Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 69

New Scientist Essential Guide Human

Society No 14 2022 Kate Douglas


(Editor)
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmeta.com/product/new-scientist-essential-guide-human-society-no-14-2
022-kate-douglas-editor/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

New Scientist Essential Guide No 04 Our Human Story


Kate Douglas (Editor)

https://ebookmeta.com/product/new-scientist-essential-guide-
no-04-our-human-story-kate-douglas-editor/

New Scientist Essential Guide Life on Earth No 11 2022


Michael Marshall (Editor)

https://ebookmeta.com/product/new-scientist-essential-guide-life-
on-earth-no-11-2022-michael-marshall-editor/

New Scientist Essential Guide No 13 The Solar System


Stephen Battersby (Editor)

https://ebookmeta.com/product/new-scientist-essential-guide-
no-13-the-solar-system-stephen-battersby-editor/

New Scientist Essential Guide No 01 The Nature of


Reality Richard Webb (Editor)

https://ebookmeta.com/product/new-scientist-essential-guide-
no-01-the-nature-of-reality-richard-webb-editor/
New Scientist New Scientist Ltd England

https://ebookmeta.com/product/new-scientist-new-scientist-ltd-
england/

Fear No Evil Alex Cross 29 1st Edition James Patterson

https://ebookmeta.com/product/fear-no-evil-alex-cross-29-1st-
edition-james-patterson/

Honor Edgeworth 1st Edition Kate Madeleine Bottomley


Douglas Lochhead

https://ebookmeta.com/product/honor-edgeworth-1st-edition-kate-
madeleine-bottomley-douglas-lochhead/

No One To Help Her 1st Edition Kate Watterson

https://ebookmeta.com/product/no-one-to-help-her-1st-edition-
kate-watterson/

The Woman With No Name 1st Edition Kate Bizos

https://ebookmeta.com/product/the-woman-with-no-name-1st-edition-
kate-bizos/
THE RISE OF CIVILISATION
SOCIETAL NORMS
MORALITY AND RELIGION
POWER AND CONFLICT

ESSENTIAL ECONOMICS AND WORK


THE FUTURE OF SOCIETY

GUIDE№14 AND MORE

HUMAN SOCIETY
HOW EVOLUTION AND PSYCHOLOGY
SHAPED OUR WORLD

EDITED BY

KATE DOUGLAS
NEW
SCIENTIST
ESSENTIAL
GUIDE
HUMAN
H
UMAN society has come a long way in the
mere 6000-odd years since the first cities
were founded and “civilisation” took root.
Our ability to live and cooperate in large

SOCIETY groups beyond kinship boundaries has no


obvious parallel in the animal world. As this 14th New
Scientist Essential Guide makes plain, only strict societal
rules and a near-universal moral code have made it
possible – as well as power structures such as religion
to make us toe the line. Even so, our innate way of
dividing the world into “them” and “us” means
prejudicial thinking and group-on-group
aggression still scar human society today.
This guide aims to take a long view of certain aspects
of why human society works as it does. With such a
complex subject it is necessarily a partial view, but I
hope a thought-provoking one. Readers of this volume
may also be interested in Essential Guide No. 4: Our
Human Story, which charts the earlier evolution of our
species and feeds into the story told here. All titles in
the Essential Guide series can be bought by visiting
shop.newscientist.com; feedback is welcome at
essentialguides@newscientist.com. Kate Douglas

NEW SCIENTIST ESSENTIAL GUIDES SERIES EDITOR Richard Webb ABOUT THE EDITOR
NORTHCLIFFE HOUSE, 2 DERRY STREET,
EDITOR Kate Douglas Kate Douglas is a features editor for New Scientist with broad interests
LONDON, W8 5TT
+44 (0)203 615 6500 DESIGN Craig Mackie across human evolution, psychology and the life sciences
© 2022 NEW SCIENTIST LTD, ENGLAND SUBEDITOR Jon White
NEW SCIENTIST ESSENTIAL GUIDES
PRODUCTION AND APP Joanne Keogh ADDITIONAL CONTRIBUTORS Pragya Agarwal, Anil Ananthaswamy, Philip Ball,
ARE PUBLISHED BY NEW SCIENTIST LTD
ISSN 2634-0151 TECHNICAL DEVELOPMENT (APP) Colin Barras, Nicolas Baumard, Jessica Bond, Michael Bond, Pascal Boyer,
PRINTED IN THE UK BY Amardeep Sian Peter Byrne, Patricia Churchland, Kate Douglas, Robin Dunbar, Jessa Gamble,
PRECISION COLOUR PRINTING LTD AND
PUBLISHER Nina Wright Alison George, Bob Holmes, Rowan Hooper, Joshua Howgego, Dan Jones,
DISTRIBUTED BY MARKETFORCE UK LTD
+44 (0)20 3148 3333 EDITOR-IN-CHIEF Emily Wilson Elizabeth Landau, Graham Lawton, Layal Liverpool, Debora MacKenzie, Jo Marchant,
DISPLAY ADVERTISING +44 (0)20 7611 1291 Carl Miller, Annie Murphy Paul, Sandy Ong, Gina Perry, Timothy Revell, David Robson,
COVER: MIKE_KIEV/ISTOCK displayads@newscientist.com Mark Sheskin, Laura Spinney, Richard Webb, Yvaine Ye, Ed Yong, Emma Young

New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society | 1


CHAPTER 1 CHAPTER 2 CHAPTER 3

THE RISE OF THE HUMAN MORALITY


CIVILISATION FACTOR AND
RELIGION

The bedrock of human civilisation is our The complexity of human society Cooperation between different groups in
ability to cooperate beyond kith and kin. requires norms that govern everything society often means putting our own
Where that came from, and how that from how we structure sexual and narrow advantage to one side. The rules
allowed the first civilisations to emerge, familial relationships to the formulation governing our interactions have evolved
is very much up for debate, however – of friendship networks. Whether we are into a near-universal moral code – one
as is the origin of the diversity we see in aware of them or not, they shape the way that, in more recent times, structures
human societies today. we interact with people from the moment such as religion have come to police.
we meet them.
p. 6 Homo sapiens: The first p. 40 The roots of morality
domesticated species p. 22 INTERVIEW: Joe Henrich p. 43 Everyday evil
p. 9 Hierarchy and the first civilisations How culture shapes our minds p. 45 ESSAY: John H. Evans
p. 11 Monuments to cooperation p. 26 Happy families? Human rights – and wrongs
p. 12 ESSAY: Harvey Whitehouse p.28 Winning friends p. 47 Why we believe in gods
Why religion matters (and influencing people) p. 49 Did Protestantism create science?
p. 16 The origins of cultural diversity p. 29 Seven pillars of friendship p. 50 INTERVIEW: Alain de Botton
p. 31 Signals of engagement Religion for atheists
p. 31 The importance of saying sorry
p.32 Empathy’s dark side
p. 34 INTERVIEW: Steven Pinker
Why rationality rules

2 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


CHAPTER 4 CHAPTER 5 CHAPTER 6

POWER AND ECONOMICS THE FUTURE


CONFLICT AND WORK OF SOCIETY

Questions of who is in charge became As society complexified and diversified, Human society has advanced
ever more central as human society we outsourced the provision of basics immeasurably since the emergence of
became more complex. At the same time, such as food, clothing and shelter to the first cities some 6000 years ago.
our evolved sense of “us” and “them” set others with particular expert skills, But environmental degradation, global
the stage for new conflicts marked by and became workers – and economic inequality and the rise of technologies
prejudice against those we deem to differ animals. But in modern societies, such as artificial intelligence mean we
from us – conflicts that still mark human production, consumption and the also face challenges that could change
society today. ownership of possessions have become the face of society once more.
about far more than just survival.
p. 54 How we choose our leaders p. 82 DISCUSSION:
p. 57 The origins of sexism p. 68 Why we’re bad at economics A fairer, greener future?
p. 59 Dehumanisation, p. 71 The rise of consumer culture p. 85 Tackling global inequality
prejudice and bias p. 74 Human hoarders p. 87 INTERVIEW: Anu Ramaswami
p. 60 DISCUSSION: How to future-proof cities
p. 75 A good place to work
How racism harms lives p. 90 The fourth industrial revolution
p. 78 INTERVIEW: Roger Kneebone
p. 62 Us vs them How to be an expert p. 93 INTERVIEW: Kate Crawford
p. 64 INTERVIEW: Gwen Adshead The challenge of AI
Are we naturally evil? p. 94 Is Western power
on the decline?

New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society | 3


CHAPTER 1

4 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


The bedrock of human civilisation is cooperation. Although it is a
trait that exists elsewhere in the animal world, the complexity of
our cooperative interactions seems unsurpassed, and underlies our
moral sense and our rules-based codes of working.

Where our unique ability to cooperate came from, and how that
allowed the first civilisations to emerge, is very much up for debate,
however – as is the origin of the differences we see between human
cultures today. Understanding the nature of human society means
going back to the beginning.

Chapter 1 | The rise of civilisation | 5


HOMO SAPIENS: THE FIRST
DOMESTICATED SPECIES
A remarkable ability to cooperate underlies VER the past 30,000 years or so,
humans have domesticated all manner
human society. A bold hypothesis suggests that of species for food, hunting, transport,
has one ultimate source – early traits we materials, to control pests and to keep
as pets. But some say that before we
evolved allowed us to tame our wilder sides. domesticated any of them, we first
had to domesticate ourselves. Mooted
by Darwin and even Aristotle, the idea
of human domestication has since
been just that: an idea. But now
genetic studies suggest there is more than a grain
of truth to the idea.
Most domestic animals were tamed by another
species – us. But evolution itself can play the same role.
Dogs, for instance, are thought by some to be partially
self-domesticated. The idea is that some wolves were
naturally bolder and less aggressive. They had an
advantage because they could approach human
settlements and dine on their leftovers. Only later
did we selectively breed them and complete their
domestication. Likewise, it is possible that being less
aggressive and more cooperative was an advantage
for early humans, giving those with these traits a
better chance of surviving and reproducing.
Genetic analyses have shown that many of the
differences between animals such as dogs and wolves
or European cattle and European bison are linked to the
neural crest, a tiny collection of cells in the developing
embryo, which are sent around the body to form a
1971YES/ISTOCK range of tissues. Several known domestic species >

6 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


Chapter 1 | The rise of civilisation | 7
Compared with Neanderthals, modern humans have facial features
that are more similar to those of a domesticated animal

Smaller brain case

Shorter nasal bone or snout

Smaller teeth

Smaller jaw
WOLF DOG HOMO SAPIENS NEANDERTHAL

have varieties of neural crest genes distinct from Nonconformists who refused to change their
those in their wild counterparts. behaviour were executed. Selection accordingly
Now studies of DNA extracted from Homo sapiens favoured the evolution of emotional responses that
and those of our extinct cousins, the Neanderthals, led individuals to feel and display unity with the
have pinpointed those self-same differences. The group. Conformity was vital.
consequences are clear in, for example, the The moral senses of individuals thus evolved to be
morphology of our skulls. Just as the domesticated dog self-protective to a degree not shown by other primates.
has a smaller brain case and jaw and a shorter nose The strongly conformist behaviours produced by the
than the wild wolf, so do we compared with the new tendencies provided a safe passage through life,
Neanderthals (see diagram, above). and they had a second effect as well. By reducing
This suggests there was an episode early in our competition and selfishness, they promoted behaviour
evolution when our species underwent the same sort that benefited the group as a whole.
of domestication as these animals did. Evidence from Several researchers are convinced that self-
the fossil record reveals the process started certainly domestication can also explain the explosion
by 200,000 years ago, and possibly with the first of culture during the Stone Age. The objects
glimmerings of H. sapiens a little more than 300,000 archaeologists have found suggest that it was only
years ago, according to Richard Wrangham at Harvard within the past 100,000 years that jewellery, musical
University. Language-based conspiracy was the key, he instruments and other cultural artefacts became a
argues, because it gave whispering subordinates the common feature of human life, 200,000 years after
power to join forces to kill bullies – presumably, alpha H. sapiens first appeared. “That’s always been a puzzle,”
males, since men tend to be more violent than women. says Steven Churchill at Duke University.
As happens in small-scale, traditional societies today, He and his colleagues have speculated that this
language allowed underdogs to agree on a plan and delayed cultural revolution might have been linked
thereby to make predictably “safe” murders out of to an intense pulse of human self-domestication
confrontations with intended victims that would 100,000 years ago. They argued that our species had
otherwise have been dangerous. Genetic selection the capacity to innovate from the start, but that our
against the alpha males’ propensity for reactive ancestors lacked the social networks for ideas to
aggression was an unforeseen result of eliminating the spread from group to group. Instead, knowledge and
would-be despots. The selection against alpha-male good ideas lived and died in the family group. Genetic
behaviour led to an increasingly calm tenor of life and archaeological evidence suggests population
within social communities of H. sapiens. densities began to rise around 100,000 years ago.
The same ability to perform capital punishment that Until that time, it may well have been beneficial for
led to self-domestication also created the moral senses, humans to be hostile towards strangers, perhaps to
cultural anthropologist Christopher Boehm has prevent others encroaching on their territories.
argued. In the past, to be a nonconformist, to offend But as people began to live more closely together, it
community standards or to gain a reputation for being would have been better to welcome them. Humans
mean were dangerous adventures; to some extent this would have experienced an evolutionary selective
is still true today. Rule breakers threatened the interests pressure to be friendly and cooperative. This suggests
of the elders – the coalition of males holding power – so that an episode of self-domestication was the true
they risked being ostracised as outsiders or sorcerers. bedrock of civilisation. ❚

8 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


Cooperation was essential for civilisation to emerge, but it wasn’t enough. There is a limit to the
number of people we can meaningfully know and cooperate with – a fundamental cognitive limit
that required another key innovation to overcome.

HIERARCHY
AND THE FIRST
CIVILISATIONS
OW many people do you know? It is circle, an individual interacted with one person from a
likely to be at the very most only higher level in the hierarchy, and typically eight people
around 150. That is the number of from lower levels, says Peter Turchin at the University
social interactions that Robin Dunbar of Connecticut. These alliances continued to enlarge
at the University of Oxford has shown and increase in complexity to perform more kinds of
that one individual can keep track of. collective actions.
Evidence for that includes studies of
villages and army units through →-
history, and the average tally of Chapter 4 has more on human-
Facebook friends. To build large-scale power structures-
societies, we had to overcome this fundamental limit
on cooperation. How? For a society to survive, its collective behaviour must be
as complex as the challenges it faces – including
→- competition from neighbours. If one group adopted a
Page 28 has more on the structure of- hierarchical society, its competitors also had to.
modern friendship networks- Hierarchies spread and social complexity grew. Larger
hierarchies not only won more wars, but also fed more
Humanity’s universal answer was the invention of people through economies of scale, which enabled
hierarchy. Several villages allied themselves under a technical and social innovations such as irrigation,
chief; several chiefdoms banded together under a food storage, record-keeping and a unifying religion.
higher chief. To grow, these alliances added more Cities, kingdoms and empires followed.
villages, and if necessary more layers of hierarchy.
Hierarchies meant leaders could coordinate large →-
groups without anyone having to keep personal track How do civilisations fall once risen?-
of more than 150 people. In addition to their immediate Page 94 explores this question- >

Chapter 1 | The rise of civilisation | 9


Covering much of the Middle East, the so-called Fertile urbanised between 6000 and 5000 years ago, with the
Crescent east of the Mediterranean Sea has often been formation of cities such as Harappa, home to tens of
called “the cradle of civilisation” thanks to the thousands of people. Intriguingly, there seems to have
emergence of city-states such as Uruk in ancient been some communication and trade between the
Mesopotamia, which became increasingly urbanised people of Harappa and Mesopotamia. It isn’t yet clear,
from around 6000 years ago. Estimates of Uruk’s however, whether the symbols found on Indus valley
population vary wildly, but, by around 4900 years ago, artefacts constituted a fully-fledged writing system.
it is thought to have housed more than 60,000 people, The Liangzhu culture on the lower Yangtze had much
making it one of the oldest cities in the world. Its in common with these early civilisations. With its
communal works included temples and canals for social elite, skilled craftwork and refined architecture,
irrigation. Uruk’s inhabitants invented the first known it demonstrated the most important characteristics of
form of writing, cuneiform, and their texts include the a state society more than 5000 years ago. Its population
earliest surviving great work of literature, The Epic of of up to 34,500 put it on a scale with Uruk, Memphis
Gilgamesh, about a legendary king of the ancient city. and Harappa, and its communal works would have
At the western end of the Fertile Crescent, another required large-scale social organisation and
civilisation was emerging at about the same time as the management. Liangzhu’s enormous hydraulic system,
Mesopotamian cities. Farming communities in Egypt which allowed its citizens to master their watery
also became increasingly urbanised and, by 5100 years landscape, was so advanced that some consider it the
ago, they had coalesced into a society ruled from the most impressive anywhere in the world at that time.
city of Memphis by the first pharaoh, Narmer. This All these urban cultures shared two features that
“first kingdom” used the waters of the Nile to irrigate set them apart from the small-scale, egalitarian
the surrounding land, had elaborate tombs – although societies that preceded them: an increased density
not yet as ambitious as the famous pyramids – and a of habitation, and evidence of a novel hierarchical
rudimentary writing system based on hieroglyphics. social structure. All? Not quite all. There is one early
The ancient ruins of the city can still be seen near experiment in urban development – in fact, comprising
the modern town of Mit Rahina, just south of the the oldest proto-cities we know of – that doesn’t fit this
Egyptian city of Giza. pattern. Megasites built in eastern Europe from
The urban settlements in Mesopotamia and Egypt 6200 years ago by a culture called the Cucuteni-
were long considered to be the first cities. However, we Trypillia indicate that these people didn’t live in dense
now know that complex societies were developing populations and, furthermore, retained the egalitarian
independently elsewhere, too. The Indus valley in social structure of their forebears, without the
south Asia, for instance, became increasingly hallmarks of social class and hierarchy.
How the story of the Trypillians might alter
the story about the origins of city living and the
Ancient Egypt’s “first kingdom” emergence of civilisation is still very much debated.
was ruled from the city of Memphis But one thing is for sure: hierarchy was an innovation
starting around 5100 years ago that stayed with us. ❚

GARGOLAS/ISTOCK

10 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


MONUMENTS TO
COOPERATION
ARCHAEOLOGIST Carl Lipo first to transport them from nearby
went to Rapa Nui, as Easter Island in quarries. Yet every so often the
the south-eastern Pacific is known workers filled in the enclosures
to its inhabitants, in 2001. Then, the with rubble and built new ones,
prevailing idea was that the famous sometimes even before an
stone heads, or moai, had been enclosure was finished. The
rolled into place using logs, and that apparent disposability of these
the resulting depletion of trees went monuments makes sense if the
on to contribute to the collapse of primary goal was building a team
the island’s human population. rather than a lasting structure.
Lipo, at Binghamton University in If human bonding was the
New York, and fellow archaeologist objective, then you might also
Terry Hunt, now at the University of predict that celebrating a project’s
Hawaii at Manoa, showed something completion was an important part of
different. They proved that moai the process – perhaps even an
could have been “walked” upright incentive to take part in the first
into place by small, cooperating place. A big party would have
bands of people using ropes, with allowed links forged through
no need for trees. In their 2011 book, Easter Island’s massive moai collaborative toil to bear fruit,
The Statues that Walked, they argued could have been walked into place cementing social ties and perhaps
further that statue-making by cooperating bands of people leading to sexual liaisons. The
benefited these people by directing rubble filling the enclosures at
their energy into peaceful these monuments get constructed, Göbekli Tepe suggests feasting
interactions and allowing them to and it’s very different from the took place: it is riddled with
share information and sexual traditional story,” he says. fragments of carbonised bones
partners. Far from causing their If this idea is correct, it explains from aurochs and gazelle.
downfall, when the going got tough, several long-standing puzzles Peter Turchin, who studies
Easter Islanders depended on this connected with ancient monuments. history and cultural evolution at the
cooperation. They only stopped One is why their builders so often University of Connecticut in Storrs,
making statues, Lipo and Hunt destroyed and rebuilt them. A prime sees bottom-up cooperation at work
claimed, precisely because life example of this can be found at the in monuments from the Roman
became easy – in part due to the temple complex of Göbekli Tepe in Colosseum to France’s Gothic
domestication of plants – and it was south-east Turkey, which at more cathedrals to the International
no longer so important that they than 11,000 years old is the earliest Space Station (ISS). In each case, he
work together. known example of monumental says, the project brought together
Likewise, according to Lipo, many architecture. Since excavation groups who hadn’t previously
ancient monuments weren’t the started there in the mid-1990s, worked together, and opened the
product of top-down power play, but archaeologists have uncovered nine door to new forms of cooperation.
of bottom-up cooperation. In other enclosures formed of massive stone As a society grew in scale and
words, they were giant team- pillars carved with pictograms and complexity, so did its builders’
building exercises instigated by the animal-themed reliefs. Given the ambitions. The Great Pyramid at
people who did the work. “We’re size of these pillars – their average Giza took 400,000 people-years to
starting to see that there’s this weight is 30 tonnes – a considerable build. Turchin estimates that the ISS
MIRALEX/ISTOCK

whole other condition under which workforce would have been needed required eight times that.

Chapter 1 | The rise of civilisation | 11


ESSAY

WHY
RELIGION
MATTERS
Its role in human affairs today may be
hotly debated, but what isn’t in question
is religion’s key role in establishing
civilisation, says Harvey Whitehouse

ELIGION has given us algebra and


the Spanish Inquisition, Bach’s
cantatas and pogroms. The debate
over whether religion lifts humanity
higher or brings out our basest
instincts is ancient and, in some ways,
reassuringly insoluble. There are so
many examples on either side. The last
word goes to the most erudite – until
someone more erudite comes along.
Alternatively, we can ask whether religion has helped
societies grow and flourish. Is it, as many believe, a
PROFILE form of social glue that builds cooperation? As it
HARVEY happens, there is surprising agreement about the
WHITEHOUSE moral significance of cooperation. A study involving
60 societies, ranging from small groups to the very
Harvey Whitehouse is largest, found that people everywhere equate “good”
with cooperative behaviours and “bad” with non-
a social anthropologist
cooperative ones. Admittedly, societies differ in the
at the University of kinds of cooperation they value: some are more
Oxford whose research authoritarian, others more egalitarian. Nevertheless,
focuses on the evolution this approach allows us to ask a more tangible question
of social complexity about religion: what role, if any, has it played in
establishing the cooperative behaviours that have
allowed human societies to grow from small hunter-
gatherer groups to vast empires and nation states?
One obvious place to begin is the Axial Age, a period

12 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


Religious rituals such as
processions and burning incense
help bind large communities

society beyond a certain size: free-riders. In smaller


communities, it is relatively easy for peer groups and
local chiefs to catch people who try to live off the fruits
of society while contributing less than their fair share.
In bigger ones, where impersonal transactions are
more commonplace, compliance is harder to police.
Here, the fear that a moralising god is watching and will
punish free-riders – for example, with eternal
damnation – could help do the trick.
THEPALMER/ISTOCK

→-
Chapter 3 has more on religion’s role-
in human morality-
Other researchers, including me, have examined the
role that sacred rituals might have as social glue. For
when many researchers believe civilisation pivoted most of prehistory, humans lived in small groups
towards modernity. Around the middle of the first whose members all knew each other. Today’s small-
millennium BC, the thinking goes, a set of cultural scale societies tend to favour infrequent but traumatic
changes swept the world. Novel notions of equality rituals that promote intense social cohesion – the kind
radically altered the relationship between rulers and that is necessary if people are to risk life and limb
ruled, stabilising societies and allowing them to take a hunting dangerous animals together. An example
leap in size and complexity. Religion is thought to have would be the agonising initiation rites still carried out
played a role. Indeed, the Axial Age concept emerged in the Sepik region of Papua New Guinea, involving
from the observation that a handful of important extensive scarification of the body to resemble the skin
prophets and spiritual leaders – among them Buddha, of a crocodile, a locally revered species.
Confucius and Zoroaster, or Zarathustra – rose to However, with the advent of farming around
prominence in that period, preaching similar 10,000 years ago, such rituals were no longer fit for
moralistic ideologies. purpose. Farming supported larger populations whose
Another popular hypothesis is that cooperation in members didn’t always know each other. They also
complex societies is intimately connected with the weren’t required to risk everything for one another, so
invention of “Big Gods”: deities who demand that their they didn’t require the same levels of social cohesion.
moral code be observed by all, and who have But they did need to feel part of a group obeying the
supernatural powers of surveillance and enforcement. same moral code and system of governance – especially
Most of today’s world religions have these moralising as their society absorbed other ethnic groups through
gods, but they are rare in small-scale societies, where military conquest. New kinds of rituals seem to have
supernatural beings tend to care only whether people provided that shared identity. These were generally
discharge their obligations to the spirit world. painless practices like prayer and meeting in holy
It has been suggested that the establishment of big places that could be performed frequently and
states with large urban populations depended on belief collectively, allowing them to be duplicated across
in such gods, who cared about how everyone, including entire states or empires.
relative strangers, treated each other. Big Gods could A puzzle, however, is that many of these early
also have helped solve a problem that plagues every civilisations also practised the brutal ritual of human >

Chapter 1 | The rise of civilisation | 13


sacrifice. This reached its zenith in the so-called archaic
states that existed between about 3000 BC and 1000
BC, and were among the cruellest and most unequal
societies ever. In some parts of the globe, human
sacrifice persisted until relatively recently. The Inca
religion, for example, had much in common with
today’s world religions: people paid homage to their
gods with frequent and, for the most part, painless
ceremonies. But their rulers had divine status, their
gods weren’t moralising and their rituals included
human sacrifice right up until they were conquered
by the Spanish in the 16th century.
The Axial Age, Big Gods, rituals – how can we test
these ideas? In 2010, Pieter François at the University of
Oxford, Peter Turchin at the University of Connecticut
and I began building a history databank. This project,
named Seshat after the Egyptian goddess of record-
keeping, provides us with the infrastructure and data to
investigate these hypotheses rigorously and on a global
scale. To date, it contains information on more than
400 societies that have existed around the world over An Inca ceremonial
the past 10,000 years. Seshat keeps growing, but we knife used in human
believe it is now mature enough to tackle the role of sacrifice rituals
religion in the rise of civilisation.
I was part of a team that used Seshat to explore the unchecked, for example. And Confucianism didn’t take
Axial Age idea. Advocates of that concept were in for a off in China until after 200 BC. It would appear that
surprise. For a start, many features characteristic of the these moralising ideologies weren’t directly linked
age – including moralistic norms and a legal code – with the rise of sizeable, cooperative civilisations.
arose in places far from the influence of the spiritual So what about Big Gods? Were they required for
leaders, and sometimes long before the middle of the societies to scale up? A large, interdisciplinary team of
first millennium BC. In what is now Turkey, for scholars, including Patrick Savage at Keio University
example, the Hittites adopted a moral code about a in Tokyo, François, Turchin and me, used Seshat to
millennium earlier. What’s more, the various features test this idea. We measured social complexity using
of the Axial Age didn’t come together until much later 51 markers – such as population size and the presence
than most scholars had thought – many thousands of of a bureaucracy or money – and found that in almost
years after the initial emergence of large-scale, complex all of the regions we analysed, moralising gods were
societies. For a long time after Zarathustra preached in adopted much later than expected. Instead of helping
Iran, the divinely sanctioned powers of rulers remained foster cooperation as societies expanded, Big Gods

14 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


“To expanded to a million or
more, societies needed a new
way to build cooperation”
appeared only after a society had passed a threshold in populations just enough to overcome the free-riding
complexity corresponding to a population of around a problem and ensure compliance with new forms of
million people. This happened first in Egypt, where governance. However, in doing so they rendered them
people believed in the supernatural enforcement of vulnerable to a new problem: power-hungry rulers.
order or Maat – personified by a goddess – as early as These were the despotic god-kings who presided over
2800 BC. Egypt had a population of some 1.1 million at archaic states. Granted the divine right to command
the time and was, by all measures, the most vast populations, they exploited it to raise militias and
sophisticated society in the world. The most priesthoods, shoring up their power through practices
parsimonious explanation is that something other we nowadays regard as cruel, such as human sacrifice
than Big Gods allowed societies to grow. and slavery. But archaic states rarely grew beyond
Our study suggests that something was the shift 100,000 people because they, in turn, became
in the nature of rituals from traumatic and rare to internally unstable and therefore less defensible
painless and repetitive. This predated Big Gods in against invasion.
nine of the 12 regions we studied – by 1100 years, on The societies that expanded to a million or more
average – giving rise to the first doctrinal religions, the were those that found a new way to build cooperation –
forerunners of today’s world religions. But there was a Big Gods. They demoted their rulers to the status of
dark side to this development: human sacrifice. mortals, laid the seeds of democracy and the rule of law
A 2016 study based on a historical analysis of more and fostered a more egalitarian distribution of rights
than 100 small-scale societies in Austronesia concluded and obligations. To our modern eyes, “bad” religions
that human sacrifice was used as a form of social gave way to “good” ones. In reality, religions were
control. The elites – chiefs and shamans – did the always “good” in the sense that they promoted
sacrificing, and the lower orders paid the price, so it cooperation. What changed was that societies began
maintained social stability by keeping the masses valuing social justice above deference to authority. In
terrorised and subservient. Seshat includes much other words, they changed their ideas about what
bigger societies, and our analysis indicates that the constituted “good” cooperative behaviours to ones that
practice started to decline when populations exceeded more closely align with our modern agenda.
about 100,000. At this point, when rulers were finding Today, many societies have transferred religion’s
it increasingly difficult to police the masses, human community-building and surveillance roles to secular
sacrifice may have become a destabilising force, institutions. Some of the wealthiest and most peaceful
providing incentive for people to revolt against have atheist majorities. But some of these same
the system. Society began to fracture, making it societies are also facing grave problems as they absorb
vulnerable to conquest. migrants and struggle to contain growing social
Piecing all this together, here is what we think tensions and xenophobia. Time will tell if they are
LEILA MELHADO/ISTOCK

happened. As societies grew by means of agricultural capable of adapting to meet the challenges of
innovation, the infrequent, traumatic rituals that had destabilising influences. But analyses of the kind we are
kept people together as small foraging bands gave way doing could at least reveal which elements of religion
to frequent, painless ones. These early doctrinal have pushed us towards our modern notion of
religions helped unite larger, heterogeneous civilisation, and so might be worth emulating. ❚

Chapter 1 | The rise of civilisation | 15


THE ORIGINS
OF CULTURAL
DIVERSITY
While human societies have universal traits in common, built on our ability to
cooperate, there exist wide differences also. Where those differences come
from is one of the most intriguing questions of our cultural evolution.

S A SPECIES, we possess remarkably easterners and westerners have distinct world views.
little genetic variation, yet we tend to Psychologists have conducted a wealth of
overlook this homogeneity and focus experiments that seem to support popular notions
instead on the differences between that Easterners have a holistic world view, rooted in
groups and individuals. At its darkest, philosophical and religious traditions such as Taoism
this tendency generates xenophobia and Confucianism, while Westerners tend to think more
and racism, but it also has a more analytically, as befits their philosophical heritage of
benign manifestation – a fascination reductionism, utilitarianism and so on. Time and again,
with the exotic. studies seem to support the same basic, contrasting
Nowhere is our love affair with pattern of thought. Westerners appear to perceive the
otherness more romanticised than in our attitudes world in an analytic way, narrowing their focus onto
towards the cultures of East and West. Artists and prominent objects, lumping them into categories and
travellers have long marvelled that on opposite sides examining them through logic. Easterners take a more
of the globe, the world’s most ancient civilisations holistic view: they are more likely to consider an object’s
have developed distinct forms of language, writing, context and analyse it through its changing
art, literature, music, cuisine and fashion. As relationships with its environment.
advances in communications, transport and the
internet shrink the modern world, some of these →-
distinctions are breaking down. But one difference Turn to page 22 for evolutionary biologist Joe-
is getting more attention than ever: the notion that Henrich’s take on the origin of “Western” values-

16 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


The way you see yourself may shape the way you think. If the characteristics associated with collectivism describe you, then your
world view will tend to be holistic. If you fit the description of an individualist, you are likely to think in a more analytical way

Concern for group harmony and that groups get along

HARMONY Wanting to belong to and enjoy being part of groups

BELONG Considering close others as an integral part of self

RELATED Duties and sacrifices being a group member entails

DUTY Self changes according to context or situation

CONTEXT Turning to close others for help with decisions

ADVICE Focus on hierarchy and status issues


COLLECTIVISM
HIERARCHY

COMPETE
INDIVIDUALISM PRIVATE Personal competition and winning

SELF-KNOWING Thoughts and actions private from others

DIRECT COMMUNICATION Knowing oneself, having a strong identity

UNIQUE Clearly articulating one’s wants and needs

INDEPENDENT Focus on one’s unique, idiosyncratic qualities

GOALS Freedom, self-sufficiency, control over one’s life

Striving for one’s own goals, desires and achievements


SOURCE: DAPHNA OYSERMAN

Richard Nisbett at the University of Michigan, Ann However, recently it has become apparent that the East-
Arbor, has suggested that historical cultural factors are West dichotomy is not as clear-cut as this.
the key to understanding these differences. The For a start, the simplistic notion of individualistic
intensive, large-scale agriculture of ancient China Westerners and collectivist easterners is undermined by
involved complex cooperation among farmers and studies designed to assess how people see themselves,
strict hierarchies from emperor down to peasant. The which suggest that there is a continuum of these traits
situation in ancient Greece, often thought of as the across the globe. In terms of individualism, for example,
fount of western culture, was very different: agriculture western Europeans seem to lie about midway between
on such a scale was impossible and most occupations people in the US and those in east Asia.
didn’t require interactions with large numbers of So it isn’t all that surprising, perhaps, that other
people. The Greeks led independent lives and valued studies find that local and current social factors, rather
individualism. That allowed them to focus better on than the broad sweeps of history or geography, tend to
objects and goals in isolation, without being overly shape the way a particular society thinks. For example,
constrained by the needs of others – traits that persist Nisbett’s group compared three communities living in
to this day in Western culture. Turkey’s Black Sea region who share the same language,
Certainly it is appealing to think that a single ethnicity and geography, but have different social lives:
dimension – individualism/collectivism – can account farmers and fishers live in fixed communities and their
for much of the difference in people’s behaviour trades require extensive cooperation, while herders are
around the world. That might explain why many more mobile and independent. He found that the
psychologists have been happy to go along with it. farmers and fishers were more holistic in their >

Chapter 1 | The rise of civilisation | 17


A study of 33 nations quantified how
strict or laissez-faire each culture is.
“Tighter” societies tend to be more
conformist, law-abiding and religious,
while “looser” ones are more creative,
tolerant and disorganised

Tightness score
<4
4-7
7-10
>10
SOURCE: DOI.ORG/10.1126/SCIENCE.1197754

psychology than herders, being more likely to group your value rests on your reputation. As a result, the
objects based on their relationships rather than their impulse to defend one’s reputation is heightened and
categories: they preferred to link gloves with hands individuals are expected to avenge insults themselves
rather than with scarves, for instance. A similar rather than seeking redress in the courts. Honour
mosaic pattern of thought can be found in the East. cultures are also characterised by contrasting gender
“Hokkaido is seen as the Wild West of Japan,” says expectations. For women, the key requirements are to
Nisbett. “The citizens are regarded as cowboys – highly be faithful and protect one’s virtue. Men should be
independent and individualistic – and sure enough, strong, self-reliant and intolerant of disrespect. They
they’re more analytic in their cognitive style than must earn this reputation, and then defend it – even if
mainland Japanese.” that requires violence. What’s more, men who score
Clearly, the dichotomy between holistic eastern and higher on ratings of honour ideology than other men
analytical Western thinking is more blurred than the are more prone to sexually objectify women and
stereotypes suggest. We are all capable of both analytic display stronger beliefs that men should have power
and holistic thought: the minds of east Asians, over women.
Americans or any other group are not wired differently. It is tempting to conclude that these attitudes are
What’s more, the supposed dichotomy is based on rooted in religious fervour. After all, places with much
limited evidence, with China and Japan representing stricter honour cultures, such as the “Bible Belt” in the
the East in most studies and the US and Canada flying US south and Pakistan, are highly religious. However,
the flag for the West. repeated studies both in the US and elsewhere have
In many regions, from southern Asia to Latin found no link between a person’s religiosity and how
America, studies are extremely scarce, and the much they endorse honour-culture attitudes. Instead,
kind of things that cue analytic or holistic thought honour cultures seem to develop wherever there is
may be very different in these neglected societies. severe economic insecurity and a degree of
Honour, for example, is a hugely important issue in lawlessness. Honour culture is a sort of natural
areas that haven’t been studied very thoroughly, like byproduct, because reputation is a way you protect
the Middle East, Africa or Latin America. And what yourself when no one else is coming to your aid.
research there is indicates that it has a big impact Perhaps a better way to understand societies and
on the way people think. their cultural differences is to look at their social
Anthropologists and social scientists distinguish norms. That is the argument made by cultural
between what are sometimes called dignity cultures psychologist Michele Gelfand at the University of
and honour cultures. Dignity cultures value people Maryland in College Park. She and her colleagues
simply by dint of being human, but in honour cultures describe societies with strict, rigorously enforced

18 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


12

10
TIGHTNESS SCORE

8
AVERAGE
6

0
Ma stan
sia

So gap a
Ko e
No rea
Tu ay
Ja y
Ch n
Ge Po ina
y( l
Me st)
o
UK
rm Au aly
y ( ia
Ice st)
ng d
Fra ng
Po ce
Be d
um
ain

Ne ustr S
Ze a
Gr nd
ne e
Ne Br la
rla l
Isr s
Hu ael
Es ry
Uk ia
ne
the azi
an ga
rke

nd
uth or

Ve eec
xic
Sin Indi

w ali
Ho lan

lan
pa

A U

ton
an str

a
rw

rai
Ko

ala
Ea

We
lay

It

Sp

zu
rm rtu

lgi

ng
ki
Pa

Ge

norms as “tight” and those with more laissez-faire revealed how constrained they felt in everyday
cultures as “loose”. They argue that this key difference situations by rating the appropriateness of
underpins all sorts of others, from creativity and 12 behaviours, including eating, crying and flirting,
divorce rates to the synchronicity of public clocks. in 15 contexts ranging from a bank to a funeral to
In fact, the idea of cultural tightness dates back to the movies. There was high agreement among
the 1960s, when anthropologist Pertti Pelto studied people from different walks of life within nations.
21 traditional societies and found big differences in Next, the team calculated national averages for
the rigour of their social norms and how these were tightness and compared these with past threats to each
enforced. The tightest included the Hutterites, while country, as gauged by a battery of measures including
the !Kung people of southern Africa came at the natural disasters, exposure to pathogens, territorial
other end of the scale. Pelto’s insight was to conflict, lack of access to clean water and high
suggest that tightness was connected to ecological population density. Sure enough, there was a
factors such as high population density and correlation. Societies that had faced a high level of
dependence on crops for survival. threat, such as Pakistan and Malaysia, did more to
Gelfand wondered how this might apply to modern regulate social behaviour and punish deviance than
societies. She suspected that tightness is determined by loose countries, which included the Netherlands, Brazil
the level of external threat to which a society was and Australia. Israel, which is also loose, was a notable
exposed historically – whether ecological, such as exception. The UK came out slightly tighter than
earthquakes or scarce natural resources, or human- average, and the US looser.
made, such as war. “Tightness is about the need for But it doesn’t end there. Gelfand and her colleagues
coordination,” she says. “The idea is that if you are found that the degree of tightness was reflected in all
chronically faced with these kinds of threats, you sorts of societal institutions and practices – even after
develop strong rules in order to coordinate for survival.” taking national wealth into consideration. Tight
To test the idea, Gelfand teamed up with colleagues societies tend to be more autocratic, with greater
from 43 institutions around the world, and compared media censorship and fewer collective actions such as
33 nations in a study published in 2011. First they asked demonstrations. They are also more conformist and
nearly 7000 people from diverse backgrounds to shed religious, and have more police, lower crime and
light on the tightness of their national culture by rating divorce rates, and cleaner public spaces. “Tightness
their agreement with statements such as: “There are brings with it a lot of order and social control,” says
many social norms that people are supposed to abide Gelfand. “Even stock markets are more synchronised.”
by in this country” and “People in this country almost Loose societies tend to be more disorganised, but also
always comply with social norms”. The volunteers also more creative, innovative and tolerant of diversity. ❚

Chapter 1 | The rise of civilisation | 19


CHAPTER 2

20 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


Human society requires rules. A huge complexity of relationships
underlies how we live together, and the only way we can manage
them is by establishing – and enforcing – norms that everyone
understands and, to greater or lesser extents, adheres to.

Those norms govern everything from how we structure sexual


and familial relationships to the formulation of friendship
networks. Whether we are aware of them or not, they shape the
way we interact with people from the moment we meet them.

Taken together, these norms add up to a culture – and the culture


we live in has surprising effects on how we think and act.

Chapter 2 | The human factor | 21


INTERVIEW

HOW CULTURE
SHAPES OUR MINDS
The norms of the society we live in have a You argue that kinship systems are key to the ways in which
different cultures develop. What do you mean by that?
huge influence on our psychology, motivations Kinship systems are collections of norms that define
and decision making, says Joe Henrich how we should behave in various contexts. They were
likely the first human social institutions to emerge
because they are built on our evolved psychology. The
institution of marriage, for example, taps into our
species’ pair-bonding psychology, and notions of
extended kin groups play on a core kinship psychology
for helping and caring for our children, siblings and
other close relatives.
The social norms that make up kinship systems
structure the world you are born into. They shape
who you can marry, what you can inherit and own,
who you form alliances with, where you live and
what kind of economic activities you engage in. As
PROFILE we grow up among the norms and institutions of
JOE our society, we develop psychological adaptations
HENRICH to navigate this social world.
In most agricultural societies, people have lived
Joe Henrich is an enmeshed in kin-based institutions within tribal
groups or networks. Inheritance and post-marital
evolutionary biologist at
residence often followed either the male or female
Harvard University and
PREVIOUS PAGE: IMAGE SOURCE/ISTOCK

line – but not both – so people often lived in extended


author of The WEIRDest unilineal households, and wives or husbands moved
RIGHT: CHAMELEONSEYE/ISTOCK

People in the World: How the to live with their spouses’ kinfolk. Many kinship units
West became psychologically collectively owned or controlled territory, and kin-
peculiar and particularly based organisations provided members with
protection, insurance and security, caring for sick,
prosperous
injured and poor members as well as the elderly.
Arranged marriages with relatives such as cousins >

22 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


Chapter 2 | The human factor | 23
were customary, and polygynous marriages were
common for high-status men. These intensive kin
networks nurture a collectivist mindset with greater
conformity, obedience to authority, nepotism and
in-group loyalty.

You have also argued that the social changes ushered in by the
Western Catholic church helped establish a unique psychology
among people you dub WEIRD - Western, Educated,
Industrialised, Rich and Democratic. How?
The Western church introduced prohibitions on
marriage to blood relatives that were extended to
include distant relatives, eventually up to sixth
cousins, which broke down ties between families,
tribes and clans. It prohibited polygamous marriage

ROCIO MONTOYA
and discouraged the adoption of children so that
some lineages simply died out because they had
no heirs. The church also encouraged, and
sometimes required, newly married couples to
set up independent households, and promoted
the individual ownership of property. with personality, the self and the cultivation of
Instead of being born into a world where you inherit personal attributes; they are more individualistic and
most of your social relationships, where everything is less loyal to their group; and they are more likely to
about social relationships and there is strong in-group judge the behaviour of others as reflecting some
loyalty, obedience and conformity, now you have to enduring disposition rather than temporary
find and develop your own mutually beneficial situational factors.
relationships. And when you are deciding which
towns, guilds or other voluntary associations to join – So is it the West versus the rest?
which will be your new safety net, rather than your kin It is important not to set up a dichotomy between the
network – you are looking for people that share your WEIRD and non-WEIRD. WEIRDness is a multi-
interests, beliefs and so on. This focuses attention on dimensional continuum, and there is a lot of variation
people’s underlying personalities, traits and even within western Europe. We took data from the
dispositions, rather than their pre-existing World Values Survey and, using techniques from
relationship to you. Your success in the world is now population genetics, analysed the cultural distance of
tied to cultivating your attributes, making yourself various populations from the US, the weirdest of
appealing to others because you are going to do WEIRD countries. This WEIRD scale shows New
business together or get married. Englanders as the WEIRDest population in the world
and substantially different to populations in the
What is the psychology of these WEIRD people like? Middle East and Africa at the other end. Interestingly,
WEIRD people tend to show greater trust in strangers although there is a huge body of research in social
and fairness towards anonymous others; think more psychology setting up an East-West dichotomy, it turns
analytically rather than holistically; make more use of out that the typical subjects studied in Japan or China
intentions in moral judgements; are more concerned are kind of in the middle of the WEIRD spectrum.

24 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


You believe WEIRDness also helps explain how the West articles, doesn’t represent the psychology of Homo
became “particularly prosperous”. What is the link? sapiens at all. Perhaps even more concerning is how
A WEIRDer and more individualistic psychology this bias hampers our efforts to understand the
provides fertile ground for the development of formal origins and nature of psychological processes
institutions and notions of individual rights and and brain development. Much of what looks like
equality before the law that would be hard to conceive reliably developing features of minds, with clear
of in a world of clans or kindreds. As people in the West developmental trajectories over childhood, turn
moved away from tight kinship networks towards out to be the result of cultural products, like the
voluntary associations of strangers in the form of institutions, values, technologies or languages
labour unions, guilds, monasteries, universities and individuals confront and must learn, internalise
businesses, they adjusted psychologically to be more and navigate to make their way in the world.
trusting of people outside their kin group and also Then there is the applied side of the WEIRD
developed contract law to buttress voluntary people problem. If people in different places are
associations. This happened much earlier than in psychologically different, then the same forms
places such as China. The WEIRD mind is also of government, social policies and economic
particularly patient, as documented in many studies, programmes will often have very different impacts
which – combined with trust and an individualistic and results. This has often been ignored, as WEIRD
drive to set yourself apart – helps drive innovation in governing institutions and economic policies have
technology and economic activities. This eventually been transplanted, often word-for-word, into
launched the Industrial Revolution. countries and communities around the world. I
suspect that some of the failure of well-intentioned
What are the downsides to WEIRDness? efforts to generate economic growth or improved
In societies where there is a strong sense of kinship, health conditions result from failures to account for
like Fiji where I have done fieldwork, there is a sense of differences in people’s cultural psychology.
security, community, oneness – a kind of comfort that
comes from the warm embrace of knowing you are at Is the world becoming WEIRDer?
the centre of a tight web of relations who will always With increasing urbanisation and globalisation
have your back. They aren’t tied to you because you are there is a trend towards smaller families and
a convenient contact or are currently smart or WEIRDer ways of thinking. Even something as
successful, they are tied to you in a deep way and they simple as the spread of Western-style schools is
will be tied to your children. This is a snug, secure, going to push people towards more analytic
happy feeling. WEIRDness undermines this feeling. thinking. So a loss of psychological and cultural
People living in tribal or clan-based societies also variation is occurring. But I think we are going
tend to see themselves as links in a chain connecting to see new ways of organising communities and
past to future, creating a sense of continuity that gives structuring the social world and people’s
people a real sense of meaning and security. Then you relationships. So, I don’t think we have to worry
get Westerners who are like “I’m an individual ape on a that the institutions that spread out of Europe over
pale blue dot in the middle of a giant black space”. recent centuries are going to crush the world’s
psychological and social differences. As people
What are the consequences of psychology’s bias reinterpret what they learned from other societies
towards WEIRD subjects? and synthesise their own way of doing things, the
It means that the picture of “human psychology” world will continue to blend and fragment in a
portrayed in the textbooks, and still in many journal mosaic of cultural and psychological diversity. ❚

Chapter 2 | The human factor | 25


HAPPY
FAMILIES?
Nothing divides human societies – or indeed ND they lived happily
ever after.” The lifelong
individual opinions – quite like our attitudes commitment of two
towards sexual and familial relationships. people to one another
may be the fairy-tale
But the cultural norms in these areas often ending, and has certainly
have relatively recent origins, and examining developed into an ideal
of Western society. Yet
them from a purely scientific standpoint throughout our early
often undercuts their validity. history, polygyny, or one
male with several females, was routine. One idea for
how monogamy came to dominate is that as we evolved
larger brains, keeping babies alive required more effort
and food. The children of men who were spread across
too many families were less likely to survive.
Indeed, a recent analysis found that, from
hunter-gatherers to industrial societies, the greater
the father’s investment, the more monogamous the
society. In turn, monogamy helped social stability:
if a few men monopolise all the women, that leaves a
lot of disgruntled bystanders. And, more recently,
religion also played a role in making monogamy
a Western norm.
But sexual relationships aren’t as rigid today as
they once were. The erosion of religious values, the
development of hormonal contraception and the
rupture of taboos around extra-marital sex and divorce
mean that rather than having one sexual partner for
life, many people are serial monogamists, moving
from one long-term relationship to the next.
Even with less pressure to make lifelong
commitments, we are pretty bad at staying true.

26 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


ALEKSANDARNAKIC/ISTOCK

Children raised by two female


parents are as well-adjusted as
those with heterosexual parents

In a UK poll, 1 in 5 people admitted to cheating on a to raise well-adjusted children as a heterosexual


partner. Another study found that up to a third of couple is. Although not as extensive, studies on gay
people who are married or cohabiting stray. Perhaps father families come to the same conclusion. In the US,
that’s why some have abandoned the ideal altogether. several large studies comparing lesbian, gay and
A 2016 survey found that 20 per cent of single people heterosexual families have found no differences in
in the US have had “consensual non-monogamous” parental warmth, child behaviour, the emotional
relationships, where people have multiple sexual problems experienced by children or their
partners, but everyone is in the loop. psychosocial adjustment, including anxiety,
For some people, these relationships work just fine. depression and self-esteem.
When researchers compared people in traditional It is a similar story with single-parent families. The
monogamous and open relationships, they found Millennium Cohort Study in the UK has been following
no significant differences in reported relationship 18,000 children born at the turn of the millennium,
satisfaction, commitment or passionate love. What’s including some in single-parent families. After
more, those in open relationships reported less accounting for factors like socio-economic status and
jealousy and higher levels of trust. Where devout the parent’s mental health, it has found no difference in
monogamy is expected, it’s no surprise that the frequency of emotional problems in children
infidelity spurs negative feelings. across family types. Another study assessed 35 stable
But norms change, and just as sexual relationships single-mother families where there was no divorce or
have become more fluid in some societies, so too changing partners. They found that children described
have family structures. In Western and many other a close emotional bond with their mother – open
societies, conventional wisdom long held that children communication, shared activities and trust.
were best nurtured in families with two parents – one Relationships within a family aren’t the only
male, one female – who were both genetically related to determinants of a child’s well-being, however. In
the child. Yet, in the US and UK, married, heterosexual one study of 117 Australian gay and lesbian families,
couples with biologically related children now form a more than two-thirds of parents reported that their
minority of families. Results from the first longitudinal high-school-age children had felt isolated or different
studies following the fates of children in non- because of their parent’s sexual orientation. Nearly
traditional family structures are now highlighting one in five had experienced discrimination by a
what is important for a child’s development. The teacher. Stigmatisation wasn’t universal; 41 per cent
results are clear-cut. of parents reported no problems, and the children
The longest-running studies, following lesbian themselves said their classmates were more
families, show that two female parents are just as likely confused than mean-spirited. ❚

Chapter 2 | The human factor | 27


WINNING FRIENDS
(AND INFLUENCING
PEOPLE)
The cooperative nature of human society creates LTHOUGH humans are a highly social
species, juggling relationships isn’t
networks of supporting social relations beyond easy. Like other primates, the size of
mere family. These friendship networks are our social network is constrained by
brain size, and requires structure to
created and maintained in surprisingly uniform organise. The typical social circle of
ways – and are absolutely crucial not just to our 150 people is made up of a series of
layers, each containing a well-defined
practical, but also to our mental, well-being. number of people and associated with
specific frequencies of contact, levels
of emotional closeness and willingness to provide help
(see diagram, above right).
In fact, our social world consists of two quite distinct
sets of people: friends and family. We tend to give
preference to the latter. With our social networks
limited in size, we first slot in family members and then
set about filling any spare places with unrelated
friends. As a result, people who come from large
families tend to have fewer friends.
People spend around 20 per cent of their waking
hours, on average, on social interactions. That is about
3.5 hours a day talking, eating and sitting with others in
a social context. This may seem like a lot, but distributed
evenly among your 150 friends and family, it works out
at just 1 minute and 45 seconds per person per day.
Of course, that isn’t what we do. Around 40 per cent
of this social time is devoted to the five people in our
innermost social circle, the support clique, with
another 20 per cent given to the 10 additional people in

28 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


We can only manage a maximum of 150 people in our
social circle, and they fall into layers depending on their
emotional closeness (all figures are cumulative)

Friends 150 the next layer, the sympathy group. That’s


Dunbar’s number about 17.5 minutes and 4.5 minutes per person,
respectively. The remaining 135 people in the two
Good friends 50 outer rings of our social circles get an average of
just 37 seconds a day each.
Sympathy group 15 These are general trends, though. In fact, each person
has a unique “social fingerprint” – an idiosyncratic way
in which they allocate their social effort. This pattern is
Support clique 5
Me quite impervious to who is in your friendship circle at
any given time. It does, however, reveal quite a lot
about your own identity. How often you contact each of
your friends probably reflects aspects of your
personality, such as extroversion, neuroticism and
conscientiousness. Other factors influencing your
social fingerprint are whether you are male or female
and whether you are an early riser or a night owl.
What is most unexpected, however, is the durability
of a person’s social fingerprint in the face of change. It
is as though exactly who our friends are doesn’t really
matter, as long as we have friends. Of course, we opt for
SEVEN PILLARS people who are as congenial as possible, but, provided
these boxes are ticked, more or less anyone will do.
OF FRIENDSHIP That may sound opportunistic or even callous, but it
makes sense. Friendship isn’t just for fun; it has huge
Our friends tend to be surprisingly like us, and there benefits for our mental and physical well-being. In a
are certain personal characteristics that predict how changing world, our approach to making and
close a friendship is likely to be: maintaining friends needs to be both flexible and
stable so that we can optimise those benefits.
1. You speak the same language or, The key is building social capital. For sociologists and
better still, dialect psychologists, this includes emotional support,
2. You grew up in the same area important information learned through the grapevine
or practical help, such as a lift to the hospital or
3. You have the same educational and cooperation at work. Having high social capital isn’t
career experiences just a matter of being popular and well-liked, though.
4. You pursue the same hobbies and interests As well as having a dense web of connections that
5. You see eye to eye on moral, religious includes close friends and more distant acquaintances,
and political matters people with more social capital tend to be more
engaged in building their community.
6. You share a sense of humour A wealth of studies has confirmed that social capital
7. You have the same taste in music makes a huge difference to our quality of life. People
with high social capital may both perform better at
work and find it easier to land a new job, for instance, >

Chapter 2 | The human factor | 29


thanks to the greater possibility of constructive risk of everything from heart attacks and cancer to
collaborations. Social capital can also soothe our dementia, depression and death, whereas people who
stresses and help us live more healthily, leading to are satisfied with their social lives sleep better, age
a lower risk of mental illness and physical disease, more slowly and respond better to vaccines. The effect
and a longer lifespan. is so strong that curing loneliness is as good for your
Even our “weak ties” have big effects. These are vague health as giving up smoking, according to John
acquaintances and fleeting interactions, say with a Cacioppo of the University of Chicago, who has spent
barista or the distant colleague queuing next to us at his career studying the effects of social isolation.
the coffee machine. People have an average of between This is partly because people who are lonely often
11 and 16 of these interactions on a typical day. Their don’t look after themselves well, but there are direct
importance to our well-being and work success physiological mechanisms too – related to, but not
shouldn’t be underestimated. And even a small effort identical to, the effects of stress. Cacioppo has found
to build on those interactions can pay great dividends. that in lonely people, genes involved in cortisol
When participants were encouraged to make small talk signalling and the inflammatory response were up-
to a stranger, for example, they reported a 17 per cent regulated, and that immune cells important in fighting
increase in a measure of happiness. The optimal ratio bacteria were more active, too. He suggests that our
of strong to weak ties is about 50:50. The results fit bodies may have evolved so that in situations of
with historical analyses of scientists’ and artists’ perceived social isolation, they trigger branches of the
networks, finding that the most productive immune system involved in wound healing and
collaborations are often forged between people bacterial infection. An isolated person would be at
of different experiences and backgrounds. greater risk of physical trauma, whereas being in a
Some benefits of social capital come through non- group might favour the immune responses necessary
verbal communication, such as physical touch. Various for fighting viruses, which spread easily between
studies have found that non-sexual physical touch – people in close contact.
rubbing someone’s arm if they are sad, say – triggers Crucially, these differences relate most strongly to
profound neurological and physiological changes, how lonely people believe themselves to be, rather than
including the release of endorphins. These painkilling to the actual size of their social network. Lonely people
compounds can produce a natural high that helps become overly sensitive to social threats and come to
create a sense of bonhomie and goodwill. Social touch see others as potentially dangerous. Tackling this
also appears to buffer our responses to stress, reducing attitude reduced loneliness more effectively than
the release of the hormone cortisol and calming our giving people more opportunities for interaction, or
heart rate following an unpleasant experience like teaching social skills. If you feel satisfied with your
public speaking. social life, whether you have one or two close friends or
The flip side of this is that being lonely increases the quite a few, there is nothing to worry about. ❚

30 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


SIGNALS OF
ENGAGEMENT
The paramount nature of trust and
cooperation in human society generates THE IMPORTANCE
complex rules of engagement. We might not OF SAYING SORRY
be aware of them, but they come into play
from the moment we meet someone. Sensitivity to the feelings of others in our social group
is key to good relations. But we don’t always get our
interactions right, of course. It can often feel very hard
to apologise, even if we know the relationship will
benefit. There is a perception that apologising
UR greetings are imbued with deep weakens your authority, lowers your self-esteem
symbolism and meaning. Any and damages your image in the workplace. And
greetings that involve bodily contact research has also shown that people make
may offer us a way to pick up chemical “forecasting” errors about the potentially negative
cues. Although the existence of human effects of apologising - they wrongly expect less
pheromones is controversial, research benefit and more costs. Indeed, one study found that
suggests that we may be able to assess when people refused to apologise for something they
someone’s physical fitness and fertility did that upset someone else, they reported feeling
from compounds in their saliva – a more powerful and felt that they’d stuck to their
possible rationale for the strange values more. However, when people did apologise,
phenomenon of romantic kissing. What’s more, there they got the same benefits.
is evidence that body odour can communicate So apologising can bring the same psychological
someone’s emotional state and even their sexual benefits as outright refusal to do so. There could
arousal. Our greetings can allow us to sample this also be material benefits to saying sorry; it can make
aroma without overtly sniffing someone’s body. you appear more approachable and trustworthy to
Our tactile greetings also allow us to assess strangers, according to work by Alison Wood Brooks
someone’s character and to establish our trust in at Harvard Business School. The most effective
them. One study found that the strength and
TAPHOUSE_STUDIOS/ISTOCK

apologies contained six elements: an expression


duration of a handshake offers a fairly accurate of regret, an explanation of what went wrong and
prediction of personality traits including extroversion, an acknowledgment of responsibility, followed by
neuroticism and open-mindedness. Another found a declaration of repentance, an offer of repair and
that students engaged in a simulated real-estate a request for forgiveness.
negotiation were more honest about the quality of >

Chapter 2 | The human factor | 31


the property if they had been encouraged to
shake hands before the task.
Then there are the social messages we convey with
our facial expressions. The orthodox view holds that
there is a group of basic emotions – at least six, but
perhaps many more – that all humans display on
their faces in fundamentally the same way. This means
that other people can reliably read your emotional
state from your face. It is an appealing idea that has
influenced everything from educational practices and
behavioural-learning programmes for children with
autism to emotion-detecting software algorithms.
But now it is being challenged. Some dissenters believe
that facial “expressions” aren’t reliable guides to our
emotions at all, but tools that we wield – usually
unconsciously – to get what we want from others.
In this view, the supposed prototypical
expressions of emotions aren’t necessarily universal –
culture influences how we perceive them – and they
take on new meanings. A smile is a signal to work
together, bond or be friends. A pout is designed to
garner care or protection rather than to indicate
sadness. Scowling, the supposed expression of
anger, may be used to trigger another person to
submit. A gasping face signals submission, not fear
(in the West at least), and so could deflect an attack.
Nose scrunching, traditionally associated with
disgust, is reconceived as a rejection of the way
a social interaction is playing out.
If this is correct, the implications for our social
interactions are enormous. If we are misinterpreting
what facial movements mean, this surely undermines
our ability to read other people, especially people
from other cultures. We aren’t just missing a trick:
this could have some serious implications. Our
assumptions about facial expressions influence
everything from how we diagnose and manage
some conditions, such as autism, to policy decisions,

32 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


“A handshake allows us to
assess someone’s character
and trustworthiness ”
national security protocols and legal judgements.
Human laughter has also evolved to be a potent and
flexible social tool. According to Adrienne Wood at the
University of Virginia it serves three main purposes.
The first is reward: when we laugh together, it shows
appreciation of a particular behaviour and reinforces
the interaction, so that we are more likely to act in the
same way in the future. Spontaneous laughter triggers
the release of opioids, which is probably what creates
these rewarding feelings.
Laughter’s second function is to signal connection.
These affiliation laughs tend to be voluntary (or “fake”)
and help to smooth over tension and embarrassment
rather than reinforcing a particular behaviour. If you
have said something potentially hurtful, for example, a
SUTEISHI/ISTOCK

polite chuckle might help to reassure someone that it


was just playful teasing. The third purpose of laughter
is to signal dominance – like when your boss laughs
dismissively at your outlandish idea. Whereas a direct
challenge, such as a cutting put-down, might trigger
Even though we know when a aggression, laughter indicates disapproval in a
laugh is fake, laughter can make more subtle way. “It maintains a facade of
someone seem more likeable social harmony,” says Wood.
The idea that laughter is the best medicine may be
something of a cliché, yet there is some evidence for
its health benefits. A big belly laugh can exercise the
heart, for example, and it works some of the trunk
muscles as hard as traditional crunches. What’s more,
the importance of laughter in our social lives means
we can use it to boost our friendships or romantic
relationships. A study across 21 societies revealed that,
in general, people are able to tell the difference between
fake and authentic laughs – but further experiments
suggest that both kinds can increase someone’s
likeability. Another study found that people
who watched a funny film together tended to
open up afterwards, disclosing more personal
information to each other. ❚

Chapter 2 | The human factor | 33


INTERVIEW

WHY RATIONALITY RULES


As fake news and conspiracy theories matter: calculating probability, distinguishing
correlation from causation, Bayesian reasoning,
abound, it can seem societies are falling statistical decision theory. Those come less naturally to
apart as a result of unfounded biases. us. And when it comes to issues that are larger than our
day-to-day physical existence, people don’t necessarily
But in fact we remain a supremely hew to the mindset that ideas should be evaluated as to
rational species, says Steven Pinker whether they’re true or false.

You also claim that some seeming irrationality can be


understood as the rational pursuit of goals. How so?
What do you mean by rationality? Rationality always has to be defined with respect to a
I define it as the use of knowledge to attain goals. There goal. What are you deploying your thought processes
is not one single tool of rationality – it depends what to attain? The goals sometimes can be dubious, but you
you’re after. If you’re seeking to derive new true can be extremely methodical at attaining them. I cite
statements from existing ones, then logic is your tool. the defenders of Donald Trump against accusations of
If you want to assess your degree of belief in a irrationality, who will say: Well, he got to be president,
hypothesis based on evidence, then Bayesian didn’t he? If the goal is glorifying Donald Trump,
reasoning. If you want to figure out what’s the rational rallying his supporters and gaining the levers of power,
thing to do when the outcome depends on what other he was quite a genius at it. From the point of view of his
rational people do, game theory. own rationality, there was a certain cunning.

Those tools don’t seem to come naturally to people, yet you But surely the current “pandemic of poppycock”, as
reject the idea that human cognition is a heffalump trap of you call it, is something new?
biases and delusions that are a legacy of our evolution. Why? Conspiracy theories are probably as old as human
Yeah, I don’t think it’s quite right. Although there’s no groups. Paranormal woo isn’t new. Neither is fake news.
question we do have outbursts of irrationality – and These are maybe the default mode of our species. For
they are all too plentiful – I’m not ready to write off our most of human history, it was hard to tell what was true
species as irrational. We can all be rational when it or false. What is the origin of fortune and misfortune?
comes to our immediate surroundings and outcomes What is the origin of the universe? What actually
that affect our lives. And if you’re upset about some happens behind closed doors in palaces and halls of
outbursts of irrationality, don’t blame your hunter- power? You can’t find out. But there are some beliefs
gatherer heritage. The San people of the Kalahari desert that will rally your coalition together – that are
deploy rationality to engage in pursuit hunting, where uplifting, that are morally edifying, that are
they’ve got to figure out where the antelope may have entertaining – and those stories for most of our history
run based on some fragmentary tracks on the ground. were as close as we could get to the truth, and they
They engage in some pretty sophisticated inference. served as a substitute for the truth.
They wouldn’t survive if they didn’t. What’s unusual now is that we have a lot of means to
All of us command some aspect of rationality. In our answer questions that formerly were just cosmic
everyday lives we package it with subject-matter mysteries. Before that, it was a matter of conjecture.
knowledge in particular areas – bringing up the kids, And a good story was the best we could do. We carry
holding down a job, getting food in the fridge. What we over that mindset when it comes to the cosmic, the
don’t wield are tools that can be applied to any subject counterfactual, the metaphysical, the highly politicised.

34 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


You describe key mechanisms through which people form
irrational beliefs – the three M’s of motivated reasoning,
myside bias and mythological belief. Can you unpack these?
Motivated reasoning is a phenomenon where we
direct our reasoning toward something that we want
to believe in the first place. There is a saying from the
[late] journalist Upton Sinclair that it’s very hard to get
a man to understand something when his livelihood
depends on not understanding it. In the lab, we see this
manifested when you give people a logical syllogism
[two statements with a logical conclusion] and ask
them if the conclusion follows from the premises. If the
conclusion is something that they want to be true, they
are apt to ratify an invalid syllogism – and vice versa if it
is a conclusion that they don’t want to be true.
JENNIE EDWARDS

And myside bias?


Motivated reasoning can be in the service not just of a
goal that favours the individual, but often the larger
coalition that he or she belongs to, in which case it’s called
the myside bias. Namely, you direct your reasoning to
end up with a conclusion that is already a belief in your
team, your coalition, your party. It’s among the most
powerful of the many cognitive biases that have been
documented by cognitive psychology. It afflicts the
PROFILE [political] left and the right. Being smart does not make
STEVEN you immune to it. And it’s rather hard to unlearn.
There is a variety of ways in which we comfort
PINKER ourselves in thinking that beliefs of our side are valid
and wise. We muster our ingenuity, we take advantage
Steven Pinker is a of ambiguities in the evidence, we feed ourselves
psychologist at Harvard evidence that supports our position and try to ignore
University and author of sources that might contradict our preferred beliefs. We
books including The Better think more like lawyers than scientists.
Angels of Our Nature: Why
What are some real-world manifestations of myside bias?
violence has declined and With politicised issues in science such as
Rationality: What it is, why it anthropogenic climate change, scientists are often
seems scarce, why it matters surprised that there is so much denial. They sometimes
attribute it to scientific ignorance or illiteracy. But that
is a less-than-rational belief because it’s not based on
empirical studies of why people deny climate change. >

Chapter 2 | The human factor | 35


What those studies show is that the deniers are actually least to take them out of the realm of evidence, but
no more ignorant of science than the believers. In fact, the boundary between the real and the mythical can be
a lot of people who endorse the scientific consensus are changed. The origin of fortune and misfortune may
really out to lunch when it comes to the science of once have been attributed to fate, but we now consider
climate change. They think it has something to do with it an empirical question. We want to know what gives
the ozone hole, toxic waste dumps, plastic straws in the you Alzheimer’s. It’s not divine retribution.
ocean. What does predict people’s belief in climate I think the general tendency since the Enlightenment
change is their politics. The farther you are to the right, has been to try to bite off chunks of the mythology zone
the more denial there is. for the reality zone. I say the more the better, and in
That’s a case in which the scientifically respectable particular areas, we can try to persuade people that,
conclusion is aligned with the left. But there are also no, you can’t just believe anything you want. There
cases where the left is out of touch with the scientific really is a fact of the matter.
facts. My claim that left and right are equally biased is
not just an attempt to be even-handed. Research on the Can that boundary between reality and mythology shift in
myside bias shows that both the left and the right are real time, say with something like covid-19 that starts as
susceptible. A given set of data – say on the efficacy of an abstract threat, but then becomes horribly real?
gun control – will be seen to support or not support a You would think that vaccine hesitancy would crumble
position depending on whether the reasoner belongs in the face of covid. It has not, although it has declined.
to a side that believes it in the first place. What I suspect happens is that with any mythological
belief, there are the true believers who will go to their
And then we get to the third and most potent M, graves believing, no matter how high the evidence
which is mythological belief. piles up. But there are always some who are more
I think this a powerful explanation for why people open to the evidence.
apparently believe so much nonsense. There are real
beliefs like “I believe there is a beer in the fridge”. But Yet it seems that the mythological zone is expanding right now,
then there’s a whole family of beliefs that are more like at least in Western democracies. Is it?
stories that capture a deeper truth: what our enemies It’s all too easy to come to a conclusion based on our own
are capable of and how dreadful they are; how noble availability biases, and on an understanding of the world
our side is, how wise and pure and good. Whether these from anecdotes, which is basically what journalism
things are true is almost beside the point. These are consists of. Unfortunately, we don’t have the good
mythological beliefs. Some of our national founding evidence over an extended period that would settle it.
myths may fall into that category. Some religious I cite a study that looked for conspiratorial content in
beliefs too, and believers are sometimes offended by letters to the editors of major American newspapers over
the idea that they should be subject to empirical a span of more than a century and found no increase.
scrutiny. For them, belief in God is a kind of belief you The data I found on belief in paranormal phenomena
hold for its moral benefits, not for its factual accuracy. among Americans – astrology, crystal power, haunted
It’s a different kind of belief. houses – is pretty much flat over 50 years, too.
I quote Bertrand Russell, who said it is undesirable
to believe a proposition when there are no grounds So humanity isn’t losing its mind today any more
whatsoever for believing it is true. And what I note is than it has in the past?
that this is at odds with the way that most people think. No. But we are squandering some of the tools that
It’s a product of the Enlightenment that we think that could make us more rational if they were more widely
every question ought to be put in the realm of reality applied. It’s not that people are saying more unfounded
and tested for its literal veracity. or outlandish things, but we’re more cognisant of the
higher standards that we ought to apply, and so the
That seems to imply that some beliefs are beyond criticism lapses are all the more salient to us. In terms of the
and impervious to evidence. Is that right? moral statement of what we ought to do, it’s now
There is a tendency to protect these beliefs or at accepted that we ought to prioritise rationality. ❚

36 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


ESSENTIAL GUIDES

NEW SCIENTIST
ESSENTIAL GUIDES
&EWIHSRXLIFIWXGSZIVEKIJVSQ2I[7GMIRXMWXXLI)WWIRXMEP+YMHIWEVIGSQTVILIRWMZI
RIIHXSORS[GSQTIRHMYQWGSZIVMRKXLIQSWXI\GMXMRKXLIQIWMRWGMIRGIERHXIGLRSPSK]
XSHE]8LI[LSPIWIVMIWMWEZEMPEFPIXSFY]RS[

BUY IN PRINT AT:


SHOP.NEWSCIENTIST.COM/ESSENTIALGUIDES
Buy each issue digitally via the New Scientist iOS or Google Play App
CHAPTER 3

38 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


Cooperation between different groups in society often means
putting our own narrow advantage to one side. That has led to
the development of a near-universal rulebook that governs
which behaviours are to be encouraged, and to be proscribed.
Traits such as courage and modesty are “good”, for example;
lying and cheating are “bad”.

As societies became more complex, this moral code has


required independent structures to keep people on the straight
and narrow and maintain social cohesion – chief among them,
perhaps, moralising religion. But our religious sensibilities
tapped into far older evolved wiring.

Chapter 3 | Morality and religion | 39


HE anthropologist Franz Boas’s

THE ROOTS description of Inuit life in the 19th


century illustrates the probable moral
code of early humans. Here, norms were

OF MORALITY
unwritten and rarely articulated, but
were well understood and heeded.
Deception and aggression were frowned
upon; leadership, food sharing, marriage
and interactions with other groups were
loosely governed by traditions. Conflict
For most of our 300,000 years on was often resolved in song duels or, failing that, in
the planet, Homo sapiens lived in small ritualised combat. Because feuding leads to
instabilities, it was strongly discouraged. With life in
groups, and it is here that our moral the unforgiving Arctic being so demanding, the Inuit’s
code was forged – for good and all practical approach to morality made good sense.
The overlap of moral virtues across cultures is
striking, even though the relative ranking of the virtues
may vary with a clan’s history and environment.
Typically, vindictiveness and cheating are discouraged,
while cooperation, modesty and courage are praised.
These universal norms far predate the concept of any
moralising god or written law. Instead, they are rooted
in the similarity of basic human needs and our shared
mechanisms for learning and problem solving.
Our social instincts include the intense urge to
belong. The approval of others is rewarding, while their
disapproval is aversive. These social emotions prime
our brains to shape our behaviour according to the
norms and values of our family and our community.
More generally, social instincts motivate us to learn
how to navigate in a socially complex world, something
that starts pulling these instincts towards particular
habitual behaviours.
The mechanism involves a repurposed reward
system originally used to develop habits important for
self-care. Our brains use the system to acquire
behavioural patterns regarding safe routes home,
efficient food gathering and dangers to avoid. Good
habits save time, energy and sometimes your life. Good
social habits do something similar in a social context.
We learn to tell the truth, even when lying is self-
PREVIOUS PAGE: LJUBAPHOTO/ISTOCK
RIGHT: THOMAS FAULL/ISTOCK serving; we help a grandparent even when it is >

40 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


Chapter 3 | Morality and religion | 41
“Acts of both good and evil are
driven by altruism - and that is
selfishness in disguise”
inconvenient. We acquire what we call a conscience. atrocities in human history, including wars of
Social benefits are accompanied by social demands: aggression and genocide. We don’t come fitted with
we must get along, but not put up with too much. categories of people that are targets of our empathy or
Hence impulse control – only being aggressive, cruelty, says psychologist Steven Pinker at Harvard
compassionate or indulgent at the right time – is University. “Whether we’re good or evil depends on
advantageous. In humans, a greatly expanded what side of the sympathy boundary a particular
prefrontal cortex boosts self-control, just as it boosts individual is found,” he says. That largely depends
problem-solving skills in the social as well as the on whether we see them as part of our “tribe” at
physical world. These aptitudes are augmented by our any given point. If we don’t, we can treat others
capacity for language, which allows social practices and exploitatively or instrumentally, says Pinker. “We
institutions to develop in exceedingly subtle ways. can keep slaves, we can engage in ethnic cleansing,
Not surprisingly, this can go awry in various ways. we treat people like vermin.”
About 1 per cent of humans seem incapable of feeling
shame, remorse or genuine affection, and they are apt →-
to lie and injure without compunction. These are Chapter 4 on power and conflict has-
psychopaths and they lack a conscience. To a lesser more on these themes-
degree, dealing with discordant urges regarding self-
care and other-care is something we all struggle with. Even members of our in-group can’t count
The human capacity for both good and evil, often on our good intentions all the time. Our sense of
within the same person, has long been recognised and justice is often indistinguishable from our sense of
puzzled over. Evolutionary biology has an answer, and revenge, so we can be cruel when we think a person
it doesn’t reflect well on human nature. Acts of both “deserves” it, says Pinker. A desire for dominance
good and evil are driven by altruism – and that is can lead us to disadvantage those we see as standing
ultimately selfishness in disguise. in our way, he says.
For a long time, altruism was a biological mystery. But humans also have the capacity for self-control
The prime directive of evolution is to pass on our genes and, perhaps uniquely, self-reflection, which has
to the next generation. Engaging in costly behaviours allowed us to suppress or moderate some of our
with no obvious survival pay-off seems to go against baser evolutionary impulses. Innovations such as
that grain. The polymath J. B. S. Haldane eventually the rule of law, courts and the police go some way to
twigged it: individuals mostly make sacrifices for close reduce our power, or our incentive, to disadvantage
relatives, and hence help to usher copies of their own others for personal gain.
genes into the next generation. Acts of true selflessness A Maasai tradition known as osotua – literally,
exist, but these are explained as reciprocal altruism, umbilical cord – provides further insight into the
where kindness to strangers (who may in fact be roots of human generosity. It allows anyone in need to
relatives) is banked for the future. request aid from their network of friends. Anyone who
That’s all good, but what about evil? Evildoers often is asked is obliged to help, often by giving livestock, as
see their acts as being for the greater good. This long as it doesn’t jeopardise their own survival. No one
“pathological altruism” lies behind some of the worst expects a recipient to repay the gift, and no one keeps

42 | New Scientist Essential Guide | Human Society


EVERYDAY
EVIL
track of how often a person asks or gives. The trigger Evil lurks in all of us beneath a civilised veneer.
for such generosity is an unpredictable crisis. This
suggests that these practices persist because they help That was the apparent lesson of a controversial
manage risk, which pays off for everyone in the long experiment carried out half a century ago – but
run. Even the best-prepared family can fall prey to
catastrophe, such as a sudden illness. These types it might not have been all it seemed.
of risk can’t be prevented, so need-based giving may
have emerged as a proto-insurance policy. Prosperous
members of many societies share with others in need,
so that this social insurance will be available if they
require it – just as wealthy homeowners insure their HILIP ZIMBARDO’S first account of his
belongings against fire. now classic experiment showcased his
However, the ability to help isn’t enough in itself. To talent for storytelling. A sensational
benefit from osotua-style generosity, a society needs to article that appeared in a supplement
prevent cheating, for example asking when not truly in of The New York Times, it kicked off by
need. Where wealth can be hidden, reputation is the detailing how, one sunny morning in
key. In Fiji there is an osotua-like practice called Palo Alto, California, in 1971, police
kerekere. “People can get reputations for being habitual swooped on the homes of nine young
kerekere-ers, implying they’re lazy,” says Matthew men. They were bundled into squad
Gervais at Brunel University London. That makes them cars, taken to the police station,
think carefully before making kerekere requests, which charged, then blindfolded and transported to the
bring a slight taint of shame. In fact, reputation doesn’t Stanford County Jail, where they met their guards.
just inhibit cheating in kerekere: it appears to be the The “jail” was actually a set-up in the basement of a
rock upon which generosity is built. People with a building at Stanford University. The prisoners were one
reputation for giving also tend to receive more. half of a group of volunteers, the other half being
The Maasai and Fijians both live in close-knit assigned the role of guards. In what Zimbardo
societies. Do humans become less generous when they described as “a gradual Kafkaesque metamorphosis of
live in more complex societies? On one hand, people good into evil”, these seemingly well-adjusted young
in Western countries often walk past beggars on the men became increasingly brutal as guards. They
street. But that could be because social institutions “repeatedly stripped their prisoners naked, hooded
exist that they expect to step in and help. On the other them, chained them, denied them food or bedding
hand, need-based giving is apparent, for example privileges, put them into solitary confinement, and
when people donate money in response to a natural made them clean toilet bowls with their bare hands,”
disaster. In fact, experiments indicate that Westerners Zimbardo wrote. “Over time, these amusements took a
and others living in complex societies often give sexual turn, such as having the prisoners simulate
generously to strangers, whereas people living in sodomy on each other.” The prisoners, humiliated and
smaller-scale societies tend to direct their generosity victimised, suffered such emotional distress that
towards people they know. ❚ Zimbardo, playing the role of all-powerful prison >

Chapter 3 | Morality and religion | 43


Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
s’agissait de sa femme, le guidait toujours juste, et ce soir-là, en lui
épargnant les questions, il lui rendit le service qu’elle désirait le plus.
— Je suis inquiète, disait madame Mustel, je vais aller la voir.
— Ne bougez pas, je vous en conjure.
— Mais elle est seule !
— C’est ce qu’il lui faut.
— Il est désolant qu’elle soit si nerveuse.
— Rien d’étonnant après toutes les émotions qu’elle a
traversées.
— Oh ! cet homme, cet homme !
— Il est convenu, n’est-ce pas, ma tante, dit assez sévèrement le
docteur, que nous n’en parlons jamais ?
— Je voudrais apprendre sa mort.
— Taisez-vous, je vous en prie. Elle pourrait vous entendre et de
pareilles réflexions ne sont pas pour la calmer.
— Je ne peux pas me maîtriser.
— C’est extrêmement malheureux. En ce cas, je vous conseille
de descendre de bonne heure ; je tiens absolument à ce que
Marguerite ne soit pas agitée.
Un peu piquée, madame Mustel termina son dîner en silence.
Habitant la même maison que le ménage, elle en profitait pour être
beaucoup chez sa fille ; elle avait fait ce mariage et il lui semblait
juste de jouir d’un bonheur qu’elle considérait comme son ouvrage.
Lorsque madame Mustel, qui avait suivi son gendre, insinua en
se penchant vers Marguerite qu’elle allait la quitter pour lui permettre
de se reposer, elle fut ravie, et Roger un peu désappointé d’entendre
Marguerite dire avec force :
— Non, non, maman, reste, je t’en conjure.
— Certainement, certainement, si tu le désires, répondit madame
Mustel.
Et elle s’assit sur un siège bas avec un sentiment de triomphe, la
main de sa fille serrant étroitement la sienne.
— Eh bien ! cela va ? avait demandé Roger d’un ton
encourageant.
— Oui, beaucoup mieux, merci… encore un peu fatiguée.
— Naturellement. Ne bouge pas.
Et avançant un fauteuil aisé, plaçant au bon angle la lumière
voilée, il s’absorba dans une revue. De temps en temps, il levait les
yeux vers sa femme : elle tenait obstinément les paupières baissées.
Il crut qu’elle dormait.
III

Quand elle fut dans son lit, Marguerite ressentit une épouvante :
Roger allait venir comme tous les soirs… et elle avait parlé quelques
heures auparavant à Albert : il vivait, et un autre homme tout à
l’heure se coucherait à son côté !
Elle se répétait : « C’est mon mari » ; mais, avec une persistance
que rien ne pouvait vaincre, l’image d’Albert surgissait. Une véritable
honte la tenaillait, un désir impérieux de se réfugier dans la solitude,
et c’était impossible.
Blesser Roger, si bon, si dévoué, elle ne le pouvait pas, — et
demain elle avait promis de revoir Albert.
— Tu as la fièvre, ma chérie, lui dit doucement Roger en lui
donnant le bonsoir.
— Peut-être, je ne suis pas bien ce soir.
— Je le vois ; tâche de reposer, et au moindre malaise, je suis là,
tu sais.
Et il étendit sur elle un bras protecteur en la baisant dans le cou.
— Dors.
— J’ai très sommeil.
Le silence se fit profond, et Roger au bout de peu de minutes
dormait paisiblement. Marguerite en eut conscience, et alors elle
rouvrit les yeux. Réfugiée au fond du lit, elle le regardait éperdue,
s’interrogeant avec une frayeur croissante. Qu’allait-elle devenir ?
Comment avait-elle pu se remarier ?… Est-ce qu’on peut avoir deux
maris ?… Albert parti, disparu de sa route depuis si longtemps, était
devenu une image insaisissable ; mais Albert revenu, lui disant qu’il
ne pouvait plus vivre sans la revoir, avait repris sa place… Et
maintenant elle était liée, liée jusqu’à la mort cette fois. Elle avait son
fils, et puis Roger qui ne la quitterait jamais, qui l’emmènerait au
bout du monde plutôt que de la perdre… Il n’y avait plus aucun
moyen de retourner en arrière, aucun…
IV

Marguerite ne vit pas son mari de la matinée ; il était parti de


meilleure heure encore que de coutume, mais lui avait fait dire qu’il
rentrerait très exactement pour le déjeuner, et qu’il la priait d’être
prête. Elle avait eu un instant l’idée de se déclarer malade ; puis sa
promesse de la veille lui revint à l’esprit : elle irait, elle avait juré, il
fallait y aller. Elle s’occupa beaucoup de son fils, se grisant de ses
caresses, le portant dans ses bras, se pénétrant de la réalité de
cette vie qui était sa vie à elle maintenant. L’obsession était moins
forte avec son Maxime sur les genoux. Elle s’efforça de ne plus
penser à Albert : elle le verrait une fois encore, et puis ce serait tout ;
elle reprendrait sa vie si bonne et douce entre son mari fidèle et son
enfant vivant ; il ne fallait pas qu’Yvonne prît à Maxime sa mère… et
elle l’embrassait en le lui promettant. A recevoir les maternelles
caresses, le petit roucoulait comme une tourterelle, puis poussait
des cris subits et triomphants.
Le docteur Lesquen rentra et trouva Marguerite et son fils ainsi
occupés ; il s’arrêta net au seuil du cabinet de toilette, ravi de ce
spectacle. Il y avait dans le visage sérieux de Roger une expression
encore plus affectueuse que de coutume, et Marguerite s’en aperçut.
Il vint à elle, cueillit du bras droit et éleva en l’air l’enfant, et du
gauche enlaça tendrement la mère, la pressant de questions sur sa
santé. Quand, sur son appel, la bonne eut emmené Tonton, il dit à
sa femme :
— Ma chérie, je veux te montrer quelque chose et te proposer
une course.
De son pas rapide et brusque il retourna jusqu’à l’antichambre et
en rapporta une gerbe merveilleuse de fleurs blanches comme
frémissantes encore de leur vie éphémère. Il les plaça dans les
mains de Marguerite en disant :
— Veux-tu que nous allions porter ces fleurs à Yvonne
aujourd’hui ? J’aurais dû y penser hier…
— Aujourd’hui ? avec toi ?
— Oui, je me suis fait libre. Nous partirons de bonne heure : les
jours deviennent courts.
Elle le regarda avec une expression si intense, se cramponnant
d’un mouvement nerveux à ses épaules, qu’il en fut bouleversé.
— Roger… murmura-t-elle.
— Quoi ? ma bien-aimée, quoi ?
— Rien… je te remercie.
Il la caressa doucement, puis le médecin et l’homme sage
reprenant le dessus :
— Pas trop d’agitation, dit-il avec fermeté.
Pendant le long trajet de la rue de Prony au Père-Lachaise,
Marguerite laissa sa main dans celle de son mari ; elle ne parla pas,
et lui s’accommodait toujours du silence, l’esprit plein de ses
préoccupations, et du reste taciturne dans toutes ses émotions.
Aux heures de clarté, par une journée d’automne, il s’exhale, de
la grande cité des morts, un infini apaisement. Le mari et la femme
allaient du même pas égal : elle, dans une tristesse assoupie ; lui
grave selon son habitude. Au détour d’une petite allée la tombe
blanche apparut : appuyé sur une croix un ange pleurait… Ils
avancèrent plus lentement, et enfin en face du nom d’« Yvonne » la
mère tomba à genoux, les yeux dilatés, le cœur battant à l’étouffer…
Sur la tombe en quantité gisaient des roses blanches : moisson déjà
fanée, mais exhalant encore un doux parfum.
— Ma pauvre chérie, ma pauvre chérie ! s’écria Roger, pourquoi
ne m’as-tu pas dit que tu étais venue ?
Elle ne répondit pas.
— Ne me cache jamais rien ! Marguerite, ouvre ton cœur à ton
mari. Je comprends l’état où tu étais hier soir. Pauvre, pauvre
petite !…
Doucement il coucha, d’un mouvement plein de respect, les
fleurs qu’il avait apportées au pied de la pierre tombale. Il se releva
et attendit. Longtemps Marguerite regarda la pierre cachée sous les
fleurs, celles apportées par le père… et les siennes… Elle les
touchait du doigt avec un mouvement de caresse, comme si elles
eussent été une relique de l’enfant et de lui… Les deux visages,
celui d’Yvonne et celui d’Albert, se confondaient… Ils étaient là tous
les deux, tous les deux comme autrefois… Ces deux visages qui lui
avaient paru l’univers, ils n’existaient plus pour elle… Oh ! Dieu !
pourquoi ? pourquoi ? Elle les avait tant aimés !
Le regard attentif de Roger vit le corps de Marguerite fléchir
d’angoisse. Sans un mot, fermement et tendrement, il la fit se relever
et affermit son bras tremblant sous le sien.
— Viens, ma chérie, dit-il.
Et d’une voix plus tendre :
— Il faut penser à Maxime, n’est-ce pas ?
— Oui… balbutia-t-elle, oui.
Elle était heureuse qu’il l’emmenât, qu’il ne la laissât pas sur le
bord de cet abîme de la mort où elle avait le vertige.
Pendant la descente, il parla gravement, paisiblement, lui
répétant combien elle lui était précieuse…
— Je voudrais arriver à te faire perdre le souvenir de ce que tu as
souffert.
— Je ne peux pas oublier Yvonne ! dit-elle avec détresse.
— Non, tu ne le peux pas…
Il lui serra le bras doucement, et, tout bas :
— Peut-être il te viendra une autre Yvonne.
Elle sourit faiblement, malgré le chagrin de son cœur, tant l’idée
lui était douce ; il s’y mêlait une superstition obscure : l’âme de
l’enfant perdue peut-être reviendrait…
La voiture roula sur les boulevards extérieurs. Marguerite
regardait attentivement autour d’elle comme reprise par la vie : la vie
était forte en elle. Elle pensa qu’elle essayerait de toute sa volonté
d’être heureuse avec son mari et son enfant.
V

Arrivés à Saint-Augustin, elle descendit laissant son mari aller à


ses malades.
— Merci, Roger, dit-elle en prenant congé.
« A ce soir, femme » fut la réponse. Il aimait à lui donner ce nom
qui revêtait dans sa bouche une signification infiniment tendre. Elle
sourit et demeura rêveuse un moment, puis, lentement, se mit à
remonter le boulevard Malesherbes.
La journée était belle comme celle de la veille, avec un peu plus
de mélancolie dans cette rapide transformation de l’automne. Cinq
heures sonnèrent à l’église ; la voix de l’horloge la fit tressaillir et lui
rendit le sentiment du temps : l’allégement qu’elle avait éprouvé, la
délivrance du passé, l’acquiescement aux choses présentes
disparurent soudain. La pensée de son fils, qui depuis quelques
moments occupait uniquement son esprit, la hâte qu’elle éprouvait
de le revoir, de l’embrasser s’atténua, tandis que, brûlant, le souvenir
des minutes passées dans l’allée déserte lui revint. Elle s’arrêta.
Pourquoi n’irait-elle pas une fois encore ? Aucune loi ne pouvait se
trouver enfreinte parce qu’elle parlerait d’Yvonne, une minute, avec
le père d’Yvonne. La tentation dominatrice, pressante, obsédante
montait dans son cœur… Aller à Albert lui semblait si légitime : elle
revivait d’autres journées d’automne ; elle entendait la voix d’Albert,
elle se rappelait les retours chez elle, par de délicieux crépuscules,
dans cet appartement si aimé de la rue Rembrandt… Une sensation
exquise l’envahissait en y pensant. Trois fois, dans l’obscur débat
qui s’agitait en elle, elle dépassa la grille du Parc, résolue à aller
droit son chemin, trois fois elle revint sur ses pas. Portée enfin, par
une volonté supérieure, elle franchit le seuil du jardin, traversa
rapidement les allées délaissées, et de loin, entre les arbres,
l’aperçut.
Il était assis et lui tournait le dos, les regards dirigés du côté où
ils étaient entrés ensemble le jour précédent. Elle s’arrêta et
contempla la silhouette qu’elle connaissait si bien : l’attitude avait
conservé sa libre élégance, son indifférence fière. Albert avait jeté
un bras sur une chaise proche, d’un geste qu’elle reconnut ; pourtant
le fléchissement des épaules, le mouvement de la main gauche qui
soutenait la tête, trahissaient le découragement. Il était solitaire là
comme dans la vie… Ni femme, ni enfant, ni mère… D’elle à lui se
dégagea quelque communication subtile, car il se leva brusquement,
fit volte-face et la vit… L’éclaircissement de son visage fut si
éclatant, la pâleur de l’émotion subite fut si manifeste, qu’elle
trembla… Puis elle s’avança, essayant de se composer une
contenance. Face à face ils ne se dirent pas un mot, mais d’un
irrésistible élan il lui saisit la tête et la baisa au front. Elle se
dégagea, se redressa, et recula de quelques pas :
— Pardonne-moi, Marguerite, je suis si malheureux.
— Je le vois, dit-elle doucement.
Il lui prit la main, la serrant à la meurtrir.
— C’est fini, c’est fini, jamais plus tu ne seras ma femme, jamais !
Ah ! Marguerite, quel mal tu nous as fait en ne pardonnant pas, car
toi aussi tu vas être malheureuse !
— Non, je ne suis pas malheureuse.
— C’est vrai ?
— Oui, c’est vrai.
— Tu as été heureuse autrefois ; dis que tu as été heureuse.
— Oui, quand vous m’aimiez.
— Mais je t’ai toujours, toujours aimée !
— Quand vous me trahissiez avec Blanche… chez moi, dans
notre maison… Ah ! non, non…
— Mais si, je t’aimais ; seulement tu pleurais, tu étais triste, et la
brute qui est en l’homme ne sait pas pleurer longtemps. J’ai cherché
une heure d’amusement… Mais est-ce que je ne demeurais pas ton
mari quand même ?
Elle ne voulut pas répondre. Les seules paroles qui lui venaient
aux lèvres étaient : « Oublions, recommençons notre vie »… et ils ne
pouvaient plus la recommencer. Elle fit appel à sa conscience qui
s’endormait sous cette voix ; elle comparait mentalement Albert à
son mari, et était honteuse de la lâcheté qui lui faisait préférer celui
qui l’avait outragée, et plus d’une fois sans doute, à celui dont
chaque battement de cœur lui appartenait… Oui, il fallait haïr Albert
pour ne pas l’aimer ! La voix de Marguerite se fit âpre. Sans le
regarder, les yeux perdus, elle dit :
— Je suis venue aujourd’hui parce que j’avais juré sur Yvonne,
mais je ne reviendrai plus…
Il ne bougea, ni ne parla.
— Je m’en vais ; adieu, Albert…
Elle continuait son chemin, troublée de ce silence, désarmée de
toute volonté. Enfin il l’arrêta d’un geste léger ; elle y obéit aussitôt.
— Adieu, Marguerite ; je n’ai pas le droit de te retenir… je ne te
demande rien… je viendrai ici tous les jours… et si tu veux jamais
voir le portrait d’Yvonne ?
— Quel portrait ?
— Un portrait très ressemblant que j’ai fait peindre d’après ses
photographies et mes souvenirs.
— Où est-il, ce portrait ?
— Chez moi… J’ai gardé l’appartement de ma mère… personne
n’y est jamais entré… Est-ce que tu ne veux pas y venir une fois voir
ta fille ?
— Je ne puis pas… je ne puis pas… J’espère que vous ne serez
pas trop malheureux.
— Adieu !… Je t’aime plus que jamais, Marguerite, il faut bien
que tu le saches.
Et alors, incapable de maîtriser son émotion croissante, il la
quitta sans regarder en arrière.
Elle eut envie de crier, de l’appeler, de le retenir ; puis, anéantie,
elle alla tomber sur une chaise et, dans le brouillard humide qui
l’enveloppait et la cachait, elle pleura comme elle n’avait jamais
pleuré… Elle le sentait, en abandonnant son poste, en
méconnaissant la force du lien qui l’unissait à son mari, elle avait
trahi autant qu’Albert, lui pour une heure de joie, elle pour n’avoir
pas su souffrir et pardonner.
VI

La vie, après cette rencontre qu’elle ne pouvait plus oublier, reprit


pour Marguerite exactement comme si ce fait connu d’elle seule
n’avait pas existé, et ceci lui fut une surprise : l’eau s’était refermée
sur l’épave que le courant emportait… Elle avait craint une lettre, elle
n’en reçut pas ; elle avait imaginé d’autres rencontres fortuites où il
lui faudrait se dérober, elles n’eurent pas lieu ; ces heures où tout
son être avait palpité d’une vie ancienne et nouvelle paraissaient ne
jamais devoir connaître de lendemains.
Il n’était question en famille que des premières dents de Maxime,
de la fatigue ou des succès professionnels du docteur, des affaires
de Bourse de madame Mustel, et des événements racontés par les
journaux. Personne ne semblait se souvenir qu’elle n’avait pas
toujours été la femme de Roger. Si madame Mustel voyait sa fille
fatiguée elle recherchait pendant quarante-huit heures, avec une
patience d’agent de la Sûreté, la cause physique qui avait pu
déterminer cette fatigue ; de raison morale, au milieu de tant
d’affection, de bien-être, de satisfaction de tous les désirs, elle
n’admettait pas qu’il pût en exister.
— Si ma fille ne se trouvait pas parfaitement heureuse, disait-elle
quelquefois, ce serait tenter Dieu.
Un matin, vers la fin de novembre, par un temps magnifique pour
la saison, la nourrice sèche qui gardait Maxime, — car sa mère lui
avait donné son propre lait — se trouva de près de vingt minutes en
retard sur l’heure fixée pour son retour. Le docteur Lesquen se
préparait déjà à aller à sa recherche afin de calmer Marguerite qui,
grippée depuis quelques jours, ne sortait pas, et en toute occasion
s’agitait facilement.
Quand enfin la nourrice et le petit parurent dans la salle à
manger, quelques minutes après avoir été signalés par la femme de
chambre en vigie, Marguerite, qui était prompte à la colère, fit
durement une observation à la nourrice sur son inexactitude :
— Vous savez bien que je ne veux pas cela ; je ne le veux
absolument pas.
L’autre, rouge pour avoir marché vite avec un gros enfant dans
les bras, s’excusa : elle avait oublié sa montre, etc., « et puis c’est ce
monsieur qui joue avec les enfants et qui amusait le petit. »
— Le monsieur ? Quel monsieur ? demanda Marguerite
impérativement.
Et soudain une idée traversant son esprit elle devint pourpre.
— Ne te bouleverse pas ainsi, ma chérie, je t’en conjure…
supplia son mari. De qui parlez-vous, nourrice ?
— C’est un monsieur, très bien, en deuil, le pauvre !… Et comme
ça il regarde les petits jouer, et il m’a demandé l’âge de celui-ci. J’ai
pensé qu’il en a perdu un comme ça peut-être.
— C’est possible, mais ne parlez pas à des personnes que vous
ne connaissez pas. Madame et moi ne le voulons pas.
— Monsieur peut bien comprendre qu’on sait ce que c’est que
quelqu’un de bien. Je ne lui ai pas parlé à ce monsieur ; c’est lui.
Et toujours ronchonnant, la nourrice sortit.
Comme Marguerite continuait de déjeuner en silence, ne
témoignant que par le mouvement saccadé de sa fourchette son
trouble intérieur, son mari entreprit de la raisonner :
— Il ne faut pas, ma chérie, que tu donnes de l’importance à ce
petit incident. Je comprends ta contrariété, mais ces choses-là
arrivent tous les jours et n’ont pas d’importance. Ce que dit la
nourrice est probablement la vérité : c’est sans doute un père qui a
perdu un enfant de l’âge du nôtre.
— Oui, tu as raison ; du reste j’y veillerai.
— C’est cela. Te voilà presque remise de ton rhume, et s’il fait
aussi beau demain qu’aujourd’hui, je t’engage à sortir.
Un télégramme vint abréger d’office le déjeuner du docteur.
Quand plus tard la nourrice apporta le petit Maxime à sa mère
afin d’aller déjeuner à son tour, elle avait le visage maussade et
boudeur qui chez elle faisait invariablement suite à la moindre
observation. Tout en disposant l’assiette où se trouvait la soupe de
l’enfant, elle secouait la tête comme se répondant à elle-même :
— V’là sa soupe… C’est tout de même malheureux de voir qu’on
n’a pas plus de confiance que ça en vous ! Comme si je laisserais
quelqu’un de pas bien amuser le petit ! Il est poli, ce monsieur.
« Quel âge qu’il a, ce beau petit ? » qu’il m’a dit. Et je n’ai pas cru
que monsieur et madame allaient me faire des arias parce que je lui
ai répondu.
— C’est bon, nourrice. Qu’il n’en soit plus question ; allez
déjeuner.
La main de Marguerite tremblait en prenant la cuiller pour donner
à manger à son fils, et elle ne voulait pas que l’œil fureteur de la
nourrice s’en aperçût… Elle était sûre maintenant.
En baisant les doux cheveux de l’enfant, qui après chaque
cuillerée ingurgitée, la remerciait d’une caresse de sa petite main sur
la joue, des larmes amères lui montaient aux yeux, dans un
transport de pitié et de désir qui l’emportait vers celui qui n’était plus
son mari.
VII

Dès lors, ce fut fini du calme trompeur ; quoi qu’elle fît pour se
défendre, l’obsession d’Albert ne la quittait plus. Presque chaque
jour, elle emmenait la nourrice et l’enfant jusqu’à l’avenue du Bois ;
le matin ils allaient encore parfois seuls au Parc Monceau, et, à une
interrogation de Marguerite, la nourrice avait répondu, sèche et
rancunière :
— Oui, madame, je l’ai vu encore, mais j’ai pris un autre chemin
bien sûr ; on n’aurait eu qu’à dire que je courais après ce monsieur. Il
y en a de mieux que moi qui pourraient courir après, car il est
joliment bien s’il n’avait pas l’air malade.
« Madame » ne répondit pas, ce qui vexait toujours la nourrice
qui se croyait éloquente ; elle ne se doutait guère de quelle façon
ses paroles avaient porté.
Quand, ce jour-là, à la nuit tombante, elles rentrèrent, Marguerite
était possédée par une idée fixe… Il fallait le revoir. Elle hésitait
cependant, résolument loyale dans son intention, et fidèle au mari
qui l’adorait avec une si parfaite confiance. Mais aussi pourquoi
n’était-il jamais là ? La présence de Roger exerçait toujours sur elle
une influence apaisante.
Depuis ces dernières semaines, elle s’était aperçue de sa
solitude fréquente, et un vague ennui surgissait dans son âme. En
refaisant sa vie, Marguerite avait rompu avec beaucoup d’anciennes
relations ; elle s’appliqua à ne retrouver personne qui ravivât trop
distinctement le passé, et, satisfaite d’être entourée de la famille de
Roger, qui était par le fait sa propre famille négligée pendant un
temps, elle se créa vraiment une vie entièrement nouvelle, et sauf
quelques rares rencontres avec d’anciennes amies rien ne venait lui
rappeler son premier ménage. Elle menait en outre une existence
toute différente. Jadis très mondaine, toujours en mouvement,
maintenant elle sortait rarement le soir. Le docteur Lesquen se
trouvait si parfaitement satisfait lorsqu’il était assis à son foyer avec
Marguerite, il aimait si passionnément leur intimité, que l’idée ne lui
venait pas que sa femme ne fût pas également comblée à vivre
ainsi. Les soirées proprement dites lui étaient en horreur ; jamais il
ne dînait en ville que chez les siens, et comme ils étaient un peu
dispersés, les réunions n’étaient pas fréquentes. Ses parents
vivaient à Versailles, où une fille mariée à un officier vivait aussi. Le
frère aîné du docteur, ingénieur de mérite, était censé habiter Paris,
seulement, comme il était établi du côté de Vincennes, l’éloignement
rendait les relations difficiles, quoique Marguerite eût grand plaisir à
fréquenter sa belle-sœur, jeune et très charmante femme, mais si
absorbée par ses trois bébés que le temps lui manquait pour tout ce
qui ne les touchait pas.
Au printemps, on déjeunait parfois les uns chez les autres, et la
rareté relative de ces rapprochements en faisait l’agrément ;
absolument indépendants, on se retrouvait avec plaisir. Madame
Étienne Lesquen affectionnait beaucoup Marguerite et adorait le
petit Maxime, car ses trois enfants ne suffisaient pas à la maternité
débordante de son cœur. Et ainsi, entre ces réelles tendresses, dans
une vie de sécurité paisible, Marguerite se trouvait heureuse et
consolée, jusqu’au jour qui la mit en présence d’Albert. Maintenant
lui revenait vibrante la mémoire des années où sa vie était
mouvement, variété ; elle se souvenait de tout ce qui alors la tenait
sans cesse en éveil : les caprices et même les exigences d’Albert,
les soins qu’elle apportait à lui plaire, à maintenir chez eux
l’animation, la gaieté.
Il fallait le revoir.
Elle était très libre ; jamais Roger n’intervenait ni ne questionnait.
Elle sortit, monta la rue de Prony avec une hâte fiévreuse. Elle
s’imagina que peut-être il ne serait plus là ! bien qu’au fond de son
cœur elle fût certaine du contraire. Déjà son excuse était toute
trouvée : elle lui dirait qu’il ne fallait plus qu’il s’occupât de l’enfant,
que des soupçons pourraient venir à l’esprit de Roger.
VIII

Peu à peu elle s’habituait à l’étrange situation : rencontrer Albert


de temps en temps, causer avec lui d’Yvonne, le consoler dans la
tristesse qui l’accablait, semblait presque naturel. Elle avait essayé
de l’empêcher de la tutoyer, le menaçant, s’il y persévérait, de ne
plus revenir :
— Moi, ne pas te tutoyer ? Pourquoi ? Je suis toujours bien
autant qu’un cousin. Est-ce que lui ne te tutoyait pas lorsque tu étais
ma femme ?
Et elle n’avait pas répliqué, soudain convaincue.
Albert, maître de lui-même et follement désireux de la
reconquérir, ne l’avait jamais alarmée. Lorsqu’il la vit de nouveau
accoutumée à lui, soumise comme dans le passé à son joug, il tenta
ce qui était l’unique objet de ses désirs. Au moment de la quitter, par
une froide journée de décembre, et comme elle toussait pour la
troisième ou quatrième fois, il lui dit :
— Je ne veux plus que tu viennes ici ; la saison est trop
rigoureuse maintenant.
— Alors ? dit-elle presque effrayée.
Il la regarda, de ce regard tendre qui jadis l’aurait précipitée dans
ses bras ; elle tenait ses yeux levés, attendant.
— Amie, avez-vous confiance en moi ? dit-il gravement.
Elle ne put parler et secoua seulement la tête affirmativement.
— Il y a le portrait d’Yvonne… continua-t-il. Marguerite, tu peux
venir. Viendras-tu, dis ?
Elle s’était juré, son fils dans ses bras, de ne jamais céder à cette
prière, et soudain à l’idée de se retrouver une heure sous le même
toit que celui dont, vierge, elle avait été l’épouse, elle se sentit
comme soulevée de terre. La face du monde lui sembla changée : la
conviction que le lien qui l’attachait à Albert était indestructible, se fit
jour dans son cœur. Elle y puisa une sorte de hardiesse nouvelle,
comme rendue à la vérité.
— Oui, dit-elle, j’irai voir Yvonne.
— Notre Yvonne.
Elle pleurait, effrayée maintenant, désemparée, ne sachant plus
ce qu’elle voulait ni ce qu’elle désirait, meurtrie dans toutes ses
pensées. Même son Maxime, qu’elle avait cru jusqu’alors sa
sauvegarde, n’avait pas la puissance de la consoler : elle aimait son
fils, à mourir pour lui, mais pour vivre, l’autre était le plus fort ! Qui la
défendrait contre de tels déchirements ? Elle serait donc
éternellement malheureuse ? Elle se cramponnait instinctivement au
bras d’Albert. Il l’enlaça :
— Nous souffrons bien, car tu souffres aussi, ma pauvre aimée.
Elle sanglota :
— J’étais heureuse, heureuse… c’est vous, c’est toi, Albert…
Puis, comme frappée du son de ses propres paroles, elle
s’échappa en courant. Il ne bougea pas, ne fit pas un geste : il savait
qu’il l’avait retrouvée.

You might also like