensive guide to attention deficit disorder O., Perrone, A., Schifsky, E., Graham, A., Fombonne, E., Langhorst, B.,
in adults (pp. 93–108). Brunner/Mazel Inc. Nigg, J., Fair, D. A., & Feczko, E. (2020). Heterogeneity of executive
Brown, T. E. (2005). Attention deficit disorder: The unfocused mind in function revealed by a functional random forest approach across ADHD
children and adults. Yale University Press. and ASD. NeuroImage. Clinical, 26, Article 102245. https://doi.org/10
Brown, T. E. (2006). Executive functions and Attention Deficit Hyperactiv- .1016/j.nicl.2020.102245
ity Disorder: Implications of two conflicting views. International Journal Corrigan, P., Watson, A., & Barr, L. (2006). The selfsStigma of mental
of Disability Development and Education, 53(1), 35–46. https://doi.org/10 illness: Implications for self-esteem and self-efficacy. Journal of Social
.1080/10349120500510024 and Clinical Psychology, 25(9), 875–884. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp
Brown, T. E. (2009). ADD/ADHD and impaired executive function in .2006.25.8.875
clinical practice. Current Attention Disorders Reports, 1(1), 37–41. Cortese, S., Imperati, D., Zhou, J., Proal, E., Klein, R. G., Mannuzza, S.,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12618-009-0006-3 Ramos-Olazagasti, M. A., Milham, M. P., Kelly, C., & Castellanos, F. X.
Brown, T. E. (2013). A new understanding of ADHD in children and adults: (2013). White matter alterations at 33-year follow-up in adults with
Executive function and impairments. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/ childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry,
9780203067536 74(8), 591–598. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.025
Brown, T. E. (2014). Smart but stuck: Emotions in teens and adults with Cortese, S., Kelly, C., Chabernaud, C., Proal, E., Di Martino, A., Milham,
ADHD. Jossey-Bass. M. P., & Castellanos, F. X. (2012). Toward systems neuroscience of
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s ADHD: A meta-analysis of 55 fMRI sudies. The American Journal of
default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of Psychiatry, 169(10), 1038–1055. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012
the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1–38. https://doi.org/10 .11101521
.1196/annals.1440.011 Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth. Psychological
Bush, G., Frazier, J. A., Rauch, S. L., Seidman, L. I., Whalen, P. J., Jenike, M. A., Review, 108(3), 593–623. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.593
Rosen, B. R., & Biederman, J. (1999). Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction Crosbie, J., Arnold, P., Paterson, A., Swanson, J., Dupuis, A., Li, X., Shan,
in ADHD revealed by fMRI and the counting Stroop. Biological Psychiatry, J., Goodale, T., Tam, C., Strug, L. J., & Schachar, R. J. (2013). Response
45(99), 1542–1552. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00083-9 inhibition and ADHD traits: Correlates and heritability in a community
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 595

sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(3), 497–507. https:// Diaz, R., & Berk, L. (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-
doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9693-9 regulation. Erlbaum.
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. (2013). Dibbets, P., Evers, E. A. T., Hurks, P. P. M., Bakker, K., & Jolles, J. (2010).
Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from Differential brain activation patterns in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity
genome-wide SNPs. Nature Genetics, 45(9), 984–994. https://doi.org/10 disorder (ADHD) associated with task switching. Neuropsychology,
.1038/ng.2711 24(4), 413–423. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018997
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-Bass. Dimic, N., & Orlov, V. (2014). Entrepreneurial tendencies among people
D’Esposito, M., Detre, J., Alsop, D., Shin, R., Atlas, S., & Grossman, M. with ADHD. SSRN, 13(3), 187–204. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn
(1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working .2405366
memory. Nature, 378(6554), 279–281. https://doi.org/10.1038/378279a0 Dipeolu, A. O. (2011). College students with adhd: Prescriptive concepts for
D’Esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Aguirre, G. K., Stallcup, M., Alsop, D. C., best practices in career development. Journal of Career Development,
Tippet, L. J., & Farah, M. J. (1997). A functional MRI study of mental 38(5), 408–427. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894845310378749
image generation. Neuropsychologia, 35(5), 725–730. https://doi.org/10 Dodge, K. A. (1991). The development of emotion regulation and dysre-
.1016/S0028-3932(96)00121-2 gulation (pp. 159–181). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human .1017/CBO9780511663963.009
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

brain. Putnam. Douglas, V. I. (1972). Stop, look and listen: The problem of sustained
Damasio, A. (1995). On some functions of the human prefrontal cortex. attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. Cana-
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769(1), 241–251. https:// dian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du
doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38142.x Comportement, 4, 259–282. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0082313
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens. Harcourt Brace. Douglas, V. I. (1980a). Higher mental processess in hyperactive children:
Damasio, A. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the Implications for training. In R. M. Knights & D. J. Bakker (Eds.),
making of consciousness. Vintage Books. Treatment of hyperactive and learning disordered children (pp. 65–92).
Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype: The long reach of the gene. University Park Press.
Oxford University Press. Douglas, V. I. (1980b). Treatment and training approaches to hyperactivity:
de Charms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective determi- Establishing internal or external control. In C. Whalen & B. Henker (Eds.),
nants of behaviour. Academic Press. Hyperactive children: The social ecology of identification and treatment
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in (pp. 283–318). Academic Press.
human behaviour. Plenum Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899- Douglas, V. I. (1983). Attention and cognitive problems. In M. Rutter (Ed.),
2271-7 Developmental neuropsychiatry (pp. 280–329). Academic Press.
Deci, E. L., Eghrarl, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). Facilitating Douglas, V. I. (1989). Can Skinnerian theory explain attention deficit
internalization: The self-determination theory perspective. Journal of disorder?—A reply to Barkley. In L. Bloomingdale & J. Swanson
Personality, 62(1), 119–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994 (Eds.), Attention Deficit Disorder: Current concepts and emerging trends
.tb00797.x in attentional and behavioural disorders in childhood (pp. 235–254).
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Pergamon Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-036508-4.50018-7
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Douglas, V. I. (2005). Cognitive deficits in children with attention deficit
Inquiry, 11(4), 227–268. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01 hyperactivity disorder: A long-term follow-up. Canadian Psychology,
Dell, N. A., Long, C., & Mancini, M. A. (2021). Models of mental health 46(1), 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0085821
recovery: An overview of systematic reviews and qualitative meta-syn- Douglas, V. I., & Peters, K. G. (1979). Towards a clearer definition of the
theses. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 44(3), 238–253. https:// attentional deficit of hyperactive children. In G. A. Hale & M. Lewis
doi.org/10.1037/prj0000444 (Eds.), Attention and the developments of cognitive skills (pp. 173–248).
Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, Plenum Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-2985-5_8
E., Baldursson, G., Belliveau, R., Bybjerg-Grauholm, J., Bækvad-Hansen, Dramsdahl, M., Westerhausen, R., Haavik, J., Hugdahl, K., & Plessen, K. J.
M., Cerrato, F., Chambert, K., Churchhouse, C., Dumont, A., Eriksson, (2012). Adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder—A diffusion-
N., Gandal, M., Goldstein, J. I., Grasby, K. L., Grove, J., … Neale, B. M. tensor imaging study of the corpus callosum. Psychiatry Research:
(2019). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for Neuroimaging, 201(2), 168–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(1), 63–75. .2011.08.005
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7 Droit-Volet, S. (2018). Intertwined facets of subjective time. Current
Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., Burgess, G. C., Bidwell, L. C., & Willcutt, E. G. Directions in Psychological Science, 27(6), 422–428. https://doi.org/10
(2010). Behavioural performance predicts grey matter reductions in the .1177/0963721418779978
right inverior frontal gyrus in young adults with combined type ADHD. Droit-Volet, S., El-Azhari, A., Haddar, S., Drago, R., & Gil, S. (2020).
Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 182(3), 231–237. https://doi.org/10 Similar time distortions under the effect of emotion for durations of several
.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.01.012 minutes and a few seconds. Acta Psychologica, 210, Article 103170.
Derks, E. M., Vink, J. M., Willemsen, G., Van Den Brink, W., & Boomsma, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103170
D. I. (2014). Genetic and environmental influences on the relationship Droit-Volet, S., Fayolle, S., Lamotte, M., & Gil, S. (2013). Time, emotion
between adult ADHD symptoms and self-reported problem drinking in and the embodiment of timing. Timing & Time Perception, 1(1), 99–126.
6024 Dutch twins. Psychological Medicine, 44(12), 2673–2683. https:// https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-00002004
doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714000361 Droit-Volet, S., Monceau, S., Berthon, M., Trahanias, P., & Maniadakis, M.
DeYoung, C. G. (2013). The neuromodulator of exploration: A unifying (2018). The explicit judgment of long durations of several minutes in
theory of the role of dopamine in personality. Frontiers in Human everyday life: Conscious retrospective memory judgment and the role of
Neuroscience, 7, Article 762. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00762 affects? PLOS ONE, 13(4), Article e0195397. https://doi.org/10.1371/
Di Domenico, S. I., & Ryan, R. M. (2017). The emerging neuroscience of journal.pone.0195397
intrinsic motivation: A new frontier in self-determination research. Fron- Droit-Volet, S., Monceau, S., Dambrun, M., & Martinelli, N. (2020).
tiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, Article 145. https://doi.org/10.3389/ Embodied time and the out-of-body experience of the self. PeerJ, 8,
fnhum.2017.00145 Article e8565. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.8565
596 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Duerden, E. G., Tannock, R., & Dockstader, C. (2012). Altered cortical psychiatric subtypes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(7), 584–601.
morphology in sensorimotor processing regions in adolescents and adults https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.009
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain Research, 1445, Fenton, A., & Krahn, T. (2007). Autism, neurodiversity and equality beyond
82–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.01.034 the ‘Normal.’. Journal of Ethics in Mental Health, 2, 1–6. https://jemh.ca/
Durston, S., Tottenham, N. T., Thomas, K. M., Davidson, M. C., Eigsti, issues/v2n2/index.html
I. M., Yang, Y., Ulug, A. M., & Casey, B. J. (2003). Differential patterns of Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Executive
striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD. Biological attention and metacognitive regulation. Consciousness and Cognition,
Psychiatry, 53(10), 871–878. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02) 9(2), 288–307. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.2000.0447
01904-2 Fink, A., Slamar-Halbed, M., Unterrainer, H. F., & Weiss, E. M. (2012).
Eisenburg, N., Smith, C. L., & Spinrad, T. L. (2011). Effortful control: Creativity: Genius, madness, or a combination of both. Psychology of
Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialisation in child- Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(1), 11–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/
hood. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: a0024874
Research, theory and applications (pp. 263–283). Guilford Press. Finkel, E. J., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2011). The effects of social relationships
Ek, A., & Isaksson, G. (2013). How adults with ADHD get engaged in and on self-regulation. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

perform everyday activities. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Ther- self-regulation: Research, theory and applications (2nd ed., pp. 390–406).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

apy, 20(4), 282–291. https://doi.org/10.3109/11038128.2013.799226 Guilford Press.


Elliot, A. J. (2006). The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance motiva- Fitzsimons, G. M., & Finkel, E. J. (2011). The effects of self-regulation
tion. Motivation and Emotion, 30(2), 111–116. https://doi.org/10.1007/ on social relationships. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.),
s11031-006-9028-7 Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory and applications
Ellison-Wright, I., Ellison-Wright, Z., & Bullmore, E. (2008). Structural (2nd ed., pp. 407–421). Guilford Press.
brain change in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder identified by Fleming, A. P., & McMahon, R. J. (2012). Developmental context and
meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 8(1), Article 51. https://doi.org/10.1186/ treatment principles for ADHD among college students. Clinical Child
1471-244X-8-51 and Family Psychology Review, 15(4), 303–329. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Erskine, H. E., Norman, R. E., Ferrari, A. J., Chan, G. C. K., Copeland, s10567-012-0121-z
W. E., Whiteford, H. A., & Scott, J. G. (2016). Long-term outcomes of Forbes, C. E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Retraining attitudes and stereotypes to
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Conduct Disorder: A sys- affect motivation and cognitive capacity under stereotype threat. Journal
tematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 740–754. https://doi.org/10
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(10), 841–850. https://doi.org/10.1016/ .1037/a0020971
j.jaac.2016.06.016 Forte, A., Orri, M., Galera, C., Pompili, M., Turecki, G., Boivin, M.,
Eslinger, P. J. (1996). Conceptualising, describing and measuring compo- Tremblay, R. E., & Côté, S. M. (2020). Developmental trajectories of
nenets of executive function: A summary. In G. R. Lyon & N. A. childhood symptoms of hyperactivity/inattention and suicidal behavior
Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory and executive function (pp. 367– during adolescence. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(2),
395). Brookes. 145–151. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-019-01338-0
Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., & Hirsch, J. (2006). Fortes, D., Sierra-Pinheiro, M. A., Coutinhi, G., Bernardes, C., Regalla,
Resolving emotional conflict: A role for the rostral anterior cingulate M. A., Sergeant, J., & Mattos, P. (2014). Quantitative measurement of
cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron, 51(6), 871–882. impairment in ADHD: Perspectives for research and clinical practice.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.029 Archives of Clinical Psychiatry, 41(5), 124–130. https://doi.org/10.1590/
Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: Suggestions for a theory. 0101-60830000000026
Psychological Inquiry, 4(3), 147–178. https://doi.org/10.1207/s153279 Franke, B., Faraone, S. V., Asherson, P., Buitelaar, J., Bau, C. H. D., Ramos-
65pli0403_1 Quiroga, J. A., Mick, E., Grevet, E. H., Johansson, S., Haavik, J., Lesch,
Eysenck, H. J. (1994). Creativity and personality: Word association, ori- K. P., Cormand, B., & Reif, A. (2012). The genetics of attention deficit/
gence, and psychoticism. Creativity Research Journal, 7(2), 209–216. hyperactivity disorder in adults, a review. Molecular Psychiatry, 17(10),
https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419409534525 960–987. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2011.138
Fabrega, H., Jr. (1990). Psychiatric stigma in the classical and medieval Freud, S. (1923). The ego and the id. Norton.
period: A review of the literature. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31(4), Frodl, T., & Skokauskas, N. (2012). Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies
289–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-440X(90)90036-R in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates
Faraone, S. V., Ghirardi, L., Kudja-Halkola, R., Lichtenstein, P., & Larsson, treatment effects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(2), 114–126.
H. (2017). The familial co-aggregation of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01786.x
Disorder and Intellectual Disability: A register-based family study. Jour- Fusar-Poli, P., Rubia, K., Rossi, G., Sartori, G., & Balottin, U. (2012).
nal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(2), Striatal dopamine transporter alterations in ADHD: Pathophysiology or
167–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.11.011 adaptation to psychostimulants? A meta-analysis. The American Journal
Faraone, S. V., & Larsson, H. (2019). Genetics of attention deficit hyperac- of Psychiatry, 169(3), 264–272. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011
tivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(4), 562–575. https://doi.org/10 .11060940
.1038/s41380-018-0070-0 Fuster, J. M. (1997). The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, physiology and
Fassbender, C., & Schweitzer, J. B. (2006). Is there evidence for neural neuropsychology of the frontal lobe. Lippincott-Raven.
compensation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? A review of the Gao, Y., Shuai, D., Bu, X., Hu, X., Tang, S., Zhang, L., Li, H., Hu, X., Lu, L.,
functional neuroimaging literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 26(4), Gong, Q., & Huang, X. (2019). Impairments of large-scale functional
445–465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.003 networks in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of
Fassbender, C., Zhang, H., Buzy, W. M., Cortes, C. R., Mizuiri, D., Beckett, resting-state functional connectivity. Psychological Medicine, 49(15),
L., & Schweitzer, J. B. (2009). A lack of default network suppression is 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171900237X
linked to increased distractibility in ADHD. Brain Research, 1273, 114– Ghirardi, L., Brikell, I., Kuja-Halkola, R., Freitag, C. M., Franke, B.,
128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.070 Asherson, P., Lichtenstein, P., & Larsson, H. (2018). The familial co-
Feczko, E., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Marr, M., Graham, A. M., Nigg, J. T., aggregation of ASD and ADHD: A register-based cohort study. Molecular
& Fair, D. A. (2019). The heterogeneity problem: Approaches to identify Psychiatry, 23(2), 257–262. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.17
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 597

Girard-Joyal, O., & Gauthier, B. (2021)-. Creativity in the predominantly Hackley, S. A., & Miller, J. (1995). Response complexity and precue interval
inattentive and combined presentations of ADHD in adults. Journal of effects on the lateralized readiness potential. Psychophysiology, 32(3),
Attention Disorders. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb02952.x
10870547211060547 Harlow, J. M. (1848). Passage of an iron rod through the head. The Journal of
Goldman-Rakic, P. (1995). Architecture of the prefrontal cortex and the Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 39, 389–393. https://
central executive. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769(1), doi.org/10.1176/jnp.11.2.281
71–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38132.x Harlow, J. M. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2011). Planning promotes goal striving. head. Publications of the Massachusettes Medical Society, 2, 237–346.
In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: https://doi.org/10.1177/0957154X9300401407
Research, theory and applications (2nd ed., pp. 162–185). Guilford Press. Hart, H., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2012). Meta-analysis of
Goodman, D. W. (2007). The consequences of attention-deficit/hyperactivity fMRI studies of timing in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
disorder in adults. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 13(5), 318–327. https:// Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2248–2256. https://
doi.org/10.1097/01.pra.0000290670.87236.18 doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.003
Gosling, J. (1998). Hedonism. In E. Craig, M. Massimi, S. Golob, G. L. Hart, H., Radua, J., Nakao, T., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2013). Meta-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hagburg, N. Stoljar, E. McRae, & C. Kassor (Eds.), Routledge encyclo- analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

pedia of philosophy. Taylor and Francis. https://www.rep.routledge.com/ and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Exploring task-
articles/thematic/hedonism/v-1 specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(2),
Götting, F. N., Borchardt, V., Demenescu, L. R., Teckentrup, V., Dinica, K., 185–198. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277
Lord, A. R., Rohe, T., Hausdörfer, D. I., Li, M., Metzger, C. D., & Walter, Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. Wiley. https://
M. (2017). Higher interference susceptibility in reaction time task is doi.org/10.1037/10628-000
accompanied by weakened functional dissociation between salience Herwig, U. (2010). Me, Myself and I: How the brain maintains a sense of
and default mode network. Neuroscience Letters, 649, 34–40. https:// self. Scientific American Mind, 21(3), 58–63. http://www.jstor.org/stable/
doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2017.03.035 24943086
Grace, A. (2001). Psychostimulant actions on dopamine and limbic system Hirvikoski, T., Waaler, E., Alfredsson, J., Pihlgren, C., Holmström, A.,
function: Relevance to the pathophysiology and treatment of ADHD. In Johnson, A., Rück, J., Wiwe, C., Bothén, P., & Nordström, A. L.
M. V. Solanto, A. F. T. Arnsten, & F. X. Castellanos (Eds.), Stimulant (2011). Reduced ADHD symptoms in adults with ADHD after structured
drugs and ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience (pp. 134–157). Oxford skills training group: Results from a randomized controlled trial. Behaviour
University Press. Research and Therapy, 49(3), 175–185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011
Grant, A., & Kara, H. (2021). Considering the Autistic advantage in .01.001
qualitative research: The strengths of Autistic researchers. Contemporary Hoogman, M., Bralten, J., Hibar, D. P., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M. P.,
Social Science, 16(5), 589–603. https://doi.org/10.1080/21582041.2021 Schweren, L. S. J., van Hulzen, K. J. E., Medland, S. E., Shumskaya,
.1998589 E., Jahanshad, N., de Zeeuw, P., Szekely, E., Sudre, G., Wolfers, T.,
Greven, C. U., Buitelaar, J. K., & Salum, G. A. (2018). From positive Onnink, A. M. H., Dammers, J. T., Mostert, J. C., Vives-Gilabert, Y.,
psychology to psychopathology: The continuum of attention-deficit hyper- Kohls, G., … Franke, B. (2017). Subcortical brain volume differences
activity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children
Disciplines, 59(3), 203–212. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12786 and adults: A cross-sectional mega-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(4),
Greven, C. U., Merwood, A., Van Der Meer, J. M. J., Haworth, C. M. A., 310–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30049-4
Rommelse, N., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2016). The opposite end of the attention Hoogman, M., Buitelaar, J. K., Faraone, S. V., Shaw, P., & Franke, B. (2017).
deficit hyperactivity disorder continuum: Genetic and environmental Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit
aetiologies of extremely low ADHD traits. Journal of Child Psychology hyperactivity disorder in children and adults—Authors’ reply. The Lancet
and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 57(4), 523–531. https://doi.org/10 Psychiatry, 4(6), 440–441. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30200-6
.1111/jcpp.12475 Hoogman, M., Stolte, M., Baas, M., & Kroesbergen, E. (2020). Creativity
Groenman, A. P., Janssen, T. W. P., & Oosterlaan, J. (2017). Childhood and ADHD: A review of behavioral studies, the effect of psychostimulants
psychiatric disorders as risk factor for subsequent substance abuse: A and neural underpinnings. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 119,
meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent 66–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.029
Psychiatry, 56(7), 556–569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.05.004 Houghton, S. (2006). Advances in ADHD research through the lifespan:
Gross, J. J. (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. Guilford Press. Common themes and implications. International Journal of Disability
Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation: Taking stock and moving forward. Development and Education, 53(2), 263–272. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Emotion, 13(3), 359–365. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032135 10349120600716208
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Hoza, B., Waschbusch, D. A., Owens, J. S., Pelham, W. E., & Kipp, H.
Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X (2001). Academic task persistence of normally achieving ADHD and
.2014.940781 control boys: Performance, self-evaluations, and attributions. Journal of
Gruber, M. J., Gelman, B. D., & Ranganath, C. (2014). States of curiosity Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69(2), 271–283. https://doi.org/10
modulate hippocampus-dependent learning via the dopaminergic circuit. .1037/0022-006X.69.2.271
Neuron, 84(2), 486–496. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.060 Hull, C. (1943). Principles of behaviour: An introduction to behaviour
Gusnard, D. A. (2005). Being a self: Considerations from functional imag- theory. Appleton-Century.
ing. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(4), 679–697. https://doi.org/10 Hutchinson, A. D., Mathias, J. L., & Banich, M. T. (2008). Corpus callosum
.1016/j.concog.2005.04.004 morphology in children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactiv-
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). ity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 22(3), 341–349.
Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.341
default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Jacob, K. (2015). Recovery model of mental illness: A complementary
Sciences of the United States of America, 98(7), 4259–4264. https:// approach to psychiatric care. Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine,
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.071043098 37(2), 117–119. https://doi.org/10.4103/0253-7176.155605
598 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Johansen, E. B., Aase, H., Meyer, A., & Sagvolden, T. (2002). Attention- Liston, C., Cohen, M. M., Teslovich, T., Levenson, D., & Casey, B. J.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) behaviour explained by dysfunc- (2011). Atypical prefrontal connectivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity
tioning reinforcement and extinction processes. Behavioural Brain disorder: Pathway to disease or pathological end point? Biological
Research, 130(1–2), 37–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01) Psychiatry, 69(12), 1168–1177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011
00434-X .03.022
Johnson, K. A., Wiersema, J. R., & Kuntsi, J. (2009). What would Karl Ljung, T., Chen, Q., Lichtenstein, P., & Larsson, H. (2014). Common
Popper say? Are current psychological theories of ADHD falsifiable? etiological factors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and suicidal
Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5(1), Article 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/ behavior: A population-based study in Sweden. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(8),
1744-9081-5-15 958–964. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.363
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. Farrar, Straus, and Giroux. Loney, J., & Milich, R. (1982). Hyperactivity, inattention and aggression in
Kasof, J. (1997). Creativity and breadth of attention. Creativity Research clinical practice. In M. Wolraich & D. Routh (Eds.), Advances in
Journal, 10(4), 303–316. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1004_2 developmental and behavioural pediatrics (Vol. 3, p. 143). JAI Press.
Kernis, M. H., & Paradise, A. W. (2002). Distinguishing between secure and Luo, Y., Weibman, D., Halperin, J. M., & Li, X. (2019). A review of
fragile forms of high self esteem. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), heterogeneity in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Fron-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 339–360). University of tiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, Article 42. https://doi.org/10.3389/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Rochester Press. fnhum.2019.00042


Kessler, R., Adler, L., Barkley, R. A., Biederman, J., Conners, C. K., Demier, Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. Basic Books.
O., Faraone, S., Greenhill, L., Howes, M., Secnik, K., Spencer, T., Unstun, Makris, N., Liang, L., Biederman, J., Valera, E. M., Brown, A. B., Petty, C.,
T. B., Walters, E., & Zaslavsky, A. (2006). The prevalence and correlates Spencer, T. J., Faraone, S. V., & Seidman, L. J. (2015). Toward defining
of adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the National Comor- the neural substrates of ADHD: A controlled structural MRI study in
bidity Survey Replication. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(4), medication-naïve adults. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19(11), 944–953.
716–723. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2006.163.4.716 https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054713506041
Knouse, L. E., & Fleming, A. P. (2016). Applying cognitive-behavioral Makris, N., Seidman, L. J., Valera, E. M., Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C.,
therapy for ADHD to emerging adults. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, Kennedy, D. N., Caviness, V. S., Jr., Bush, G., Crum, K., Brown, A. B., &
23(3), 300–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2016.03.008 Faraone, S. V. (2010). Anterior cingulate volumetric alterations in treat-
Konrad, A., Dielentheis, T. F., El Masri, D., Dellani, P. R., Stoeter, P., ment-naïve adults with ADHD: A pilot study. Journal of Attention
Vucurevic, G., & Winterer, G. (2012). White matter abnormalities and Disorders, 13(4), 407–413. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054709351671
their impact on attentional performance in adult attention-deficit/hyperac- Maranges, H. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). Self-control and ego deple-
tivity disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosci- tion. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-
ence, 262(4), 351–360. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-011-0251-1 regulation: Research, theory and applications (3rd ed., pp. 42–61).
Konrad, A., Dielentheis, T. F., El Masri, D., Fehr, C., Gesierich, T., Guilford Press.
Vucurevic, G., Stoeter, P., & Winterer, G. (2010). Disturbed structural Mash, E., & Barkley, R. A. (2006). Treatment of childhood disorders
connectivity is related to inattention and impulsivity in adult attention (3rd ed.). Guilford Press.
deficit hyperactivity disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31(5), McAdams, D. P. (1990). The person: An introduction to personality
912–919. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07110.x psychology. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Kooij, J., Bijlenga, D., Salerno, L., Jaeschke, R., Bitter, I., Balázs, J., Thome, McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A
J., Dom, G., Kasper, S., Nunes Filipe, C., Stes, S., Mohr, P., Leppämäki, contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry,
S., Casas Brugué, M., Bobes, J., Mccarthy, J. M., Richarte, V., Kjems 7(4), 295–321. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0704_1
Philipsen, A., Pehlivanidis, A., … Asherson, P. (2019). Updated Euro- McAdams, D. P. (2013). The psychological self as actor, agent, and author.
pean consensus statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 272–295. https://doi.org/10
European Psychiatry, 56(1), 14–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018 .1177/1745691612464657
.11.001 McCarthy, H., Skokauskas, N., & Frodl, T. (2014). Identifying a consistent
Lackschewitz, H., Hüther, G., & Kröner-Herwig, B. (2008). Physiological pattern of neural function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A
and psychological stress responses in adults with attention-deficit/hyper- meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(4), 869–880. https://doi.org/10
activity disorder (ADHD). Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(5), 612–624. .1017/S0033291713001037
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.01.016 McCullough, M. E., & Carter, E. C. (2011). Waiting, tolerating, and
LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain: The mysterious underpinnings of cooperating: Did religion evolve to prop up humans’ self-control abilities?
emotional life. Simon & Schuster. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation:
Lee, G. P., Strauss, E., Loring, D. W., McCloskey, L., Haworth, J. M., & Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 422–440). Guilford Press.
Lehman, R. A. W. (1997). Sensitivity of figural fluency on the Five-Point McNally, R. J. (2016). Can network analysis transform psychopathology?
test to focal neurological dysfunction. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Behaviour Research and Therapy, 86, 95–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
11(1), 59–68. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049708407030 .brat.2016.06.006
Lee, W., & Reeve, J. (2017). Identifying the neural substrates of intrinsic Mendelsohn, G. (1976). Associative and attentional processes in creative
motivation during task performance. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral performance. Journal of Personality, 44(2), 341–369. https://doi.org/10
Neuroscience, 17(5), 939–953. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-017- .1111/j.1467-6494.1976.tb00127.x
0524-x Menon, V. (2015). Salience network. In Brain mapping: An encyclopedic
Lei, D., Du, M., Wu, M., Chen, T., Huang, X., Du, X., Bi, F., Kemp, G. J., reference (Vol. 2). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1
& Gong, Q. (2015). Functional MRI reveals different response inhibi- .00052-X
tion between adults and children with ADHD. Neuropsychology, 29(6), Middeldorp, C. M., Hammerschlag, A. R., Ouwens, K. G., Groen-Blokhuis,
874–881. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000200 M. M., St. Pourcain, B., Greven, C. U., Pappa, I., Tiesler, C. M. T., Ang,
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychologi- W., Nolte, I. M., Vilor-Tejedor, N., Bacelis, J., Ebejer, J. L., Zhao, H.,
cal assessment (4th ed.). Oxford University Press. Davies, G. E., Ehli, E. A., Evans, D. M., Fedko, I. O., Guxens, M., …
Liberman, R. P. (2008). Recovery from disability: Manual of psychiatric Boomsma, D. I. (2016). A genome-wide association meta-analysis of
rehabilitation. American Psychiatric Publishing. attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in population-based
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 599

pediatric cohorts. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Ninowski, J. E., Mash, E. J., & Benzies, K. M. (2007). Symptoms of
Psychiatry, 55(10), 896–905.e6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016 attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in first-time expectant women:
.05.025 Relations with parenting cognitions and behaviors. Infant Mental Health
Miller, M., Musser, E. D., Young, G. S., Olson, B., Steiner, R. D., & Nigg, J. T. Journal, 28(1), 54–75. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20122
(2019). Sibling recurrence risk and cross-aggregation of Attention-Deficit/ Norman, L. J., Carlisi, C., Lukito, S., Hart, H., Mataix-Cols, D., Radua, J., &
Hyperactivity Disorder and Autism Spectrum Disorder. JAMA Pediatrics, Rubia, K. (2016). Structural and functional brain abnormalities in atten-
173(2), 147–152. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.4076 tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive–compulsive disorder: A
Milton, D. E. M., & Bracher, M. (2013). Autistics speak but are they heard? comparative meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(8), 815–825. https://
Medical Sociology Online, 7(2), 61–69. doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0700
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive–affective system theory Ochsner, K., Bunge, S., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. (2002). Rethinking feelings:
of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotions. Journal of
and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102(2), Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215–12. https://doi.org/10.1162/08989
246–268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.246 2902760807212
Mitchell, J. T., Benson, J. W., Knouse, L. E., Kimbrel, N. A., & Anasto- Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

poulos, A. D. (2013). Are negative automatic thoughts associated with Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

adhd in adulthood? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(4), 851–859. .2005.03.010


https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9525-4 Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. R., Hughes, B., Mcrae, K., Cooper, J. C., Weber, J.,
Moore, A. (2019). Hedonism. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), Stanford encyclopedia of Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Bottom-up and top-down
philosophy. Metaphysics Research Lab, Centre for the Study of Language processes in emotion generation: Common and distinct neural mechan-
and Information, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/ isms. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1322–1331. https://doi.org/10.1111/
win2019/entries/hedonism/ j.1467-9280.2009.02459.x
Mueller, A. K., Fuermaier, A. B. M., Koerts, J., & Tucha, L. (2012). Stigma in Onnink, A. M. H., Zwiers, M. P., Hoogman, M., Mostert, J. C., Dammers, J.,
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Kan, C. C., Vasquez, A. A., Schene, A. H., Buitelaar, J., & Franke, B.
Disorders, 4(3), 101–114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-012-0085-3 (2015). Deviant white matter structure in adults with attention-deficit/
Murray, C., & Johnston, C. (2006). Parenting in mothers with and without hyperactivity disorder points to aberrant myelination and affects neuro-
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, psychological performance. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology &
115(1), 52–61. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.52 Biological Psychiatry, 63, 14–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015
Nadeau, K. G. (2005). Career choices and workplace challenges for in- .04.008
dividuals with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(5), 549–563. Oram, R., Rogers, M., & DuPaul, G. (2020). Explaining the relationship
https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20119 between ADHD symptomatology and amotivation in the undergraduate
Newark, P. E., & Stieglitz, R. D. (2010). Therapy-relevant factors in adult population: The role of basic psychological need frustration. Canadian
ADHD from a cognitive behavioural perspective. Attention Deficit and Journal of School Psychology, 35(2), 139–153. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Hyperactivity Disorders, 2(2), 59–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402- 0829573519880063
010-0023-1 Oxford University Press. (n.d.-a). Definition of disorder [online].
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: On the relations among self- Lexico.Com; Oxford University Press. Retrieved December 23, 2020,
regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive from https://www.lexico.com/definition/disorder
control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psy- Oxford University Press. (n.d.-b). Definition of syndrome [online].
chopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Lexico.Com. Retrieved December 23, 2020, from https://www.lexico
Disciplines, 58(4), 361–383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675 .com/definition/syndrome
Nigg, J. T., & Casey, B. J. (2005). An integrative theory of attention-deficit/ Partridge, L., & Williams, N. (2008). What’s in it for me? New perspectives
hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. on motivating students with AD/HD. In P. L. Jeffery (Ed.), AARE 2007
Development and Psychopathology, 17(3), 785–806. https://doi.org/10 international education research conference, Fremantle, Western Aus-
.1017/S0954579405050376 tralia (Vol. 1, pp. 1–19). The Australian Association for Research in
Nigg, J. T., Goldsmith, H. H., & Sachek, J. (2004). Temperament and Education.
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The development of a multiple Payer, D., Williams, B., Mansouri, E., Stevanovski, S., Nakajima, S., Le Foll,
pathway model. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, B., Kish, S., Houle, S., Mizrahi, R., George, S. R., George, T. P., &
33(1), 42–53. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_5 Boileau, I. (2017). Corticotropin-releasing hormone and dopamine release
Nigg, J. T., Karalunas, S. L., Feczko, E., & Fair, D. A. (2020). Toward a in healthy individuals. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 76, 192–196. https://
revised nosology for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder heterogene- doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.034
ity. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, Peterson, B. S., Potenza, M. N., Wang, Z., Zhu, H., Martin, A., Marsh, R.,
5(8), 726–737. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.02.005 Plessen, K. J., & Yu, S. (2009). An fMRI study of the effects of
Nigg, J. T., Sibley, M. H., Thapar, A., & Karalunas, S. L. (2020). Develop- psychostimulants on default-mode processing during stroop task perfor-
ment of ADHD: Etiology, heterogeneity, and early life course. Annual mance in youths with ADHD. The American Journal of Psychiatry,
Review of Developmental Psychology, 2(1), 559–583. https://doi.org/10 166(11), 1286–1294. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08050724
.1146/annurev-devpsych-060320-093413 Pfander, A. (1967). Motives and motivation. In A. Pfander (Ed.), Munch-
Nigg, J. T., Stavro, G., Ettenhofer, M., Hambrick, D. Z., Miller, T., & ener philosophische abhandlungen (Festschrift fur Theodor Lipps)
Henderson, J. M. (2005). Executive functions and ADHD in adults: [Phenomenology of willing and motivation: And other phaenomenlo-
Evidence for selective effects on ADHD symptom domains. Journal of gica]. (H. Spiegelberg, Trans., pp. 163–195). Barth.
Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 706–717. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021- Philipsen, A., Graf, E., Tebartz van Elst, L., Jans, T., Warnke, A., Hesslinger,
843X.114.3.706 B., Ebert, D., Gerlach, M., Matthies, S., Colla, M., Jacob, C., Sobanski, E.,
Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Alm, B., Rösler, M., Ihorst, G., Gross-Lesch, S., Gentschow, L., Kis, B.,
Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we Huss, M., … Berger, M. (2010). Evaluation of the efficacy and effec-
need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, tiveness of a structured disorder tailored psychotherapy in ADHD in
57(11), 1224–1230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025 adults: Study protocol of a randomized controlled multicentre trial.
600 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 2(4), 203–212. https:// College Student Psychotherapy, 21(1), 3–20. https://doi.org/10.1300/
doi.org/10.1007/s12402-010-0046-7 J035v21n01_02
Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge: An essay on the relations between Ramsay, J. R., & Rostain, A. L. (2015). The adult ADHD toolkit. Routledge.
organic regulations and cognitive processes. University of Chicago Press. Redshaw, R., & McCormack, L. (2022). “Being ADHD”: A qualitative
Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relation during child study. Advances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6(1), 20–28. https://
development (T. A. Brown & C. E. Kaegi, Trans.). Annual Reviews. doi.org/10.1007/s41252-021-00227-5
Piatt, A. L., Fields, J. A., Paolo, A. M., & Tröster, A. I. (1999). Action (verb Reeve, J., & Lee, W. (2019a). A neuroscientific perspective on basic
naming) fluency as an executive function measure: Convergent and psychological needs. Journal of Personality, 87(1), 102–114. https://
divergent evidence of validity. Neuropsychologia, 37(13), 1499–1503. doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12390
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00066-4 Reeve, J., & Lee, W. (2019b). Motivational neuroscience. The Oxford
Piekoff, L. (1993). Objectivism: The philosophy of Ayn Rand. Meridian. Handbook of Human Motivation, December, 353–372. https://doi.org/
Pironti, V. A., Lai, M. C., Müller, U., Dodds, C. M., Suckling, J., Bullmore, 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190666453.013.20
E. T., & Sahakian, B. J. (2014). Neuroanatomical abnormalities and Reiss, J., & Sprenger, J. (2017). Scientific objectivity. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.),
cognitive impairments are shared by adults with attention-deficit/hyperac- The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/entrie
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tivity disorder and their unaffected first-degree relatives. Biological Psychi- s/scientific-objectivity/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

atry, 76(8), 639–647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.025 Ricoeur, P. (1966). Freedom and nature: The voluntary and the involuntary
Posner, J., Park, C., & Wang, Z. (2015). Connecting the dots: A review of (E. V. Kohak, Trans.). Northwestern University Press.
resting connectivity MRI studies in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor- Roberts, W., Milich, R., & Barkley, R. A. (2014). Primary symptoms,
der. Journal of Investigative Dermatology, 135(2), 612–615. https:// diagnostic criteria, subtyping and prevalence of ADHD. In R. A. Barkley
doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9251-z (Ed.), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis
Posner, J., Polanczyk, G. V., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2020). Attention-deficit and treatment (4th ed., pp. 51–80). Guilford Press.
hyperactivity disorder. Lancet, 395(10222), 450–462. https://doi.org/10 Rogers, C. (1963). The acutalizing tendency inrelation to “motives” and to
.1016/S0140-6736(19)33004-1 consciousness. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation
Posner, J., Rauh, V., Gruber, A., Gat, I., Wang, Z., & Peterson, B. S. (2013). (Vol. 11, pp. 1–24). University of Nebraska Press.
Dissociable attentional and affective circuits in medication-naïve children Rosqvist, H. B., Stenning, A., & Chown, N. (2020a). Introduction. In H. B.
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuro- Rosqvist, N. Chown, & A. Stenning (Eds.), Neurodiversity studies: A
imaging, 213(1), 24–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.01.004 new critical paradigm (pp. 1–5). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/
Prendergast, M., Taylor, E., Rapoport, J. L., Bartko, J., Donnelly, M., 9780429322297-1
Zametkin, A., Ahearen, M. B., Dunn, G., & Wieselberg, H. M. (1988). Rosqvist, H. B., Stenning, A., & Chown, N. (2020b). Neurodiversity studies:
The diagnosis of childhood hyperactivity: A U.S.–U.K. cross-national Proposing a new field of inquiry. In H. B. Rosqvist, N. Chown, & A.
study of DSM III and ICD-9. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Stenning (Eds.), Neurodiversity studies: A new critical paradigm (pp.
and Allied Disciplines, 29(3), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469- 226–229). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429322297-23
7610.1988.tb00717.x Roth, G., Benita, M., Amrani, C., Shachar, B. H., Asoulin, H., Moed, A.,
Pribram, K. H., & McGuinness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation, and effort in Bibi, U., & Kanat-Maymon, Y. (2014). Integration of negative emo-
the control of attention. Psychological Review, 82(2), 116–149. https:// tional experience versus suppression: Addressing the question of adap-
doi.org/10.1037/h0076780 tive functioning. Emotion, 14(5), 908–919. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Prince, J. B., & Wilens, T. E. (2009). Pharmacotherapy of ADHD and a0037051
cormibidities. In T. E. Brown (Ed.), ADHD comorbidities (pp. 339–384). Roth, G., Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2019). Integrative emotion
American Psychiatric Publishing. regulation: Process and development from a self-determination theory
Proal, E., Reiss, P. T., Klein, R. G., Mannuzza, S., Gotimer, K., Ramos- perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 1–12. https://
Olazagasti, M. A., Lerch, J. P., He, Y., Zijdenbos, A., Kelly, C., Milham, doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000403
M. P., & Castellanos, F. X. (2011). Brain gray matter deficits at 33-year Rubia, K. (2018). Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity
follow-up in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder estab- disorder (ADHD) and its clinical translation. Frontiers in Human Neuro-
lished in childhood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(11), 1122–1134. science, 12, Article 100. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00100
https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.117 Rubia, K., Halari, R., Cubillo, A., Mohammad, A. M., Brammer, M., &
Ramsay, J. R. (2010). Nonmedication treatments for adult ADHD. American Taylor, E. (2009). Methylphenidate normalises activation and functional
Psychological Association. connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in medication-
Ramsay, J. R. (2016). “Turning intentions into actions”: CBT for naïve children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous performance
adult ADHD focused on implementation. Clinical Case Studies, task. Neuropharmacology, 57(7–8), 640–652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ne
15(3), 179–197. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534650115611483 uropharm.2009.08.013
Ramsay, J. R. (2020). Rethinking adult ADHD. American Psychological Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C. R.,
Association. Simmons, A., Andrew, C., & Bullmore, E. T. (2000). Functional fronta-
Ramsay, J. R., Giwerc, D., McGrath, R. E., & Niemiec, R. (2016). Are there lisation with age: Mapping neurodevelopmental trajectories with fMRI.
virtues associated with adult ADHD? Comparison of ADHD adults and Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(1), 13–19. https://doi.org/
controls on the VIA inventory of strengths [Poster presentation]. The 2016 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00055-X
APSARD National Conference. https://www.academia.edu/22356984/Are_ Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Attentional control
There_Character_Strengths_Associated_with_Adult_ADHD_Comparison_ and self-regulation. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of
of_ADHD_Adults_and_Controls_on_the_VIA_Inventory_of_Strengths self-regulation: research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 284–300).
Ramsay, J. R., & Rostain, A. L. (2008a). Adult ADHD research. Journal of Guilford Press.
Attention Disorders, 11(6), 624–627. https://doi.org/10.1177/108705470 Rushworth, M. F. S., Behrens, T. E. J., Rudebeck, P. H., & Walton, M. E.
8314590 (2007). Contrasting roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in deci-
Ramsay, J. R., & Rostain, A. L. (2008b). Cognitive behaviour therapy for sions and social behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(4), 168–176.
college students with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.004
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 601

Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(3), 397–468. https://doi.org/10.1017/
needs in motivation, development, and wellness. Guilford Press. https:// S0140525X05000075
doi.org/10.1521/978.14625/28806 Saint-Cyr, J. A. (2003). Frontal–striatal circuit functions: Context, sequence,
Ryan, R. M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic motivation, auton- and consequence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Soci-
omy and the self in psychological development. In J. Jacobs (Ed.), ety, 9(1), 103–127. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617703910125
Nebraska symposium on motivation: Vol. 40. Developmental perspectives Sanders, A. F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and human performance.
on motivation (pp. 1–56). University of Nebraska Press. Acta Psychologica, 53(1), 61–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(83)
Ryan, R. M., & Brown, K. W. (2013). What is optimal self-esteem? The 90016-1
cultivation and consequences of contingent vs. true self esteem as viewed Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron,
from the self-determination theory perspective. In M. H. Kernis (Ed.), 36(2), 241–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00967-4
Self-esteem issues and answers: A sourcebook on current perspectives Schweitzer, J. B., Lee, D. O., Hanford, R. B., Zink, C. F., Ely, T. D.,
(pp. 125–131). Psychology Press. Tagamets, M. A., Hoffman, J. M., Grafton, S. T., & Kilts, C. D. (2004).
Ryan, R. M., Curran, R. R., & Deci, E. L. (2013). What humans need: Effect of methylphenidate on executive functioning in adults with atten-
Flourishing in Aristotelian philosophy and Self-Determination Theory. In tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Normalization of behavior but not
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

A. S. Waterman (Ed.), The best within us: Positive psychology perspec- related brain activity. Biological Psychiatry, 56(8), 597–606. https://
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tives on eudaimonia (pp. 57–75). American Psychological Association. doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.07.011


https://doi.org/10.1037/14092-004 Sedgwick, J. A., Merwood, A., & Asherson, P. (2018). The positive aspects
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1999). Approaching and avoiding self- of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A qualitative investigation of
determination: Comparing cybernetic and organismic paradigms of successful adults with ADHD. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Dis-
motivation. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Perspectives on Behavioural Self- orders. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-018-
Regulation (pp. 193–215). Routledge. 0277-6
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Liang, L., Valera, E. M., Monuteaux,
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. M. C., Brown, A., Kaiser, J., Spencer, T., Faraone, S. V., & Makris, N.
American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X (2011). Gray matter alterations in adults with attention-deficit/hyper-
.55.1.68 activity disorder identified by voxel based morphometry. Biological
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). A Self-determination theory approach to Psychiatry, 69(9), 857–866. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010
psychotherapy: The motivational basis for effective change. Canadian .09.053
Psychology, 49(3), 186–193. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012753 Sergeant, J. (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: An empirical approach
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., Grolnick, W. S., & La Guardia, J. G. (2006). The to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehav-
significance of autonomy and autonomy support in psychological devel- ioral Reviews, 24(1), 7–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(99)
opment and psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), 00060-3
Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and method (2nd ed., Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Modeling attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A
pp. 795–849). Wiley. critical appraisal of the cognitive–energetic model. Biological Psychiatry,
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2016). Autonomy and 57(11), 1248–1255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.010
autonomy disturbances in self-development and psychopathology: Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., Huijbregts, S., Scheres, A., & Oosterlaan, J.
Research on motivation, attachment, and clinical process. Development (2003). The top and the bottom of ADHD: A neuropsychological per-
and Psychopathology, I, 1–54. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556 spective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(7), 583–592.
.devpsy109 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.004
Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J., & Deci, E. L. (1997). Nature and autonomy: An Sergeant, J. A., Oosterlaan, J., & Van der Meer, J. (1999). Information
organizational view of social and neurobiological aspects of self- processing and energetic factors in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
regulation in behavior and development. Development and Psychopathol- In H. C. Quay & A. E. Hogan (Eds.), Handbook of disruptive behavior
ogy, 9(4), 701–728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579497001405 disorders (pp. 75–105). Kluwer Academic/Plenum. https://doi.org/10
Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. (1996). All goals are .1007/978-1-4615-4881-2_4
not created equal: An organismic perspective on the nature of goals and Shafritz, K. M., Marchione, K. E., Gore, J. C., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz,
their regulation. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Linking B. A. (2004). The effects of methylphenidate on neural systems in
cognition and motivation to behaviour (pp. 7–26). The psychology of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The American Journal of
action. Guilford Press. Psychiatry, 161(11), 1990–1997. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.161
Ryding, E., Brådvik, B., & Ingvar, D. H. (1996). Silent speech activates .11.1990
prefrontal cortical regions asymmetrically, as well as speech-related areas Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J. P., Greenstein,
in the dominant hemisphere. Brain and Language, 52(3), 435–451. https:// D., Clasen, L., Evans, A., Giedd, J., & Rapoport, J. L. (2007). Attention-
doi.org/10.1006/brln.1996.0023 deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8- maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928. https://doi.org/10 United States of America, 104(49), 19649–19654. https://doi.org/10
.1126/science.274.5294.1926 .1073/pnas.0707741104
Safren, S. A., Perlman, C., Sprich, S., & Otto, M. (2005). Mastering your Shaw, P., Lerch, J., Greenstein, D., Sharp, W., Clasen, L., Evans, A., Giedd,
adult ADHD. Oxford University Press. J., Castellanos, F. X., & Rapoport, J. (2006). Longitudinal mapping of
Safren, S. A., Sprich, S., Chulvick, S., & Otto, M. W. (2004). Psychosocial cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with
treatments for adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 27(2), 349–360. https://doi.org/ 63(5), 540–549. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.540
10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00089-3 Shaw, P., Sudre, G., Wharton, A., Weingart, D., Sharp, W., & Sarlls, J.
Sagvolden, T., Johansen, E. B., Aase, H., & Russell, V. A. (2005). A (2015). White matter microstructure and the variable adult outcome of
dynamic developmental theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharma-
(ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes. cology, 40(3), 746–754. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.241
602 CHAMP, ADAMOU, AND TOLCHARD

Sidlauskaite, J., Sonuga-Barke, E., Roeyers, H., & Wiersema, J. R. (2016). and Allied Disciplines, 37(2), 189–194. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-
Altered intrinsic organisation of brain networks implicated inattentional 7610.1996.tb01390.x
processesin adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A resting state Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (1994). Annotation: On dysfunction and function in
study of attention, default mode and salience networkconnectivity. Euro- psychological theories of childhood disorder. Journal of Child Psychology
pean Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 266(4), 349–357. and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 35(5), 801–815. https://doi.org/10
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-015-0630-0 .1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb02296.x
Silk, T. J., Beare, R., Malpas, C., Adamson, C., Vilgis, V., Vance, A., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Taylor, E., Sembi, S., & Smith, J. (1992). Hyperac-
Bellgrove, M. A. (2016). Cortical morphometry in attention deficit/ tivity and delay aversion—I. The effect of delay on choice. Journal of
hyperactivity disorder: Contribution of thickness and surface area to Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 33(2), 387–398.
volume. Cortex, 82, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.05.012 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00874.x
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behaviour. Macmillan. Steele, L. M., Pindek, S., & Margalit, O. (2021). The advantage of disad-
Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. Knopf. vantage: Is ADHD associated with idea generation at work? Creativity
Slade, M. (2010). Mental illness and well-being: The central importance of Research Journal, 33(3), 275–283. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419
positive psychology and recovery approaches. BMC Health Services .2021.1916368
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Research, 10(1), Article 26. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6963-10-26 Stenning, A., & Rosqvist, H. B. (2021). Neurodiversity studies: Mapping
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Slusarek, M., Velling, S., Bunk, D., & Eggers, C. (2001). Motivational out possibilities of a new critical paradigm. Disability & Society, 36(9),
effects on inhibitory control in children with ADHD. Journal of the 1532–1537. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2021.1919503
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(3), 355–363. Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200103000-00016 lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 289–298.
Smalley, S. L., McGough, J. J., Moilanen, I. K., Loo, S. K., Taanila, A., https://doi.org/10.1007/s004269900007
Ebeling, H., Hurtig, T., Kaakinen, M., Humphrey, L. A., McCracken, Surman, C. B. H., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Miller, C. A., Petty, C. R., &
J. T., Varilo, T., Yang, M. H., Nelson, S. F., Peltonen, L., & Järvelin, Faraone, S. V. (2015). Neuropsychological deficits are not predictive of
M. R. (2007). Prevalence and psychiatric comorbidity of attention-deficit/ deficient emotional self-regulation in adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention
hyperactivity disorder in an adolescent Finnish population. Journal of Disorders, 19(12), 1046–1053. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054713476548
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(12), Swanson, J., Baler, R. D., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Understanding the
1575–1583. https://doi.org/10.1097/chi.0b013e3181573137 effects of stimulant medications on cognition in individuals with attention-
Solanto, M. V. (2010). Cognitive-behavioural therapy for adult ADHD. deficit hyperactivity disorder: A decade of progress. Neuropsychophar-
Guildford Press. macology, 36(1), 207–226. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.160
Solanto, M. V., Abikoff, H., Sonuga-Barke, E., Schachar, R., Logan, G. D., Taylor, C. L., Zaghi, A. E., Kaufman, J. C., Reis, S. M., & Renzulli, J. S.
Wigal, T., Hechtman, L., Hinshaw, S., & Turkel, E. (2001). The ecological (2018). Characteristics of ADHD related to executive function: Differen-
validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impul- tial predictions for creativity-related traits. The Journal of Creative
sivity in AD/HD: A supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment study Behavior, 54(2), 350–362. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.370
of AD/HD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(3), 215–228. Thapar, A., Cooper, M., & Rutter, M. (2017). Neurodevelopmental dis-
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010329714819 orders. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(4), 339–346. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Solanto, M. V., Marks, D. J., Mitchell, K. J., Wasserstein, J., & Kofman, S2215-0366(16)30376-5
M. D. (2008). Development of a new psychosocial treatment for adult Tomasi, D., Volkow, N. D., Wang, R., Telang, F., Wang, G. J., Chang, L.,
ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11(6), 728–736. https://doi.org/10 Ernst, T., & Fowler, J. S. (2009). Dopamine transporters in striatum
.1177/1087054707305100 correlate with deactivation in the default mode network during visuospa-
Sonuga-Barke, E., Bitsakou, P., & Thompson, M. (2010). Beyond the dual tial attention. PLOS ONE, 4(6), Article e6102. https://doi.org/10.1371/
pathway model: Evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory, and journal.pone.0006102
delay-related impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Jour- Toner, M., O’Donoghue, T., & Houghton, S. (2006). Living in chaos and
nal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(4), striving for control: How adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
345–355. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-201004000-00009 deal with their disorder. International Journal of Disability Development and
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2003). The dual pathway model of AD/HD: An Education, 53(2), 247–261. https://doi.org/10.1080/10349120600716190
elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics. Neuroscience and Toplak, M. E., Connors, L., Shuster, J., Knezevic, B., & Parks, S. (2008).
Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(7), 593–604. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ne Review of cognitive, cognitive-behavioral, and neural-based interventions
ubiorev.2003.08.005 for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Clinical Psychol-
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2004). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperac- ogy Review, 28(5), 801–823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.008
tivity disorder: From common simple deficits to multiple developmental Toplak, M. E., Dockstader, C., & Tannock, R. (2006). Temporal information
pathways. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1231–1238. https://doi.org/10 processing in ADHD: Findings to date and new methods. Journal of
.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.008 Neuroscience Methods, 151(1), 15–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Spontaneous atten- .2005.09.018
tional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: A Tricomi, E., & DePasque, S. (2016). The role of feedback in learning and
neurobiological hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, motivation. In S. Kim, J. Reeve, & M. Bong (Eds.), Recent developments
31(7), 977–986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005 in neuroscience research on human motivation (Advances in Motivation
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., De Houwer, J., De Ruiter, K., Ajzenstzen, M., & and Achievement) (pp. 175–202). Emerald Group Publishing. https://
Holland, S. (2004). AD/HD and the capture of attention by briefly exposed doi.org/10.1108/S0749-742320160000019015
delay-related cues: Evidence from a conditioning paradigm. Journal of Tuckman, A. (2009). More attention, less deficit: Success strategies for
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 45(2), 274–283. adults with ADHD. Specialty Press, Inc.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00219.x Valera, E. M., Faraone, S. V., Murray, K. E., & Seidman, L. J. (2007). Meta-
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Williams, E., Hall, M., & Saxton, T. (1996). analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Hyperactivity and delay aversion III: The effect on cognitive style of disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61(12), 1361–1369. https://doi.org/10
imposing delay after errors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, .1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.011
ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, THEORY 603

Van Ewijk, H., Heslenfeld, D. J., Zwiers, M. P., Faraone, S. V., Luman, M., White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2011). Creative style and achievement in adults
Hartman, C. A., Hoekstra, P. J., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J. K., & Oosterlaan, with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Individ-
J. (2014). Different mechanisms of white matter abnormalities in atten- ual Differences, 50(5), 673–677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010
tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A diffusion tensor imaging study. .12.015
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2016). Scope of semantic activation and innovative
53(7), 790–799.e3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.001 thinking in college students with ADHD. Creativity Research Journal,
Vasile, C. (2015). Time perception, cognitive correlates, age and emotions. 28(3), 275–282. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2016.1195655
Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences, 187, 695–699. https://doi.org/ White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of compe-
10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.03.129 tence. Psychological Review, 66(5), 297–333. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Viering, T., Hoekstra, P. J., Philipsen, A., Naaijen, J., Dietrich, A., Hartman, h0040934
C. A., Buitelaar, J. K., Hildebrandt, A., Gießing, C., & Thiel, C. M. White, R. W. (1963). Ego and reality in psychoanalytic theory: A proposal
(2021). Functional network topology of the right insula affects emotion regarding independent ego energies. International Universities Press.
dysregulation in hyperactive—impulsive attention—deficit/hyperactivity Willcut, E. (2014). Theories of ADHD. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention
disorder. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598- deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

021-94426-8 (4th ed., pp. 391–404). Guilford Press.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Understanding self regulation. In Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F.
K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self regulation: (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/
Research, theory and applications (2nd ed., pp. 1–12). Guildford Press. hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry,
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., & Ding, Y. S. (2005). Imaging the 57(11), 1336–1346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006
effects of methylphenidate on brain dopamine: New model on its therapeutic Wolf, L. E., Simkowitz, P., & Carlson, H. (2009). College students with
actions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports,
57(11), 1410–1415. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.006 11(5), 415–421. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-009-0062-5
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Newcorn, J. H., Wolfers, T., Beckmann, C. F., Hoogman, M., Buitelaar, J. K., Franke, B., &
Telang, F., Fowler, J. S., Zhu, W., Logan, J., Ma, Y., Pradhan, K., Wong, Marquand, A. F. (2019). Individual differences v. the average patient:
C., & Swanson, J. M. (2009). Evaluating dopamine reward pathway in Mapping the heterogeneity in ADHD using normative models. Psychologi-
ADHD: Clinical implications. JAMA, 302(10), 1084–1091. https:// cal Medicine, 50(2), 314–323. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000084
doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1308 Wolgast, M., Lundh, L. G., & Viborg, G. (2013). Experiential avoidance as
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Newcorn, J. H., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., an emotion regulatory function: An empirical analysis of experiential
Telang, F., Fowler, J. S., Goldstein, R. Z., Klein, N., Logan, J., Wong, C., avoidance in relation to behavioral avoidance, cognitive reappraisal, and
& Swanson, J. M. (2011). Motivation deficit in ADHD is associated with response suppression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 42(3), 224–232.
dysfunction of the dopamine reward pathway. Molecular Psychiatry, https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2013.773059
16(11), 1147–1154. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2010.97 Young, S. (2000). ADHD children grown up: An empirical review. Coun-
Von Mises, L. (1990). Human action: A treatise on economics (3rd ed.). selling Psychology Quarterly, 13(2), 191–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Laissez-Faire Books. (Original work published 1948). 095150700411728
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Harvard University Press. Young, S., & Bramham, J. (2012). Cognitive-behavioural therapy for ADHD
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In The collected works of L.S. in adolescents and adults: A psychological guide to practice (2nd ed.).
Vygotsky: Vol. 1. Problems in general psychology (N. Minick, Trans.). Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119943440
Plenum. Young, S., Bramham, J., Gray, K., & Rose, E. (2008). A phenomenological
Wagner, D. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Giving in to temptation: The analysis of the experience of receiving a diagnosis and treatment of ADHD
emerging cognitive neuroscience of self-regulatory failure. In K. D. Vohs in adulthood: A partner’s perspective. Journal of Attention Disorders,
& R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, 12(4), 299–307. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054707311659
and applications (2nd ed., pp. 41–63). The Guilford Press. Zedelius, C. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). Mind wandering “Ahas” versus
Walker, N. (2021). Neuroqueer heresies: Notes on the neurodiversity mindful reasoning: Alternative routes to creative solutions. Frontiers in
paradigm, autistic empowerment, and post normal possibilities. Psychology, 6, Article 834. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00834
Autonomous Press, LLC. Zentall, S. S., Falkenberg, S. D., & Smith, L. B. (1985). Effects of color
Waltz, M. (2020). The production of the “normal” child: Neurodiversity stimulation and information on the copying performance of attention-
and the commodification of parenting. In H. B. Rosqvist, A. Stenning, & problem adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13(4), 501–
N. Chown (Eds.), Neurodiversity studies: A new critical paradigm 511. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00923137
(pp. 15–26). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429322297-3 Zylowska, L., Ackerman, D., Yang, M., Futrell, J., Horton, N., Hale, T. S.,
Wender, P. H. (1995). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Pataki, C., & Smalley, S. L. (2008). Mindfulness meditation training in
Oxford University Press. adults and adolescents with ADHD: A feasibility study. Journal of Attention
White, H. A. (2018). Thinking “outside the box”: Unconstrained creative Disorders, 11(6), 737–746. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054707308502
generation in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Journal
of Creative Behavior, 54(2), 472–483. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.382
White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Uninhibited imaginations: Creativity in
adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Indi- Received September 10, 2021
vidual Differences, 40(6), 1121–1131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005 Revision received July 17, 2022
.11.007 Accepted September 8, 2022 ▪

You might also like