Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Selected Determinants of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaint Reports

Author(s): William O. Bearden and Jesse E. Teel


Reviewed work(s):
Source: Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 20, No. 1 (Feb., 1983), pp. 21-28
Published by: American Marketing Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3151408 .
Accessed: 15/01/2013 12:26

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Marketing Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Journal of Marketing Research.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
I

0. BEARDENand JESSE E. TEEL*


WILLIAM

Data obtainedfrom 375 membersof a consumerpanel in a two-phase study of


consumerexperienceswith automobilerepairsand serviceswere used to examine
the antecedentsand consequencesof consumersatisfaction.Theresultssupportpre-
vious findingsthat expectationsand disconfirmationare plausibledeterminantsof
satisfaction,and suggest that complaintactivity may be includedin satisfaction/
dissatisfactionresearchas suggested by earlier descriptionsof consumercomplain-
ing behavior.

Selected Determinants of Consumer Satisfaction


and Complaint Reports
I

Consumer satisfaction is important to the marketer COMPLAININGBEHAVIOR2


because it is generally assumed to be a significant de-
terminant of repeat sales, positive word-of-mouth, and Complaining behavior research has been largely lim-
ited to retrospective self-reports of behavior which oc-
consumer loyalty. Satisfaction is important to the indi- curred prior to assessment of the antecedents of satis-
vidual consumer because it reflects a positive outcome
from the outlay of scarce resources and/or the fulfill- faction/dissatisfaction. Though this limitation stems
from the inherent shortcomings of cross-sectional data
ment of unmet needs (Day and Landon 1977; Landon collection designs typically used in consumer satisfac-
1977). tion research, surveys of sources of consumer dissatis-
Growing recognition of consumer satisfaction as a faction and reports of complaint actions after unsatisfac-
critical construct in marketing and consumer behavior have enhanced our
has generated substantial research interest in the pro- tory purchase experiences
cesses preceding judgments of satisfaction/dissatisfac- understandingof consumer problems and consumer ef-
forts to seek redress.
tion and the consequences of those decisions. This re-
search has consisted primarily of experimental Unsatisfactory purchases, though varying by product
or service category, appear to be prevalent. For exam-
investigations of consumer expectations (e.g., Anderson
1973; Cardozo 1965) and surveys of problem incidences ple, Andreasen and Best (1977) report that as many as
one in five purchase experiences results in some dissat-
and reports of redress-seeking activity (e.g., Andreasen
isfaction. Similarly, Day and Bodur (1978) and Day and
1977; Day and Ash 1979).1 In an effort to integrate the- Ash (1979) report frequent incidences of dissatisfaction
ory and prior empirical research in the area of consumer for services and durable products.
satisfaction, Oliver (1980a) reported the development Reaction to dissatisfaction in terms of complaining
and test of a model encompassing interrelationships
behavior and redress seeking, however, varies consid-
among expectations, disconfirmation, satisfaction, and
the traditional criteria of attitudes and intentions. Our erably. Frequently, consumers do not take action to al-
leviate marketplace problems. Day and Bodur (1978, p.
study is an attempt to extend this research effort by in- 265) found that reported cases of extreme dissatisfaction
corporating consumer complaining behavior into a the- for which no action of any kind was taken were 49.6%
oretical model of consumer satisfaction.

'For an excellent review of these experimental studies, see LaTour 2Day (1980) has classified consumer responses to dissatisfaction
into three categories: (1) redress seeking in which a specific remedy
and Peat (1979).
is sought, (2) complaining or communications for reasons other than
redress seeking, and (3) boycotting or personal decisions to discon-
*William 0. Bearden and Jesse E. Teel are Associate Professors tinue usage or patronage. For purposes of our study, consumer com-
of Marketing, University of South Carolina. plaint reports are used in the generic sense to include a range of these
activities.

21

Journal of Marketing Research


Vol. XX (February 1983), 21-8

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
22 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING FEBRUARY
RESEARCH, 1983

for nondurable products, 29.4% for durable products, Figure 2


and 23.2% for services. Similarly, Andreasen and Best CONSUMER
COMPLAINT BEHAVIORa
(1977, p. 96) report that well over half of all the non-
price purchase difficulties uncovered in their study pre-
cipitated no action. These results are corroboratedby the
earlier national survey of Warland, Herrmann, and Wil-
lits (1975) and a recent regional study reported by Shup-
trine and Wenglorz (1981).
These findings are discouraging for several reasons.
First, failure to express dissatisfaction prevents the con-
sumer from achieving redress from an unpleasant mar-
ketplace experience. Second, limited action on the part
of consumers may mask marketplace problems which
the firm could and/or should correct. Further, wide-
spread failure to express complaints limits the usefulness
of complaint data as a basis for policy. For example, aBased on Day and Landon (1977).
complaint data have been suggested as being useful for
analysis of consumer discontent over time and across tentions. Satisfaction/dissatisfaction is, in turn, pre-
products (e.g., Gronhaug and Arndt 1980). Munns sumed to influence subsequent attitudes, intentions, and
(1978) has advocated providing complaint data to con- complaint behavior.
sumers as prepurchase information. However, the lim- The hypothesized relationships between satisfaction
ited research on reactions to dissatisfaction and the lack and expectations and disconfirmation are based on
of representative data limit these potential applications. Oliver's (1980a, b) interpretation of Helson's (1964)
Our study was designed to further understanding of adaptation level theory which posits that one perceives
consumer satisfaction by integrating complaint behavior stimuli in relation to an adapted standard. Expectations
into an explanation of consumer satisfaction. The frame- are assumed to perform the function of an adaptation
work examined (Figure 1) reflects a revision of Oliver's level in that they define the standardagainst which sub-
(1980a, p. 463) original model to include complaint be- sequent performance is judged. Disconfirmation is as-
havior. Satisfaction is depicted as a function of con- sumed to serve as a major force causing movement away
sumer expectations operationalized as product attribute from the standard-the net effect of which is satisfaction
beliefs (Olson and Dover 1979) and disconfirmation. or dissatisfaction (Oliver 1981, p. 28).
Expectations and disconfirmation are presented as being Though a substantial body of research supports the
unrelated, additive, and exogenous to the system. As is hypothesized causal chain between beliefs, attitudes, and
consistent with the multiattribute explanation of atti- intentions, the effects of disconfirmation in conjunction
tudes, expectations/beliefs about product attributes are with expectations on satisfaction have received less at-
included as determinants of attitudes which precede in- tention. Some support for the additive and unrelated as-
sumption about expectations and disconfirmation was
Figure 1 provided by the zero order correlations reported by
Oliver (1980a). However, disconfirmation may also in-
THEORETICAL
ANTECEDENTSAND CONSEQUENCES
OF fluence satisfaction and postpurchase attitudes through
CONSUMER SATISFACTION an interactive relationship with expectations. In an effort
to help clarify this issue, an alternative version of the
model incorporating disconfirmation as a moderator
variable was tested (Sharma, Durand, and Gur-Arie
1981, p. 293).
All of the relationships with the exception of the path
between satisfaction and complaining are hypothesized
to be positive. For this negative path, satisfaction is as-
sumed to be related inversely to complaint behavior. The
nature of the satisfaction/dissatisfaction and complaint
process as conceptualized by Day and Landon (1977) is
shown in Figure 2. Though many factors may inhibit or
encourage complaint expressions, dissatisfaction is rec-
ognized as a primarydeterminantof legitimate consumer
complaints. As shown, the various actions that con-
sumers take may be subdivided into private and public
responses. Private actions include decisions to stop fur-
T2 ther purchases and warnings to friends; public actions

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
DETERMINANTS
OF CONSUMER
SATISFACTION 23

include redress-seeking efforts directed toward the seller attitude research (e.g., Fishbein and Ajzen 1975). Dis-
and complaints to third-party consumer affairs institu- confirmation of expectations and satisfaction were as-
tions. sessed by procedures similar to those described by
The potential for other individual and situational fac- Oliver (1980a). An index of complaint behavior was
tors to influence complaining behavior should be ac- developed on the basis of the research of Day and his
knowledged. Many of these factors have been examined colleagues into the range of actions consumers may take
in the consumer satisfaction/dissatisfaction literature after an unsatisfactory purchase experience. All scales
and include socioeconomic characteristics (Bourgeois were worded to reflect the automobile repair service con-
and Barnes 1979), personality differences (Fornell and text. An initial version of the questionnaire was tested
Westbrook 1979), the costs and benefits from complain- by a mail survey of the general population of a medium-
ing (Richins 1982), propensity to complain (Day and sized metropolitan area.
Landon 1977), and importance of purchase (Gr6nhaug Expectations were collected for the six service attri-
1977). Many of these influences on complaining behav- butes identified by Adler and Hlavacek (1978): (1) rea-
ior have been reviewed by Landon (1977) and Day and sonableness of costs, (2) location, (3) quality of repairs,
Landon (1977). (4) reputation of firm, (5) speed of service, and (6)
friendliness of management. Each was operationalized
METHOD as a seven-item bipolar statement labeled "likely"-"un-
Data were collected in a two-phase longitudinal study likely" and scored +3 to -3. As is consistent with the
with a four-month measurement interval from members multiattributedepiction of belief structure and the pro-
of a 1200-family, two-state consumer panel. Expecta- cedures suggested by Oliver (1980a), expectations were
tions, attitudes, intentions, satisfaction, and complaint formed as a sum of the six belief scores. Evaluation di-
reactions were collected for the use of automobile repair mensions were not included because of the assumptions
service outlets.3 The results described are based on the of unit positive evaluation for each attribute and over-
375 user adult respondents from an original sample of time stability of attribute evaluations. Attitudes toward
749 households participating in both waves. patronizingthe outlet were assessed by three seven-place
As part of the initial data collection, respondents were scaled statements labeled "good"-"bad," "wise"-
requested to provide the name and address of an auto "foolish," and "beneficial"-"harmful." Statements
service outlet which might be used during the coming were similar to: "My taking vehicles for needed repairs
months. Beliefs, attitudes, and intentions were then col- to (business) is. .. ." Behavioral intentions were op-
lected with respect to that outlet. These variables were erationalized via two seven-place scaled statements la-
measured again in a followup mailing which also as- beled "likely"-"unlikely" and "probable"-"improba-
sessed reports of complaint behavior. Panel members ble." Similar items were used in the followup
excluded from the final sample were respondents pro- questionnaire to represent both attitudes and intentions.
viding incomplete responses to one or both mailings, In the followup analyses, disconfirmation was as-
nonusers of the originally identified repair service facil- sessed by a single measure labeled "better than"-
ity, and those respondents who were not familiar with "worse than" reflecting each respondent's overall re-
a repair outlet. Names and addresses of the auto busi- actions to the disconfirmation of expectations. Satisfac-
nesses were also collected in the followup mailing for tion was defined via four "agree"-"disagree" state-
help in verification and matching of surveys. Fifty-four ments similar to those used by Oliver (1980a).4 The
percent of the respondents were men; 93% were white. satisfaction items were similar to the following two ex-
The median family income category was $25-30,000 amples: (1) "Our choice to use the repair outlet was a
and the respondents' age was 49.94 years. The 749 sam- wise one," or (2) "If I had it to do all over again, I
ple members were compared with the 451 nonpartici- would feel differently about using the business."
pants by means of the permanentdemographic file main- An index of complaint activity was constructed to rep-
tained on each panel member and updated annually. No resent complaint behavior. The operational measure of
significant differences were found in terms of demo- complaint behavior consisted of a scale containing re-
graphic characteristics. sponses to the personal and direct complaint alternatives
identified and investigated in prior studies of consumer
Operational Measures reactions to marketplace dissatisfaction (Day and Ash
Expectations, attitudes, and intentions were assessed 1979; Day and Bodur 1978; Day and Landon 1977).
by using multi-item measures commonly employed in These actions ranged from "warned family and friends"

3Problems with automobile repairs and service are one of the most 4Data were collected for the complete set of six items used by
frequently mentioned consumer complaints. Because of yearly ex- Oliver (1980a, p. 463) to operationalize satisfaction. Two of these
penditures now in excess of $30 billion and the pervasive use, im- items were omitted on the basis of the results of a reliability analysis
portance, and complexity of automobiles, consumer problems with (coefficient alpha) which indicated that the two items lowered the
auto repairs and service are worthy of study in themselves (Survey overall reliability of the scale and were apparently perceived as am-
1979; Webbink 1978). biguous by the respondents.

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
24 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING FEBRUARY
RESEARCH, 1983

Table 1
INDICATOR MEANS,AND STANDARD
CORRELATIONS, DEVIATIONS0

Repli-
Initial cation
Yi Y2 Y3 Y4 Y5 Y6 Y7 Y8 Y9 Yio Yii Y12 Y13 Y14 Y15 xI x2 Mean SD Mean SD
y, - .56 .55 .46 .43 .31 .31 .30 .34 .37 .44 .34 .34 .31 -.11 .65 .15 5.74 1.43 5.79 1.49
Y2 .70 - .81 .68 .69 .39 .40 .41 .31 .45 .49 .33 .34 .33 -.11 .66 .20 5.96 1.28 6.16 1.14
Y3 .70 .71 - .69 .71 .39 .39 .38 .33 .47 .51 .43 .40 .41 -.09 .66 .25 5.90 1.30 6.21 .98
,4 .66 .85 .63 .75 .20 .24 .27 .24 .30 .36 .29 .38 .38 -.08 .45 .15 6.26 1.21 6.42 1.04
Y5 .70 .83 .69 .90 - .26 .29 .28 .21 .28 .34 .29 .38 .38 -.01 .52 .08 6.28 1.18 6.46 .91
Y6 .50 .49 .43 .43 .46 - .83 .80 .70 .68 .72 .59 .65 .61 -.23 .50 .46 5.89 1.70 6.01 1.54
Y7 .49 .47 .36 .36 .42 .81 - .83 .69 .65 .73 .59 .60 .56 -.25 .46 .44 5.94 1.67 5.95 1.54
Y8 .42 .42 .30 .30 .39 .73 .90 - .75 .65 .72 .58 .63 .59 -.32 .45 .46 6.14 1.57 6.04 1.57
Y9 .44 .43 .33 .36 .35 .76 .77 .77 - .59 .64 .52 .60 .54 -.31 .37 .44 6.01 1.76 5.91 1.74
Yio .48 .48 .42 .38 .42 .70 .68 .68 .61 - .85 .73 .65 .61 -.34 .61 .48 5.96 1.33 5.95 1.44
Yii .49 .54 .42 .42 .43 .73 .72 .70 .66 .90 - .77 .75 .71 -.31 .59 .52 5.95 1.46 5.90 1.51
Y12 .50 .54 .46 .42 .42 .68 .69 .67 .65 .82 .83 - .71 .73 -.27 .48 .45 6.06 1.35 5.95 1.38
Yl3 .43 .47 .38 .38 .39 .66 .69 .70 .62 .87 .90 .80 - .85 -.26 .46 .45 5.99 1.64 6.02 1.62
Y14 .48 .54 .42 .43 .44 .72 .74 .74 .69 .84 .90 .81 .93 - -.26 .43 .41 6.04 1.59 5.98 1.62
Yl5 -.25 -.19 -.24 -.15 -.21 -.44 -.36 -.34 -.37 -.36 -.33 -.25 -.32 -.36 - -.28 -.34 .52 .93 .52 .94
x, .74 .70 .63 .62 .64 .54 .50 .40 .45 .49 .48 .50 .40 .45 .11 - .19 9.23 7.12 9.77 7.04
x2 .22 .21 .15 .19 .18 .50 .45 .39 .42 .36 .39 .33 .37 .42 .24 .28 - 4.37 1.55 4.44 1.40
'Correlations below the diagonal are based on the initial sample data; correlations above the diagonal are based on the replication sample.

to "contacted lawyer or took some legal action." The model to the data could be improved. Specifically, the
construct was operationalized as a Guttman scale in an two exogenous variables-expectations and disconfir-
effort to reflect increasing intensity of possible com- mation-were allowed to be correlated and four paths
plaint actions. The coefficients of reproducibility and among error terms within three of the constructs were
scalability were .98 and .78, respectively.5 freed. Two correlated errors were allowed among the
four satisfaction indicators and one each within the time
RESULTS 1 and time 2 attitudeconstructs. The resulting chi square
The 375 user respondents were split randomly into is 263.82 (103 d.f., p < .01). These results are shown
initial (n = 188) and replication (n = 187) samples. The in Figure 3.
models for both samples were estimated by the system Normally, a significant chi square value is taken as
estimationproceduresdeveloped by J6reskog and S6rbom evidence that the model is an inadequate fit or poor rep-
(1978) and reviewed by Aaker and Bagozzi (1979). Con- resentation of the data.7 Bentler and Bonett (1980) sug-
struct indicator intercorrelations, means, and standard gest computation of an incremental fit index for com-
deviations for both groups are reported in Table 1. paring a theoretical model with a relevant null model.
For purposes of our study, the null model chosen for
Initial Sample
comparison was that of no relationships in which only
Overall analysis. Analysis of the hypothesized model the error term paths were allowed to be free. This is
(see Figure 1) for the initial sample resulted in a chi analogous to the test of the hypothesis that the vector of
square statistic of 305.13 (108 d.f., p < .01).6 Exami- regression coefficients is the zero vector in a single cri-
nation of the first derivatives of the endogenous indicator terion variable multiple regression analysis.
error terms and tests of the model allowing additional Tests of this null model based on the initial sample
paths among constructs suggested that the fit of the resulted in a chi square value of 3599.43 (136 d.f.). This
finding suggests that the model does result in a substan-
tial improvement over the assumption of no relation-
5The items included, in order of increasing complaining intensity, ships. This conclusion is supported by a normalized in-
were: (1) warned family and friends, (2) returned vehicle for rework
and/or complained to management, (3) contacted manufacturer, (4)
contacted Better Business Bureau, state office of consumer affairs,
or private consumer agency, and (5) took some legal action. 7The split-half samples in this study were 188 and 187 respondents
6As is consistent with procedures used in multiwave studies, the for the initial and replication studies, respectively, and could not be
indicator coefficients for the two identical measures of attitude and described as especially large. However, the eight constructs and 17
of intentions were restricted to be equal over the two waves. These indicators result in a somewhat more complex framework than typi-
restrictions result in exactly equal estimates for the unstandardized cally used in most marketing applications. Though Bagozzi (1981)
parameter values; however, the standardized coefficients may differ. has suggested that LISREL is appropriateif the sample size minus the
Further, the structural error terms and the errors in measurement for degrees of freedom exceeds 50, the effects on the distribution of chi
the two identical attitude and the two intentions measures were al- square from small sample sizes and the use of nonmultivariate nor-
lowed to be correlated. mally distributed data are currently unknown.

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
OF CONSUMER
DETERMINANTS SATISFACTION 25

Figure 3
PATHANALYSIS RESULTSa

"Standardizedcoefficients for the initial sample analyses are shown above each arrow; the chi square statistic is 263.82 (103 d.f., p < .01).
Coefficients for the replication sample are shown in parentheses; the resulting chi square statistic is 179.94 (103 d.f., p < .01). All indicator
coefficients in both analyses are significant.
bSignificantin both analyses.
'Significant in only the replication (p < .01).

cremental fit index of .93 and a non-normed fit index amount of variance due to measurement errorrange from
of .90 (Bentler and Bonett 1980, p. 599). It is further .70 to .90.
supported by the average residual between the original Structural model. Eight of the 10 hypothesized paths
and reproduced correlations of .051 in comparison with are significant (p < .05) and each is consistent with the
an average input correlation of .521. hypothesized direction of influence. Expectations are
Measurement model. Each of the indicators is signif- related positively to attitudes as anticipated, and across
icant as a reflection of the unobservable construct it was time to satisfaction. Similarly, disconfirmation is a sig-
designed to assess. These coefficients and their error nificant predictor of satisfaction. Satisfaction, in turn,
terms are also shown in Figure 3. The measurement is significantly correlated with postpurchase attitudes.
model was analyzed furtherby the procedures suggested As is consistent with the normal depiction of attitude
by Bagozzi (1980) and Fornell and Larcker (1981). The structure, the strongest relationships are between atti-
resulting indicator and construct reliabilities and shared tudes and intentions within time periods. Last, satisfac-
variance estimates for both the initial and replication tion is related negatively to complaint behavior as hy-
analyses are reported in Table 2.8 In general, these es- pothesized. The two nonsignificant paths involve the
timates suggest adequate indicator and construct relia- relationship between disconfirmation and followup atti-
bilities. With the exception of one indicator in the atti- tudes and between the two intentions measures.
tude measures, all indicator reliabilities are above .70.
The construct reliability estimates range from .88 to .95. Replication Sample
The shared variance estimates representing the amount Overall analysis. Analysis of the model allowing the
of variance captured by the construct in relation to the exogenous variables to be correlated and the within-con-
struct correlated errors was replicated on the remaining
half of the sample. The resulting chi square for the rep-
lication analysis is 179.94 (103 d.f., p < .01). Test of
8Individualindicatorreliabilitiesfor those constructsmeasuredby the null model resulted in a chi square value of 2750.63
multiple items are computed from model estimates derived from the (136 d.f.). The normalized and non-normed incremental
LISREL analysis. Also, on the basis of the responses of 35 under-
test-retestreliability fit indices are .93 and .91, respectively. The average
graduatestudentsto a three-weekadministration,
estimatesfor summatedversionsof the expectations,attitude,andin- residual between the original input correlation matrix
tentionmeasureswere .70, .76, and .96, respectively. (averaging .454) and the reproduced correlations is .043.

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
26 OF MARKETING
JOURNAL FEBRUARY
RESEARCH, 1983

Table 2
RELIABILITY
AND SHARED ESTIMATES
VARIANCE

Indicator Construct
No. of reliability range reliability Shared variance
Construct indicators Initial Replication Initial Replication Initial Replication
Attitude (1) 3 .64-.83 .66-.81 .88 .87 .70 .70
Intentions (1) 2 .90-.90 .74-.76 .95 .85 .90 .75
Satisfaction 4 .76-.96 .66-.83 .95 .93 .82 .76
Attitude (2) 3 .71-.90 .72-.88 .94 .90 .84 .79
Intentions (2) 2 .90-.92 .88-.92 .96 .92 .92 .85

Measurement model. As in the initial sample analysis, isfaction and postpurchase attitudes, and this possibility
each of the indicator coefficients is significant. Exami- should be considered for future research.
nation of their reliabilities (see Table 2) and the mag- All-respondent analysis. With the caveat that corre-
nitude of their error variances indicates that the individ- lations among constructs may be caused by all measures
ual reliability estimates are above .60. The reliability assessing the same construct, correlations between pur-
estimates for the constructs with multiple indicators ported measures of intentions, attitudes, and attitude
range from .85 to .93. The shared variance estimates components have been accepted as evidence of nomo-
range from .70 for the first attitude construct to .85 for logical validity (Peter 1981). Consequently, a reduced
the second intentions variable. model incorporatingonly expectations, attitudes, and in-
Structural model. In the replication, nine of the 10 tentions was tested on all 749 user and nonuser respond-
proposed causal paths are significant. Again, each is ents. Again the indicators for identical constructs were
consistent with the hypothesized direction of influence. constrained to be equal over time and correlated mea-
The eight significant paths based on the initial sample surement errors were allowed.
analyses are also significant in the replication and are The magnitude and signs of the path coefficients
comparable in magnitude to the initial sample results. among the constructs closely parallel the relationships
The path between the two intentions measures is again shown in Figure 3. That is, the relationships between
not significant. expectations, attitudes, and intentions within time pe-
In sum, the results support the previous findings that riods are positive, relatively large, and significant. The
expectations and disconfirmation appear to be plausible indicator coefficients are again significant and provide
determinants of satisfaction. The traditional directional evidence of high construct and indicator reliability. The
relationships between beliefs, attitudes, and intentions correlations of the indicators with the constructs are
are also confirmed. Moreover, satisfaction is related higher in all cases for the construct each set of indicators
negatively to complaint activity as hypothesized. was designed to measure than those for constructs mea-
sured by different indicators. Though the resulting chi
Additional Tests
square value is comparable to the initial and replication
Disconfirmation as a moderator. In an effort to ex- user analyses (272.44, d.f. = 36, p < .01), given the
amine the effect of disconfirmation on the relationships smaller number of degrees of freedom, the overall fit of
between expectations and satisfaction and postpurchase the model is somewhat worse. This finding is not un-
attitudes, we tested an alternative model incorporating expected given the omission of the satisfaction construct
disconfirmation as a quasimoderator (Sharma, Durand, and the probable lower degree of involvement or famil-
and Gur-Arie 1981, p. 295) of these relationships for iarity with the repair service outlet for the nonusers.
both the initial and replication samples. A quasimoder-
ator variable is allowed to affect a criterion variable di- DISCUSSION
rectly and also indirectly through an interactive relation- These results provide additional support for the find-
ship with another variable. For this test, the model of ings of Oliver (1977, 1980a) on expectations and dis-
Figure 3 was reformulated with an additional exogenous confirmation as determinants of consumer satisfaction.
variable-expectations multiplied with disconfirmation. In both analyses, expectations and disconfirmation are
The resulting chi square values are 301.41 (116 d.f.) and related positively to satisfaction. Satisfaction, in turn,
194.87 (116 d.f.). The paths from the interaction vari- appears to partially mediate the relationship between
able to satisfaction and to postpurchase attitudes are not disconfirmation and followup attitudes.
significant for the initial sample (p < .05). The corre- Though our results are largely consistent with the re-
sponding path to satisfaction is significant in the repli- search hypotheses, several caveats are in order. First,
cation sample, but the path to postpurchase attitudes is the study is limited by its focus on only a single type of
not. Given the ambiguity of these results, we cannot rea- service and the use of a single-item, overall measure of
sonably either reject or accept disconfirmation as a mod- disconfirmation. The reliability of this measure is un-
erator of the influence of expectations on consumer sat- known because no test-retest analysis was performed.

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
OF CONSUMER
DETERMINANTS SATISFACTION 27

Second, in spite of measurement at two time periods tion measures badly need resolving. It should also be
separatedby four months, several constructs were mea- noted that, though tests of our model using disconfir-
sured simultaneously on the same questionnaire and are, mation as a moderator variable did not improve face
hence, subject to common response bias. Further, the validity of the results or appreciably the overall fit, we
complaint behavior measure is a unidimensional variable found some evidence that disconfirmation and expecta-
(i.e., from no actions to severe actions) and the corre- tions are correlated constructs.
lation of this measure with a bidirectional satisfaction The consumer satisfaction paradigm examined appears
measure may have been attenuated. Last, the large sig- capable of encompassing more concrete behavioral cri-
nificant chi square values suggest some model misspe- teria in a manner that is consistent with previous theories
cification which in this case is probably due to omission of consumer complaining (e.g., Andreasen 1977; Day
of constructs likely to influence such a complex behavior 1977; Day and Landon 1977; Landon 1977). Specifi-
as complaining (e.g., costs and benefits of complaining, cally, satisfaction is found to be related negatively to an
individual circumstances) and/or measurement error. index of complaint reports in both analyses. Because of
The study does, however, overcome some of the the positive contributions to both consumers and mar-
shortcomings of previous consumer satisfaction/dissat- keters that are possible from early expressions of dis-
isfaction research. The use of panel data helped avoid satisfaction, understanding the determinants of com-
the tendency to sample only extreme cases (typically plaint behavior seems important. The ability of a single
found in complaint files or self-reported in single sur- satisfaction variable to moderately explain complaint
veys covering the preceding 12 months). Further, the behavior is encouraging. Much as interpersonal and
integration of two existing and complementary theories, other affective influences have been shown to affect con-
i.e., explanations of both consumer satisfaction and sumer satisfaction (cf. Westbrook 1980), both internal
complaint behavior (topics which are normally not in- characteristics (e.g., perceived risk, assertiveness) and
cluded simultaneously in empirical research) circum- external structural factors (e.g., economic constraints,
vents further"overtheorizing" an area already criticized the benefits and costs from complaining) may also in-
for that tendency (Russo 1979). In contrast to Oliver's hibit or encourage complaint behavior (Richins 1980,
inoculation study, the focus on automobile services and 1982). Future efforts need to incorporate these variables
repairs represents a situation in which consumers select into further research on the determinants of consumer
from a number of alternative outlets and pay for ser- satisfaction and complaining behavior.
vices, and so more closely reflects normal consumer ac- Additionally, several other issues related to consumer
tivity. The availability of alternative outlets also pro- complaining behavior are unresolved and in need of con-
vides the consumer with more ways to respond to an sideration. For example, the role of the relative intensity
unsatisfactory experience (e.g., changing outlets). of satisfaction and the interaction of (cf. Day 1980) dis-
satisfaction with the benefits (or the potential payoffs)
Implicationsfor Satisfaction Research from complaining need addressing. Further, the con-
Several issues pertaining to expectations and discon- sumer complaining behavior construct itself warrants
firmation as determinants of satisfaction remain to be additional conceptualization and measurement attention.
addressed. For the relationship between expectations and
satisfaction, several possibilities seem plausible. First, REFERENCES
satisfaction as operationalized may be simply another
form of attitude. In this case, the expectations -> sat- Aaker,DavidA. andRichardP. Bagozzi (1979), "Unobserv-
isfaction link represents another approximation of the able Variablesin StructuralEquationModels with an Ap-
general depiction of attitude structure. Second, because plicationin IndustrialSelling," Journal of MarketingRe-
users were the focus of the study, their responses may search, 16 (May), 147-58.
have been biased by attempts to respond consistently Adler,Lee andJamesD. Hlavacek(1978), "Key RepairSer-
vice Factors for ConsumerDurable Goods," Journal of
with the first wave of data collection. One could also MarketingResearch, 15 (November),634-8.
argue that higher levels of satisfaction are reported in Anderson,RolphE. (1973), "ConsumerDissatisfaction:The
efforts to rationalize earlier high expectations. Oliver Effect of DisconfirmedExpectancyon Perceived Product
(1980b) has suggested that certain dogmatic tendencies Performance,"Journalof MarketingResearch, 10 (Febru-
resulting from involvement or commitment may make ary) 38-44.
some individuals rely more heavily on expectation lev- Andreasen,Alan R. (1977), "A Taxonomyof ConsumerSat-
els. isfaction/DissatisfactionMeasures,"Journal of Consumer
For disconfirmation, it is apparent that there is still a Affairs, 11 (Winter),11-23.
and ArthurBest (1977), "ConsumersComplain-
need for further research into why consumers displace Does Business Respond," Harvard Business Review, 55
themselves at various points along the disconfirmation
(July-August),93-101.
continuum. Problems remain in the operationalization of Bagozzi, R. B. (1980), Causal Models in Marketing.New
disconfirmation. As suggested by Day (1980) and Oliver York:JohnWiley & Sons, Inc.
(1980a), issues of the relative appropriateness and effi- (1981), "EvaluatingStructuralEquationModels with
cacy of overall versus individual attribute disconfirma- UnobservableVariablesand MeasurementError:A Com-

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
28 JOURNALOF MARKETINGRESEARCH,FEBRUARY1983

ment," Journal of Marketing Research, 18 (August), 375- and Methodological Issues in Consumer Satisfaction Re-
81. search," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 6, W.
Bentler, P. M. and Douglas G. Bonett (1980), "Significance L. Wilkie, ed. Miami: Association for Consumer Research,
Tests and Goodness of Fit in the Analysis of Covariance 431-7.
Structures," Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606. Munns, Joyce M. (1978), "Consumer Complaints as Pre-pur-
Bourgeois, J. C. and James G. Barnes (1979), "Viability and chase Information:An Evaluation of Better Business Bureau
Profile of the Consumerist Segment," Journal of Consumer Reports to Consumers," Journal of Consumer Affairs, 12
Research, 5 (March), 217-28. (Summer), 76-87.
Cardozo, Richard N. (1965), "An Experimental Study of Oliver, Richard L. (1977), "Effect of Expectation and Dis-
Consumer Effort, Expectation and Satisfaction," Journal of confirmation on Postexposure Product Evaluations: An Al-
Marketing Research, 2 (August), 244-9. ternative Interpretation," Journal of Applied Psychology,
Day, Ralph L. (1977), "Extending the Concept of Consumer 62, 480-6.
Satisfaction," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 4, (1980a), "A Cognitive Model of the Antecedents and
W.D. Perreault, ed. Atlanta: Association for Consumer Re- Consequences of Satisfaction Decisions," Journal of Mar-
search, 149-54. keting Research, 17 (November), 460-9.
(1980), "Research Perspectives on Consumer Com- (1980b), "Theoretical Bases of Consumer Satisfaction
plaining Behavior," in Theoretical Developments in Mar- Research: Review, Critique, and Future Direction," in The-
keting, C. W. Lamb and P. M. Dunne, eds. Chicago: oretical Developments in Marketing, C. W. Lamb and P.
American Marketing Association, 211-15. M. Dunne, eds. Chicago: American Marketing Association,
and Stephen B. Ash (1979), "Consumer Response to 206-10.
Dissatisfaction with Durable Products," in Advances in (1981), "Measurement and Evaluation of Satisfaction
Consumer Research, Vol. 6, W. L. Wilkie, ed. Miami: Processes in Retail Settings," Journal of Retailing, 57
Association for Consumer Research, 438-44. (Fall), 25-47.
and Muzaffer Bodur (1978), "Consumer Response to Olson, JerryC. and Philip A. Dover (1979), "Disconfirmation
Dissatisfaction with Services and Intangibles," in Advances of Consumer Expectations Through Product Trial," Journal
in Consumer Research, Vol. 5, H. Keith Hunt, ed. Ann of Applied Psychology, 64, 179-89.
Arbor: Association for Consumer Research, 263-72. Peter, J. Paul (1981), "Construct Validity: A Review of Basic
and E. Laird Landon (1977), "Toward a Theory of Issues and Marketing Practices," Journal of Marketing Re-
Consumer Complaining Behavior," in Consumer and In- search, 18 (May), 133-45.
dustrial Buying Behavior, A. G. Woodside, J. N. Sheth, Richins, Marsha L. (1980), "Consumer Perceptions of Costs
and P. D. Bennett, eds. New York: North-Holland, 425- and Benefits Associated With Complaining," in Refining
37. Concepts and Measures of Consumer Satisfaction and Com-
Fishbein, Martin and Icek Ajzen (1975), Belief, Attitude, In- plaining Behavior, H. Keith Hunt and Ralph L. Day, eds.
tention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Re- Bloomington: Indiana University, 50-3.
search. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co. (1982), "An Investigation of Consumers' Attitudes
Fornell, Claes and David F. Larcker (1981), "Evaluating Toward Complaining," in Advances in Consumer Research,
Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables Vol. 9, Andrew Mitchell, ed. St. Louis: Association for
and Measurement Error,"Journal of Marketing Research, Consumer Research (forthcoming).
18 (February), 39-50. Russo, J. Edward (1979), "Consumer Satisfaction/Dissatis-
and Robert A. Westbrook (1979), "An Exploratory faction: An Outsider's View," in Advances in Consumer
Study of Assertiveness, Aggressiveness, and Consumer Research, Vol. 6, W. L. Wilkie, ed. Miami: Association
Complaining Behavior," in Advances in Consumer Re- for Consumer Research, 453-5.
search, Vol. 6, W. L. Wilkie, ed. Miami: Association for Sharma, Subhash, Richard M. Durand, and Oded Gur-Arie
Consumer Research, 105-10. (1981), "Identification and Analysis of Moderator Vari-
Gronhaug, Kjell (1977), "Exploring Consumer Complaining ables," Journal of Marketing Research, 18 (August), 291-
Behavior: A Model and Some Empirical Results," in Ad- 300.
vances in Consumer Research, Vol. 4, W. D. Perreault, ed. Shuptrine, F. Kelly and GerhardWenglorz (1981), "Compre-
Atlanta: Association for Consumer Research, 159-65. hensive Identification of Consumers' Marketplace Problems
and Johan Arndt (1980), "Consumer Dissatisfaction and What They Do About Them," in Advances in Con-
and Complaining Behavior as Feedback: A Comparative sumer Research, Vol. 8, Kent Monroe, ed. Washington:
Analysis of Public and Private Delivery Systems," in Ad- Association-for Consumer Research, 687-92.
vances in Consumer Research, Vol. 7, Jerry Olson, ed. San "Survey of Automobile Repair Services" (1979), Office of
Francisco: Association for Consumer Research, 324-8. the Secretary of Transportation, Division of Consumer Af-
Helson, Harry (1964), Adaptation Level Theory. New York: fairs, Washington, DC.
Harper and Row. Warland, Rex H., Robert O. Herrmann, and Jane Willits
Joreskog, Karl and Dag Sorbom (1978), LISRELIV: Analysis (1975), "Dissatisfied Consumers: Who Gets Upset and Who
of Linear Structural Relationships by the Method of Maxi- Takes Action," Journal of Consumer Affairs, 9 (Winter),
mum Likelihood. Chicago: National Educational Resources, 148-63.
Inc. Webbink, Douglas W. (1978), "Automotive Repair: Does
Landon, E. Laird (1977), "A Model of Consumer Complaint Regulation or Consumer Information Matter," Journal of
Behavior," in Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction, and Consumer Research, 5 (December), 206-9.
Complaining Behavior, Ralph L. Day, ed. Bloomington: Westbrook, Robert A. (1980), "Intrapersonal Affective Influ-
Indiana University, 31-5. ences Upon Consumer Satisfaction with Products," Journal
LaTour, Stephen A. and Nancy C. Peat (1979), "Conceptual of Consumer Research, 7 (June), 49-54.

This content downloaded on Tue, 15 Jan 2013 12:26:20 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like