Full Ebook of Neurorehabilitation Technology 3Rd Edition David J Reinkensmeyer Laura Marchal Crespo Volker Dietz Online PDF All Chapter

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 63

Neurorehabilitation Technology 3rd

Edition David J Reinkensmeyer Laura


Marchal Crespo Volker Dietz
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmeta.com/product/neurorehabilitation-technology-3rd-edition-david-j-rein
kensmeyer-laura-marchal-crespo-volker-dietz/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Technology Transfer 3rd Edition Ispe

https://ebookmeta.com/product/technology-transfer-3rd-edition-
ispe/

AI for Physics Volker Knecht

https://ebookmeta.com/product/ai-for-physics-volker-knecht/

Anger Management For Dummies 3rd Edition Smith Laura L

https://ebookmeta.com/product/anger-management-for-dummies-3rd-
edition-smith-laura-l/

PCR Technology 3rd Edition Tania Nolan

https://ebookmeta.com/product/pcr-technology-3rd-edition-tania-
nolan/
Examining Internet and Technology around the World
Global Viewpoints Laura M. Steckman

https://ebookmeta.com/product/examining-internet-and-technology-
around-the-world-global-viewpoints-laura-m-steckman/

That Kind of Happy Phoenix Poets 1st Edition Maggie


Dietz

https://ebookmeta.com/product/that-kind-of-happy-phoenix-
poets-1st-edition-maggie-dietz/

Health Technology: Sourcebook 3rd Edition Health


Reference

https://ebookmeta.com/product/health-technology-sourcebook-3rd-
edition-health-reference/

Education and Technology 3rd Edition Neil Selwyn

https://ebookmeta.com/product/education-and-technology-3rd-
edition-neil-selwyn/

The Roommate Route A Situationship Sports Romance 1st


Edition Mariah Dietz

https://ebookmeta.com/product/the-roommate-route-a-situationship-
sports-romance-1st-edition-mariah-dietz/
Neurorehabilitation
Technology

David J. Reinkensmeyer
Laura Marchal-Crespo
Volker Dietz
Editors
Third Edition

123
Neurorehabilitation Technology
David J. Reinkensmeyer .
Laura Marchal-Crespo . Volker Dietz
Editors

Neurorehabilitation
Technology
Third Edition

123
Editors
David J. Reinkensmeyer Laura Marchal-Crespo
Department of Mechanical Delft University of Technology
Aerospace Engineering Delft, The Netherlands
and Department of Anatomy
and Neurobiology
University of California
Irvine, CA, USA

Volker Dietz
Spinal Cord Injury Center
University Hospital Balgrist
Zürich, Switzerland

ISBN 978-3-031-08994-7 ISBN 978-3-031-08995-4 (eBook)


https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-08995-4

1st edition: © Springer-Verlag London Limited 2012


2nd edition: © Springer International Publishing 2016
3rd edition © The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license
to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are solely and exclusively licensed by the Publisher,
whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation,
reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any
other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation,
computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors, and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor
the authors or the editors give a warranty, expressed or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains
neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
Introduction to the Third Edition

When I want to discover something, I begin by


reading up everything that has been done along that
line in the past—that’s what all these books in the
library are for. I see what has been accomplished at
great labor and expense in the past. I gather data of
many thousands of experiments as a starting point,
and then I make thousands more.
Attributed to Thomas Edison

The aim of this book is to provide a comprehensive overview of the ongoing


revolution in neurorehabilitation technology. World leaders have taken the
time to step back from their work, evaluate the state of the art in their field,
and trace the development of their own work in creating this state of the art.
We wish to provide a cutting-edge resource for those seeking to use,
evaluate, and improve these technologies.
There are four unique features of the book. First, we have attempted to
ground the discussion of neurorehabilitation technology on neurorehabilita-
tion science. Thus, you will find less information about the details of
mechanical design or low-level machine controllers than information about
the physiology of sensorimotor impairments, strategies for human-machine
interaction, and the results of clinical testing.
Second, we have chosen to emphasize movement rehabilitation after
stroke and spinal cord injury, thereby focusing on the leading causes of
disability and the largest user groups of neurorehabilitation technology.
However, we note that many of the design principles discussed can transfer
to a broader user group and that several chapters cover applications for
people with neuromuscular disease, cognitive impairment, and cerebral palsy
as well.
Third, we have chosen to dedicate a greater amount of attention to robotic
therapy than other approaches. This is because robotics therapy technologies
have experienced the greatest growth over the past 40 years. Yet we recognize
that other technologies—including neural stimulation, sensor-based devices,
passive mechanical devices, exoskeletons, virtual reality, and mobile devices
—show promise and are increasingly playing greater roles in the clinical
delivery of rehabilitation, supplanting “traditional” robotic therapy in some
cases. Therefore, we have expanded this third edition to include a greater
amount of discussion of other emerging technologies.
Finally, we have focused on therapeutic rather than assistive technology.
That is, the emphasis here is on technologies that assist the motor system in
v
vi Introduction to the Third Edition

improving its intrinsic capacity to respond through training. We note


however that the line between therapeutic and assistive devices is blurring
because of emerging technologies like legged exoskeletons and spinal
stimulation systems.
We have organized the book into seven sections, which can roughly be
divided into two halves. The first half of the book (Sects. 1–4) focuses on the
design and implementation of neurorehabilitation technology. Section 1
contains three chapters that explain the relevant principles of neuroplasticity,
motor learning, and sensorimotor recovery that can be used to inform neu-
rorehabilitation technology development. Section 2 contains a set of chapters
that exemplify how neurorehabilitation technology can be implemented to
treat specific aspects of movement pathophysiology. Section 3 overviews
principles of interactive rehabilitation technology—including issues of
optimal challenge, psychophysical interaction, error manipulation, haptic
interactions, expectations of end-users, and device implementation in a
pediatric context. Section 4 contains three chapters on assessment technology
and predictive modeling, all of which suggest working toward a precision
medicine approach in rehabilitation.
The second half of the book concentrates on specific technologies.
Section 5 surveys a broad scope of neurorehabilitation technologies: spinal
cord stimulation, functional electrical stimulation, virtual reality, wearable
sensors, brain-computer interfaces, passive devices, mobile technologies for
cognitive rehabilitation, and telerehabilitation. Sections 6 and 7 then provide
detailed overviews of upper extremity and lower extremity robotics tech-
nologies. The book concludes with an Epilogue in the form of a debate over
the current efficacy and ongoing potential of neurorehabilitation robotics.
New chapters selected for the third edition include the neurophysiological
basis of rehabilitation, the role of challenge for optimizing motor
performance, the role of haptic interactions in promoting motor learning,
computational neurorehabilitation, precision rehabilitation, spinal cord
stimulation to enable walking, wearable sensors for stroke rehabilitation,
mobile technologies for cognitive rehabilitation, telerehabilitation, body
weight support devices, and the debate on the current and future value of
rehabilitation robotics. Other chapters published in the third edition have also
been substantially updated and reorganized to reflect the ongoing revolution.
We hope that this book will inspire the next generation of innovators—
clinicians, neuroscientists, and engineers—to move neurorehabilitation
technology forward, thus benefiting the next generation of people with a
neurologic impairment.

Irvine, USA David J. Reinkensmeyer


Delft, The Netherlands Laura Marchal-Crespo
Zürich, Switzerland Volker Dietz
Contents

Part I Basic Framework: Motor Recovery, Learning,


and Neural Impairment
1 Learning in the Damaged Brain/Spinal Cord:
Neuroplasticity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
Andreas Luft, Amy J. Bastian, and Volker Dietz
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations
of Neurorehabilitation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
Robert L. Sainburg and Pratik K. Mutha
3 Recovery of Sensorimotor Functions After Stroke and SCI:
Neurophysiological Basis of Rehabilitation Technology . . . . . 41
Volker Dietz, Laura Marchal-Crespo,
and David Reinkensmeyer

Part II From Movement Physiology to Technology Application


4 Use of Technology in the Assessment and Rehabilitation
of the Upper Limb After Cervical Spinal Cord Injury . . . . . . 57
José Zariffa, Michelle Starkey, Armin Curt,
and Sukhvinder Kalsi-Ryan
5 Implementation of Impairment-Based Neurorehabilitation
Devices and Technologies Following Brain Injury . . . . . . . . . 89
Julius P. A. Dewald, Michael D. Ellis, Ana Maria Acosta,
M. Hongchul Sohn, and Thomas A. M. Plaisier
6 The Hand After Stroke and SCI: Restoration of Function
with Technology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113
Mohammad Ghassemi and Derek G. Kamper
7 Neural Coordination of Cooperative Hand Movements:
Implications for Rehabilitation Technology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135
Volker Dietz and Miriam Schrafl-Altermatt

vii
viii Contents

8 Robotic Gait Training in Specific Neurological Conditions:


Rationale and Application . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145
Markus Wirz, Jens Bansi, Marianne Capecci,
Alberto Esquenazi, Liliana Paredes, Candy Tefertiller,
and Hubertus J. A. van Hedel

Part III Principles for Interactive Rehabilitation Technology


9 Designing User-Centered Technologies for Rehabilitation
Challenge that Optimize Walking and Balance
Performance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 191
David A. Brown, Kelli L. LaCroix, Saleh M. Alhirsan,
Carmen E. Capo-Lugo, Rebecca W. Hennessy,
and Christopher P. Hurt
10 Psychophysiological Integration of Humans
and Machines for Rehabilitation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 207
Vesna D. Novak, Alexander C. Koenig, and Robert Riener
11 Sensory-Motor Interactions and the Manipulation
of Movement Error . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 223
Pritesh N. Parmar, Felix C. Huang, and James L. Patton
12 The Role of Haptic Interactions with Robots
for Promoting Motor Learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 247
Niek Beckers and Laura Marchal-Crespo
13 Implementation of Robots into Rehabilitation Programs:
Meeting the Requirements and Expectations
of Professional and End Users . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 263
Rüdiger Rupp and Markus Wirz
14 Clinical Application of Rehabilitation Therapy
Technologies to Children with CNS Damage . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 289
Hubertus J. A. van Hedel, Tabea Aurich Schuler,
and Jan Lieber

Part IV Assessment Technology and Predictive Modeling


15 Robotic Technologies and Digital Health Metrics
for Assessing Sensorimotor Disability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 321
Christoph M. Kanzler, Marc Bolliger, and Olivier Lambercy
16 Computational Neurorehabilitation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 345
Nicolas Schweighofer
17 Precision Rehabilitation: Can Neurorehabilitation
Technology Help Make It a Realistic Target? . . . . . . . . . . . . . 357
W. Zev Rymer and D. J. Reinkensmeyer
Contents ix

Part V General Technological Approaches


in Neurorehabilitation
18 Spinal Cord Stimulation to Enable Leg Motor Control
and Walking in People with Spinal Cord Injury . . . . . . . . . . . 369
Ismael Seáñez, Marco Capogrosso, Karen Minassian,
and Fabien B. Wagner
19 Functional Electrical Stimulation Therapy: Mechanisms
for Recovery of Function Following Spinal Cord Injury
and Stroke . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 401
Milos R. Popovic, Kei Masani, and Matija Milosevic
20 Basis and Clinical Evidence of Virtual Reality-Based
Rehabilitation of Sensorimotor Impairments
After Stroke . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 429
Gerard G. Fluet, Devraj Roy, Roberto Llorens,
Sergi Bermúdez i Badia, and Judith E. Deutsch
21 Wearable Sensors for Stroke Rehabilitation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 467
Catherine P. Adans-Dester, Catherine E. Lang,
David J. Reinkensmeyer, and Paolo Bonato
22 BCI-Based Neuroprostheses and Physiotherapies
for Stroke Motor Rehabilitation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 509
Jeffrey Lim, Derrick Lin, Won Joon Sohn,
Colin M. McCrimmon, Po T. Wang, Zoran Nenadic,
and An H. Do
23 Passive Devices for Upper Limb Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 525
Marika Demers, Justin Rowe, and Arthur Prochazka
24 Mobile Technology for Cognitive Rehabilitation . . . . . . . . . . . 549
Amanda R. Rabinowitz, Shannon B. Juengst,
and Thomas F. Bergquist
25 Telerehabilitation Technology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 563
Verena Klamroth-Marganska, Sandra Giovanoli,
Chris Awai Easthope, and Josef G. Schönhammer

Part VI Robotic Technologies for Neurorehabilitation: Upper


Extremity
26 Forging Mens et Manus: The MIT Experience
in Upper Extremity Robotic Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 597
Hermano Igo Krebs, Dylan J. Edwards, and Bruce T. Volpe
27 Three-Dimensional Multi-Degree-of-Freedom Arm
Therapy Robot (ARMin) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 623
Tobias Nef, Verena Klamroth-Marganska, Urs Keller,
and Robert Riener
x Contents

28 Upper-Extremity Movement Training with Mechanically


Assistive Devices . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 649
David J. Reinkensmeyer, Daniel K. Zondervan,
and Martí Comellas Andrés

Part VII Robotic Technologies for Neurorehabilitation:


Gait and Balance
29 Technology of the Robotic Gait Orthosis Lokomat . . . . . . . . . 665
Laura Marchal-Crespo and Robert Riener
30 Using Robotic Exoskeletons for Overground Locomotor
Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 683
Arun Jayaraman, William Z. Rymer, Matt Giffhorn,
and Megan K. O’Brien
31 Beyond Human or Robot Administered Treadmill
Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 701
Hermano Igo Krebs, Conor J. Walsh, Tyler Susko, Lou Awad,
Konstantinos Michmizos, Arturo Forner-Cordero,
and Eiichi Saitoh
32 A Flexible Cable-Driven Robotic System: Design
and Its Clinical Application for Improving Walking
Function in Adults with Stroke, SCI, and Children
with CP . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 717
Ming Wu
33 Body Weight Support Devices for Overground Gait
and Balance Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 745
Andrew Pennycott and Heike Vallery
34 Epilogue: Robots for Neurorehabilitation—The Debate . . . . . 757
John W. Krakauer and David J. Reinkensmeyer

Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 765
Part I
Basic Framework: Motor Recovery,
Learning, and Neural Impairment
Learning in the Damaged
Brain/Spinal Cord: Neuroplasticity 1
Andreas Luft, Amy J. Bastian,
and Volker Dietz

Abstract Keywords

Neuroplasticity refers to the ability of the .


Recovery Rehabilitation . Stroke . Spinal
central nervous system (CNS) to undergo .
cord injury Brain lesion . Plasticity
persistent or lasting modifications to the
function or structure of its elements. Neuro-
plasticity is a CNS mechanism that enables
successful learning. Likely, it is also the 1.1 Learning in the CNS
mechanism by which recovery after CNS
lesioning is possible. The chapter gives an Rehabilitation technologies that support move-
overview of the phenomena that constitute ment recovery make use of different brain and
plasticity and the cellular events leading to body mechanisms, one of which is the brain’s
them. Evidence for neural plasticity in differ- ability to learn. Likely, the learning in the
ent regions of the brain and the spinal cord is lesioned brain that mediates functional recovery
summarized in the contexts of learning, is not identical to learning in the healthy state.
recovery, and rehabilitation therapy. Nevertheless, there are certain mechanisms on
the cellular and the systems level, that can be
broadly called neuroplasticity mechanisms,
which are shared by healthy learning and
recovery. Clearly, the main behavioral determi-
nants of healthy learning of novel movements,
activity, and repetition are also important in
A. Luft (&) recovery.
Department of Neurology, University of Zurich,
Hence, movement recovery may depend in
Frauenkliniksrasse 26, 8091 Zurich, Switzerland
e-mail: andreas.luft@usz.ch part on motor learning. Motor learning is a
general term that encompasses many different
A. J. Bastian
Department of Neuroscience, The Johns Hopkins processes. Distinct behavioral and neural mech-
School of Medicine, Kennedy Krieger Institute, 707 anisms are engaged depending on the level of
N. Broadway, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA complexity of the movement to be learned and
e-mail: bastian@kennedykrieger.org
the stimulus driving learning. A few different
V. Dietz forms of motor learning are briefly reviewed.
Spinal Cord Injury Center, University Hospital
Motor adaptation is a type of motor learning
Balgrist, Forchstr. 340, 8008 Zurich, Switzerland
e-mail: vdietz@paralab.balgrist.ch that acts on a time scale of minutes to hours in

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 3


D. J. Reinkensmeyer et al. (eds.), Neurorehabilitation Technology,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-08995-4_1
4 A. Luft et al.

order to account for predictable perturbations to a obvious or consciously perceived. Unconscious


movement. Adaptation occurs on a trial-by-trial rewarding feedback may play a role. The con-
basis, correcting a given movement from one scious reward of playing the piece well typically
trial to the next. It is driven by sensory prediction comes late and temporally unrelated to the
errors, which represent the difference between movement (e.g., the audience applauds). Thus,
the brain’s estimate of the sensory consequences implicit motor learning may be mediated through
of movement and the actual sensory feedback use-dependent or Hebbian-like plasticity rather
[1]. Once a movement has been adapted, it can than reinforcement mechanisms.
be de-adapted when the predictable perturbation All these forms of motor learning rely on
is reversed or removed. Discontinuation of networks of neural structures rather than single
training also leads to “forgetting” the adaptations areas, but there are some key regions that seem to
over relatively short periods of time [2]. play especially important roles in each. Adapta-
Associative learning can also occur on a time tion is known to be cerebellum-dependent [8].
scale of minutes to hours. Classical conditioning Classical conditioning can involve the cerebel-
is perhaps the most studied form of associative lum and hippocampus depending on the specific
learning. It links two previously unrelated phe- timing between stimuli [3]. Reward and avoid-
nomena to improve behavior. For example, in ance learning are dependent on basal ganglia
eye-blink conditioning, a “conditioned” stimulus circuitry [9]. Use-dependent learning likely
like a sound or tone can be repeatedly paired with occurs at many levels of the nervous system,
a second, slightly delayed “unconditioned” including spinal cord, brain stem, and cerebral
stimulus like a puff of air to the eye [3]. Early in (cortical and subcortical) structures. Complex
the learning process, the eye blinks in response to motor skill learning induces plasticity in the
the puff of air (i.e., unconditioned response). motor cortex, especially during the consolidation
However, with repeated exposure, the eye begins of the learned movement [9–11]. Importantly, all
to blink when the tone is presented, therefore forms of motor learning are dependent on cellular
anticipating the air puff by closing the eye (i.e., mechanisms of plasticity including gene/protein
conditioned response). This type of conditioning expression, synaptic and fiber growth, and
can be used to make associations between many functional changes such as long-term potentia-
types of behaviors. tion and long-term depression. As such, these
Motor learning can also be driven by feed- mechanisms are reviewed below.
back. Feedback can be given in close temporal
association with the movement (knowledge of
performance) and is used to adjust movement 1.2 Mechanisms of Neuroplasticity
parameters on a trial-to-trial basis. It may be in Learning and After Lesions
given later, e.g., after several repetitions, and can
reflect the average outcome (knowledge of result) 1.2.1 Gene Expression
[4]. Performance feedback may be associated
with a rewarding or discouraging element Learning a motor skill requires gene expression
thereby inducing reinforcement learning [5] or in the primary motor cortex (M1) [10, 11]. If this
avoidance learning [6], respectively. These expression is pharmacologically blocked, learn-
learning processes can occur on short- or long ing is inhibited. Gene and subsequent protein
time scales depending on the type and com- expression is a common requirement of various
plexity of the movement. The rewarding or dis- learning processes [12, 13] as well as for cellular
couraging feedback can be explicit (e.g., earning equivalents of learning, i.e., the changes in neu-
money) or implicit [7], i.e., small improvements ronal structure [14] and synaptic strength in the
after repeating a novel movement, e.g., when form of long-term potentiation (LTP) and
learning to play a piano piece, are often not depression (LTD) [15]. For motor skill learning,
1 Learning in the Damaged Brain/Spinal Cord: Neuroplasticity 5

proteins are not only expressed during training undergo LTP has recovered and is now expressed
but also thereafter while the subject is resting on a higher level of synaptic strength [24].
[10]. This delayed synthesis can be regarded as In addition to changes in synaptic strength, the
reflecting intersession consolidation processes structure of neuronal networks is reorganized in
[16]. The genes induced by learning are mani- association with motor skill learning. Apical and
fold, including immediate early genes (IEG, basal dendrites expand in association with skill
transcription factors). Expression of the IEG Arc training [26, 27]. This expansion is specific to the
in M1 was shown to occur specifically during neurons involved in the control of the muscles
skill learning but not during movement without used in the trained movement but not in the other
learning [17]. Dopamine-related gene expression musculature [28]. It remains open whether these
has been shown to be related to motor skill changes are permanent or reflect a temporary
learning in mice [18]. expansion of M1 connectivity. Changes in den-
Gene expression is induced by ischemia, dritic spines, in contrast, were shown to be
especially in the peri-infarct cortex [19]. Some of temporary and return to baseline 1 week after
these genes could also promote cellular plasticity training has ended [29].
offering the potential for stroke-induced plasticity Microglia also play a role in plasticity by
as self-healing mechanism of the brain. Genes promoting synapse formation via a BDNF (brain-
and proteins induced by ischemia include axonal derived neurotrophy factor)-mediated mechanism
growth stimulators while growth inhibitors are [30].
suppressed [20, 21]. In the context of recovery after a brain or
spinal cord injury, the role of LTP and LTD is
unclear. LTP is facilitated in the peri-infarct
1.2.2 Cellular Plasticity cortex [31]. This result may be incompatible with
the hypothesis that LTP is consumed during
Long-term potentiation (LTP) and depression recovery as it is after healthy skill learning;
(LTD) are commonly seen as cellular equivalents hence, LTP would be reduced in the peri-infarct
of the brain’s learning abilities [22]. Either by cortex not facilitated. But, the study lacks infor-
repetitive stimulation, seen as the equivalent to mation about the recovery of function or lesion
repetitive training, or by synchronizing two sig- size, so a valid comparison to healthy learning is
nals that converge at one neuron, potentially impossible, and the issue of LTP utilization
reflecting associative learning phenomena, an during recovery is left unanswered. In the hip-
increase in synaptic strength is induced that lasts pocampus, short-term ischemia leads to a dis-
from hours to days, termed LTP [23]. LTD is ruption of LTP formation [32]. In humans,
induced by low-frequency stimulation and leads preliminary evidence indicates that LTP-like
to a lasting reduction in synaptic strength [22]. phenomena elicited in M1 of the lesioned
Both LTP and LTD have been described in hemisphere (cortical or subcortical lesions) by
various brain regions including the primary repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation
motor cortex (M1) [24]. The observation that the (TMS) predict good recovery in 6 months [33].
ability of M1 neurons to undergo LTP and LTD Paired associative stimulation (peripheral muscle
is reduced after training provides indirect evi- and TMS stimulation of M1)—a potential human
dence for the hypothesis that primary motor equivalent of associative LTP—can be elicited in
cortex LTP/LTD is consumed by training, and the affected hemisphere M1, especially in those
hence can be considered as one candidate patients with limited deficits [34]. LTP-like
mechanism of motor skill learning [25]. Two phenomena are enhanced by serotonin [35] pos-
months after a skill has been learned in a 2 week sibly explaining the beneficial effect of serotonin
training period and is well remembered, the reuptake inhibitors in stroke recovery [36].
synaptic strengthening that is observed in M1 Hence, the ability of the lesioned cortex to
shortly after training persists. But, the ability to undergo LTP may contribute to recovery.
6 A. Luft et al.

1.2.3 Systems Plasticity in the Brain While movement-related activation observed


with functional imaging methods demonstrates the
Plasticity phenomena not only exist on the level brain areas that are involved in the control of the
of single neurons or networks but also in distinct movement performed during imaging, TMS can
functional systems of the brain. The input–output directly assess the output efficacy and the viability
organization and the somatotopy of M1 undergo of descending pathways in the lesioned hemi-
persistent changes during motor skill learning. sphere. Larger motor evoked potentials in response
Skill learning leads to an expansion of the cor- to TMS and the absence of ipsilateral responses to
tical representation of the trained limb [37, 38]. stimulation of the intact hemisphere are correlated
Longitudinal motor cortex mapping experiments with good functional recovery [53, 54].
in rats show that this expansion is transient and is Studies in animals and humans emphasized
reversed after training ends although the skill is the importance of the GABAergic inhibition and
maintained [39]. In humans who continuously the balance between excitation and inhibition in
train new motor skills, e.g., professional pianists, the peri-infarct cortex. Although the evidence
task-related activation is smaller in the area and from different studies in animals and humans
more focused [40, 41]. Musicians also have using different methodologies is difficult to unify,
enlarged gray matter volumes in various areas of it is assumed that inhibition is abnormally high
the cortex including the motor cortices [42]. The early after stroke, then is reduced below the
M1 of musicians contains memory traces of levels of healthy controls and returns to normal
practiced skills that can be probed by TMS [43]. afterwards [55]. In addition, the interhemispheric
Representations in the primary motor cortex (transcallosal) interactions of contralesional and
are also modified while recovering from a stroke. ipsilesional motor cortices are altered after stroke
Initially, large areas of motor and adjacent cor- and change during recovery [56], but it is yet
tices are recruited in the attempt to accomplish a unclear whether these observations are a cause or
movement as detected by functional magnetic a consequence of (un)successful recovery [57].
resonance imaging (fMRI) [44, 45]. If M1 itself
is lesioned, expanded activation is found in the
peri-infarct cortex [46] or the premotor cortex 1.2.4 Plasticity in Spinal Cord
[47]. As subjects recover, this hyper-activation is
reduced, and movement-related activity focuses There is convincing evidence in cats with a
on the ipsilesional hemisphere contralateral to the transected spinal cord that use-dependent plas-
moving limb [48–50]. If recovery is unsuccess- ticity of neuronal circuits within the spinal cord
ful, cortices remain hyper-activated in the exists [58, 59]. When stepping is practiced in
lesioned as well as the non-lesioned hemisphere spinal cat, this task can be performed more suc-
which has been interpreted as a sign of a con- cessfully than when it is not practiced, but the
tinuous attempt to initiate recovery [47]. But standing duration is not improved and vice versa,
recovery is not only accompanied by cortical i.e., training of standing has only an effect on this
activation changes. Larger activation in the task [60, 61]. The training effects of any motor
cerebellum ipsilateral to the moving limb [48] task critically depend on the provision of suffi-
and smaller activation in the contralateral cere- cient and appropriate proprioceptive feedback
bellum are associated with better recovery [49]. information to initiate a reorganization of neural
Connectivity between different cortical networks within the spinal cord. This is usually
regions in the brain is impaired after stroke, not achieved by functional training. In contrast, fol-
only in areas in the vicinity of the lesion but also lowing a complete SCI, when locomotor net-
in the intact hemisphere [51]. There is reduced works are no longer used, a neuronal dysfunction
interhemispheric connectivity after stroke, espe- develops below the level of the spinal cord lesion
cially between primary motor cortices [52]. in humans [62] and rodents [46].
1 Learning in the Damaged Brain/Spinal Cord: Neuroplasticity 7

1.2.4.1 Spinal Reflex Plasticity In the cat, recovery of locomotor function


The isolated spinal cord can exhibit some neu- following spinal cord transection can be
ronal plasticity. Evidence for such plasticity at a improved using regular training, even in adult
spinal level has been obtained for the relatively animals [70, 71]. The provision of an adequate
simple monosynaptic reflex arc [62]. Monkeys proprioceptive input to the spinal cord during
could either be trained to voluntarily increase or training is essential to achieve an optimal output
decrease the amplitude of the monosynaptic of the spinal neuronal circuitry with the conse-
stretch reflex in response to an imposed muscle quence of an improved function. This aspect of
lengthening [62], as well as to its analogue, the training could meanwhile also be demonstrated
H-reflex [46]. The fact that the training effects for the locomotor training of subjects suffering an
persist after spinal cord transection [60] indicates SCI [72]. Furthermore, in association with hind
that learning by spinal neuronal circuits is pos- limb exercise, the reflex activity becomes nor-
sible. Similarly, humans can be trained to change malized in adult rats following spinal cord tran-
the gain of the monosynaptic stretch reflex ([63]; section [73]. Exercise obviously helps to
for review, see [64]). normalize the excitability of spinal reflexes.
The idea that neuronal circuits within the Several neurotransmitter systems within the
spinal cord can learn is also supported by studies spinal cord (glycinergic and GABAergic sys-
of spinal reflex conditioning. Simple hind limb tems) are suggested to be involved in the medi-
motor responses to cutaneous or electrical stim- ation of plastic changes following repetitive task
ulation are enhanced in animals with a transected performance [58]. In cats with a spinal cord
spinal cord by reflex conditioning (i.e., pairing transection, stepping can be induced by the
the stimulus with another stimulus that evokes a administration of the noradrenergic agonist
stronger motor response) [65]. These reflex clonidine, which enhances the activity in spinal
responses become enhanced within minutes of neuronal circuits that generate locomotor activity
conditioning indicating that synaptic efficacy [74–76]. However, the application of Dopamine
along the reflex arc has changed, possibly in patients with an SCI has no effect on the
through long-term potentiation [65]. outcome of function [77]. Furthermore, serotonin
seems to be involved in the production of loco-
1.2.4.2 Task-Specific Neural Plasticity motor rhythms [78].
Today, it is obvious that there is also consid- Training paradigms of stepping and standing
erable task-specific plasticity of the sensori- can modify the efficacy of the inhibitory neuro-
motor networks of the adult mammalian transmitter, glycine [58]. For example, when
lumbosacral spinal cord (for review, see [58, glycine is applied to a chronic spinal cat that has
59, 66]). The detailed assessment of the mod- acquired the ability to step successfully, there is
ifiability of neuronal network function is little change in its locomotor capability. If it is
reflected in the research on central pattern administered to a stand-trained cat, it becomes
generators (CPGs) underlying stepping move- able to successfully step with body support [58,
ments [65, 67–69]. Observations made in 66]. These findings suggest that the effect of
spinal cats indicate that the lumbosacral spinal glycine is so far specific in its action as it enables
cord obviously can execute stepping or stand- spinal networks to integrate proprioceptive input
ing more successfully if that specific task is by reducing inhibition [75, 76].
practiced. If the training of a motor task is
discontinued and no similar task is subse-
quently trained, then the performance of the 1.2.5 Subcortical Contributions
task previously trained is degraded [58]. Con- to Movement Learning
sequently, plasticity can be exploited for
rehabilitative purposes using specific training The cerebellum is thought to use adaptive
approaches following a spinal injury. learning mechanisms to calibrate internal models
8 A. Luft et al.

for predictive control of movement. Such models [82], balance [83], and eye movements [84]. To
are needed because sensory feedback is too slow date, there has been no systematic way to sub-
for movements that need to be both fast and stitute or compensate for deficits in this form of
accurate; without internal models, corrections learning.
would be issued too late. Instead, the brain The microcircuit involved in cerebellar adap-
generates motor commands based on internal tation was first proposed by Marr [85], Albus
predictions of how the command would move [86], and Ito [87]. These works continue to
the body [79]. This feedforward control requires provide the basis for many of the current theories
stored knowledge (i.e., models) of the body’s of cerebellar function. Central to the idea of
dynamics, the environment, and any object to be cerebellar involvement in learning was the dis-
manipulated. For example, recent work has covery that Purkinje cell output can be radically
demonstrated that cerebellar damage causes a altered by climbing fiber induction of long-term
bias in the brain’s representation of limb inertia depression (LTD) of the parallel fiber-Purkinje
relative to actual inertia, which results in char- cell synapse [88]. Hence, climbing fiber inputs
acteristic patterns of reaching dysmetria, i.e., onto Purkinje cells can be viewed as providing a
over- or under-shooting targets [80]. This speci- unique type of teaching or error signal to the
fic deficit was confirmed in simulation and in cerebellum. It has been shown that the climbing
behavioral studies of control subjects reaching fiber may not simply be an all-or-none signal
with their limb while inertia is unexpectedly indicating error [89]. Instead, the duration of
changed via an exoskeleton robot. Perhaps most climbing fiber bursts is predictive of the magni-
importantly, this work also demonstrated a way tude of plasticity and learning, making it a graded
of correcting this mismatch using cerebellar instructive signal for adaptation. In addition to
patient-specific compensations rendered by an the climbing fiber-dependent LTD, there are
exoskeleton robot. This suggests that there may many other sites of plasticity in the cerebellar
be ways to compensate for biases in internal cortex and deep nuclei that involve LTP and non-
model representations using robotics. Unfortu- synaptic plasticity (for review, see [90]). Thus,
nately, cerebellar patients cannot learn to correct there are multiple avenues for activity-dependent
their internal model biases due to a loss of a plasticity to occur within the cerebellum over
cerebellum-dependent learning process often relatively short time scales. It is presumed that
referred to as adaptation. the plastic changes in cerebellar output are
Many studies have shown that the cerebellum responsible for changing motor behavior during
is essential for adapting a motor behavior the process of adaptation which is processes that
through repeated practice—it uses error infor- occur on relatively fast time scales. Purkinje cells
mation from one trial to improve performance in are organized in microzones that either increase
subsequent trials. It is important to note that or decrease the output activity during learning
cerebellum-dependent motor learning is driven (for review, see [91]). These zones interact with
by errors directly occurring during the movement widespread regions of the cortex, thereby
(knowledge of performance), rather than knowl- influencing learning processes across different
edge of results after the movement is completed domains of cognition and motor behavior.
(e.g., hit or miss). Studies have suggested that the While the cerebellum operates on faster time
type of error that drives cerebellum-dependent scales, the brain’s reward system encodes the
learning is not the target error (i.e., “How far am outcome of a behavior, i.e., the knowledge of the
I from the desired target?”), but instead what has result, and can thereby influence learning (rein-
been referred to as a sensory prediction error forcement learning) (for review, see [92]).
(i.e., “How far am I from where I predicted I Dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra
would be?”) [1]. Damage to the cerebellum (SN)/ventral tegmental areas (VTA) complex
impairs the ability to adapt many types of encode reward prediction errors and feed this
movements, including reaching [81], walking information to the cortex. Especially neurons in
1 Learning in the Damaged Brain/Spinal Cord: Neuroplasticity 9

Fig. 1.1 Schematic representation of the integration of reward circuits in the sensorimotor network. Via a dopamine
(DA) signal encoding a form of immediate “implicit” reward, this signal can directly modulate synaptic plasticity in
sensorimotor cortex synapses enabling motor skill learning

the VTA project to the primary motor cortex and an action or goal attainment (explicit reward),
are involved in motor skill learning. Ipsilateral the second a more immediate result of a sin-
dopaminergic projections from VTA to M1 [93] gle movement component [99]. This latter
are specifically necessary for acquiring but not form is not consciously experienced but rather
for performing a skill once acquired. Elimination causes a “good feeling” about the ongoing
of dopaminergic terminals in M1 [94] or movement and may be termed “implicit reward”
destruction of dopaminergic neurons in VTA (Fig. 1.1).
impairs the acquisition of a reaching skill in rats
[95]. Dopamine modulates the excitability of M1
[96] and S1 [97] and, more importantly, is nec- 1.3 Learning and Plasticity During
essary for the formation of LTP in layer II/III Rehabilitation Therapy
synapses [94] that link M1 and S1 via horizontal
connections. Plasticity at these synapses is 1.3.1 Lesions of Cortex and
involved in skill learning—as evidenced by the Descending Pathways
fact that the capacity of these synapses under-
going LTP is reduced after skill learning [17]. It Rehabilitative training is associated with neuro-
seems plausible that the VTA-to-M1 projection physiological alterations that are related to the
relays signals of the same nature as compared to improvement in motor function observed in
those that activate dopaminergic neurons from individual stroke survivors [100]. Although cor-
VTA to nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cor- relation is not proof of causation, these studies
tex, i.e., information about reward value and provide an argument for neuroplasticity being
expectedness [98]. In the complex interplay of one possible mechanism by which rehabilitative
these circuits that include the basal ganglia training can operate effectively. While bilateral
(esp. the ventral striatum) and the cerebellum, it arm training was associated with an increase in
may be that dopaminergic neurons signal two premotor cortex activation in both hemispheres
forms of reward, one that reflects the outcome of that correlated with functional improvement in
10 A. Luft et al.

the Fugl-Meyer and Wolf tests [101, 102], con- individualize therapy for the patient. It seems
ventional physical therapy (based on Bobath likely that different patients with different brain
exercises) did not show altered brain activation injury and lesion profiles will require different
despite being equally effective [102]. Conven- therapeutic approaches.
tional physical exercise may have utilized a
mechanism other than those detectable by fMRI,
e.g., by inducing changes in muscle, peripheral 1.3.2 Cerebellar Lesions
nerves, or spinal cord.
Lower extremity repetitive exercises in the Recovery from a first ischemic cerebellar stroke
form of aerobic treadmill training likely utilize yet is often very good, with minimal to no residual
another form of brain reorganization to improve deficits in up to 83% of patients [108–110]. On
gait. As compared with stretching exercises, the other hand, individuals with degenerative
improvements by treadmill training were related cerebellar disorders tend to have persistent or
to increased activation of cerebellum and brain- progressively worsening clinical signs and
stem as detected with fMRI of paretic knee symptoms [111]. One study has shown that
movement [103]. Interestingly, the areas recruited people with damage to the deep nuclei do not
in the cerebellum and brainstem corresponded to recover as well as those with damage to only the
regions that control spinal pattern generators cerebellar cortex and white matter [112]. Thus,
(cerebellar and midbrain locomotor region). These the etiology of the lesion and the extent of
regions may have compensated for the loss of damage are major indicators in recovery.
corticospinal projections that were injured by the There is a growing body of literature on the
stroke. It has also been shown that individuals effectiveness of rehabilitation interventions for
with a cerebral stroke can improve walking sym- individuals with primary cerebellar damage.
metry using adaptive mechanisms of learning on a There are studies on the effects of rehabilitation
split-belt treadmill [104–106]. Repeated split-belt interventions in this patient population, but most
training over a 1 month time resulted in are noncontrolled small series (e.g., [113]) or
improvements in step length symmetry in chronic case studies (e.g., [114]). Most work has been
stroke survivors [107]. Importantly, the split-belt done on walking rehabilitation with common
treadmill was used to augment the step asymmetry interventions including combinations of exer-
errors that the stroke survivors produced. This was cises targeting gaze, static stance, dynamic
done to drive a cerebellum-dependent learning stance, gait, and complex gait activities [113,
process that would correct their error. Stated 114]. Dynamic balance activities in sitting,
simply, making their error bigger drove the ner- kneeling, and quadruped have also been advo-
vous system to learn how to correct it. After cated [113]. Individuals with acute cerebellar
training, when they walked over ground, they had stroke seem to recover similarly regardless of
learned to correct their step length asymmetry. whether they participated in a 2 week treadmill
Training over 4 weeks led to improvements that training intervention [115]. Further, individuals
lasted (and even improved further) for 3 months with superior cerebellar artery infarcts tend to
post training. Here again, the hypothesis is that show more severe ataxia than those with poste-
intact cerebellar mechanisms are responsible for rior inferior cerebellar artery infarcts early on,
this form of motor learning. Hence, subcortical but both groups tend to recover to the same
reorganization may be the mechanism to target in extent after 3 months. People with degenerative
lower extremity, and particularly walking, disorders tend to benefit more from rehabilitation
rehabilitation. training. Ilg and coauthors found that 4 weeks of
The availability of treatments that operate an intensive coordination training followed by
through distinct mechanisms may provide the 8 weeks of home exercise could improve walk-
rehabilitation clinician with many tools to ing coordination and static and dynamic balance
1 Learning in the Damaged Brain/Spinal Cord: Neuroplasticity 11

scores. It has also been shown that a 6 week 1.3.3.2 Functional Training in Persons
home balance exercise program can improve with a Spinal Cord Injury
balance and walking measures in people with The coordination of human gait seems to be
cerebellar degeneration [116]. In that study, it controlled in much the same way as in other
was shown that the difficulty of the balance mammals [123]. Even arm swing during loco-
exercise is what predicted the best outcomes, motion represents a residual function of quad-
with more challenging balance activities result- rupedal coordination of bipedal gait [124].
ing in the greatest improvement. It was also Therefore, it is not surprising that in persons with
shown that the effects of home exercise lasted for complete or incomplete paraplegia, due to a
a month after therapy. In all these studies, it is spinal cord injury, locomotor EMG activity and
not known whether such changes translate to movements can be both elicited and trained
improved real-world function. similarly as in the cat. This is achieved by par-
Locomotor training over ground and on tially unloading (up to 80%) the patients who are
treadmills, and with and without body weight standing on a moving treadmill ([72, 125, 126]
support, has also been used with some success in for review, see [127]). In severely affected
single case examples [117, 118]. It is not clear patients, the leg movements usually must be
how imbalance is corrected in the body weight assisted externally, especially during the trans-
support environment, however. With all gait and mission from stance to swing. In addition, leg
balance activities, it seems critical that the exer- flexor activation can be enhanced by flexor reflex
cise be sufficiently and increasingly challenging, stimulation of the peroneal nerve during the
to facilitate plasticity in other intact areas of the swing phase [128]. The timing of the pattern of
nervous system [119, 120]. leg muscle EMG activity recorded in such a
Two controlled trials investigated the use of condition is like that seen in healthy subjects.
cerebellar direct current stimulation on ataxia and However, the amplitude of leg muscle EMG is
found a positive effect in patients with neurode- considerably reduced, corresponding to the
generative forms of ataxias (spinocerebellar paresis, and is less well-modulated. This makes
ataxia, Friedreich’s ataxia, and cerebellar form of the body unloading necessary for locomotor
multiple system atrophy) [121, 122]. training. There are several reports about the
beneficial effect of locomotor training in incom-
plete paraplegic patients (for review, see [71,
1.3.3 Spinal Lesions 129, 130]), and patients who undergo locomotor
training have greater mobility compared to a
1.3.3.1 Plasticity of Spinal Neuronal control group without training [131]. The neu-
Circuits: Rehabilitation ronal networks below the level of an SCI can be
Issues activated to generate locomotor activity even in
Based on the knowledge gained from animal the absence of supraspinal input [75, 76, 132].
experiments, the aim of rehabilitation after stroke The analysis of the locomotor pattern induced
or SCI should be focused on the improvement of in complete paraplegic patients indicates that it is
function by taking advantage of the plasticity of unlikely to be due to rhythmic stretches of the leg
spinal and supraspinal neuronal circuits and muscle because leg muscle EMG activity is, as in
should less be directed to the correction of iso- healthy subjects, equally distributed during
lated clinical signs, such as the reflex excitability muscle lengthening and shortening phases [133].
and muscle tone. For the monitoring of outcome In addition, recent observations indicate that
and the assessment of the effectiveness of any locomotor movements induced by a robotic
interventional therapy, standardized functional device in patients who are completely unloaded
tests should be applied. do not lead to a significant leg muscle activation
12 A. Luft et al.

[134]. This implies that the generation of the leg changes and consequently to an improvement of
muscle EMG pattern in these patients is pro- function [134].
grammed at a spinal level and requires appro- Proprioceptive input from receptors signaling
priate proprioceptive input from load and hip contact forces during the stance phase of gait is
joint signaling receptors [123]. essential for the activation of spinal locomotor
During daily locomotor training, the amplitude centers [134, 142–145] and is important to
of the EMG in the leg extensor muscles increases achieve training effects in paraplegic patients
and becomes better modulated during the stance [135] (Fig. 1.2). Furthermore, hip joint-related
phase, and inappropriate leg flexor activity afferent input seems to be required to generate a
decreases. Such training effects are seen both in locomotor pattern [134, 146]. In addition, for a
complete and incomplete paraplegic patients successful training program for stroke and SCI
[135]. The training effects lead to a greater weight- subjects, spastic muscle tone must be present as
bearing function of the leg extensors, i.e., body partial compensation for paresis [147].
unloading during treadmill locomotion can be Only in patients with moderately impaired
reduced during training. This indicates that even motor function, a close relationship between
the isolated human spinal cord has the capacity motor scores (clinical assessment of voluntary
not only to generate a locomotor pattern but also muscle contraction: ASIA motor score) and
to show some neuroplasticity which can be locomotor ability exists. More severely affected
exploited by a functional training [136–139].
However, only persons with incomplete paraple-
gia benefit from the training program in so far as
they can learn to perform unsupported stepping
movements on solid ground [135]. Neuroplastic
changes also occur in elderly SCI subjects. This
becomes reflected in a recovery of neurological
deficits like that of young subjects. However, the
translation of this improvement into a better
function is significantly worse in elderly subjects
[140]. Therefore, it is required to develop and
apply specific and focused rehabilitation proce-
dures in elderly subjects.
In complete paraplegic patients, the training
effects on leg muscle activation become lost after
the training has been stopped [132]. Furthermore,
after about 1 year after injury, complete para-
plegic patients develop a neuronal dysfunction
below the level of injury, especially in the long
flexor muscles [141]. According to rodent
experiments, this dysfunction is thought to be
due to undirected sprouting within neuronal cir-
cuits [46].

1.3.3.3 Prerequisites for a Successful


Training
The spinal pattern generator must be activated by
Fig. 1.2 Schematic demonstration of proprioceptive
the provision of an appropriate proprioceptive input during locomotor training in SCI subjects. The
feedback that leads to a meaningful muscle input from load and hip joint afferents was shown to be
activation associated with plastic neuronal essential to achieving training effects
1 Learning in the Damaged Brain/Spinal Cord: Neuroplasticity 13

SCI subjects require a threshold motor score In the future, in these patients a combination of
which allows performing stepping movements. regeneration-inducing therapy and exploitation
During the course of a locomotor training, sub- of neuronal plasticity possibly by using novel
jects can achieve an improved locomotor func- training devices could have a beneficial effect on
tion without or with little change in motor scores the recovery of function. In this aspect, the
[138, 148, 149]. In these cases, a relatively low research in spinal cord regeneration appears to be
voluntary force level in the leg muscles associ- quite encouraging (for review, see [156]). Novel
ated with an automatic synergistic muscle acti- training devices (often referred to as rehabilita-
vation leads to an improved ability to walk. tion robots) become increasingly important and
A considerable degree of locomotor recovery popular in clinical and rehabilitation settings for
can be attributed to a reorganization of spared functional training and standardized assessments
neural pathways ([150]; for review, see [151]). It if neurophysiological requisites are met [146].
has been estimated that if as little as 10–15% of Such devices allow a prolonged training dura-
the descending spinal tracts are spared, some tion, increased number of repetitions of move-
locomotor function can recover [152, 153]. In ments, improved patient safety, and less physical
addition, by a training approach with the provi- demands for the therapists. Supportive therapies
sion of appropriate proprioceptive input, direc- that enhance the brain’s potential to undergo
ted, meaningful sprouting within neural circuits plastic changes could supplement the training
takes place below the level of lesion with the itself. For all these developments, testing in
consequence of an improved recovery of function clinical trials will be required to prove efficacy
in the rat [46]. and optimize the treatment for various disease
The improvement of locomotor activity might and lesion types.
be attributed to a spontaneous recovery of spinal
cord function that can occur over several months Acknowledgements This work was supported by the
P&K Pühringer Foundation.
following a spinal cord injury [151, 154]. How-
ever, several observations indicate that the
increase of leg extensor EMG activity also occurs
independently of the spontaneous recovery of References
spinal cord function, as assessed by clinical and
electrophysiological means [126, 137, 149, 151, 1. Tseng Y, Diedrichsen J, Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R,
Bastian AJ. Sensory prediction errors drive
155]. Thus, functional training effects on spinal cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J Neu-
locomotor centers most likely contribute to an rophysiol. 2007;98(1):54–62.
improvement of locomotor function in incomplete 2. Shmuelof L, Huang VS, Haith AM, Delnicki RJ,
SCI subjects [126, 155], even in a chronic stage Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW. Overcoming motor
“Forgetting” through reinforcement of learned
[149]. However, part of the recovery in locomo- actions. J Neurosci. 2012;32(42):14617–21.
tion corresponding to observations in the rat [58] 3. Christian KM, Thompson RF. Neural substrates of
might also be attributed to changes in muscle eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention.
properties that occur during the training period. Learn Mem. 2003;10(6):427–55.
4. Sharma DA, Chevidikunnan MF, Khan FR,
Gaowgzeh RA. Effectiveness of knowledge of
result and knowledge of performance in the learning
1.4 Conclusion of a skilled motor activity by healthy young adults.
J Phys Ther Sci. 2016;28(5):1482–6.
5. Schultz W, Apicella P, Ljungberg T. Responses of
Neuroplasticity mechanisms and training meth- monkey dopamine neurons to reward and condi-
ods can improve function in patients with cere- tioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a
bral, cerebellar, and spinal cord injuries. delayed response task. J Neurosci. 1993;13(3):900–
However, patients with complete or almost 13.
6. Iordanova MD. Dopaminergic modulation of appet-
complete hemi- or paraplegia do not, yet, profit itive and aversive predictive learning. Rev Neu-
from training because they cannot actively train. rosci. 2009;20(5–6):383–404.
14 A. Luft et al.

7. Tranel D, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Brandt JP. Sen- 24. Rioult-Pedotti M-S, Donoghue JP, Dunaevsky A.
sorimotor skill learning in amnesia: additional Plasticity of the synaptic modification range. J Neu-
evidence for the neural basis of nondeclarative rophysiol. 2007;98(6):3688–95.
memory. Learn Mem. 1994;1(3):165–79. 25. Rioult-Pedotti M-S, Friedman D,
8. Galea JM, Vazquez A, Pasricha N, de Xivry J-JO, Donoghue JP. Learning-induced LTP in neocortex.
Celnik P. Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum Science. 2000;290(5491):533–6.
and motor cortex during adaptive learning: the 26. Greenough WT, Larson JR, Withers GS. Effects of
motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. unilateral and bilateral training in a reaching task on
Cereb Cortex. 2010;21(8):1761–70. dendritic branching of neurons in the rat motor-
9. O’Doherty J, Dayan P, Schultz J, Deichmann R, sensory forelimb cortex. Behav Neural Biol.
Friston K, Dolan RJ. Dissociable roles of ventral 1985;44(2):301–14.
and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. 27. Kolb B, Cioe J, Comeau W. Contrasting effects of
Science. 2004;304(5669):452–4. motor and visual spatial learning tasks on dendritic
10. Luft AR, Buitrago MM, Ringer T, Dichgans J, arborization and spine density in rats. Neurobiol
Schulz JB. Motor skill learning depends on protein Learn Mem. 2008;90(2):295–300.
synthesis in motor cortex after training. J Neurosci. 28. Wang L, Conner JM, Rickert J, Tuszynski MH.
2004;24(29):6515–20. Structural plasticity within highly specific neuronal
11. Luft AR, Buitrago MM, Kaelin-Lang A, Dichgans J, populations identifies a unique parcellation of motor
Schulz JB. Protein synthesis inhibition blocks learning in the adult brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci.
consolidation of an acrobatic motor skill. Learn 2011;108(6):2545–50.
Mem. 2004;11(4):379–82. 29. Xu T, Yu X, Perlik AJ, Tobin WF, Zweig JA,
12. D’Agata V, Cavallaro S. Gene expression profiles Tennant K, et al. Rapid formation and selective
—a new dynamic and functional dimension to the stabilization of synapses for enduring motor mem-
exploration of learning and memory. Rev Neurosci. ories. Nature. 2009;462(7275):915–9.
2002;13(3):209–20. 30. Parkhurst CN, Yang G, Ninan I, Savas JN,
13. D’Agata V, Cavallaro S. Hippocampal gene expres- Yates JR, Lafaille JJ, et al. Microglia promote
sion profiles in passive avoidance conditioning. learning-dependent synapse formation through
Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18(10):2835–41. brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Cell. 2013;155
14. Steward O, Schuman EM. Protein synthesis at (7):1596–609.
synaptic sites on dendrites. Annu Rev Neurosci. 31. Hagemann G, Redecker C, Neumann-Haefelin T,
2001;24(1):299–325. Freund H-J, Witte OW. Increased long-term poten-
15. Miyamoto E. Molecular mechanism of neuronal tiation in the surround of experimentally induced
plasticity: induction and maintenance of long-term focal cortical infarction. Ann Neurol. 1998;44
potentiation in the hippocampus. J Pharmacol Sci. (2):255–8.
2006;100(5):433–42. 32. Gasparova Z, Jariabka P, Stolc S. Effect of transient
16. Davis HP, Squire LR. Protein synthesis and mem- ischemia on long-term potentiation of synaptic
ory: a review. Psychol Bull. 1984;96(3):518–59. transmission in rat hippocampal slices. Neuroendocr
17. Hosp JA, Mann S, Wegenast-Braun BM, Cal- Lett. 2008;29(5):702–5.
houn ME, Luft AR. Region and task-specific 33. Lazzaro VD, Profice P, Pilato F, Capone F,
activation of arc in primary motor cortex of rats Ranieri F, Pasqualetti P, et al. Motor cortex
following motor skill learning. Neuroscience. plasticity predicts recovery in acute stroke. Cereb
2013;250:557–64. Cortex. 2009;20(7):1523–8.
18. Qian Y, Chen M, Forssberg H, Heijtz RD. Genetic 34. Castel-Lacanal E, Marque P, Tardy J, de Boisse-
variation in dopamine-related gene expression zon X, Guiraud V, Chollet F, et al. Induction of
influences motor skill learning in mice. Genes Brain cortical plastic changes in wrist muscles by paired
Behav. 2013;12(6):604–14. associative stimulation in the recovery phase of
19. Carmichael ST. Plasticity of cortical projections stroke patients. Neurorehabil Neural Repair.
after stroke. Neurosci. 2003;9(1):64–75. 2008;23(4):366–72.
20. Li S, Carmichael ST. Growth-associated gene and 35. Batsikadze G, Paulus W, Kuo M-F, Nitsche MA.
protein expression in the region of axonal sprouting Effect of serotonin on paired associative
in the aged brain after stroke. Neurobiol Dis. stimulation-induced plasticity in the human motor
2006;23(2):362–73. cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38
21. Carmichael ST. Cellular and molecular mechanisms (11):2260–7.
of neural repair after stroke: making waves. Ann 36. Chollet F, Tardy J, Albucher J-F, Thalamas C,
Neurol. 2006;59(5):735–42. Berard E, Lamy C, et al. Fluoxetine for motor
22. Malenka RC, Bear MF. LTP and LTD. Neuron. recovery after acute ischaemic stroke (FLAME): a
2004;44(1):5–21. randomised placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol.
23. Bennett MR. The concept of long term potentiation 2011;10(2):123–30.
of transmission at synapses. Prog Neurobiol. 37. Nudo R, Milliken G, Jenkins W, Merzenich M.
2000;60(2):109–37. Use-dependent alterations of movement
1 Learning in the Damaged Brain/Spinal Cord: Neuroplasticity 15

representations in primary motor cortex of adult 52. Carter AR, Astafiev SV, Lang CE, Connor LT,
squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci. 1996;16(2):785–807. Rengachary J, Strube MJ, et al. Resting state inter-
38. Kleim JA, Barbay S, Nudo RJ. Functional reorga- hemispheric fMRI connectivity predicts perfor-
nization of the rat motor cortex following motor mance after stroke. Ann Neurol. 2009;67(3).
skill learning. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80(6):3321–5. 53. Turton A, Wroe S, Trepte N, Fraser C, Lemon RN.
39. Molina-Luna K, Hertler B, Buitrago MM, Luft AR. Contralateral and ipsilateral EMG responses to
Motor learning transiently changes cortical soma- transcranial magnetic stimulation during recovery
totopy. Neuroimage. 2008;40(4):1748–54. of arm and hand function after stroke. Electroen-
40. Lotze M, Scheler G, Tan H-RM, Braun C, Bir- cephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Electromyogr Mot
baumer N. The musician’s brain: functional imag- Control. 1996;101(4):316–28.
ing of amateurs and professionals during 54. Escudero JV, Sancho J, Bautista D, Escudero M,
performance and imagery. Neuroimage. 2003;20 López-Trigo J. Prognostic value of motor evoked
(3):1817–29. potential obtained by transcranial magnetic brain
41. Jäncke L, Shah NJ, Peters M. Cortical activations in stimulation in motor function recovery in patients
primary and secondary motor areas for complex with acute ischemic stroke. Stroke. 1998;29
bimanual movements in professional pianists. Cogn (9):1854–9.
Brain Res. 2000;10(1–2):177–83. 55. Grigoras I-F, Stagg CJ. Recent advances in the role
42. Gaser C, Schlaug G. Brain structures differ between of excitation–inhibition balance in motor recovery
musicians and non-musician. Neuroimage. 2001;13 post-stroke. Fac Rev. 2021;10:58.
(6):1168. 56. Murase N, Duque J, Mazzocchio R, Cohen LG.
43. Gentner R, Gorges S, Weise D, Kampe K aufm, Influence of interhemispheric interactions on motor
Buttmann M, Classen J. Encoding of motor skill in function in chronic stroke. Ann Neurol. 2004;55
the corticomuscular system of musicians. Curr Biol. (3):400–9.
2010;20(20):1869–74. 57. Cirillo J, Mooney RA, Ackerley SJ, Barber PA,
44. Marshall RS, Perera GM, Lazar RM, Krakauer JW, Borges VM, Clarkson AN, et al. Neurochemical
Constantine RC, DeLaPaz RL. Evolution of cortical balance and inhibition at the subacute stage after
activation during recovery from corticospinal tract stroke. J Neurophysiol. 2020;123(5):1775–90.
infarction. Stroke. 2000;31(3):656–61. 58. Edgerton VR, de Leon RD, Tillakaratne N, Reck-
45. Calautti C, Leroy F, Guincestre J-Y, Marié R-M, tenwald MR, Hodgson JA, Roy RR. Use-dependent
Baron J-C. Sequential activation brain mapping plasticity in spinal stepping and standing. Adv
after subcortical stroke: changes in hemispheric Neurol. 1997;72:233–47.
balance and recovery. NeuroReport. 2001;12 59. Pearson KG. Plasticity of neuronal networks in the
(18):3883–6. spinal cord: modifications in response to altered
46. Beauparlant J, van den Brand R, Barraud Q, sensory input. Prog Brain Res. 2000;128:61–70.
Friedli L, Musienko P, Dietz V, et al. Undirected 60. Lovely RG, Gregor RJ, Roy RR, Edgerton VR.
compensatory plasticity contributes to neuronal Effects of training on the recovery of full-weight-
dysfunction after severe spinal cord injury. Brain. bearing stepping in the adult spinal cat. Exp Neurol.
2013;136(11):3347–61. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih. 1986;92(2):421–35.
gov/pubmed/24080153. 61. Lovely RS, Falls LA, Al-Mondhiry HA, Cham-
47. Ward NS, Brown MM, Thompson AJ, Frack- bers CE, Sexton GJ, Ni H, et al. Association of
owiak RSJ. Neural correlates of motor recovery gammaA/gamma’ fibrinogen levels and coronary
after stroke: a longitudinal fMRI study. Brain. artery disease. Thromb Haemost. 2002;88(1):26–
2003;126(11):2476–96. 31.
48. Cramer SC, Nelles G, Benson RR, Kaplan JD, 62. Dietz V, Macauda G, Schrafl-Altermatt M, Wirz M,
Parker RA, Kwong KK, et al. A functional MRI Kloter E, Michels L. Neural coupling of cooperative
study of subjects recovered from hemiparetic stroke. hand movements: a reflex and fMRI study. Cereb
Stroke. 1997;28(12):2518–27. Cortex. 2015;25(4):948–58.
49. Seitz RJ, Höflich P, Binkofski F, Tellmann L, 63. Wolpaw JR. Operant conditioning of primate spinal
Herzog H, Freund H-J. Role of the premotor cortex reflexes: the H-reflex. J Neurophysiol. 1987;57
in recovery from middle cerebral artery infarction. (2):443–59.
Arch Neurol (Chicago). 1998;55(8):1081–8. 64. Wolpaw JR, Seegal RF, O’Keefe JA. Adaptive
50. Small SL, Hlustik P, Noll DC, Genovese C, plasticity in primate spinal stretch reflex: behavior
Solodkin A. Cerebellar hemispheric activation ipsi- of synergist and antagonist muscles. J Neurophysiol.
lateral to the paretic hand correlates with functional 1983;50(6):1312–9.
recovery after stroke. Brain. 2002;125(7):1544–57. 65. Harris-Warrick RM, Marder E. Modulation of
51. Rehme AK, Grefkes C. Cerebral network disorders neural networks for behavior. Annu Rev Neurosci.
after stroke: evidence from imaging-based connec- 1991;14(1):39–57.
tivity analyses of active and resting brain states in 66. Edgerton VR, Roy RR, Hodgson JA, Prober RJ, de
humans. J Physiol. 2013;591(1):17–31. Guzman CP, de Leon R. Potential of adult
16 A. Luft et al.

mammalian lumbosacral spinal cord to execute and 82. Morton SM, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar contributions to
acquire improved locomotion in the absence of locomotor adaptations during splitbelt treadmill
supraspinal input. J Neurotrauma. 1992;9(Suppl 1): walking. J Neurosci. 2006;26(36):9107–16.
S119-28. 83. Horak FB, Diener HC. Cerebellar control of
67. Dickinson PS. Interactions among neural networks postural scaling and central set in stance. J Neuro-
for behavior. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1995;5 physiol. 1994;72(2):479–93.
(6):792–8. 84. Yagi T, Shimizu M, Sekine S, Kamio T,
68. Katz PS. Intrinsic and extrinsic neuromodulation of Suzuki J. A new neurotological test for detecting
motor circuits. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1995;5 cerebellar dysfunction. Ann N Y Acad Sci.
(6):799–808. 1981;374(1):526–31.
69. Pearson KG, Misiaszek JE. Use-dependent gain 85. Marr D. A theory of cerebellar cortex. J Physiol.
change in the reflex contribution to extensor activity 1969;202(2):437–70.
in walking cats. Brain Res. 2000;883(1):131–4. 86. Albus JS. A theory of cerebellar function. Math
70. Barbeau H, Rossignol S. Recovery of locomotion Biosci. 1971;10(1–2):25–61.
after chronic spinalization in the adult cat. Brain 87. Ito M. Long-term depression. Annu Rev Neurosci.
Res. 1987;412(1):84–95. 1989;12(1):85–102.
71. Barbeau H, Rossignol S. Enhancement of locomo- 88. Ito M, Kano M. Long-lasting depression of parallel
tor recovery following spinal cord injury. Curr Opin fiber-Purkinje cell transmission induced by con-
Neurol. 1994;7(6):517–24. junctive stimulation of parallel fibers and climbing
72. Dietz V, Fouad K. Restoration of sensorimotor fibers in the cerebellar cortex. Neurosci Lett.
functions after spinal cord injury. Brain. 2014;137 1982;33(3):253–8.
(3):654–67. 89. Yang Y, Lisberger SG. Purkinje-cell plasticity and
73. Skinner RD, Houle JD, Reese NB, Berry CL, cerebellar motor learning are graded by complex-
Garcia-Rill E. Effects of exercise and fetal spinal spike duration. Nature. 2014;510(7506):529–32.
cord implants on the H-reflex in chronically spinal- http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24814344.
ized adult rats. Brain Res. 1996;729(1):127–31. 90. Hansel C, Linden DJ, D’Angelo E. Beyond parallel
74. Chau C, Barbeau H, Rossignol S. Effects of fiber LTD: the diversity of synaptic and non-
intrathecal alpha1- and alpha2-noradrenergic ago- synaptic plasticity in the cerebellum. Nat Neurosci.
nists and norepinephrine on locomotion in chronic 2001;4(5):467–75.
spinal cats. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79(6):2941–63. 91. Zeeuw CID. Bidirectional learning in upbound and
75. de Leon RD, Hodgson JA, Roy RR, Edgerton VR. downbound microzones of the cerebellum. Nat Rev
Locomotor capacity attributable to step training Neurosci. 2021;22(2):92–110.
versus spontaneous recovery after spinalization in 92. Lerner TN, Holloway AL, Seiler JL. Dopamine,
adult cats. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79(3):1329–40. updated: reward prediction error and beyond. Curr
76. Leon RDD, Hodgson JA, Roy RR, Edgerton VR. Opin Neurobiol. 2021;67:123–30.
Full weight-bearing hindlimb standing following 93. Luft AR, Schwarz S. Dopaminergic signals in
stand training in the adult spinal cat. J Neurophysiol. primary motor cortex. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2009;27
1998;80(1):83–91. (5):415–21.
77. Maric O, Zörner B, Dietz V. Levodopa therapy in 94. Molina-Luna K, Pekanovic A, Röhrich S, Hertler B,
incomplete spinal cord injury. J Neurotrauma. Schubring-Giese M, Rioult-Pedotti M-S, et al.
2008;25(11):1303–7. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/ Dopamine in motor cortex is necessary for skill
pubmed/19061374. learning and synaptic plasticity. PLoS ONE. 2009;4
78. Schmidt BJ, Jordan LM. The role of serotonin in (9):e7082.
reflex modulation and locomotor rhythm production 95. Hosp JA, Pekanovic A, Rioult-Pedotti MS,
in the mammalian spinal cord. Brain Res Bull. Luft AR. Dopaminergic projections from midbrain
2000;53(5):689–710. to primary motor cortex mediate motor skill learn-
79. Shadmehr R, Krakauer JW. A computational neu- ing. J Neurosci. 2011;31(7):2481–7.
roanatomy for motor control. Exp Brain Res. 96. Hosp JA, Molina-Luna K, Hertler B, Atiemo CO,
2008;185(3):359–81. Luft AR. Dopaminergic modulation of motor maps
80. Bhanpuri NH, Okamura AM, Bastian AJ. Predicting in rat motor cortex: an in vivo study. Neuroscience.
and correcting ataxia using a model of cerebellar 2009;159(2):692–700.
function. Brain. 2014;137(7):1931–44. 97. Hosp JA, Hertler B, Atiemo CO, Luft AR. Dopamin-
81. Smith MA, Shadmehr R. Intact ability to learn ergic modulation of receptive fields in rat sensori-
internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington’s motor cortex. Neuroimage. 2011;54(1):154–60.
disease but not cerebellar degeneration. J Neuro- 98. Schultz W. Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends
physiol. 2005;93(5):2809–21. Neurosci. 2007;30(5):203–10.
1 Learning in the Damaged Brain/Spinal Cord: Neuroplasticity 17

99. Ramnani N. Chapter 10 automatic and controlled 113. Ilg W, Synofzik M, Brotz D, Burkard S, Giese MA,
processing in the corticocerebellar system. Prog Schols L. Intensive coordinative training improves
Brain Res. 2014;210:255–85. motor performance in degenerative cerebellar dis-
100. Schaechter JD. Motor rehabilitation and brain ease. Neurology. 2009;73(22):1823–30.
plasticity after hemiparetic stroke. Prog Neurobiol. 114. Gill-Body KM, Popat RA, Parker SW, Krebs DE.
2004;73(1):61–72. Rehabilitation of balance in two patients with
101. Luft AR, McCombe-Waller S, Whitall J, For- cerebellar dysfunction. Phys Ther. 1997;77
rester LW, Macko R, Sorkin JD, et al. Repetitive (5):534–52.
bilateral arm training and motor cortex activation in 115. Bultmann U, Pierscianek D, Gizewski ER,
chronic stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Schoch B, Fritsche N, Timmann D, et al. Functional
JAMA. 2004;292(15):1853. recovery and rehabilitation of postural impairment
102. Whitall J, Waller SM, Sorkin JD, Forrester LW, and gait ataxia in patients with acute cerebellar
Macko RF, Hanley DF, et al. Bilateral and unilateral stroke. Gait Posture. 2014;39(1):563–9.
arm training improve motor function through 116. Keller JL, Bastian AJ. A home balance exercise
differing neuroplastic mechanisms: a single- program improves walking in people with cerebellar
blinded randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabil ataxia. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2014;28
Neural Repair. 2010;25(2):118–29. (8):770–8.
103. Luft AR, Macko RF, Forrester LW, Villagra F, 117. Vaz DV, de Schettino RC, de Castro TRR, Teix-
Ivey F, Sorkin JD, et al. Treadmill exercise activates eira VR, Furtado SRC, de Figueiredo EM. Tread-
subcortical neural networks and improves walking mill training for ataxic patients: a single-subject
after stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Stroke. experimental design. Clin Rehabil. 2008;22(3):234–
2008;39(12):3341–50. 41.
104. Reisman DS, Wityk R, Silver K, Bastian AJ. Loco- 118. Cernak K, Stevens V, Price R, Shumway-Cook A.
motor adaptation on a split-belt treadmill can Locomotor training using body-weight support on a
improve walking symmetry post-stroke. Brain. treadmill in conjunction with ongoing physical
2007;130(7):1861–72. therapy in a child with severe cerebellar ataxia.
105. Reisman DS, McLean H, Bastian AJ. Split-belt Phys Ther. 2008;88(1):88–97.
treadmill training poststroke: a case study. J Neurol 119. Kleim JA, Swain RA, Armstrong KA, Nap-
Phys Ther. 2010;34(4):202–7. per RMA, Jones TA, Greenough WT. Selective
106. Reisman DS, Wityk R, Silver K, Bastian AJ. Split- synaptic plasticity within the cerebellar cortex
belt treadmill adaptation transfers to overground following complex motor skill learning. Neurobiol
walking in persons poststroke. Neurorehabil Neural Learn Mem. 1998;69(3):274–89.
Repair. 2009;23(7):735–44. 120. Kleim JA, Pipitone MA, Czerlanis C, Gree-
107. Reisman DS, McLean H, Keller J, Danks KA, nough WT. Structural stability within the lateral
Bastian AJ. Repeated split-belt treadmill training cerebellar nucleus of the rat following complex
improves poststroke step length asymmetry. Neu- motor learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1998;69
rorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27(5):460–8. (3):290–306.
108. Jauss M, Krieger D, Hornig C, Schramm J, 121. Benussi A, Koch G, Cotelli M, Padovani A,
Busse O. Surgical and medical management of Borroni B. Cerebellar transcranial direct current
patients with massive cerebellar infarctions: results stimulation in patients with ataxia: a double-blind,
of the German–Austrian cerebellar infarction study. randomized, sham-controlled study. Mov Disord.
J Neurol. 1999;246(4):257–64. 2015;30(12):1701–5.
109. Tohgi H, Takahashi S, Chiba K, Hirata Y. Cere- 122. Benussi A, Dell’Era V, Cantoni V, Bonetta E,
bellar infarction. Clinical and neuroimaging analy- Grasso R, Manenti R, et al. Cerebello-spinal tDCS
sis in 293 patients. The Tohoku Cerebellar in ataxia. Neurology. 2018;91(12):e1090-101.
Infarction Study Group. Stroke. 2018;24 123. Dietz V. Proprioception and locomotor disorders.
(11):1697–701. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(10):781–90.
110. Kelly PJ, Stein J, Shafqat S, Eskey C, Doherty D, 124. Dietz V. Do human bipeds use quadrupedal coor-
Chang Y, et al. Functional recovery after rehabil- dination? Trends Neurosci. 2002;25(9):462–7.
itation for cerebellar stroke. Stroke. 2001;32 125. Jenson L, Colombo G, Dietz V. Locomotor activity
(2):530–4. in spinal man. Gait Posture. 1995;3(3):180–1.
111. Morton SM, Tseng Y-W, Zackowski KM, 126. Dietz V, Wirz M, Colombo G, Curt A. Locomotor
Daline JR, Bastian AJ. Longitudinal tracking of capacity and recovery of spinal cord function in
gait and balance impairments in cerebellar disease: paraplegic patients: a clinical and electrophysiolog-
longitudinal tracking of cerebellar disease. Mov ical evaluation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuro-
Disord. 2010;25(12):1944–52. physiol Electromyogr Mot Control. 1998;109
112. Schoch B, Dimitrova A, Gizewski ER, Timmann D. (2):140–53.
Functional localization in the human cerebellum 127. Dietz V. Neurophysiology of gait disorders: present
based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of and future applications. Electroencephalogr Clin
90 patients. Neuroimage. 2006;30(1):36–51. Neurophysiol. 1997;103(3):333–55.
18 A. Luft et al.

128. Popovic MR, Curt A, Keller T, Dietz V. Functional 143. Dietz V, Müller R. Degradation of neuronal func-
electrical stimulation for grasping and walking: tion following a spinal cord injury: mechanisms and
indications and limitations. Spinal Cord. 2001;39 countermeasures. Brain. 2004;127(10):2221–31.
(8):403–12. 144. Dobkin BH, Harkema S, Requejo P, Edgerton VR.
129. Fung J, Stewart JE, Barbeau H. The combined Modulation of locomotor-like EMG activity in
effects of clonidine and cyproheptadine with inter- subjects with complete and incomplete spinal cord
active training on the modulation of locomotion in injury. J Neurol Rehabil. 1995;9(4):183–90.
spinal cord injured subjects. J Neurol Sci. 1990;100 145. Harkema SJ, Hurley SL, Patel UK, Requejo PS,
(1–2):85–93. Dobkin BH, Edgerton VR. Human lumbosacral
130. Wernig A, Müller S. Laufband locomotion with spinal cord interprets loading during stepping.
body weight support improved walking in persons J Neurophysiol. 1997;77(2):797–811.
with severe spinal cord injuries. Spinal Cord. 146. Gassert R, Dietz V. Rehabilitation robots for the
1992;30(4):229–38. treatment of sensorimotor deficits: a neurophysio-
131. Wernig A, Müller S, Nanassy A, Cagol E. Laufband logical perspective. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15
therapy based on “rules of spinal locomotion” is (1):46.
effective in spinal cord injured persons. Eur J 147. Dietz V, Sinkjaer T. Spastic movement disorder:
Neurosci. 1995;7(4):823–9. impaired reflex function and altered muscle
132. Wirz M, Colombo G, Dietz V. Long term effects of mechanics. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6(8):725–33.
locomotor training in spinal humans. J Neurol 148. Moseley AM, Stark A, Cameron ID, Pollock A.
Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;71(1):93. Treadmill training and body weight support for
133. Dietz V, Colombo G, Jensen L, Baumgartner L. walking after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev.
Locomotor capacity of spinal cord in paraplegic 2003;(3):CD002840.
patients. Ann Neurol. 1995;37(5):574–82. 149. Wirz M, Zemon DH, Rupp R, Scheel A,
134. Dietz V, Müller R, Colombo G. Locomotor activity Colombo G, Dietz V, et al. Effectiveness of
in spinal man: significance of afferent input from automated locomotor training in patients with
joint and load receptors. Brain. 2002;125(12):2626– chronic incomplete spinal cord injury: a multicenter
34. trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005;86(4):672–80.
135. Dietz V, Colombo G, Jensen L. Locomotor activity 150. Curt A, Dietz V. Ambulatory capacity in spinal cord
in spinal man. Lancet. 1994;344(8932):1260–3. injury: significance of somatosensory evoked poten-
136. Cramer SC, Riley JD. Neuroplasticity and brain tials and ASIA protocol in predicting outcome.
repair after stroke. Curr Opin Neurol. 2008;21 Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1997;78(1):39–43.
(1):76–82. 151. Curt A, Keck ME, Dietz V. Functional outcome
137. Dietz V, Harkema SJ. Locomotor activity in spinal following spinal cord injury: significance of motor-
cord-injured persons. J Appl Physiol. 2004;96 evoked potentials and ASIA scores. Arch Phys Med
(5):1954–60. Rehabil. 1998;79(1):81–6.
138. Martino G. How the brain repairs itself: new 152. Basso DM. Neuroanatomical substrates of func-
therapeutic strategies in inflammatory and degener- tional recovery after experimental spinal cord
ative CNS disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3 injury: implications of basic science research for
(6):372–8. human spinal cord injury. Phys Ther. 2000;80
139. Dietz V. Body weight supported gait training: from (8):808–17.
laboratory to clinical setting. Brain Res Bull. 153. Metz GAS, Curt A, van de Meent H, Klusman I,
2009;78(1):I–VI. Schwab ME, Dietz V. Validation of the weight-drop
140. Wirz M, Dietz V, Network EMS of SCI (EMSCI). contusion model in rats: a comparative study of
Recovery of sensorimotor function and activities of human spinal cord injury. J Neurotrauma. 2000;17
daily living after cervical spinal cord injury: the (1):1–17.
influence of age. J Neurotrauma. 2015;32(3):194–9. 154. Katoh S, el Masry W. Neurological recovery after
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24963966. conservative treatment of cervical cord injuries.
141. Dietz V, Grillner S, Trepp A, Hubli M, Bolliger M. J Bone Jt Surg Br Vol. 1994;76B(2):225–8.
Changes in spinal reflex and locomotor activity after 155. Dietz V, Wirz M, Curt A, Colombo G. Locomotor
a complete spinal cord injury: a common mecha- pattern in paraplegic patients: training effects and
nism? Brain. 2009;132(8):2196–205. recovery of spinal cord function. Spinal Cord.
142. Dietz V. Body weight supported gait training: from 1998;36(6):380–90.
laboratory to clinical setting. Brain Res Bull. 156. Schwab ME, Bartholdi D. Degeneration and regen-
2008;76(5):459–63. eration of axons in the lesioned spinal cord. Physiol
Rev. 1996;76(2):319–70.
Movement Neuroscience
Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 2
Robert L. Sainburg and Pratik K. Mutha

Abstract cerebral hemispheres. This organization leads


to hemisphere-specific motor deficits in both
Research into the neural control of movement
the ipsilesional and contralesional arms of
has elucidated important principles that can stroke patients. Ipsilesional deficits increase
provide guidelines to rehabilitation profession- with the severity of contralesional impairment
als for enhancing the recovery of motor func-
level and have a substantial effect on functional
tion in stroke patients. In this chapter, we independence. Finally, motor learning research
elaborate on principles that have been derived has indicated that different neural mechanisms
from research on neural control of movement,
underlie different aspects of motor learning,
including optimal control, impedance control, such as adaptation vs skill learning, and that
motor lateralization, and principles of motor learning different aspects of tasks can general-
learning. Research on optimal control has
ize across different coordinates. In this chapter,
indicated that two major categories of cost we discuss the neurobiological basis of these
contribute to motor planning: explicit task level principles and elaborate on the implications for
costs, such as movement accuracy and speed, designing and implementing occupational and
and implicit costs, such as energy and move- physical therapy treatment for movement
ment variability. Impedance control refers to deficits in stroke patients.
neural mechanisms that modulate rapid senso-
rimotor circuits, such as reflexes, in order to Keywords
impede perturbations that cannot be anticipated
prior to movement. Research on motor lateral- .
Rehabilitation Motor control . Motor
ization has indicated that different aspects of .
learning Motor lateralization
motor control have been specialized to the two

2.1 Introduction
R. L. Sainburg (&)
Kinesiology and Neurology, Penn State University,
University Park, PA 16801, United States Deficits that result from strokes in sensory and
e-mail: rls45@psu.edu motor regions of the brain represent a major
P. K. Mutha impediment to the recovery of function in activi-
Department of Biological Engineering and Centre for ties of daily living for stroke survivors. Such def-
Cognitive Science, Indian Institute of Technology icits most commonly include hemiparesis, a
Gandhinagar, Ahmedabad, Gujarat 382424, India
syndrome encompassing unilateral motor
e-mail: pm@iitgn.ac.in

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 19


D. J. Reinkensmeyer et al. (eds.), Neurorehabilitation Technology,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-08995-4_2
20 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

dysfunction on the side of the body opposite to the has been to make movements more “normal”.
brain lesion, and spasticity, characterized by Thus, the goal is to develop movement patterns
abnormally high muscle tone and atypical that are similar to those exhibited by non-impaired
expression of reflexes. Occupational and physical individuals. This idea emerged from the observa-
therapy interventions often focus on reducing tion that certain characteristics of movements
motor impairment, following stroke, by exposing made by healthy individuals are fairly similar
patients to a range of movement activities, with a within a given task, and even across tasks. For
major focus on repetitive experience or practice. In example, when reaching for an object in space,
general, the amount of practice corresponds to movement trajectories across healthy individuals
improvements in motor function, as measured by a appear fairly straight and smooth [7]. Such relia-
variety of scales [1]. Unfortunately, gains made bility of motor behavior is particularly interesting
during therapy often show the limited translation because of the abundance of possible solutions to
to activities of daily living (ADLs) and carry over most movement tasks and the variety of environ-
to the home environment. ments we move in. For example, when reaching
Over the past decade, rehabilitation approa- for a cup of coffee in front of us, we have the
ches have incorporated technological innovations choice of using one arm or both arms, standing up
that can provide more cost-effective means of or remaining seated, leaning the trunk forward or
achieving higher intensity practice over longer reaching further with the selected arm(s), twisting
periods of time. These computer-based and our trunk to require shoulder abduction, or keeping
robotic technologies [2–5] have been shown to it straight to require shoulder flexion, among other
match, or even exceed the efficacy of traditional options (see Fig. 2.1). In addition, the relative
therapy in promoting improvements in motor motions between our body segments can produce a
performance [6]. However, these interventions wide variety of curved trajectories of the hand, in
hold greater promise than simply replicating order to procure the cup. Each possible motion can
traditional therapy, by providing therapists with also be achieved at a variety of speeds, as well as a
an unprecedented ability to specify and measure variety of possible muscle activation patterns.
movement features such as speed, direction, There are literally infinite solutions to this
amplitude, as well as joint coordination patterns. simple task.
As these technologies become more readily Regardless of these vast possibilities, people
available in the clinic, the most pressing question tend to display movement patterns that are con-
is how therapists can best utilize them to accel- sistent across different instances of the same
erate recovery of function. In this chapter, we movement or even across different movements,
will discuss principles that have been derived whether made by the same or different individ-
from research in motor control and learning that uals. These similarities are often referred to as
could be applied to training strategies using “invariant characteristics” of movement. Many
computer-based movement interventions. studies have shown that when different people
make reaching movements, invariant character-
istics include approximate straightness of the
2.2 Principle 1: Optimal Control hand trajectory and smooth bell-shaped velocity
profiles (see Fig. 2.2) [7–11]. How do different
While most therapists recognize that practice and people arrive at similar solutions within and
repetition of motor activities lead to improvements across tasks despite the extensive redundancy in
in motor performance, a systematic identification the musculoskeletal system and the diversity and
of which movements should be practiced is often uncertainty of the environments we move in?
lacking. This is partly because the question of what One way to arrive at the “best” solution when
defines a desirable movement has yielded no clear confronted with many different options is to
answer. Traditionally, a common guiding princi- employ optimization strategies when planning
ple employed in occupational and physical therapy the movement. Optimization procedures have
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 21

Fig. 2.1 Different ways of picking up a coffee cup shoulder flexion, and more elbow extension. The right
starting from the same initial posture. The left pose pose shows some trunk flexion, shoulder abduction,
involves shoulder flexion and elbow extension. The elbow flexion, and forearm pronation
middle pose involves flexion of the trunk, slightly less

been developed for use in engineering applica- functions. For example, some researchers spec-
tions, and seek the minimum or maximum for a ulated that mechanical aspects of movements
given “cost function”, subject to a set of con- might reflect important costs for planning
straints. For example, we can find the minimum movements. Such cost functions have included
price of a pound of coffee (function) for all the mean squared torque change [10], peak work
stores within a 10-mile radius of our house [12], or muscle energy [13, 14] among others.
(constraint). Whereas this particular problem These models accounted for some experimental
may be quite trivial, optimization routines are observations that could not be accounted for by
typically employed to find values for more optimizations based on kinematic parameters
complex problems, such as might be applied to [11]. While minimization of cost functions such
human movement. Researchers have tested var- as smoothness or torque change accurately pre-
ious cost functions that make sense heuristically dicted average behavior, Wolpert and colleagues
and have shown that optimization of these costs [8] also accounted for the small, yet important
reproduces many invariant characteristics trial-to-trial variability seen during repetitions of
observed in human motion. For example, Flash the same task. They proposed that motor com-
and Hogan [9] tested the idea that the smoothness mands are corrupted with variability-inducing
of hand trajectories might reflect an important noise, and in the presence of such noise, the CNS
cost in the planning of reaching movements and seeks to minimize the variance of the final arm
proposed a model that minimized the jerkiness of position. This model also predicted many
the hand trajectory (mathematically defined as observed invariant characteristics of movements
the derivative of acceleration with respect to such as trajectory smoothness and the tradeoff
time). Their simulation predicted straight move- between movement accuracy and speed.
ments with symmetrical, single-peaked, bell- Two important inferences can be drawn from
shaped velocity profiles. studies that have attempted to explain movement
However, under several experimental condi- patterns based on optimization principles: (1) the
tions, minimum jerk trajectories and experimen- nature of the costs associated with different tasks
tally observed hand paths diverged, which led are often different and (2) costs such as endpoint
researchers to examine other plausible cost variability and mechanical energy do not reflect
22 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

Fig. 2.2 Some “invariant


characteristics” of point-to-
point movements. The top
panel shows fairly straight
hand trajectories for multiple
movements. The bottom panel
shows fairly similar bell-
shaped velocity profiles for
six different movements at
different speeds. When time-
normalized, the bell-shaped
velocity profiles closely
overlap. (Adapted from
Atkeson C.G. and
Hollerbach J.M. Kinematic
Features of Unrestrained
Vertical Arm Movements.
Journal of Neuroscience
1985, Voll. 5, No. 9.
pp. 2318–2330. Copyright
1985 Society for
Neuroscience)

variables that we tend to have conscious aware- planned. The role of sensory feedback mecha-
ness of, yet they appear to be accounted for nisms in these models is simply to correct devi-
during the process of motor planning. In other ations from the planned or desired trajectory,
words, the planning of movements not only regardless of whether these deviations resist or
entails explicit performance criteria that are assist in task completion. Thus, the output of
associated with successful task performance, feedback circuits is not incorporated in the opti-
such as getting hold of a cup of coffee, but also mization phase. More recently, the idea that the
entails implicit criteria that we don’t consciously determination of an optimal “control policy”
consider, such as making energetically efficient incorporates knowledge about the “state” of the
and reliable movements. body and the environment, as relayed by feed-
An important aspect of the models discussed back circuits and mechanisms that predict sen-
above is that optimization of a single cost func- sory consequences of motor commands, has
tion yields a desired trajectory that is then simply gained prominence. According to this idea, the
executed in an open-loop manner, once it is optimal solution is the best possible
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 23

transformation from the current state to the motor joint power will also become available. While
commands that aid in achieving the task goal most clinical assessments of function include
[15]. Not too surprisingly then, this optimal either the ability to perform certain ADL tasks
feedback control scheme yields task-specific cost (Functional Independence Measure—FIM [18,
functions that often represent a hybrid mix of 19], or the ability to perform simulated ADL
explicit task-level variables that relate to perfor- tasks in particular times (Jebsen-Taylor Hand
mance goals, such as movement precision, as Function Test—[20] we suggest that direct
well as implicit mechanically related costs that analysis of biomechanical costs may provide an
correspond to muscle force or effort. For exam- important supplement to these tests, as an indi-
ple, in a task that examined corrections to target cator of energetic efficiency. It may also be
displacements that occurred late in the move- important to assess one’s subjective sense of
ment, Liu and Todorov [16] showed that subject effort, which does not always accurately reflect
performance could be best described using a measures of biomechanical cost [21] This should
composite cost function that optimized for provide a valuable addition to therapeutic
movement duration, accuracy, endpoint stability, assessment because even when ADLs are com-
and energy consumption. More importantly, pleted independently if they are not performed
subjects implicitly changed the relative contri- within reasonable energetic costs, one might
bution of these costs as the accuracy and stability expect minimal carry over into the patient’s
requirements of the task were changed. Thus, spontaneous behavior.
rather than adopt a fixed policy across task It should be stressed that the role of task-level
conditions, subjects were able to flexibly adapt costs is also important for determining optimal
their control strategy in order to ensure maximum control strategies for a given task. Such costs
task success. These ideas of flexible control might include the accuracy and duration of
strategies and hybrid cost functions that include movements. Computer-based technologies allow
task-related and intrinsic biomechanical variables therapists to modify feedback to stress particular
have important implications for designing ther- performance criteria, so as to emphasize certain
apy regimes. costs. For example, in a targeted reaching task,
Implications for Rehabilitation. It is one could provide reward based on duration,
important to recognize that damage to the CNS when focusing on improving movement time.
from stroke and the associated secondary chan- However, if movement direction and straightness
ges in the musculoskeletal system could induce need to be stressed, visual feedback can be
changes in the set of possible solutions as well as modified to amplify errors perpendicular to the
the costs associated with any given task. There- desired trajectory while reducing errors in the
fore, patients may arrive at solutions to a motor direction of the desired movement. Such changes
task that may not look “normal”, but may be would penalize deviations from the desired
“optimal” given physiological and biomechanical movement path while allowing errors in the
pathologies [17] Thus, rather than simply direction of movement. This approach would
attempting to make movements look more assign different costs to errors that contribute to
“normal”, it is important to understand the task success versus those that don’t. In fact,
biomechanical costs associated with different Ballester et al. [22] recently reported exactly this
tasks. Most importantly, if movements of the manipulation, using a virtual reality environment
hemiparetic arm elicit energetic costs that are to train reaching hemiparetic stroke patients. The
substantially higher than those of the ipsilesional movements of a virtual representation of the
arm, it is very unlikely that the hemiparetic arm patients' paretic limb were amplified in only the
use will be spontaneously integrated into activi- dimension parallel to the target direction. Fol-
ties of daily living. As the technologies discussed lowing virtual reality training, the authors
in this volume become available in the clinic, reported that the probability of using the paretic
assessment of biomechanical variables, such as limb during a subsequent real-world task was
24 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

increased by the reinforcing experience of seeing cord. Following this, a medium latency response,
the virtual limb reach the target during training. M2, is observed some 60–80 ms following the
These types of capabilities are now becoming perturbation onset and is thought to reflect longer
available in the clinic, due to the increasing latency spinal as well as transcortical circuits.
availability of computer-based robotic and virtual This is followed by M3, a longer latency reaction
reality technologies. that is thought to reflect a voluntary corrective
process. Studies examining how these responses
are modulated have shown differential effects of
2.3 Principle 2: Impedance Control different task conditions on the early and later
phases of the reflex.
Optimal feedback control theory emphasizes that Early studies in which subjects were instruc-
the derivation of the optimal control signal ted to resist or to not resist a perturbation showed
incorporates knowledge about the state of the that M1 was not modified by such commands,
body and the environment. If the state changes while M2 could be greatly attenuated by the
unexpectedly due to an external perturbation, or instruction to not resist, and M3 could actually be
random noise, what should its influence be on the completely eliminated by this instruction [25].
control strategy? For example, when a passenger More recent studies have shown that M2 can be
in a vehicle drinks a cup of coffee, what should modulated by spatial conditions in a task, such as
the control system do when the movement of the when subjects are told to allow their hand to
cup is unexpectedly perturbed by a bump in the displace toward a particular target: when the arm
road? Ideally, the components of the perturbing is pushed toward the target, the later phases (M2,
forces that assist in bringing the cup to the mouth M3) of the stretch reflex that resist the pertur-
smoothly should not be impeded. However, the bation are reduced. However, when the arm is
components of the forces that resist in the pushed away from the target, the gains of these
achievement of the task goal, such as accelerat- responses are increased. More importantly, this
ing the cup too rapidly, or in the outward or modulation varies with both the direction and the
downward directions, should be compensated. distance of the target [18]. This demonstrates that
According to the principle of minimal interven- feedback circuits such as reflexes can be modu-
tion proposed by optimal feedback control, the lated in accord with task goals through implicit
central nervous system “intervenes” only when mechanisms. In fact modulation of reflexes
errors are detrimental to goal achievement. Such appears to be a fundamental mechanism that our
a selective compensation of errors might explain nervous system employs to control limb impe-
why people allow slight variability in their per- dance and thus resist perturbations. An elegant
formance as long as the overall goals of the task example of such reflex modulation was provided
are satisfied. by Lacquaniti and colleagues for a ball-catching
This type of selective modulation of feedback task [26]. This study demonstrated not only
gains is consistent with evidence that even the modulation but also reversal of the stretch reflex,
simplest feedback circuits, reflexes, can be in response to ball impact. Both the amplitude
modulated based on task demands. The stretch and expression of the stretch reflex were modu-
reflex represents the simplest and most ubiqui- lated in a systematic way as the ball dropped
tous feedback circuit in the mammalian system. toward the hand. The result of this reflex mod-
The typical response to a stretch of a muscle ulation was to generate impedance to the forces
includes a characteristic three-phase response imposed by ball impact, thereby generating a
[23, 24], measured in the electromyogram smooth and effective catching response.
(EMG) as shown in Fig. 2.3: the shortest latency Why is active impedance control through
response often referred to as M1 occurs within reflex modulation important for motor perfor-
some 20–50 ms following perturbation onset, mance? During everyday tasks, many environ-
and reflects circuitry contained within the spinal mental perturbations cannot be predicted prior to
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 25

Fig. 2.3 Typical reflex response to muscle stretch. An muscle stretch: the short-latency component M1 and the
example of the wrist extensor being stretched using a longer latency components M2 and M3. (Adapted from
motor is shown on the left. The right panel shows the Matthews PB. The human stretch reflex and the motor
typical components of the electromyographic response to cortex. Trends Neurosci. 1991 Mar;14(3):87–91.)

movement. In the example of a passenger is able to simultaneously maintain stability


drinking coffee in a moving vehicle, changes in through impedance control and coordinate
vehicle acceleration due to bumps and breaking movements in a manner consistent with opti-
can rarely be anticipated. One can increase mized energy expenditure.
overall arm stiffness by co-activating muscles, We recently showed that such selective
but this uses a great deal of metabolic energy and modification of limb impedance occurs through
interferes with the ability to bring the cup to the continuous modulation of short- and long-latency
mouth. Franklin [27] and colleagues directly reflexes [28]. In our study, participants reached a
tested how subjects might selectively modify visual target that occasionally jumped to a new
impedance without interfering with the coordi- location during movement initiation, thus
nation of the intended movement. In this study, changing the task goal during the course of
subjects performed reaching movements with the motion. Unpredictable mechanical perturbations
arm attached to a robotic manipulandum that were occasionally applied, 100 ms after the tar-
imposed unstable force fields that had compo- get jump. Our results showed that reflex
nents directed perpendicular to the required responses were tuned to the direction of the tar-
movement (see Fig. 2.4a). With practice, the get jump: response amplitudes were increased or
participants were able to adapt to the novel decreased depending on whether the perturbation
dynamics and produce straight trajectories. They opposed or assisted the achievement of the new
achieved this adaptation by selectively increasing task goal, respectively. We also showed that this
stiffness in the direction of the instability, but not reflex modulation resulted in changes in limb
along the movement direction (see Fig. 2.4b). impedance to the perturbations. However, under
Remarkably, at the joint level, this impedance conditions in which the movements were not
modification was achieved without changing mechanically perturbed, no changes in EMG or
baseline force and torque profiles (see Fig. 2.4c): joint torque occurred at reflex latency relative to
the coordination strategy remained kinetically movements made with mechanical perturbations.
efficient, even though subjects were also able to These findings supported those of Franklin and
effectively impede the imposed perturbations. colleagues by confirming that limb impedance is
These authors concluded that the nervous system controlled without interfering with optimal
26 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

a b 1,000 1,000

Endpoint stiffness X-axis [ N/m]

Endpoint stiffness y-axis [ N/m]


800 800

600 600

Target 400 400

200 200
Force
Field
0 0
S1 S2 S3 S4 S5 S1 S2 S3 S4 S5
Subject Subject
Start
c 160

Shoulder joint stiffness


Y
120

[Nm/rad]
X 80
0,0
40

0
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50
Shoulder torque [Nm]
100

80
Elbow joint stiffness
[Nm/rad]

60

40

20

0
0 2 4 6 8 10 12
Elbow torque [Nm]

Fig. 2.4 Modulation of limb impedance. a. The typical joint stiffnesses were independent of the respective joint
setup and the perturbing force field. The field acts to push torques. Adapted from Franklin DW, So U, Kawato M,
the arm perpendicular (along X-axis) to the direction of Milner TE. Impedance control balances stability with
motion (Y-axis). b. An increase in limb stiffness along the metabolically costly muscle activation. J Neurophysiol.
X-, but not Y-axis for all subjects. c. Shoulder and elbow 2004 Nov;92(5):3097–105

coordination, by selectively modulating the perturbations are countered in a task-specific


expression of short- and long-latency reflex manner. Reflexive resistance to a perturbation is
responses. increased when it is inconsistent with the task
The studies discussed above point to the goal but decreased when the perturbation is
remarkable ability of the nervous system to congruent with the goal of the task. These find-
determine optimal responses to unpredictable ings agree with the “minimum intervention
situations. Such control policies appear to principle” within the optimal feedback control
mediate the modulation of limb impedance framework. Thus, controlling limb impedance in
through the regulation of feedback circuits such a task-specific manner appears to be an integral
as reflexes to ensure that unexpected component of the motor control process.
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 27

Implications for Rehabilitation. The 2.4 Principle 3: Motor


research summarized above indicates that the Lateralization
central nervous system invokes at least two
aspects of control to achieve coordinated move- As discussed thus far, both optimal control and
ments. First, the commands are specified that impedance control are component mechanisms
result in optimal coordination patterns that satisfy underlying the control of voluntary movements.
both costs associated with task performance and Our recent work has suggested that these two
energetic costs. In addition, the nervous system mechanisms are lateralized to the left and right
appears to set control policies that modulate brain hemispheres, respectively. The seminal
sensorimotor circuits such as reflexes, to account research of Sperry and Gazzaniga [30] on dis-
for perturbations from unexpected changes in connection syndrome in split-brain patients first
environmental or internal conditions. The established neural lateralization as a fundamental
importance of recognizing both of these features principle of the cerebral organization. Gazzaniga
of control in clinical environments is funda- proposed that distributing different neural pro-
mentally important because brain damage due to cesses across the hemispheres was a natural
stroke can have differential effects on these two consequence of developing complex functions
aspects of coordination. For example, Beer et al. during the course of evolution. His research
[29] showed that hemiparesis disrupts optimal provided elegant support for this view of cerebral
intersegmental coordination, resulting in ineffi- lateralization as a neural optimization process.
cient coordination that fails to account for the Interestingly, early research on hemispheric
dynamic interactions between the segments. This lateralization was largely limited to cognitive and
deficit does not appear to depend on the extent of perceptual processes, with little attention to the
hemiparesis. motor systems. We introduced the dynamic
Traditional therapeutic strategies, as well as dominance hypothesis of motor lateralization
more recent robot-aided rehabilitation strategies, [31], based on left- and right-arm advantages in
tend to target the optimal control process by reaching performance in healthy adults, and
practicing fairly consistent patterns of coordina- expanded this hypothesis based on computational
tion, and reinforcing task success. While this modeling studies [32, 33] and studies in patients
type of practice is critical for improving coordi- with unilateral brain lesions [34–39]. The
nation and voluntary control, focusing on repet- dynamic dominance model proposes that the left
itive movements under consistent environmental hemisphere, in right-handers, is specialized for
conditions should only be the first step in reha- predictive processes that specify smooth and
bilitation training. In itself, this training may efficient movement trajectories under mechani-
improve voluntary control of optimal coordina- cally stable environmental circumstances, while
tion patterns, but is unlikely to train impedance the right hemisphere is specialized for impedance
control mechanisms. Because of this, patients control mechanisms that confer robustness to
may become adept at the training protocols, but movements performed under unpredictable and
show limited transfer to activities of daily living. mechanically unstable environmental conditions.
We suggest that as patients improve their In fact, this type of division of labor between the
movement patterns under predictable conditions, two sides of the brain appears to predate humans
training protocols should progressively incorpo- by half a billion years [40]. Rogers and col-
rate unpredictable conditions. Such conditions leagues have proposed a single organizing prin-
might include random changes in target posi- ciple that might account for the large array of
tions and varying force perturbations, thereby emotional, language, perceptual, and cognitive
training patients to impede variations in envi- asymmetries that have been described across the
ronmental conditions that interfere with task evolutionary spectrum of vertebrates. While the
performance. left hemisphere appears “specialized for control
28 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

of well-established patterns of behavior, under light on motor lateralization. These studies have
ordinary and familiar circumstances”, the right confirmed that right and left sensorimotor strokes
hemisphere is specialized for “detecting and produce predictable deficits in impedance control
responding to unexpected stimuli in the envi- or optimal control, respectively [51, 62]. For
ronment” [41]. The dynamic dominance model example, Schaefer et al. [51] compared reaching
provides the movement analog to Roger’s model, movements in the ipsilesional arm of
and thus places handedness in the context of a hemisphere-damaged patients with those of
larger array of neurobehavioral asymmetries healthy control subjects matched for age and
across the animal kingdom [42]. other demographic factors. Subjects performed
An important feature of these models is that targeted reaching movements in different direc-
both hemispheres are recruited for their compli- tions within a workspace to the same side of the
mentary contributions to integrated functional midline as their reaching arm. The left
activities. Thus, during the movement of a single hemisphere-damaged group showed deficits in
arm, both hemispheres contribute their specific controlling the arm’s trajectory due to impaired
aspects of control.[43]. Because each hemisphere interjoint coordination but showed no deficits in
contributes specialized processes to control each achieving accurate final positions. In contrast, the
arm, unilateral brain damage actually produces right hemisphere-damaged group showed deficits
hemisphere-specific movement deficits in the in final position accuracy but not in interjoint
non-paretic, ipsilesional arm, as well as the coordination. These findings are exemplified in
contralesional arm. Remarkably, this is the arm the hand paths shown in Fig. 2.5a. While control
that is usually considered unaffected by unilateral subjects made relatively straight and accurate
brain damage. The idea that each hemisphere movements, patients with left hemisphere dam-
contributes to motor coordination of both arms is age made movements that were very curved, but
an important implication of ipsilesional, non- nevertheless were accurate in the final position.
paretic arm motor deficits. While the role of In contrast, patients with right hemisphere dam-
contralateral motor areas in controlling limb age made straight movements with poor final
movements is well-understood [44] the role of position accuracies. This double dissociation
the ipsilateral hemisphere has more recently been between the type of error (trajectory or final
implicated by the robust occurrence of ipsile- position) and the side of hemisphere damage
sional motor deficits in both animal models of (right or left) is emphasized in Fig. 2.5b, which
unilateral brain damage [45–47] as well as shows the variance in hand positions during the
human stroke survivors [34, 36, 39, 48–58]. In initial trajectory phase (cross), or the final posi-
addition, both electrophysiological and neural tion phase (circle) of the movement. The ratio of
imaging studies have shown that unilateral arm errors at these two points in movement (peak
and hand movements recruit motor-related areas velocity, movement termination) is quantified
in both cerebral hemispheres [43, 59–61]. Thus, across subjects in the bar graphs, revealing that
it is the loss of the contributions of the ipsilateral RHD patients had the greatest variance in final
hemisphere to movement control that gives rise position, while LHD patients had the greatest
to motor deficits in the non-paretic arm of stroke variance in trajectory. Thus, these results indicate
patients. Most importantly, these deficits can the distinct lateralization of optimal trajectory
substantially limit functional performance [51, control and impedance-mediated final position
54], a particularly concerning phenomenon, control to the left and right hemispheres,
given that patients with severe contralesional respectively. It should be emphasized that these
paresis depend on the ipsilesional arm for the errors were associated with functional impair-
majority of their activities of daily living. ments in the ipsilesional arm, as measured by the
Our recent studies have examined the specific Jebsen–Taylor Hand Function Test (JHFT).
nature of the ipsilesional movement deficits that Thus, motor lateralization leads to deficits that
result from left or right brain damage, shedding depend on the side of the stroke and can lead to
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 29

a Left arm (L) Right arm (R)

Healthy
control (HC) 5 cm

Hemisphere
damage (HD)

Lateral Medial Medial Lateral

b
LHD RHD
Position at peak velocity
Final position

5 cm

1.1
At peak velocity > Final
Ratio of variables

1
error (cm)

Final > At peak velocity


0.7

LHC LHD RHC RHD

Fig. 2.5 Lateralization of motor deficits after stroke. 6A confidence intervals. The bottom panel shows the mean
shows typical hand paths for healthy control subjects ratio of variable errors at peak velocity to variable error at
performing with their right or left arm (top panel) and left movement end across all subjects for the control and
and right hemisphere-damaged stroke patients performing stroke groups. (Adapted from Schaefer SY, Haaland KY,
with their ipsilesional arm (bottom panel). 6B shows hand Sainburg RL. Hemispheric specialization and functional
locations at peak velocity (crosses) and movement end impact of ipsilesional deficits in movement coordina-
(circles) for a typical left and right hemisphere-damaged tion and accuracy. Neuropsychologia. 2009 Nov;47
stroke patient (top panel). Ellipses represent 95% (13):2953–66.)
30 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

400
Percentage Time to Complete JTHFT

350

300

250

200

150

100

50

0
Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right
Control Mild Moderate Severe
Hand within Severity

Fig. 2.6 JTHFT score, normalized to control group right- and included 18 participants each. The left hemisphere-
hand score. Scores for non-paretic arm of stroke survivors. damaged group comprised 22 stroke survivors, whereas the
Control subjects were matched to gender and age distribu- right hemisphere-damaged group comprised 29 stroke
tion of each stroke survivor group. The two control groups survivors. On the X-axis, these groups are stratified by
were comprised of those that used their left or right hands severity of contralesional arm paresis

significant deficits, as tested with clinical longer than the right arm to carry out these tasks.
assessments, such as the JHFT. For reference, this reflects the frustration a typi-
Figure 2.6 shows data from 72 age and cal adult would experience when trying to get
gender-matched control subjects, 22 left through their day with only the non-dominant
hemisphere-damaged stroke survivors, and 29 arm, for example, due to a broken dominant arm.
right hemisphere-damaged stroke survivors. The In our stroke survivors, there is a substantial
Y-axis represents the JHFT score, taken as a effect of both the severity of impairment in the
percentage of the right dominant arm function in paretic arm, as well as the side of the brain lesion
our control group. Thus, 100% is the mean for on JTHF performance with the non-paretic arm.
the right hand of 36 of the control subjects (those First, the more severe the contralesional paresis,
who used their right hand). The JHFT is a rather the greater the impairment in the non-paretic arm.
thorough assessment of unilateral arm function This effect is potentiated by the side of lesion,
that includes a large range of tasks that elicit the such that left hemisphere-damaged survivors
coordination requirements of functional daily who have severe paresis in their contralesional
activities, such as writing, turning pages, placing arm, take 216% longer to complete the JTHF
large and small objects on a table, stacking than the dominant arm of control subjects,
checkers, and feeding. The left column (control) whereas, right hemisphere-damaged survivors
shows the difference between healthy subjects with severe contralesional paresis, take 51%
performing with the left arm and right arm. longer than do control subjects. Functionally, this
The data are stratified on the X-axis by both effect is concerning for two reasons: First, the
hands (right/left: in the case of stroke survivors finding that the extent of ipsilesional deficit
this is only the ipsilesional arm) and severity of varies with the extent of contralesional paresis
contralesional paresis, as measured by the upper indicates that the survivors who must depend
limb component of the Fugl–Meyer [63] assess- most on the ipsilesional arm for function have the
ment of motor impairment (mild >= 55, moder- greatest impairments in that arm. Second, these
ate > 35, Severe <= 35). In healthy subjects, the stroke survivors were tested, on average
left non-dominant arm takes, on average, 33% 1.8 years following their stroke, suggesting that
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 31

these deficits do not spontaneously change over successfully addressed by constraining the non-
time. Even right hemisphere-damaged patients paretic arm in patients with moderate to mild
with severe paresis take nearly 52% longer than paresis [66–69]. While the pilot results cited
aged-matched control subjects to complete the above suggest positive effects of non-paretic arm
JTHF, regardless of the “forced use” of the training on paretic arm function, there currently
ipsilesional arm imposed by severe contrale- is no conclusive evidence to predict whether non-
sional paresis. This introduces the questions of paretic arm training will influence paretic arm
whether focused remedial therapy might improve function, either positively or negatively. This is
function by increasing the speed and dexterity of an important area for future research in reha-
the non-paretic arm in patients with moderate to bilitation intervention for stroke patients.
severe contralesional paresis. In contrast to focused non-paretic arm training,
Implications for Rehabilitation. While most bilateral training has a long history in rehabilita-
robotic rehabilitation devices have been focused tion research and practice and should represent a
on training movements in the contralesional arm, critical component of therapeutic intervention in
the research discussed above provides com- unilateral stroke. In fact, most activities of daily
pelling evidence that ipsilesional practice should living are performed with both hands contributing
also be encouraged. In fact, for many patients, to different aspects of the activity [54, 64]. For
the ipsilesional arm will become the primary example, when buttoning a shirt, the non-
manipulator, thus efficient coordination of this dominant arm tends to stabilize the buttonhole,
arm and hand should be critical for the effective while the dominant arm manipulates the button
performance of activities of daily living [64]. through the hole. Bilateral training is not only
It is, thus likely that intensive training of the important to facilitate remediation in the ipsile-
ipsilesional, non-paretic arm could substantially sional arm but also because unilateral training
improve functional independence in patients with may not automatically carry over to spontaneous
hemiparesis. However, it should be noted that bilateral performance. In fact, recent research has
remediation of the non-paretic arm is so novel indicated that learning novel kinetic and visuo-
that little empirical evidence exists as to whether motor environments with a single arm transfers
such intervention might lead to positive effects on only partially to bilateral movements, in which the
motor performance and functional indepen- same arm experiences the imposed environments
dence. One recent pilot intervention study com- [70, 71]. It is, therefore critical that rehabilitation
pared a group of patients who received therapy focus not only on unilateral performance but that
that included training of the non-paretic arm to training be extended to bilateral movements.
another group who only received traditional While some robotic devices are designed for
therapy, without non-paretic arm training [65]. bilateral movements [72], unilateral robotic
The results indicated that when traditional ther- training can be followed by bilateral training, even
apy was combined with non-paretic arm training, in the absence of bilateral robotic systems. In fact,
the speed and accuracy of non-paretic arm bilateral training has a long history in Occupa-
movements improved, as did the impairment tional Therapy treatment, where manipulation of
level of the paretic arm when compared to dowels and rolling pins has often been used to
patients who received traditional therapy alone. encourage bilateral arm use.
This suggests that focused non-paretic arm More importantly is the question of whether
training might produce both improvements in remediation focused on the non-paretic arm
non-paretic arm motor performance and modest might improve stroke survivors’ participation in
improvements in paretic arm function, both of daily activities, for those patients who rely on
which should facilitate improvements in func- this arm as their sole or primary manipulator and
tional independence. However, some caution is have substantial ipsilesional motor deficits. Cur-
indicated because of the phenomenon of learned rently, the usual standard of care in rehabilitation
non-use of the paretic arm, an effect that has been for patients with low-moderate to severe paresis
32 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

tends to focus on task training in essential ADL movement patterns to various kinds of altered
activities, rather than on intensive remediation. environments. Typically, subjects are exposed to
We suggest that the combination of moderate to novel task conditions such as when a cursor,
severe paresis with persistent motor deficits in representing the location of the hand on a screen,
the non-paretic arm limits the performance and deviates from the actual hand location, or when
participation in activities of daily living. We, the hand is pushed from its intended trajectory
thus, predict that intense rehabilitation, sequen- using force perturbations. Under such conditions,
tially focused on each arm should provide a subjects readily adapt to the new environment, a
durable and substantial improvement in func- process that appears to occur, at least in part,
tional performance. However, this approach must through changes in predictive control, or in other
be addressed with some caution because while words, movement planning [81, 82]. The pre-
sequential arm training has never been studied in dictive nature of such adaptation is reflected by
human stroke survivors, Jones et al. [73] showed, the occurrence of “aftereffects” following
in an acute model of stroke in rats, that initial removal of the imposed environmental pertur-
training of ipsilesional forelimb reaches can limit bation. Such after-effects tend to mirror image
the subsequent response to training in the con- the movement patterns seen on early exposure to
tralesional forelimb (2010). On the other hand, the imposed perturbation, and are based on the
interlimb transfer of motor learning often shows subject’s expectation that they will continue to
a positive effect in healthy individuals [74–77], experience the novel environment. In other
and mirror training has shown positive transfer words, the effects of the perturbation are pre-
between the arms in stroke patients [78–80]. It is dicted and accounted for, and the motor output is
critical to carry out studies of ipsilesional arm appropriately modified [83, 84]. Computation-
intervention in survivors with moderate to severe ally, such adaptation can be modeled as an iter-
contralesional paresis to determine whether such ative update of a forward model, defined as a
training can positively affect functional outcomes transformation from movement commands to
and participation in human stroke survivors. their desired sensory consequences. In this
scheme, sensory prediction errors, or the differ-
ence between the intended and actual sensory
2.5 Principle 4: Motor Learning feedback should drive the process of improving
the accuracy of the forward model so that the
The discussion so far noted that rehabilitation predicted sensory consequences of motor com-
should focus on improving both optimal control mands coincide with the actual sensory feedback.
as well as impedance control while bearing in This process has been shown to occur implicitly
mind that these control mechanisms are likely [83], although new research suggests that adap-
lateralized to different brain hemispheres. How- tation may also involve explicit or declarative
ever, rehabilitation itself rests on the assumption strategies [85] as well as reinforcement mecha-
that patients can re-learn such control with nisms that are driven by task success [86].
repeated practice. As such, knowledge of how In order to examine how motor learning might
motor learning occurs, how it is retained, and be represented in the nervous system, many stud-
how it generalizes to other conditions that ies have examined conditions to which the learn-
haven’t been practiced, is central to the devel- ing generalizes. Interestingly, these studies have
opment of effective rehabilitation strategies. generally suggested that the generalization of
Motor learning is used as an umbrella term to visuomotor adaption is different from the gener-
incorporate any practice-related improvement in alization of adaptation to novel dynamic condi-
motor performance. The primary paradigm used tions such as force fields. For example, Krakauer
in recent motor learning research has been et al. [87] examined the generalization of visuo-
focused on fairly short-term motor adaptation, motor adaptation and found that subjects gener-
where researchers have explored adjustments in alized to movements that were made in the same
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 33

direction, but from a different starting configura- properties of motor cortical or cerebellar neurons
tion of the arm. We have also shown that such could not reproduce behavioral data. Thus, more
adaptation can transfer between the limbs [31]. recent findings have strongly implicated posterior
These results are consistent with other studies that parietal regions for adaptation, particularly under
have suggested adaptation to errors introduced at conditions in which visuomotor errors are
the extrinsic task-level transfers along with the imposed. The neural substrates critical for
same coordinates [88, 89]. Generalization of dynamic adaptation are less clear.
adaptation to dynamic conditions such as novel In contrast to adaptation, which requires
force fields in contrast has been shown to occur improvement in performance in response to
along with intrinsic or joint coordinates [77, 90]. environmentally induced errors, learning in the
Malfait et al. [91] in fact showed that learning of absence of such errors has not been as extensively
novel force fields transferred to movements made studied. Newer studies term such learning in the
in different regions of the workspace, but that absence of sensory prediction errors as “skill
required similar joint excursions, but poorly to learning”, and it is thought that mechanisms that
movements in which joint excursions changed. drive learning of new skills are different from
Thus, the representation of the applied force field those that drive adaptation [98]. Behaviorally,
appeared to be linked to joint motions, or intrinsic adaptation only focuses on return to baseline level
coordinates. Mussa-Ivaldi and colleagues [92] of performance in the presence of error-inducing
have proposed that the generalization of learning perturbations, progresses rapidly, is short-lived,
novel mechanical conditions is tightly linked to the and shows limited generalization. In contrast, skill
dynamic state of the arm, indicated by the velocity learning occurs over much slower time scales and
and positions of the arm experienced during learned skills are rarely forgotten (86]. Research
learning. In support of this idea, when novel suggests that learning skills may recruit
dynamics are learned with the dominant arm, they reinforcement-like processes, where a successful
appear to transfer to the non-dominant arm along action is found through trial and error and is then
with intrinsic coordinates [77, 90]. Thus, while repeated since it leads to a rewarding outcome.
learning of novel visual-motor conditions appears However, this needs to be explored further.
to generalize in extrinsic coordinates, learning of Neurophysiologically, skill learning has been
novel dynamic conditions appears to transfer mapped onto substrates that appear to be different
along with intrinsic coordinates. from adaptation. For instance, the primary motor
To explore the neural basis of adaptation, cortex and basal ganglia are believed to be crucial
which has important implications for rehabilita- for learning new skills, but not for adaptation. For
tion post Stroke, we recently examined the impact example, transcranial magnetic stimulation
of different brain lesions on the ability to adapt to applied over M1 does not appear to impair adap-
novel visuomotor conditions. In general, we have tation [99], but facilitation of M1 via anodal direct
found that left hemisphere damage, particularly to current stimulation enhances skill learning [99],
posterior parietal regions, impairs visuomotor suggesting that M1 might play a different role in
adaptation [39]. Our results significantly expan- these two processes.
ded on prior studies that focused on the cerebel- Despite these differences, however, there is
lum as the neural substrate critical for adaptation good reason to believe that adaptation and skill
[93–96]. Our results also agreed with Tanaka et al. learning processes interact during the learning of
[97] who showed that experimentally observed real-life tasks. For instance, recent results suggest
visuomotor adaptation and generalization patterns that even in what would otherwise be classified as
could be reproduced using a population-coding a pure adaptation task, reinforcement mechanisms
model in which adaptation induced changes in the are recruited [86]. Under certain conditions,
synaptic weights between narrowly tuned, adaptation to errors can in fact be driven com-
parietal-like neurons and units in the motor cortex. pletely by reward-based reinforcement mecha-
Importantly, models that utilized tuning nisms [100]. Other mechanisms, including the use
34 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

of explicit strategies [101, 102] and declarative dynamic requirements of the two tasks, in terms
memory [85] have also been suggested to con- of both postural and limb movement require-
tribute significantly during motor learning. ments. Whether the two tasks are similar in terms
Implications for Rehabilitation. The array of of joint torques, or joint power might depend on
findings on motor learning and its underlying subtle differences in body configurations, and
neural substrates have several potential implica- relative segment motions. This would be difficult
tions for rehabilitation. First, it is critical to rec- to determine for any given target task, let alone a
ognize that multiple mechanisms, presumably large range of ADL activities. It is, therefore,
dependent on distinct neural substrates, con- important to provide a great deal of variation in
tribute to an improvement in motor performance dynamic experience when practicing a given
with practice. Loss of a particular component task, particularly as patients become proficient at
process because of focal lesions in different a given set of movement patterns. Robotic and
regions of the brain therefore does not automat- technology-aided rehabilitation, which have the
ically imply a complete loss of learning capacity. capacity to provide a large range of interactive
Different processes and alternate “routes” can be visual and dynamic environments along with the
exploited for improvement in motor function. For capacity for high-intensity and high-dose prac-
instance, for a patient with an injury to parietal tice, hold great promise in this regard.
regions, which might affect his/her capacity to
adapt to a novel environment, reinforcement
mechanisms could be exploited for learning in 2.6 Summary and Conclusions
the same environment. Second, given that adap-
tation and skill might recruit different neural As the technology-based intervention tools dis-
resources, rehabilitation approaches must focus cussed in this volume enter the clinic, they will
on training or facilitating both these processes, provide rehabilitation professionals with the
possibly along with other mechanisms such as ability to prescribe and monitor movement
the use of explicit strategies and declarative experiences with unprecedented precision. This
memory processes. Third, the fact that learning introduces the question of what specific aspects
might occur and generalize in different coordi- of movement should be practiced and monitored
nate systems must be taken into account. While with these tools. In this chapter, we presented
learning in environments that perturb perfor- four tenets derived from research in movement
mance in the extrinsic, task space allows adap- neuroscience that have an impact on this ques-
tation to task constraints, such as improving the tion, and that have been derived from literature
accuracy and precision, learning in dynamic on the neural control of movement. These tenets
environments allows the central nervous system are optimal control, impedance control, motor
to optimize intrinsic coordination and mechanical lateralization, and motor learning. We will
energy. It is therefore important for therapists to review these principles and the implications for
consider both intrinsic and extrinsic aspects of rehabilitation below:
task performance. It is typical to consider the Optimal control theory has examined plausi-
similarities between two tasks in terms of ble costs that might be considered by the nervous
extrinsic, task-related coordinates because one during motor planning, and that might account
can readily determine whether the task is in the for the reliable, or “invariant,” features of
same region of space, is oriented similarly, and is movements that occur across tasks and individ-
performed at similar speeds as the task or tasks uals. This line of research has indicated two
that are targeted for transfer. For example, one major categories of cost that contribute to motor
can practice stacking cones on a surface and planning: explicit task level costs, such as
expect that this might transfer to the task of movement accuracy and speed, and implicit
procuring a glass from the cupboard (target ADL costs, such as energy and movement variability.
skill). However, one must also consider the When designing movement practice for patients,
2 Movement Neuroscience Foundations of Neurorehabilitation 35

it is important to consider both types of costs, damage and deficits in achieving accurate steady-
when grading the difficulty of the task. We also state positions following right hemisphere dam-
suggest that it is critical to consider biomechan- age. The implications for rehabilitation are sub-
ical variables related to energetic efficiency when stantial: patients with persistent hemiparesis will
evaluating patients’ progress. While many clini- need to use the ipsilesional arm as the lead, or
cal tests assess the ability to perform ADLs, as often the sole, manipulator for activities of daily
well as the time of such performance, a critical living. Thus, efficient performance of ADL will
factor that should determine carryover into require well-coordinated movements of this arm.
spontaneous daily activities is whether the This is particularly important for patients who
movement can be performed at a reasonable have severe contralesional paresis, which tends
energy cost. As the technologies discussed in this to be associated with substantial ipsilesional
volume become available in the clinic, many of motor deficits. Intensive training focused on the
the devices will allow measures of mechanically ipsilesional arm can improve coordination, but
related variables, such as work, power, and tor- research is needed to determine whether this will
que. Such variables can be exploited to monitor impact function either positively or negatively, of
progress in making not only accurate and rapid the contralesional arm. Because most ADL tasks
but also energetically efficient movements. require some degree of bilateral coordination, we
Impedance control refers to neural mecha- recommend that following sequential unilateral
nisms that modulate rapid sensorimotor circuits, training with each arm, both arms be trained
such as stretch reflexes, in order to impede per- simultaneously using bilateral tasks. Virtual
turbations that cannot be anticipated during reality environments provide an excellent para-
motor planning. These include forces that arise digm to manipulate task conditions during
from the environment, such as inertial forces that bilateral arm training, such as requiring both
result from braking and acceleration of a vehicle, arms to coordinate with each other for goal
or even inaccurate movements of one’s own achievement and manipulating virtual objects.
body, such as the effect on the upper body and Motor learning research has shown that mul-
arms of stepping on an uneven surface while tiple brain regions represent distinct motor
holding a cup. Robot-aided and virtual reality learning processes. These processes include skill
technologies allow the introduction of “pertur- learning, in which one develops new sensori-
bations into patients’ movement training experi- motor patterns that were not previously learned,
ence. While it is currently most common to and adaptation, in which one learns to compen-
practice repetitive patterns under stereotyped sate for an environmental or sensory disturbance
conditions, introducing unpredictable perturba- in order to perform a previously well-practiced
tions should consolidate this learning and prepare task, such as reaching a force field, or under the
patients for movement under natural environ- influence of altered visuomotor feedback. It
mental conditions. should be stressed that as stroke survivors learn
Motor lateralization research has indicated to adapt to their new sensory and motor condi-
that different aspects of motor control have been tions, both of these forms of learning should be
specialized to the different cerebral hemispheres. required. Even well-learned tasks, such as
The hypothesis that both hemispheres are nor- brushing one’s teeth, may require substantially
mally recruited for each respective control new skill development, given altered motor
mechanism, optimal trajectory control, and capacities. Similarly, distortions in sensory
impedance control, predicts that damage to a feedback including visual field deficits, and
single hemisphere should produce deficits in the proprioceptive and tactile deficits can require
ipsilesional arm, often considered the unaffected adaptation to recover old skills. Generalization is
arm in stroke patients. Recent research has veri- also an aspect of motor learning with particular
fied this prediction, demonstrating deficits in application to neurorehabilitation. It should be
trajectory control following left hemisphere stressed that one cannot assume a particular
36 R. L. Sainburg and P. K. Mutha

pattern of motor generalization, following train- 10. Uno Y, Kawato M, Suzuki R. Formation and
ing. This is because some aspects of learning control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint
arm movement. Minimum torque-change model.
transfer along with different coordinates than Biol Cybern. 1989;61(2):89–101.
others. For example, task dynamics seems to be 11. Atkeson CG, Hollerbach JM. Kinematic features of
learned and transferred in intrinsic coordinates, unrestrained vertical arm movements. J Neurosci.
whereas visuomotor distortions are transferred 1985;5(9):2318–30.
12. Soechting JF, Buneo CA, Herrmann U, Flanders M.
across extrinsic coordinates. Since it is not sim- Moving effortlessly in three dimensions: does
ple, or even possible, to segregate these aspects Donders’ law apply to arm movement? J Neurosci.
of learning in a clinical environment, it is 1995;15(9):6271–80.
important to provide a range of training experi- 13. Goble DJ, Brown SH. Task-dependent asymmetries
in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback for
ences that can ensure generalization across a goal-directed movement. Exp Brain Res.
range of tasks. Task-specific training, of course, 2007 Jul;180(4):693–704.
should be done for key activities of daily living, 14. Goble DJ, Brown SH. The biological and behav-
but limiting training to specific tasks severely ioral basis of upper limb asymmetries in sensori-
motor performance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
limits the potential of physical rehabilitation. We 2008;32(3):598–610.
therefore strongly recommend providing a range 15. Todorov E, Jordan MI. Optimal feedback control as
of dynamic and kinematic training experiences a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci.
that include the requirement for variability, and 2002;5(11):1226–35.
16. Liu D, Todorov E. Evidence for the flexible
response to unpredictable perturbations. sensorimotor strategies predicted by optimal feed-
back control. J Neurosci. 2007;27(35):9354–68.
17. Latash ML. What are “normal movements” in atypical
References populations? Behav Brain Sci. 1996;19:55–68.
18. Pruszynski JA, Kurtzer I, Scott SH. Rapid motor
responses are appropriately tuned to the metrics of a
1. Teasell R, Meyer MJ, McClure A, et al. Stroke visuospatial task. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100(1):224–38.
rehabilitation: an international perspective. Topics 19. Wright PJ, Fortinsky RH, Covinsky KE, Ander-
in stroke rehabilitation. 2009;16(1):44–56. son PA, Landefeld CS. Delivery of preventive
2. Krebs HI, Hogan N, Aisen ML, Volpe BT. Robot- services to older black patients using neighbor-
aided neurorehabilitation. IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng. hood health centers. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48
1998;6(1):75–87. (2):124–30.
3. Krebs HI, Hogan N, Volpe BT, Aisen ML, Edel- 20. Jebsen RH, Taylor N, Trieschmann RB, Trotter MJ,
stein L, Diels C. Overview of clinical trials with Howard LA. An objective and standardized test of
MIT-MANUS: a robot-aided neuro-rehabilitation hand function. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1969;50
facility. Technol Health Care Offic J Eur Soc Eng (6):311–9.
Med. 1999;7(6):419–23. 21. Burgess PR, Cooper TA, Gottlieb GL, Latash ML.
4. Volpe BT, Krebs HI, Hogan N, Edelsteinn L, The sense of effort and two models of single-joint
Diels CM, Aisen ML. Robot training enhanced motor control. Somatosens Mot Res. 1995;12(3–
motor outcome in patients with stroke maintained 4):343–58.
over 3 years. Neurology. 1999;53(8):1874–6. 22. Ballester BR, Nirme J, Duarte E, et al. The visual
5. Krebs HI, Volpe BT, Aisen ML, Hogan N. Increas- amplification of goal-oriented movements counter-
ing productivity and quality of care: robot-aided acts acquired non-use in hemiparetic stroke patients.
neuro-rehabilitation. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2000 Nov- J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2015;12:50.
Dec;37(6):639–52. 23. Tatton WG, Forner SD, Gerstein GL, Cham-
6. Fazekas G, Horvath M, Troznai T, Toth A. Robot- bers WW, Liu CN. The effect of postcentral cortical
mediated upper limb physiotherapy for patients with lesions on motor responses to sudden upper limb
spastic hemiparesis: a preliminary study. J Rehabil displacements in monkeys. Brain Res. 1975;96
Med. 2007;39(7):580–2. (1):108–13.
7. Morasso P. Spatial control of arm movements. Exp 24. Matthews PB. The human stretch reflex and the
Brain Res. 1981;42(2):223–7. motor cortex. Trends Neurosci. 1991;14(3):87–91.
8. Harris CM, Wolpert DM. Signal-dependent noise 25. Hammond PH. The influence of prior instruction to
determines motor planning. Nature. 1998;394 the subject on an apparently involuntary neuro-
(6695):780–4. muscular response. J Physiol. 1956;132(1):17–8.
9. Flash T, Hogan N. The coordination of arm 26. Lacquaniti F, Borghese NA, Carrozzo M. Transient
movements: an experimentally confirmed mathe- reversal of the stretch reflex in human arm muscles.
matical model. J Neurosci. 1985;5(7):1688–703. J Neurophysiol. 1991;66(3):939–54.
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
Z.

Zacchias, Peter, remarkable story, related by, 39

Plummer and Brewis, Printers, Love-Lane, Eastcheap.


Transcriber’s Notes
Obvious punctuation errors have been corrected.
Page 13: “towards the large vesse” changed to “towards the large vessels”
Page 23: “and immedaitely” changed to “and immediately”
Page 35: “Remarkable Rescuscitation” changed to “Remarkable Resuscitation”
Page 42: “the generou sfriend” changed to “the generous friend”
Page 80: “the means recommeuded” changed to “the means recommemded”
Page 96: “cometaries in populous” changed to “cemeteries in populous”
Page 97: “offensive cemetry” changed to “offensive cemetery”
Page 102: “of the the living” changed to “of the living”
Page 105: “heavier hant air” changed to “heavier than air” “died convuled” changed to
“died convulsed”
Page 108: “ormidable enemy” changed to “formidable enemy”
Page 118: “convenience of out funerals” changed to “convenience of our funerals”
Page 138: “the view of of” changed to “the view of”
*** END OF THE PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK THE DANGER
OF PREMATURE INTERMENT ***

Updated editions will replace the previous one—the old editions


will be renamed.

Creating the works from print editions not protected by U.S.


copyright law means that no one owns a United States copyright
in these works, so the Foundation (and you!) can copy and
distribute it in the United States without permission and without
paying copyright royalties. Special rules, set forth in the General
Terms of Use part of this license, apply to copying and
distributing Project Gutenberg™ electronic works to protect the
PROJECT GUTENBERG™ concept and trademark. Project
Gutenberg is a registered trademark, and may not be used if
you charge for an eBook, except by following the terms of the
trademark license, including paying royalties for use of the
Project Gutenberg trademark. If you do not charge anything for
copies of this eBook, complying with the trademark license is
very easy. You may use this eBook for nearly any purpose such
as creation of derivative works, reports, performances and
research. Project Gutenberg eBooks may be modified and
printed and given away—you may do practically ANYTHING in
the United States with eBooks not protected by U.S. copyright
law. Redistribution is subject to the trademark license, especially
commercial redistribution.

START: FULL LICENSE


THE FULL PROJECT GUTENBERG LICENSE
PLEASE READ THIS BEFORE YOU DISTRIBUTE OR USE THIS WORK

To protect the Project Gutenberg™ mission of promoting the


free distribution of electronic works, by using or distributing this
work (or any other work associated in any way with the phrase
“Project Gutenberg”), you agree to comply with all the terms of
the Full Project Gutenberg™ License available with this file or
online at www.gutenberg.org/license.

Section 1. General Terms of Use and


Redistributing Project Gutenberg™
electronic works
1.A. By reading or using any part of this Project Gutenberg™
electronic work, you indicate that you have read, understand,
agree to and accept all the terms of this license and intellectual
property (trademark/copyright) agreement. If you do not agree to
abide by all the terms of this agreement, you must cease using
and return or destroy all copies of Project Gutenberg™
electronic works in your possession. If you paid a fee for
obtaining a copy of or access to a Project Gutenberg™
electronic work and you do not agree to be bound by the terms
of this agreement, you may obtain a refund from the person or
entity to whom you paid the fee as set forth in paragraph 1.E.8.

1.B. “Project Gutenberg” is a registered trademark. It may only


be used on or associated in any way with an electronic work by
people who agree to be bound by the terms of this agreement.
There are a few things that you can do with most Project
Gutenberg™ electronic works even without complying with the
full terms of this agreement. See paragraph 1.C below. There
are a lot of things you can do with Project Gutenberg™
electronic works if you follow the terms of this agreement and
help preserve free future access to Project Gutenberg™
electronic works. See paragraph 1.E below.
1.C. The Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation (“the
Foundation” or PGLAF), owns a compilation copyright in the
collection of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works. Nearly all the
individual works in the collection are in the public domain in the
United States. If an individual work is unprotected by copyright
law in the United States and you are located in the United
States, we do not claim a right to prevent you from copying,
distributing, performing, displaying or creating derivative works
based on the work as long as all references to Project
Gutenberg are removed. Of course, we hope that you will
support the Project Gutenberg™ mission of promoting free
access to electronic works by freely sharing Project
Gutenberg™ works in compliance with the terms of this
agreement for keeping the Project Gutenberg™ name
associated with the work. You can easily comply with the terms
of this agreement by keeping this work in the same format with
its attached full Project Gutenberg™ License when you share it
without charge with others.

1.D. The copyright laws of the place where you are located also
govern what you can do with this work. Copyright laws in most
countries are in a constant state of change. If you are outside
the United States, check the laws of your country in addition to
the terms of this agreement before downloading, copying,
displaying, performing, distributing or creating derivative works
based on this work or any other Project Gutenberg™ work. The
Foundation makes no representations concerning the copyright
status of any work in any country other than the United States.

1.E. Unless you have removed all references to Project


Gutenberg:

1.E.1. The following sentence, with active links to, or other


immediate access to, the full Project Gutenberg™ License must
appear prominently whenever any copy of a Project
Gutenberg™ work (any work on which the phrase “Project
Gutenberg” appears, or with which the phrase “Project
Gutenberg” is associated) is accessed, displayed, performed,
viewed, copied or distributed:

This eBook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United


States and most other parts of the world at no cost and with
almost no restrictions whatsoever. You may copy it, give it
away or re-use it under the terms of the Project Gutenberg
License included with this eBook or online at
www.gutenberg.org. If you are not located in the United
States, you will have to check the laws of the country where
you are located before using this eBook.

1.E.2. If an individual Project Gutenberg™ electronic work is


derived from texts not protected by U.S. copyright law (does not
contain a notice indicating that it is posted with permission of the
copyright holder), the work can be copied and distributed to
anyone in the United States without paying any fees or charges.
If you are redistributing or providing access to a work with the
phrase “Project Gutenberg” associated with or appearing on the
work, you must comply either with the requirements of
paragraphs 1.E.1 through 1.E.7 or obtain permission for the use
of the work and the Project Gutenberg™ trademark as set forth
in paragraphs 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.

1.E.3. If an individual Project Gutenberg™ electronic work is


posted with the permission of the copyright holder, your use and
distribution must comply with both paragraphs 1.E.1 through
1.E.7 and any additional terms imposed by the copyright holder.
Additional terms will be linked to the Project Gutenberg™
License for all works posted with the permission of the copyright
holder found at the beginning of this work.

1.E.4. Do not unlink or detach or remove the full Project


Gutenberg™ License terms from this work, or any files
containing a part of this work or any other work associated with
Project Gutenberg™.
1.E.5. Do not copy, display, perform, distribute or redistribute
this electronic work, or any part of this electronic work, without
prominently displaying the sentence set forth in paragraph 1.E.1
with active links or immediate access to the full terms of the
Project Gutenberg™ License.

1.E.6. You may convert to and distribute this work in any binary,
compressed, marked up, nonproprietary or proprietary form,
including any word processing or hypertext form. However, if
you provide access to or distribute copies of a Project
Gutenberg™ work in a format other than “Plain Vanilla ASCII” or
other format used in the official version posted on the official
Project Gutenberg™ website (www.gutenberg.org), you must, at
no additional cost, fee or expense to the user, provide a copy, a
means of exporting a copy, or a means of obtaining a copy upon
request, of the work in its original “Plain Vanilla ASCII” or other
form. Any alternate format must include the full Project
Gutenberg™ License as specified in paragraph 1.E.1.

1.E.7. Do not charge a fee for access to, viewing, displaying,


performing, copying or distributing any Project Gutenberg™
works unless you comply with paragraph 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.

1.E.8. You may charge a reasonable fee for copies of or


providing access to or distributing Project Gutenberg™
electronic works provided that:

• You pay a royalty fee of 20% of the gross profits you derive from
the use of Project Gutenberg™ works calculated using the
method you already use to calculate your applicable taxes. The
fee is owed to the owner of the Project Gutenberg™ trademark,
but he has agreed to donate royalties under this paragraph to
the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation. Royalty
payments must be paid within 60 days following each date on
which you prepare (or are legally required to prepare) your
periodic tax returns. Royalty payments should be clearly marked
as such and sent to the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive
Foundation at the address specified in Section 4, “Information
about donations to the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive
Foundation.”

• You provide a full refund of any money paid by a user who


notifies you in writing (or by e-mail) within 30 days of receipt that
s/he does not agree to the terms of the full Project Gutenberg™
License. You must require such a user to return or destroy all
copies of the works possessed in a physical medium and
discontinue all use of and all access to other copies of Project
Gutenberg™ works.

• You provide, in accordance with paragraph 1.F.3, a full refund of


any money paid for a work or a replacement copy, if a defect in
the electronic work is discovered and reported to you within 90
days of receipt of the work.

• You comply with all other terms of this agreement for free
distribution of Project Gutenberg™ works.

1.E.9. If you wish to charge a fee or distribute a Project


Gutenberg™ electronic work or group of works on different
terms than are set forth in this agreement, you must obtain
permission in writing from the Project Gutenberg Literary
Archive Foundation, the manager of the Project Gutenberg™
trademark. Contact the Foundation as set forth in Section 3
below.

1.F.

1.F.1. Project Gutenberg volunteers and employees expend


considerable effort to identify, do copyright research on,
transcribe and proofread works not protected by U.S. copyright
law in creating the Project Gutenberg™ collection. Despite
these efforts, Project Gutenberg™ electronic works, and the
medium on which they may be stored, may contain “Defects,”
such as, but not limited to, incomplete, inaccurate or corrupt
data, transcription errors, a copyright or other intellectual
property infringement, a defective or damaged disk or other
medium, a computer virus, or computer codes that damage or
cannot be read by your equipment.

1.F.2. LIMITED WARRANTY, DISCLAIMER OF DAMAGES -


Except for the “Right of Replacement or Refund” described in
paragraph 1.F.3, the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive
Foundation, the owner of the Project Gutenberg™ trademark,
and any other party distributing a Project Gutenberg™ electronic
work under this agreement, disclaim all liability to you for
damages, costs and expenses, including legal fees. YOU
AGREE THAT YOU HAVE NO REMEDIES FOR NEGLIGENCE,
STRICT LIABILITY, BREACH OF WARRANTY OR BREACH
OF CONTRACT EXCEPT THOSE PROVIDED IN PARAGRAPH
1.F.3. YOU AGREE THAT THE FOUNDATION, THE
TRADEMARK OWNER, AND ANY DISTRIBUTOR UNDER
THIS AGREEMENT WILL NOT BE LIABLE TO YOU FOR
ACTUAL, DIRECT, INDIRECT, CONSEQUENTIAL, PUNITIVE
OR INCIDENTAL DAMAGES EVEN IF YOU GIVE NOTICE OF
THE POSSIBILITY OF SUCH DAMAGE.

1.F.3. LIMITED RIGHT OF REPLACEMENT OR REFUND - If


you discover a defect in this electronic work within 90 days of
receiving it, you can receive a refund of the money (if any) you
paid for it by sending a written explanation to the person you
received the work from. If you received the work on a physical
medium, you must return the medium with your written
explanation. The person or entity that provided you with the
defective work may elect to provide a replacement copy in lieu
of a refund. If you received the work electronically, the person or
entity providing it to you may choose to give you a second
opportunity to receive the work electronically in lieu of a refund.
If the second copy is also defective, you may demand a refund
in writing without further opportunities to fix the problem.

1.F.4. Except for the limited right of replacement or refund set


forth in paragraph 1.F.3, this work is provided to you ‘AS-IS’,
WITH NO OTHER WARRANTIES OF ANY KIND, EXPRESS
OR IMPLIED, INCLUDING BUT NOT LIMITED TO
WARRANTIES OF MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR
ANY PURPOSE.

1.F.5. Some states do not allow disclaimers of certain implied


warranties or the exclusion or limitation of certain types of
damages. If any disclaimer or limitation set forth in this
agreement violates the law of the state applicable to this
agreement, the agreement shall be interpreted to make the
maximum disclaimer or limitation permitted by the applicable
state law. The invalidity or unenforceability of any provision of
this agreement shall not void the remaining provisions.

1.F.6. INDEMNITY - You agree to indemnify and hold the


Foundation, the trademark owner, any agent or employee of the
Foundation, anyone providing copies of Project Gutenberg™
electronic works in accordance with this agreement, and any
volunteers associated with the production, promotion and
distribution of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works, harmless
from all liability, costs and expenses, including legal fees, that
arise directly or indirectly from any of the following which you do
or cause to occur: (a) distribution of this or any Project
Gutenberg™ work, (b) alteration, modification, or additions or
deletions to any Project Gutenberg™ work, and (c) any Defect
you cause.

Section 2. Information about the Mission of


Project Gutenberg™
Project Gutenberg™ is synonymous with the free distribution of
electronic works in formats readable by the widest variety of
computers including obsolete, old, middle-aged and new
computers. It exists because of the efforts of hundreds of
volunteers and donations from people in all walks of life.

Volunteers and financial support to provide volunteers with the


assistance they need are critical to reaching Project
Gutenberg™’s goals and ensuring that the Project Gutenberg™
collection will remain freely available for generations to come. In
2001, the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation was
created to provide a secure and permanent future for Project
Gutenberg™ and future generations. To learn more about the
Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation and how your
efforts and donations can help, see Sections 3 and 4 and the
Foundation information page at www.gutenberg.org.

Section 3. Information about the Project


Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation
The Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation is a non-
profit 501(c)(3) educational corporation organized under the
laws of the state of Mississippi and granted tax exempt status by
the Internal Revenue Service. The Foundation’s EIN or federal
tax identification number is 64-6221541. Contributions to the
Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation are tax
deductible to the full extent permitted by U.S. federal laws and
your state’s laws.

The Foundation’s business office is located at 809 North 1500


West, Salt Lake City, UT 84116, (801) 596-1887. Email contact
links and up to date contact information can be found at the
Foundation’s website and official page at
www.gutenberg.org/contact

Section 4. Information about Donations to


the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive
Foundation
Project Gutenberg™ depends upon and cannot survive without
widespread public support and donations to carry out its mission
of increasing the number of public domain and licensed works
that can be freely distributed in machine-readable form
accessible by the widest array of equipment including outdated
equipment. Many small donations ($1 to $5,000) are particularly
important to maintaining tax exempt status with the IRS.

The Foundation is committed to complying with the laws


regulating charities and charitable donations in all 50 states of
the United States. Compliance requirements are not uniform
and it takes a considerable effort, much paperwork and many
fees to meet and keep up with these requirements. We do not
solicit donations in locations where we have not received written
confirmation of compliance. To SEND DONATIONS or
determine the status of compliance for any particular state visit
www.gutenberg.org/donate.

While we cannot and do not solicit contributions from states


where we have not met the solicitation requirements, we know
of no prohibition against accepting unsolicited donations from
donors in such states who approach us with offers to donate.

International donations are gratefully accepted, but we cannot


make any statements concerning tax treatment of donations
received from outside the United States. U.S. laws alone swamp
our small staff.

Please check the Project Gutenberg web pages for current


donation methods and addresses. Donations are accepted in a
number of other ways including checks, online payments and
credit card donations. To donate, please visit:
www.gutenberg.org/donate.

Section 5. General Information About Project


Gutenberg™ electronic works
Professor Michael S. Hart was the originator of the Project
Gutenberg™ concept of a library of electronic works that could
be freely shared with anyone. For forty years, he produced and
distributed Project Gutenberg™ eBooks with only a loose
network of volunteer support.

Project Gutenberg™ eBooks are often created from several


printed editions, all of which are confirmed as not protected by
copyright in the U.S. unless a copyright notice is included. Thus,
we do not necessarily keep eBooks in compliance with any
particular paper edition.

Most people start at our website which has the main PG search
facility: www.gutenberg.org.

This website includes information about Project Gutenberg™,


including how to make donations to the Project Gutenberg
Literary Archive Foundation, how to help produce our new
eBooks, and how to subscribe to our email newsletter to hear
about new eBooks.

You might also like