Full Ebook of Handbook of Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders Dean Mckay Online PDF All Chapter

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 69

Handbook of Child and Adolescent

Anxiety Disorders Dean Mckay


Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmeta.com/product/handbook-of-child-and-adolescent-anxiety-disorders-d
ean-mckay/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

McDonald and Avery's Dentistry for the Child and


Adolescent, 11th Edition Jeffrey A. Dean

https://ebookmeta.com/product/mcdonald-and-averys-dentistry-for-
the-child-and-adolescent-11th-edition-jeffrey-a-dean/

Infant, Child and Adolescent Nutrition: A Practical


Handbook 2nd Edition Judy More

https://ebookmeta.com/product/infant-child-and-adolescent-
nutrition-a-practical-handbook-2nd-edition-judy-more/

The Cambridge Handbook of Anxiety and Related Disorders


1st Edition Bunmi O Olatunji Editor

https://ebookmeta.com/product/the-cambridge-handbook-of-anxiety-
and-related-disorders-1st-edition-bunmi-o-olatunji-editor/

Study Guide to Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Second


Edition A Companion to Dulcan s Textbook of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry Third Edition Tapan Parikh

https://ebookmeta.com/product/study-guide-to-child-and-
adolescent-psychiatry-second-edition-a-companion-to-dulcan-s-
textbook-of-child-and-adolescent-psychiatry-third-edition-tapan-
Child and Adolescent Psychopathology for School
Psychology Terry Diamanduros

https://ebookmeta.com/product/child-and-adolescent-
psychopathology-for-school-psychology-terry-diamanduros/

Child and Adolescent Psychopathology 3rd Edition


Theodore P. Beauchaine

https://ebookmeta.com/product/child-and-adolescent-
psychopathology-3rd-edition-theodore-p-beauchaine/

Case Studies in Abnormal Child and Adolescent


Psychology 1st Edition Robert Weis

https://ebookmeta.com/product/case-studies-in-abnormal-child-and-
adolescent-psychology-1st-edition-robert-weis/

Eating Disorders and Child Sexual Abuse 1st Edition


Lisa Hodge

https://ebookmeta.com/product/eating-disorders-and-child-sexual-
abuse-1st-edition-lisa-hodge/

Handbook of Adolescent Digital Media Use and Mental


Health 1st Edition Jacqueline Nesi

https://ebookmeta.com/product/handbook-of-adolescent-digital-
media-use-and-mental-health-1st-edition-jacqueline-nesi/
Dean McKay
Eric A. Storch Editors

Handbook
of Child and
Adolescent
Anxiety Disorders
Second Edition
Handbook of Child and Adolescent
Anxiety Disorders
Dean McKay • Eric A. Storch
Editors

Handbook of Child
and Adolescent Anxiety
Disorders
Second Edition
Editors
Dean McKay Eric A. Storch
Department of Psychology Department of Psychiatry
Fordham University and Behavioral Sciences
Bronx, NY, USA Baylor College of Medicine
Houston, TX, USA

ISBN 978-3-031-14079-2    ISBN 978-3-031-14080-8 (eBook)


https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-14080-8

© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature
Switzerland AG 2011, 2023
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are solely and exclusively licensed by the Publisher,
whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation,
reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any
other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation,
computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors, and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor
the authors or the editors give a warranty, expressed or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains
neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
Contents

Part I

1 
Classification of Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders ����������   3
Dean McKay and Eric A. Storch
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias
and Phobic Conditions��������������������������������������������������������������������   7
Mark B. Powers, Kiara Leonard, Maris Adams, Emma Turner,
Jamie R. Pogue, Marjorie L. Crozier, Emily Carl,
and Seth J. Gillihan
3 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Considering Generalized
Anxiety Disorder, Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder,
and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder������������������������������������������������ 29
Nicole Fleischer, Jonathan Rabner, Julia Spandorfer,
and Philip C. Kendall
4 Cognitive Behavioral Models of Phobias
and Pervasive Anxiety���������������������������������������������������������������������� 45
Dean McKay
5 
Neurochemistry of Childhood Anxiety Disorders ������������������������ 55
Lauren Havel, Pranav Mehta, Ankit Gautam,
Edward Danielyan, and Kirti Saxena
6 
Genetics of Childhood and Adolescent Anxiety
and Obsessive-­Compulsive Disorders�������������������������������������������� 73
Paul D. Arnold, Lilit Antonyan, Francis Routledge,
and Sandra Meier
7 Taxometric Methods in Child and Adolescent
Anxiety Disorders���������������������������������������������������������������������������� 97
Christian A. Hall and Joshua J. Broman-Fulks
8 
Dimensional Diagnosis of Anxiety in Youth ���������������������������������� 111
Dean McKay
9 Transdiagnostic Treatment Models for Child
and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders�������������������������������������������������� 125
Ashley R. Karlovich, Elizabeth R. Halliday,
and Jill Ehrenreich-May

v
vi Contents

Part II

10 Differential
 Diagnosis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity
Disorder (ADHD) in Child and Adolescent Anxiety �������������������� 141
Alasdair Vance, Jo Winther, and Elham Shoorcheh
11 Comorbid
 and Secondary Depression in Child
and Adolescent Anxiety������������������������������������������������������������������� 157
Robert W. Garvey, Michelle K. Hiner, Chris A. Kelly,
and Margaret S. Andover
12 The
 Role of Disgust in Childhood Anxiety Disorders ������������������ 173
Ana Rabasco and Dean McKay
13 Problems
 in Emotion Regulation in Child and Adolescent
Anxiety Disorders Section: Diagnostic Components
of Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders ���������������������������������� 191
Kristel Thomassin, Marni L. Jacob, Kara B. West,
Molly E. Hale, and Cynthia Suveg
14 Emergent
 Personality Features in Adolescent
Anxiety Disorders���������������������������������������������������������������������������� 203
Amanda Venta and Jaime L. Anderson
15 Family
 Components of Child and Adolescent
Anxiety Disorders���������������������������������������������������������������������������� 217
Amanda Palo and Abigail Candelari

Part III

16 Specific
 Phobias in Children and Adolescents������������������������������ 235
Thompson E. Davis III, Jerrica Guidry,
and Thomas H. Ollendick
17 Separation
 Anxiety Disorder in Children and Adolescents���������� 249
Nicole E. Caporino, Joyce X. Wong,
and Arianna O’Brien Cannon
18 The
 Treatment of Generalized Anxiety Disorder in Youth���������� 271
Carl F. Weems and R. Enrique Varela
19 Selective Mutism������������������������������������������������������������������������������ 287
Karin L. Price, Natalie Delgado, and Kelly N. Banneyer
20 Treatment
 of Social Anxiety in Children and Adolescents ���������� 299
Tracy L. Morris and Johann D’Souza
21 Treatment
 of Pediatric Post-­traumatic Stress Disorder �������������� 315
Elissa J. Brown, Komal Sharma-Patel, Kaitlin Happer,
and Amy Hyoeun Lee
Contents vii

22 Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder in Children


and Adolescents�������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 331
Allie N. Townsend, Johann M. D’Souza, Andrew G. Guzick,
and Eric A. Storch
23 Pharmacological Treatment of Anxiety Disorders
in Children and Adolescents ���������������������������������������������������������� 347
Sohail Nibras, Anh Truong, and Laurel L. Williams
24  Common Mechanism for Anxiety Disorders
A
and Drug Addiction: Implications for Current
and Novel Pharmacological Treatments���������������������������������������� 357
Marco A. Grados and Bushra Rizwan

Part IV

25 Food Neophobia in Children: Misnomer, Anxious Arousal,


or Other Emotional Avoidance? ���������������������������������������������������� 367
Dean McKay and Charlene Minaya
26 
Anxiety-Related Problems in Developmental Disabilities������������ 379
Morgan M. McNeel, Emily R. Jellinek, and Eric A. Storch
27 Treatment of Youth Anxiety in the Context of Family
Dysfunction and Accommodation�������������������������������������������������� 395
Rebecca G. Etkin and Eli R. Lebowitz
28 Parent Training for Childhood Anxiety ���������������������������������������� 411
Adam B. Lewin and Kelly Kudryk
29 School-Based Interventions for Child
and Adolescent Anxiety������������������������������������������������������������������� 425
Jeremy K. Fox, Samantha Coyle, Taylor Walls, Avi Kalver,
Marcus Flax, Aleta Angelosante, and Carrie Masia Warner
30 
Social Disability and Impairment in Childhood Anxiety ������������ 445
Laura John-Mora, Abigail M. Ross, and Jordana Muroff

Index���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 469
Part I
Classification of Child
and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders 1
Dean McKay and Eric A. Storch

Classification in psychopathology has moved classify newly discovered entities. Again, contro-
through several important stages, based on the tra- versies exist (e.g., cloud theory versus heliocen-
jectory of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual tric theory of atomic structure; Cox, 1996), but
from its first edition to the current, fifth edition. these do not substantially alter the manner of uti-
The initial two editions were marked by a unify- lizing the classification system.
ing theoretical basis whereby specific diagnoses Unlike other branches of science, however,
were conceptualized in psychodynamic terms. psychiatry, psychology, and their associated pro-
This tradition is similar to the formulation of tax- fessions are not unified by a single theory of mind,
onomies in other branches of science. For exam- and most conditions likely have multiple determi-
ple, in biology the reliance on a hierarchical nants. Further, most users of the original DSMs
arrangement from kingdom down to species is noted the limited reliability of the taxonomy it laid
based on a specific theoretical framework whereby out, and with the third edition came a radical
all newly discovered organisms may be readily change in how psychiatric classification was con-
classified. While not totally without controversy, ceptualized: purely descriptive and atheoretical.
such as the movement toward cladistics (whereby This allowed users to arrive at diagnoses with
organisms are classified by ancestry rather than much greater precision, and the aim was to estab-
present biological structure; Scott-Ram, 2008; lish a set of conditions that had ecological and syn-
Williams & Ebach, 2020), these represent mere dromal validity. This has served the field well and
refinements rather than sea-­change level altera- has led to important advances in assessment, treat-
tions in classification. Another example is in ment, and etiological understanding. However,
chemistry, where elements are classified by a the- unlike classification systems in other branches of
ory-driven framework regarding the organization science, should a new condition arise, there is no
of atoms, with specifications within the periodic inherent mechanism for classifying it. Instead, any
table of elements (such as noble gases, metals, new diagnosis must wait until the revisions are
etc.) that also readily guides researchers in how to planned for the next edition of the DSM, where-
upon the proposed diagnosis is determined by
D. McKay (*) committee. The current edition (the DSM-5-TR;
Department of Psychology, Fordham University,
American Psychiatric Association, 2022) lays out
Bronx, NY, USA
e-mail: mckay@fordham.edu diagnoses in a single-­axial framework that remains
committee-driven. The recent text revision aims to
E. A. Storch
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, address racial and cultural disparities present in
Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA prior editions (Canady, 2022).

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2023 3


D. McKay, E. A. Storch (eds.), Handbook of Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-14080-8_1
4 D. McKay and E. A. Storch

This process of committee-driven descriptive LeDoux, 2009; LeDoux & Schiller, 2009; Marek
diagnoses has led to a growth of diagnoses, but & Sah, 2018). This leaves us with the curious
rarely have any been eliminated. In the anxiety problem of a biologically based theoretical
disorders, two disorders stand out as illustrative framework from which many disorders will be
of this point. On the one hand, agoraphobia with- classified but that also fails to adequately explain
out history of panic has been in the DSM since a serious and debilitating condition that is con-
the arrival of the third edition. However, this par- sidered a putative member. Moreover, although
ticular diagnosis has long been recognized as other etiological features are considered in this
either so rare as to not exist, or when actually framework, there is the distinct possibility of an
diagnosed to likely have had a panic-related ori- inaccurate bias towards a neurobiological expla-
gin even if panic is completely and successfully nation for disorders against a more holistic under-
avoided by the sufferer (McNally, 1994). On the standing that incorporates multiple
other hand, of the very few diagnoses that have determinants.
been eliminated is overanxious disorder of child-
hood. Interestingly, this diagnosis was only elim-
inated in that the criteria for a different disorder  urrent Standing of Childhood
C
(generalized anxiety disorder) were extended to Anxiety Diagnosis
encompass diagnosis in children. While these are
but two examples, they typify the approach to the At the present time, most of the anxiety disorders
DSM as contemporarily construed. Committees in the DSM are age-downward extensions of
determine putative diagnoses for larger catego- adult diagnoses. The exceptions to this are sepa-
ries, based on the existing literature on etiology, ration anxiety, school refusal, and selective mut-
incidence, and prevalence. At the same time, ism. While there are exceptions, all anxiety
existing diagnoses are very rarely eliminated, disorders, when present in children, have unique
even if infrequently encountered or its basis is manifestations that call for special clinical skill
seriously questioned by the broader community in assessment and intervention. One important
of researchers for that disorders’ member class. distinction between childhood and adult manifes-
The advent of the DSM-5 has included several tations of anxiety is that it is not required or even
new diagnoses and categories. For example, there expected that children have clear insight into the
is now the obsessive-compulsive related disor- nature of their fears. The only adult disorder
ders, which includes the new diagnoses of hoard- where insight is not required is OCD (now part of
ing disorder (a condition rarely present in a separate class of disorders, but still marked by
children), and excoriation disorder. No anxiety extreme anxiety), and in this case, the modifier
disorders were eliminated in the most recent “with poor insight” or “with mixed insight” is
version. available in the DSM-5-TR, and this manifesta-
When the first edition of this book was being tion has come under specific scrutiny as a poor
prepared, the DSM-5 was in the later stages of prognostic indicator for treatment response (i.e.,
development. At the time, many of the proposed McKay et al., 2010).
changes for the fifth edition of the DSM included A second major distinction involves the
the potential role of fear circuitry modeling for behavioral manifestation of different anxiety dis-
conceptualizing candidate disorders (i.e., Britton orders. In children, it is not unusual for the pre-
& Rauch, 2009). This did not fully manifest in sentation to have clear developmental
the final version of the manual. Additionally, the consequences. For example, children with school
various proposed neural circuitry models did not refusal, when untreated, face significant develop-
readily account for learning processes that might mental limitations resulting from reduced social-
influence changes in the connectivity among ization and limited opportunities for establishing
putative brain areas, despite repeated findings normative age-related behaviors. This is likewise
from neuroscientists to the contrary (Debiec & true in social anxiety and the dimensionally less
1 Classification of Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders 5

severe problem of chronic shyness, whereby the such as behavioral and cognitive theory (Taylor
inhibition associated with the disorder leads to et al., 2009).
developmental lags (Beidel & Turner, 2006; This leaves the field in a difficult predicament.
Ranta et al., 2015). Given the importance of It appears that, in consolidating a research agenda
socialization to cognitive and emotional growth that would advance our approach to classifica-
(Konner, 2010), when treating children with anx- tion, it will be necessary to identify the variables
iety disorders, it is often also necessary to attend associated with the greatest amount of variance
to socialization problems resulting from the in the developmental trajectory of anxiety per se
avoidance behaviors involved. and all its manifestations. This would no doubt
A third major distinction involves the role of narrow the class of disorders, but would also
caregivers in the etiology and maintenance of allow for a comprehensive theory for which clas-
anxiety. While this is in part associated with sification would readily flow without simply
socialization (i.e., Lawrence et al., 2019; Okagaki adhering to a diagnosis by committee approach
& Luster, 2005), it is unique in that each is mutu- to classification. It would also require that
ally dependent. This unique association can lead researchers remain open to a wide range of dispa-
to anxiety problems in children if one or both rate theoretical influences (i.e., biological, psy-
caregivers are themselves anxious, or if they chological, developmental) to converge into a
engage in behaviors designed to accommodate single meaningful theoretical framework.
anxious avoidance (see Chap. 20, this volume). Since the state of the field is not integrated
This is distinct from mere genetic transmission, into a meaningful theoretical framework, there
since there are specific behaviors parents may are numerous perspectives on conceptualization,
exhibit that propagate anxiety exclusive of herita- diagnosis, and treatment. It is our hope that the
bility by virtue of reducing the child’s anxiety. field will continue to advance whereby the mul-
Indeed, genetic data has been inconclusive with tiple perspectives in the field may be meaning-
respect to transmission of anxiety disorders, fully integrated to allow practitioners to
while behavioral theory has offered an empiri- seamlessly provide high quality services. In the
cally robust method of describing disorder onset meantime, this text is intended to provide a criti-
and maintenance (see Chap. 20, this volume). cal analysis of the state of the field in child and
Instead, it could be better stated that anxiety adolescent anxiety disorders across multiple per-
begets anxiety, but that there are no specific risks spectives. Since the publication of the first edi-
conferred on individual anxiety disorders. tion of this text, there has not, unfortunately, been
much progress on developing an integrated theo-
retical framework.
 uture Directions in Classifying
F
Childhood Anxiety Disorders
Structure of the Present Text
The adequacy of a purely descriptive model of
psychopathology, with specific reference to anxi- We have arranged the book into four major sec-
ety disorders in childhood, is limited. Formerly, tions. The first is a foundational section related to
theoretically driven models seem inadequate diagnostic issues and the directions anticipated in
given the difficulties in operationalization and the coming years with respect to anxiety classifi-
reliability (such as that noted in the early editions cation in children. The second section examines
of the DSM). Modern medical conceptualiza- the full scope of alternative ways of classifying,
tions (such as the fear circuitry) do not yet have the adequacy of these approaches, and limita-
adequate empirical support to use in developing a tions, as well as complicating factors in anxiety
classification scheme. Further, purely biological disorder. The third section is devoted to specific
models are often viewed as overly reductionistic, childhood anxiety disorders and their treatment,
ignoring other important sources of influence as well as integrative approaches to therapy (such
6 D. McKay and E. A. Storch

as cognitive-behavioral therapy and psychophar- on the anxiety disorders: Implications for DSM-V and
beyond (pp. 107–126). Springer.
macology). Finally, the fourth section covers Konner, M. (2010). The evolution of childhood:
novel and emergent areas within the anxiety dis- Relationships, emotion, mind. Harvard University
orders in children. Press.
It is our hope that this book will serve the mul- Lawrence, P. J., Murayama, K., & Creswell, C. (2019).
Systematic review and meta-analysis: Anxiety and
tiple goals of providing clinicians with a deeper depressive disorders in offspring of parents with anxi-
understanding of the full breadth of childhood ety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of
anxiety disorders, their assessment, treatment, Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58, 46–60.
and a critical understanding of classification. We LeDoux, J. E., & Schiller, D. (2009). The human amyg-
dala: Insights from other animals. In P. J. Whelan &
also hope that this book will advance multiple E. A. Phelps (Eds.), The human amygdala (pp. 43–60).
research agendas such as those in specific anxiety Guilford.
disorders, as well as in areas that are debilitating Marek, R., & Sah, P. (2018). Neural circuits mediating
but have as yet received limited research scrutiny. fear learning and extinction. Systems Neuroscience,
21, 35–48.
Finally, and perhaps most importantly, we hope McKay, D., Taylor, S., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2010).
that this book will vastly improve the lives of Obsessive-compulsive disorder. In D. McKay, J. S.
children affected by anxiety disorders. Abramowitz, & S. Taylor (Eds.), Cognitive –behav-
ior therapy for refractory cases: Turning failure
into success (pp. 89–109). American Psychological
Association.
References McNally, R. J. (1994). Panic disorder: A critical analysis.
Guilford.
American Psychiatric Association. (2022). Diagnostic Okagaki, L., & Luster, T. (2005). Research on parental
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed., socialization of child outcomes: Current controver-
text revision). Author. sies and future directions. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki
Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2006). Shy children, pho- (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological perspective (pp. 377–
bic adults: Nature and treatment of social anxiety dis- 410). Erlbaum.
order (2nd ed.). American Psychological Association. Ranta, K., La Greca, A. M., Garcia-Lopez, J., &
Britton, J. C., & Rauch, S. L. (2009). Neuroanatomy and Marttunen, M. (2015). Social anxiety and phobia in
neuroimaging of anxiety disorders. In M. M. Antony adolescents. Springer Nature.
& M. B. Stein (Eds.), Oxford handbook of anxiety and Scott-Ram, N. R. (2008). Transformed cladistics, taxon-
related disorders (pp. 97–110). Oxford University omy, and evolution. Cambridge University Press.
Press. Taylor, S., Asmundson, G. J. G., Abramowitz, J. S., &
Canady, V. A. (2022). DSM-5 text revision reflects updates McKay, D. (2009). Classification of anxiety disorders
in ethnic, racial issues. Mental Health Weekly, 32, 5–6. for DSM-V and ICD-11: Issues, proposals, and con-
Cox, P. A. (1996). Introduction to quantum theory and troversies. In D. McKay, J. S. Abramowitz, S. Taylor,
atomic structure. Oxford University Press. & G. J. G. Asmundson (Eds.), Current perspectives
Debiec, J., & LeDoux, J. E. (2009). The amygdala net- on the anxiety disorders: Implications for DSM-V and
works of fear: From animal models to human psycho- beyond (pp. 481–511). Springer.
pathology. In D. McKay, J. S. Abramowitz, S. Taylor, Williams, D. M., & Ebach, M. C. (2020). Cladistics: A
& G. J. G. Asmundson (Eds.), Current perspectives guide to biological classification. Cambridge Press.
Issues in Differential Diagnosis:
Phobias and Phobic Conditions 2
Mark B. Powers, Kiara Leonard, Maris Adams,
Emma Turner, Jamie R. Pogue, Marjorie L. Crozier,
Emily Carl, and Seth J. Gillihan

The purpose of this chapter is to summarize the objects or situations, specific phobias (formerly
current status of research with respect to the clin- “simple phobia” in DSM-III-R) can develop in
ical features, course, and prognosis of specific response to nearly anything (Marks, 1987).
phobias, social anxiety disorder (social phobia), Commonly occurring fears include animals,
panic disorder, agoraphobia, and separation anxi- heights, flying, enclosed spaces, darkness, receiv-
ety disorder in children. In this context, we will ing an injection, and seeing blood. Because chil-
consider the salient factors involved in the dif- dren naturally experience developmentally
ferential diagnosis of these five disorders. Finally, appropriate fears, it is important to distinguish
we will provide some directions to improve phobias from those fears that are typical for the
assessment of these disorders in children. developmental stage of the child and to recognize
their different forms of expression (e.g., tan-
trums, crying, freezing, clinging). A phobia diag-
Specific Phobia nosis should be considered when the fear is
excessive and causes marked interference in the
Description child’s life. In children, the fear must be present
for at least 6 months. According to DSM-5, spe-
Specific phobias are the most prevalent anxiety cific phobia should be diagnosed when all of the
disorder according to nearly all epidemiological following criteria are met (Table 2.1).
studies of the general population (e.g., Kessler These criteria for diagnosing specific phobias
et al., 2012). Defined in the Diagnostic and in children have been slightly modified from the
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fifth criteria for the diagnosis in adults. The DSM-5
Edition (DSM-5; American Psychiatric categorizes specific phobias into five subtypes:
Association, 2013) as intense fears of specific animal phobias (e.g., spiders, dogs, snakes), nat-
ural environment phobias (e.g., storms, heights,
or water), blood-injection-injury phobias (e.g.,
M. B. Powers (*) · K. Leonard · M. Adams ·
seeing blood, receiving an injection/needles),
E. Turner · J. R. Pogue · E. Carl
Baylor University Medical Center, Dallas, TX, USA situational phobias (e.g., enclosed spaces, eleva-
e-mail: mark.powers1@bswhealth.org tors, flying), and other phobias for fears that do
M. L. Crozier not fit into one specific category (e.g., choking,
Brown University, Providence, RI, USA vomiting, loud sounds, costumed characters).
S. J. Gillihan The ICD-10 has similar diagnostic criteria but
Private Practice, Haverford, PA, USA identifies fewer subtypes.

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2023 7


D. McKay, E. A. Storch (eds.), Handbook of Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-14080-8_2
8 M. B. Powers et al.

Table 2.1 DSM-5 Diagnostic criteria for specific approximately 16%, with 0.6% reporting severe
phobia impairment (APA, 2013). Some of the higher
Pronounced fear or anxiety about a specific object or prevalence rates have been found in the United
situation (e.g., blood, animals, getting a shot). In States, but it is likely that these differences are a
children, the fear or anxiety may be expressed by
crying, tantrums, freezing, or clinging (criterion A) result of variations in assessment methods or cul-
 The phobic object or situation almost always causes tural differences (Wardenaar et al., 2017). Along
fear or anxiety immediately following exposure with generalized anxiety disorder and separation
(criterion B) anxiety disorder, specific phobias are one of the
 The phobic object or situation is avoided or suffered more commonly diagnosed anxiety disorders in
through with intense fear or anxiety (criterion C)
children (Costello & Angold, 1995). Additionally,
 The fear or anxiety is disproportionate to the actual
danger presented by the specific object or situation Costello and Angold (1995) found that specific
and to the sociocultural context (criterion D) phobias in a community sample occur more fre-
 The symptoms are not transient lasting 6 months or quently without comorbid diagnoses than any
more (criterion E) other anxiety disorder in children. Community
 The fear, anxiety, or avoidance causes clinically samples have also shown that adults with a spe-
significant distress or disability in important areas
of functioning (e.g., social, education, development) cific phobia are significantly more likely to have
(criterion F) had a specific phobia as an adolescent but no
 Difficulties are not better explained by symptoms of other previous anxiety diagnoses (Gregory et al.,
another mental disorder, including fear, anxiety, and 2007).
avoidance of situations associated with panic like or
other embarrassing or incapacitating symptoms (as
in agoraphobia), obsession-related objects or Comorbidity Clinical samples have shown dif-
situations (as in obsessive-compulsive disorder), ferent rates of co-occurring anxiety and internal-
traumatic event reminders (as in posttraumatic izing disorders in children. A sample of children
stress disorder), separation from home or
attachment figures (as in separation anxiety
referred to an outpatient anxiety center showed a
prevalence rate of 15% with specific phobia as
disorder), or social situations (as in social anxiety
disorder) (criterion G) the primary diagnosis; 64% of children with a
Note: Adapted from American Psychiatric Association primary specific phobia met diagnostic criteria
(2013, pp. 197) for a secondary diagnosis (Last et al., 1987b). A
similar study found that 72% of children between
Avoidance behaviors in children often take the the ages of 6 and 16 years, who were referred to
form of tantrums, crying, anger attacks, clinging, a phobia treatment clinic, had at least one comor-
and hiding. When the feared stimuli are present, bid diagnosis (Silverman et al., 1999). Some of
the severity of the fear response and avoidance the more common comorbid conditions included
behaviors indicate the extent of the child’s dis- an additional specific phobia (19%), separation
tress. Often the child is brought in for treatment anxiety (16%), and ADHD (6%) (Silverman
not because of the fear itself but rather due to et al., 1999). Additionally, there has been some
severity of the disruption to the family’s daily evidence that phobias, specifically fears of the
routine as a result of the avoidance and distress-­ dark, in children and adolescents increase the
related behaviors. likelihood of a co-occurring major depressive
disorder (Pine et al., 2001). Literature has also
demonstrated specific phobias in children can be
Epidemiology a predictor of later internalizing disorders, par-
ticularly if there are multiple phobias present (de
Prevalence In international community sam- Vries et al., 2019). For example, a retrospective
ples, the prevalence rate for specific phobias in study showed that out of participants who
children is 2.6–9.1% with the average near 5% reported childhood-specific phobias with one or
(Ollendick et al., 2002; APA, 2013). In 13- to more subtypes, lifetime prevalence of an inter-
17-year-olds, however, the prevalence rate is nalizing disorder was 46.3%; comparatively,
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 9

those without childhood phobias had a preva- age of onset (Stinson et al., 2007); however, there
lence of 18.2%, while those with four or more is a paucity of longitudinal research beginning in
phobia subtypes had an increased prevalence of early childhood. A meta-analysis (Lijster et al.,
75.6% (de Vries et al., 2019). 2016) revealed specific phobia had a mean onset
at 11 years of age. This, along with separation
Cultural differences The prevalence for specific anxiety and social phobia, is significantly earlier
phobias has been reported to be higher in African-­ than other anxiety disorders which begin, on
American children as compared to white children average, between 21.1 and 34.9 years of age
(Last & Perrin, 1993); however, rates may vary (Lijster et al., 2016). Other studies have reported
due to differences in specific phobia domains. even earlier age of onset around 7 and 8 years of
Fears indicated by African-American young age (Kessler et al., 2005; Wardenaar et al., 2017),
adults differ from their Caucasian American often ranging across specific phobia domains.
counterparts, with the former endorsing more For instance, animal, environmental, and blood-­
specific phobias (Chapman et al., 2008). Mexican injection-­injury phobias typically begin in early
American adults born in the USA also report childhood (Kessler et al., 2005; Wittchen et al.,
higher rates of specific phobia when compared to 1999), while situational phobias may start much
immigrant Mexican Americans as well as native later (Becker et al., 2007). Furthermore, several
non-Hispanic whites (Karno et al., 1989). There studies (Burstein et al., 2012; de Vries et al.,
have also been higher rates of specific phobias 2019) demonstrate that individuals who present
reported in Brazil than in the USA (Da Motta with multiple types of phobias at an early age
et al., 2000). Lower risk for specific phobias has experience increased severity and impairment
been reported among Asians and Hispanics and have higher rates of additional psychiatric
(Stinson et al., 2007) as well as in Japan disorders. Considering its early onset and high
(Kawakami et al., 2005) compared to Western psychiatric comorbidity rates, specific phobia
countries. In a study of children and adolescents may be a useful indicator for subsequent psycho-
in Seoul, Korea, the prevalence rate reported was pathology (Wittchen et al., 2003).
7.9% (Kim et al., 2010). In Uganda, the preva-
lence rate was much higher at 15.8%, with the Research on gender effects in children with
highest rates in children under 5 years of age specific phobias has generally shown few signifi-
(Abbo et al., 2013). A number of factors include cant differences under the age of 10 years (Strauss
operational definitions, ages sampled, and the & Last, 1993). According to the DSM-5, females
manner and content in which specific phobias experience specific phobia twice as frequently as
present may contribute to differences in sampling males (Bekker & van Mens-Verhulst, 2007),
and bias. Lastly, research including children in although this varies by phobia type. Literature on
non-Western countries remains sparse, making it gender differences has remained mixed, though
difficult to determine whether these results reflect most support a higher prevalence in females than
methodological differences or a true cultural dis- males (Beedso-baum et al., 2009; Fredrikson
parity. Diagnostic criteria in the DSM-5 address et al., 1996). For instance, a German study with a
cultural differences by stipulating that the fear community adolescent sample found that more
and anxiety caused by the specific phobia must girls than boys were diagnosed with specific pho-
be out of proportion to the sociocultural context bia (Essau et al., 2000). Researchers have posited
(APA, 2013). that this gender distinction may follow differ-
ences in how boys and girls are socialized and
Age and gender differences The prevalence of how the expression of fear is often viewed as
specific phobias tends to be higher in children more acceptable from females than males (Kane
and adolescents than in adults (Emmelkamp & et al., 2014). Strauss and Last (1993) have also
Wittchen, 2008). Most adults who meet diagnos- suggested that this gender disparity may be either
tic criteria for a specific phobia report an early based on methodological variations or a ­reflection
10 M. B. Powers et al.

of the different rates of referral for treatment in (LeBeau et al., 2010), many researchers assert
boys versus girls. the number of specific phobias present is more
Despite the varied results of gender preva- predictive of severity and impairment (Burstein
lence across studies, there have been some con- et al., 2012; Stinson et al., 2007), regardless of
sistent findings related to gender differences the domain.
across subtypes of phobias. The DSM-5 indicates
higher rates of females than males experience
animal, environment, and situational-specific Structure of Fear
phobias, while blood-injection-injury phobia
affects both males and females equally. A study (Cox et al., 2003) using both exploratory
Environmental phobias tend to have an earlier and confirmatory factor analyses examined the
age of onset in boys (Wittchen et al., 1999), but factor structure of all specific phobia domains
are overall more prevalent in females (Beesdo-­ and found the following elements:
baum et al., 2009). Animal phobias are also more
common in girls with a 3:1 ratio clearly present • Agoraphobia: Public places, crowds, being
by age 10 years (Wittchen et al., 1998a, b). away from home, travel by car, train, or bus
Craske (2003) described adolescence as a period • Speaking: Public speaking, speaking to a
during which women develop fears and phobias group, talking to others
more rapidly than men do. McLean and Anderson • Heights/water: Flying, heights, crossing a
(2009) posit that this may be attributed to the bridge, water
effects of gender socialization during adoles- • Being observed: Public eating, public toilet
cence, where boys are encouraged to face their use, writing in front of others
fears, whereas girls are permitted to avoid them. • Threat: Blood/needles, storms/thunder,
Whatever factors contribute, it is clear that gen- snakes/animals, being alone, enclosed spaces
der differences in prevalence rates of specific
phobia become apparent during adolescence Higher-order analyses showed two second-­
(Craske, 2003). order factors: social fears and specific fears.
Another factor analytic study of specific pho-
Specific phobias and subtypes Some of the bia subtypes used data from a large sample of
more commonly occurring phobias in children young adults from 11 countries. Results of this
include fear of heights, darkness, injections, study supported blood-injection-injury phobia
dogs, loud noises, small animals, and insects and animal phobia as two of the major classes of
(Essau et al., 2000; King, 1993; Silverman & fears across cultures (Arrindell et al., 2003).
Rabian, 1993; Strauss & Last, 1993). However, Environmental (e.g., storms, heights) and situa-
there have been few studies specifically examin- tional (e.g., flying, elevators) phobias were
ing the prevalence of subtypes, and most studies grouped together on one factor in this sample.
have focused on adult populations. Most recently, Additional studies have found similar results sug-
the National Epidemiological Study on Alcohol gesting that there may be few differences between
and Related Conditions found that fear of ani- environmental and situational phobias
mals and heights were the most commonly (Fredrikson et al., 1996). While these studies
reported phobias among adults, comprising more have been primarily with adults, there has been
than half of the diagnosed cases of specific pho- some research specifically examining children.
bia. This observation is consistent with Stinson Muris et al. (1999) found similar results in a sam-
et al.’ (2007) finding that animals are among the ple of children, indicating that environmental and
more commonly feared stimuli in children, situation types of phobias tend to cluster together
whereas blood-injury-injection phobias are in factor analyses. In a study examining mental
among the least common. While much attention disorders on a three-factor model consisting of
is placed on the subtype of specific phobia “anxious-misery,” “fear,” and “externalizing,”
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 11

Wittchen et al. (2009) discovered that animal and et al. (2013) go on to suggest that unique environ-
natural environment subtypes were routinely mental factors (i.e., individual events) account
attributed to the same factor, while blood-injury-­ for most of the variance beyond genetic factors,
injection subtype could not be precisely assigned whereas common environmental factors (i.e.,
to a single factor. These consistent results across events affecting multiple individuals) only con-
samples indicate that phobia subtyping may need tribute a small effect on variance.
to be refined. While there has been a range of results found
for the heritability of specific phobias, the herita-
bility of anxiety more generally has been demon-
Genetic Patterns strated consistently in the literature. Fyer et al.
(1995) found moderate aggregation for specific
There has been some evidence in family studies phobias in families where one family member
of a moderate degree of concordance for specific had an anxiety disorder. Hettema et al. (2001)
phobia diagnosis among family members. found similar results in a meta-analysis of the
Another consistent finding has been the relation- heritability of anxiety disorders in both family
ship between the fears of a mother and her child and twin studies. Hettema et al. (2005) examined
(Emmelkamp & Scholing, 1997). For example, anxiety disorders in a community sample of
mothers who fear insects may also have children twins and determined that for all the anxiety dis-
who exhibit fear in the presence of insects. While orders there appears to be two genetic factors that
there are a variety of factors such as temperament contribute to the development of symptomology.
and modeling that may contribute to the familial One of these factors is specifically associated
relationship among anxiety disorders, genetic with situational and animal phobias but no other
factors may also be responsible for some of the forms of anxiety. Because these two subtypes of
co-occurrence of this diagnosis. phobias are loaded together but separate from
Bolton et al. (2006) studied over 4500 6-year-­ other forms of anxiety, it suggests that there may
old twins to determine genetic and environmental be a unique genetic factor related to the develop-
influences on the development of early-onset ment of these two specific types of phobia mak-
anxiety disorders. For specific phobias, the heri- ing them distinct from the etiology of other forms
tability was around 60% with the remaining 40% of anxiety. Additional evidence has shown that
of variance attributed to differences in environ- individuals with the blood-injection-injury sub-
ment. As this study was conducted on young chil- type of specific phobia have more relatives with
dren and differs in results from other studies done similar problems indicating that this subtype may
on older children or adults, it is likely that early-­ be a separate category (Marks, 1987; Öst, 1992).
onset phobias may be more genetically deter- A more recent meta-analysis examined five stud-
mined than those developing later in childhood or ies and largely found similar results to previous
adulthood (Bolton et al., 2006). These findings literature (Van Houtem et al. (2013). The herita-
provide support for a non-associative model of bility rate range was 28–63% for blood-injury-­
phobias which suggests an evolutionary basis to injection phobias, 22–44% for animal phobias,
fears rather than a conditioned fear model 0–41% for miscellaneous phobias, and 0–33%
(Menzies & Clarke, 1995). Another study exam- for situational phobias (Mean = 33%, 32%, 25%,
ining heritability of specific phobias used a sam- and 25%, respectively). The presence of unique
ple of 319 sets of twins between the ages of 8 and physiological attributes in blood-injection-injury
18 (Stevenson et al., 1992). The results of this phobia, including the risk for fainting which is
study suggested that differences in genes rare in other phobia subtypes (Connolly et al.,
accounted for 29% of the variance in specific 1976), also supports differentiating this subtype
phobia diagnosis, with shared and non-shared from other specific phobia subtypes.
environmental factors each accounting for a Contrary to the above results, the VATSPSUD
remaining third of the variance. Van Houtem study (Kendler & Prescott, 2006) found the
12 M. B. Powers et al.

l­owest rates of specific heritability for blood-­ those having a single phobia (Bianchi & Carter,
injection-­injury phobias (7%). That is, those with 2012).
a relative with this specific type of phobia are not Despite the general conception that disgust
as likely to inherit that particular phobia. Kendler sensitivity is a genetically based vulnerability,
and Prescott (2006) also found similarly low there is little evidence of a genetic component.
rates for the specific heritability of situational Correlations in twin studies have shown very
phobias (15%). However, this study did find small genetic contribution (r = 0.29 for monozy-
common genetic factors contributing to all pho- gotic twins and r = 0.24 for dizygotic twins;
bias, with the largest contribution for animal Rozin et al., 2000). While a significant relation-
(21%) and blood-injection-injury (22%). ship exists between parent and child levels of dis-
gust (r = 0.52; Rozin et al., 2000), there are
environmental factors that could be contributing
Disgust Sensitivity to this relationship other than genetics.
Additionally, some researchers have suggested
Disgust sensitivity refers to the propensity for that gender differences in specific phobias may
experiencing disgust in a wide variety of set- be related to gender differences in disgust sensi-
tings. This sensitivity has been proposed to con- tivity (Davey, 1994). While early studies have
tribute to the development of a variety of been inconclusive, a recent study (Connolly
disorders particularly blood-injection-injury et al., 2008) found that disgust sensitivity medi-
phobias, animal phobias, and obsessive-­ ated the association between gender and specific
compulsive disorder (OCD; Olatunji & Deacon, phobias.
2008). Individuals with phobias related to spi-
ders frequently report feelings of disgust rather
than fear (Davey, 1992). In fact, disgust responses Social Anxiety Disorder (Social
to images of spiders have been shown to be pres- Phobia)
ent even when fear is not present (Olatunji,
2006). While little research has examined dis- Description
gust responses to in vivo spider exposure, people
with spider phobias report more disgust than Social anxiety disorder (social phobia) is charac-
non-phobic individuals (e.g., Olatunji & Deacon, terized by intense fear or discomfort in social
2008). There is also some evidence that disgust, situations. This fear can be limited to one specific
more so than anxiety, is a better predictor of situation (e.g., eating in front of others) or it can
avoidance of spiders (Olatunji & Deacon, 2008; be generalized to all social settings. Individuals
Woody et al., 2005). There are a few studies sug- with this type of anxiety fear embarrassment in
gesting that disgust sensitivity may be related these situations which often includes fear of
more to concerns about cleanliness and potential being ridiculed, laughed at, or disliked by peers.
for disease rather than concern related to physi- Individuals often have an overestimated percep-
cal harm in the presence of spiders and other tion of how anxious they appear physically. In
small animals and insects (Davey, 1992; Olatunji children, symptoms must persist for at least
& Deacon, 2008). Disgust sensitivity has also 6 months and must result in significant interfer-
shown to be significantly associated with certain ence in the child’s social functioning. In addition
psychopathological symptoms in children, to these criteria, the DSM-5 (pp. 202–208)
including blood-injection-injury phobia, animal requires the following:
phobia, and agoraphobia (Muris et al., 2008).
There is evidence that having both spider and • Marked fear or anxiety about one or more
blood-injection-injury phobias may have a com- social situations in which the individual is
pounding effect, such that people with both to exposed to possible scrutiny by others.
exhibit greater disgust sensitivity compared to Examples include social interactions (e.g.,
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 13

having a conversation, meeting unfamiliar anxiety disorder in childhood are less likely to
people), being observed (e.g., eating or drink- have problems with substance use in later adoles-
ing), and performing in front of others (e.g., cence (Kendall et al., 2004). Children with a pri-
giving a speech). *Note: In children, the anxi- mary anxiety disorder (including social anxiety
ety must occur in peer settings and not just disorder) are also at a much higher risk for OCD
during interactions with adults. and mood disorders, such as MDD or dysthymic
• The individual fears that he or she will act in a disorder (now persistent depressive disorder;
way or show anxiety symptoms that will be Waite & Creswell 2014) and educational prob-
negatively evaluated (i.e., will be humiliating lems, including decreased academic performance
or embarrassing; will lead to rejection or and early discontinuation (Kerns et al., 2013),
offend others). particularly in later adolescence (Kessler et al.,
• The social situations almost always provoke 1995). These heightened risks could be explained
fear or anxiety. *Note: In children, the fear or by consistent negative self-perceptions (Alfano
anxiety may be expressed by crying, tantrums, et al., 2006).
freezing, clinging, shrinking, or failing to
speak in social situations.
• The social situations are avoided or endured Epidemiology
with intense fear or anxiety.
• The fear or anxiety is out of proportion to the The lifetime prevalence of social phobia in an
actual threat posed by the social situation and adolescent population has been reported as 1.6%
to the sociocultural context (Essau et al., 1999b), although more recent litera-
• The symptoms are not transient lasting 6 months ture has suggested the lifetime prevalence rates to
or more. be higher, between 4.20% for men and 5.67% for
women (Clauss & Blackford, 2012). A meta-­
For children and adults alike, the fear or anxi- analysis reported social anxiety disorder preva-
ety is considered excessive in relation to the lence in 28 countries to be 4.0% (Stein et al.,
actual threat posed by the social situation. In chil- 2017). The 12-month prevalence rate in US chil-
dren, these symptoms must be present in social dren is approximately 7% and is comparable to
situations involving similarly aged peers and not the rate among adults (APA, 2013). Prevalence
only around adults. In addition, the child must rates of social phobia in children in the general
demonstrate the capacity to engage in age-­ population range from 1% to 6% (Verhulst et al.,
appropriate social interactions with individuals 1997), although, again, more recent reviews have
with whom the child is familiar. The distress and suggested the rates to be higher, between 9.1%
avoidance seen in social settings is often demon- (Xu et al., 2012) and 12.1% (Ruscio, 2008). One
strated in tantrums, crying, clinging to caretakers, possible reason for this large range in prevalence
and hiding. Moreover, the fear and avoidance rates is the way certain forms of social anxiety
situations or objects experienced by children tend are coded by researchers. For example, both
to be narrower in range compared to those expe- school phobia and fear of public speaking could
rienced by adults (APA, 2013). be classified under either social anxiety or spe-
Social phobia in children and adolescents is cific phobia. Different studies have chosen to cat-
associated with a number of long-term negative egorize these types of fears differently which
outcomes. Children and adolescents with social may contribute to the inconsistent prevalence
phobia are at a high risk for developing substance rates across studies. In a more recent study con-
use earlier than their peers and tend to have a ducted with 8- to 13-year-olds in Norway, 2.3%
shorter interval between first use of substances of all children were reported to have significant
and problems associated with substance use symptoms of social anxiety (Van Roy et al.,
(Marmorstein et al., 2010). There is some evi- 2009). The rates of social phobia among a clini-
dence that those who receive treatment for an cal population have been reported around 15%
14 M. B. Powers et al.

(Last et al., 1987b). As with all anxiety disorders, evidence showing that it does occur in children
there is a high level of comorbidity in social pho- (Wittchen et al., 2008), the typical age of onset
bia with one sample reporting that 63% of chil- for panic disorder is late adolescence into adult-
dren with social anxiety had a comorbid anxiety hood (Kessler et al., 2005), and the prevalence
disorder (Last et al., 1987b). rate among children younger than 14 years old is
Additionally, there is some evidence of sociode- less than 0.4% (APA, 2013). For many individu-
mographic differences in the prevalence of social als with panic disorder, the first panic attack
phobia. Inconsistent findings have been reported occurred during a time of psychosocial stress
for gender differences in social phobia. One study (Craske, 1999).
of a clinical sample found that boys were more
likely to have social anxiety than were girls
(Compton et al., 2000), while other studies have Symptoms of Panic
found that up to 70% of clinical samples of social
phobia are females (Beidel & Turner, 1988). There According to DSM-5 (pp. 208), a panic attack is
has been little cross-cultural research or research an “abrupt surge of intense fear or intense dis-
related to racial background in social phobia. comfort that reaches a peak within minutes, and
There is some evidence, however, that European during which time four (or more) of the follow-
American children are more likely to report more ing symptoms occur: palpitations, pounding
symptoms of social anxiety than are African heart, or accelerated heart rate; sweating; trem-
American children in a community sample bling or shaking; sensations of shortness of
(Compton et al., 2000), but these findings have not breath or smothering; feelings of choking; chest
yet been replicated. Social phobia is correlated pain or discomfort; nausea or abdominal distress;
with individuals who are single, never married or feeling dizzy, unsteady, light-headed, or faint;
divorced, and without children (APA, 2013). chills or heat sensations; paresthesias (numbness
or tingling sensations); derealization (feelings of
unreality) or depersonalization (being detached
Panic Disorder from one-self); fear of losing control or ‘going
crazy’; and fear of dying” (Table 2.2).
Description In order for panic attacks to be considered part
of panic disorder, at least one must be followed
The hallmark symptom of panic disorder is the by a month or more of one or both of the follow-
presence of recurrent and unexpected panic
attacks that cause the individual great anticipa- Table 2.2 DSM-5 Symptoms of panic attacks
tory anxiety. Panic attacks themselves are brief
Palpitations, pounding heart, or accelerated heart rate
periods of numerous physiological symptoms Sweating
accompanied by intense fear. For a majority of Trembling or shaking
individuals experiencing panic disorder, there is Sensations of shortness of breath or smothering
also agoraphobic avoidance—that is, avoidance Feeling of choking
of situations from which escape might be diffi- Chest pain or discomfort
cult in the event of a panic attack. Panic disorder Nausea or abdominal distress
was once thought to be a disorder found only in Feeling dizzy, unsteady, lightheaded, or faint
adults and very rarely in adolescents. This notion Chills or heat sensations
Paresthesias (numbness or tingling sensations)
was based on the idea that there is a strong cogni-
Derealization (feelings of unreality) or
tive component to panic disorder that children depersonalization (feeling detached from oneself)
were incapable of experiencing (Nelles & Barlow, Fear of losing control or going crazy
1988). However, there is now a large body of evi- Fear of dying
dence showing that panic disorder does occur in Note: Adapted from American Psychiatric Association
children (e.g., Kearney et al., 1997). Despite the (2013, pp. 208)
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 15

ing: (1) persistent concern or worry about having disorder. The defining feature is “marked, or
another attack or their consequences (e.g., losing intense, fear or anxiety triggered by the real or
control, a heart attack, going crazy) and (2) a sig- anticipated exposure to a wide range of situa-
nificant maladaptive behavior change related to tions” (pp. 218). However, there remains some
having these attacks. In children, making a diag- overlap in symptoms and diagnostic criteria
nosis of panic disorder can be challenging as (Asmundson et al., 2014). A diagnosis of agora-
some of the fears may present differently. For phobia requires marked fear or anxiety about at
example, young children may report a fear of least two of five situations:
becoming ill without any clear physical symp-
toms reported. In older children, reports of anxi- • Using public transportation (e.g., automo-
ety about becoming sick are common, as are biles, buses, trains, ships, planes)
fears of uncontrollable vomiting. Only in adoles- • Being in open spaces (e.g., parking lots, mar-
cence do individuals tend to start reporting fears ketplaces, bridges)
related to specific physiological symptoms. • Being in enclosed spaces (e.g., shops, the-
Children and adolescents report many of the aters, cinemas)
same physiological symptoms as adults, such as • Standing in line or being in a crowd
heart palpitations, nausea, shakiness, dizziness, • Being outside of the home alone (DSM-5,
sweating, headaches, and chills or heat sensa- pp. 217)
tions (Masi et al., 2000; Essau et al., 1999a;
Kearney et al., 1997). Somatic symptoms are Generally, these “situations are actively
more common than cognitive complaints, which avoided, require the presence of a companion, or
are reported more frequently among adolescents are endured with intense fear/anxiety” (pp. 218).
than children (Moreau & Follett, 1993). Phobic avoidance may be motivated by unrealis-
Nevertheless, there is evidence that some chil- tic fears of the consequences of having panic
dren and adolescent experience cognitive symp- symptoms, particularly in situations where the
toms, such as the fear of dying, the fear of going person feels trapped or far from help. For chil-
crazy (e.g., “I feel I am losing control”), or deper- dren, commonly feared situations are being out-
sonalization/derealization (e.g., “I don’t know side the home and becoming lost (DSM-5,
who I am” or “I don’t know where I am”). Twin pp. 220). There are some concerns for children
studies have demonstrated that panic disorder is failing to meet the new DSM-5 criteria and not
moderately heritable with a concordance rate of receiving proper treatment, mainly due to crite-
73% among monozygotic twins compared to 0% rion A (requiring symptoms elicited from two or
in dizygotic twins (Perna et al., 1997). The more of the situations listed above; Cornacchio
Virginia Adult Twin Study of Psychiatric and et al., 2015). However, the result and impact of
Substance Use Disorders (2005) found a panic this needs to be researched further.
disorder heritability of 28%, revealing that envi-
ronmental factors also play a considerable role.
However, two recent genome-wide association Epidemiology
studies on panic disorders did not produce sig-
nificant associations (Na et al., 2011). Agoraphobia is diagnosed in about 1.7% of ado-
lescents and adults each year, with a “substantial
incidence risk in late adolescence and early
Agoraphobia ­adulthood” (DSM-5, pp. 219). In a study of US
adolescents, Roberts et al. (2007) found a 1-year
Description prevalence rate of 4.5% (significantly higher than
the rates found in adults). In fact, this study found
In the DSM-5, agoraphobia was designated as its that agoraphobia was the most frequent anxiety
own diagnostic category independent of panic disorder in their sample, although the prevalence
16 M. B. Powers et al.

dropped to 1.6% when impairment was required Table 2.3 DSM-5 Diagnostic criteria for separation anx-
iety disorder
for a diagnosis. In a 2010 study of mental disor-
ders in US adolescents (aged 13–18), the lifetime Recurrent excessive distress when anticipating or
experiencing separation from home or major
prevalence of agoraphobia was 2.4%, with higher attachment figures occurs or is anticipated
rates for females than males (3.4% versus 1.4%) Persistent and excessive worry about losing major
(Merikangas et al., 2010). Wittchen et al. (2008) attachment figures or about possible harm to them,
examined the prevalence of agoraphobia in such as illness, injury, disasters, or death
German adolescents. Adolescents with panic dis- Persistent and excessive worry about experiencing an
untoward event (e.g., getting lost, being kidnapped,
order or panic attacks were only moderately having an accident, becoming ill) that causes
more likely to develop subsequent agoraphobia, separation from a major attachment figure
while the majority of adolescents meeting crite- Persistent reluctance or refusal to go to out, away from
ria for agoraphobia had never experienced a home, to school, to work, or elsewhere because of fear
panic attack. While cultural/racial groups do not of separation
Persistent and excessive fear of or reluctance about
seem to have different prevalence rates, cultural
being alone or without major attachment figures at
context is considered in the DSM-5 when deter- home or in other settings
mining if symptoms are “out of proportion to the Persistent reluctance or refusal to sleep away from
actual danger posed” (pp. 219). home or to go to sleep without being near a major
attachment figure
Repeated nightmares involving the theme of
separation
Separation Anxiety Disorder Repeated complaints of physical symptoms (such as
headaches, stomachaches, nausea, or vomiting) when
Description separation from major attachment figures occurs or is
anticipated
Separation anxiety disorder is a somewhat unique Note: Adapted from American Psychiatric Association
(2013, pp. 190–191)
diagnosis in that, up until the DSM-5, it was the
only anxiety disorder limited to children and ado-
lescents. Separation anxiety disorder is defined in • Persistent and excessive fear of or reluctance
DSM-5 (pp. 190) as “developmentally inappro- about being alone or without major attach-
priate and excessive fear or anxiety concerning ment figures at home or in other settings
separation from those to whom the individual is • Persistent reluctance or refusal to sleep away
attached, as evidenced by three or more of the from home or to go to sleep without being
following (Table 2.3): near a major attachment figure
• Repeated nightmares involving the theme of
• Recurrent excessive distress when anticipat- separation
ing or experiencing separation from home or • Repeated complaints of physical symptoms
from major attachment figures (such as headaches, stomachaches, nausea, or
• Persistent and excessive worry about losing vomiting) when separation from major attach-
major attachment figures or about possible ment figures occurs or is anticipated.”
harm to them, such as illness, injury, disasters,
or death To be considered clinically significant, these
• Persistent and excessive worry about experi- symptoms must be present in children and ado-
encing an untoward event (e.g. getting lost, lescents for at least 4 weeks. Comparatively,
being kidnapped, having an accident, becom- adults must present these symptoms for 6 or more
ing ill) that causes separation from a major months. Particular to children, the symptoms
attachment figure must be developmentally inappropriate for the
• Persistent reluctance or refusal to go to out, child’s biological age. Many of these symptoms
away from home, to school, to work, or else- would be considered developmentally appropri-
where because of fear of separation ate in children ages 7 months to 6 years old
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 17

(Bernstein & Borchardt, 1991), and thus it is Prevalence rates in community samples for
important to consider both age and developmen- separation anxiety disorder ranged from 2.0% to
tal level when making a diagnostic determina- 12.9% (Anderson et al., 1987; Kashani &
tion. The underlying fear found in separation Orvaschel, 1988; McGee et al., 1990). Among
anxiety disorder is an exaggerated fear of losing children 12 years old and younger, separation
or becoming separated from parents or other pri- anxiety disorder is the most prevalent disorder
mary caregivers. In addition to these fears, many and has been found to decrease throughout the
children experience nightmares related to becom- lifespan (APA, 2013), consistent with previous
ing separated from caregivers (Bell-Dolan & literature. The range in rates may be attributable
Brazeal, 1993). to the age at which symptoms were assessed.
Symptom differences have been found The lower rates of prevalence were found in
between ages but not between genders (Francis studies examining adolescents, while the higher
et al., 1987; Paulus et al., 2015). Young children rates were found in community samples of
(ages 5–8 years) are most likely to report fears of younger children. Rates among clinical popula-
harm to self or caregivers, nightmares, and school tions are higher than the general population,
refusal. Children between the ages of 9 and with 33% of a sample of anxious children meet-
12 years present with more excessive distress at ing diagnostic criteria for separation anxiety
the time of separation, while adolescents are disorder (Last et al., 1987b). Results of this
more likely to experience somatic symptoms and study also indicated that 41% of the children
school refusal. Similarly, children tend to exhibit with a primary diagnosis of separation anxiety
physical symptoms such as headaches and nau- disorder had a comorbid anxiety diagnosis, the
sea, whereas adolescents and adults tend to expe- most common being GAD or specific phobia
rience cardiovascular symptoms (APA, 2013). (APA, 2013).
Some children have also described perceptual A number of sociodemographic variables
experiences. Additionally, older children and have been associated with separation anxiety
adolescents are most likely to experience a disorder. Most samples examining separation
smaller number of symptoms than are younger anxiety disorder have been primarily with chil-
children. dren of European descent, although this find-
ing may reflect biased sampling rather than
true cultural differences (Strauss & Last,
Epidemiology 1993). However, one study in Uganda found a
child/adolescent prevalence rate of 5.8% (Abbo
While separation anxiety disorder can present in et al., 2013). As with most other anxiety disor-
children of all ages, it is most common in preado- ders, rates of separation anxiety disorder are
lescent age ranges. Typically, the onset is acute higher in females than males (Compton et al.,
and follows a significant change in the child’s life 2000); however, there is evidence of equal
(e.g., start of school, moving, death of a parent or rates in a clinical sample (APA, 2013).
close relative) or developmental changes (Last, Contrarily, a few published reports found no
1989). Several studies have shown that separation gender differences (Bird et al., 1989; Last
anxiety disorder follows an intermittent course et al., 1992; Paulus et al., 2015). Additionally,
over time. Children often experience remissions lower SES and parental education levels have
and relapses around times of school holidays, been associated with higher rates of separation
vacations, and life stressors (Cantwell & Baker, anxiety disorder in children (Bird et al., 1989;
1988; Hale et al., 2008). When followed over a Last et al., 1987b). In a study examining sepa-
period of 4 years, 96% of children initially diag- ration anxiety disorder heritability, researchers
nosed with separation anxiety disorder no longer estimated a heritability rate of 73% in a com-
met diagnostic criteria, the highest recovery rate munity sample of 6-year-old twins (Bolton
of any anxiety disorder studied (Last et al., 1996). et al., 2006).
18 M. B. Powers et al.

Role of Avoidance Differential Diagnosis

In addition to the many fears that children with  evelopmentally Appropriate Fear
D
separation anxiety disorder experience, the Versus Anxiety Disorders
avoidance of situations is a key element of this
disorder. Additionally, avoidance behaviors An important diagnostic issue to consider in chil-
play an important sustaining role in anxiety dis- dren is whether the anxiety is developmentally
orders (Foa & Kozak, 1986). There is a large appropriate or is part of a disorder. Anxiety and
range of avoidance behaviors common to chil- its various associated physiological symptoms
dren with separation anxiety disorder, and types are considered to be basic human emotions
of avoidance may vary by age. Reluctance to be (Barlow, 2002). In young children, common
being alone or without an adult and reluctance developmental fears include fear of the dark, fear
to sleep away from caregivers or from home are of new situations including the first day of school,
the most frequently reported avoidance behav- fear of separation from parents or other caretak-
iors (Allen et al., 2010). Milder forms of avoid- ers, and fear of large animals. In adolescents,
ance include hesitation to leave home, common developmental fears include anxiety
requesting that the caregiver be accessible via related to job interviews, college applications,
phone during outings, and frequent questions and dating.
about schedules. More moderate forms of An important distinction between develop-
avoidance in younger children can include mentally appropriate fears and phobias is both
clingy behaviors with parents or caregivers the duration and severity of the anxiety. For the
(e.g., following the adult around the house). anxiety to become clinically significant, it must
Older children may be more likely to have dif- persist for a period of at least 6 months and
ficulty leaving home without caregivers or include significant avoidance and interference in
refuse to participate in social activities with daily functioning (Albano et al., 2001). While
peers if the caregiver is not present. More seri- this distinction often is based on clinical judg-
ous forms of avoidance can include faking ill- ment, there has been research showing that a spe-
nesses, school refusal, or refusal to sleep alone cific phobia diagnosis can be reliably achieved
at night. According to the DSM-5 (2013), girls through the use of structured clinical interviews
may exhibit more reluctance or avoidance to and standardized self-report measures (Schniering
attend school than boys. et al., 2000). One common assessment used for
Avoidance behaviors may slowly increase the diagnosis of anxiety disorders in children is
over time. Albano et al. (2003) describe a pattern the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children
of increasing avoidance that starts with occa- Second Edition (MASC; March et al., 1997;
sional nightmares and subsequent requests to March, 2012). This self-report scale is used to
sleep with parents. From this relatively mild differentiate clinical from nonclinical samples as
behavior change, the child can become increas- well as distinguish different forms of anxiety. It
ingly avoidant until he or she is sleeping with has been found to be sensitive to the differences
one or both parents every night. Similarly, in these groups (Dierker et al., 2001). The Anxiety
Livingston et al. (1988) describe a pattern of Disorders Interview Schedule for Children
increasingly serious physical complaints on the (ADIS-C; Silverman & Albano, 1996) is another
part of the child. This behavior often progresses useful structured interview for diagnosis of
from very vague complaints of not feeling well ­anxiety disorders in children. The updated ADIS-
to frequent complaints of stomachaches or head- 5-­
C/P is under development. The structured
aches. It is often these avoidance behaviors that Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID-5) specifi-
will prompt the parent to bring the child in for cally tailored for children and adolescents is also
treatment. currently under development.
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 19

 istinguishing Between Different


D develop a panic attack, when confronted with the
Anxiety Disorders feared stimuli. The presence of panic attacks is
not sufficient to warrant the diagnosis of panic
Given the substantial overlap in symptoms across disorder, given that only a small minority of indi-
the disorders presented in this chapter, it may be viduals who experience panic attacks go on to
difficult at times to identify which diagnosis a develop panic disorder; results from the National
given child’s symptom presentation warrants. Comorbidity Survey Replication revealed a
The task can be all the more challenging in light 22.7% lifetime prevalence estimate for panic
of children’s difficulty at times in reporting attacks versus a 3.8% rate for panic disorder
clearly what they are experiencing. Even if they (Kessler et al., 2006, 2012). Specific phobia is
are willing to discuss their experiences, there indicated when the child’s fear, including panic
may be limitations in their vocabulary or concept attacks, is provoked by the phobic stimulus
formation to fully describe their fears. Accurate itself—for example, a dog. The content of the
diagnosis is important for case conceptualization fear in this case would have to do with the possi-
such that the most appropriate treatment can be bility of injury as a result of contact with the dog.
administered. For example, a cognitive-­At the core of panic disorder, on the other hand,
behavioral clinician would expose an individual is a fear of the panic attacks themselves (the so-­
with panic disorder to interoceptive cues (e.g., called “fear of fear”; e.g., Chambless et al.,
pounding heart) but would follow a different 1984).
treatment plan for an individual with separation
anxiety disorder. The following section covers Differential diagnosis can be more difficult
common distinctions that must be made in the when the feared stimulus or situation is one that
differential diagnosis of specific phobia, social commonly is associated with panic disorder—for
phobia, panic disorder, agoraphobia, and separa- example, a fear of elevators. In these cases, it is
tion anxiety disorder. In most cases, the correct imperative that the diagnosing clinician ascertain
diagnosis can be derived by understanding what whether the patient is afraid of panicking in these
is at the core of the patient’s fears. situations or simply is afraid of the situations
themselves (e.g., fears that the elevator will fall).
Specific phobia vs. social phobia Of the disor- Finding that the individual fears several situa-
ders under consideration, the two that share the tions that provoke panic attacks (e.g., car trips,
most symptom criteria may be the most straight- elevators, crowds) makes a diagnosis of panic
forward to distinguish based simply on the con- disorder more likely than diagnosis of a specific
tent of the fears. Specific and social phobia phobia to multiple situations.
overlap in nearly all of their diagnostic criteria
except that social phobia involves a fear of social Specific phobia vs. separation anxiety disor-
situations (e.g., talking to a group, answering der Specific phobia and separation anxiety dis-
questions in class), whereas specific phobia order both may include significant levels of
involves a fear of other stimuli. In cases where avoidance. The primary distinction between these
the distinction may be somewhat difficult—for disorders is based on whether the avoidance is
example, fear of clowns—the differential diagno- driven by fear of the avoided stimulus, as in
sis is based on whether the fear is primarily social ­specific phobia, or by fear of separation from
(e.g., being publicly embarrassed by the clown) attachment figures, which defines separation anx-
or involves fear of the stimulus itself (e.g., being iety disorder. Although children with specific
attacked by the clown). phobia may cling to their caregivers when con-
fronted with the phobic stimulus, the clinging
Specific phobia vs. panic disorder Children behavior represents the child’s looking to the
with specific phobias often will experience many caregiver for safety and protection. In contrast,
physiological symptoms of panic, and may even the core fear in separation anxiety disorder is
20 M. B. Powers et al.

separation from the caregiver in and of itself. For blush. In this case, the child will fear the social
this reason, the fear of separation is likely to be situation itself, not their possible public panic
more pervasive than in specific phobia in which response.
fear of separation is provoked by the presence of
a relatively limited range of stimuli (e.g., dogs). Social anxiety disorder (social phobia) vs. sepa-
ration anxiety disorder As with panic disorder,
Specific phobia vs. agoraphobia Specific pho- separation anxiety disorder can also resemble
bia and agoraphobia share similar criteria, par- social anxiety disorder in some respects. For
ticularly regarding feared situations. DSM-5 example, school refusal may be driven by social
guidelines state that if the individual fears one anxiety or by the distress associated with separa-
situation, specific phobia should be considered, tion from one’s caregiver. Careful questioning of
as agoraphobia requires two or more feared situ- the child and, if necessary, the parents may reveal
ations. Additionally, the motive for the feared the underlying fear. Whereas social anxiety dis-
situation is an important factor in distinguishing order is characterized by the fear of being judged
the two diagnoses. For example, an individual negatively by others, separation anxiety is defined
who displays crowd phobia tendencies due to by the fear of being separated from attachment
fear of being harmed may be diagnosed with spe- figures (APA, 2013). For example, if the child has
cific phobia, whereas an individual who fears no trouble socializing with peers when the par-
crowds due to fear of displaying panic-like symp- ents are present but refuses to go to school,
toms would be appropriate for an agoraphobia sleepovers, and other events where the parents
diagnosis. are not present, a diagnosis of separation anxiety
disorder is likely. On the other hand, if the child
Social anxiety disorder (social phobia) vs. panic is still terribly afraid of social settings even in the
disorder A child who presents with panic attacks presence of the parents, the accurate diagnosis is
and a fear of social situations could be living with likely social anxiety disorder.
either panic disorder or social anxiety disorder.
Additionally, both conditions lead to avoidance Social anxiety disorder (social phobia) vs. ago-
of social situations, such as school refusal. raphobia The main factor differentiating social
Indeed, the Panic Appraisal Inventory (Telch, anxiety disorder from agoraphobia is the stimu-
1987), which is commonly used to measure lus triggering symptoms and the cognitive ide-
panic-related concerns, comprises a subscale of ation (DSM-5). A diagnosis of agoraphobia will
panic consequences that include social concerns. be defined by marked by fear, anxiety, and avoid-
Though frequently co-occurring (Schneier et al., ance of certain places or situations. Conversely,
1992), social anxiety disorder and panic disorder fear of negative evaluation will be at the core of a
can be distinguished by the primary fear driving social anxiety disorder diagnosis.
the anxiety. While social anxiety disorder is char-
acterized by fear of negative evaluation, panic Panic disorder vs. separation anxiety disor-
disorder is characterized by fear of the panic der The final differential diagnosis, between
attacks themselves (APA, 2013). For example, a panic disorder and separation anxiety disorder,
child may fear that they will panic in school, can be one of the more difficult distinctions to
faint, and have to be carried out of the classroom make. In fact, there is strong evidence that sepa-
while the whole class watches. In this case, the ration anxiety disorder is a risk factor for panic
child is unlikely to fear social situations per se, disorder (Kossowsky et al., 2013). Both disorders
but rather the possibility of having a panic attack may include clinging to “safe” persons, often the
in a social setting. Similarly, children with social parents. Once again, making the right diagnosis
phobia may fear embarrassing themselves in depends on identifying the child’s specific fear.
public due to their anxiety response—for exam- In panic disorder, the strong desire to be close to
ple, that they will shake, trip over their words, or a safe person is driven by fears related to panic—
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 21

for example, the person with agoraphobia who is orders is also in the excellent range (Brown et al.,
concerned that she will have a panic attack when 2001). The reliability of diagnosis specifically in
help is not available. In this case, the safe person children has also been found to be good when
provides a sense of comfort in the face of a poten- using structured diagnostic interviews
tial panic attack, similar to the function of having (Schniering et al., 2000). This high level of reli-
a bottle of benzodiazepines always nearby. With ability has improved the ease of communication
separation anxiety disorder, the fear is related to between mental health professionals about a
separation from the caregiver in its own right. given patient’s clinical status.
Unwanted separation from the caregiver may While there are positive aspects to the current
trigger a bout of anxiety that leads to a panic diagnostic system, there also are significant limi-
attack, but the root of the anxiety is the separation tations to the way disorders are defined. First,
and not the panic symptoms. many diagnoses contain words like “persistent,”
“clinically significant,” and “excessive” without
Panic disorder vs. agoraphobia Agoraphobia defining the threshold for such criteria. This
should only be diagnosed when avoidance behav- vagueness can lead to disagreement across clini-
iors associated with panic attacks extend to two cians. With respect to children specifically, the
or more agoraphobic situations. current DSM does not address developmental
norms that can be expected across ages. It also
Agoraphobia vs. separation anxiety disor- does not address how specific disorders may
der Much like with panic disorder, the differen- present themselves differently in different age
tiation between agoraphobia and separation groups. Therefore, the clinician often must make
anxiety disorder lies in the specific cognitive ide- a judgment call as to whether a particular behav-
ation. In agoraphobia, the focus is on panic-like ior falls outside the realm of developmentally
or other incapacitating or embarrassing symp- appropriate behavior in a child, creating a lack of
toms in feared situations, whereas the cognitive reliability in diagnosis. By improving this defini-
ideation in separation anxiety disorder is more tion, a clearer threshold would be established that
likely thoughts on detachment from parents or would ideally incorporate developmental norms
other attachment figures. for diagnosis in children. A clearer definition of
this threshold would dramatically improve diag-
nostic reliability as much of the lack of ­diagnostic
Diagnostic Reliability agreement in this area is caused by differing defi-
nitions of what is “developmentally appropriate”
In light of the often-challenging differential diag- (Albano et al., 2003).
nosis of the disorders described in this chapter, Second, diagnoses could be improved by
arriving at a reliable diagnosis is imperative to increasing the reliability of subtypes of specific
provide treatment recommendations. The current phobias. There is significant co-occurrence of
diagnostic system was adopted in an attempt to multiple subtypes in individuals diagnosed with
increase the reliability of diagnoses across clini- specific phobias and a lack of empirical support
cians. Attempts to determine diagnostic reliabil- for the current subtypes. Blood-injection-injury
ity often rely on test-retest or interrater reliability phobias seem to have both different physiologi-
approaches, including the audio/video-recording cal responses and psychometric properties and
method. Knappe et al. (2013) demonstrated high likely represent a clear subtype. However, the
test-retest reliability of the dimensional anxiety other subtypes do not seem to have the same psy-
scales, including social anxiety disorder, agora- chometric differentiation. As with social phobia,
phobia, and panic disorder. In accordance with it may make sense to refer to specific phobias in
previous research (Lebeau et al., 2012), Knappe terms of simple type (one specific phobia) and
et al. (2013) reported low test-retest reliability for generalized type (more than one specific phobia;
specific phobia. Interrater reliability for these dis- Piqueras et al., 2008).
22 M. B. Powers et al.

Third, symptoms of panic disorder should DSM, changes in several areas of the system
more clearly be differentiated by age range. could lead to more reliable diagnosis and clearer
There is evidence that children of different ages differentiation between anxiety disorders.
report different types and numbers of symptoms.
This developmental variability needs to be
reflected in the diagnostic criteria for children. References
There may also be a need for the addition of sev-
eral symptoms currently missing from the diag- Abbo, C., Kinyanda, E., Kizza, R. B., Levin, J.,
Ndyanabangi, S., & Stein, D. J. (2013). Prevalence,
nostic criteria for children. comorbidity and predictors of anxiety disorders in chil-
Finally, there have been criticisms of the dren and adolescents in rural north-eastern Uganda.
validity of the current diagnostic categories. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health,
There is high comorbidity of the current diagnos- 7(1), 21. https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-­2000-­7-­21
Albano, A. M., Causey, D., & Carter, B. (2001). Fear and
tic criteria which often results in multiple diagno- anxiety in children. In C. E. Walker & M. C. Roberts
ses, although it is unclear whether the current (Eds.), Handbook of clinical child psychology (3rd ed.,
disorders represent distinct entities. Not only pp. 291–316). Wiley.
does this present issues for diagnostic reliability, Albano, A. M., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (2003).
Childhood anxiety disorders. In E. J. Mash & R. A.
but this causes challenges for conducting research Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (2nd ed.,
on the etiological and treatment differences pp. 279–329). Guilford Press.
among disorders (Asmundson et al., 2014). One Alfano, C. A., Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2006).
proposed option is for a quantitative hierarchical Cognitive correlates of social phobia among chil-
dren and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child
model for diagnosis (Watson, 2005). Under this Psychology, 34(2), 182–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/
model, diagnoses are categorized by empirically s10802-­005-­9012-­9
supported phenotypic and genotypic similarities. Allen, J. L., Lavallee, K. L., Herren, C., Ruhe, K., &
This system would decrease the overlap of diag- Schneider, S. (2010). DSM-IV criteria for childhood
separation anxiety disorder: Informant, age, and sex
nosis and aim to increase the validity of the diag- differences. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(8), 946–
nostic system while maintaining reliability. 952. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.06.022
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
Author.
Summary Anderson, D. J., Williams, S., McGee, R., & Silva, P. A.
(1987). DSM-III disorders in preadolescent children:
Anxiety disorders, including specific phobia, Prevalence in a large sample from the general popula-
social anxiety disorder (social phobia), panic dis- tion. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 69–76.
Arrindell, W. A., Eisemann, M., Richter, J., Oei, T. P.,
order, agoraphobia, and separation anxiety disor- Caballo, V. E., van der Ende, J., et al. (2003). Phobic
der, are common in children. Correct diagnostic anxiety in 11 nations. Part I: Dimensional constancy
assignment requires an understanding of the core of the five-factor model. Behaviour Research and
fears in each of these disorders and the various Therapy, 41, 461–479.
Asmundson, G. J. G., Taylor, S., & Smits, J. A. J. (2014).
ways that children may manifest these fears. In Panic disorder and agoraphobia: An overview and
specific phobia and social anxiety disorder, anxi- commentary on DSM-5 changes. Depression and
ety is provoked by confronting the feared stimu- Anxiety, 31(6), 480–486. https://doi.org/10.1002/
lus. Anxiety in those with agoraphobia is da.22277
Barlow, D. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature
triggered by the fear of anticipated or real expo- and treatment of anxiety and panic (2nd ed.). Guilford
sure to feared places or situations. Panic disorder Press.
is defined by fear of having panic attacks and of Becker, E. S., Rinck, M., Turke, V., Kause, P., Goodwin,
what their implications might be. Separation anx- R., Neumer, S., et al. (2007). Epidemiology of specific
phobia subtypes: Findings from the Dresden mental
iety disorder is driven by fear of being separated health study. European Psychiatry, 22, 69–74.
from one’s parents or other attachment figures. Beesdo-baum, K., Hofler, M., Gloster, A. T., Klotsche, J.,
While the current diagnostic system represents Lieb, R., Beauducel, A., et al. (2009). The structure
an improvement over previous versions of the of common mental disorders: A replication study in a
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 23

community sample of adolescents and young adults. Clauss, J. A., & Blackford, J. U. (2012). Behavioral inhi-
International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric bition and risk for developing social anxiety disor-
Research, 18(4), 203–220. der: A meta-analytic study. Journal of the American
Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (1988). Comorbidity Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(10),
of test anxiety and other anxiety disorders in chil- 1066.e1–1075.e1.
dren. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, Compton, S. N., Nelson, A. H., & March, J. S. (2000).
275–287. Social phobia and separation anxiety symptoms in
Bekker, M. H. J., & van Mens-Verhulst, J. (2007). Anxiety community and clinical samples of children and ado-
disorders: Sex differences in prevalence, degree, and lescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child
background, but gender-neutral treatment. Gender and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1040–1046.
Medicine, 4, S178–S193. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Connolly, J., Hallam, R. S., & Marks, I. M. (1976).
s1550-­8579(07)80057-­x Selective association of fainting with blood-injury-­
Bell-Dolan, D., & Brazeal, T. J. (1993). Separation anxi- illness fear. Behavior Therapy, 7(1), 8–13. https://doi.
ety disorder, overanxious disorder, and school refusal. org/10.1016/s0005-­7894(76)80214-­6
Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North Connolly, K. M., Olatunji, B. O., & Lohr, J. M. (2008).
America, 2, 563–580. Evidence for disgust sensitivity mediating the sex dif-
Bernstein, G. A., & Borchardt, C. M. (1991). Anxiety ferences found in blood-injection-injury phobia and
disorders of childhood and adolescence: A critical spider phobia. Personality and Individual Differences,
review. Journal of the American Academy of Child and 44, 898–908.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 519–532. Cornacchio, D., Chou, T., Sacks, H., Pincus, D., & Comer,
Bianchi, K. N., & Carter, M. M. (2012). An experimental J. (2015). Clinical consequences of the revised DSM-5
analysis of disgust sensitivity and fear of contagion definition of agoraphobia in treatment-seeking anx-
in Spider and Blood Injection Injury Phobia. Journal ious youth. Depression and Anxiety, 32(7), 502–508.
of Anxiety Disorders, 26(7), 753–761. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22361
org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.06.004 Costello, E. G., & Angold, A. (1995). Epidemiology. In
Bird, H. R., Gould, M. S., Yager, T., Staghezza, B., & J. S. March (Ed.), Anxiety disorders in children and
Canino, G. (1989). Risk factors for maladjustment adolescents (pp. 109–122). Guilford.
in Puerto Rican children. Journal of the American Cox, B. J., McWilliams, L. A., Clara, I. P., & Stein,
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, M. B. (2003). The structure of feared situations in a
847–850. nationally representative sample. Journal of Anxiety
Bolton, D., Eley, T. C., O’Connor, T. G., Perrin, S., Rabe-­ Disorders, 17(1), 89–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Hesketh, S., Rijskijk, F., et al. (2006). Prevalence and s0887-­6185(02)00179-­2
genetic and environmental influence on anxiety disor- Craske, M. G. (1999). Anxiety disorders. Westview press.
ders in 6-year-old twins. Psychological Medicine, 36, Craske, M. G. (2003). Origins of phobias and anxiety dis-
335–344. orders: Why more women than men? Elsevier.
Brown, T. A., Di Nardo, P. A., Lehman, C. L., & Campbell, Da Motta, W. R., de Lima, M. S., de Oliveira Soares,
L. A. (2001). Reliability of DSM-IV anxiety and B. G., Paixao, N. R., & Busnello, E. D. (2000, May).
mood disorders: Implications for the classification of An epidemic of phobic disorders in Brazil? Results
emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, from a population-based cross-sectional survey.
110, 49–58. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American
Burstein, M., Georgiades, K., He, J. P., Schmitz, A., Feig, Psychiatric Association, Chicago.
E., Khazanov, G. K., et al. (2012). Specific phobia Davey, G. C. L. (1992). Characteristics of individuals with
among U.S. adolescents: Phenomenology and typol- fear of spiders. Anxiety Research, 4, 299–314.
ogy. Depression and Anxiety, 29(12), 1072–1082. Davey, G. C. L. (1994). Self-reported fears to com-
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22008 mon indigenous animals in an adult UK population:
Cantwell, D. P., & Baker, L. (1988). Issues in the clas- The role of disgust sensitivity. British Journal of
sification of child and adolescent psychopathol- Psychology, 85, 541–554.
ogy. Journal of the American Academy of Child & de Lijster, J. M., Dierckx, B., Utens, E. M. W. J., Verhulst,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(5), 521–533. https://doi. F. C., Zieldorff, C., Dieleman, G. C., et al. (2016). The
org/10.1097/00004583-­198809000-­00001 age of onset of anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis. The
Chambless, D. L., Caputo, G. C., Bright, P., & Gallagher, Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 62(4), 237–246.
R. (1984). Assessment of fear of fear in agoraphobics: De Vries, Y. A., Al-Hamzawi, A., Alonso, J., Borges, G.,
The body sensations questionnaire and the agorapho- Bruffaerts, R., Bunting, B., et al. (2019). Childhood
bic conditions questionnaire. Journal of Consulting generalized specific phobia as an early marker of
and Clinical Psychology, 52, 1090–1097. internalizing psychopathology across the lifespan:
Chapman, L. K., Kertz, S. J., Zurlage, M. M., & Woodruff-­ Results from the World Mental Health Surveys.
Borden, J. (2008). A confirmatory factor analysis of BMC Medicine, 17(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/
specific phobia domains in African American and s12916-­019-­1328-­3
Caucasian American young adults. Journal of Anxiety Dierker, L., Albano, A. M., Clarke, G. N., Heimberg,
Disorders, 22(5), 763–771. R. G., Kendall, P. C., Merikangas, K. R., et al. (2001).
24 M. B. Powers et al.

Screening for anxiety and depression in early adoles- referred children with specific phobia. Journal of
cence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Child and Family Studies, 24(7), 2127–2134. https://
Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 929–936. doi.org/10.1007/s10826-­014-­0014-­5
Emmelkamp, P. M. G., & Scholing, A. (1997). Anxiety Karno, M., Golding, J. M., Burnam, M. A., Hough, R. I.,
disorders. In C. A. Essau & F. Petermann (Eds.), Escobar, J. I., Wells, K. M., et al. (1989). Anxiety dis-
Developmental psychopathology (pp. 219–263). orders among Mexican Americans and non-Hispanic
Harwood Academic Publishers. Whites in Los Angeles. Journal of Nervous and
Emmelkamp, P. M. G., &Wittchen, H. U. (2008). Stress-­ Mental Disease, 177, 202–209.
induced and fear circuitry disorders, specific phobias Kashani, J. H., & Orvaschel, H. (1988). Anxiety disorders
(Chap 4). In G. Andrews, D. Charney, P. J. Sirovatka, in midadolescence: A community sample. American
D. A. Regier, & V. A. A. P. A. Arlington (Eds.), DSM Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 960–964.
V. American Psychiatric Publishing (in press). Kawakami, N., Takeshima, T., Ono, Y., Uda, H., Hata,
Essau, C. A., Conradt, J., & Petermann, F. (1999a). Y., Nakane, Y., et al. (2005). Twelve-month preva-
Frequency of panic attacks and panic disorder in ado- lence, severity, and treatment of common mental dis-
lescents. Depression and Anxiety, 9, 19–26. orders in communities in Japan: Preliminary finding
Essau, C. A., Conradt, J., & Petermann, F. (1999b). from the World Mental Health Japan Survey 2002-­
Frequency and comorbidity of social phobia and 2003. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 59(4),
social fears in adolescents. Behaviour Research and 441–452.
Therapy, 37, 831–843. Kearney, C. A., Albano, A. M., Eisen, A. R., Allan, W. D.,
Essau, C. A., Conradt, J., & Petermann, F. (2000). & Barlow, D. H. (1997). The phenomenology of panic
Frequency, comorbidity, and psychosocial impairment disorder in youngsters: An empirical study of a clinical
of specific phobia in adolescents. Journal of Clinical sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11(1), 49–62.
Child Psychology, 29, 221–231. Kendall, P. C., Safford, S., Flannery-Schroeder, E.,
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional process- & Webb, A. (2004). Child anxiety treatment:
ing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Outcomes in adolescence and impact on substance
Psychological Bulletin, 90, 20–35. use and depression at 7.4-year follow-up. Journal of
Francis, G., Last, C. G., & Strauss, C. C. (1987). Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 276–287.
Expression of separation anxiety disorder: The roles Kendler, K. S., & Prescott, C. A. (2006). Genes, envi-
of age and gender. Child Psychiatry and Human ronment, and psychopathology: Understanding the
Development, 18, 82–89. causes of psychiatric and substance use disorders.
Fredrikson, M., Annas, P., Fischer, H., & Wik, G. (1996). Guilford Press.
Gender and age differences in the prevalence of spe- Kerns, C. M., Read, K. L., Klugman, J., & Kendall,
cific fears and phobias. Behaviour Research and P. C. (2013). Cognitive behavioral therapy for
Therapy, 34, 33–39. youth with social anxiety: Differential short and
Fyer, A. J., Mannuzza, S., Chapman, T. F., Martin, L. L., & long-term treatment outcomes. Journal of Anxiety
Klein, D. F. (1995). Specificity in familial aggregation Disorders, 27(2), 210–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
of phobic disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, janxdis.2013.01.009
52, 564–573. Kessler, R. C., Foster, C. L., Saunders, W. B., & Stang,
Gregory, A. M., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Koenen, K., Eley, P. E. (1995). Social consequences of psychiatric disor-
T. C., & Poulton, R. (2007). Juvenile mental health ders, I: Educational attainment. American Journal of
histories of adults with anxiety disorders. American Psychiatry, 152, 1026–1032.
Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 301–308. Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R.,
Hale, W. W., Raaijmakers, Q., Muris, P., van Hoof, A., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime
& Meeus, W. (2008). Developmental trajectories of prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV
adolescent anxiety disorder symptoms: A 5-year pro- disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey
spective community study. Journal of the American Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62,
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 593–602.
556–564. Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Jin, R., Ruscio, A. M., Shear,
Hettema, J. M., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2001). A K., & Walters, E. E. (2006). The epidemiology of
review and meta-analysis of the genetic epidemiology panic attacks, panic disorder, and agoraphobia in the
of anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of
158, 1568–1578. General Psychiatry, 63, 415–424.
Hettema, J. M., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J. M., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky,
& Kendler, K. S. (2005). The structure of genetic and A. M., & Wittchen, H. (2012). Twelve-month and life-
environmental risk factors for anxiety disorders in time prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety
men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, and mood disorders in the United States. International
182–189. Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21(3),
Kane, E. J., Braunstein, K., Ollendick, T. H., & Muris, 169–184. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1359
P. (2014). Relations of anxiety sensitivity, control Kim, S. J., Kim, B. N., Cho, S. C., Kim, J. W., Shin, M. S.,
beliefs, and maternal over-control to fears in clinic-­ Yoo, H. J., et al. (2010). The prevalence of specific pho-
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 25

bia and associated co-morbid features in children and Marks, I. M. (1987). Fears, phobias and rituals. Oxford
adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(6), 629– University Press.
634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.04.004 Marmorstein, N. R., White, H. R., Loeber, R., &
King, N. J. (1993). Simple and social phobias. In T. H. Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2010). Anxiety as a predic-
Ollendick & R. J. Prinz (Eds.), Advances in clini- tor of age at first use of substances and progression
cal child psychology (Vol. 15, pp. 305–341). Plenum to substance use problems among boys. Journal of
Press. Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 211–224.
Knappe, S., Klotsche, J., Heyde, F., Hiob, S., Siegert, J., Masi, G., Favilla, L., Mucci, M., & Millepidei, S. (2000).
Hoyer, J., et al. (2013). Test–retest reliability and sen- Panic disorder in clinically referred children and ado-
sitivity to change of the dimensional anxiety scales for lescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development,
DSM-5. CNS Spectrums, 19(03), 256–267. https://doi. 31, 139–151.
org/10.1017/s1092852913000710 McGee, R., Fehan, M., Williams, S., Partridge, F., Silva,
Kossowsky, J., Pfaltz, M. C., Schneider, S., Taeymans, J., P. A., & Kelly, J. (1990). DSM-III disorders in a
Locher, C., & Gaab, J. (2013). The separation anxi- large sample of adolescents. Journal of the American
ety hypothesis of panic disorder revisited: A meta-­ Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29,
analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(7), 611–619.
768–781. McLean, C. P., & Anderson, E. R. (2009). Brave men
Last, C. G. (1989). Anxiety disorders of childhood or ado- and timid women? A review of the gender dif-
lescence. In C. G. Last & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook ferences in fear and anxiety. Clinical Psychology
of child psychiatric diagnosis (pp. 156–169). Wiley. Review, 29(6), 496–505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Last, C. G., & Perrin, S. (1993). Anxiety disorders in cpr.2009.05.003
African-American and white children. Journal of Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995). The etiology
Abnormal Child Psychology, 21(2), 153–164. of phobias: A non-associative account. Clinical
Last, C. G., Strauss, C. C., & Francis, G. (1987b). Psychology Review, 15, 23–48.
Comorbidity among childhood anxiety disorders. Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson,
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 726–730. S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., et al. (2010). Lifetime
Last, C. G., Perrin, S., Hersen, M., & Kazdin, A. E. prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents:
(1992). DSM-III-R anxiety disorders in children: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey
Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics. Replication--Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A).
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 1070–1076. Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(10), 980–989. https://doi.
Last, C. G., Perrin, S., Hersen, M., & Kazdin, A. E. org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017
(1996). A prospective study of childhood anxiety dis- Moreau, D., & Follett, C. (1993). Panic disorder in
orders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and children and adolescents. Child and Adolescent
Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1502–1510. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 2, 581–602.
LeBeau, R. T., Glenn, D., Liao, B., Wittchen, H. U., Muris, P., Schmidt, H., & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The
Beesdo-baum, K., Ollendick, T., et al. (2010). Specific structure of specific phobia symptoms among children
phobia: A review of DSM-IV specific phobia and pre- and adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37,
liminary recommendations for DSM-V. Depression 863–868.
and Anxiety, 27(2), 148–167. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Muris, P., van der Heiden, S., & Rassin, E. (2008). Disgust
da.20655 sensitivity and psychopathological symptoms in non-­
Lebeau, R. T., Glenn, D. E., Hanover, L. N., Beesdo-­ clinical children. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Baum, K., Wittchen, H. U., & Craske, M. G. (2012). A Experimental Psychiatry, 39(2), 133–146. https://doi.
dimensional approach to measuring anxiety for DSM-­ org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.02.001
5. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Na, H. R., Kang, E. H., Lee, J. H., & Yu, B. H. (2011).
Research, 21(4), 258–272. https://doi.org/10.1002/ The genetic basis of panic disorder. Journal of Korean
mpr.1369 Medical Science, 26(6), 701. https://doi.org/10.3346/
Livingston, R., Taylor, J. L., & Crawford, S. L. (1988). A jkms.2011.26.6.701
study of somatic complaints and psychiatric diagnosis Nelles, W. B., & Barlow, D. H. (1988). Do children panic?
in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 359–372.
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 185–187. Olatunji, B. O. (2006). Evaluative learning and emotional
March, J. S. (2012). Multidimensional anxiety scale responding to fearful and disgusting stimuli in spider
for children (2nd ed.). (MASC 2)™. MultiHealth phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 858–876.
Systems. Olatunji, B. O., & Deacon, B. (2008). Specificity of dis-
March, J. S., Parker, J. D. A., Sullivan, K., Stallings, P., gust sensitivity in the prediction of fear and disgust
& Conners, C. (1997). The Multidimentional Anxiety responding to a brief spider exposure. Journal of
Scale for Children (MASC): Factor structure, reliabil- Anxiety Disorders, 22, 328–336.
ity, and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Ollendick, T. H., King, N. J., & Muris, P. (2002). Fears
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 123, 554–565. and phobias in children: Phenomenology, epidemi-
26 M. B. Powers et al.

ology, and aetiology. Child and Adolescent Mental Stevenson, J., Batten, N., & Cherner, M. (1992). Fears
Health, 7, 98–106. and fearfulness in children and adolescents: A genetic
Öst, L.-G. (1992). Blood and injection phobia: Background analysis of twin data. Journal of Child Psychology and
and cognitive, physiological, and behavioral variables. Psychiatry, 33, 977–985.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 68–74. Stinson, F. S., Dawson, D. A., Chou, S. P., Smith, S.,
Paulus, F. W., Backes, A., Sander, C. S., Weber, M., & von Goldstein, R. B., Ruan, W. J., et al. (2007). The epi-
Gontard, A. (2015). Anxiety disorders and behavioral demiology of DSM-IV specific phobia in the USA:
inhibition in preschool children: A population-based Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey
study. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 46(1), on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psychological
150–157. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-­014-­0460-­8 Medicine, 37, 1047–1059.
Perna, G., Caldirola, D., Arancio, C., & Bellodi, L. Strauss, C. C., & Last, C. G. (1993). Social and simple
(1997). Panic attacks: A twin study. Psychiatry phobias in children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 7,
Research, 66(1), 69–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/ 141–152.
s0165-­1781(97)85177-­3 Telch, M. J. (1987). The panic appraisal inventory.
Pine, D. S., Cohen, P., & Brook, J. (2001). Adolescent fears Unpublished manuscript, University of Texas.
as predictors of depression. Biological Psychiatry, 50, Van Houtem, C. M. H. H., Laine, M. L., Boomsma, D. I.,
721–724. Ligthart, L., van Wijk, A. J., & De Jongh, A. (2013).
Piqueras, J. A., Olivares, J., & López-Pina, J. A. (2008). A review and meta-analysis of the heritability of spe-
A new proposal for the subtypes of social phobia in cific phobia subtypes and corresponding fears. Journal
a sample of Spanish adolescents. Journal of Anxiety of Anxiety Disorders, 27(4), 379–388. https://doi.
Disorders, 22, 67–77. org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.04.007
Roberts, R. E., Ramsay, C., & Yun Xing, R. (2007). Rates Van Roy, B., Kristensen, H., Groholt, B., & Clench-­
of DSM-IV psychiatric disorders among adolescents Aas, J. (2009). Prevalence and characteristics of sig-
in a large metropolitan area. Journal of Psychiatric nificant social anxiety in children aged 8–13 years:
Research, 41, 959–967. A Norwegian cross-sectional population study.
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2000). Disgust. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 44,
In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of 407–415.
emotions (pp. 637–653). Guilford. Verhulst, F. C., van der Ende, J., Ferdinand, R. F., &
Ruscio, A. M., Brown, T. A., Chiu, W. T., Sareen, J., Stein, Kasius, M. C. (1997). The prevalence of DSM-III-R
M. B., & Kessler, R. C. (2008). Social Fears and Social diagnoses in a national sample of Dutch adolescents.
Phobia in the United States: Results from the National Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 329–336.
Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Waite, P., & Creswell, C. (2014). Children and adoles-
Medicine, 38(1), 15–28. cents referred for treatment of anxiety disorders:
Schneier, F. R., Johnson, J., Hornig, C. D., Liebowitz, Differences in clinical characteristics. Journal of
M. R., & Weissman, M. M. (1992). Social phobia: Affective Disorders, 167(100), 326–332. https://doi.
Comorbidity and morbidity in an epidemiologic sam- org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.06.028
ple. Archives of general psychiatry, 49(4), 282–288. Wardenaar, K. J., Lim, C. C. W., Al-Hamzawi, A. O.,
Schniering, C. A., Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2000). Alonso, J., Andrade, L. H., Benjet, C., et al. (2017).
Issues in the diagnosis and assessment of anxi- The cross-national epidemiology of specific phobia
ety disorders in children and adolescents. Clinical in the World Mental Health Surveys. Psychological
Psychology Review, 20, 453–478. Medicine, 47(10), 1744–1760.
Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). The anxi- Watson, D. (2005). Rethinking the mood and anxiety dis-
ety disorders interview schedule for children for orders: A quantitative hierarchical model for DSM-5.
DSM-IV (child and parent versions). Psychological Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 522–536.
Corporation. Wittchen, H.-U., Nelson, C. B., & Lachner, G. (1998a).
Silverman, W. K., & Rabian, B. (1993). Simple phobias. Prevalence of mental disorders and psychoso-
Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North cial impairments in adolescents and young adults.
America, 2, 603–622. Psychological Medicine, 28, 109–126.
Silverman, W. K., Kurtines, W. M., Ginsburg, G. S., Wittchen, H.-U., Reed, V., & Kessler, R. C. (1998b). The
Weems, C. R., Rabian, B., & Serafini, L. T. (1999). relationship of agoraphobia and panic disorder in a
Contingency managment, self-control, and education community sample of adolescents and young adults.
support in the treatment of childhood phobic disorders: Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 1017–1024.
A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Wittchen, H. U., Lieb, R., Schuster, P., & Oldehinkel, A. J.
Clinical Psychology, 67, 675–687. (1999). When is onset? Investigations into early devel-
Stein, D. J., Lim, C. C. W., Roest, A. M., de Jonge, P., opmental stages of anxiety and depressive disorders.
Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., & Al-Hamzawi, A. (2017). The In J. L. Rappaport (Ed.), Childhood onset of “adult”
cross-national epidemiology of social anxiety dis- psychopathology: Clinical and research advances
order: Data from the World Mental Health Survey (pp. 259–302). American Psychiatric Press.
Initiative. BMC Medicine, 15(143), 1–12. Wittchen, H.-U., Lecrubier, Y., Beesdo, K., & Nocon,
A. (2003). Relationships among anxiety disorders:
2 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Phobias and Phobic Conditions 27

Patterns and implications. In D. J. Nutt & J. C. els of mental disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North
Ballenger (Eds.), Anxiety disorders (pp. 23–37). John America, 32(3), 465–481.
Wiley & Sons. Ltd. Woody, S. R., McLean, C., & Klassen, T. (2005). Disgust
Wittchen, H.-U., Nocon, A., Beesdo, K., Pine, D. S., Hofler, as a motivator of avoidance of spiders. Journal of
S., Lieb, R., & Gloster, A. T. (2008). Agoraphobia and Anxiety Disorders, 19, 461–475.
panic: Prospective-longitudinal relations suggest a Xu, Y., Schneier, F., Heimberg, R. G., Princisvalle, K.,
rethinking of diagnostic concepts. Psychotherapy and Liebowitz, M. R., Wang, S., et al. (2012). Gender dif-
Psychosomatics, 77, 147–157. ferences in social anxiety disorder: Results from the
Wittchen, H.-U., Beesdo, K., & Gloster, A. T. (2009). national epidemiologic sample on alcohol and related
The position of anxiety disorders in structural mod- conditions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26(1),
12–19.
Issues in Differential Diagnosis:
Considering Generalized Anxiety 3
Disorder, Obsessive-Compulsive
Disorder, and Posttraumatic Stress
Disorder

Nicole Fleischer, Jonathan Rabner,


Julia Spandorfer, and Philip C. Kendall

Introduction order also met criteria for an additional disorder


(Kendall et al., 2010). Differential diagnosis
As a rule, an accurate diagnosis provides a foun- among disorders (i.e., anxiety and related disor-
dation for case conceptualization and facilitates ders) poses challenges to both researchers and
effective treatment practices, and accurate diag- clinicians.
noses are critical to the organization of partici- The diagnostic assessment of children and
pants for empirical research. Although not adolescents carries with it additional consider-
without its own problems, the Diagnostic and ations not present when working with adults. For
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), example, DSM-5 identifies some developmental
currently in its fifth iteration (American differences in the diagnostic criteria for general-
Psychiatric Association, 2013), is the most fre- ized anxiety disorder: only one physical symp-
quently used taxonomic system for organizing tom is required for children and adolescents,
psychological disorders. Within this framework, whereas three physical symptoms are required
disorders are presented as categories (discrete for adults. When diagnosing obsessive-­
entities) characterized by specific criteria. compulsive disorder, the criterion requiring that
Although specifying criteria is a decided compulsions be aimed at reducing distress is
improvement, one of the shortcomings of a cate- laxed for children. Additionally, separate criteria
gorical approach is the existence of considerable exist for diagnosing posttraumatic stress disorder
overlap in symptomatology among disorders. in children 6 years and younger. Thus, features
Indeed, comorbidity is common, and among that may serve to differentiate disorders among
youth with anxiety disorders, it is the norm adults may or may not apply to youth. Both chil-
(Kendall et al., 2001; Merikangas et al., 2010). dren and parents typically provide information
For example, in a large sample of 7–17-year-olds, about the presenting youth. However, the agree-
55% of youth who met criteria for an anxiety dis- ment between parent and child reports of anxiety
disorders is usually limited (Choudhury et al.,
N. Fleischer 2003; De Los Reyes, 2011). Clinicians can
Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine, resolve this discrepancy by assigning a diagnosis
Philadelphia, PA, USA if the child meets criteria by either the child’s
J. Rabner · J. Spandorfer · P. C. Kendall (*) report or the parents’ report. Nevertheless, the
Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA reasons underlying parent–child discrepancies
e-mail: philip.kendall@temple.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2023 29


D. McKay, E. A. Storch (eds.), Handbook of Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-14080-8_3
30 N. Fleischer et al.

may be important for case conceptualization and youths’ symptomatology within each domain
treatment and should not be overlooked (De Los will help our understanding. Next, the reliability
Reyes & Kazdin, 2005). For example, differences and validity of the diagnoses will be considered.
may be contingent upon the observability of the Last, this chapter will discuss current research
symptoms being reported (Comer & Kendall, findings in terms of their diagnostic
2004). Given the limited number of studies spe- implications.
cific to issues of differential diagnosis among
youth, the present discussion also draws upon
findings from the adult literature. That said, it is  ssential Features of GAD, OCD,
E
clear that research is needed before conclusions and PTSD
can be drawn regarding differential diagnosis
among youth. Generalized Anxiety Disorder
This chapter addresses issues of differential
diagnosis pertaining to generalized anxiety disor- The hallmark of GAD is the presence of exces-
der (GAD), obsessive-compulsive disorder sive, uncontrollable, and persistent worry about a
(OCD), and posttraumatic stress disorder number of events or activities, more days than
(PTSD), with an emphasis on the potential diag- not for at least 6 months (APA, 2013). Worry has
nostic confusions among these three disorders. been defined as “a chain of thoughts and images,
As is often the case among psychological disor- negatively affect-laden and relatively uncontrol-
ders, GAD, OCD, and PTSD share similar symp- lable” (Borkovec et al., 1983, p. 10). For youth,
tom presentations. Perhaps most prominent, and these worries frequently concern health, school,
potentially most troublesome, all three disorders and personal harm (Silverman et al., 1995; Muris
are characterized by the presence of intrusive, et al., 2000) and must be associated with at least
repetitive cognition that causes distress. Among one physiological symptom (i.e., feeling keyed
adults and youth, it can be difficult to delineate up or on edge; being easily fatigued; difficulty
diagnostic boundaries. For example, a child may concentrating or mind going blank; irritability;
report that they frequently experience thoughts of muscle tension; or sleep disturbance). In addi-
their mother in a car crash. This presentation tion, the worry or physical symptoms must cause
could suggest worry regarding the safety of fam- distress or impairment in important areas of func-
ily members, obsessional thinking, or flashbacks tioning, which for youth often include school/
of an earlier trauma, corresponding to GAD, academics, peer relationships (i.e., Verduin &
OCD, and PTSD, respectively. How best to make Kendall, 2008), and family/home life.
sense of such a youth report? Critical to differential diagnosis, the focus of
This chapter will first provide a description of the anxiety and worry present in GAD cannot be
the essential diagnostic features of GAD, OCD, better accounted for by features of another disor-
and PTSD in youth along with a brief overview der. For example, if the excessive and interfering
of epidemiological findings. We then examine worry is entirely focused on peer evaluation,
specific areas of diagnostic overlap and confu- social anxiety disorder may be more apt than
sion. These domains are organized into five cate- GAD, or if the worry is solely about gaining
gories of symptoms: (1) fear/anxiety, (2) recurrent weight, a diagnosis of anorexia nervosa may be
thoughts, (3) intrusive images, (4) physical appropriate. In addition, the symptoms of GAD
symptoms, and (5) avoidance. These symptom must not be attributable to the effects of a sub-
domains are present in nearly every anxiety dis- stance or a medical condition.
order, help distinguish anxiety disorders from Onset of GAD in childhood does exist (e.g.,
other disorders, and may facilitate differential Kendall et al., 2010), but it is the case that the
diagnosis. Given the high rate of comorbidity, main incidence period for GAD is adolescence
symptom overlap, and heterogeneity within diag- and adulthood (Beesdo et al., 2010), with preva-
nostic categories, a nuanced examination of lence estimates in adolescence at about 3%
3 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Considering Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Obsessive-Compulsive… 31

(Burstein et al., 2014). Onset of GAD is often tion disorder. Regarding differential diagnosis,
earlier in females than in males, with prevalence DSM-5 specifies that obsessions are not simply
estimates increasing with age in children and excessive worries about real-life problems, but
adolescents (Beesdo et al., 2009). Additional instead center around irrational, magical worries
research is needed to examine the gender ratio in (APA, 2013). The obsessions and compulsions
youth. cannot fall within the circumscribed content
domains of other anxiety or obsessive-­compulsive
disorders. For example, obsessions related to
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder one’s own appearance may better fall under the
diagnosis of BDD, and hair-pulling (not cos-
OCD is characterized by the presence of obses- metic) may be better accounted for by a diagnosis
sions or compulsions. Most individuals with of trichotillomania.
OCD experience both obsessions and compul- Similar to GAD, OCD likely develops between
sions, and the presence of pure obsessions is childhood and mid-adolescence, with the average
uncommon (APA, 2013; Foa et al., 1995). age of onset falling between 7.5 and 12.5 years
Obsessions are defined as recurrent and persis- (Geller et al., 1998). Several epidemiologic stud-
tent thoughts, impulses, or images that are intru- ies conducted with adolescents in the United
sive, inappropriate, and experienced as distressing States and elsewhere report prevalence rates
(APA, 2013). The most common obsessions ranging from approximately 2% to 4% of the
among clinically affected youth involve themes pediatric (Geller, 2006). Among clinic-referred
of contamination, harm or death, and symmetry youth, the lifetime prevalence rate is approxi-
(Hanna, 1995), though recent research demon- mately 15% (Last et al., 1992). The gender ratio
strates that children and adolescents may differ in is not clear in children: some data suggest that
presentations of obsessions, including an OCD is more common in boys (Geller et al.,
increased presentation of sexual obsessions in 1998; Zohar et al., 1997), whereas other data
adolescents (Selles et al., 2014). Youth with OCD indicate no difference in sex distribution (Anholt
often perform compulsions to ignore, hold back, et al., 2014; Chabane et al., 2005).
or neutralize obsessive thoughts and related feel-
ings. Compulsions are repetitive, intentional
behaviors performed to reduce anxiety or distress Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
and are often performed stereotypically or
according to rigid rules. Unlike adults, youth do PTSD is characterized by a constellation of
not have to recognize that the obsessions or com- symptoms that develop in response to a trauma.
pulsions are excessive or unreasonable. While Specifically, it can occur when an individual
children and adolescents do not necessarily differ experiences, witnesses, learns about a close fam-
in severity of symptoms, adolescents may experi- ily member or friend experiencing, or is repeat-
ence more control over compulsions (Selles edly exposed to as part of one’s job (school), an
et al., 2014). Given that intrusive thoughts and event involving actual or threatened death, seri-
images regularly occur in the general population, ous injury, or sexual violence (APA, 2013).
symptoms must be distressing, time consuming In DSM-5, PTSD was relocated to a new cat-
(lasting more than 1 h per day), or interfering egory, “Trauma and Stressor-related Disorders.”
with academic functioning, social activities, or Further, the DSM-5 includes separate diagnostic
relationships to warrant a diagnosis of OCD. criteria for PTSD for children and for adults older
The DSM-5 separates anxiety and obsessive-­ than six and children younger than six. For those
compulsive disorders, creating a separate cate- older than six, there must be at least one intrusion
gory of obsessive-compulsive disorders that symptom (e.g., recurrent distressing dreams), at
include OCD, body dysmorphic disorder (BDD), least one avoidance symptom (e.g., avoidance of
hoarding disorder, trichotillomania, and excoria- memories), at least two negative alterations in
32 N. Fleischer et al.

cognitions and mood (e.g., distorted blame about much lower prevalence rate of 0.1% (Lavigne
the traumatic event, persistent negative emotional et al., 1996).
state), and at least two alterations in arousal and
reactivity (e.g., irritable behavior, hypervigi-
lance). The disturbance must last for longer than Domains of Symptom Overlap
1 month and cause meaningful distress or impair-
ment in important areas of functioning. For those Fear/Anxiety
younger than six, a lower threshold of symptoms
must be met, and there is a focus on observable All anxiety disorders are characterized by the
symptoms rather than reports on internal presence of fear or apprehension in some form,
experiences. and the presence of fear/anxiety can help differ-
A history of adverse or traumatic events can entiate anxiety disorders from other psychopa-
be present in individuals with diagnoses other thology, such as mood disorders. This domain
than PTSD. The stress associated with traumatic can include fear of specific stimuli, situations, or
events may serve as a catalyst for the manifesta- feelings. Fear of specific stimuli is characteristic
tion of an underlying vulnerability, as described of OCD and among the possible symptoms of the
in the diathesis-stress model. Thus, the presence disorder. It may also be present in GAD and
of a traumatic event is necessary but not suffi- PTSD, but it is not explicitly included as a symp-
cient for a diagnosis of PTSD. Moreover, differ- tom of the disorders. Likewise, fear of a specific
ential diagnosis was informed by the timing of situation (e.g., riding the bus) may be present in
symptom onset. As noted in DSM-5, symptoms GAD, OCD, and PTSD, requiring a more specific
of intrusion, avoidance, negative alterations of understanding of the fear. For example, a child
cognitions and mood, and alterations of arousal with GAD may fear that the bus will get lost or
and reactivity must begin or worsen after the will cause him to be late, whereas a child with
trauma. Based on this framework, symptoms of OCD may fear that every time the bus hits a bump
PTSD were distinguished from psychotic halluci- it is running over a person; a child with PTSD
nation, agoraphobia, specific phobia, and depres- may have previously been in a bus accident.
sion. Of course, the presence of PTSD does not An interesting notion is the “fear of fear” (e.g.,
exclude the presence of other disorders, as evi- fear of experiencing fear; Goldstein & Chambless,
denced by the high comorbidity rates of both 1978), sometimes referred to as anxiety sensitiv-
adults and children diagnosed with PTSD ity, that is often thought of as the signature fea-
(Salloum et al., 2018). ture of panic disorder, but it may also be present
Notably, not all youth who experience trauma in GAD (Knapp et al., 2016) and PTSD (Hensley
develop PTSD, and there is evidence to suggest & Varela, 2008; Viana et al., 2018). Individuals
that age, gender, and environmental factors all with heightened anxiety sensitivity evidence a
play a role in differential outcomes (e.g., Furr fear of physiological symptoms of anxiety due to
et al., 2010; Bokszczanin, 2007, 2008; McNally, the belief that those sensations are deleterious to
1993). Approximately 16% of children who are their physical, psychological, and/or social well-­
exposed to a traumatic event develop PTSD being (Schmidt et al., 2010; Reiss & McNally,
(Alisic et al., 2014). A survey of adolescents in a 1985). In GAD, anxiety sensitivity has been asso-
population-based sample found a 4.7% preva- ciated with worry regarding uncertainty (Floyd
lence rate for PTSD, which was significantly et al., 2005). Regarding PTSD, anxiety sensitiv-
higher among females (7.3%) compared to males ity has been thought to be a vulnerability and
(2.2%; McLaughlin et al., 2013). While boys are maintenance factor (Elwood et al., 2009) and has
more likely to be victims of physical violence, been implicated as such in children and adoles-
girls are more likely to experience sexual vio- cents (e.g., Kiliç et al., 2008).
lence (McLaughlin et al., 2013). A large-scale Intolerance of uncertainty (IU) has been
survey of PTSD in children aged 2–5 revealed a defined as “an individual’s dispositional incapac-
3 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Considering Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Obsessive-Compulsive… 33

ity to endure the aversive response triggered by cated by the social context of threat that emerges
the perceived absence of salient, key, or sufficient during times of terrorism (Comer & Kendall,
information, and sustained by the associated per- 2007).
ception of uncertainty” (Carleton, 2016, p. 31). Obsessions and worry are also frequently con-
Research has demonstrated a strong relationship fused/conflated with rumination. Rumination,
between IU and GAD in youth (Read et al., 2013; with reference to its role in depression, has been
Donovan et al., 2016), and some evidence exists defined as “repetitively focusing on the fact that
for the association between IU and OCD (Wright one is depressed; on one’s symptoms of depres-
et al., 2016). Although IU does not appear to dif- sion; and on the causes, meanings and conse-
ferentiate between OCD and GAD, it may be quences of depressive symptoms”
useful in distinguishing the two disorders from (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). Worry and rumination
others. Further research is needed to examine the are two forms of the transdiagnostic construct of
utility of IU in youth for differentiating among repetitive negative thinking differentiated in tem-
the anxiety disorders (Kendall et al., 2020). poral orientation, such that worry is more future-­
oriented, whereas rumination is past-oriented
(McEvoy et al., 2013). These distinctions
Recurrent Thoughts between worry and rumination have been repli-
cated in adolescent samples (e.g., Hong, 2007;
The fact that there is a similar presentation of Muris et al., 2004). Complicating matters, fea-
recurrent, intrusive thoughts among GAD, OCD, tures of rumination may play etiological and
and PTSD (and other psychopathology) is per- maintenance roles in versions of anxiety. For
haps one of the most challenging aspects of dif- example, the compulsion to ruminate and repeat-
ferential diagnosis. Research and theoretical edly ask questions such as “why” and “what if”
discussions have emphasized the similarities of has been associated with the onset and mainte-
intrusive cognition for youth with GAD and OCD nance of PTSD (Michael et al., 2007) and can be
(Comer et al., 2004) and youth with OCD and a part of GAD (reassurance seeking) in youth.
PTSD (Huppert et al., 2005). Although there is a meaningful overlap among
Turner et al. (1992) suggest that worries and the various forms of recurrent thoughts, some
obsessions present similarly regarding presence, cognitive features may help to differentiate
form, and content within both clinical and non- between them. One distinction is the content of
clinical adults. Furthermore, worries and obses- recurrent thought. As described by DSM-5, wor-
sions are often experienced as frequent, ries characteristic of GAD consist of everyday
uncontrollable, and facilitate negative mood and life events or activities, such as health, relation-
attention biases in these groups. However, wor- ships, school, and finances. In contrast, obses-
ries and obsessions have a vulnerability factor sions tend to be more irrational and bizarre (less
distinguishing between clinical and nonclinical logically connected), although children may lack
adults. Given the overlap, some have suggested this insight (Krebs & Heyman, 2015). The con-
that obsession and worry may coexist on a single tent of obsessions tend to fall into more circum-
continuum (Langlois et al., 2000b). Huppert et al. scribed areas, including dirt/contamination, sex,
(2005) demonstrated that items on rating scales, aggression, self-doubt, and order. Children’s
such as “unpleasant thoughts come into my mind worries seem to be more logical, whereas obses-
against my will and I cannot get rid of them” and sions have an illogical (disconnected) quality
“I find it difficult to control my own thoughts,” (Comer et al., 2004). The content of recurrent
are characteristic of both OCD and PTSD. It is thoughts present in PTSD is typically associated
not surprising, and it is understandable that clini- with re-experiencing the trauma. However, such
cians and researchers find it difficult to capture cognition is not limited to the recollection of the
the exact nature of an adult’s or child’s cognitive trauma per se and may also include themes of
intrusions. This effort may be further compli- danger, negative self-schema, and evaluation of
34 N. Fleischer et al.

the meaning of the trauma (De Silva & Marks, depression, and certain psychotic disorders
1999). (Starcevic & Berle, 2006). Studies support the
Distinctions have also been made regarding presence of thought-action fusion in youth with
the evaluation of the thought content. In particu- OCD (Farrell & Barrett, 2006; Libby et al. 2004);
lar, content of obsessions tends to be experienced however, one study did not find a significant dis-
as contradictory to one’s own beliefs and values, tinction in thought-action fusion between youth
whereas worries tend to be experienced as con- with OCD and youth with anxiety disorders
sistent (Langlois et al., 2000a, b; Turner et al., (Barrett & Healy, 2003). According to one study,
1992; Brakoulias & Starcevic, 2011). Given the thought-action fusion plays a minor role in anxi-
contradictory nature of some obsessions, they ety disorders but may have a particular associa-
may be accompanied by feelings of responsibil- tion with OCD, as evidenced by significantly
ity for having the thoughts (Langlois et al., 2000a; higher thought-action fusion scores in youth with
Salkovskis, 1985). However, this distinction may OCD than youth with anxiety disorders who in
vary by the content area of the obsessions (Wells turn has higher scores than non-clinical youth
& Papageorgiou, 1998). (Libby et al., 2004). Attention should be paid to
The presence or absence of identifiable trig- the interaction between thought content and
gers may help to distinguish between different meta-cognitive processes. The presence and
forms of recurrent thought, although as with quality of recurrent thoughts should be consid-
other phenomena should not be considered in ered within the full constellation of symptoms.
isolation. Studies have found that adults with
intrusive worries are more aware of specific
external or internal precipitants of the recurrent Intrusive Images
thoughts as compared to adults with intrusive
obsessions (Langlois et al., 2000a; Turner et al., The symptom presentation of both OCD and
1992), although research in youth is needed. A PTSD may include intrusive images. Intrusive
key feature of PTSD is the psychological and images associated with PTSD are typically
physiological distress in response to triggers related to the initial trauma and are frequently
associated with the trauma, which are often fragmented sensory memories (Ehlers et al.,
avoided. However, individuals with PTSD are 2004; Lafleur et al., 2011). Youth with PTSD do
frequently unaware of the triggers that give rise not engage in compulsions in response to intru-
to intrusive memories (Ehlers et al., 2004). These sive images (Lafleur et al., 2011). However,
triggers may only be loosely associated with the recent research has demonstrated that previous
trauma and may not be directly meaningful to the history of traumatic experiences may increase the
individual. presence of intrusive obsessions that may overlap
Thought-action fusion is a meta-cognitive with OCD symptoms (Barzilay et al., 2019).
construct that may help differentiate between While images in OCD are typically characterized
OCD and other disorders. This construct consists as bizarre or irrational, individuals with OCD can
of (1) believing that the presence of a thought also experience intrusive images associated with
increases the probability of an event actually a prior adverse event (De Silva & Marks, 2001;
occurring and (2) that the presence of a thought Lipinski & Pope, 1994; Speckens et al., 2007).
that is inconsistent with one’s beliefs is equiva- In OCD, sexual or violent intrusive images
lent to actually carrying out the thought (Shafran may be the most distressing or stigmatizing,
et al., 1996). Higher thought-action fusion was despite their frequent occurrence in nonclinical
found in adults with obsessive thinking than in populations (Cole & Warman, 2019). Regarding
adults with pathological worry (Coles et al., the distinction between OCD and GAD, studies
2001). It has also been suggested that thought-­ suggest that obsessions more frequently occur in
action fusion may play an etiological and main- the form of intrusive visual images than do wor-
taining role in additional disorders, such as GAD, ries (Gillet et al., 2018). Pathological worry typi-
3 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Considering Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Obsessive-Compulsive… 35

cally takes the form of verbal cognition rather increases with age, suggesting that children may
than visual images and has been described as a be better able to identify their physiological
“chain of thoughts” (Borkovec et al., 1983). In symptoms associated with anxiety as they mature
contrast, intrusive images are characterized as (Crawley et al., 2014; Ginsburg et al., 2006;
brief mental flashes that are shorter in duration Choudhury et al., 2003).
(Langlois et al., 2000a). Among youth with anxiety disorders, those
endorsing somatic symptoms have been found to
have more severe psychopathology compared to
Physical Symptoms those with anxiety disorders who do not endorse
somatic symptoms. This finding includes more
Somatic symptoms are commonly experienced severe anxiety, poorer global functioning, poorer
by youth with anxiety disorders and have been academic performance, and higher rates of school
associated with anxiety severity and impairment refusal (Ginsburg et al., 2006; Storch et al.,
(Ginsburg et al., 2006; Storch et al., 2008a, b). In 2008a, b; Last, 1991; Hughes et al., 2008).
a sample of anxiety-disordered youth, over 95% Although evidence of somatic complaints is
endorsed at least one somatic symptom (Crawley not required for a diagnosis of OCD, physical
et al., 2014). Children with anxiety frequently symptoms are common among youth with this
report headaches, stomachaches, muscle tension, disorder. The most frequently experienced physi-
sweating, drowsiness, and jittery feelings (e.g., cal symptoms include tension and restlessness
Crawley et al., 2014; Eisen & Engler, 1995; Last, (Storch et al., 2008a). Storch et al. (2008b) found
1991). that sleep-related difficulties were associated
The substantial overlap among the physical with anxiety severity in children with OCD and
symptoms associated with the various anxiety may be relatively common among these youth.
disorders in youth makes it difficult to determine Children with OCD and hoarding symptoms have
a diagnosis based on this factor alone. Studies been found to exhibit higher levels of somatic
examining the relationship between anxiety dis- complaints relative to non-hoarders with OCD,
order diagnosis type and somatic symptoms have suggesting that physical symptoms may vary
found mixed results. Crawley et al. (2014) found within OCD (Storch et al., 2007). The role of
that youth with GAD and SAD reported more physical symptoms in OCD is particularly
frequent and severe somatic symptoms than nuanced as somatic concerns also characterize
youth with social anxiety disorder, which may be the nature of some children’s obsessions and/or
due to diagnostic criteria of somatic symptoms compulsions (Ivarsson & Valderhaug, 2006).
DSM IV for GAD and SAD rather than social Thus, it may be difficult to differentiate between
anxiety. Hofflich et al. (2006) found that children a child’s actual experience of somatic complaints
with GAD, social anxiety, and separation anxiety and preoccupation with such concerns. Gathering
disorder did not differ in the frequency with detailed information regarding children’s true
which they reported somatic symptoms or with physical symptoms, beliefs about bodily sensa-
regards to the presence of any specific somatic tions, and mental/behavioral responses may aid
symptom. Interestingly, children with a principal clinicians in distinguishing OCD from other anx-
diagnosis of GAD reported a wider variety of iety disorders.
physical symptoms than those listed in DSM-5, Somatic symptoms appear to be a common
(i.e., shaky and jittery, having chest pain, feeling reaction to trauma in children (Bailey et al., 2005;
strange, weird or unreal, heart racing or skipping Escobar et al., 1992; Gobble et al., 2004) and
beats, and feeling sick to their stomach). Given warrant specific attention as they are associated
the lack of significant group differences across with negative social, emotional, and academic
anxiety disorders, these somatic complaints may outcomes (Campo et al., 1999). In particular,
not be specific to GAD. The number of somatic symptoms can include physical reactivity and
complaints reported by children with GAD symptoms that are similar to those experienced
36 N. Fleischer et al.

during the traumatic event. (APA, 2013). A study what is being avoided (e.g., an object, a situation,
of PTSD symptoms among children in the New an image), and the function of the avoidance,
Orleans area following Hurricane Katrina found when the avoidance takes place and what, if any,
headaches, nausea, and upset stomach to be the circumstances facilitate coping. Answers to these
most commonly reported somatic symptoms questions inform an accurate diagnosis and effec-
(Hensley & Varela, 2008). Consistent with the tive treatment and can help differentiate between
earlier discussion of anxiety sensitivity in PTSD, OCD, PTSD, and GAD.
children in the study with high anxiety sensitivity Research suggests that worry may act as the
and high trait anxiety may have had a higher risk avoidance mechanism in GAD. Worry has been
of developing PTSD and somatic symptoms fol- defined as an attempt at problem-solving to pre-
lowing exposure to the traumatic event. Knowing vent the occurrence of negative outcomes
whether children possess these characteristics (Borkovec et al., 1983). Children and adolescents
may help identify youth who are most likely to with GAD may specifically engage in cognitive
develop trauma reactions, which can in turn aid avoidance techniques by means of thought sup-
with diagnosis and intervention. pression, distraction, and thought substitution to
Garnering contextual information surround- decrease the intensity of worries (Hearn et al.,
ing the onset of physical symptoms is likely to be 2017), and parents may facilitate and maintain
more useful in determining a diagnosis than this avoidance through over-controlling their
solely assessing the presence of a particular child’s environment and decreasing response to
somatic complaint. If physical symptoms are the exposure situations (Aktar et al., 2017). Overall,
primary presenting problem, it is necessary to the evidence supports the “avoidance theory of
examine the context in which these symptoms worry” (Borkovec et al., 2004), suggesting that a
occur. When a child reports a physical symptom, primary function of worry in GAD is to enable
it can merit parental attention – more attention individuals to avoid negative outcomes, negative
than would be assigned if the child only felt emo- bodily feelings, as well as other even more dis-
tionally unsettled. Indeed, the functional impact tressing thoughts. Of interest, this notion has
of a child’s reporting physical complaints (stay applicability to the parents of anxious youth
home from school, receive care) may unwittingly (Tiwari et al., 2008).
buttress such reports. Individuals with OCD engage in a wide range
of compulsions that are believed to deactivate
and avoid threatening images, thoughts, or out-
Avoidance comes. According to Salkovskis (1985, 1989),
this behavior stems from a person’s inaccurate
Avoidance is central but not unique to GAD, belief that they have control over whether such
OCD, and PTSD. Avoidance is characteristic of outcomes will occur (recall thought-action
the anxiety disorders generally and may be pres- fusion). The implied responsibility that comes
ent in other forms of psychopathology. Avoidance with this way of thinking translates into a pattern
refers to making a response that the individual of behavioral neutralizing responses which reflect
thinks/believes is necessary to prevent a negative attempts to escape or avoid the feared outcome
condition, despite the response being unneces- (Salkovskis, 1996). These avoidance-focused
sary. Individuals with an anxiety disorder tend to efforts include compulsions that are consistent
avoid an event, outcome, or thought which is with the associated fear (i.e., repetitive hand
greatly feared and may engage in avoidance washing to avoid catching germs) as well as those
when the outcome of the feared situation or event that lack a rational connection (i.e., touching
is unknown or ambivalent (Palitz et al., 2019). objects in a symmetrical fashion to prevent harm
Individuals use avoidance to minimize or obviate from befalling a loved one). Additionally, inter-
the potential for a negative outcome. Clinicians nal avoidance may be present in the form of men-
should determine the details of the avoidance, tal rituals such as repetitively thinking about a
3 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Considering Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Obsessive-Compulsive… 37

word or phrase until the interfering discomfort toms warrants comprehensive assessment to rule
has been alleviated. Parents may facilitate youth’s out the presence of OCD.
maintenance of compulsions, aiding youth in rit-
ualistic behaviors to avoid feared outcomes (e.g.,
reducing demands to allow time for compulsions; Reliability and Validity of Diagnosis
Stewart et al., 2017) and through accommoda-
tions of the avoidance (Kagan et al., 2018). GAD typically presents first in school-aged chil-
Cognitive processes present in adults with OCD dren and is characterized by excessive worries
that have been linked to compulsions, such as that a child finds difficult to control. The course
appraisals of responsibility, have been found to of anxiety symptoms can follow many different
exist in children with OCD as well. However, patterns through development, with GAD and
many of these processes were not found to distin- social anxiety disorder being the most persistent
guish between youth with OCD and youth with (Voltas et al., 2017), and some anxiety symptoms
other anxiety disorders (Barrett & Healy, 2003). having links to later mood disorders (Cummings
Such findings provide further evidence of the et al., 2014). In a follow-up study of youth
considerable diagnostic overlap among anxiety-­ patients treated for an anxiety disorder through
related disorders. Although there are several either cognitive-behavioral therapy, a selective
overlapping features of anxiety disorders, serotonin reuptake inhibitor, their combination,
research does indicate that compulsions are typi- or placebo, 30% were chronically ill, and 48%
cally unique to OCD and flashback-related avoid- had relapsed (Ginsburg et al., 2018). Additionally,
ance is typically unique to PTSD (Goodwin, youth with GAD are frequently diagnosed with a
2015). second anxiety disorder, as well as other disor-
Youth with PTSD may engage in avoidance ders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disor-
with the express purpose of distancing them- der (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder
selves from stimuli associated with the trauma. (ODD), and depression (Kendall et al., 2001;
During a traumatic event, a strong association is Walkup et al., 2008).
formed with corresponding contextual cues When assessing for GAD in youth, clinicians
which then come to signal the presence of danger typically obtain information from adults such as
(APA, 2013; Runyon et al., 2013). This associa- parents and teachers and should pay attention to
tion leads youth to avoiding places, people, and behavior changes and physical complaints (e.g.,
activities that trigger painful memories. In addi- upset stomach, sleeping patterns, school avoid-
tion to being directed at external triggers, avoid- ance). When assessing among adolescents, clini-
ance in PTSD can also be directed at internal cians can rely more on the patient’s report
experiences. Youth with PTSD will often make a (Panganiban et al., 2019). Measures that are con-
concerted effort to avoid thoughts and feelings sidered validated screening tools for GAD (see
that might remind them of the immense distress also Creswell et al., 2020; Fleischer et al., 2020)
they previously experienced, including hyper- in youth include the Screen for Childhood
arousal symptoms (e.g., increased heart rate, etc.; Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED;
Runyon et al., 2013). Research demonstrates that Birmaher et al., 1999), the Multidimensional
avoidance in individuals with PTSD may further Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC; March et al.,
generalize to aversive situations not related to 1997; Villabø et al., 2012), and the Spence
their trauma experience (Sheynin et al., 2018). Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS; Spence, 1998).
Compulsive urges can strictly occur within the Approximately half of all OCD cases have
context of PTSD (i.e., compulsion of thinking their onset in childhood and adolescence
“good thoughts to cancel out images of a dying (Janowitz et al., 2009). OCD that begins in child-
friend”; De Silva & Marks, 2001, p. 173) and are hood, and goes untreated, frequently persists into
not necessarily indicative of a comorbid diagno- adulthood, with one meta-analysis finding that of
sis of OCD. However, the presence of such symp- children and adolescents diagnosed with OCD,
38 N. Fleischer et al.

60% had either full, or subthreshold OCD symp- degree of commonality that makes differential
toms persist (Stewart et al., 2004). Without ade- diagnosis a challenge. Consideration of this issue
quate treatment, the majority of patients begs the question of whether the disorders under
experience symptoms with a chronic and fluctu- examination truly reflect distinct entities, each
ating course. with their own unique etiology and underpin-
Comorbid diagnoses are especially common nings, or whether these disorders are better con-
in OCD in youth and include but are not limited ceptualized using a unified construct with varying
to depression, social anxiety, and substance use manifestations. Speaking to this question, symp-
(Douglass et al., 1995). High rates of comorbidi- toms of GAD, PTSD, and OCD are typically not
ties can make differential diagnoses particularly specific to one particular disorder. Treatment
challenging. To assess for OCD in youth, scales strategies that emerge from a cognitive-­behavioral
that are considered to have acceptable psycho- framework (Kendall, Suveg, & Kingery, 2006),
metrics include the Children’s Yale-Brown-­ though applied with some variations for the spe-
Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (CY-BOCS; cific disorders, are similar and consistent and
Goodman et al., 1991) and the Obsessive-­ have been found to be effective for several of the
Compulsive Inventory – Child Version (OCI-CV; emotional disorders in youth (see Ollendick &
Foa et al., 2010). King, 1998).
Epidemiologic studies have found high rates Additionally, although genetics are not the
of childhood and adolescent exposure to trau- focus of this review, there are data suggesting
matic events, but only about 16% of youth that the genes for anxiety disorders may be
exposed to a traumatic event develop PTSD shared, as opposed to distinct (Hudson & Rapee,
(Alisic et al., 2014). Approximately one-third of 2004; Levey et al., 2020), and that similar changes
youth with PTSD experience a chronic course in the brain occur in several anxiety disorders
that lasts several years (McLaughlin et al., 2013). (see Sinha, Mohlman, & Gorman, 2004). As
When assessing for PTSD in youth, one must advances in the field (e.g., genetics, neuropsy-
determine whether the symptoms follow and are chology, cognition, behavior, emotion) provide
due to exposure to a traumatic event, rather than findings, a reconceptualization of anxiety, or sev-
a different psychological disorder. Childhood eral types of anxiety, may be reconsidered as one
trauma exposure and PTSD often present with disorder. This potential reconceptualization has
frequent comorbidity, particularly anxiety disor- important implications for how anxiety disorders
ders, depression, externalizing problems, self-­ are studied and treated.
harm, and substance use disorders (McLaughlin Overall, anxiety, OCD, and PTSD may be bet-
et al., 2013). New to the DSM-5, among children ter viewed as dimensional, rather than categori-
under the age of 6, a lower threshold of symp- cal. Individuals with the highest levels of
toms is needed to diagnose PTSD. Measures with symptoms often move in and out of meeting cri-
acceptable psychometrics for assessing PTSD in teria for an anxiety disorders overtime (Caspi &
children and adolescents include the UCLA Moffitt, 2018). Further, it may be difficult to dis-
PTSD Reaction Index for Children and tinguish between developmentally appropriate
Adolescents (PTSD-RI; Steinberg et al., 2013) anxiety and pathological anxiety (Costello &
and the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for Angold, 1995; Pine, 1997), or anxiety that is jus-
Children and Adolescents (CAPS-CA; Nader tified by current environmental circumstances
et al., 1996). (covid19). Although one or two symptoms may
distinguish between persons with or without a
diagnosis according to DSM-5 classification, in
Implications actuality these individuals may nevertheless look
quite similar to their nondiagnosed counterparts.
The essential features of GAD, OCD, and PTSD, In a study of European adolescents, subthreshold
as currently defined by DSM-5, have substantial anxiety was associated with increased suicidality
3 Issues in Differential Diagnosis: Considering Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Obsessive-Compulsive… 39

and functional impairment (Balazs et al., 2013). intrusive images, physical symptoms, and avoid-
Since the inception of DSM, the number of child- ance. Further, mention was made of recom-
hood diagnoses has expanded quite rapidly and mended screeners. Differentiating between GAD,
additional diagnoses have been suggested (Silk OCD, and PTSD requires an informed, skillful,
et al., 2000). However, before the field incorpo- and nuanced approach.
rates new diagnostic categorizations, bigger and
overarching issues surrounding classification –
categorical or dimensional approaches (Drabick, References
2009; Maser et al., 2009) – merit consideration
and research evaluation (see also Jensen et al., Aktar, E., Nikolic, M., & Bögels, S. M. (2017).
Environmental transmission of generalized anxiety
2006). A refinement of our classification scheme, disorder from parents to children: Worries, expe-
reflecting the empirical research, will likely facil- riential avoidance, and intolerance of uncertainty.
itate better understanding and care for youth with Dialogues of Clinical Neuroscience, 19(2), 137–147.
anxiety and its disorders. Alisic, E., Zalta, A. K., Van Wesel, F., Larsen, S. E.,
Hafstad, G. S., Hassanpour, K., & Smid, G. E. (2014).
As the current classification system stands, Rates of post-traumatic stress disorder in trauma-­
diagnostic categorization can serve both to facili- exposed children and adolescents: Meta-analysis. The
tate and hinder accurate assessment and effective British Journal of Psychiatry, 204(5), 335–340.
treatment. That is, categorical distinctions help American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
organize constellations of symptoms and partici- Author.
pants into cohesive groups, and these distinctions Anholt, G. E., Aderka, I. M., Van Balkom, A. J. L. M.,
can provide a framework for identifying com- Smit, J. H., Schruers, K., Van Der Wee, N. J. A.,
monalities among individuals with similar prob- et al. (2014). Age of onset in obsessive–compulsive
disorder: Admixture analysis with a large sample.
lems. On the other hand, given the symptom Psychological Medicine, 44(1), 185–194.
overlap among diagnostic categories and the het- Bailey, B. N., Delaney-Black, V., Hannigan, J. H., Ager, J.,
erogeneity within diagnostic categories, disorder Sokol, R. J., & Covington, C. Y. (2005). Somatic com-
level distinctions may obscure fundamental char- plaints in children and community violence exposure.
Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics,
acteristics of the psychopathology present at the 26(5), 341–348.
symptom level. The present discussion of such Balázs, J., Miklósi, M., Keresztény, Á., Hoven,
issues as they relate to GAD, OCD, and PTSD C. W., Carli, V., Wasserman, C., et al. (2013).
emphasizes the importance of exploring the form, Adolescent subthreshold-­ depression and anxi-
ety: Psychopathology, functional impairment and
function, quality, and associated features of increased suicide risk. Journal of Child Psychology
symptoms within their context. and Psychiatry, 54(6), 670–677.
Barrett, P. M., & Healy, L. (2003). An examination of the
cognitive processes involved in childhood obsessive-­
compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and
Summary Therapy, 41, 285–299.
Barzilay, R., Patrick, A., Calkins, M. E., Moore, T. M.,
This chapter discussed the considerations for dif- Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. (2019). Association between
ferential diagnoses between GAD, OCD, and early-life trauma and obsessive-compulsive symptoms
in community youth. Depression & Anxiety, 36(7),
PTSD. With the new edition of the DSM-5 (APA, 586–595.
2013), these three diagnoses are no longer sub- Beesdo, K., Knappe, S., & Pine, D. S. (2009). Anxiety
sumed under the anxiety disorder umbrella. and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents:
Practitioners should consider the overlap in Developmental issues and implications for DSM-V.
Psychiatric Clinics, 32(3), 483–524.
symptoms and presentations when considering Beesdo, K., Pine, D. S., Lieb, R., & Wittchen, H. U.
the diagnosis of GAD, OCD, or PTSD in youth (2010). Incidence and risk patterns of anxiety and
presenting with anxiety symptoms. This chapter depressive disorders and categorization of general-
also discussed the differentiation between these ized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry,
67(1), 47–57.
three disorders within the specific domains of
fear and anxiety presentation, recurrent thoughts,
40 N. Fleischer et al.

Birmaher, B., Brent, D. A., Chiappetta, L., Bridge, J., role of thought–action fusion. Behaviour Research
Monga, S., & Baugher, M. (1999). Psychometric and Therapy, 39(8), 947–959.
properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Comer, J. S., & Kendall, P. C. (2004). A symptom-level
Emotional Disorders (SCARED): A replication examination of parent–child agreement in the diag-
study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & nosis of anxious youths. Journal of the American
Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(10), 1230–1236. Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(7),
Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & DePree, 878–886.
J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some Comer, J. S., & Kendall, P. C. (2007). Terrorism: The
characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and psychological impact on youth. Clinical Psychology:
Therapy, 21(1), 9–16. Science and Practice, 14(3), 179–212.
Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O., & Behar, E. W. E. L. Y. N. Comer, J. S., Kendall, P. C., Franklin, M. E., Hudson,
(2004). Avoidance theory of worry and general- J. L., & Pimentel, S. S. (2004). Obsessing/worry-
ized anxiety disorder. Generalized anxiety disorder: ing about the overlap between obsessive–compulsive
Advances in research and practice. disorder and generalized anxiety disorder in youth.
Bokszczanin, A. (2007). PTSD symptoms in children and Clinical Psychology Review, 24(6), 663–683.
adolescents 28 months after a flood: Age and gen- Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (1995). Epidemiology. In
der differences. Journal of Traumatic Stress: Official J. S. March (Ed.), Anxiety disorders in children and
Publication of The International Society for Traumatic adolescents (pp. 109–124). Guilford.
Stress Studies, 20(3), 347–351. Creswell, C., Waite, P., & Hudson, J. (2020). Practitioner
Bokszczanin, A. (2008). Parental support, family conflict, Review: Anxiety disorders in children and young
and overprotectiveness: Predicting PTSD symptom people–assessment and treatment. Journal of Child
levels of adolescents 28 months after a natural disaster. Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(6), 628–643.
Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 21(4), 325–335. Crawley, S. A., Caporino, N. E., Birmaher, B., Ginsburg,
Brakoulias, V., & Starcevic, V. (2011). The character- G., Piacentini, J., Albano, A. M., et al. (2014). Somatic
ization of beliefs in obsessive–compulsive disorder. complaints in anxious youth. Child Psychiatry &
Psychiatric Quarterly, 82(2), 151–161. Human Development, 45(4), 398–407.
Burstein, M., Beesdo-Baum, K., He, J. P., & Merikangas, Cummings, C., Caporino, N., & Kendall, P. C. (2014).
K. R. (2014). Threshold and subthreshold generalized Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in children and
anxiety disorder among US adolescents: Prevalence, adolescents: 20 years after. Psychological Bulletin,
sociodemographic, and clinical characteristics. 140, 816–845.
Psychological Medicine, 44(11), 2351–2362. De Los Reyes, A. (2011). Introduction to the special
Campo, J. V., Jansen-McWilliams, L., Comer, D. M., & section: More than measurement error: Discovering
Kelleher, K. J. (1999). Somatization in pediatric pri- meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical
mary care: Association with psychopathology, func- assessments of children and adolescents. Journal of
tional impairment, and use of services. Journal of the Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 1–9.
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant
38(9), 1093–1101. discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psycho-
Carleton, R. N. (2016). Into the unknown: A review and pathology: A critical review, theoretical framework,
synthesis of contemporary models involving uncer- and recommendations for further study. Psychological
tainty. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 39, 30–43. Bulletin, 131(4), 483.
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2018). All for one and one de Silva, P., & Marks, M. (1999). The role of traumatic
for all: Mental disorders in one dimension. American experiences in the genesis of obsessive–compulsive
Journal of Psychiatry, 175(9), 831–844. disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(10),
Chabane, N., Delorme, R., Millet, B., Mouren, M. C., 941–951.
Leboyer, M., & Pauls, D. (2005). Early-onset de Silva, P., & Marks, M. (2001). Traumatic experi-
obsessive-­compulsive disorder: A subgroup with a ences, post-traumatic stress disorder and obsessive-­
specific clinical and familial pattern? Journal of Child compulsive disorder. International Review of
Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(8), 881–887. Psychiatry, 13(3), 172–180.
Choudhury, M. S., Pimentel, S. S., & Kendall, P. C. Donovan, C. L., Holmes, M. C., & Farrell, L. J. (2016).
(2003). Childhood anxiety disorders: Parent–child Investigation of the cognitive variables associated with
(dis) agreement using a structured interview for the worry in children with generalised anxiety disorder
DSM-IV. Journal of the American Academy of Child and their parents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 192,
& Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(8), 957–964. 1–7.
Cole, J. L., & Warman, D. M. (2019). An examination Douglass, H. M., Moffitt, T. E., Dar, R., McGee, R. O.
of continuum beliefs versus biogenetic beliefs in B., & Silva, P. (1995). Obsessive-compulsive disorder
reducing stigma toward violent intrusive thoughts in in a birth cohort of 18-year-olds: Prevalence and pre-
OCD. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related dictors. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Disorders, 23, 100478. Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(11), 1424–1431.
Coles, M. E., Mennin, D. S., & Heimberg, R. G. (2001). Drabick, D. A. (2009). Can a developmental psychopa-
Distinguishing obsessive features and worries: The thology perspective facilitate a paradigm shift toward
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
feroce!’ (Look ferocious!). A fine presence, a pompous style of
speech, and a theatrical attitude are everything with the French. So
long as it sounds right and looks well the substance is a matter of
indifference. It reminds me of a citizen of Potsdam who once told me
he had been deeply impressed by a speech of Radowitz’s. I asked
him to show me the passage that had particularly stirred his feelings.
He could not mention one. I then took the speech itself and read it
through to him in order to discover its beauties, but it turned out that
there was nothing in it either pathetic or sublime. As a matter of fact
it was merely the air and attitude of Radowitz, who looked as if he
were speaking of something most profound and significant and
thrillingly impressive,—the thoughtful mien, the contemplative eye,
and the sonorous and weighty voice. It was much the same with
Waldeck, although he was not nearly such a clever man nor so
distinguished looking. In his case it was more the white beard and
the staunch convictions. The gift of eloquence has greatly spoilt
Parliamentary life. A great deal of time is consumed as every one
who thinks he has anything in him wants to speak, even when he
has nothing new to say. There are far too many speeches that simply
float in the air and pass out through the windows, and too few that go
straight to the point. The parties have already settled everything
beforehand, and the set speeches are merely intended for the public,
to show what members can do, and more especially for the
newspapers that are expected to praise them. It will come to this in
the end, that eloquence will be regarded as dangerous to the public
welfare, and that people will be punished for making long speeches.
We have one body,” he continued, “that is not in the least eloquent,
and has nevertheless done more for the German cause than any
other, that is the Federal Council. I remember, indeed, that at first
some attempts were made in that direction. I cut them short,
however, though as a matter of fact I had no right to do so, albeit I
was President. I addressed them much as follows: ‘Gentlemen,
eloquence and speeches intended to affect people’s convictions are
of no use here, as every one brings his own convictions with him in
his pocket—that is to say, his instructions. It is merely waste of time.
I think we had better restrict ourselves to statements of fact.’ And so
we did. No one made a big speech after that, business was speedily
transacted, and the Federal Council has really done a great deal of
good.”
Friday, February 3rd.—In addition to a violently warlike
proclamation, Gambetta has issued a decree declaring a number of
persons ineligible for the new Representative Assembly. “Justice
demands that all those who have been accessory to the acts of the
Government which began with the outrage of the 2nd of December,
and ended with the capitulation of Sedan, should now be reduced to
the same political impotence as the dynasty whose accomplices and
tools they were. That is a necessary consequence of the
responsibility which they assumed in carrying out the Emperor’s
measures. These include all persons who have occupied the
positions of Minister, Senator, Councillor of State, or Prefect from the
2nd of December, 1851, to the 4th of September, 1870. Furthermore,
all persons who, in the elections to the legislative bodies during the
period from the 2nd of December, 1851, to the 4th of September,
1870, have been put forward in any way as Government candidates,
as well as the members of those families that have reigned in France
since 1789, are ineligible for election.”
The Chief instructs me to telegraph to London and Cologne with
respect to this decree, that the Government at Bordeaux has
declared whole classes of the population—Ministers, Senators,
Councillors of State, and all who have formerly been official
candidates—as ineligible for election. The apprehension expressed
by Count Bismarck during the negotiations for the Convention of the
28th of January, that freedom of suffrage could not be secured, has
thus been confirmed. In consequence of that apprehension the
Chancellor of the Confederation at that time proposed the
convocation of the Corps Législatif, but Favre would not agree to it.
The Chancellor has now protested in a Note against the exclusion of
these classes. Only an Assembly that has been freely elected, as
provided by the Convention, will be recognised by Germany as
representing France.
Count Herbert Bismarck arrived this evening from Germany.
Saturday, February 4th.—The Chief has protested against
Gambetta’s decree in a telegram to Gambetta himself and in a note
to Favre. The telegram runs: “In the name of the freedom
guaranteed by the Armistice Convention, I protest against the decree
issued in your name which robs numerous classes of French citizens
of the right to be elected to the Assembly. The rights guaranteed by
that Convention to the freely elected representatives of the country
cannot be acquired through elections conducted under an
oppressive and arbitrary rule.” The despatch to Favre after giving an
epitome of Gambetta’s decree, goes on to say: “I have the honour to
ask your Excellency if you consider this to be in harmony with the
stipulation of the Convention that the Assembly is to be freely
elected? Allow me to recall to your Excellency’s memory the
negotiations which preceded the arrangement of the 28th of January.
Already at that time I expressed the apprehension that in presence
of the conditions then prevailing it would be difficult to secure the
entire freedom of the elections, and to prevent attempts being made
to restrict it. In consequence of that apprehension, the justice of
which M. Gambetta’s circular of to-day seems to confirm, I raised the
question whether it would not be better to convoke the Corps
Législatif, which would constitute a legal authority returned by
universal suffrage. Your Excellency declined to adopt that suggestion
and expressly promised that no pressure should be exercised upon
the electors, and that perfect freedom of voting should be secured. I
appeal to your Excellency’s sense of rectitude in requesting you to
say whether the exclusion of whole categories laid down as a matter
of principle in the decree in question is in harmony with the freedom
of election guaranteed in the Convention of the 28th of January? I
believe I may confidently express the hope that the decree in
question, the application of which would appear to be an infraction of
the stipulations of that Convention, will be immediately withdrawn
and that the Government of National Defence will take the necessary
measures to ensure the freedom of election guaranteed by Article II.
We could not grant to persons elected in pursuance of the Bordeaux
decree the rights secured by the Armistice to the members of the
Assembly.”
After 10 o’clock I was called to the Chief, who said: “They
complain in Berlin that the English papers are much better informed
than ours, and that we have communicated so little to our journals
respecting the negotiations for the armistice. How has that come
about?” I replied: “The fact is, Excellency, that the English have more
money and go everywhere to get information. Besides, they stand
well with certain august personages who know everything, and finally
the military authorities are not always very reserved with regard to
matters that ought, for the time being, to be kept secret. I, of course,
can only make public what it is proper that the public should know.”
“Well, then,” he said, “just write and explain how it is that the
extraordinary state of affairs here is to blame, and not we.”
I then took the opportunity of congratulating him on the freedom
of the city of Leipzig, which has been conferred upon him within the
last few days, and I added that it was a good city, the best in Saxony,
and one for which I had always had a great regard. “Yes,” he replied.
“Now I am a Saxon, too, and a Hamburger, for they have also
presented me with the freedom of Hamburg. One would hardly have
expected that from them in 1866.”
As I was leaving he said: “That reminds me—it is also one of the
wonders of our time—please write an article showing up the
extraordinary action of Gambetta, who after posing so long as the
champion of liberty and denouncing the Government for influencing
the elections, is now laying violent hands on the freedom of suffrage.
He wants to disqualify all those who differ from him, i.e., the whole
official world of France with the exception of thirteen Republicans. It
is certainly very odd that I should have to defend such a principle
against Gambetta and his associate and ally Garibaldi.” I said: “I do
not know whether it was intended, but in your despatch to Gambetta
the contrast is very striking where you protest, au nom de la liberté
des élections against les dispositions en votre nom pour priver des
catégories nombreuses du droit d’être élues.” “Yes,” he replied, “you
might also mention that Thiers, after his negotiations with me,
described me as an amiable barbarian—un barbare aimable. Now
they call me in Paris a crafty barbarian—un barbare astutieux, and
perhaps to-morrow I shall be un barbare constitutionnel.”
The Chief had more time and interest for the newspapers this
morning than during the past few days. I was called to him six times
before midday. On one occasion he handed me a lying French
pamphlet, “La Guerre comme la font les Prussiens,” and observed:
“Please write to Berlin that they should put together something of this
description from our point of view, quoting all the cruelties,
barbarities, and breaches of the Geneva Convention committed by
the French. Not too much however, or no one will read it, and it must
be done speedily.” Later on the Minister handed me a small journal
published by a certain Armand le Chevalier at 61 Rue Richelieu, with
a woodcut of the Chancellor of the Confederation as frontispiece.
The Chief said: “Look at this. Here is a man who refers to the
attempt by Blind, and recommends that I should be murdered, and at
the same time gives my portrait—like the photographs carried by the
franctireurs. You know that in the forests of the Ardennes the
portraits of our rangers were found in the pockets of the franctireurs
who were to shoot them. Luckily it cannot be said that this is a
particularly good likeness of me—and the biography is no better.”
Then reading over a passage and handing me the paper, he said:
“This portion should be made use of in the press, and afterwards be
introduced in the pamphlet.”
Finally he gave me some more French newspapers saying: “Look
through these and see if there is anything in them for me or for the
King. I must manage to get away or I shall be caught by our Paris
friends again.”
Prince Putbus and Count Lehndorff joined us at dinner. The Chief
related how he had called Favre’s attention to the singular
circumstance that he, Count von Bismarck, who had been
denounced as a tyrant and a despot, had to protest in the name of
liberty against Gambetta’s proclamation. Favre agreed, with a “Oui,
c’est bien drôle.” The restriction on the freedom of election decreed
by Gambetta has, however, now been withdrawn by the Paris
section of the French Government. “He announced that to me this
morning in writing, and he had previously given me a verbal
assurance.”
It was then mentioned that several German newspapers were
dissatisfied with the capitulation, as they expected our troops to
march into Paris at once. “That comes,” said the Chief, “of a
complete misapprehension of the situation here and in Paris. I could
have managed Favre, but the population! They have strong
barricades and 300,000 men of whom certainly 100,000 would have
fought. Blood enough has been shed in this war—enough German
blood. Had we appealed to force much more would have been spilt
—in the excited condition of the people. And merely to inflict one
additional humiliation upon them—that would have been too dearly
bought.” After reflecting for a moment, he continued: “And who told
them that we shall not still enter Paris and occupy a portion of it? Or
at least march through, when they have cooled down and come to
reason. The armistice will probably be prolonged, and then, in return
for our readiness to make concessions, we can demand the
occupation of the city on the right bank of the river. I think we shall
be there in about three weeks.” “The 24th”—he reflected for a
moment—“yes, it was on the 24th that the Constitution of the North
German Confederation was made public. It was also on the 24th of
February, 1859, that we had to submit to certain particularly mean
treatment. I told them that it would have to be expiated. Exoriare
aliquis. I am only sorry that the Würtemberg Minister to the
Bundestag, old Reinhard, has not lived to see it. Prokesch has
though, and I am glad of that, because he was the worst. According
to a despatch from Constantinople, which I read this morning,
Prokesch is now quite in agreement with us, praises the energies
and intelligence of Prussia’s policy, and (here the Minister smiled
scornfully) has always, or at least for a long time past, recommended
co-operation with us.”
The Chief had been to Mont Valérien to-day. “I was never there
before,” he said, “and when one sees the strong works and the
numerous contrivances for defence—we should have terrible losses
in storming it. One dares not even think of it.”
The Minister said one of the objects of Favre’s visit to-day was to
request that the masses of country people who had fled to Paris in
September should be allowed to leave. They were mostly inhabitants
of the environs and there must be nearly 300,000 of them, “I
declined permission,” he continued, “explaining to him that our
soldiers now occupied their houses. If the owners came out and saw
how their property had been wrecked and ruined they would be
furious, and no blame to them, and they would upbraid our people
and then there might be dangerous brawls and perhaps something
still worse.” The Chancellor had also been to St. Cloud, and whilst he
was looking at the burnt palace and recalling to mind the condition of
the room in which he had dined with Napoleon, there was a well-
dressed Frenchman there—probably from Paris—who was being
shown round by a man in a blouse. “I could catch every word they
said, as they spoke aloud, and I have sharp ears. ‘C’est l’œuvre de
Bismarck,’ said the man in the blouse, but the other merely replied
‘C’est la guerre.’ If they had only known that I was listening to them!”
Count Bismarck-Bohlen mentioned that the Landwehr,
somewhere in this neighbourhood, gave a refractory Frenchman,
who tried to stab an officer with a penknife, seventy-five blows with
the flat of the sword. “Seventy-five!” said the Chief. “H’m, that, after
all, is somewhat too much.” Somebody related a similar instance that
had occurred in the neighbourhood of Meaux. As Count Herbert was
passing recently, a miller, who had abused Count Bismarck and said
he wished he had him between two millstones, was laid flat by the
soldiers and so fearfully beaten that he was not able to stir for a
couple of hours.
The election addresses posted on the walls by the candidates for
the National Assembly were then discussed, and it was observed
that, in general, they were still very aggressive, and promised to
achieve wonders at Bordeaux. “Yes,” said the Chief; “I quite believe
that. Favre also tried once or twice to ride the high horse. But it did
not last long. I always brought him down with a jesting remark.”
Some one referred to the speech made by Klaczko on the 30th of
January in the Delegation of the Reichsrath against Austria’s co-
operation with Prussia, and to Giskra’s revelation in the morning
edition of the National Zeitung of the 2nd of February. Giskra said
that Bismarck wished to send him from Brünn to Vienna with
proposals for peace. These were, in effect: Apart from the
maintenance in Venetia of the status quo before the war, the Main
line was to be recognised as the limit of Prussian ascendancy, there
was to be no war indemnity, but French mediation was to be
excluded. Giskra sent Baron Herring to Vienna with these proposals.
The latter was, however, coolly received by Moritz Esterhazy, and
after waiting for sixteen hours obtained only an evasive answer. On
proceeding to Nikolsburg, Herring found Benedetti already there, and
was told: “You come too late.” As Giskra points out, the French
mediation accordingly cost Austria a war indemnity of thirty millions.
It was observed that Prussia could have extorted more from Austria
at that time, and also a cession of territory, for instance, Austrian
Silesia, and perhaps Bohemia. The Chief replied: “Possibly, as for
money, what more could the poor devils give? Bohemia would have
been something and there were people who entertained the thought.
But we should have created difficulties for ourselves in that way, and
Austrian Silesia was not of much value to us; for just there the
devotion to the Imperial house and the Austrian connection was
greater than elsewhere. In such cases one must ask for what one
really wants and not what one might be able to get.”
In this connection he related that on one occasion, as he was
walking about in mufti at Nikolsburg, he met two policemen who
wished to arrest a man. “I asked what he had done, but of course as
a civilian I got no answer. I then inquired of the man himself, who told
me that it was because he had spoken disrespectfully of Count
Bismarck. They nearly took me along with him because I said that
doubtless many others had done the same.”
“That reminds me that I was once obliged to join in a cheer for
myself. It was in 1866, in the evening, after the entry of the troops. I
was unwell just then, and my wife did not wish to let me go out. I
went, however—on the sly—and as I was about to cross the street
again near the palace of Prince Charles, there was a great crowd of
people collected there, who desired to give me an ovation. I was in
plain clothes, and with my broad brimmed hat pulled down over my
eyes, I perhaps looked like a suspicious character—I don’t know
why. As some of them seemed inclined to be unpleasant, I thought
the best thing to do was to join in their hurrah.”
From 8 p.m. on read drafts and despatches, including Favre’s
answer to the Chief in the matter of Gambetta’s electioneering
manœuvre. It runs as follows:—
“You are right in appealing to my sense of rectitude. You shall
never find it fail me in my dealings with you. It is perfectly true that
your Excellency strongly urged upon me as the sole way out of the
difficulty to convoke the former legislative bodies. I declined to adopt
that course for various reasons which it is needless to recall, but
which you will doubtless not have forgotten. In reply to your
Excellency’s objections, I said I was convinced that my country only
desired the free exercise of the suffrage, and that its sole resource
lay in the popular sovereignty. That will make it clear to you that I
cannot agree to the restrictions that have been imposed upon the
franchise. I have not opposed the system of official candidatures in
order to revive it now for the benefit of the present Government. Your
Excellency may therefore rest assured that if the decree mentioned
in your letter to me has been issued by the Delegation at Bordeaux,
it will be withdrawn by the Government of National Defence. For this
purpose I only require to obtain official evidence of the existence of
the decree in question. This will be done by means of a telegram to
be despatched to-day. There are, therefore, no differences of opinion
between us, and we must both continue to co-operate in resolutely
carrying into execution the Convention which we have signed.”
Called to the Chief at 9 p.m. He wants to have an article written
pointing out that the entry of our troops into Paris is at present
impracticable, but may be possible later on. This is in answer in the
National Zeitung to an article criticising the terms of armistice.
With regard to an article in the Cologne Volkszeitung showing
that the Ultramontanes have offered a subsidy to the leaders of the
General Association of German Workers on condition that they
promote the election of clerical candidates, the Minister says: “Look
here. Please see that the newspapers speak of a ‘Savigny-Bebel
party’ whenever an opportunity occurs, and that must be repeated.”
And just as I am going out of the room he calls after me: “Or the
‘Liebknecht-Savigny party.’” We take note of that, and shall speak
from time to time of this new party.
Sunday, February 5th.—We are joined at dinner by Favre,
d’Hérisson, and the Director of the Western Railway, a man with a
broad, comfortable, smiling face, apparently about thirty-six years of
age. Favre, who sits next to the Chief, looks anxious, worried and
depressed. His head hangs on one side, and sometimes for a
change sinks on to his breast, his underlip following suit. When he is
not eating, he lays his two hands on the table-cloth, one on top of the
other, in submission to the decrees of fate, or he crosses his arms in
the style of Napoleon the First, a sign that, on closer consideration,
he still feels confident in himself. During dinner the Chief speaks only
French, and mostly in a low voice, and I am too tired to follow the
conversation.
The Chief instructs me to send the following short paragraph to
one of our newspapers: The Kölnische Zeitung has made itself the
organ, it is true with some reservations, of those who complain of the
alleged destruction of French forests by our officials. One would
think it could have found some other occupation than to scrutinise
our administration of the public forests of France. We act in
accordance with the principles of forestry, even if we do not follow
the French system. Moreover, we should be within our rights if we
exploited these resources of the enemy in the most ruthless manner,
as that would render the French more disposed to conclude peace.
He also warmly praised the active part taken by the Duke of
Meiningen in the conduct of the war. He concluded: “I wish that to be
mentioned in the press. The background is ready to hand in the
princely loafing and palace looting of the rest of them.”
Monday, February 6th.—The Chief desires to have an article
against Gambetta published in the Moniteur. I write the following:—
“The Convention of the 28th of January, concluded between
Count von Bismarck and M. Jules Favre, has revived the hopes of all
sincere friends of peace. Since the events of the 4th of September
the military honour of Germany has received sufficient satisfaction,
so that it may now yield to the desire to enter into negotiations with a
Government which truly represents the French nation for a peace
that will guarantee the fruits of victory and secure our future. When
the Governments represented at Versailles and Paris finally
succeeded in coming to an understanding, of which the conditions
were prescribed by the force of circumstances, and France was
restored to herself, they were justified in expecting that these
preliminaries of a new era in the relations of the two countries would
be generally respected. The decree issued by M. Gambetta
disqualifying all former functionaries and dignitaries, senators, and
official candidates from election to the National Assembly was
perhaps necessary to show France the abyss towards which it has
been gravitating since the dictatorship, sacrificing the best blood of
the country, refused to convoke the representatives of the nation in
the regular way.
“The second article of the Convention of the 28th of January
shows clearly and plainly that the freedom of the elections is one of
the conditions of the Convention itself. In entering into such an
arrangement for the elections, Germany only took into consideration
the existing French laws, and not the good will and pleasure of this
or that popular Tribune. It would be just as easy to call together a
Rump Parliament in Bordeaux, and make it a tool for the subjection
of the other half of France. We are convinced that all honourable and
sincere French patriots will protest against the action of the
Delegation at Bordeaux, which is entirely arbitrary and opposed to all
sound reason. If there were any prospect that this action would be
allowed to unite all the anarchical parties who tolerate the
dictatorship in so far as it represents their favourite ideas, the most
serious complications would inevitably ensue.
“Germany does not intend to interfere in any way in the domestic
affairs of France. She has, however, through the agreement of the
28th of January, secured the right to see that a public authority is
established which will possess the attributes necessary to enable it
to negotiate peace in the name of France. If Germany is denied the
right to negotiate for peace with the whole nation, if an attempt is
made to substitute the representatives of a faction for the
representatives of the nation, the armistice convention would thereby
become null and void. We readily acknowledge that the Government
of National Defence has immediately recognised the justice of the
complaints made by Count von Bismarck in his despatch of the 3rd
of February. That Government has addressed itself to the French
nation in language marked by nobility and elevation of feeling,
setting forth the difficulties of the situation and the efforts made to
relieve the country from the last consequences of an unfortunate
campaign. At the same time, it has cancelled the decree of the
Delegation at Bordeaux. Let us hope, therefore, that the action of M.
Gambetta will receive no support in the country, and that it will be
possible to conduct the elections in perfect harmony with the spirit
and letter of the Convention of the 28th of January.”
I am called to the Minister again at 11 o’clock, and instructed to
defend Favre against the rabid attacks of some French newspapers.
The Chief says: “They actually take him to task for having dined with
me. I had much trouble in getting him to do so. But it is unfair to
expect that, after working with me for eight or ten hours, he should
either starve as a staunch Republican, or go out to a hotel where the
people would run after him and stare at him.”
The Frenchmen are again here between 2 and 4 p.m. They are
six or seven in number, including Favre and, if I rightly heard the
name, General Leflô. The Chief’s eldest son and Count Dönhoff join
us at dinner.
Subsequently I despatch a démenti of a Berlin telegram
published by The Times, according to which we propose to demand
the surrender of twenty ironclads and the colony of Pondicherry,
together with a war indemnity of ten milliards of francs. This I
describe as a gross invention which cannot possibly have been
credited in England, or have created any anxiety there. I then hint at
the probable source, namely, the clumsy imagination of an unfriendly
and intriguing diplomatist. “That comes from Loftus,” says the Chief,
as he gives me these instructions. “An ill-mannered fellow who was
always seeking to make mischief with us.”
Tuesday, February 7th.—From Bucarest despatches it seems as
if the reign of Prince Charles were really coming to a speedy end.
With the retention of Dalwigk at Darmstadt, the old confederacy of
opponents of German unity remains firmly entrenched, and the well-
known intrigues continue unhindered. A telegram from Bordeaux
brings the expected news. Gambetta yesterday announced in a
circular to the Prefects that his Parisian colleagues having annulled
his decree with regard to the elections, he has informed them of his
resignation. A good sign. He can hardly have a strong party behind
him or he would scarcely have resigned.
Wednesday, February 8th.—The Chief is up at an unusually early
hour, and drives off at 9.45 to see the King. Favre arrives shortly
before 1 o’clock, accompanied by a swarm of Frenchmen. There
must be ten or twelve of them. He confers with the Minister after first
lunching with us.
In the evening the Chief and his son dined with the Crown Prince,
but first remained for a while with us. He again observed with
satisfaction that Favre had not taken offence at his “spiteful letter,”
but, on the contrary, had thanked him for it. The Chief had repeated
to him verbally that it was his duty to share the dish which he had
helped to cook. To-day they had discussed the way of raising the
Paris war contribution; the French wanted to pay the greater part of it
in bank notes, and we might lose in that way. “I do not know the
value of what they offer,” he said; “but in any case it is to their
advantage. They must, however, pay the whole amount agreed
upon. I will not remit a single franc.”
Thursday, February 9th.—Speaking again of the Paris
contribution, the Chancellor observed at dinner: “Stosch tells me he
can dispose of fifty million francs in bank notes to pay for provisions,
&c., in France. We must have proper security, however, for the
remaining hundred and fifty millions.” Then alluding to the foolish
story about our wanting Pondicherry, he continued: “I do not want
any colonies at all. Their only use is to provide sinecures. That is all
England at present gets out of her colonies, and Spain too. And as
for us Germans, colonies would be exactly like the silks and sables
of the Polish nobleman who had no shirt to wear under them.”
CHAPTER XIX
FROM GAMBETTA’S RESIGNATION TO THE CONCLUSION OF THE
PRELIMINARIES OF PEACE

Friday, February 10th. Fresh complaints respecting the intrigues of


Dalwigk, and especially the measures for depriving the national
constituencies in Hesse of their representatives and securing the
victory of the Ultramontane and Democratic coalition. The Chief
desires me to see that an “immediate and energetic campaign in the
press” is organised against these and other mischievous
proceedings inspired by Beust’s friends. He also wishes the Moniteur
to reprint the long list of French officers who have broken their parole
and escaped from Germany.
We were joined at dinner by the Duke of Ratibor and a Herr von
Kotze, the husband of the Chief’s niece. Strousberg, a business
friend of the Duke’s, was mentioned, and the Chief observed that
nearly all, or at least very many of the members of the Provisional
Government were Jews: Simon, Cremieux, Magnin, also Picard,
whose Semitic origin he would hardly have suspected, and “very
probably Gambetta also, from his features.” “For the same reason, I
suspect even Favre,” he added.
Saturday, February 11th.—In the morning I read the newspapers,
and particularly certain debates in the English Parliament at the end
of last month. It really looks as if our good friends across the
Channel had a suspicious leaning towards France, and as if they
were not at all disinclined to interfere once more—indeed, in certain
circumstances, an Anglo-French alliance would appear quite
possible. It is a question, however, whether they might not fall
between two stools. A very different result might well ensue. From
what one hears and reads in the newspapers, the feeling in this
country is almost as hostile to the English as to ourselves, and in
certain circles more so. It may well happen that if England adopts a
threatening attitude towards us, we may surprise our cousins in
London with the very reverse of a Franco-English alliance against
Germany. We may even be obliged to seriously consider the forcible
restoration of Napoleon, which we have not hitherto contemplated.
According to a telegram of the 2nd inst., Bernstorff is to see that
these ideas are cautiously ventilated in the press.
Count Henckel and Bleichröder dined with us. It seems that in the
negotiations with the French financiers, Scheidtmann described
them to their faces in language more vigorous than flattering, talking
of them as pigs, dogs, rabble, &c., in ignorance of the fact that some
of them understood German. The Chief then spoke of the insolence
of the Parisian press, which behaved as if the city were not in our
power: “If that goes on we must tell them that we will no longer stand
it. It must cease, or we shall answer their articles by a few shells
from the forts.” Henckel having alluded to the unsatisfactory state of
public opinion in Alsace, the Chief said that, properly speaking, no
elections ought to have been allowed there at all, and he had not
intended to allow them. But inadvertently the same instructions were
sent to the German officials there as elsewhere. The melancholy
situation of the Prince of Rumania was then referred to, and from the
Rumanian Radicals the conversation turned to Rumanian stocks.
Bleichröder said that financiers always speculated on the ignorance
of the masses, and upon their blind cupidity. This was confirmed by
Henckel, who said: “I had a quantity of Rumanian securities, but after
I had made about 8 per cent. I got rid of them, as I knew they could
not yield 15 per cent, and that alone could have saved them.” The
Chief then related that the French were committing all sorts of fraud
in the revictualling of Paris. It was not out of pride that they refused
our contributions, but merely because they could make no profit out
of them. Even members of the Government were involved, and
Magnin was understood to have recently made 700,000 francs on
the purchase of sheep. “We must let them see that we know that,”
said the Chief, glancing at me; “it will be useful in the peace
negotiations.” This was done without delay.
After dinner I wrote some paragraphs on the instructions of the
Chief. The first was to the effect that we ought no longer to tolerate
the insolence of the Parisian journalists. However generous and
patient we might be, it was past endurance that the French press
should venture to deride and insult to his face the victor who stood
before the walls of the capital which he had absolutely in his power.
Moreover, such mendacity and violence would prove an obstacle to
the conclusion of peace, by producing bitterness on both sides and
delaying the advent of a calmer spirit. This could not be foreseen
when the armistice Convention was concluded, and in discussing
any prolongation of the truce, effective means would have to be
found for preventing further provocation of the kind. Undoubtedly the
best way would be the occupation of the city itself by our troops. We
should thus relieve the French Government of a source of grave
anxiety, and avert the evil consequences of inflammatory articles in
the press, which they are perhaps not in a position to repress.
Sunday, February 12th.—It is announced in a telegram from
Cassel that Napoleon has issued a proclamation to the French. The
Minister handed it to me, saying: “Please have this published in our
local paper. It is in order to lead them astray, so that they may not
know where they stand. But for God’s sake don’t date it from
Wilhelmshöhe, or they will think that we are in communication with
him. ‘Le bureau Wolff télégraphie.’” The Chief seems to be unwell.
He does not come to dinner.
Wednesday, February 15th.—I again draw attention in the
Moniteur to the disgraceful tone of the Parisian press. I intimate that
this agitation is delaying the conclusion of peace, and that the most
certain way of putting an end to it would be the occupation of Paris.
Wednesday, February 22nd.—During the last week I have written
a number of articles and paragraphs, and despatched about a dozen
telegrams.
The Assembly at Bordeaux shows a proper appreciation of the
position. It has declined to support Gambetta, and has elected Thiers
as chief of the Executive and spokesman on behalf of France in the
negotiations for peace which began here yesterday. At dinner
yesterday, at which we were joined by Henckel, the Chief remarked,
with reference to these negotiations, “If they were to give us another
milliard we might perhaps leave them Metz, and build a fortress a
few miles further back, in the neighbourhood of Falkenberg or
towards Saarbrücken—there must be some suitable position there. I
do not want so many Frenchmen in our house. It is the same with
Belfort, which is entirely French. But the soldiers will not hear of
giving up Metz, and perhaps they are right.”
Generals von Kameke and von Treskow dined with us to-day.
The Chief spoke about his second meeting with Thiers to-day: “On
my making that demand” (what the demand was escaped me), “he
jumped up, although he is otherwise quite capable of controlling
himself, and said, ‘Mais c’est une indignité!’ I did not allow that to put
me out, however, but began to speak to him in German. He listened
for a while, and evidently did not know what to make of it. He then
said in a querulous voice, ‘Mais, Monsieur le Comte, vows savez
bien que je ne sais point l’allemand.’ I replied, speaking in French
again, ‘When you spoke just now of indignité I found that I did not
know enough French, and so preferred to use German, in which I
understand what I say and hear.’ He immediately caught my
meaning, and wrote down as a concession the demand which he
had previously resented as an indignité.”

You might also like