Adult Sleep Duration Methods 8

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 1

NF Watson, MS Badr, G Belenky et al.

3.9 Mortality associated with sleep duration. Studies in the cardiovascu-


This literature includes numerous studies, of which many lar, metabolic, and mental health categories also include both
were included in two independent meta-analyses.38,111 Both laboratory experiments and epidemiological cohort studies.
meta-analyses found increased mortality risk associated with Numerous, large cross-sectional and longitudinal population-
short sleep and long sleep durations. “Short” and “long” sleep based observational studies provide largely concordant find-
were defined differently across studies. Nevertheless, self-re- ings linking short sleep duration to obesity, cardiovascular
ported sleep duration of 7–8 hours was generally associated disease, diabetes, and depression. Meta-analyses further sup-
with the lowest mortality risk, and both short and long sleep port the findings reported in individual studies. The immuno-
duration were associated with increased risk. Given the het- logic health and pain categories are supported by fewer studies,
erogeneity of short and long sleep definitions, the most reliable but these also include both observational and laboratory de-
comparisons examined these broad categories. However, ex- signs. In particular, studies of immune function in relation to
amination of one hour sleep duration categories in large pro- sleep duration examine possible mechanisms at the cellular
spective studies suggests that the most extreme sleep durations level. The literature also includes a large number of studies
are associated with the greater risk, especially in the case of spanning several decades in the general health, cancer, and
long sleep. This U-shaped relationship has been demonstrated mortality categories. Studies in the mortality category include
repeatedly (but not universally) and has been replicated using large samples and demonstrate largely convergent results that
objective sleep measurement with actigraphy. The sleep dura- are supported by multiple meta-analyses.
tion-mortality risk relationship appears relatively stable across A number of important limitations in both epidemiologic
demographic groups, with one exception: Increased mortal- and laboratory-based studies are also evident, as described in
ity risk associated with long sleep may be partially explained previous reviews.113 Epidemiologic studies maximize gener-
by age. alizability at the expense of measurement precision, whereas
The panel’s recommendation statement focuses on adults up experimental studies maximize measurement precision at the
to age 60 years, but many of the deaths in the studies exam- expense of generalizability. However, these limitations are
ining mortality likely took place after subjects were 60 years mitigated by the general agreement in findings between labo-
old, given the longitudinal study designs. One meta-analysis111 ratory-based and epidemiologic studies.
did not find a significant difference in mortality risk by age. Epidemiologic studies pose several specific limitations.
Further, the role of medical conditions that could lead to both First, most of the studies reviewed were cross-sectional, pre-
mortality and either short or long sleep duration is unclear. Al- cluding any statements regarding causation. Second, sleep du-
though many potential confounders can be entered into analy- ration is typically assessed for a limited time frame around the
ses of large data sets, it may be difficult to interpret mortality assessment, whereas most of the health conditions have been
risk after accounting for many of the leading causes of death, developing or present for years prior to assessment. While
a large number of which may also be related to sleep duration. cross-sectional associations may be valid, “predicting” chronic
For example, the only study to assess the relationship between health conditions from concurrent sleep duration presents a
sleep duration and mortality in healthy individuals showed no conceptual challenge. Third, many epidemiologic studies have
association;40 an association was evident only in those who limited ability to explore potential mediators and effect modi-
were in poor health at the start of the study. These findings fiers. Fourth, some studies may have insufficient adjustment
suggest that sleep duration-mortality risk associations may be for confounders. Conversely, excluding too many health con-
driven more by underlying diseases than by sleep per se. Fi- ditions may make interpretation difficult (e.g. If likely causes
nally, one of the reviewed studies112 included a sample so dis- of death are removed from the analysis, it is difficult to study
proportionately large that it may exert undue influence on the mortality).
overall conclusion. However, another meta-analysis38 found Specific methods of sleep assessment also present some
that similar overall effects remained even after excluding this limitations in epidemiologic studies. First, most of these stud-
study from analysis. ies rely on retrospective self-report of habitual sleep duration,
which may be less accurate than averages from daily self-re-
4.0 STRENGTHS AND WEAKNESSES OF THE port (i.e., sleep diary). Self-reported sleep duration can over- or
LITERATURE under-estimate sleep duration measured objectively with actig-
raphy or polysomnography. Second, studies have varied in how
The panel recommendation statement55 was based on a lit- they assess self-reported sleep duration. For instance, differ-
erature characterized by numerous strengths. Taken together, ent studies may ask participants to report “typical,” “average,”
studies on sleep duration include data on millions of par- “weeknight,” or “24-hour” sleep duration. Some epidemiologic
ticipants, studied across several continents, aggregated over studies capture napping, while others do not. Third, while the
several decades. The studies include cross-sectional and lon- survey items addressing sleep duration may have good face va-
gitudinal epidemiologic designs, randomized controlled trials, lidity, most have not been formally validated with psychomet-
meta-analyses, and a range of other designs. Studies in the hu- ric analyses. Fourth, sleep duration is not consistently defined
man performance category may have the strongest evidence across studies. For example, short sleep may be categorized
base, which included experimental laboratory studies, objec- as < 5, 5, 6, or < 8 hours, and reference groups may have sleep
tive measure of sleep and outcomes, evidence of cumulative durations of 7, 8, 7–8, 7–9, or 6–8 hours. Fifth, measures of
effects, and support from population-based cohort studies sleep duration do not capture information about the regularity
documenting “real life” outcomes (e.g., driving performance) of sleep patterns, the timing of sleep, or the quality of sleep.

Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 8, 2015 938

You might also like