Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Presidential Address

Doing clinical psychology research

What is
interesting
isn’t always useful
KATE GRAY

HAT is clinical psychology

W research? Did you do


a practical class in it during
your undergraduate degree? The chances
are that you didn’t. Is it a core content At the Annual Conference in Bournemouth, GRAHAM
area of the curriculum? Usually not.
The scientist-practitioner model of clinical DAVEY gave his Presidential Address.
psychology research is rarely encountered
at undergraduate level, yet applied in the UK, and asked why training clinical provision of clinical services
psychology generally and clinical psychologists to doctoral standards had not and research means that clinical
psychology specifically represent some of prevented a decline in clinical research. psychologists often do research as an
the most common reasons that applicants There are some obvious reasons for this: afterthought, without the necessary skills
give for wanting to take a psychology ● Clinical psychology research requires development required to do it properly.
degree. The intense competition for a wide range of research skills (many
postgraduate clinical training places in of which are not taught in a relevant It is this last issue that I would like to
many countries, including the UK and the context at undergraduate level) and spend the rest of this article pursuing.
US, attests to the continued enthusiasm for a clear understanding of theory and I am not clinically trained, but came
this branch of psychology after graduation. explanation. to psychopathology research as an
Arguably, clinical psychology has ● Clinical populations represent only experimental psychologist with an interest
a central importance as both a profession a small proportion of the population, in understanding how cognitive and
and a discipline. In many countries it is and access to such populations is often psychological processes contribute to the
a valued profession with a high profile, difficult to obtain. development and maintenance of anxiety-
and in any discipline understanding how ● Many forms of research are either based disorders. To this extent, I may well
processes that contribute to normal difficult to conduct with clinical be seen by clinicians as an outsider
functioning can also lead to dysfunction populations (because of the severity preaching heathen philosophy. But from
represents an important and necessary part of the symptoms observed in the standpoint that I have come from,
of the body of knowledge. psychopathology) or raise important clinical psychology research in the last
In the June 2002 issue of The ethical issues (e.g. conducting 10 years or so has developed some sloppy
Psychologist, Thomas et al. wrote an experimental manipulations that methodological habits, is often unclear
article entitled ‘Clinical psychology under might precipitate clinical symptoms about what constitutes an explanation of
threat’. It outlined the difficulties of or interfere with ongoing treatment). a clinical phenomenon, and, in some areas,
conducting research in clinical psychology ● Having to split their time between the has become obsessed with ‘clinical

412

The Psychologist Vol 16 No 8 August 2003


Presidential Address

experience’ and diagnostic categories categorical symptoms, or may give rise to psychopathology for many years now,
to the detriment of understanding the quite different symptoms under different and who can quite easily see that those
psychological processes involved in circumstances. For example, research that cognitive processes that underlie ‘normal’
psychopathology. The following sections we have recently conducted suggests that functioning activities (subclinical worrying
describe some of these issues, and are in some of those disorders characterised by and checking) may subsequently come to
illustrated in the context of some of the perseverative activities (e.g. pathological maintain extreme forms of these activities
psychopathology research carried out by worrying, compulsive checking, (e.g. pathological worrying in GAD and
the Clinical Experimental Psychopathology perseverative rumination), perseveration compulsive checking in obsessive
Research Group at the University of Sussex can at least in part be explained across all compulsive disorder).
over the past 10 years. these symptom types by the contribution of
a single psychological mechanism (Davey Beware constructs derived
‘Processes’ not categories et al., in press; Startup & Davey, 2001). from ‘clinical experience’
A significant majority of clinical Understanding these types of perseverative Clinical researchers are very attached
psychology research (and psychiatric disorders will probably require intensive to their ‘clinical experiences’ and often
research) is carried out around diagnostic study of this common mechanism rather attempt to bring these experiences into
categories and on populations with specific than a blinkered focus on the individual clinical research by creating constructs
diagnostically defined symptoms. In 2002 diagnostic categories. This will at least help from them. Such constructs are not
both the British Journal of Clinical us to understand, rather than ignore, the directly underpinned by observable
Psychology and, arguably the most importance of comorbidity, and to develop actions, behaviours or symptoms, but
evidence-based and experimentally treatments that are soundly based on the have a validity through the fact that the
oriented of psychopathology journals, cognitive and psychological mechanisms clinician commonly experiences a set of
Behaviour Research and Therapy, that develop and maintain the critical characteristics within a particular clinical
published around 60 per cent of their features of psychopathology. population. The usual practice is then to
articles on research dedicated to DSM develop a tool or instrument to measure the
diagnostic categories or their direct construct, and subsequently to identify how
analogues. Through their training, the ‘I may well be seen by the construct relates to clinical symptoms –
clinical psychologist’s understanding of clinicians as an outsider all in the belief that this will enable an
psychopathology is based on the diagnosis preaching heathen philosophy’ understanding of the processes that develop
and categorisation of disorders as described and maintain those symptoms.
in the most recent version of the DSM. But Unfortunately, more often than
this may not be a good starting point for Another consequence of researching not, these constructs can detract from
undertaking research that is attempting to by diagnostic category is that there is often attempting to elucidate the cognitive and
understand aetiology and its associated an implicit tendency to view a diagnostic psychological mechanisms that underlie the
psychological mechanisms. In a recent category as representing a ‘disorder’ of disorder, confuse descriptions of symptoms
article Krueger and Piasecki (2002) note: a normal psychological process. with causes and consequences of the
Generalised anxiety disorder (GAD), for symptoms, and generally set off
The typical approach to research on example, is often referred to as a ‘disorder unproductive chains of research.
psychopathology involves defining of worrying’ – as though there is some Even in issues of Behaviour Research
‘pure groups’ of individuals (persons normal, functional worry mechanism that and Therapy since January 2002 the reader
who meet criteria for a specific DSM has broken down and needs mending. Not will find articles espousing the virtues of
disorder, but not for other disorders) or only does this have the effect of the following constructs: ‘not just right
‘impure groups’ of individuals (persons stigmatising clinical patients as experiences’ (Coles et al., 2003), over-
who meet criteria for a specific DSM psychologically dysfunctional and valued ideas (Veale, 2002), intolerance of
disorder, regardless of the other somehow qualitatively ‘different’ to uncertainty (Buhr & Dugas, 2002),
disorders for which they might meet everyone else (e.g. Hayes & Follette, interpersonal sensitivity (Harb et al., 2002),
criteria) and contrasting them with 1992), it also seems to imply that thought–action fusion (Zucker et al., 2002),
‘comparison groups’ (persons who do psychopathologies exist in discrete and inflated responsibility (Barrett &
not meet criteria for psychopathology, categories that have quite different Healy, 2003). All are used in contexts
or who meet criteria for a putatively dynamics to any ‘normal’ processes that where the construct is assumed to give
distinct form of psychopathology). (p.489) they might resemble. Recent research using a better insight into the psychological
psychometric technologies such as item characteristics experienced in certain
There are at least two immediate response modelling is beginning to indicate disorders (e.g. not just right experiences)
difficulties with this typical approach. that psychopathology can be or have some explanatory purpose (e.g.
First, comorbidity among DSM-defined conceptualised as continuous and thought–action fusion), or both (e.g.
psychopathology is the rule and not the dimensional, ranging from relatively intolerance of uncertainty).
exception (Kessler et al., 1994). This not ‘normal’ functional characteristics at It is tempting to say that clinical
only challenges DSM’s categorical one end to the more severe pathological psychology researchers avidly develop
conception of psychopathology, but symptoms at the other (e.g. Cooke & such constructs because they are consumed
suggests that a full understanding of Michie, 1997; Santor & Coyne, 2001). by their clinical experiences and have little
psychopathology will be dependent These findings are perhaps not so time (or training) for maintaining
on describing underlying aetiological surprising to those of us who have been a knowledge and understanding of how
processes that either give rise to cross- doing analogue research on other relevant areas of pure psychological

413

August 2003 The Psychologist Vol 16 No 8


Presidential Address

research might contribute to understanding FIGURE 1 The interaction convert negative mood into inflated
psychopathology (although recent research between variables thought to be responsibility, and cause inflated
on the role of memory processes in PTSD involved in obsessive compulsive responsibility and negative mood to trigger
is a welcome exception to this, e.g. disorder compulsive actions and rituals? Arguably,
Hellawell & Brewin, 2002). If so, not only these are the most interesting aspects of the
does the development of constructs based psychopathology! The arrows represent the
on clinical experience fail to address NEGATIVE mechanisms by which variables interact,
relevant issues about underlying MOOD and a case can be made that clinical
psychological and cognitive processes, (distress, psychology researchers have often
but the construct may also even actively anxiety, neglected to address how these
obscure the detection and understanding of depression) mechanisms work, and have neglected to
these processes. look elsewhere in the psychology literature
The example of ‘not just right for what might be suitable mechanisms to
experiences’ (NJREs) is an instructive one. explain how their variables interact (see
Individuals with OCD frequently report Startup & Davey, 2001).
INFLATED
uncomfortable sensations of things not Furthermore, when arrows are included
RESPONSIBILITY
being quite right, and they will continue in schematic models they are often
(for preventing
with their ritualistic compulsions until this seductive enough to convince that they are
harm to self or
feeling has gone. Coles et al. (2003) report unidirectional processes with linear effects
others)
that preliminary investigation of this (rather than representative of what might
construct reveals that NJREs are correlated be quite complex mechanisms); this can
with OCD features but not with features of lead to other assumptions about the model
other domains of psychopathology, that that may not be true. For example, one of
NJREs may represent a specific form of the challenges for psychopathology
COMPULSIVE
perfectionism, and that compulsions may research is to explain the severity of the
ACTIONS,
represent attempts to relieve the anxiety symptoms that the sufferer experiences,
RITUALS
caused by NJREs. Quite possibly. But in and clinical psychology researchers have
(checking, etc.)
creating constructs such as this, researchers a tendency to do this by inventing ‘vicious
need to ask a series of very important cycles’ within their models.
questions. Does the construct merely An early example of this is David
redescribe the symptomatology of OCD? psychopathology. They represent Clark’s (1986) famous catastrophic
Is the construct (and its experience by the a convenient way of describing the misinterpretation model of panic disorder
client) simply a trivial, epiphenomenal relationships between the variables that (see Figure 2), which postulates that
consequence of the mechanisms underlying have been identified, or, quite regularly, a triggering stimulus gives rise to anxious
OCD? And does the construct help or created from clinical experience. But often apprehension, which in turn causes
hinder us in understanding how established such models leave the really interesting and ambiguous bodily sensations, which are
psychological processes and mechanisms informative features of the interpreted catastrophically and leads to
contribute to the psychopathology? psychopathology unexplained. further anxious apprehension, and so on.
Because they are derived mainly from Figure 1 represents an illustrative This sequence is repeated until a panic
clinical observation of patients with a schematic model of how some of the attack occurs. (But note that the outcome,
DSM-diagnosed disorder, such constructs variables thought to be importantly a ‘panic attack’, doesn’t constitute part of
also run the risk of becoming reified into involved in OCD interact to cause the model!) Arrows that viciously recycle
characteristics of the DSM category and symptoms (there are several similar models can be found in a substantial number of
dispositional features of its sufferers, when in the literature – e.g. Rachman, 2002; schematic psychopathology models, and
such constructs provide little explanation Salkovskis, 1999 – this example draws often attempt to explain symptom severity
of the symptoms or understanding of the some common features from these separate (examples include OCD, social phobia and
mechanisms underlying the models). On the face of it, such models depression, to mention just a few). However,
psychopathology. More on this next. look compelling and instructive, and there such accounts assume that severity can
is no lack of evidence suggesting that the only be achieved by repeatedly recycling
Explain how the arrow works arrowed links between boxes exist in some the same arrows around a system that
Deriving constructs from clinical form or other. For example, there is builds severity in a simple cumulative way.
experience and then attempting to evidence from both survey studies (e.g. Foa
understand how these constructs relate to et al., 2002) and studies that have involved Let’s do some correlations
symptoms can have implications for how experimental manipulation (Ladouceur et Because of its very nature, clinical
the psychopathology is understood and al., 1995; Lopatka & Rachman, 1995) that psychology lends itself to research that
explained. For example, box and arrow there is a link between inflated emphasises the relationships between
schematic models are a popular feature of responsibility and compulsive checking. symptoms, psychopathology constructs,
psychological explanation generally, and However, such models tell us little, if and psychological processes. Correlational
clinical psychology research is no anything, about the psychological or studies are a means of getting a handle on
exception. But we have to remember that cognitive mechanisms involved in OCD. whether predicted relationships between
in very many cases these models do not In particular, how do the arrows in Figure 1 psychopathology measures and
represent the mechanisms underlying do what they do? How do they respectively psychological processes exist. They are

414

The Psychologist Vol 16 No 8 August 2003


Presidential Address

almost a methodology of choice for many


clinical researchers for whom experimental
methodologies are too invasive or raise
complicated ethical issues when studying
a clinical population. This may be very
well if correlational studies are planned and
executed with clear forethought about how
the results can be interpreted – but very
often they are not.
A good example of how poor
planning can cloud understanding rather
than enhance it comes from the study of
the role of the disgust emotion in anxious
psychopathology. For the past 10 years,
research on the role of disgust has
burgeoned to the point where in 2002
there were 28 papers published in clinically
related research journals with ‘disgust’ in
the title – only six of these involved an
experimental manipulation. Recent review
articles have proclaimed disgust as the
KATE GRAY

‘forgotten emotion of psychiatry [sic]’


(Phillips et al., 1998), and studies have
linked the disgust emotion to animal
The arrows in schematic models may represent quite complex mechanisms
phobias, OCD, blood-inoculation-injury
phobia, and eating disorders. To be sure,
disgust is certainly experienced in these predicted disgust-relevant psychopathologies, some have begun to
disorders, but 10 years of research has so psychopathologies are compared with reveal significant relationships between
far failed to provide real convincing suspected disgust-irrelevant disgust and those psychopatholologies that
evidence that disgust has any precipitating psychopathologies (e.g. de Jong et al., would not have been predicted at the outset
or causal function. 1997; Sawchuk et al., 2000). Because (e.g. between disgust and situational
We all know that correlation does not anxiety and disgust are closely related phobias; Muris et al., 1999). Furthermore,
imply causation, but the issue here is that emotions, studies that do not include many studies have not taken any measure
the design of many disgust correlational disgust-irrelevant control comparisons of trait or state anxiety. Thus they will
studies blurs rather than sharpens our do not provide differential predictions that inevitably be unable to determine whether
understanding of the would rule out the implication of anxiety any relationship between disgust and
disgust–psychopathology relationship. as a mediating variable – thus rendering psychopathology measures is independent
First, many studies have failed to use the findings relatively trivial. When studies of levels of anxiety (e.g. Arrindell et al.,
properly balanced designs in which have included disgust-irrelevant 1999). It is quite clearly possible that
elevated disgust levels are a consequence
of the effects of anxiety raising levels of
FIGURE 2 The catastrophic misinterpretation of panic disorder (adapted all negative emotions.
from Clarke, 1986) in which a trigger stimulus sets off a cycle of events that This lack of foresight in planning such
repeats until a panic attack occurs studies – but with a willingness to publish
them anyway – has created a literature that
TRIGGER STIMULUS is difficult to unravel and that encourages
(internal or external) the belief that disgust is involved in the
aetiology of a range of anxious
psychopathologies, when there is
insufficient evidence to come to this
conclusion. It is also indicative that clinical
PERCEIVED THREAT psychology research has spent over 10
years nurturing this literature without
generating a single explanatory model
INTERPRETATION or describing a single mechanism that
OF SENSATIONS AS APPREHENSION implicates disgust in the aetiology of
CATASTROPHIC anxious psychopathology.

Conclusions
BODY I suspect many readers think I may
SENSATIONS have been too harsh – and, OK, relatively
selective in the examples and areas of

415

August 2003 The Psychologist Vol 16 No 8


Presidential Address

psychopathology that I have dipped into. professional development;


However, there are many other examples of ● have had the opportunity to learn
naive and ill-thought-through contributions directly about clinical psychology
to research in clinical psychology and research methods in their undergraduate
psychopathology that space does not careers; and
permit me to mention. But understanding ● have a good knowledge of ‘pure’
psychopathology is an honourable goal that psychology research that is relevant to
will contribute significantly to alleviating their clinical research specialities.
suffering in the world. As such, it deserves
to be done properly, with clinicians at all If psychologists do not regularly,
levels making informed and valuable efficiently and effectively become involved
contributions. In the UK at least, this is in psychopathology research, then other
not yet as it should be (see Thomas et al., disciplines – notably psychiatry – are ready
2002). In particular, clinical psychologists and waiting to hijack this subject matter.
not only need to be involved more
regularly in research, they need to ■ Graham C.L. Davey is Professor of
● have research skills as a significant Psychology at the University of Sussex.
component of their continuing E-mail: grahamda@cogs.susx.ac.uk.

References
Arrindell,W.A., Mulkens, S., Kok, J. & Anxiety Disorders, 16, 443–453. anxiety disorders symptoms in
Vollenbroek, J. (1999). Disgust Harb, G.C., Heimberg, R.G., Fresco, normal children. Behaviour
sensitivity and the sex difference D.M., Schneier, F.R. & Liebowitz, Research and Therapy, 37,
in fears to common indigenous M.R. (2002).The psychometric 953–961.
animals. Behaviour Research and properties of the Interpersonal Phillips, M.L., Senior, C., Fahy,T. &
Therapy, 37, 273–280. Sensitivity Measure in social David,A.S. (1998). Disgust – The
Barrett, P.M. & Healy, L.J. (2003).An anxiety disorder. Behaviour forgotten emotion of psychiatry.
examination of the cognitive Research and Therapy, 961–979. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173,
processes involved in childhood Hayes, S.C. & Follette,W.C. (1992). 373–375.
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Can functional analysis provide a
Rachman, S. (2002).A cognitive theory
Behaviour Research and Therapy, substitute for syndromal
of compulsive checking. Behaviour
41, 285–299. classification? Behavioural
Research and Therapy, 40,
Buhr, K. & Dugas, M.J. (2002).The Assessment, 9, 349–360.
625–639.
intolerance of uncertainty scale. Hellawell, S.J. & Brewin, C.R. (2002).A
Salkovskis, P.M. (1999). Understanding
Behaviour Research and Therapy, comparison of flashbacks and
40, 931–945. ordinary autobiographical and treating obsessive-compulsive
Clark, D.M. (1986).A cognitive memories of trauma. Behaviour disorder. Behaviour Research and
approach to panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, Therapy, 37, 529–552.
Research and Therapy, 24, 1143–1156. Santor D.A. & Coyne J.C. (2001).
461–470. Kessler, R.C., McGonagle, K.A., Zhao, Examining symptom expression
Coles, M.E., Frost, R.O., Heimberg, S., Nelson, C.B., Hughes, M., as a function of symptom
R.G. & Rheaume, J. (2003).‘Not Eshleman S. et al. (1994). Lifetime severity. Psychological Assessment,
just right experiences’. Behaviour and 12-month prevalence of 13, 127–139.
Research and Therapy, 41, DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in Sawchuk, C.N., Lohr, J.M.,Tolin, D.F,
681–700. the United States. Archives of Lee,T.C. & Kleinknecht, R.A.
Cooke, D.J. & Michie, C. (1997).An General Psychiatry, 51, 8–19. (2000). Disgust sensitivity and
item response theory analysis of Krueger, R.F. & Piasecki,T.M. (2002). contamination fears in spider and
the Hare Psychopathy Checklist- Toward a dimensional and blood-injection-injury phobias.
Revised. Psychological Assessment, psychometrically-informed Behaviour Research and Therapy,
9, 3–14. approach to conceptualizing 38, 753–762.
Davey, G.C.L., Startup, H.M., Zara,A., psychopathology. Behaviour Startup, H.M. & Davey, G.C.L. (2001).
MacDonald, B.C. & Field,A.P. (in Research and Therapy, 40, Mood-as-input and catastrophic
press).The perseveration of 485–499. worrying. Journal of Abnormal
checking thoughts and mood-as- Ladouceur, R., Rheaume, J., Freeston,
Psychology, 110, 83–96.
input hypothesis. Journal of M.H.,Aublet, F., Jean, K., Lachance,
Thomas, G.V.,Turpin, G. & Meyer, C.
Behavior Therapy and Experimental S. et al. (1995). Experimental
(2002). Clinical research under
Psychiatry. manipulation of responsibility.
threat. The Psychologist, 15,
de Jong, P.J.,Andrea, H. & Muris, P. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
286–289.
(1997). Spider phobia in children: 33, 937–946.
Disgust and fear before and after Lopatka, C. & Rachman, S. (1995). Veale, D. (2002). Over-valued ideas:A
treatment. Behaviour Research and Perceived responsibility and conceptual analysis. Behaviour
Therapy, 35, 559–562. compulsive checking:An Research and Therapy, 40,
Foa, E.B., Sacks, M.B.,Tolin, D.F., experimental analysis. Behaviour 383–400.
Przeworksi,A. & Amir, N. (2002). Research and Therapy, 33, Zucker, B.G., Craske, M.G., Barrios,V.
Inflated perception of 673–684. & Holguin, M. (2002).Thought
responsibility for harm in OCD Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Schmidt, H. action fusion: Can it be
patients with and without & Tierny, S. (1999). Disgust corrected? Behaviour Research and
checking compulsions. Journal of sensitivity, trait anxiety and Therapy, 40, 653–664.

416

The Psychologist Vol 16 No 8 August 2003


417

August 2003 The Psychologist Vol 16 No 8

You might also like