Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Blackwell Publishing AsiaMelbourne, AustraliaPCNPsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences1323-13162006 Folia Publishing Society2006604422428Original ArticleEffect of age and education on TMTR.

Hashimoto et al.

Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences (2006), 60, 422–428 doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x

Regular Article
Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and
determination of normative data for Japanese elderly
people: The Tajiri Project
RYUSAKU HASHIMOTO, phd,1,2 KENICHI MEGURO, md, phd,2,3 EUNJOO LEE, msc,1,2
MARI KASAI, phd,1 HIROSHI ISHII, md, phd1,4 AND SATOSHI YAMAGUCHI, md, phd2
Departments of 1Behavioral Neurology and Cognitive Neuroscience, and 3Geriatric Behavioral Neurology,
Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, Sendai, 2Tajiri SKIP Center, Tajiri and 4Kawasaki-Kokoro
Hospital, Kawasaki, Japan

Abstract The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two-part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial
ability (TMT-A) and executive function (TMT-B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have
not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to inves-
tigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical demen-
tia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years,
75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years
and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT-A and TMT-B were measured, and the difference in
score between TMT-A and TMT-B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indi-
ces of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in
the ≥85-years group. For TMT-A, there was a significant difference between the 6-years and 8-years
groups, and between the 6-years and ≥10-years groups, and for TMT-B, there was a significant dif-
ference between the 6-years and ≥10-years groups, and between the 8-years and ≥10-years groups.
The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85-years group, but the educational level did not
significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT-A
and TMT-B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT-A, an educa-
tional effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this
part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education
(>6 years). The time to complete TMT-B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous
reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT-A [(B-A) or (B/A)], the educational
effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive func-
tion was unclear in normal elderly subjects.

Key words aging effect, educational effect, normal aging, Trail Making Test.

INTRODUCTION general mental disorder,1,2 although recent studies


have suggested that subjects with MCI have mild
The concept of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is of
impairment in executive function3,4 and visuospatial
importance in normal aging and dementia.1 Subjects
ability.5,6 Because mixing of MCI subjects with healthy
with MCI have some memory disorder, but no marked
elderly people may result in underestimation of the
ability of the healthy elderly, distinguishing healthy
elderly people from subjects with MCI is particularly
Correspondence address: Kenichi Meguro, MD, PhD, Department
important in determining normative data for cognitive
of Geriatric Behavioral Neurology, Tohoku University Graduate
School of Medicine, 2-1, Seiryo-machi, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8575,
tests.7
Japan. Email: k-meg@umin.ac.jp The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a frequently used
Received 7 June 2005; revised 17 January 2006; accepted 22 screening test for brain damage or dementia. The TMT
January 2006. consists of parts A and B (TMT-A and TMT-B). The

© 2006 The Authors


Journal compilation © 2006 Folia Publishing Society
Effect of age and education on TMT 423

first, TMT-A, requires an individual to connect ran- Based on data from this project, we published the
domly located numbers in numerical order as rapidly results of a survey on the prevalence of dementia in
as possible, whereas TMT-B contains both numbers 1998.22 Five years later, we provided an analysis of 284
and letters, and the subject is required to connect the subjects who were rated as healthy or as having ques-
numbers and letters alternately (e.g. 1-A-2-B-3-C). The tionable dementia using the Clinical Dementia Rating
score on each part of the TMT is the amount of time (CDR) scale (CDR 0 and CDR 0.5, respectively).23,24
required to complete the task. The TMT-A score In the current study, neuropsychological tests were
reflects visual search ability and motor skills, whereas conducted on these 284 subjects and their CDR was re-
the TMT-B score additionally reflects the ability for assessed. A clinical team of medical doctors and public
cognitive alternation.8–10 Thus, the time for completion health nurses, independent of those performing the
of TMT-B is usually used as an index of executive neuropsychological assessment, determined the CDR
function. in the following manner: (i) before the participants
The influences of age and educational level on the were interviewed by doctors, public health nurses vis-
time to complete the TMT have been widely reported. ited the participants’ homes to evaluate their daily
Most studies have examined how age affects the time activities; (ii) observations by family members regard-
to complete TMT-A and TMT-B;11–18 these studies have ing the participants’ lives were obtained using a semi-
concluded that the time for TMT completion increases structured questionnaire; (iii) the participants were
with age. In contrast, conclusions regarding the influ- examined by doctors to assess episodic memory, orien-
ence of educational level on the TMT have been incon- tation and judgment; and (iv) with reference to the
sistent between studies; some studies have shown that information provided by family members, the doctors
educational level affects both parts of the TMT,11–14,16,19 and public health nurses held a joint meeting to deter-
with the time for completion of TMT-A and TMT-B mine the CDR for each participant.
being shorter for persons of higher educational level, Among the 284 participants, 160 subjects were rated
but four studies have found that education affects only as CDR 0 (healthy) and were included in the assess-
TMT-B.15,17,18,20 However, these latter studies included ment of the effects of age and educational level on the
participants with relatively high educational levels TMT test. The 160 subjects met the following criteria:
only, suggesting that the effects in a population with (i) they were assessed as normal from neurological and
a relatively low educational level were not fully psychiatric perspectives; (ii) they showed no signs of
investigated. active central nervous system or psychiatric conditions
Because motor speed is affected by the aging pro- that might adversely affect cognitive function; (iii) they
cess,21 there may be effects of age on TMT-A and B. were not taking psychoactive medication; and (iv) they
However, simply regarding the TMT-B score as the had no past history of disorders which might have
index of executive function may overestimate or affected cognition, such as head injury, alcohol or drug
underestimate the effects of aging and educational abuse, or stroke. Demographic data for the subjects are
level. Thus, to adjust for the influence of motor speed shown in Table 1. Ultimately, four of the 160 subjects
and visual search, we used the difference between were unable to perform the TMT due to physical prob-
TMT-A and TMT-B (B–A) and the ratio (B/A) as indi- lems, and therefore 156 subjects were included in the
ces of executive function in the present study. Using data analysis. Written informed consent was obtained
this procedure, we were able to use TMT-B to evaluate from all the participants and from their family mem-
executive function more accurately. Hence, the pur- bers, and the study was approved by the Ethics Com-
pose of the study was to examine the influence of age mittee of Tajiri.
and educational effect on the time for TMT completion
and to assess executive function in healthy elderly
Table 1. Study population
people, thereby obtaining normative TMT data for
Japanese subjects. Age group (years)
70–74 75–84 ≥85
METHODS
n 90 53 17
Participants % female 53 57 65
Age (years) 72.1 ± 1.4 77.7 ± 2.6 87.7 ± 2.2
Since 1988, we have been conducting a community- Years of education 8.9 ± 1.8 8.1 ± 1.7† 8.8 ± 1.9
based project on stroke, dementia, and bed-
confinement prevention in Tajiri, a typical agricultural †
Mean years of education of the 75–84-years group is
area in northern Japan (the so-called Tajiri Project). different from that of the 70–74-years group (P = 0.016).

© 2006 The Authors


Journal compilation © 2006 Folia Publishing Society
424 R. Hashimoto et al.

Tasks Analysis 2: educational effect


Trail Making Test-A and TMT-B were administered by The 156 subjects were classified into three groups
clinical psychologists and a trained occupational ther- according to educational level: 6 years (n = 15), 8 years
apist who were blinded to the CDR score. The tests (n = 101), and ≥10 years (n = 40). Based on the old
were conducted according to the standard adminis- Japanese educational system, 6 years corresponds to
tration procedure described by Spreen and Strauss.25 elementary school, 8 years corresponds to junior high
During the tests, the examiner corrected each error school, and ≥10 years corresponds to high school or
immediately. The time to complete each part of the college. ancova in spss 11.5 J was performed to identify
TMT was recorded, and raw time scores were used as the educational effect on the TMT, with the age of
the dependent variables. In the Japanese version of the subjects used as a covariate.
TMT-B, the test is revised by changing letters from
the Roman alphabet (A B C . . .) into Kana letters
RESULTS
Kana is the Japanese phonogram. The
test–retest reliability for TMT-A and TMT-B was 0.737 One subject exceeded the time limit for completing
and 0.595, respectively,18 and the time limits for per- TMT-A, and a second subject exceeded the time limit
forming TMT-A and TMT-B were set at 180 s and 480 s, for TMT-B. Eleven subjects (approx. 7% of all sub-
respectively. jects) could not perform the TMT-B because they
could not follow the rules for the test. Three of these
subjects were in the 6-years education group (20% of
Analysis 1: aging effect
the 6-years group) and eight were in the 8-years edu-
The 156 healthy elderly subjects were classified into cation group (8% of the 8-years group).
three groups according to age: 70–74 years (n = 88), 75–
84 years (n = 51), and ≥85 years (n = 17), respectively.
Analysis 1: aging effect
Using spss 11.5 J for Windows (spss Japan Inc., Tokyo,
Japan), analysis of covariance (ancova) was performed The results of analysis 1 are given in Fig. 1. Both parts
to examine the effect of aging on the TMT, with the of the TMT were affected by normal aging (TMT-A,
educational level of the subjects used as a covariate. P = 0.002; TMT-B, P < 0.001). The time to complete

(a) (b) (c) (d)


110 375 300 5.5
* * * *
* * 5.0
100 * 325 250 *
4.5
275
Time (s)

90
Time (s)

Time (s)

200 4.0

80 225 150 3.5


3.0
70 175 100
2.5
60 125 50 2.0
70–74 75–84 85+ 70–74 75–84 85+ 70–74 75–84 85+ 70–74 75–84 85+

Age groups (years)

Figure 1. Means and standard errors for (a) Trail Making Test (TMT)-A, (b) TMT-B, (c) difference of the two scores and (d)
ratio of the two scores, *P < 0.05. (a) For TMT-A, ancova showed a significant effect of age (F = 6.687, P = 0.002), and the cova-
riate of years of education was significant (F = 10.757, P < 0.001). Post-hoc tests showed that the ≥85-years group was significantly
different from the 70–74-years (P < 0.001) and 75–84-years groups (P = 0.017). (b) For TMT-B, ancova also showed a significant
effect of age (F = 9.095, P < 0.001), and the covariate of years of education was significant (F = 7.155, P = 0.008). Post-hoc tests
showed that the ≥85-years group was significantly different from the 70–74-years (P < 0.001) and 75–84-years groups (P = 0.006).
(c) For the difference of the two scores, ancova showed a significant effect of age (F = 7.507, P < 0.001), and the covariate of years
of education was significant (F = 5.196, P < 0.024). Post-hoc tests showed that the ≥85-years group was significantly different from
the 70–74-years (P < 0.001) and 75–84-years groups (P = 0.003). (d) For the ratio of the two scores, ancova showed a significant
effect of age (F = 4.12, P = 0.018), but the covariate of years of education was not significant. Post-hoc tests showed that the ≥85-
years group was significantly different from the 70–74-years (P = 0.006) and 75–84-years groups (P = 0.01).

© 2006 The Authors


Journal compilation © 2006 Folia Publishing Society
Effect of age and education on TMT 425

both parts of the test was especially long in the ≥85- for both TMT-A and TMT-B. Almost all healthy sub-
years group. The difference and ratio scores were also jects were able to perform TMT-A, while approxi-
affected by normal aging (difference score: P < 0.001, mately 7% could not complete TMT-B.
ratio score: P < 0.018) and were higher in the ≥85-years In TMT-A, a person is required to connect randomly
group. arranged numbers as fast as possible, and the main cog-
nitive functions required for this part of the test are
rapid visual searching and motor speed. In TMT-B, the
Analysis 2: educational effect
subject is required to connect numbers and letters in an
The results of analysis 2 are shown in Fig. 2. Both parts alternate manner, which requires simultaneous reten-
of the TMT were affected by educational level (TMT- tion of two sequences and flexible exchange between
A, P = 0.006; TMT-B, P = 0.027), and the time to com- these sequences. Therefore, the TMT-B score is
plete TMT-A was particularly long in the 6-years thought to reflect executive function, while the TMT-A
group. There was no difference in the completion time score reflects visuospatial function.
between the 8-years and ≥10-years groups. For TMT-B, Previous studies have indicated that the time for
there was no difference in completion time between completion of the TMT is affected by aging, and that
the 6-years and 8-years groups, but the time to com- the time for TMT-B completion is affected by educa-
plete TMT-B was shorter in the ≥10-years group. tional level.11–20 There is inconsistency in previous
Although the effect of educational level on the differ- reports regarding the effect of educational level on
ence score was not significant, P approached statistical TMT-A times.
significance (P = 0.056). The educational effect on the
ratio score was not significant (P = 0.309).
Aging effect on the TMT
Only limited normative TMT data are available for
DISUCUSSION
older elderly people.15,17 The sample size in our ≥85-
A summary of the mean values and standard devia- years group was relatively small (n = 17), but subjects
tions is given for each age and educational-level sub- with MCI were carefully excluded using the CDR
group for both parts of the TMT in Table 2. These are scale, and therefore we believe that our data are rep-
normative TMT data for Japanese healthy elderly sub- resentative of the functional ability of normal older
jects (CDR 0) in a population properly distinguished elderly people. We found that the times for completion
from subjects with questionable dementia (CDR 0.5). of the two parts of the TMT were affected by normal
Significant aging and educational effects were found aging, consistent with previous reports.11–18 The time for

(a) (b) (c) (d)


100 300 300 5.5
5.0
90 260 250
4.5
Time (s)
Time (s)

80 220
Time (s)

200 4.0

70 180 150 3.5


* 3.0
60 140 *
100
* * 2.5
50 100 50 2.0
6 8 10+ 6 8 10+ 6 8 10+ 6 8 10+

Educational levels

Figure 2. Means and standard errors for (a) Trail Making Test (TMT)-A, (b) TMT-B, (c) difference of the two scores and (d)
ratio of the two scores, *P < 0.05. (a) For TMT-A, ancova showed a significant effect of educational level (F = 5.259, P = 0.006),
and the covariate of age was significant (F = 18.187, P < 0.001). Post-hoc tests showed significant differences between the 6-years
and 8-years groups (P = 0.019) and between the 6-years and ≥10-years groups (P = 0.002). (b) For TMT-B, ancova also showed
a significant effect of education (F = 3.852, P = 0.024), and the covariate of age was significant (F = 18.699, P < 0.001). Post-hoc
tests showed a significant difference between the 6-years and ≥10-years groups (P = 0.017) and between the 8-years and ≥10-
years groups (P = 0.027). (c) For the difference of the two scores, ancova did not show a significant effect of education (F = 2.939,
P = 0.056), but the covariate of age was significant (F = 13.167, P < 0.001). (d) For the ratio of the two scores, ancova also did not
show a significant effect of education (F = 1.185, P = 0.308), but again the covariate of age was significant (F = 4.068, P = 0.046).

© 2006 The Authors


Journal compilation © 2006 Folia Publishing Society
426 R. Hashimoto et al.

Table 2. Normative data for the TMT in Japanese subjects

TMT-A time (s) TMT-B time (s)


Age group Years of
(years) education Mean ± SD n Mean ± SD n

70–74 6 83.1 ± 39.3 7 259.8 ± 97.0 5


8 63.4 ± 23.1 53 190.6 ± 86.0 49
≥10 57.3 ± 16.2 28 154.4 ± 71.7 27
Total 63.0 ± 23.5 88 182.8 ± 85.2 81
75–84 6 84.9 ± 29.8 7 234.7 ± 81.2 6
8 73.9 ± 26.0 35 229.1 ± 94.9 35
≥10 61.0 ± 22.0 8 182.0 ± 43.8 8
Total 73.3 ± 26.3 50 222.1 ± 87.6 49
≥85 6 121.0 ± 0. 1 295.0 ± 0. 1
8 89.7 ± 41.5 12 289.7 ± 96.8 9
≥10 75.0 ± 33.8 4 276.3 ± 112.8 4
Total 88.1 ± 38.8 17 286.2 ± 93.5 14
Total 6 86.5 ± 33.7 15 250.2 ± 82.3 12
8 70.2 ± 27.9 100 214.7 ± 94.6 93
≥10 59.8 ± 19.6 40 172.6 ± 79.1 39
Total 69.1 ± 27.5 155 206.2 ± 92.0 144

TMT, Trail Making Test.


An 80-year-old woman who had an 8-years education level could not finish within the time limit for TMT-A, and a 71-year-
old man who had a ≥10-years education level could not finish within the time limit for TMT-B.

completion of the TMT was markedly increased in the data is that the threshold for influence on the TMT dif-
≥85-years group (Fig. 1), suggesting that motor skills, fers between educational levels. For TMT-A, the time
visual searching and executive function do not decline for completion may not be influenced by educational
very significantly in normal aging until the age of level, provided that the subjects have at least some
85 years. Confirmation of this conclusion will require education (>6 years). Therefore, for surveys targeted at
accumulation of more data, including data for younger relatively highly educated participants, the influence of
adults. educational level on TMT-A may not be detectable sta-
tistically. In contrast, a statistically significant differ-
ence in the time for completion of TMT-B was found
Educational effect on the TMT
between educational levels 8 years and ≥10 years. This
The mean time to complete TMT-A and TMT-B result is consistent with previous reports, especially
decreased with higher educational level, consistent those including highly educated subjects, and our data
with several previous reports.11–14,16,19 However, there is confirm that highly educated persons (>8 years) are
controversy concerning the effect of educational level likely to complete TMT-B in a shorter time than those
on the TMT, especially for TMT-A, because other with less education.
reports have concluded that TMT-A is not influenced
by education, while still finding an influence on TMT-
Executive function
B.15,17,18,20 It should be noted that the subjects in these
reports were highly educated; almost all had educa- The results of analysis 1 showed that the aging effect is
tional levels of ≥10 years. In contrast, the present pop- significant after adjusting for the influence of motor
ulation included subjects with education only up to the speed and visual search. Thus, the aging process clearly
elementary school level (6-years group). affects executive function. Stuss et al. found a relation-
The educational effects on TMT-A and TMT-B were ship between TMT-B impairment and frontal lobe
statistically significant for persons of low educational damage, by comparing patients with damage to the
level in the present subjects, but interestingly we did frontal and non-frontal regions with control subjects.26
not find a significant difference between the 8-years Recently, Tisserand et al. reported a longitudinal study
and ≥10-years groups for TMT-A. Moreover, the mean using voxel-based morphometry, which showed that
time to complete TMT-A was long, especially in the 6- the largest age-related decreases in gray matter in
years group. Thus, one possible interpretation of these healthy elderly people were found in the frontal lobe.27

© 2006 The Authors


Journal compilation © 2006 Folia Publishing Society
Effect of age and education on TMT 427

Thus atrophy or dysfunction in the frontal lobe may be cognitive functioning in aging. J. Gerontol. B Psychol.
reflected by the TMT-B score, and hence may be an Sci. Soc. Sci. 1996; 51: 217–225.
index of executive function. In addition, our results 8. Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indi-
showed that the influence of aging is particularly cator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 1958;
8: 271–276.
important in the ≥85 years age group.
9. Crowe SF. The differential contribution of mental track-
In the present study we were unable to show a clear
ing, cognitive flexibility, visual search, and motor speed
educational effect on executive function, using both to performance on parts A and B of the Trail Making
the difference and the of the TMT-A and -B scores. Test. J. Clin. Psychol. 1998; 54: 585–591.
One of the possible explanations for this result is that 10. Loewenstein DA, Ownby R, Schram L et al. An evalua-
educational level did not affect executive function, as tion of the NINCDS-ADRDA neuropsychological
measured by TMT-B, when the effects of motor speed criteria for the assessment of Alzheimer’s disease: a
and visual searching ability were properly controlled. confirmatory factor analysis of single versus multi-
Thus, our data may suggest that reduced TMT-B com- factor models. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2001; 23:
pletion times for more educated subjects overestimate 274–284.
the effect of educational level. Another possibility is 11. Goul WR, Brown M. Effects of age and intelligence on
trail making test performance and validity. Percept. Mot.
that although executive function is influenced by edu-
Skills 1970; 30: 319–326.
cational level, the effect might not have been detect-
12. Kennedy KJ. Age effects on trail making test perfor-
able statistically because of the small sample size of the mance. Percept. Mot. Skills 1981; 52: 671–675.
low educational level group (n = 12). In addition, a rel- 13. Stanton BA, Jenkins CD, Savageau JA, Zyzanski SJ,
atively large number of subjects with low educational Aucoin R. Age and educational differences on the trail
level (approx. 20% of the 6-years group) were unable making test and Wechsler memory scales. Percept. Mot.
to complete TMT-B. However, differences in the indi- Skills 1984; 58: 311–318.
ces of executive function were not found between the 14. Stuss DT, Stethem LL, Poirier CA. Comparison of three
8-years and ≥10-years groups, and hence further work tests of attention and rapid information processing across
is needed to confirm the relationship between educa- six age groups. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1987; 1: 139–152.
tion and executive function as measured by TMT-B. 15. Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE et al. Neuropsychological
tests’ norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE Token,
WRAT-R Reading, AMNART, Stroop, TMT and JLO.
Clin. Neuropsychol. 1996; 10: 262–278.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 16. Giovagnoli AR, Del Pesce M, Mascheroni S, Simoncelli
We are grateful to all staff members of the Tajiri SKIP M, Laiacona M, Capitani E. Trail making test: normative
Center. values from 287 normal adult controls. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci.
1996; 17: 305–309.
17. Tombaugh TN. Trail making test A and B: normative
REFERENCES data stratified by age and education. Arch. Clin. Neurop-
sychol. 2004; 19: 203–214.
1. Flicker C, Ferris SH, Reisberg B. Mild cognitive impair- 18. Abe M, Suzuki K, Okada K et al. Normative data on tests
ment in the elderly: predictors of dementia. Neurology for frontal lobe functions: trail making test, verbal flu-
1991; 41: 1006–1009. ency, Wisconsin card sorting test (Keio version). No To
2. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC et al. Mild cognitive Shinkei 2004; 57: 567–574 (in Japanese).
impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. 19. Bornstein RA, Suga LJ. Educational level and neurop-
Neurol. 1999; 56: 303–308. sychological performance in healthy elderly subjects.
3. Chen P, Ratcliff G, Belle S et al. Cognitive tests that best Dev. Neuropsychol. 1988; 4: 17–22.
discriminate between presymptomatic AD and those 20. Ernst J. Neuropsychological problem-solving skills in the
who remain nondemented. Neurology 2000; 55: 1847– elderly. Psychol. Aging 1987; 2: 363–365.
1853. 21. Shimoyama I, Ninchoji T, Uemura K. The finger-tapping
4. Albert MS, Moss MB, Tanzi R et al. Preclinical predic- test: a quantitative analysis. Arch. Neurol. 1990; 47: 681–
tion of AD using neuropsychological tests. J. Int. Neu- 684.
ropsychol. Soc. 2001; 7: 631–639. 22. Meguro K, Ishii H, Yamaguchi S et al. Prevalence of
5. Kaskie B, Storandt M. Visuospatial deficit in dementia of dementia and dementing diseases in Japan: the Tajiri
the Alzheimer type. Arch. Neurol. 1995; 52: 422–425. Project. Arch. Neurol. 2002; 59: 1109–1114.
6. Sato M, Meguro K, Ishizaki J et al. Early detection of 23. Hughes C, Berg L, Danziger WL et al. A new clinical
patients with Alzheimer’s disease with the Benton Visual scale for the staging of dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 1982;
Form Discrimination Test. Jpn. J. Neuropsychol. 2001; 17: 140: 566–572.
62–68 (in Japanese). 24. Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): cur-
7. Sliwinski M, Lipton RB, Buschke H, Stewart W. The rent version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993; 43: 2412–
effect of preclinical dementia on estimates of normal 2414.

© 2006 The Authors


Journal compilation © 2006 Folia Publishing Society
428 R. Hashimoto et al.

25. Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsycho- 27. Tisserand DJ, Van Boxtel MPJ, Pruessner JC et al. A
logical Tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary, voxel-based morphometric study to determine individual
2nd edn. Oxford University Press, New York, 1998. differences in gray matter density associated with age
26. Stuss DT, Bisschop SM, Alexander MP, Levine B, and cognitive change over time. Cereb. Cortex 2004; 14:
Izukawa D. The trail making test: a study in focal lesion 966–973.
patients. Psychol. Assess. 2001; 13: 230–239.

© 2006 The Authors


Journal compilation © 2006 Folia Publishing Society

You might also like