Bruno, 2017

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

not permitted.

It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA

© 2016 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness 2017 July-August;57(7-8):976-84
Online version at http://www.minervamedica.it DOI: 10.23736/S0022-4707.16.06411-2

REVIEW
EXERCISE PHYSIOLOGY AND BIOMECHANICS

Effect of pre-task music on sports or exercise performance


Bruno P. SMIRMAUL *

Department of Physical Education, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil
*Corresponding author: Bruno P. Smirmaul, Department of Physical Education, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Avenida 24-A 1515, Bela Vista, 13506-
900, Rio Claro (SP), Brazil. E-mail: brunosmirmaul@gmail.com

A B S T RAC T
INTRODUCTION: Pre-task music is a very common strategy among sports competitors. However, as opposed to in-task music, the scientific
evidence to support its ergogenic effects on either sports or exercise performance is limited. This brief review critically addresses the existing
literature investigating the effects of pre-task music on sports and exercise performance, focusing on the methods and results of experimental
studies, and offers basic and practical recommendations.
EVIDENCE ACQUISITION: In July 2015, a comprehensive literature search was performed in Web of Science, PubMed, and Google Scholar
using the following key words in combination: “pre-task music,” “pre-test music,” “pre-exercise music,” “exercise performance,” “sports per-
formance.” The literature search was further expanded by both hand searching review articles on the topic and by searching the reference lists
from the articles retrieved for any relevant references. Overall, a total of 15 studies in 14 articles were included.
EVIDENCE SYNTHESIS: Pre-task music research has been unsystematic, methodologically limited and infrequent. Using this review as a
starting point to overcome previous methodological limitations when designing future experiments may contribute to the development of pre-
task music research, which is still in its infancy.
CONCLUSIONS: Currently, there is no sufficient evidence to support the overall ergogenic effects of pre-task music on sports or exercise per-
formance. Nonetheless, pre-task music has showed a likely ergogenic effect on shorter and predominantly anaerobic tasks such as grip strength,
Wingate test, and short-duration sports or sports-like tasks, in contrast to longer and predominantly aerobic tasks.
(Cite this article as: Smirmaul BP. Effect of pre-task music on sports or exercise performance. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 2017;57:976-84. DOI:
10.23736/S0022-4707.16.06411-2)
Key words: Music - Athletic performance - Central nervous system stimulants.

Introduction exercise and sports context include improved mood,


arousal control, dissociation, reduced perception of ef-
M usic has played a noticeable role in exercise and
sports activities since the ancient Greek culture,
in which both training procedures and events at the ear-
fort and enhanced performance.4
Although individuals may benefit from the effects of
in-task music during recreational exercise/sports partic-
ly Olympic Games were often practiced accompanied ipation and training routines, most, if not all, sports gov-
by music.1 Today, music has become ubiquitous within erning bodies forbid the use of electronic devices (e.g.,
the exercise and sports context. The scientific research personal music devices) during actual competition.5
on this area has been prolific in the past 40 years and, Nevertheless, competitors still have the opportunity to
collectively, more than 100 peer-reviewed published use music through a different and feasible approach in
articles have investigated several aspects of the music- an attempt to improve performance, that is, in the min-
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

exercise/sports relationship. Most of them have focused utes before actual competition (e.g., during the warm-
on the effects of in-task asynchronous and synchronous up or preparation/concentration phase). In fact, pre-task
music on psychological states and exercise/sports per- music has been used in a variety of sports and is a very
formance.2-4 Generally, the benefits of music within the common strategy among competitors.5-10

976 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness July-August 2017
not permitted. It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA
EFFECT OF PRE-TASK MUSIC ON PERFORMANCE SMIRMAUL

Despite its popularity, there is limited scientific evi- included. Studies were divided in three categories, ac-
dence regarding the ergogenic effects of pre-task mu- cording to the nature of the task investigated. Due to
sic on either sports or exercise performance. Specific the low number of studies, muscular strength (i.e., grip
sections within publications have highlighted some of strength) and muscular endurance tasks (i.e. bent-knee
the findings in this topic.4, 8, 11 These sections, however, sit-ups and dumbbell shoulder flexion), were grouped
have addressed a very small number of studies, only under “muscular strength/endurance tasks” (Table I;
partially covering the existing literature, thus leaving a N.=4); 30-45 s Wingate tasks were grouped under “an-
significant part of the evidence unexplored. Addition- aerobic tasks” (Table II; N.=6); and sports or sports/like
ally, there has been no critical appraisal on the studies tasks (e.g., 200-m swimming and 60-meter dash) were
published on this topic. Therefore, the purpose of this grouped under “sports and sports-like tasks” (Table III;
brief review is to critically address the existing studies N.=5). All tables depict the studies’ characteristics and
investigating the ergogenic effects of pre-task music on the main exercise or sports performance results based
exercise and sports performance, focusing on the meth- on both P<0.05 and respective effect sizes. Effect sizes
ods and results of experimental studies, and to offer ba- (Cohen’s d) were calculate as the mean difference be-
sic and applied recommendations. tween conditions divided by the standard deviation of
the control condition (when reported).
Evidence acquisition
Evidence synthesis
Firstly, a literature search was performed in July
2015 to identify published articles on the effects of pre- Pre-task music and muscular strength/endurance tasks
task music on exercise or sports performance. The data- From the four studies in Table I, two investigated
bases used were Web of Science, PubMed, and Google the effects of pre-task music on maximum force (grip
Scholar, and there was no limit to the search period. In strength).15, 16 Both studies, using undergraduate stu-
order to retrieve the highest number of articles, broad dents, showed a statistically significant improvement
search terms were used in combination: “pre-task mu- in performance after stimulative music. While it was
sic,” “pre-test music,” “pre-exercise music,” “exercise not possible to calculate the effect size for one study
performance,” “sports performance.” Collectively, only due to the lack of information reported,15 the other 16
5 articles on the topic were found. The literature search showed an effect size of 0.14. Pearce,15 who did not
was expanded by both hand searching review articles report the fitness level of the individuals, selected
on the effects of music on exercise performance and stimulative and sedative music using a purely anec-
sports performance in general and by searching the ref- dotal proposition by a previous paper.17 Such propo-
erence lists from the articles retrieved for any relevant sition defined stimulative and sedative music based
references. Specifically, “pre-task music” sections from on its rhythm, with stimulative music presenting “de-
review articles 4, 8, 11 were identified and provided rel- tached notes” and sedative music being “melodic.”17
evant references. The criteria for inclusion in the pres- On the other hand, Karageorghis et al.,16 who repli-
ent review were: 1) studies investigating the effect of cated Pearce’s study with individuals who engaged in
pre-task music on exercise or sports performance ob- regular physical activity (no further details provided),
jectively measured; 2) studies comparing at least two improved the music’s selection process by using a
conditions (either two pre-task music conditions or music rating inventory, as well as other limitations
one pre-task music condition and a control condition); such as music intensity (volume) and experimental
3) studies published in scientific journals employing condition ordering.16 Nonetheless, the duration of mu-
peer review that use English as their primary language. sic exposure was different between conditions (longer
Studies in which the dependent variable was not charac- for stimulative music) and time between exposure and
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

terized as exercise or sport performance,5, 12, 13 in which task was not reported.16
exercise performance was not objectively measured,14 The remaining two studies of this section investigat-
or involving only qualitative interviews 10 were not in- ed the effects of pre-task music on muscular endurance
cluded. Thus, a total of 15 studies in 14 articles were (bent-knee sit-ups and dumbbell shoulder flexion).18, 19

Vol. 57 - No. 7-8 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness 977
not permitted. It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA
SMIRMAUL EFFECT OF PRE-TASK MUSIC ON PERFORMANCE

Table I.—Characteristics and findings of the pre-task music effects on exercise performance during muscular strength/endurance tasks.

Study N. Sample characteristics Music conditions Music selection

Pearce (1981) 15 49 Undergraduate students Stimulative, sedative, and no Selected by the investigator; artist and title provided
(33 M, 16 F) music

Karageorghis et al. (1996) 16 50 Physically active sport Stimulative music, sedative Top 10 of the British music chart; categorized as
science undergraduates music, and white noise stimulative (134 bpm) or sedative (90 bpm);
(25 M, 25 F) graded by a modified music rating inventory by 10
students; artist and title provided
Dorney et al. (1992) 18 40 University students Positive imagery with and without Selected by the investigators; artist and title
Study 2 (40 M) music provided; 148 bpm, popular vocal track
Crust (2004) 19 9 Sport and exercise Self-selected music and white Self-selected according to: 1) top 40 British music
science undergraduates noise charts; 2) fast tempo (>120 bpm); 3) 180-270 s; 4)
(9 M) >18.3 points in the Brunel Music Rating Inventory
NR: not reported; >: statistically higher; =: no statistical difference; <: statistically lower.

Both studies, which did not report the fitness level of the Wingate anaerobic test. All but one 20 employed 30-sec-
participants, did not find a significant effect of pre-task ond exercise duration tests. While this same study 20
music on muscular endurance (Table I). One of the stud- only compared pre-task music rhythms (slow versus
ies actually investigated the additional effect of pre-task fast), finding no differences between conditions, 4 out
music on a positive imagery technique, confounding the of the remaining 5 studies showed a statistically signifi-
relationship between pre-task music and performance.18 cant improvement in peak power 21-24 and 3 out of 5 on
Although both conditions (imagery-only and imagery- mean power 22-24 during the Wingate test using pre-task
plus-music) improved performance, the addition of music (calculated effect sizes ranging from 0.17 to 0.50;
music had no further effect (both conditions, d=1.00). Table II). These results appear to be consistent regard-
While one study used fast music (148 bpm) selected by less of the fitness levels of individuals, which varied
the investigators,18 the other used stricter criteria in an from physically active students to national-level ath-
attempt to control for the motivational qualities of the letes. Two distinguishing characteristics were consis-
music.19 Both studies performed the task immediately tent among the studies which improved performance:
after listening to music and, while the duration of the 1) all utilized pre-task music of at least 120 bpm; and
exposure to music varied within studies (190-230 s and 2) all performed the anaerobic task immediately after
180-270 s), it was similar between them.18, 19 music exposure. Loizou and Karageorghis,25 who did
In summary, the results of this section suggest a likely not find a statistically significant improvement in per-
ergogenic effect of pre-task music on muscular strength formance (although presenting an effect size of 0.17
performance, as both studies reviewed demonstrated a for peak power), employed a 3-min warm-up plus ad-
statistically significant improvement in grip strength. It ditional measurements between the pre-task music ex-
is noteworthy that it was not possible to calculate the ef- posure and task initiation, not reporting the exact time
fect size for one of the studies, while the other showed between them. In addition, they did not specify music
an effect size of d=0.14. In contrast, there has been no rhythm.25 A new aspect observed in this section is that
indication of a statistically significant influence of pre- three studies 21, 22, 24 utilized pre-task music during the
task music on muscular endurance performance. warm-up, rather than at rest, raising a topic for further
discussion. Lastly, 4 out of 6 studies measured ratings
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

Pre-task music and anaerobic tasks of perceived exertion (RPE).21-24 The results were con-
flicting, with two studies finding a higher RPE after the
All 6 studies within this section investigated the ef- warm-up during the music condition,21, 22 and two find-
fects of pre-task music on the same exercise task, i.e., ing no differences between conditions.23, 24 At the end of

978 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness July-August 2017
not permitted. It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA
EFFECT OF PRE-TASK MUSIC ON PERFORMANCE SMIRMAUL

Duration of Time between Performance


exposure Task Results (based on P<0.05) Effect size (d)
the exposure measurement
and task
2 min NR Grip strength Maximum force Stimulative > sedative –
No music = sedative –
Stimulative = no music –
190-230 s NR Grip strength Maximum force Stimulative > sedative 0.14
Stimulative > white music 0.06
White music > sedative 0.08

30 s 0 min Muscular endurance (30-s bent-knee sit-ups) Repetitions Imagery + music = imagery Imagery: 1.00
Imagery + music: 1.00
180-270 s 0 min Isometric strength endurance (shoulder flexion) Time Self-selected = white noise 0.08

the Wingate tests, only one study 22 found a difference study reported the exact time between pre-task music
for RPE between conditions, with a higher RPE for the exposure and task initiation.
music condition. Of the remaining three studies included in this sec-
In summary, the results from this section suggest tion, both the 200-m swimming and the 60-m dash
a very likely ergogenic effect of fast pre-task music tasks presented a statistically significant improvement
(>120 bpm) immediately prior anaerobic exercise per- in performance after pre-task music.27, 29 Effect size
formance (Wingate tests), irrespective of the individu- was 0.09 for pre-task self-selected music when com-
als’ fitness level. pared to the no music control condition in the 200-m
swimming task.29 However, in the 60-m dash study,
Pre-task music and sports and sports-like tasks authors did not provide neither standard deviation data
nor individual results, precluding the effect size cal-
Although this section involved sports and sports-like culation.27 It is noteworthy that both studies in which
tasks, the five studies included presented considerable pre-task music significantly improved performance
methodological heterogeneity among them. Both the involved relatively short and predominantly anaero-
dart throwing 18 and karate drill 26 tasks presented a quite bic tasks (60-m dash and 200-m freestyle swimming),
particular nature of performance, once it involves par- compared to the 5-km cycling time trial, a rather lon-
ticular skills such as concentration and precision, rather ger and predominantly aerobic task, which resulted
than aspects such as muscular power and fatigue tol- in no differences between pre-task music and control
erance as in the running,27 cycling,28 and swimming 29 conditions.28 The music selection process, as well
tasks. While fast or slow tempo pre-task music did not as the duration of exposure to pre-task music varied
alter dart throwing performance, both positive and neg- among studies. While in the 60-m dash study music
ative pre-task music resulted in a statistically significant was selected by the investigators and duration of expo-
improvement in the karate drill performance, with effect sure was only 1min, in the 5-km cycling time trial and
sizes of 2.00 and 1.98, respectively. Both studies, how- 200-m swimming study, participants self-selected the
ever, did not provide much information regarding music pre-task music, and duration of exposure was 10 and
selection. While in the dart throwing study 18 there was 5 minutes, respectively. Only two studies measured
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

no details regarding music tempo (only the labels of fast RPE.28, 29 During the 5-km cycling time-trial, RPE was
and slow), in the karate drill study 26 positive and nega- measured at each kilometer, with no statistical dif-
tive music were chosen according to an unpublished ferences between conditions, although “likely” lower
study, precluding further analysis. Additionally, neither for the music condition using the smallest worthwhile

Vol. 57 - No. 7-8 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness 979
not permitted. It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA
SMIRMAUL EFFECT OF PRE-TASK MUSIC ON PERFORMANCE

Table II.—Characteristics and findings of the pre-task music effects on exercise performance during anaerobic tasks.

Study N. Sample characteristics Music conditions Music selection

Yamamoto et al. (2003) 20 6 Physically active students Slow- and fast-paced music Selected by the investigators; artist provided
(6 M)
Eliakim et al. (2007) 21 24 Elite, adolescent, Arousing music and no music Selected by the investigators; title provided;
national-team level 140 bpm
volleyball players
(12 M, 12 F)

Chtourou et al. (2012) 22 12 Physical education High tempo and no music Selected by the investigators, based on the
students (morning and evening) recommendations of Karageorghis et al.;2 >120-
(12 M) 140 bpm

Chtourou et al. (2012) 24 9 National- and regional- High tempo and no music Selected by the investigators; 120-140 bpm
level sprinters
(9 M)

Jarraya et al. (2012) 23 12 National- and regional- High tempo and no music Selected by the investigators; based on the
level sprinters recommendations of Karageorghis et al.;2 >120-
(12 M) 140 bpm

Loizou et al. (2015) 25 15 Heterogeneous sports Music and control Selected by the investigators; artist and title
participation level provided
(15 M)

NR: not reported; >: statistically higher; =: no statistical difference; <: statistically lower.

Table III.—Characteristics and findings of the pre-task music effects on exercise performance during sports and sports-like tasks.

Study N. Sample characteristics Music conditions Music selection

Dorney et al. (1992) 18 30 University students Slow-tempo classical and fast- Selected by the investigators; artist and title
Study 1 (13 M, 17 F) tempo popular music provided

Ferguson et al. (1994) 26 14 Shotokan karate Positive, negative, and white noise Selected by the investigators; classified as positive
practitioners or negative by a previous unpublished study
(10 M, 4 F)
Hall et al. (1995) 27 15 NCAA division II track Music and control (counting Selected by the investigators; track provided
and field athletes backwards)
(15 M)
Bigliassi et al. (2012) 28 10 National- and regional- Self-selected and no music Selected by the participants
level cyclists
(10 M)
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

Smirmaul et al. (2015) 29 18 Regional-level Self-selected and no music Selected by the participants
swimmers
(18 M)
NR: not reported; >: statistically higher; =: no statistical difference; <: statistically lower.
*Shorter time means better performance.

980 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness July-August 2017
not permitted. It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA
EFFECT OF PRE-TASK MUSIC ON PERFORMANCE SMIRMAUL

Duration of Time between Performance Effect


exposure Task Results (based on P<0.05)
the exposure measurement size (d)
and task
20 min 1 min 45-s Wingate anaerobic test Mean power Slow music = fast music –
output
10 min 0 min 30-s Wingate anaerobic test Peak power Peak power
Mean power Males: music > no music 0.43
Females: music > no music 0.19
Mean power
Males: music = no music 0.15
Females: music = no music 0.15
10 min 0 min 30-s Wingate anaerobic test Peak power > Peak power and mean power with music in the morning –
Mean power and evening
> Improvement in mean power with music in the morning –
10 min 0 min 30-s Wingate anaerobic test Peak power Peak power
Mean power High tempo > no music 0.43
Mean power
High tempo > no music 0.77
10 min 0 min 30-s Wingate anaerobic test Peak power Peak power
Mean power High tempo > no music 0.42
Mean power
High tempo > no music 0.50
5 min NR 30-s Wingate anaerobic test Peak power Peak power
Mean power Music = control 0.17
Mean power
Music = control -0.04

Duration of Time between Performance Effect


exposure Task Results (based on P<0.05)
the exposure measurement size (d)
and task
90 s NR Dart throwing Points achieved Fast = slow 0.12
No-music = slow -0.08
Fast = no-music 0.04
1 min NR Karate drill Karate drill ratings Positive > white noise 2.00
Negative > white noise 1.98
Positive = negative 0.02
1 min NR 60-meter dash Time Music < control* –

10 min 2 min 5-km cycling timed trial Time Self-selected = no music* -0.55
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

5 min 1 min 200-m freestyle swimming Time Self-selected < no music* 0.09
timed trial

Vol. 57 - No. 7-8 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness 981
not permitted. It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA
SMIRMAUL EFFECT OF PRE-TASK MUSIC ON PERFORMANCE

change analysis.28 RPE was also measured at the end viduals.16 Studies utilized music entirely selected by the
of the 200-m swimming time-trial, presenting no dif- investigators (11 studies), entirely self-selected by the
ferences between conditions.29 participants (2 studies), or selected within specific top
In summary, both studies investigating tasks requir- music charts (2 studies). Most of the studies employed
ing specific skills such as concentration and precision a two-condition design including music and no-music
(dart throwing and karate drill) presented mixed results, conditions (10 studies), while some employed a two-
precluding any conclusion or indication of a possible condition design with differing music’s characteristics
ergogenic effect of pre-task music on this kind of sport (2 studies) or a three-condition design with two music
performance. On the other hand, while the longer and and one no-music condition (3 studies). The duration
predominantly aerobic task (5-km cycling performance) of pre-task music exposure varied from 30 seconds 18
was not improved by pre-task music, both shorter and to 20 minutes,20 with the most prevalent duration being
predominantly anaerobic tasks (60-m dash and 200-m 10 minutes (5 studies). The most prevalent time length
swimming) presented an improvement in performance between pre-task music exposure and beginning of the
after self-selected pre-task music, indicating a possible task was 0 (i.e., immediately) (6 studies), but varied
relationship between the duration of the task on the er- from 0 to 2 minutes, with 8 studies not providing (or not
gogenic effects of pre-task music on performance. clearly stating) this information. Lastly, while most of
the studies utilized pre-task music at rest (11 studies),
Discussion 5 studies employed music during the warm-up period
before the exercise task.21-24, 28
This brief review addressed 15 intervention studies in Overall, the utilization of pre-task music significantly
14 scientific peer-reviewed articles investigating the er- improved exercise performance in 9 of 15 studies, in-
gogenic effects of pre-task music on exercise and sports dicating a likely pattern distinguishing among exercise
performance. This represents a pioneer effort to critically tasks characteristics (i.e., anaerobic × aerobic tasks).
review the existing scientific evidence on this topic and Within the section “muscular strength/endurance tasks,”
offers basic and applied recommendations. Since the first while both studies investigating grip strength showed
study by Pearce 34 years ago,15 research on the effects of improvements in performance after pre-task music,
pre-task music on exercise and sports performance has both studies involving muscular endurance tasks did
been unsystematic, methodologically limited and infre- not. For the “anaerobic tasks” section, 4 out of 6 studies
quent (less than one publication every two years: one resulted in improvements in performance immediately
study in 1980s; five studies in 1990s; three studies in after fast (>120 bpm) pre-task music, whereas only the
2000s; six studies in 2010s). This is somewhat surpris- shorter and predominantly anaerobic tasks from the
ing, giving the widespread utilization of this strategy by “sports and sports-like tasks” section were affected by
competitors from a variety of sports worldwide.5-10 pre-task music.
From the 15 studies reviewed, the dependent vari- By focusing on the methods and results of the articles,
ables investigated were: muscular strength/endurance this brief review has identified several significant limi-
tasks in 4 studies (2 grip strength and 2 muscular endur- tations on overall pre-task music research to date. A first
ance tasks); anaerobic tasks in 6 studies (all Wingate major limitation in pre-task music research has been the
tests); and sports and sports-like tasks in 5 studies (dart absence of a guiding rationale for the music selection
throwing; karate drill; 60-meter dash; 5-km cycling process. According to Karageorghis and Priest,4 consid-
time trial; 200-m freestyle swimming time trial). Sam- eration of the music’s motivational qualities during its
ple characteristics in terms of level of fitness exclude selection process are crucial, once motivational quali-
varied greatly, from recreationally active university stu- ties are closely linked to the music’s effects on arousal,
dents to elite national athletes. However, a fairly poor perception of effort and mood. For instance, percep-
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

description of participant’s fitness level was provided tion of effort and motivation are proposed as crucial
throughout studies. Exclusively males (10 studies) or variables affecting/limiting exercise performance.30, 31
both male and female adults (5 studies) were investi- However, 11 of the 15 pre-task music studies reviewed
gated, with sample sizes varying from six 20 to 50 indi- employed music arbitrarily and entirely selected by

982 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness July-August 2017
not permitted. It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA
EFFECT OF PRE-TASK MUSIC ON PERFORMANCE SMIRMAUL

either the investigators or participants for their experi- porting procedures in sufficient details, such as music’s
mental conditions, without a valid consideration of tempo and intensity, as well as the time length between
its motivational properties. In order to overcome such music exposure and beginning of the task; 3) adding
limitation, effective and standard selection of motiva- a pre-task music condition designed to be neutral re-
tional music is needed. Utilization of instruments such garding its motivational properties. Moreover, various
as The Brunel Music Rating Inventory-3,32 which has research questions on the ergogenic effects of pre-task
been used to assess music’s motivational properties for music on exercise performance remains to be answered
in-task utilization may be a feasible solution. However, in order to improve this research topic and enable rec-
future research should address the need for a specific ommendations for its practical application. For instance,
instrument for pre-task music selection, as none are cur- the influence of the following factors may be further
rently available. Another limitation has been the lack explored: 1) the characteristics of the task (muscular
of controlling/reporting methodological procedures in strength/endurance or whole-body anaerobic/endurance
sufficient detail. Information regarding music’s char- with varying durations); 2) pre-task music exposure at
acteristics such as tempo and intensity, as well as the rest vs. during the warm-up; 3) the combination of pre-
time between music exposure and task initiation has task music with pre-task video, primes, imagery, etc.; 4)
been largely disregarded. For instance, only 7 studies the time between pre-task music exposure and begin-
controlled for music tempo, and 6 studies did not report ning of the task; 5) the duration of pre-task music ex-
the time length between music exposure and beginning posure; 6) the baseline motivational level of individuals
of the task. While the former is known as an important (especially in high-level athletes); and 7) the effects of
feature for the ergogenic properties of music,33 the latter pre-task music during actual sports competition.
information is potentially important once it is reason-
able to speculate, despite the lack of direct scientific ev- Conclusions
idence, that the carryover effects of pre-task music may
be time-dependent. Lastly, as previously suggested,34 Previous work reviewing early general research on
no study has employed an additional oudeterous music music and exercise-related tasks prior to the mid-1990’s
condition (neither motivating nor demotivating). Such identified several methodological and theoretical limi-
additional condition would work similar to a placebo tations.2 Currently, music research in the exercise and
condition, allowing the specific determination of the er- sports context, particularly in-task music, has markedly
gogenic effects of motivational/neutral pre-task music overcome these earlier shortcomings,3, 4 arguably mov-
compared to a no-music condition. ing from its infancy to its adulthood. However, research
Due to the methodological limitations and mixed re- on the particular topic of pre-task music still appears to
sults identified in this review, there is no sufficient sci- be on its infancy, once similar limitations were identi-
entific evidence to support the overall ergogenic effects fied throughout the studies reviewed here. In addition to
of pre-task music on exercise performance. Nonethe- the recommendations here proposed, taking a page out
less, pre-task music has showed a likely ergogenic ef- of the book of its more mature sibling, in-task music
fect on short and predominantly anaerobic tasks such research,3, 4 and learning from its development is likely
to be of great benefit to the pre-task music research, per-
as grip strength, Wingate, and short-duration sports/
haps facilitating its growth from infancy to adulthood
sports-like tasks, in contrast to longer and predominant-
more rapidly.
ly aerobic tasks. In order to strengthen such prelimi-
nary applied recommendations and advance scientific
knowledge on this topic, researchers should be aware References
of previous methodological limitations when designing 1. Romano DG. Culture and Tradition: The Ancient Olympic Games
future experiments on the effects of pre-task music on Salt Lake Organizing Committee for the Olympic Winter Games and
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

Paralympic Winter Games; 2000.


exercise performance. A good starting point may consist 2. Karageorghis CI, Terry PC. The psychophysical effects of music in
in: 1) improving the quality of the music selection pro- sport and exercise: a review. J Sport Behav 1997;20:54-68.
3. Karageorghis CI, Priest DL. Music in the exercise domain: a review
cess by considering the music’s motivational properties, and synthesis (Part II). Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 2012;5:67-
working with a guiding rationale; 2) controlling and re- 84.

Vol. 57 - No. 7-8 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness 983
not permitted. It is not permitted to remove, cover, overlay, obscure, block, or change any copyright notices or terms of use which the Publisher may post on the Article. It is not permitted to frame or use framing techniques to enclose any trademark, log
means which may allow access to the Article. The use of all or any part of the Article for any Commercial Use is not permitted. The creation of derivative works from the Article is not permitted. The production of reprints for personal or commercial use i
This document is protected by international copyright laws. No additional reproduction is authorized. It is permitted for personal use to download and save only one file and print only one copy of this Article. It is not permitted to make additional copies
(either sporadically or systematically, either printed or electronic) of the Article for any purpose. It is not permitted to distribute the electronic copy of the article through online internet and/or intranet file sharing systems, electronic mailing or any other
©
COPYRIGHT 2017 EDIZIONI MINERVA MEDICA
SMIRMAUL EFFECT OF PRE-TASK MUSIC ON PERFORMANCE

4. Karageorghis CI, Priest DL. Music in the exercise domain: a review 20. Yamamoto T, Ohkuwa T, Itoh H, Kitoh M, Terasawa J, Tsuda T, et
and synthesis (Part I). Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 2012;5:44-66. al. Effects of pre-exercise listening to slow and fast rhythm music on
5. Bishop DT, Wright MJ, Karageorghis CI. Tempo and intensity of pre- supramaximal cycle performance and selected metabolic variables.
task music modulate neural activity during reactive task performance. Arch Physiol Biochem 2003;111:211-4.
Psychol Music 2014;42:714-27. 21. Eliakim M, Meckel Y, Nemet D, Eliakim A. The effect of music dur-
6. Laukka P, Quick L. Emotional and motivational uses of music in ing warm-up on consecutive anaerobic performance in elite adoles-
sports and exercise: A questionnaire study among athletes. Psychol cent volleyball players. Int J Sports Med 2007;28:321-5.
Music 2013;41:198-215. 22. Chtourou H, Jarraya M, Aloui A, Hammouda O, Souissi N. The ef-
7. Terry PC, Dinsdale SL, Karageorghis CI, Lane AM. Use and per- fects of music during warm-up on anaerobic performances of young
ceived effectiveness of pre-competition mood regulation strategies sprinters. Sci Sports 2012;27:e85-8.
among athletes. In: Katsikitis M, editor. Proceedings of the 2006 Joint 23. Jarraya M, Chtourou H, Aloui A, Hammouda O, Chamari K, Cha-
Conference of the APS and the NZPS. Melbourne, VIC: Australian ouachi A, et al. The Effects of Music on High-intensity Short-term
Psychological Society; 2006. p. 420-4. Exercise in Well Trained Athletes. Asian J Sports Med 2012;3:233-8.
8. Terry PC, Karageorghis CI. Psychophysical Effects of Music in Sport 24. Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Hammouda O, Chamari K, Souissi N. Lis-
and Exercise: An Update on Theory, Research and Application. In: tening to music affects diurnal variation in muscle power output. Int J
Katsikitis M, editor. Proceedings of the 2006 Joint Confrence of the Sports Med 2012;33:43-47.
APS and the NZPS. Melbourne, VIC: Australian Psychological Soci- 25. Loizou G, Karageorghis CI. Effects of psychological priming, vid-
ety; 2006. p. 415-9. eo, and music on anaerobic exercise performance. Scand J Med Sci
9. Thiese KE, Huddleston S. The Use of Psychological Skills by Female Sports 2015;25:909-20.
Collegiate Swimmers. J Sport Behav 1999;22:602-10. 26. Ferguson AR, Carbonneau MR, Chambliss C. Effects of positive and
10. Gluch P. The use of music in preparing for sport performance. Con- negative music on performance of a karate drill. Percept Mot Skills
temp Thought 1993;2:33-53. 1994;78:1217-8.
11. Terry PC, Karageorghis CI. Chariots of fire: The role of music in 27. Hall KG, Erickson B. The effects of preparatory arousal on sixty-
sport and exercise. In: Morris T, Terry PC, editors. Sport and exercise meter dash performance. Appl Res Coach Athl Annu 1995;10:70-9.
psychology: The cutting edge. Morgantown, WV: Fitness Informa- 28. Bigliassi M, Dantas JL, Carneiro JG, Smirmaul BPC, Altimari LR.
tionTechnology; 2011. Influence of music and its moments of application on performance
12. Bishop DT, Karageorghis CI, Kinrade NP. Effects of Musically-In- and psychophysiological parameters during a 5km time trial. Rev An-
duced Emotions on Choice Reaction Time Performance. Sport Psy- daluza Med del Deport 2012;5:83-90.
chol 2009;23:1-19. 29. Smirmaul BP, Dos Santos R V, Da Silva Neto L V. Pre-task mu-
13. Becker N, Brett S, Chambliss C, Crowers K, Haring P, Marsh C, et al. sic improves swimming performance. J Sports Med Phys Fitness
Mellow and frenetic antecedent music during athletic performance of 2015;55:1445-51.
children, adults, and seniors. Percept Mot Skills 1994;79:1043-6. 30. Smirmaul BPC, Dantas JL, Nakamura FY, Pereira G. The Psycho-
14. Pain MA, Harwood C, Anderson R. Pre-Competition Imagery and biological model: a new explanation to intensity regulation and
Music: The Impact on Flow and Performance in Competitive Soccer. (in)tolerance in endurance exercise. Brazilian J Phys Educ Sport
Sport Psychol 2011;25:212-32. 2013;22:333-40.
15. Pearce KA. Effects of different types of music on physical strength. 31. Marcora S. Counterpoint: Afferent feedback from fatigued locomo-
Percept Mot Skills 1981;53:351-2. tor muscles is not an important determinant of endurance exercise
16. Karageorghis CI, Drew KM, Terry PC. Effects of pretest stimulative performance. J Appl Physiol 2010;108:454-6; discussion 456-7.
and sedative music on grip strength. Percept Mot Skills 1996;83:1347- 32. Karageorghis CI. The scientific application of music in sport and ex-
52. ercise. In: Lane A, editor. Sport and Exercise Psychology. London:
17. Gaston ET. Dynamic Music Factors in Mood Change. Music Educ J Hodder Education; 2008. p. 109-38.
1951;37:42-4. 33. Karageorghis CI, Terry PC. The BASES Expert Statement on use of
18. Dorney L, Goh EK, Lee C. The impact of music and imagery on music in exercise. J Sports Sci 2012;30:953-6.
physical performance and arousal: Studies of coordination and endur- 34. Karageorghis CI, Terry PC, Lane AM. Development and initial vali-
ance. J Sport Behav 1992;15:21-33. dation of an instrument to assess the motivational qualities of music
19. Crust L. Carry-over effects of music in an isometric muscular endur- in exercise and sport: the Brunel Music Rating Inventory. J Sports Sci
ance task. Percept Mot Skills 2004;98:985-91. 1999;17:713-24.

Conflicts of interest.—The authors certify that there is no conflict of interest with any financial organization regarding the material discussed in the manuscript.
Acknowledgements.—The author would like to thank Warren K. Simpson and Paul D. Gluch for providing specific references.
Article first published online: May 31, 2016. - Manuscript accepted: May 26, 2016. - Manuscript revised: May 18, 2016. - Manuscript received: January 14,
2016.
or other proprietary information of the Publisher.

984 The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness July-August 2017

You might also like