Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical

Psychology
Emotional Education for Personal Growth in the Early
Years
José Víctor Oron, Sonsoles Navarro-Rubio, and Elkin O. Luis
Online First Publication, March 5, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/teo0000150

CITATION
Oron, J. V., Navarro-Rubio, S., & Luis, E. O. (2020, March 5). Emotional Education for Personal
Growth in the Early Years. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. Advance online
publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/teo0000150
Journal of Theoretical and
Philosophical Psychology
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 2, No. 999, 000
ISSN: 1068-8471 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/teo0000150

Emotional Education for Personal Growth in the Early Years

José Víctor Oron Sonsoles Navarro-Rubio


University of Navarra and UptoYou Foundation, University of Navarra
Pamplona, Spain

Elkin O. Luis
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

University of Navarra and Institute of Health Research of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article offers a proposal related to early childhood emotional education. It is in line
with Erikson’s (1959) ideas surrounding psychosocial development and his theory of
attachment and social referencing, Bridges’ (1932) emotional development scheme, and
Rogers’ notion related to “acceptance” of emotional complexity toward healthy develop-
ment. This proposal is reinforced with approaches from Winnicott (1986) and Kohut
(2009), who demonstrate that children’s emotional education is indeed a process of
self-consciousness development through interaction with their primary caregiver(s). These
approaches demonstrate that early childhood emotional education is really a process related
to children’s development of self-consciousness through interaction, or lack thereof, with
their primary caregiver(s). We coin our approach “emotional integration,” which is con-
ceptualized as a response to the dominant “emotional regulation” narrative. While emo-
tional regulation focuses on behavioral and structured routines for facing diverse emotional
situations, emotional integration is centered on interpersonal relationship improvements in
different emotional contexts. For the emotional regulation approach, the child regulates her
behavior depending on the primary caregiver’s reactions. In emotional integration, the child
acquires certain dispositions toward the self, starting from her interaction with her caregiver.
In small children, caregiver behavior certainly has a determining role. For the emotional
regulation approach, the caregiver tries to solve possible problems in behavior, whereas for
emotional integration, the caregiver seeks to develop inner processes of personal growth
through the child’s interaction with the other.

Public Significance Statement


This study offers a new proposal regarding children’s emotional education and is based
on Erikson’s psychosocial development, attachment theory, and social reference,
Bridges’ emotional development, and Rogers’ notions, alongside Winnicott and Ko-
hut’s approaches. It points toward a kind of interaction between the infant and his
primary caregiver that focuses on emotional integration instead of emotional regulation,
which allows for the infant’s formation of self-consciousness and healthy emotional growth.

Keywords: emotional education, education, early childhood and emotional develop-


ment

in the “Home Renaissance Foundation, International


X José Víctor Oron, Institute for Culture and Society, Conference on the topic of A home: a place of growth,
Mind-Brain Group, University of Navarra, and UptoYou care and wellbeing” of The Royal Society of Medicine,
Foundation, Pamplona, Spain; X Sonsoles Navarro- London, England, November 17, 2017.
Rubio, Institute for Culture and Society, Mind-Brain Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
Group, University of Navarra; X Elkin O. Luis, School of dressed to Elkin O. Luis, School of Education and Psy-
Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, and chology, Edificio Ismael Sánchez Bella, Universidad de
Institute of Health Research of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain. Navarra, Campus Universitario s/n, Pamplona, Navarra
Some preliminary ideas of this article were presented 31008, Spain. E-mail: eoswaldo@unav.es

1
2 ORON, NAVARRO-RUBIO, AND LUIS

Human development literature considers playing, and the mother wills him to eat because
childhood the fundamental stage for educational she is focused on the child’s resistance and
development. Those early years generally focus emotional state evidenced in crying. With this
on acquiring healthy routines through which the case as an explanatory framework, we develop
child regulates her own behavior through inter- an alternative intervention proposal, which we
actions with her primary caregiver (Sander, present initially and then, at the end of the
1977). This article, however, takes a different article, more specifically regarding the situation
position by exploring an educational alternative in question.
that starts with the knowledge of the child’s
direct reality and its effect on development dur- Erikson’s Proposal of Development
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ing the rest of the maturation processes that take in the First 2 Years of Life and His
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

place in infancy (Blair & Razza, 2007), which Attachment Theory


are relevant for later stages of the life cycle
(Bodrova, 2008). In 1963, Erikson pointed out that, in the first
In the following text, we introduce the au- 2 years of life, children have certain experiences
thors of reference for this proposal at the same that engender in them an attitude of either basic
time that we outline our proposal, specifically trust or mistrust (Erikson, 1963). If children feel
focusing on children’s personal development. that their caregiver— usually their parent(s)—
This development is understood as a state of recognizes and satisfies their essential needs,
interpersonal improvement resulting from ini- they grow up with a basic sense of trust. But if
tial processes of intrapersonal integration in in- they find that their needs are not met, or that
fancy (including emotional, cognitive, and only some of them are (e.g., only the physical,
moral aspects). These processes lead to a ma- but not the psychological ones, such as playing,
ture state that flourishes depending on the rela- caressing, etc.), then they lean more toward
tionships established between infants or be- distrust that manifests itself in the type of care
tween infants and adults. they lacked. Both basic trust and basic mistrust
To better understand our proposal, let us first exist on a spectrum and can be measured.
start with the example of a common family Undoubtedly, contemporary psychology con-
situation between a mother and her 2-year-old tinues to develop attachment theory after con-
child that requires resolution. The child is play- firmation of its original formulation in the
ing, and her mother stops the play to feed her at 1970s and 80s (Ainsworth & Bell, 1970;
the established time. The child starts to cry and Bowlby, 1982). A child’s emotional attachment,
throws a tantrum. What is the parent to do in with which he or she feels safe and protected by
this situation? Two typical solutions immedi- her primary caregiver(s), enables her to survey
ately arise: (a) Let the child play and forget the the world under the paradigm of exploratory
food, or (b) make the child eat and forget the curiosity (Carson, 2012; Domhardt, Münzer,
play. This second option admits two variations: Fegert, & Goldbeck, 2015; Glaser, 2014; Hong
(2.1) The mother does not pay attention to the & Park, 2012; L’Ecuyer, 2014).
child’s cries, but rather focuses on feeding. This The impact that childrens relationship with
generates some tension, but the mother wins. their caregivers has on themselves is now well
(2.2) The mother interacts with the child’s emo- understood and supported by evidence. Indeed,
tional state and starts playing with him to calm many have found that it affects children’s con-
him down and then moves on to feeding him. siderations of the world and their emotional
From our point of view, these three options states (Atzil et al., 2017; Bornsteina et al., 2017;
all have something in common. In each, they Morales et al., 2017; Pratt, Goldstein, Levy, &
face a problem that could be summarized as Feldman, 2017; Zhang, Chen, Deng, & Lu,
follows, “The child needs to learn certain rou- 2014); it also helps to forms children’s “per-
tines, but refuses,” which necessitates resolu- sonal premise system” (Berghout Austin, God-
tion. In our view, focusing on solving the prob- frey, Weber, Martin, & Holmes, 1991) and even
lem is an educational mistake that emerges from affects their brain structure (Silk, Redcay, &
these two options (three alternatives). It is easy Fox, 2014) and epigenome (Provençal &
for a mother to conceptualize the situation as a Binder, 2015), which is clearest in pathological
“struggle of wills”; the child wills to continue cases (de Rosnay, Cooper, Tsigaras, & Murray,
EMOTIONAL EDUCATION FOR PERSONAL GROWTH IN INFANTS 3

2006; Pelaez, Virues-Ortega, Field, Amir-Kiaei, wonder, which encourages the child to search
& Schnerch, 2013). for knowledge and promotes her will to learn
In keeping with authors such as Pittman, Kei- because she feels secure. A relationship of mis-
ley, Kerpelman, and Vaughn (2011), we pro- trust between the primary caregiver and child
pose a consideration of the possibility of com- promotes a control approach, engendering fear
plementarity between these two models. in the child and a search for ways to master
Bowlby’s, 1982 model offers Erikson’s model reality toward self-protection because the child
inclusion of various secure and insecure attach- does not feel safe. Secure attachment results in
ment representations that have implications for the child relinquishing the need for control; for
the implementation of interpersonal strategies example, children who live in secure attachment
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

in life cycle stages that follow childhood; on the do not exert control over their peers but instead
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

other hand, Erikson’s model offers Bowlby’s develop close relationships with them (Park &
model diverse social contexts for each develop- Waters, 1989).
ment stage, in which various secure representa- In short, the following three aspects not only
tions can be generated. In addition, we discover represent meeting points between these two the-
that, to understand anything, we must first refer ories but also serve as arguments for our pro-
to the child’s relationship with her primary care- posal. First, the life cycle period (childhood)
giver. This implies that the child trustingly as- described by both proposals gives similar im-
sumes whatever her primary caregiver presents portance to the caregiver; on the one hand,
to her. For instance, if the person in charge Bowlby, 1982 and Ainsworth understood the
hands a ball to a child, she holds the ball in a caregiver as determinant in the establishment of
relationship of trust and will not see the ball as secure versus insecure relationships, where, on
a hostile object. The child projects onto objects the secure end of the spectrum, the infant feels
the relationship of trust or mistrust she has with cared for and assisted, which allows her to
her primary caregiver, thus engendering con- explore their reality. This is certainly compara-
crete relational styles. This social interaction ble to Erikson’s description of caregivers’ care
defines the type of access the child has to the and assistance as oriented toward trust or mis-
world, even when she is alone. trust in the infant. Second, the development of
A dynamic of trust causes the child to per- the way in which the infant understands her
ceive novelty with astonishment and awakens surroundings results from the desire to explore/
her desire to learn new things, whereas a dy- learn or search for situational control; both are a
namic of mistrust causes the child to perceive result of the caregiver’s attachment style (the
newness with a certain suspicion and insecurity result of attachment history) with regard to the
and causes her to immediately search for new child, which causes the infant to project this
ways to feel protected and safe. Therefore, we style in future relationships. In short, the care-
can initially conclude that the child’s meaning giver determines the way in which the infant
and intentional approaches are related, and after interprets new environments of greater social
accepting this premise, we define two different complexity. Third, and probably most impor-
procedures for approaching reality as follows tant, both authors recognize that the attachment
(Figure 1). style and the psychosocial transfer produced in
A relationship of trust between the primary the relationship (between caregiver and infant)
caregiver and child promotes an approach of have interpersonal results that feed the child’s
beliefs about the self, others, and the world.
This has serious implications for the way in
Confident Wonder Searching which the infant relates in the future.
relaonship approach for knowing

Social Referencing Theory


Mistrust Control Searching
relaonship approach for security In the 1980s, psychology literature began to
explore the power of the affective bond between
Figure 1. Two personal relationship styles related to two children and their primary caregiver(s), show-
ways of approaching reality. See the online article for the ing how children learn a particular view of their
color version of this figure. context depending on how their mother or father
4 ORON, NAVARRO-RUBIO, AND LUIS

understand the world, as well as in terms of the otherwise, the child treats isolated sounds as
quality of the relationship(s) between them. noise and finds them to be meaningless. For
Children understand the emotional meaning of young children, things are not what they are in
affection that their primary caregivers show themselves; instead, their meaning is taken or
them not as a mere modulator, but as an aid or “borrowed” from adults’ emotional reactions.
an impediment that permits or hinders their This is the most obvious basis of social refer-
development (Tronick, 1989). encing in situations of ambiguity (Klinnert,
Going deeper into this idea, researchers have Emde, Butterfield, & Campos, 1986; Mireault et
found that communication in early childhood al., 2014; Pelaez, Virues-Ortega, & Gewirtz,
fundamentally relates to the child-caregiver- 2012), although it could be argued that most
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

object visual triangle, by which children learn things that appear for the first time are largely
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the meaning of reality and which also guides ambiguous. Another important issue is that so-
their behavior based on their primary caregiv- cial referencing does not just provide emotional
ers’ facial expressions. This phenomenon has meaning to the world of objects or events but
been coined as “social referencing” (Walden & also does so for other people (Feinman &
Ogan, 1988). Lewis, 1983) assuming the emotional meaning
Trevarthen (2005) took up attachment theo- that an adult’s emotional state gives off (Murray
ries, arguing that it is not just a matter of feeling et al., 2008; Moses, Baldwin, Rosicky, & Tid-
safe but also the basis from which a child gets to ball, 2001; Repacholi & Meltzoff, 2007).
know the world and assigns meaning to reality. As a whole, it is encouraging that social
Therefore, the infant’s behavior is not a mere referencing gives meaning to objects, events,
“reflection or mirror” of brain motor processes and personal relationships. This issue is gaining
but rather the result of emotional–sympathetic relevance in neuroscience because meaning
negotiation (Trevarthen, 2005, p. 59). In other emerges as a result of these processes (Clark-
words, intersubjectivity constructs meaning and
Polner & Clark, 2014). When the caregiver and
emotional experience (Trevarthen, 2005, pp.
child share an intentional activity (staring at
66 –71). This meaning jumps across the inter-
each other, singing together, etc.), both brains
generational divide (Trevarthen, 2005, p. 61),
experience bidirectional synchronization in the
which is why the author propounds that it “goes
beyond concerns with” (Trevarthen, 2005, p. frontal region (Leong et al., 2017). All this
71) the search for well-being, regulating activity influences the brain’s configuration such that a
and stress levels. predetermined or default mode of thinking may/
Accordingly, we can uncover the importance can be created for understanding the world, a
of constructing meaning from an interpersonal worldview or a belief system.
encounter with language (emotional in this Drawing conclusions from Erikson—includ-
case; Trevarthen, 2005, p. 71). More explicitly, ing attachment and social referencing theory—
it first corresponds to the interpersonal encoun- all this seems to lead to the fact that children do
ter as a source of significance, which conse- not neutrally access the world; rather, they proj-
quently enables an emotional state. This inter- ect the quality of their relationship with their
personal encounter requires both the child and caregiver onto the world, as well as the meaning
primary caregiver’s bidirectional intentions, as that their caregiver assigns to a shared object.
well as the latter’s emotional expression. This Children learn the meaning of reality through
encounter condenses for the child her emotional the emotional expression that their primary
experience. caregiver exudes. We could say that the parent
Studies on social referencing also reveal that, or primary caregiver “lends” his mind to the
for human beings, the social sphere is not just child, clarifying that, in this case, we understand
the environment in which learning takes place; “mind” as the sense in which he understands the
it is not another modulator just because learning world. This equates to saying that the child
happens in that context. That explanation lacks knows the world through the representation her
consistency because, beyond it, the social parent or primary caregiver offers her; hence,
sphere provides meaning. As we know, to help the child “acts with her primary caregiver’s
a young child learn a new language, someone mind.” The following figures collect and sum-
must be present (Kuhl, Tsao, & Liu, 2003); marize the ideas presented earlier (Figures 2).
EMOTIONAL EDUCATION FOR PERSONAL GROWTH IN INFANTS 5

Aachment / Erikson (first weeks to first month after birth). Although


the relationship between a newborn and his
MAIN
CAREGIVER CHILD environment is minimal, he responds to (inter-
nal or external) stimulation instinctively, while
the caregiver meets his physiological needs,
which outweigh psychological ones. (b) Normal
Social referencing Meaning symbiotic phase (2 to 5 months): The infant
must adapt to the environment that surrounds
him through his caregiver, as his rudimentary
self is not sufficiently structured to face the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

OBJECT / WORLD
various environmental demands. M. S. Mahler
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(1974) referred to this phenomenon as symbio-


Figure 2. Relationship between Erikson’s attachment the- sis, that is, a fused state or interdependent psy-
ory and social referencing theory. chobiological bond between the caregiver and
infant that supplements the infant’s rudimentary
and undifferentiated self. (c) Separation–
When faced with novelty in a situation, such individualization phase (5 months to 2 years):
as when a primary caregiver shows something The infant shows a growing ability to recognize
new to a child, she will instantly react by staring others, especially his caregiver, to gradually
at her caregiver to glean the meaning of the new inspect the world and move away from the
object. Then, in accordance with the quality of caregiver perspective through two interrelated
the relationship (attachment theory and Erik- processes—separation, which entails intrapsy-
son’s idea of the trust–mistrust spectrum) and chic awareness of separation, and individualiza-
the emotional meaning that the caregiver gives tion, which allows for the infant’s distinctive
to the object (social referencing), the child as- and unique individuality to emerge.
signs meaning and significance to the object as These three phases reveal that processes of
reflected in the emotional experience therein. biological and psychological birth are different
The world as a whole, and any one object in over time. While biological birth implies greater
particular, initially holds no meaning for the circumscription and speed in terms of the phys-
child. Consequently, the child finds meaning iological acts involved, for psychological
thanks to the quality of the personal relationship birth— or the separation–individualization pro-
with her primary caregiver (as Erikson and at- cess, as Mahler’s psychological development
tachment theory points out) and thanks to her theory called it—intrapsychic acts become evi-
primary caregiver’s emotional expression in dent later and are slower to develop (Ritvo,
light of the object (as social referencing tells 2018). Thus, the separation–individualization
us). process is another way of saying that the child
Before applying this proposal to emotional gradually acquires awareness of the self (which
education, we turn to better understanding how he obviously does not know how to account
emotional development occurs, and, to do so, for). However, contrary to Mahler’s consid-
we rely on Bridges’ research as well as Rogers’ eration, and supported by studies with evi-
reference to acceptance of reception. Herein, we dence from intrauterine stages, fetuses with a
will add other authors who encompass and give developed brainstem show psychological acts
meaning to these theories when applied to the directed to action outside of mere reactivity
child’s inner self so that she might grow through (Delafield-Butt & Gangopadhyay, 2013; De-
creativity rather than an established emotional lafield-Butt & Trevarthen, 2015). Thus, we
regulation regime. believe that elements of a proto-self are al-
ready present in intrauterine stages.
Psychological and Emotional Development Regarding infants’ emotional development at
the age of two, there are four relevant aspects to
For authors such as Mahler, Pine, and Berg- consider (Greenspan, 2007a): (a) The child’s
man (2018), an infant’s psychological develop- connection to her caregivers is characterized by
ment can be understood sequentially in three a balance between basic dependence and auton-
phases as follows: (a) normal autistic phase omy, between initiative and capacity for self-
6 ORON, NAVARRO-RUBIO, AND LUIS

organization at the behavioral level (“crawlers” in that development stage, their differentiation
who go get what they want; Sroufe, 2005; and emotional knowledge displays itself as a
Theisen & Erikson, 2007); (b) the mood and simple duality between the pleasant and un-
feelings that predominate are more diversified, pleasant. For example, a child displays the same
organized, and stable, together with greater per- corporal expression after being satiated or after
formance of tasks associated with recognition of feeling her mother’s caress because she cannot
emotions compared with emotional perspective- yet distinguish them. To varying extents, other
taking tasks (Downs, Strand, & Cerna, 2007; authors also followed Bridges’ approach (Cam-
Wellman, Fang, & Peterson, 2011); (c) regard- pos, Campos, & Barrett, 1989; Jack, Garrod, &
ing the variety, depth, and adequacy of feelings, Schyns, 2014). From it, we can conclude that a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the presence of feelings such as security, curi- child must learn what an inner emotional state
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

osity, and the need for exploration is evident. is, which she discovers through social relation-
Likewise, the child tends to experience self- ships.
reference (the child incorporates “the self” into As Bridges’ research developed, she found
language), possessiveness (“mine”), and nega- that a baby learns to distinguish different un-
tivism (“no” to whatever he does not want). pleasant situations more clearly and precisely
Fear also often arises in situations where the than pleasant ones because her expressive cor-
caregiver momentarily detaches (Groh, Fearon, poral repertoire becomes more specialized ac-
van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Ro- cording to diverse emotional situations. To the
isman, 2017; Malik & Marwaha, 2018); (d) at extent that children relate everything they have
this stage of development, the presence of emo- experienced and learn about emotional diver-
tional states expressed in the infant’s behavior, sity, their ability to differentiate and identify
play, and verbalization is observed through de- emotional situations will emerge, and their de-
velopment of the capacity to organize behavior velopment/growth will simultaneously take
in more complex causal chains that demonstrate place, meaning that, integration, differentiation,
her interests and pleasures (Tomasello & Car- identity, and growth occur together through
penter, 2007). The child expresses themes of children’s emotional reality. Camras (2011)
love, curiosity, exploration and protest, anger, summarized this idea as follows: “The data sug-
denial, and jealousy, all in an organized way. gest that as development proceeds, infants re-
Toward the end of the second year, capacities sponses become differentiated, and their inte-
emerge that integrate themes that reflect love– gration during an emotion episode will reflect
hate and passivity–activity polarities, and the contextual factors as much as the identity of the
child’s first symbolic capacities also emerge emotion” (Camras, 2011, p. 142). A similar
(Greenspan, 2007b). All of these milestones process occurs with the development of bodily
demonstrate the child’s gains in terms of ex- and verbal languages; children achieve a rela-
pressiveness in her emotional repertoire. tively high level of bodily expressiveness, al-
In line with these states of emotional polarity though their verbal expression remains at sim-
and contrary to common belief, authors such as ple duality (good and bad). Just as children
Katherine Bridges (1932) postulated that, when develop expressive bodily capacity, they must
we are born, we lack all basic emotions (joy, undergo the same process in the verbal realm,
sadness, fear, disgust, and anger) and only in- although of course the existence of language
herit a capacity for basic excitability. Bridges opens up more complicated processes (Lewis,
assumed that we are able to recognize the 2007).
child’s differentiation of two emotional situa-
tions if she uses different bodily expressions. Toward an Educational Proposal
That is, if something disgusts, scares, or hurts
her, and she demonstrates the same bodily re- To offer an emotional education proposal
action, we can assume that she does not yet centered on the family, we will start by connect-
distinguish between disgust, fear, and pain. ing Erikson’s attachment and social referencing
Bridges observed that, at birth, children react theory with Bridges’ and Roger’s theories.
with the same corporal expression to every In short, a state of basic trust permits infants
event. Soon after, however, they learn to differ- to initiate on a path of differentiation in a way
entiate unpleasant from pleasant situations, but, that is much more in line with reality because
EMOTIONAL EDUCATION FOR PERSONAL GROWTH IN INFANTS 7

they do not approach reality with excessive healthy living involves recognizing feelings as
caution. Amazement does not seek to control one’s own and not as merely awakened “from
but rather to learn about and assume reality as it the outside.” When recognizing emotions for
is. We could say that amazement facilitates an what they are, a person is thus able to take
involuntary— but at the same time intentional— responsibility for his actions and then act on the
approach and respectful knowledge of the matter.
world. “Involuntary intentionality” seeks to On the contrary, Rogers pointed out that
know (intentional), but without trying to control when someone feels welcomed and experiences
(involuntary). On the other hand, a situation of a trusting relationship, he gradually discovers
basic distrust leads infants to approach the that emotions do not simply occur or take place
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

world from an attitude of suspicion rather than inside one’s self, but that they belong to him. He
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

amazement; their search for knowledge will begins to consider them as signals that refer to
therefore be intentional as they try to learn the complexity of personal life and, through
about and assume reality in order to control it them, can discover the world of meaning and
and feel safe. Thus, in the end, basic trust pro- lived reality, while recognizing his own experi-
motes an encounter with reality as it is, setting ences through, quite often, conflicting feelings.
aside any intention of changing it, which is very In this case, emotions no longer need to be
different from an attitude of basic mistrust. controlled. They are fine as they are, without
As the identification path goes forward, it is need for modification or manipulation, that is,
important to note that emotions are not a mere on their own, because they speak to the com-
affective reaction to stimulus, but rather a crys- plexity of one’s life and, starting from them, one
tallized or condensed version of a whole bodily, can grow in autonomy. Denying or controlling
intentional, emotional, social, and cognitive ex- an emotion literally prevents growth and self-
perience. Emotion is defined, therefore, as a knowledge, which leads to situations that do not
condensed version of the understanding of align with reality. The point is not to control but
one’s personal experience within the world and rather to assume and accept emotions to under-
a social network (Jack et al., 2014). stand and learn about one’s self and reactions.
As previously indicated, a child identifies and In adult therapy, upon uncovering complex-
gives meaning to reality in terms of the rela- ity, the patient discovers that emotional reality
tionship she has with her primary caregiver. A itself compels him take a personal position in
child uses her primary caregiver’s behavioral his life, which enables him to face the larger
reactions to identify the emotional meaning of a question of what kind of person he wants to be
variety of events or situations. or become. The answer to this question is found
Studies in psychotherapy, such as the ones through defining the type and quality of inter-
from Rogers (1961), have revealed that adults personal relationships therein. Rogers made it
who developed in an atmosphere of mistrust do clear that a patient cannot undergo this process
not recognize feelings as their own, nor can they before establishing a diminished climate of in-
conceptualize that these feelings are due to terpersonal reception, which permits the patient
something happening in them. From their point to accept and recognize his complex reality.
of view, feelings are provoked externally and This requires that the patient be capable of
occur internally, but they do not possess them as perceiving that the therapist admits his emo-
such. Because this kind of person does not tional reality without trying to change or modify
recognize himself as in possession of anything, it. This reception enables the patient to embrace
he obviously cannot accept even the slightest and be open to his complexity and reality, with-
responsibility or change, taking a defensive po- out which growth is impossible. This brief
sition and assigning blame elsewhere. These sketch outlines a dual perspective in terms of
adults face their emotional world by going on emotional education that coincides with two
the defense and constantly trying to control their dominant approaches concerning the emotional
feelings. sphere, that is, emotional regulation focused on
Certainly, Rogers spoke from the perspective control and emotional integration focused on
of adult therapy, but he outlined necessary ele- reception.
ments for a healthy life, which can be extended One of them can be identified as “emotional
to the entire life span. We would also argue that regulation,” which is the prevailing approach
8 ORON, NAVARRO-RUBIO, AND LUIS

(Gross, 2014; Vohs & Baumeister, 2011). The The question here is not whether emotional
widespread use of the term “self-regulation” regulation is useful for solving problems. In
makes it clear that many consider it the domi- fact, Lengua’s work (2002) demonstrates its
nant term for human development proposals short-term utility. Rather, we intend to reflect on
and the objective of education, as part of a whether emotional regulation’s intentional ap-
terminological universe that coincides in its ab- proach fosters children’s growth, which implies
solute reference to the “self.” It undoubtedly learning the internal complexity of emotions,
rests on the psychological process of the will, welcoming them and facing up to one’s per-
which reveals a powerful “self” that dominates sonal situation beyond the problem itself.
the environment and seeks to be independent The approach to emotional education dis-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Burman, Green, & Shanker, 2015). Emotional cussed herein—namely, emotional regulation—
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

regulation has typically been developed in the ignores the complex way in which emotions
area of problem resolution or avoidance since it emerge, as described earlier and as Bridges
is used as one of the various behavioral strate- pointed out because it attributes emotion to a
gies to prevent certain stimuli or control certain stimulus and assumes Erikson’s model of basic
behavior (Lengua, 2002). As a whole, this pro- mistrust, which leads the person to seek to con-
posal seeks to identify in order to control; it trol of her surroundings. Bridges helped us un-
aims to identify emotion and to channel it when- derstand that emotion does not arise through
ever it is considered disruptive. Following this stimulation because a stimulus’s significance
idea, emotions happen within the person, but are depends on the child’s cognitive, social, and
always meant to be external to subjectivity be- experiential development, and, without knowl-
cause an external stimulus is thought to control edge of that complex process, the reason for a
the emotion. Thus, emotion is not to be con- behavior remains known (e.g., the reason be-
fused with a veridical perception obtained hind a tantrum in the supermarket). Emotional
regulation also contradicts Rogers’ indications
through encounters with the external world.
because a child does not recognize what he or
Moreover, it often proposes an elimination of
she has not accepted and will only accept it if
the given stimulus, or, when that is not possible,
her primary caregiver first demonstrates accep-
practitioners of this approach are meant to de-
tance.
velop psychological resources to increase dis- The alternative approach, coined here as
tance relative to the stimulus. “emotional integration,” follows Erikson’s line
When presenting Erikson’s attachment and of basic trust and considers the emotional
social referencing theories, the importance of world’s complexity in accordance with Bridges,
the intention’s quality comes to fore. Thus, it is along with Rogers’ growth process and social
important to pay attention to the intentionality referencing theory. Accordingly, emotions are
of emotional regulation. In the family environ- the concentration or crystallization of a state in
ment and with children, this approach to emo- life at a specific moment. With trust, one is
tional control takes place with the control of amazed by lived and unknown emotional reali-
stimuli and internal reactions while the stimulus ties, but one does not feel the need to control
is present. The mentality with which the situa- them because one does not perceive the world
tion and the associated intentionality is ad- as hostile. This approach to emotional reality is
dressed corresponds to “problem-solving,” much more respectful because it asks after what
which sees everything prestimuli as fine and is happening in one’s self and the reason for the
everything poststimuli as a new situation that same: “Why is this happening?”
causes trouble, which requires intervention to The desire to control emerges from a per-
somehow recover the previous stability. This ceived threat rather than from a feeling of won-
kind of education is aimed at avoiding prob- der. An approach starting with wonder allows
lems, in the face of which the previous balance us to understand by respecting our surrounding
is sought. In this way, everything appears to be reality because it lacks any manipulative im-
in order, but focusing on education aimed at pulse. In this way, we discover the nuanced
problem avoidance (e.g., a tantrum at the super- aspects of an emotion, as well as what gave rise
market) does not amount to educating for de- to it. In the end, an approach starting from the
velopment. perspective of control prevents and impedes
EMOTIONAL EDUCATION FOR PERSONAL GROWTH IN INFANTS 9

understanding. Reconciliation of an education 1961, p. 86); we could say that the therapist’s
based on emotional regulation and an education acceptance opens up the possibility of accepting
aimed at emotional integration is not advisable. one’s self (Rogers, 1961, p. 133) and entails a
Philosophically speaking, these positions are disposition toward the acceptance of others
“contrarias pero no contradictorias” (meaning (Rogers, 1961, p. 86). If the therapist accepts
they are contrary, but not contradictory posi- the patient, then a true growth-enabling rela-
tions; Altarejos, 2004). They are “contrary” be- tionship will emerge (Rogers, 1961, pp. 34, 62).
cause one excludes the other, but they are “not This relationship will be extended to the facts
contradictory” because both, rooted in very dif- because they will be considered inimical (Rog-
ferent frameworks, give rise to human proactiv- ers, 1961, p. 25). The social triangle found in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ity. early childhood then resurfaces, but instead of


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

For emotional integration, emotions do not involving the caregiver-child-object, it becomes


constitute whims and are not driven by stimuli; a therapist-patient-fact triangle. Although Rog-
following Bridges’ proposal, certain caregiving ers moved in the field of adult therapy, we
and interpersonal relationship styles explain the believe that because he focused on personal
predominance of some emotions and not others growth, his ideas can be extended to the non-
in specific situations. From this perspective, we therapeutic realm as related to early childhood
do not seek to solve problems, but rather rely on development.
emotions to help us discover the complexity of Bearing in mind attachment and social refer-
a person’s situation, giving way to the growth encing theories, together with Erikson’s healthy
associated with self-knowledge. The key is dynamic of trust and Rogers’ idea that we need
growing and getting to know oneself better be- to believe in the acceptance of emotional real-
cause emotions, on the one hand, open the way ity, the most coherent emotional education pro-
toward self-knowledge, but, on the other hand, posal corresponds to emotional integration be-
propel us toward taking a position whose exe- cause it takes place in a climate of acceptance,
cution requires a more global consideration and amazement, appreciation, and intensification of
acceptance of the situation at hand. social relationships according to the child’s
In Rogers’ psychotherapeutic proposition, the needs. This approach does not seek control, but
term “acceptance” appears as the necessary psy- rather looks for acceptance in the complexity of
chological experience that initiates a path to- interpersonal relationships.
ward personal growth (Rogers, 1961, pp. 17, 82, Just as a child needs her primary caregiver to
133). There is no way to bridge or avoid such an introduce her to the outside world, the same is
experience, without which the person becomes true of her inner world. A child’s emotional
blocked (Rogers, 1961, p. 122). Acceptance world initially surfaces as an enigma to the child
represents personal growth both at the begin- herself, but if the primary caregiver accepts her
ning of this process and in later stages, which is emotional reality in an environment that safe-
understood in the key of acceptance in the ser- guards the interpersonal relationship, then she
vice of the self’s growth. Thus, the goal goes can calmly approach her own emotional reality
from fixing the problem to seeing it as an op- in a dynamic of wonder and openness to getting
portunity for personal growth (Rogers, 1961, p. to know herself and growing in self-knowledge.
150). Instead, if the primary caregiver reacts to a
Acceptance necessarily implies staving off child’s emotional situation as if it were a mis-
the immediate desire to change reality as re- fortune or something in need of regulation
ceived because there are contrary dynamics in- (which usually happens when we seek to avoid
volved throughout (Rogers, 1961, p. 21). In- problems), she will perceive her emotional sit-
stead, it implies assuming reality “as it is” uation as negative and disruptive and as some-
without pretending to fix anything (Rogers, thing that needs to be controlled or eliminated,
1961, p. 115). Feelings that thwart acceptance preventing self-knowledge and acceptance of
represent fear of one’s self (Rogers, 1961, p. reality.
52). If emotions originate in the complexity of
The acceptance process is relational rather social interaction and the multidimensional de-
than individual. In fact, acceptance itself is pre- velopment that Bridges (and others like Barrett,
ceded by the therapist’s acceptance (Rogers, 2017) demonstrated, then only amazement (and
10 ORON, NAVARRO-RUBIO, AND LUIS

not fear or shock) from the primary caregiver giver displays mistrust in the face of the child’s
and the dynamics of emotional integration help emotional state. Consequently, the child ap-
the child grow in the knowledge of that com- proaches her inner world with suspicion, which
plexity. Namely, if the primary caregiver gets leads the child toward controlling her inner re-
close to his child by trying to negatively impose ality, and thus impedes learning.
upon the child’s emotional reactions, he will
neither be able to access the complex world of
the child’s reality, nor will the child progress Winnicott and Kohut: Two Psychoanalysts
because she will seek control through conflict in Defense of Emotional Integration
just as her primary caregiver does. The pair
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

reacts with fear or suspicion toward reality, Winnicott (1986) clearly stated that, at birth,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

impeding their mutual relationship and personal the child’s foremost task is to form an image of
growth. her mother and of herself. Both images are
Yet, when it comes to a child’s emotional formed in the same process (pp. 31, 33, 50,
world, if the primary caregiver takes up an 130 –131), which is done thanks to emotional
approach of knowledge, wonder, and dialogue integration (p. 28). Therein, feelings are expe-
to find out what is really going on (when the rienced on their own (p. 82), and the newborn
child is old enough), the child will assume that child is an active and creative agent (pp. 28,
same approach and she will grow in self- 40 – 49, 144). If the child does not perform said
knowledge. Successful interpersonal encounters integration, she will possess a false image of
are not possible through control, as a child that herself (p. 33). From the moment of birth, the
is frightened by her reality, because her care- child begins to endure constant frustration (p.
giver is also fearful, tends to hide and minimize 22) because the reality that her mother is an
herself to avoid her primary caregiver’s rejec- independent entity is imposed on her and con-
tion. trasted with a second principle—the satisfaction
Thus far, these two approaches can be sum- of needs. This game is fundamental (pp. 62–
marized as follows (Figure 3): 63). The mother’s behavior should be such that
In approaches related to emotional integra- she always demonstrates acceptance in the face
tion, the primary caregiver exudes confidence of the child’s diverse reactions. If the mother
when faced with the child’s emotional state; does not show this unconditional acceptance,
from there, he approaches the child’s inner the child will not be able to integrate a diversi-
world with wonder. This, in turn, encourages fied emotional range, nor form a good image of
the child to wonder and wish to discover more herself or of her mother (p. 94) because the
of her inner reality. Eventually, then, the child child needs to accept both as contrasting ten-
will master inner complexity. Regarding emo- dencies and will only do so if the mother does
tional regulation approaches, the primary care- so (pp. 87– 88, 95). For this reason, the mother

EMOTIONAL INTEGRATION
Mother shows Child will
Child will learn
confidence approach with Child wants to
inner
before the child’s wonder to his know
complexity
emoonal state inner world

EMOTIONAL REGULATION
Mother shows Child will
Child will not
distrust before approach with Child wants to
learn inner
the child’s suspicion and control /master
complexity
emoonal state doubt

Figure 3. Two emotional education approaches. See the online article for the color version
of this figure.
EMOTIONAL EDUCATION FOR PERSONAL GROWTH IN INFANTS 11

is meant to abandon exclusive focus on behav- Theory in Practice


ioral control (p. 120).
Winnicott realized that it is better to under- Returning to our example of the child who
stand development starting from birth in terms wants to play and the mother who wants the
of identity than as a game of competing forces child to eat, a third option based on our educa-
(Winnicott, 1986, p. 26), whereas Kohut com- tional proposal emerges when the mother stops
pleted this idea by proposing that both healthy to think, “What is my child experiencing?”
and pathological development are understood in From birth, whenever a child faces a problem,
a much more holistic way if we focus on the “I she cries. She does not know the cause; she
or me” (self). Kohut overcame Freud’s proposal merely detects a problem and cries. Crying is
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of the “ego” as a place that reconciles and like “sounding an alarm bell” to which the care-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

balances these forces (Kohut, 2009, pp. 41, 70 – taker responds and tries to fix the situation. The
71, 75). child discovers that crying translates into a re-
Kohut (2009) argued that understanding the quest for help. In our case, the child wills to
self as a conglomerate of tensions or drives play and sees that what she wants is not coming
(drive is the term he used) sets up a pathological to fruition. This is called frustration. Feeling
situation. He argued that the child is born into frustrated in that context is a new experience for
trust, and the primary caregiver confirms or the child. The child does not know what to do
denies that trust (p. 119). When denied, the with this frustration; in fact, she does not even
person will end up being a “bag of drives” as a know that this experience is called “frustration.”
She only notices his discomfort associated with
result of empathy failures (p. 122). When the
frustration and “sounds the alarm bell,” asking
mother interacts with the child, this interaction
for help with her cry.
does not involve a mere balance of forces, but
The mother must ask herself, “How can I
rather the formation of the self (p. 75); the
understand this situation?” If she understands
caregiver’s reflection of what he perceives in-
that the child is experiencing frustration, she
fluences how the child is able to understand can focus on relating to the child so that the
herself (p. 76). If the caregiver presents the child learns to use frustration as an opportunity
child with a game of tensions that balance or for growth. Frustration often comes up in life,
channel into concrete behavior— either pleasing and it is more important to learn how to channel
or correcting (p. 79 – 81)—the child forms an frustration toward growth than it is to set up
understanding of herself as reflected in that routines around meals. If the mother uses the
game. The child needs to form an image of child’s frustration to improve the interpersonal
herself as a self beyond a mere interplay of relationship between them, the child will repli-
forces, but the child will do so only if she has an cate this dynamic in the future.
overall joyful experience (p. 81). Such an expe- Following Kohut’s proposal, we focus on the
rience originates in the emotional experience “self” and not on the qualities of the self. That
that the caregiver transmits, with the tone of her is, we focus on the person who cries and not on
voice and through other means (p. 86), to the the cries of the person. The child then discovers
child in everyday interactions (pp. 179 –180). that her mother is focused on her and not on her
Fundamentally, feelings arise as result of in- behavior. Obviously, learning that lesson takes
teraction (p. 87). Kohut also insisted that we not time. After discarding Solutions 1 and 2, how
focus on conflict, but rather on the self (p. 95, might the mother intervene? The third option
130). A sole focus on the forces in play fails to consists in the child discovering that her crying
recognize that this game of forces is not the does not separate her from her mother and that
cause of bad experiences, but rather the effect of her mother continues to interact with her as if
previous disintegration of the self (pp. 104 – she had not cried while feeding her. At the
105, 116, 122). In addition, focusing on them behavioral level, the third option and Option 2.2
and seeking their equilibrium entails cornering are very similar. In fact, the child will likely fail
the child into a pathological structure that is far to not notice any difference between the two,
from the creative and joyful experience of the but the mother notes the difference. In the third
self that the child needs to develop healthily (p. option, the mother knows that the crying is not
134). a problem, nor does she attempt to impose her
12 ORON, NAVARRO-RUBIO, AND LUIS

will. The situation is not understood as a “strug- tegration is assumed as the starting point, and
gle of wills,” but rather as a request for help and experience confirms or denies that assumption.
she relates to her child as someone asking her Confirmation of relational integration occurs
for help. In Case 2.2, the mother understands through the integration of the child’s experience
the situation as a “struggle of wills”; the child and action within the preexisting caregiver–
wants to impose her will, and the mother has to child relationship.
juggle it all to achieve her end. The mother Another philosophical debate that underlies
therefore notes the difference, which is reflected this article revolves around the term intention-
in her experience of stress, as Situation 2.2 is ality. John Wilson (1972) studied various au-
much more stressful and exhausting. thors and unraveled the existing controversy
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

This article awakens many philosophical top- over this term in the field of emotions. He
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ics specifically about the formation of self- believes the debate surrounding intentionality
awareness, human action, and the integration of needs to clarify how the subject is understood,
experience. We have eschewed a philosophical his activity and the object itself. In addition, he
debate to focus on children’s lived experience, adds the question of whether every emotional
instead of asking adults to understand children’s situation is intentional (in regards to an object)
world through adult philosophical categories. or if the possibility of unintentional emotional
On a philosophical level, the issue of the states also exists. For his part, Goldie (2000, pp.
integration of experience can be placed along a 16 –36, 62–71) indicates that intentionality at-
spectrum. On the one hand, we find authors that tributed to emotion depends on how emotional
start from the distance between the self and the reality is understood since, if we accept that
world and seek to integrate experience, for ex- understanding of a reality is implicit in emo-
ample, Helm and Goldie (2006), Hume and tional experience, the emotion’s intentionality
Hume (2014), and Kant (2008). On the other could come from one of its constituent elements
hand, others are closer to a unified interpreta-
(the understanding implicit in the emotion) and
tion, such as the Gestalt theory (Metz-Göckel,
not necessarily from the emotion in itself.
2015) or Whitehead and Griffin (1978) who
Resolving this debate goes far beyond the
start from the internal relationship.
limitations of this article, although it certainly
On the one hand, we would like to set aside
the debate about the emergence of the self be- reveals that the emotional value of an object
cause it is a very slow process that begins in does not reside in the object, but rather in the
utero (Delafield-Butt & Gangopadhyay, 2013; interpersonal, child– caregiver encounter sur-
Delafield-Butt & Trevarthen, 2015) and comes rounding an object. In addition, as Kohut indi-
to a close in adolescence (Erikson, 1968). But, cated, in the child’s emotional experience, the
on the other hand, we believe this article con- object to be understood is the child herself and
tributes to the philosophical debate regarding not simply an outside reality. The child is in
questions that even a toddler somehow asks formation at the same time that she forms her
after, such as, “What do I feel? Who am I? Who understanding of the world and of herself, and it
is the other? And how do I interact with my is difficult to separate these understandings.
caregiver(s)?” In reality, they are all the same This greatly complicates the issue of intention-
question. That is, the child answers them all at ality because, historically, discussion of inten-
the same time—without forgetting that the child tionality usually focuses on considering an ob-
does not respond directly to these questions, but ject as external.
rather acquires a general, basic disposition com- In addition to this complexity, it should be
mon to all these questions that emerges during added that emotional experience is not closed,
the maturation process. but rather is highly sensitive to reworking, even
This position is confirmed in psychoanalysis in adults, as Scruton studied in relation to aes-
(Kohut, 1977; Winnicott, 1986) as its starting thetic experience (Scruton, 1983, pp. 138 –152).
point is not the distance between the self and the This is because meaning and value is first de-
world, and therefore integration is not seen as a termined through an interpersonal relationship,
problem to solve; rather it starts with the rela- such that, as interpersonal relationships evolve,
tionship between the mother and the child as a it is to be expected that meaning and value also
unit (M. S. Mahler, 1974). Thus, relational in- change.
EMOTIONAL EDUCATION FOR PERSONAL GROWTH IN INFANTS 13

Conclusion lationships and expectations for peer interaction.


Early Education and Development, 2, 214–226.
As Bridges and social referencing theory pro- http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed0203_4
pose, emotions do not result from one stimulus, Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful
but rather derive from life’s complexity. As control, executive function, and false belief un-
Rogers proposed, we can only understand our derstanding to emerging math and literacy abil-
ity in kindergarten. Child Development, 78,
complexity by accepting it. As Erikson and at- 647– 663. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624
tachment theory suggest, the child can only .2007.01019.x
accept what her mother or primary caregiver Bodrova, E. (2008). Make-believe play versus aca-
accepts. Social referencing reveals that the child demic skills: A Vygotskian approach to today’s
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

understands reality (including her own reality) dilemma of early childhood education. European
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in accordance with her primary caregiver’s un- Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 16,
derstanding of that same reality. As Winnicot 357–369. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13502930
(1986) proposed, only when the primary care- 802291777
giver integrates the child’s emotional situations Bornsteina, M. H., Putnicka, D. L., Rigo, P., Es-
can she also integrate them. As Kohut pur- posito, G., Swaind, J. E., Suwalskya, J. T. D., . . .
Venutib, P. (2017). Neurobiology of culturally
ported, nothing less than the construction of the
common maternal responses to infant cry. Pro-
self is at stake. ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
Good early childhood emotional education the United States of America, 114, E9465–E9473.
involves the caregiver welcoming the child’s http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1712022114
emotional reality at a personal level, which al- Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss (2nd ed.,
lows the child to develop an adequate inner Vol. 1, Attachment). New York, NY: Basic Books.
disposition. Said caregiver uses everyday situa- Bridges, K. M. B. (1932). Emotional development in
tions to communicate personal acceptance of early infancy. Child Development, 3, 324–341.
the child, while teaching her how to live in a http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1125359
home without losing sight of the fact that the Burman, J. T., Green, C. D., & Shanker, S. (2015).
caregiver is ultimately teaching the child who On the meanings of self-regulation: Digital hu-
manities in service of conceptual clarity. Child
she is (the self). We call this proposal “emo-
Development, 86, 1507–1521. http://dx.doi.org/10
tional integration” because its key point in- .1111/cdev.12395
volves integrating emotional reality for the im- Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C.
provement of personal relationships. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional
development and emotion regulation. Develop-
References mental Psychology, 25, 394– 402. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.394
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attach- Camras, L. (2011). Differentiation, dynamical inte-
ment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated by gration and functional emotional development.
the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situa- Emotion Review, 3, 138–146. http://dx.doi.org/10
tion. Child Development, 41, 49– 67. http://dx.doi .1177/1754073910387944
.org/10.2307/1127388 Carson, R. (2012). The sense of wonder. Madrid,
Altarejos, F. (2004). Self-regulation and integration: Spain: Encuentro.
Two proposals in the education of affectivity (D. Clark-Polner, E., & Clark, M. S. (2014). Understand-
Goleman and Tomás de Aquino). Studies on edu- ing and accounting for relational context is critical
cation, 7, 45– 66. for social neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neu-
Atzil, S., Touroutoglou, A., Rudy, T., Salcedo, S., roscience, 8, 127. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum
Feldman, R., Hooker, J. M., . . . Barrett, L. F. .2014.00127
(2017). Dopamine in the medial amygdala network Delafield-Butt, J. T., & Gangopadhyay, N. (2013).
mediates human bonding. Proceedings of the Na- Sensorimotor intentionality: The origins of inten-
tional Academy of Sciences of the United States of tionality in prospective agent action. Developmen-
America, 114, 2361–2366. tal Review, 33, 399– 425. http://dx.doi.org/10
Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made. Lon- .1016/j.dr.2013.09.001
don, United Kingdom: Macmillan. Delafield-Butt, J. T., & Trevarthen, C. (2015). The
Berghout Austin, A. M., Godfrey, M. K., Weber, C., ontogenesis of narrative: From purposeful move-
Martin, C. A., & Holmes, L. B. (1991). The rela- ments to shared meaning-making. Frontiers in
tionship between security of attachment and the Psychology, 6, 1157. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/
development of a personal premise system of re- fpsyg.2015.01157
14 ORON, NAVARRO-RUBIO, AND LUIS

de Rosnay, M., Cooper, P. J., Tsigaras, N., & Murray, view, 111, 132–135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1215/
L. (2006). Transmission of social anxiety from 00318108-111-1-132
mother to infant: An experimental study using a Hong, Y. R., & Park, J. S. (2012). Impact of attach-
social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research ment, temperament and parenting on human devel-
and Therapy, 44, 1165–1175. http://dx.doi.org/10 opment. Korean Journal of Pediatrics, 55, 449–
.1016/j.brat.2005.09.003 454. http://dx.doi.org/10.3345/kjp.2012.55.12.449
Domhardt, M., Münzer, A., Fegert, J. M. J., & Gold- Hume, D., & Hume, D. (2014). A treatise of human
beck, L. (2015). Resilience in survivors of child nature. In D. F. Norton & M. J. Norton (Eds.), The
sexual abuse: A systematic review of the literature. clarendon edition of the works of David Hume: A
Trauma, Violence and Abuse, 16, 476– 493. http:// treatise of human nature, Vol. 1: Texts. http://dx
dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524838014557288 .doi.org/10.1093/oseo/instance.00032872
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Downs, A., Strand, P., & Cerna, S. (2007). Emotion Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B. B., & Schyns, P. G.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

understanding in English- and Spanish-speaking (2014). Dynamic facial expressions of emotion


preschoolers enrolled in head start. Social Devel- transmit an evolving hierarchy of signals over
opment, 16, 410– 439. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j time. Current Biology, 24, 187–192. http://dx.doi
.1467-9507.2007.00391.x .org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.064
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the cycle of life: Kant, I. (2008). Critique of pure reason. New York,
Selected papers. Psychological Issues, 1, 5–165. NY: Willey Book Co. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd 11654-000
ed.). New York, NY: Norton. Klinnert, M., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Cam-
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. pos, J. J. (1986). Social referencing: The infant’s
New York, NY: Norton. use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with
Feinman, S., & Lewis, M. (1983). Social referencing mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22,
at ten months: A second-order effect on infants’ 427– 432. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22
responses to strangers. Child Development, 54, .4.427
878– 887. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1129892 Kohut, H. (Ed.). (1977). The termination of the anal-
Glaser, D. (2014). The effects of child maltreatment ysis of narcissistic personality disorders. The res-
on the developing brain. The Medico-Legal Jour- toration of the self (pp. 1– 62). Chicago, IL: The
nal, 82, 97–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ University of Chicago Press.
0025817214540395 Kohut, H. (2009). The restoration of the Self. Chi-
Goldie, P. (2000). The emotions: A philosophical cago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. http://
exploration. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. dx.doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226450155.001
Greenspan, S. I. (2007a). Great kids: Helping your .0001
baby and child develop the ten essential qualities Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M., & Liu, H.-M. (2003). For-
for a healthy, happy life. Retrieved from http:// eign-language experience in infancy: Effects of
ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T⫽JS&PAGE⫽ short-term exposure and social interaction on pho-
reference&D⫽psyc5&NEWS⫽N&AN⫽2007- netic learning. Proceedings of the National Acad-
08601-000 emy of Sciences of the United States of America,
Greenspan, S. I. (2007b). Levels of infant-caregiver 100, 9096–9101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas
interactions and the DIR model: Implications for .1532872100
the development of signal affects, the regulation of L’Ecuyer, C. (2014). The wonder approach to learn-
mood and behavior, the formation of a sense of ing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 764.
self, the creation of internal representation, and the Lengua, L. J. (2002). The contribution of emotional-
construction of defenses and character structure. ity and self-regulation to the understanding of chil-
Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psycho- dren’s response to multiple risk. Child Develop-
therapy, 6, 174–210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ ment, 73, 144–161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
15289160701657662 1467-8624.00397
Groh, A. M., Fearon, R. M. P., van IJzendoorn, Leong, V., Byrne, E., Clackson, K., Georgieva, S.,
M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Roisman, Lam, S., & Wass, S. (2017). Speaker gaze in-
G. I. (2017). Attachment in the early life course: creases information coupling between infant and
Meta-analytic evidence for its role in socioemo- adult brains. Proceedings of the National Academy
tional development. Child Development Perspec- of Sciences of the United States of America, 114,
tives, 11, 70–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdep 13290–13295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas
.12213 .1702493114
Gross, J. J. (2014). Handbook of emotion regulation. Lewis, M. (2007). Early emotional development. In
New York, NY: The Guilford Press. A. Slater & M. Lewis (Eds.), Introduction to infant
Helm, B. W., & Goldie, P. (2006). The emotions: A development (2nd ed., pp. 217–224). Oxford,
philosophical exploration. The Philosophical Re- United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.
EMOTIONAL EDUCATION FOR PERSONAL GROWTH IN INFANTS 15

Mahler, M. S. (1974). Symbiosis and individua- intimacy: Parallels between Bowlby’s and Erik-
tion. The psychological birth of the human in- son’s paradigms. Journal of Family Theory and
fant. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 29, Review, 3, 32– 46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
89–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00797308 .1756-2589.2010.00079.x
.1974.11822615 Pratt, M., Goldstein, A., Levy, J., & Feldman, R.
Mahler, M. S., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (2018). The (2017). Maternal depression across the first years
psychological birth of the human infant: Symbiosis of life impacts the neural basis of empathy in
and individuation. London, United Kingdom: preadolescence. Journal of the American Academy
Routledge. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780429 of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56, 20–29.e3.
482915 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.10.012
Malik, F., & Marwaha, R. (2018). Development, Provençal, N., & Binder, E. B. (2015). The effects of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

stages of social emotional development in chil- early life stress on the epigenome: From the womb
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

dren. Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing. to adulthood and even before. Experimental Neu-
Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/ rology, 268, 10–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
books/NBK534819/ .expneurol.2014.09.001
Metz-Göckel, H. (2015). Gestalt theory. Dialogue Repacholi, B. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Emo-
and Universalism, 25, 21–36. http://dx.doi.org/10 tional eavesdropping: Infants selectively respond
.5840/du201525482 to indirect emotional signals. Child Development,
Mireault, G. C., Crockenberg, S. C., Sparrow, J. E., 78, 503–521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
Pettinato, C. A., Woodard, K. C., & Malzac, K. 8624.2007.01012.x
(2014). Social looking, social referencing and hu- Ritvo, S. (2018). The psychological birth of the hu-
mor perception in 6- and-12-month-old infants. man infant. Symbiosis and individuation. By Mar-
Infant Behavior and Development, 37, 536–545. garet S. Mahler, Fred Pine, and Anni Bergman.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.06.004 New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1975. 308 pp. The
Morales, S., Brown, K. M., Taber-Thomas, B. C., Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 45, 620– 645. http://dx
LoBue, V., Buss, K. A., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. .doi.org/10.1080/21674086.1976.11926782
(2017). Maternal anxiety predicts attentional bias Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston,
towards threat in infancy. Emotion, 17, 874– 883. MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000275 Sander, L. W. (1977). The regulation of exchange in
Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., & the infant caretaker system and some aspects of the
Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential under- context-content relationship. In M. Lewis & L.
standing in the emotions domain at twelve and Rosenblum (Eds.), Interaction conversation and
eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718– the development of language (pp. 133–156). New
735. York, NY: Wiley Press.
Murray, L., de Rosnay, M., Pearson, J., Bergeron, C., Scruton, R. (1983). Freedom and custom. Royal In-
Schofield, E., Royal-Lawson, M., & Cooper, P. J. stitute of Philosophy Lecture Series, 15, 181–196.
(2008). Intergenerational transmission of social http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0957042x00009056
anxiety: The role of social referencing processes in Silk, J. S., Redcay, E., & Fox, N. A. (2014). Contri-
infancy. Child Development, 79, 1049–1064. butions of social and affective neuroscience to our
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008 understanding of typical and atypical develop-
.01175.x ment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8,
Park, K. A., & Waters, E. (1989). Security of attach- 1– 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.02.002
ment and preschool friendships. Child Develop- Sroufe, L. A. (2005). Attachment and development:
ment, 60, 1076–1081. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/ A prospective, longitudinal study from birth to
1130781 adulthood. Attachment and Human Development,
Pelaez, M., Virues-Ortega, J., Field, T. M., Amir- 7, 349–367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14616730
Kiaei, Y., & Schnerch, G. (2013). Social referenc- 500365928
ing in infants of mothers with symptoms of de- Theisen, S., & Erikson, E. H. (2007). Childhood and
pression. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, society. The American Catholic Sociological Re-
548–556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013 view, 12, 33. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3707421
.05.003 Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared
Pelaez, M., Virues-Ortega, J., & Gewirtz, J. L. intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121–
(2012). Acquisition of social referencing via dis- 125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007
crimination training in infants. Journal of Applied .00573.x
Behavior Analysis, 45, 23–36. http://dx.doi.org/10 Trevarthen, C. (2005). Stepping away from the mir-
.1901/jaba.2012.45-23 ror: Pride and shame in adventures of companion-
Pittman, J. F., Keiley, M. K., Kerpelman, J. L., & ship. In C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossman, S.
Vaughn, B. E. (2011). Attachment, identity, and Hardy, M. E. Lamb, & S. W. Proges (Eds.), At-
16 ORON, NAVARRO-RUBIO, AND LUIS

tachment and bonding: A new synthesis (pp. 55– during the session 1927–28. New York, NY: The
84). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Free Press.
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional com- Wilson, J. (1972). Motivation and morality. Journal
munication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, of Moral Education, 2, 25–29. http://dx.doi.org/10
112–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44 .1080/0305724720020104
.2.112 Winnicott, D. W. (1986). Home is where we start
Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2011). Handbook from. Essays by a psychoanalist. London, United
of self-regulation. New York, NY: The Guilford Kingdom: Penguin books.
Press. Zhang, M., Chen, X., Deng, H., & Lu, Z. (2014).
Walden, T. A., & Ogan, T. A. (1988). The develop- Infant behavior and development identifying the
ment of social referencing. Child Development, 59, interaction of maternal sensitivity and two sero-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1230–1240. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130486 tonin-related gene polymorphisms on infant. Infant


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Behavior and Development, 37, 606– 614. http://
Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale: dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.06.009
Longitudinal perspectives. Child Development, 82,
780–792. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624
.2011.01583.x Received May 28, 2019
Whitehead, A. N., & Griffin, D. R. (1978). Process Revision received October 29, 2019
and reality an essay in cosmology of Edinburgh Accepted January 27, 2020 䡲

You might also like