1981 Functions IE

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/15921268

The Functions of Immediate Echolalia in Autistic Children

Article in Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders · September 1981


DOI: 10.1044/jshd.4603.241 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
316 11,544

2 authors:

Barry M Prizant Judith Felson Duchan


Brown University University at Buffalo, The State University of New York
78 PUBLICATIONS 6,004 CITATIONS 103 PUBLICATIONS 2,876 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Barry M Prizant on 06 August 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


THE FUNCTIONS OF IMMEDIATE ECHOLALIA
IN A U T I S T I C C H I L D R E N

BARRY M. PRIZANT JUDITH F. DUCHAN


State Universit~l of New York at Buffalo

This research was intended to discover how immediate eeholalia fimctioned tbr autistic children in interactions with tamiliar
adults. Four echolalic children were videotaped at school and at home, in both group and dyadic interactions in natural situa-
tions such as hmchtime, family activities, and play activities in school. After conducting a multilevel analysis (of over 1,000
utterances) of verbal and nonverbal factors, response latency, and intonation, it was discovered that immediate echolalia is fiir
more than a meaningless hehavior, as has been previously reported. Seven functional categories of eeholalia were discovered
and are discussed in reterence to behavioral and linguistic features of each category. It is argued that researchers who protx)se
interxention progrmns of echo-abatement may be overlooking the important communicative and cognitive timctions echolalia
may serve ~br the autistic child.

Language abnormalities and problems in communica- maintain social interaction in the face of a severe com-
tion are primary criteria in the diagnosis of autism. Of prehension problem. He believed that autistic echolalia
the language abnormalities, immediate echolalia is the has as its basis " v e r b a l c o m p r e h e n s i o n difficulties
most frequently cited characteristic of verbal autistic coupled with an urge to sustain . . . social contact" (pg.
children (Prizant, 1975). Rutter (1968) and Wing (1971) 45). Similarly, Shapiro (1977) has attributed the function
found that echolalia was characteristic of some three- of social facilitation to immediate eeholalia, and Philips
quarters of the autistic children they studied. Fay (1969) and Dyer (1977) have gone as far as to hypothesize that
defined immediate echolalia as the "meaningless repeti- immediate eeholalia is a necessary stage of language de-
tion of a word or word group just spoken by another per- velopment for verbal autistic children. In describing "af-
son." (pg. 39). In attempting to differentiate between au- firmation by repetition," Kanner (1946) has been the
tistic and non-autistic manifestations of echolalia, he only researcher to ascribe a specific function for im-
noted that in autism, eeholalia is evidenced at preschool mediate echolalia. Although these authors have at-
and school ages and is characterized by longer echoic tempted to understand immediate eeholalia from a func-
utterances, a larger percentage of echoic utterances, de- tional perspective, the functions immediate echolalia
layed echolalia, and minimal mitigation (change or revi- may serve still need to be specified.
sion). Previous research has not provided information on its
There is much controversy regarding the significance functions, due to a number of related reasons. First of all,
of immediate echolalia for autistic children. On one the primary focus of such research has been on the lan-
hand, behaviorally oriented researchers have considered guage structure of echolalic utterances. For example,
eeholalia a nonfunctional (Koegel, Lovaas, & Schreib- Fay (1967) and Shapiro, Roberts, and Fish (1970) de-
man, 1974) or an undesirable symptom (Coleman & vised classifications of echolalic utterances that de-
Stedman, 1974) of the language behavior of autistic chil- scribed structural linguistic change imposed on the orig-
dren. Such researchers have considered echolalia a inal utterance. Voeltz (1977) and Buium and Steucher
communication disorder in itself and are therefore advo- (1974) demonstrated that autistic echolalia was not just
cates of the extinction or replacement of echolalic be- rote repetition but involved structural changes that dem-
haviors through the use of behavior modification proce- onstrated syntactic rule mediation. Although these
dures (Lovaas, 1977). studies provided important information regarding struc-
On the other hand, a number of theorists have recently tural linguistic considerations of immediate echolalia,
considered immediate echolalia in terms of how it may they offered little insight regarding functionality.
function for autistic children. Fay (1969) has suggested A second characteristic of previous studies, which may
that immediate echolalia enables autistic children to be partially due to the focus on language structure, is the
lack of description of immediate echolalia as it occurs in
natural communicative interactions. Without exception,
because communicative context and nonverbal behavior
I Barr!! M. Prizant, PhD., is affiliated with the Southern ]
Illinois Universit!t at Carbondale. Judith F, Duchan were of little or no interest to the researchers, previous
is affiliated with State Universit!l of New York at
B,(ffalo.
J studies have used audiotapes or written transcripts. In
addition, researchers (Schreibman & Carr, 1978; Voeltz,
1977) have commonly used a corpus of stimulus utter-

Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 241 © 1981, American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
0022-4677/81/4603-024151.00/0

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


242 Journalof Speech and Hearing Disorders 241-249 August 1981

ances that did not relate to a stream of interaction. output and conclude that they are meaningless or non-
Therefore, one may question whether the utterances that functional by reference only to the structures them-
were elicited in such controlled experiments are repre- selves. Unfortunately, this has been the situation in most
sentative of the phenomenon of immediate echolalia as it of the literature concerned with immediate eeholalia of'
oceurs in natural interactions. autistic children.
Finally, immediate echolalia of autistic children has The present study considered many of the issues
most often been considered a unitary phenomenon that raised by the pragmatic literature, including concern for
represents a child's lack of comprehension of the model natural e'ontexts, situational factors, and nonverbal be-
utterance. Previous researchers have not considered haviors co-occurring with the production of the eeholalie
immediate eeholalia as possibly reflecting "degrees" of utterances. The major purpose of this study was to de-
comprehension, with the surface manifestations func- termine the answer to the following question: Is im-
tioning as realizations of differing underlying processes. mediate echolalia a unitary phenomenon that reflects a
Shapiro and Lucy (1978) have claimed that immediate lack of comprehension, or does it serve a variety of func-
echolalia is reflective of a lack of comprehension and tions?
low level cognitive processing. They measured response
latencies (time between offset of a model utterance and METHOD
onset of a child's response) of eehoie utterances and
spontaneous utterances of autistie children. The authors
Subjects
found that echoic responses were produced with signifi-
cantly shorter mean latencies than were the Spontaneous Four boys were the subjects. Each had been diag-
utterances, thereby suggesting a response process of less nosed as autistic by a psychologist, psychiatrist, or pro-
complexity for immediate echolalia. Along these lines, fessional evaluation team. In general, each of the chil-
Schuler (1979) has suggested that it may be fruitful to dren displayed the four central characteristics of the
evaluate eeholalic behavior in terms of automaticity vs. autistic syndrome (Rutter, 1974), including (1) disturb-
intentionality. In addition, there is little discussion of au- ances of language and communication, (2) ritualistic and
tistic echolalia as a transitional phenomenon, in which compulsive behaviors (insistence on sameness), (3) dis-
the evolution of linguistic knowledge may be traced in turbed social relationships, and (4) onset of the disorder
the structure and function ofecholalic responses. prior to 30 months of age. At the beginning of' the study,
In consideration of such deficiencies, the major focus the ages of the subjects were 4:8, 5:2, 6:3, and 9:3.
of this research was to analyze immediate eeholalia of As an attempt to obtain an adequate data base, the
autistic children as it occurs in natural interactions. The children had to demonstrate that at least 25% of all ver-
motivation for undertaking this study emerges from our bal productions were echolalic responses. Information to
own clinical interactions, informal observations, and dis, determine this was obtained through a preliminary ses-
eussions with many parents and teachers who indicate sion with the examiner in which sta'uctured activities de-
that immediate eeholalia appears to serve different func- signed to elicit verbal responses were used. The per-
tions for autistic children. Typical comments of those centage of echoic responses was based on the first 100
who interact with eeholalic autistic children include "he utterances that each child produced in the session. Table
tells himself what to do," "he learns language through 1 presents the results of standardized intelligence testing
repeating," and "echoing helps him to understand." and a breakdown of each child's expressive language
Recent literature in pragmatics (Bates, 1976; Dore, behavior according to utterance types. Except for im-
1975: Halliday, 1975), the study of language usage in mediate eeholalia, the percentages cited are approxima-
context, has emphasized that to understand fully the sig- tions.
nificance of verbal language or vocalization, it must be
studied in reference to nonverbal behavior, paralingnis- Data Collection
tic f~atures of the utteranee, and situational context. The
literature in pragmaties has also emphasized that lan- IMbrmation and data fbr the study were collected
guage development involves much more than the acqui- through three distinct methods:
sition of language strueture. It involves the mastery of 1. Interactions with the subjects provided the inves-
basic fimctions of language, and researchers such as tigators with the opportunity to evaluate each child's
Bruner (1975), Bates (1976), and Halliday (1975) have eommunieative abilities (approximately 500 hours).
demonstrated that such functions are expressed in a Analysis of language samples collected in a variety of
child's vocalizations prior to and along with the use of contexts was the primary means of evaluation.
words. However, one apparent weakness of the recent 2. Further considerations of' each child's communicative
pragmatie literature is that such functional categories abilities were based on information acquired through
were most often predetermined and were not derived discussions with the teachers and parents of the chil-
from the data. dren.
The pragmatic literature does offer, however, a basic 3. Systematic analyses of the videotapes provided the
fi'amework fbr the study of immediate echolalia of autis- primary data concerning the structure and usage of
tic children. Such literature has suggested that one immediate eeholalia produced by the children in the
cannot examine surf?tee structures of a child's linguistic study. The specific results of the study were based

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


PRIZANT,DUCHAN:Immediate Echolalia 243

TABLE 1. Breakdown by percentage of each child's expressive language behavior according to utterance types, and results of
standardized intelligence testing.

Other verbal
productions
Approximations of (delayed Results of
% of immediate Spontaneous eeholalia, Intelligence
Child Age echolalia* Speech** rote routines +) Test

Brian (6:3) 27.7%(205/740)


++ 60% 10% 76 (Leiter International
Scale)
Jeff (5:2) 40.4%(334/827) 50% 10% 69 (Stanford-Binet Scale)
David (4:8) 29.4%(183/623) 10% 60% Untestable
Robbie (9:3) 53.0%(287/541) Negligible 50% Untestable

*Based on analysis of the tapes.


**Approximationsbased on the videotapesand judgements of the language pathologist, teacher, and teacher assistant.
+Memorized verbal routines which are not necessarily reflective of comprehension (e.g., Adult: What do you say?; Child: Thank you.).
+ + Number of immediate echoes/numberof total utterances.

solely on the analysis (approximately 250 hours) of cause of the objective of deriving a categorical system
the echoic utterances from the videotapes. from the data, we needed to analyze the factors involving
Each of the subjects was videotaped in the following (1) c o m m u n i c a t i v e context, (2) structural linguistic
situations: characteristics of the echoic utterances, and (3) latency
1. H o m e - - T h i s situation involved direct interaction be- between the end of the original utterance and the onset
tween the child and one or more family members in of the echoic utterance. The importance of the factors
the home, in which specific requests were made of was suggested by informal observations and pilot inves-
the child. The only instruction given to the parents tigations conducted by the first author (Prizant, 1975).
was to engage the child in activities that were familiar The following are the factors that were deemed appro-
to him and would be conducive to social interaction. priate for analysis:
2. School-Individual--These situations involved each Communicative Context. Responses were examined
child in his classroom setting, in whieh he was in- for the following characteristics:
teracting directly and on an individual basis with the a) Was the child being addressed directly?
teacher or language clinician. Specific requests were b) Did the child change his behavior in any way at the
presented to the child in the framework of the child's time of the production of the echoic utterance?
daily school activities. e) What or who was the child looking at prior to, during,
3. Sehool-GroupmThese situations were characteristic and after the echoic response? (Gaze behavior)
of group activities and interaction in each child's d) Was the echoic utterance functionally appropriate to
classroom, (e.g., lunchtime physical games). the task?
Each child was videotaped three times in individual e) Did the child display any indications of expecting a
interactions with diftbrent, but familiar adults, and one fhrther response from the previous speaker (verbal or
time at home, In addition, five periods of group activities nonverbal?)
were videotaped at approximately one-month intervals. Structural Characteristics. Each echoic response was
All echoic utterances were extracted for analysis from compared to the speaker's utterance according to struc-
the 21 videotapes, which averaged 25 minutes per re- tural change such as a) addition, deletion, or substitution
cording. The data collection extended over an eight- of segmentals; b) tempo and loudness; and c) intonation
month period fbr each child. contour.
The echoic utterances were of the Wpe of repetition Measurement of latenc~t of onset of the echoic utter-
that is commonly referred to as immediate eeholalia. The ance. Subsequent to the fnnctional categorization, re-
child's echoic response must have occurred subsequent sponse latencies were measured on a graphic level re-
to the interlocuter's utterance, and it must have con- corder to determine if the differences among the mean
sisted of segmental and/or suprasegmental similarities to latencies of the echoes in each functional category
the utterance of the previous speaker, involving either helped to differentiate the categories.
rigid echoing of the model utterance (pure echolalia) or
selective repetition of elements occurring within two ut-
terances of the original utterance. Data Analysis
An important aspect of the research was to determine
if a categorical system representing the variety of uses of In the first level of analysis, model utterances and
echolalia could be derived from the tapes. Therefore, the echoic utterances were extracted fl'om the videotapes
classification was not developed on a priori basis. Be- and each pair of utterances was coded as part of a stream

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


244 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 241-249 August 1981

TABLE2. Total echo breakdown by functional categories for each of the subjects.

%of
Functional
Category
Relative
Total to Total
Functions Brian David Jeff Robbie Echoes Echolalia
NF (non-focused) 2 13 11 14 40 3.97
TT (Turn-Taking 84 44 108 100 336 33.30
D (Declarative) 56 40 82 81 259 25.67
R (Rehearsal) 24 25 66 21 136 13.48
SR (Self-regulatory) 17 24 40 52 133 13.18
YA (Yes-Answer) 14 17 12 9 52 5.15
Req. (Request) 8 20 15 10 53 5.25
Total Immediate Echolalia 205 183 334 287 1,009
Total Utterance for each child 740 623 827 541
% Immediate Echolalia Relative to Total
Utterances 27.70 29.37 40.38 53.04

of interaction. Included was analysis of gaze behavior RESULTS


and other nonverbal c o m p o n e n t s such as pointing,
touching, general body orientation and movement, and Seven distinct categories of echolalia based on unique
manipulation of toys or objects. The second level of constellations of behavioral and linguistic features were
analysis determined the structural change in the echoic derived from the multilevel structural analyses of the
utterance, including changes in segmental, supraseg- echoic utterances. A function was attributed to each
mental, and paralinguistic characteristics. The third level structural category. Table 2 presents the proportion of
of analysis included intonation and latency analysis per- eeholalia in each category relative to the total amount of
formed on a sample of echoic utterances from each func- echolalia (1,009 examples) analyzed in the study. Nonlb-
tional category for each child. Determination of the eused echolalia was relatively rare, while turn-taking
categories was based primarily on the clustering of echoes were the most frequently (33.3%) produced of all
characteristics from the first two levels of analysis, with the categories. However, interesting patterns of the fre-
the third level providing supportive evidence in deter- quency of" functional usage are apparent by viewing the
mining integrity of the categories. data in alternative ways. Table 3 compares the produc-
One thousand and nine model utterances and the tion of echolalia produced with evidence of compre-
echoic utterances of the four children were transcribed, hension (NF, TT) for each of the children. For all four
using orthographic transcription for clearly articulated children in the study, regardless of individual com-
words and broad phonetic transcription for utterances municative abilities, the percentage of' eeholalia pro-
that deviated significantly from clearly articulated duced with evidence of comprehension was greater than
speech. Above and below each utterance, a coding sys- the percentage produced with no evidence of com-
tem described the child's gaze behavior and nonverbal prehension.
behavior in relation to the model utterance and the Interjudge reliability was measured by comparison of
echoic utterance (see Appendix A). the results obtained by the investigator with those of a
The notation above the dialogue reflects nonverbal judge who had previously observed the children. After
movements deemed significant or potentially significant being trained on the significant features of the fimctional
fbr differentiating the echoes. Simultaneous nonverbal categories, the second judge was shown a videotape of
movement was transcribed by using additional lines of
nonverbal notation above the utterance, with the accom-
panying notes providing specific information about the TABLE 3. Echoes with evidence of comprehension vs. no com-
movement. In a manner similar to the nonverbal nota- prehension.
tion, the notation under the utterance indicates the
child's gaze behavior and shifts in gaze direction. A let- Evidence of Comprehension No Comprehension
ter code was used to indicate points of significant ibcus (D, R, SR, YA, Req.) (NF, TT)
that might be important in determining the child's at-
tending behaviors (e.g., 0 = object, P = person, NS = Brian *119 (58.0%) 86 (4g.0%)
David 126 (68.9%) 57 (31.1%)
nonspecifie). In addition to nonverbal and gaze analysis, Jeff 213 (63.8%) 121 (36.2%)
notes were made as to the seating arrangement, objects Robbie 173 (60.3%) 114 (39.7%)
present, and other factors not taken into account in the
nonverbal and gaze analysis. *Number of echoic utterances.

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


PRIZANT, DUCHAN:Immediate Eeholalia 245

the children, which he had never seen. After analyzing cations of expecting a further response from the inter-
and categorizing all 53 echoic utterances from the vid- locutor (gaze hold, grabbing interloeutor's hand or arm,
eotape, his categorization was compared to the original etc.).
analysis previously conducted by the investigator. Inter- By documenting observable behaviors and determin-
judge reliability tbr fimctional categorization was .96. ing how these behaviors clustered with different echoie
Disagreement dealt with Ibatures of timing of the utter- utterances, the seven categories were derived. Each eat-
ance in relation to the child's activity. Intrajudge relia- egory was described in terms of the features needed for
bility was .97 based on reanalysis of a videotape with 63 inclusion in that category, while other co-occurring be-
echoic utterances. The reanalysis was c o n d u c t e d 6 haviors indicated variations within a particular category.
months aIker the original analysis. Therefore, some behaviors were relegated to a secondary
role in category determination.

DISCUSSION
Ascribing Functions
Deriving Structural Categories From the Tapes
Inasmuch as the categories were structural patterns
The most striking differences that were initially dis- dervied from observable behaviors, the next problem in-
covered among the echoes was that many echoes were v o l v e d the a t t r i b u t i o n of functions to each of the
produced with no change in behavior nor any task at- categories. For example, w h e n p r e s e n t e d with the
tempt (if a task was requested of the child), while others echoic utterance that Let's get the boat produced with
were produced with either an accompanying or sub- evidence of comprehension (i.e., child gets boat), there
sequent task attempt providing evidenee of some com- was the dilemma of attributing to the child either full
prehension of the previous utterance. (However, some knowledge of the semantic relations of the utterance, or
echoes not accompanied by an attempt to perfbrm an ac- little or no knowledge of the meaning of the utterance. It
tivity may reflect a lack of willingness to comply, rather is not implied that the child fully understood the seman-
than a lack of comprehension.) tic and syntactic relationships in the utteranee. However,
Another dichotomy was between echoes that appeared comprehension was considered in terms of a child's rel-
interactive in that they were directed to the interlocutor evant, intentional actions that occur immediately prior
with appropriate gaze behavior or a gaze check sub- to, during, or subsequent to the echoic utterance. Mean-
sequent to the utterance, and echoes that were nonin- ing was considered in terms of usage, or how an utter-
teraetive, as suggested by lack of directedness of the ut- ance funetioned for a child.
terance (love volume, whispering) and lack of specifie
gaze orientation. It was therefbre determined that, in
general, the fbllowing categorical possibilities existed: The Functional Categories
1. Interactive echoes with concomitant evidence indi-
Following are the defining characteristics of the func-
cating some degree of comprehension of the model
tional categories according to core attributes (see Ap-
utterance.
pendix B for summary). The descriptions represent ca-
2. Non-interactive echoes with concomitant evidence
nonical forms of each category, with the presence of the
indicating some degree of c o m p r e h e n s i o n of the
cenh:al features or core attributes being necessary tbr the
model utterance.
inclusion of an echoic utteranee in that category. Core
3. Interactive echoes with no evidence of comprehen-
attributes for the functional categories are designated by
sion of the model utterance.
an asterisk. In addition, frequently occurring secondary
4. Non-interactive echoes with no evidence of com-
attributes based on structural linguistic and/or latency
prehension of the model utterance.
analyses are listed subsequent to the core attributes.
Further subcategorization of the four major categories
dealt specifically with timing of the echoic utterance in Non-focused
relation to the child's behavior (in cases in which there *1. No evidence o£ attention to person or object as de-
were significant behavioral changes) and the specific na- termined by gaze or body orientation.
ture of the behavioral changes. Such changes included *2. No significant behavioral change indicating evidence
gestures of pointing, showing, and requesting, and such of comprehension.
*3. No evidence of echoic utterance being directed to-
actions as picking up, grabbing, and accepting objects. wards the interlocutor nor any evidenee of expecta-
With these further considerations, Category 2 (non- tion of a fhrther response from the interloeutor.
interactive, some e v i d e n c e of comprehension) sub- 4. Echo may occur during agitated or highly aroused
divided into two distinct categories as determined by state (e.g., pain, temper tantrum, etc.).
5. Echo is usually rigidly congruent to the model utter-
whether the echo occurred during the behavioral change ance.
or prior to the behavioral change (with some interval be-
tween the echoic utterance and the s u b s e q u e n t re- The category of non-tbeused echolalia appeared the
sponse). Category 1 s u b d i v i d e d into three distinct most automatic, and relatively nonflmetional variety;
categories, that were primarily determined by the nature therefore, it may be a misnomer to call it a thnctional
of the child's action on an object, and/or nonverbal indi- category. Although the occurrence of noMbcused echoes

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


246 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 241-249 August 1981

was s o m e w h a t rare (ranging from 1.0%-7.1% o f e a c h 4. Echoic utterance may range from rigidly congruent
child's utterances), e a c h child d i s p l a y e d some produc- reproduction to selective repetition involving dele-
tions and/or additions.
tions in this category. Some interesting examples from
the tapes i n c l u d e d echoes that occurred d u r i n g highly
aroused states. F o r e x a m p l e , after hitting his mouth on a D e c l a r a t i v e echoic utterances a p p e a r e d to be l a b e l i n g
large plastic rocking toy, one child e c h o e d O.K! (in a b e h a v i o r s , in w h i c h a c h i l d d e m o n s t r a t e d some com-
screaming voice) w h e n asked Are you O.K?, although he p r e h e n s i o n b y n o n v e r b a l l y i n d i c a t i n g a c o n n e c t i o n be-
was o b v i o u s l y in g r e a t p a i n and not a t t e n d i n g to the t w e e n the e c h o i c u t t e r a n c e and an object, action, or
speaker. T h e echoic response was, in essence, a synthe- location. In all instances, an object, b o d y part, or en-
sis of crying and articulating O.K. vironmental referent was central to the c h i l d ' s gesture.
O t h e r e x a m p l e s in this c a t e g o r y i n c l u d e d a c h i l d ' s T h e gesture most often i n d i c a t e d attention to the object,
echoing o f utterances d i r e c t e d to other children. T h e r e but examples i n d i c a t i n g action p e r f o r m e d on an object or
were few examples o f this t y p e o f e c h o l a l i a found in this location o f an o b j e c t w e r e also found for some of the
study (less than .5% o f all echoes). O n e other interesting children.
characteristic of this category was that the p e r c e n t a g e o f
non-focused echolalia was a reflection o f the child's lin- Rehearsal
guistic abilities. That is, non-focused echolalia d e c r e a s e d "1. Evidence of attention to person or object as deter-
as a function of a child's linguistic abilities. This cate- mined by gaze or body orientation.
gory is r e m i n i s c e n t o f t h e a u t o m a t i c i n d i s c r i m i n a t e *2. Some evidence of comprehension as indicated by
echolalia of the visually i m p a i r e d girl in F a y and Cole- demonstrative gesture, action on object, or appropri-
ate verbal response.
man's (1977) study. *3. Verbal or nonverbal response is initiated subsequent
to the echoic utterance and after a short delay.
Turn-taking 4. Echoic utterance is most often not directed to inter-
*1. Evidence of attention to person or object as deter- locutor as evidenced by paralanguage (low volume,
mined by gaze or body orientation. whisper) and/or lack of gaze to interlocutor.
"2. No significant behavioral change indicating evidence
of comprehension. E c h o i c utterances o f this category a p p e a r e d to serve a
3. Echoic utterance is most often directed to inter- cognitive rather than c o m m u n i c a t i v e function. This cate-
locutor as determined by paralanguage, gaze be-
havior, or gaze check subsequent to the utterance. gory is non-interactive in that the echo is not d i r e c t e d to
4. Echoic utterance is usually rigidly congruent to the the interlocutor and was o ~ e n w h i s p e r e d or of low vol-
model utterance, with some exceptions. ume. T h e notion of " t h i n k i n g out l o u d " is often m e n -
t i o n e d in the literature on autistic language, and these
Turn-taking echoes a p p e a r e d to function as a d e v i c e echoes may w e l l b e an e x a m p l e o f that p h e n o m e n o n .
for social closure (Shapiro & Lucy, 1978) or for mainte- T h e child s e l e c t i v e l y r e p e a t e d words (usually of high in-
nance of social interaction (Fay, 1973). T h e child s e e m e d formation value) or e c h o e d i n t a c t utterances as an aid to
to be p l a y i n g out his role in d y a d i c interaction through further p r o c e s s i n g o f the utterance. A s u b s e q u e n t re-
repetition. Although the child d i s p l a y e d no e v i d e n c e of sponse, e i t h e r v e r b a l or n o n v e r b a l , i n d i c a t e d that the
comprehension, the echoic utterance was u s u a l l y very echoic utterance was a processing aid and that e c h o i n g
interactive. O n e r e l a t i v e l y f r e q u e n t o c c u r r e n c e was a was a t y p e of rehearsal strategy. This strategy is analo-
gaze check, in w h i c h a child l o o k e d at the interlocutor gous to what normal speakers often do w h e n a t t e m p t i n g
apparently e x p e c t i n g some a c k n o w l e d g e m e n t . An exam- to learn a foreign language or w h e n there is some dis-
ple from the tapes s h o w e d that Robbie, the oldest child traction in the e n v i r o n m e n t , that is, r e p e a t the utterance
in the study, r e a c t e d with extreme sensitivity to the in- to aid in c o m p r e h e n s i o n . On the tapes, it was not un-
terlocutor's r e s p o n s e to his turn-taking echoes. R o b b i e c o m m o n to find m a n y e x a m p l e s of the c h i l d r e n e c h o i n g
often b e c a m e u p s e t if the r e s p o n s e was not a c c e p t e d utterances in a low voice and s u b s e q u e n t l y " s n a p p i n g
through indications of verbal or n o n v e r b a l approval. Ap-
in" with a directed, interactive response. Apparently, the
parently, for some children, turn-taking echoes m a y de- echo a i d e d the c h i l d in focusing on the verbal message.
velop as part of an interactional routine that results from
intervention t e c h n i q u e s stressing rote verbal imitation.
F o r other c h i l d r e n , it m a y r e f l e c t a n a t u r a l r e s p o n s e Self-regulatory
strategy to c o m m u n i c a t i v e pressures. *1. Evidence of attention to person or object as deter-
mined by gaze or body orientation.
*2. Some evidence of comprehension as indicated by in-
Declarative itiation ofa motoric response involving the child's ac-
*1. Evidence of attention to person or objeet as deter- tion on an object or self.
mined by gaze or body orientation. *3. Initiation of response oecurs during the production of
*2. Demonstrative gesture (touching, pointing, showing) the echoic utterance.
to object, body part, etc., displaying some evidence of 4. Echoic utterance is most often not directed to inter-
comprehension of the model utterance. Gesture oc- locutor as evidenced by paralanguage (low volume,
curs prior to or during echoic utterance. whisper) and/or lack of gaze to interlocutor.
3. Echoic utterance is most often directed to inter- 5. Utterance is usually echoed in an intact form, al-
locutor as determined by paralanguage, gaze be- though deletions, substitutions, and/or additions to
havior or gaze check subsequent to the utterance. the utterance may infrequently occur.

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


PRIZANT, DUCHAN: Immediate Echolalia 247

Self-regulatory echoes are similar to r e h e a r s a l echoes 5. Child's response usually consists of echoic segment
in that t h e y a p p e a r e d to serve a cognitive rather than with added elements, although some rigidly con-
gruent echoic utterances also serve the request func-
c o m m u n i c a t i v e function. Therefore, most sell-regulatory tion.
echoes w e r e not clearly d i r e c t e d to the interlocutor. T h e
major distinction between the rehearsal and self- In general, the r e q u e s t category p r o b a b l y exemplifies
regulatory categories was that r e h e a r s a l echoes a p p e a r e d the most intentional and least automatic r e s p o n s e cate-
to l e a d to a response, w h i l e self-regulatory echoes ap- gory. T h e c h i l d m o s t o f t e n u s e d s o m e o f t h e i n t e r -
p e a r e d to h e l p a c h i l d d i r e c t his own b e h a v i o r d u r i n g the locutor's words, a n d then a d d e d e l e m e n t s to indicate a
p r o d u c t i o n o f the utterance. This p h e n o m e n o n s e e m s desire to obtain an o b j e c t or perform an action. T h e r e -
similar to "self-talk" in y o u n g n o r m a l - s p e a k i n g children, q u e s t funetion was most often s e r v e d b y echolalie struc-
in w h i c h t h e y may v e r b a l l y d i r e c t their own behavior. ture that w e r e affected b y some changes (usually addi-
C o n s i d e r also the n o r m a l - s p e a k i n g a d u l t w h o is attempt- tions) a n d may therefore be the most extreme examples
ing to m a s t e r a difficult motoric task. F o r e x a m p l e , w h e n of intentional mitigation.
l e a r n i n g a n e w dance step, adults may v e r b a l i z e their in- F o r the autistic child, the first c o m m u n i c a t i v e and
t e n d e d actions w h i l e p e r f o r m i n g them, e.g., Left foot functional s p o n t a n e o u s speech, and/or d e l a y e d echolalia,
forward, right foot back, etc. Some of the c h i l d r e n u s e d a p p e a r s to serve a r e q u e s t fimetion. One w o u l d e x p e c t a
m u l t i p l e r e p e t i t i o n s o f an utterance if a task c o u l d n ' t b e c h i l d ' s motivation to be relatively strong w h e n he de-
c o m p l e t e d promptly. F o r example, w h i l e searching for sires certain n e e d s met. Some v e r b a l aut'istie c h i l d r e n
h i d d e n objects, a c h i l d r e p e a t e d , Go find the dog until w h o are r e l a t i v e l y n o n i n t e r a e t i v e are m o s t often ob-
he was able to c o m p l e t e the task. (See Luria, 1959, 1966 s e r v e d initiating interaction d u r i n g m e a l t i m e or during
for discussions of the role of s p e e c h in the r e g u l a t i o n of favorite activities, situations w h i c h p r o v i d e a strong in-
behavior.) h e r e n t motivation to c o m m u n i c a t e .
R e q u e s t e c h o e s a p p e a r e d r e l a t e d to Y e s - A n s w e r
echoes in t h a t the c h i l d ' s goal was acquisition o f a de-
Yes-Answer
"1. Evidence of attention to person or object as deter- sired object, h e l p from the interlocutor, etc. T h e major
mined by gaze or body orientation. differentiation b e t w e e n t h e s e two categories was that
*2. Verbal or nonverbal evidence of affirmation prior to with the q u e s t i o n in the Yes-Answer echoes (e.g., Do
or subsequent to the echoic response (e.g., accept- you want ?), the c h i l d was p r o v i d e d the
ance of object, reaching for object, initiation of ap- language, and all he n e e d e d to do was indicate affirma-
propriate action.)
*3. Echoic utterance is directed to interlocutor as evi- tion b y r e p e a t i n g the utterance. H o w e v e r , with r e q u e s t
denced by paralanguage or gaze. echoes, in w h i c h the r e q u e s t was often in response to an
4. Utterance is usually echoed in an intact form with oc- o p e n - e n d e d question, a c h i l d most often n e e d e d to a d d
casional change in intonation. words to an e c h o e d s e g m e n t to specify his desires (e.g.,
5. The child often displays some nonverbal indication
Q u e s t i o n : What do you want? A n s w e r : What do you
of expecting a further response from the interlocutor
(e.g., gaze hold, child grasping hand or arm of inter- want a pringle.).
locutor, open-handed reach).

T h e Yes-Answer category has b e e n referred to as "af- CONCLUSIONS


firmation by r e p e t i t i o n " (Kanner, 1946) a n d is the only
specific function a t t r i b u t e d to i m m e d i a t e e e h o l a l i a in the T h e results of this study suggest some directions fbr
autism literature. H o w e v e r , to avoid r e l y i n g on infer- r e s e a r c h on the language b e h a v i o r of autistic children.
ence, an echoic r e s p o n s e to a "Yes-No" q u e s t i o n was not F u t u r e r e s e a r c h s h o u l d a t t e m p t to e x a m i n e d e v i a n t
sufficient for inclusion in this category. T h e child had to characteristics of autistic language within a ffmnework of
d i s p l a y some n o n v e r b a l i n d i c a t i o n o f affirmation inde- how such eharaeteristies may function for the c h i l d r e n
p e n d e n t o f the v e r b a l r e s p o n s e . T h e r e f o r e , r e s p o n s e s a n d how t h e y p o s s i b l y fit into a theory of language ac-
c o n s i d e r e d " a f f i r m a t i o n b y r e p e t i t i o n " b y o t h e r re- q u i s i t i o n for autistie children. Most p r e v i o u s r e s e a r c h
searchers may not have fit the Yes-Answer category un- has e x a m i n e d characteristics such as i m m e d i a t e and de-
less e v i d e n c e o f affirmation was d o c u m e n t e d . l a y e d e e h o l a l i a as isolated p h e n o m e n a , rather than as in-
tegral parts of the d e v e l o p i n g finguistic and cognitive
system of autistic children.
Request In addition, the results of this study raise m a n y ques-
*1. Evidence of attention to person or object as deter- tions c o n c e r n i n g the w i d e variety o f b e h a v i o r modifica-
mined by gaze or body orientation. tion programs that advocate extinction or r e p l a c e m e n t of
*2. Verbal or nonverbal evidence of the child's desire to
obtain an object or perform an aetion prior to, during, i m m e d i a t e e c h o l a l i a with rotely t r a i n e d surface struc-
or subsequent to the echoic utterance (open-handed tures. Specifically, indiscriminate extinction of" all fbrms
reach, carrying out action when givenpermission). of i m m e d i a t e e c h o l a l i a is i l l - a d v i s e d b e c a u s e of the time-
*3. Echoic utterance is directed to interlocutor as evi- tions that e c h o l a l i a may serve fbr autistic c h i l d r e n . It
denced by paralanguage or gaze.
4. The child displays some nonverbal and/or verbal in- may b e fi-uitful to view i m m e d i a t e echolalia fi'onl a time-
dication of expecting a further response from the in- tional p e r s p e c t i v e and to a t t e m p t to d i s c o v e r patterns of
terlocutor. usage tbr i n d i v i d u a l children.

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


248 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 241-249 August 1981

C o n c e i v a b l y , a c h i l d w h o has b e e n p r e d o m i n a t e l y FAY, W. H., & COLEMAN, R. O. A human sound transducer/


echolalic for m a n y years with little e v i d e n c e o f com- reproducer: Temporal capabilities of a profoundly echolalic
p r e h e n s i o n or change in his echolalic b e h a v i o r should child. Brain and Language, 1977, 4, 396-402.
HALLIDAY,M. A. K. Learning how to mean. In E. Lenneberg &
be treated differently from a y o u n g e r child who displays E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Foundations of language development
evidence of comprehension and progressive change. (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press, 1975.
Considering his l i m i t e d linguistic abilities, the ~brmer KANNER, L. Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early in-
child may n e e d to b e taught rote verbal routines that fantile autism. American Journal of Psychiatary, 1946, 103,
242-246.
w o u l d be useful for d a i l y functioning. Such r o u t i n e s LOVAAS, O. I. The autistic child: Language development
should be taught in a natural context so that the child through behavior modification. New York: Halstead Press,
may realize the full i m p a c t of the use of language and 1977.
thus increase g e n e r a l i z a t i o n o f a c q u i r e d structures. LOVAAS, O. I., SCHREIBMAN, L., & KOEGEL, R. L. A behavior
modification approach to the treatment of autistic children.
The latter child, however, n e e d s to accept and exploit
Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1974, 4,
immediate eeholalia and learn to relate the repetitions to 111-129.
aspects o f the e n v i r o n m e n t and c o m m u n i c a t i v e interac- LURIA, A. R. The directive fnnction of speech in development
tions. T h r o u g h modification and expansion of a child's and dissolution. Part 1: Development of the directive function
echolalic utterances in a natural context, a w e l l - t r a i n e d of speech in early childhood, Word, 1959, 15, 341-352.
LURIA, A. R. The development of the regulatory, role of speech.
clinician can h e l p an autistic child d e v e l o p more effec- In J. Harper, C. Anderson, C. Christenson, & S. Hunka (Eds.),
tive c o m m u n i c a t i o n with p e o p l e and the environment. The cognitive processes. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-
Success in c o m m u n i c a t i o n would, therefore, m o t i v a t e Hall, 1966.
the autistic c h i l d to w a n t to learn language, initiate in- PHILIPS, G., & DYER, C. Late onset echolalia in autism and al-
lied disorders. British Journal of Disorders of Communica-
teraction with others, and b e c o m e an active m e m b e r of
tion, 1977, 12, 47-59.
the world a r o u n d him. PRIZANT, B. M. Verbal behavior of autistic children: A review
of the literature and a guide for parents. Unpublished mas-
ter's thesis, State University of New York at Buffalo, 1975.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS RUTTER, M. Concepts of autism: A review of research. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1968, 9, 1-25.
To Rita Joyce, Bart Ferraro, the children and their parents for RUTTER, M. The development of infantile autism. Psychological
all of their help and support. This research was supported in Medicine, 1974, 9, 147-163.
full by BEH grant #G00-77-00017. Requests for reprints should SCHREIBMAN, L., & CARR, E. Elimination of echolalie respond-
be directed to Barry M. Prizant, Ph.D., Department of Speech ing to questions through the training of a generalized verbal
Pathology and Audiology, Southern Illinois University, Car- response. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 1978, 11,
bondale, IL. 62901. 453-464.
SCHULER, A. L. Echolalia: Issues and clinical implications.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1979, 4, 411-434.
REFERENCES SHAPIRO, T. The quest for a linguistic model to study the
speech of autistic children. Journal of American Academy of
Psychiatry, 1977, 16, 608-619.
BATES, E. Language and context: The acquisition of prag- SHAPIRO, T., & LUCY, P. Echoing in autistic children: A
matics. New York: Academic Press, 1976. chronometric study of semantic processing. Journal of Child
BBUNER, J. S. The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1978, 19, 373-378.
Language, 1975, 2, 1-19. SHAPIRO, T., ROBERTS, A., & FISH, B. Imitation and echoing in
BUIUM, N., • STEUCHER, H. On some language parameters of young schizophrenic children. Journal of American Academy
autistic echolalia. Language and Speech, 1974, 17, 353-357. of Child Psychiatry, 1970, 9, 548-565.
COLEMAN, S., & STEDMAN,J. Use of a peer model in language VOELTZ, L. M. Syntactic rule mediation and echolalia in autistic
training in an echolalic child. Journal of Behavioral children. Unpublished manuscript, University of Hawaii,
Therapeutic Experiments in Psychiatry, 1974, 5, 275-279. 1977.
DORE, J. Holophrases, speech acts, and language universals. WING, L. Perceptual and language development in autistic chil-
Journal of Child Language, 1975, 2, 21-40. dren: A comparative study. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Infantile au-
FAY, W. H. Mitigated echolalia of children. Journal of Speech tism: Concepts, characteristics, and treatment. Edinburgh:
and Hearing Research, 1967, 10, 305-310. Churchill Livingstone, 1971.
FAY, W. H. On the basis of autistic echolalia. Journal of Com-
municative Disorders, 1969, 2, 38-47.
FAY, W. H. On the echolalia of the blind and of the autistic
child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1973, 38, Received May 9, 1979
478-489. Accepted April 9, 1980 '

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


PRIZANT,DUCHAN: Immediate Echolalia 249

APPENDIX

A. Example of the Transcription of an Echoic Utterance


Barry (clinician) Brian (child) Nonverbal
(9 --
® , ® Barry's left index finger pointing to Bert puppet and
pulled back
What does Barry do Bert do (~)
Brian's right hand extends to Bert puppet
*0 *, o . ® Brian's right hand pushes Bert puppet off block
Gaze ® Brian's right hand picks up Bert puppet
® .®
Gaze
**- down *O - Object
+p - Person
0 P +p ** - indicates pause

Situation
Brian and Barry sitting at table
Book open on table
Acting out scenes in book with blocks and finger
puppets
Bert puppet on top of block
The notation above the dialogue (circled numbers) represents significant gestures and bodily movement, and the notation below the
dialogue represents the child's direction and shifts in gaze behavior in temporal relation to the utterance.

B. Features of the Functional Categories


Timing of Evidence
Evidence Echo directed Degree Echo Re: of Expectation
Echo of to of Behavioral Compre- of Response
Category Attention Person Change Change hension From Adult Comments
Non-focused No* No* Minimal N.A.** No* No
Turn-Taking Yes* Yes Minimal N.A. No* No
Declarative Yes* Yes Variable During or Yes* Checking * Demonstrative
subsequent gaze gesture indi-
to possible cating object,
location
Self- Yes* No, with Variable During* Yes* No
regulatory exceptions
Rehearsal Yes* No, with Selective; Prior to* Yes* No *Delay between
exceptions high echo and verbal
information or nonverbal
segmentals
Yes-Answer Yes* Yes* Minimal N.A. Yes* Yes* *Verbal or non-
verbal evidence
of affirmation
Request Yes* Yes* Variable; N.A. Yes* Yes *Verbal or non-
usually verbal evidence
elements of child's desire
are added to obtain an object
or have action
performed
*Core attributes.
**N.A. - Not applicable.

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by Barry Prizant on 06/04/2014


View publication stats

You might also like