Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Research

JAMA Pediatrics | Original Investigation

Screen Time and Developmental Performance Among Children


at 1-3 Years of Age in the Japan Environment and Children’s Study
Midori Yamamoto, PhD; Hidetoshi Mezawa, MD, PhD; Kenichi Sakurai, MD, PhD; Chisato Mori, MD, PhD; for the
Japan Environment and Children’s Study Group

Supplemental content
IMPORTANCE It is unclear whether increased television (TV) and DVD viewing in early
childhood from age 1 year decreases development or whether poor development increases
TV/DVD viewing.

OBJECTIVE To investigate the directional association between TV/DVD screen time and
performance on developmental screeners in children aged 1 to 3 years.

DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS This longitudinal cohort study analyzed data from
57 980 children and mothers from a national birth cohort, the Japan Environment and
Children’s Study. Data were collected in collaboration with 15 regional centers across Japan.
The mothers were recruited between January 2011 and March 2014. Analyses using random
intercept, cross-lagged panel models were performed for children aged 1, 2, and 3 years. Of
100 303 live births, children with missing developmental screening test scores and screen
time data, those with congenital diseases or cerebral palsy, and those diagnosed with an
autism spectrum disorder were excluded. Statistical analyses were conducted from October
2022 to July 2023.

EXPOSURES TV and DVD screen time.

MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Child development at ages 1, 2, and 3 years was assessed via
the mother’s or guardian’s report using the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, third edition.

RESULTS Of 57 980 included children, 29 418 (50.7%) were male, and the mean (SD)
maternal age at delivery was 31.5 (4.9) years. A negative association between screen time and
developmental scores was observed. Increased TV/DVD screen times at age 1 and 2 years
were associated with lower developmental scores at age 2 and 3 years, respectively (2 years:
β = −0.05; 95% CI, −0.06 to −0.04; 3 years: β = −0.08; 95% CI, −0.09 to −0.06). An obverse
association was observed from the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, third edition, score in the
communication domain at age 1 and 2 years to subsequent screen time (2 years: γ = −0.03;
95% CI, −0.04 to −0.02; 3 years: γ = −0.06; 95% CI, −0.07 to −0.04).

CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE In this study, increased TV/DVD screen time from age 1 year
negatively affected later development. To reduce the negative consequences of excessive
media use, researchers and health care professionals should encourage family media
management and recommend social support for parents who tend to rely on the media.

Author Affiliations: Author


affiliations are listed at the end of this
article.
Group Information: A complete list
of the members of the Japan
Environment and Children’s Study
Group appears in Supplement 2.
Corresponding Author: Midori
Yamamoto, PhD, Department of
Sustainable Health Science, Center
for Preventive Medical Sciences,
Chiba University, 1-33 Yayoi-cho,
JAMA Pediatr. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2023.3643 Chiba 263-8522, Japan (midoriy@
Published online September 18, 2023. faculty.chiba-u.jp).

(Reprinted) E1

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 10/16/2023


Research Original Investigation Screen Time and Developmental Performance Among Children at 1-3 Years of Age

R
esearchers have reported a significant increase
in the number of children with neurodevelopmental Key Points
and mental health problems, including language,
Question Is increased television/DVD screen time in infants
l e a r n i ng , b e h a v i o r a l , a n d e m o t i o n a l d i s o r d e r s . 1 , 2 associated with poor performance on developmental screeners?
This increase indicates the need to better understand the
Findings In this cohort study of 57 980 children, increased
social, medical, and environmental factors underlying
television/DVD screen time in children aged 1 and 2 years was
developmental problems in early childhood.
associated with lower developmental scores at 2 and 3 years,
Screens, such as television (TV), are ubiquitous in respectively. Lower development scores were associated with
t h e h o m e s o f yo u ng c h i l d r e n . A l t h o u g h p e d i a t r i c increased screen time in children with maternal psychological
guidelines recommend that screen viewing be avoided for distress.
infants younger than 2 years and limited to 1 hour per
Meaning In this study, increased screen time in early childhood
day between the ages 2 and 5 years, 3-5 many children was negatively associated with poor performance on
fail to adhere to these recommendations.6 Excessive screen developmental screeners, suggesting the need to support parents
time in children younger than 3 years is assoc iated in creating family media plans.
with adverse effects on cognitive, language, motor skills,
and soc ial-behav ioral development. 7 - 1 1 Conversely,
c h i ld re n w it h p o o r s e l f - re g u l at i o n a re m o re l i ke l y
to experience more exposure to screens.12-16 However, there
is limited evidence for a causal relationship in early Methods
childhood regarding whether media exposure leads
to developmental delays or whether children w ith Study Design and Participants
developmental delays are more exposed to the media. After The participants included children from the Japan Environ-
accounting for individual traitlike differences, several stud- ment and Children’s Study (JECS). The study design of JECS
ies have used the random-intercepts, cross-lagged panel has been described previously.29 Briefly, 103 060 pregnan-
model (RI-CLPM) to clarify the bidirectional association cies were registered between January 2011 and March 2014 in
between screen time and development (from age 2 to 3 15 regional centers across Japan. We used the dataset jecs-ta-
years).17,18 20190930. We included 100 303 live births, excluding 1254 mis-
Using the RI-CLPM and Ages and Stages Questionnaires carriages, 382 stillbirths, and 2123 with missing birth data. Next,
(ASQ) as a developmental screener, Madigan et al17 showed children were excluded if they met any of the following crite-
that increased screen time at ages 2 and 3 years led to ria: missing developmental score data (n = 35 914) or missing
delayed achievement of developmental milestones at ages 3 TV/DVD screen time data (n = 794) at age 1, 2, or 3 years; had
and 5 years, respectively. The current study provides congenital diseases, cerebral palsy, or missing data (n = 5376);
more convincing evidence of the role of screen use on and diagnosed with ASD (eg, autism, pervasive developmen-
child development and justifies the RI-CLPM used to tal disorder, and Asperger syndrome) at age 3 years or miss-
analyze the bidirectional association. Although brain devel- ing data (n = 239). A total of 57 980 children were included in
opment during the first 3 years of life is critical, as it sets the analysis (eFigure in Supplement 1). Written informed con-
the foundation for future cognitive and emotional sent was obtained from all the participants. The JECS proto-
functioning,19,20 to our knowledge, no study has found a col was reviewed and approved by the Ministry of the Envi-
bidirectional relationship between screen time and develop- ronment’s Institutional Review Board on Epidemiological
ment in young children, including those younger than 2 Studies and the ethics committees of all participating institu-
years. We replicated and extended the study of Madigan tions. The study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of
et al17 with a larger sample size, at a slightly early age, and Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting
with differential cultural backgrounds. Maternal prenatal or guideline.30
postpartum stress and child’s sex may be associated with
c o g n it ive f u n c t i o n i ng a n d b e h av i o r a l p ro b l e m s i n Measures
children. 21-26 Detecting interactions is crucial for public Developmental Screener
health if maternal stress influences the fetal or children’s Children’s development was assessed by the scores on the Japa-
environment. nese version of the ASQ, third edition (ASQ-3), completed by
We aimed to examine the bidirectional association between the parents or guardians of children aged 1, 2, and 3 years. The
TV/DVD screen time and developmental milestones during early ASQ-3 is widely used for assessing development in commu-
childhood. Additionally, we examined the association in 5 spe- nication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving, and per-
cific domains and moderation or interaction effect by child’s sex sonal-social domains (eTable 1 in Supplement 1).31 Each do-
and maternal distress. Children with psychiatric disorders, such main comprises 6 questions, and their total scores range from
as autism spectrum disorder (ASD), have been reported to have 0 to 60 points per domain, with higher scores indicating bet-
extreme screen time.27,28 Therefore, we analyzed data from the ter development. The Japanese version of ASQ-3 was vali-
Japanese nationwide birth cohort study on 57 980 children aged dated with moderate to high sensitivity (75.3% to 100.0%) and
1, 2, and 3 years with primarily typical development, excluding specificity (66.7% to 100.0%).32 The mean ASQ-3 scores of 5
those diagnosed with ASD. domains or each domain were used as continuous variables.

E2 JAMA Pediatrics Published online September 18, 2023 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 10/16/2023


Screen Time and Developmental Performance Among Children at 1-3 Years of Age Original Investigation Research

Figure. Random-Intercepts Cross-Lagged Panel Model for Associations Between Television (TV)/DVD
Screen Time and Developmental Outcomes (Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Third Edition [ASQ-3], Scores)

Screen time
(stable)
1a
1a 1a

TV/DVD screen αS = 0.15; TV/DVD screen αS = 0.14; TV/DVD screen


time at age 1 y 95% CI, 0.14 to 0.16 time at age 2 y 95% CI, 0.12 to 0.15 time at age 3 y

γ = –0.01; γ = –0.02;
95% CI, –0.03 to 0.01 95% CI, –0.04 to –0.01
σSA = –0.15;
95% CI, –0.18 to –0.12
β = –0.05; β = –0.08;
95% CI, –0.06 to –0.04 95% CI, –0.09 to –0.06
Standardized estimates with 95% CIs
ASQ–3 score αA = 0.09; ASQ–3 score αA = 0; ASQ–3 score
at age 1 y at age 2 y at age 3 y are presented. Solid lines in cross-lag
95% CI, 0.07 to 0.11 95% CI, –0.03 to 0.03
and autoregressive links represent
1a 1a the absolute value of the
1a standardized estimate, which is 0.03
ASQ–3 score or more. Dashed lines represent
(stable)
those less than 0.03.
a
The factor loadings constrained to 1.

Screen Time intercepts to the standard cross-lagged panel model. 34


At ages 1, 2, and 3 years, parents or guardians were asked to re- Owing to the nonnormal distribution of screen time and
port their children’s TV/DVD screen time on by asking, “How ASQ-3 scores, we used an asymptotic distribution-free esti-
many hours do you let your children watch TV or DVD in a typi- mation method.
cal day?” The responses were collected from among 5 catego- Cross-lag association of the screen time with ASQ-3 scores
ries: none, less than 1 hour, 1 hour or more but less than 2 hours, (β) and the association of ASQ-3 scores with screen time (γ) were
2 hours or more but less than 4 hours, and 4 hours or more. estimated in 3 types of RI-CLPM. First, to identify associa-
The categories were converted to corresponding numbers (0 tions between screen time and overall or domain-specific de-
hours, 0.5 hours, 1.5 hours, 3 hours, and 4.5 hours per day, velopment, we performed estimation using the basic
respectively). RI-CLPM (Figure) using mean ASQ-3 scores or scores of each
domain, respectively. Second, to explore the interaction ef-
Predictors fects on the association between screen time and child devel-
In the model to explore the contribution of time-invariant fac- opment, we performed a multiple-group RI-CLPM stratified by
tors on screen time and child development, possible predic- the child’s sex and maternal psychological distress. To evalu-
tors were selected a priori based on the literature,17,27 biologi- ate the interaction effect, we compared a model in which the
cal plausibility, and using a directed acyclic graph. z Scores for lagged regression coefficients were constrained to be equal be-
the following variables were included in the predictor model tween the 2 groups with an unconstrained model, with dif-
using the forced entry method: sex of the child, maternal age ferences in the fit indices between models. We considered the
at delivery, maternal education, annual household income, interaction effect to be insignificant when the difference in the
maternal psychological distress assessed by the Kessler comparative fit index (CFI) was less than 0.010, the change in
Psychological Distress Scale Japanese version33 at 1 year post the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was less
partum, the presence of an elder sibling(s), attendance at a than 0.015, and the change in the standardized root mean
childcare facility, frequency of reading to the child, fre- square residual (SRMR) was less than 0.010.35 Third, to ex-
quency of taking the child out of the house (other than the plore the contribution of predictors for screen time and child
childcare facility), and child’s sleep time per day at age 1 year. development, we constructed a predictor-included RI-CLPM
Categorization is illustrated in eTable 2 in Supplement 1. by including between-level predictors affecting the random in-
Data were obtained from the questionnaire completed by the tercepts simultaneously, as presented by Mulder and
children’s parents or guardians as well as medical record Hamaker.36 In all models, benchmark values for interpreting
transcripts. the size of the RI-CLPM cross-lag effect were set at 0.03 (small
effect), 0.07 (medium effect), and 0.12 (large effect).37
Statistical Analysis To assess the robustness of the findings, we performed sen-
Descriptive statistics were used to summarize the distribu- sitivity analyses using the basic RI-CLPM for children with com-
tion of participant characteristics. We examined the longitu- plete data at 3 years (n = 70 226). In these analyses, we esti-
dinal and directional associations between TV/DVD screen mated missing TV/DVD screen time data and ASQ-3 scores at
time and ASQ-3 scores using a RI-CLPM (Figure; eAppendix 1 and 2 years using full information maximum likelihood es-
in Supplement 1). This model is a structural equation-based timation and Bayesian estimation with the Markov chain Monte
approach to better approximate within-person (time- Carlo algorithm. The number of cohort participants included
variant) relationships by accounting for between-person and excluded from the analyses and a comparison of basic
(time-invariant, traitlike) stability by including random demographic characteristics are summarized in eTable 3 in

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online September 18, 2023 E3

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 10/16/2023


Research Original Investigation Screen Time and Developmental Performance Among Children at 1-3 Years of Age

Table 1. Directional Association Between Television/DVD Screen Time and Development (Ages and Stages Questionnaires,
Third Edition [ASQ-3] Scores) in Each Domain

Standardized estimate (95% CI)a


Association Communication Gross motor Fine motor Problem-solving Personal-social
Cross-lagged effects
Screen time at age 1 y −0.06 (−0.07 to −0.01 (−0.02 to −0.01 (−0.03 to −0.01 (−0.02 to −0.01 (−0.03 to
and ASQ-3 score at age 2 y, β −0.05) 0) 0) 0) 0)
ASQ-3 score at age 1 y −0.03 (−0.04 to 0.01 (0 to 0 (−0.01 to 0) −0.01 (−0.02 to 0 (−0.01 to
and screen time at age 2 y, γ −0.02) 0.02) 0) 0.01)
Screen time at age 2 y −0.02 (−0.04 to −0.03 (−0.05 to −0.07 (−0.08 to −0.01 (−0.03 to −0.12 (−0.14 to
and ASQ-3 score at age 3 y, β −0.01) −0.01) −0.06) 0) −0.11)
ASQ-3 score at age 2 y −0.06 (−0.07 to 0.02 (0.01 to −0.02 (−0.04 to 0.02 (0 to −0.01 (−0.02 to
and screen time at age 3 y, γ −0.04) 0.03) −0.01) 0.03) 0.01)
Fit indices
CFI 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
RMSEA (90% CI) 0.01 (0.01 to 0.02 (0.01 to 0.03 (0.02 to 0.02 (0.01 to 0 (0 to
0.02) 0.03) 0.03) 0.02) 0.01)
SRMR 0 0.01 0.01 0 0
a
Abbreviations: CFI, comparative fit index; RI-CLPM, random-intercepts, Benchmark values for interpreting the size of the RI-CLPM cross-lag effect
cross-lagged panel model; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation; were set at 0.03 (small effect), 0.07 (medium effect), and 0.12 (large effect).37
SRMR, standardized root mean square residual.

Supplement 1. All statistical analyses were conducted using Association Between Screen Time
SPSS and SPSS Amos version 27 (IBM). and ASQ-3 Scores in Each Domain
Each model demonstrated a good fit (Table 1). We observed
small to medium linking from screen time at age 1 year to ASQ-3
scores at age 2 years in the communication domain (β = −0.06;
Results 95% CI, −0.07 to −0.05) and small to large linking from screen
Descriptive Statistics time at age 2 years to ASQ-3 scores at age 3 years in gross mo-
eTable 2 in Supplement 1 summarizes the characteristics of chil- tor, fine motor, and personal-social domains (β range, −0.03
dren and mothers. Of 57 980 included children, 29 418 (50.7%) [95% CI, −0.05 to −0.01] to −0.12 [95% CI, −0.14 to −0.11]). The
were male, and the mean (SD) maternal age at delivery was 31.5 small to medium obverse linking was observed from ASQ-3
(4.9) years. At ages 1, 2, and 3 years, 15 051 children (26.0%), scores in the communication domain to screen time (2 years:
16 430 children (28.3%), and 17 403 children (30.0%) viewed γ = −0.03; 95% CI, −0.04 to −0.02; 3 years: γ = −0.06; 95% CI,
TV/DVD for at least 2 hours per day, respectively. −0.07 to −0.04).

Directional Association Between Screen Time Moderation or Interaction Effects on the Cross-Lags Link
and ASQ-3 Scores Table 2 and Table 3 present the results of 2 multiple-group
The Figure and eTable 4 in Supplement 1 summarize the es- RI-CLPMs stratified by the child’s sex and maternal psycho-
timates in the basic RI-CLPM. Fit indices demonstrated good logical distress. Both models demonstrated a good fit. In the
model fit. Significant variances of random intercepts were ob- model stratified by the child’s sex, we observed no difference
served for both TV/DVD screen time (σS2 = 0.46; 95% CI, 0.44- in the cross-lag effect between boys and girls (Table 2). For the
0.48) and ASQ-3 scores (σA2 = 33.95; 95% CI, 32.31-35.59). This model stratified by maternal psychological distress, we ob-
finding indicated stable individual differences in both screen served no distinct difference between the groups as assessed
time and developmental scores; furthermore, it was reason- by a change in fit indices (change in CFI, 0; change in RMSEA,
able to set a random intercept in the model. Positive autore- 0; change in SRMR, 0). However, a small to medium cross-lag
gressive associations in the screen time (αS2 and αS3) implied association between ASQ scores at age 2 years and screen time
that children with more TV screen time were more likely to con- at age 3 years was observed only in children with maternal psy-
tinue this behavior on subsequent occasions. We observed chological distress (γ = −0.05; 95% CI, −0.07 to −0.02) (Table 3).
negative covariances between random intercepts of TV/DVD
screen time and ASQ-3 scores (σSA = −0.15; 95% CI, −0.18 to Predictors of Screen Time and ASQ-3 Scores
−0.12), suggesting that children with more screen time tended The RI-CLPM with 10 predictors demonstrated a close model
to have lower ASQ-3 scores at ages 1 to 3 years. fit, although poorer than the basic RI-CLPM (CFI, 0.94; RM-
Furthermore, we observed small to medium negative SEA, 0.04; 90% CI, 0.04-0.04; SRMR, 0.02) (data not shown).
cross-lags linking from screen time at age 1 year to ASQ-3 score Increased TV/DVD screen time was associated with not attend-
at age 2 years (β = −0.05; 95% CI, −0.06 to −0.04) and me- ing a childcare facility, no elder sibling(s), infrequent reading
dium linking from screen time at age 2 years to ASQ-3 score at to children, lower maternal education level, psychological dis-
age 3 years (β = −0.08; 95% CI, −0.09 to −0.06). tress, lower income, seldom going outdoors, less sleep time,

E4 JAMA Pediatrics Published online September 18, 2023 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 10/16/2023


Screen Time and Developmental Performance Among Children at 1-3 Years of Age Original Investigation Research

Table 2. Directional Association Between Television/DVD Screen Time Table 3. Directional Association Between Television/DVD Screen Time
and Development (Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Third Edition and Development (Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Third Edition
[ASQ-3] Scores) Stratified By Child’s Sex [ASQ-3] Scores) Stratified By Maternal Psychological Distress

Standardized estimate (95% CI)a Standardized estimate (95% CI)a


Association Boys Girls Mentally stable Mental distress
Association (<5)b (≥5)b
Cross-lagged effects
Cross-lagged effects
Screen time at age 1 y and −0.05 (−0.07 to −0.03 (−0.05 to Screen time at age 1 y and −0.04 (−0.05 to −0.06 (−0.09 to
ASQ-3 score at age 2 y, β −0.04) −0.02) ASQ-3 score at age 2 y, β −0.03) −0.04)
ASQ-3 score at age 1 y and −0.01 (−0.03 to 0 (−0.01 to ASQ-3 score at age 1 y and −0.01 (−0.03 to −0.02 (−0.04 to
screen time at age 2 y, γ 0.01) 0.01) screen time at age 2 y, γ 0.01) 0)
Screen time at age 2 y and −0.09 (−0.11 to −0.07 (−0.09 to Screen time at age 2 y and −0.08 (−0.09 to −0.09 (−0.12 to
ASQ-3 score at age 3 y, β −0.07) −0.05) ASQ-3 score at age 3 y, β −0.06) −0.06)
ASQ-3 score at age 2 y and −0.02 (−0.04 to −0.01 (−0.03 to ASQ-3 score at age 2 y and −0.02 (−0.03 to −0.05 (−0.07 to
screen time at age 3 y, γ 0.01) 0.02) screen time at age 3 y, γ −0.01) −0.02)
Fit indices Fit indices
CFI 1.00 CFI 1.00
RMSEA (90% CI) 0.01 (0.01 to RMSEA (90% CI) 0.01 (0 to
0.02) 0.01)
SRMR 0
SRMR 0
Abbreviations: CFI, comparative fit index; NA, not applicable; RI-CLPM,
Abbreviations: CFI, comparative fit index; RI-CLPM, random-intercepts,
random-intercepts, cross-lagged panel model; RMSEA, root mean square error
cross-lagged panel model; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation;
of approximation; SRMR, standardized root mean square residual.
SRMR, standardized root mean square residual.
a
a Benchmark values for interpreting the size of the RI-CLPM cross-lag effect
Benchmark values for interpreting the size of the RI-CLPM cross-lag effect
were set at 0.03 (small effect), 0.07 (medium effect), and 0.12 (large effect).37
were set at 0.03 (small effect), 0.07 (medium effect), and 0.12 (large effect).37
b
Maternal psychological distress was assessed by the Kessler Psychological
Distress Scale Japanese version33 at 1 year post partum.
and younger maternal age (Table 4). In contrast, higher devel-
opmental performance was associated with being female, fre-
quent reading to the child, attending a childcare facility, el- Compared with another Japanese cohort that began in
der sibling(s), younger maternal age, no psychological distress, 2001,38 TV screen time for young children in the JECS cohort born
frequently going outdoors, higher income, and more sleep time. between 2011 and 2014 appeared to decline. This finding was
Even after accounting for the effects of these predictors, the consistent with the decrease in TV and DVD screen times at age
association between random intercepts of screen time and 0 to 8 years from 2011 to 2017 and is likely related to increased
ASQ-3 remained negative (σSA = −0.11; 95% CI, −0.14 to −0.08) use of mobile devices.39 For TV/DVD screen time alone, only 25%
(data not shown). to 29% of the children at ages 2 and 3 years met the guideline
limiting screen time to no more than 1 hour each day.
Sensitivity Analyses Our study results support the findings of a Canadian co-
The basic RI-CLPM was performed using full information maxi- hort study using the RI-CLPM that demonstrated a direc-
mum likelihood and Bayesian estimation for children with com- tional association of greater screen time (TVs, DVDs, comput-
plete data only at age 3 years (n = 70 226). The results were simi- ers, and game consoles) at age 24 months with a lower ASQ-3
lar to the RI-CLPM results for children with complete data at score at age 36 months.17 In addition, we found that screen time
ages 1, 2, and 3 years (n = 57 980) (eTable 5 in Supplement 1). at age 1 year affects poorer communication skills at age 2 years.
The effects on cognitive function may be partially mediated
by reduced activity in the frontal and parietal brain regions.40
Conversely, screen time at age 2 years affected subsequent mo-
Discussion tor and personal-social skills. This may be attributed to miss-
Our study demonstrated that the negative cross-lag associa- ing opportunities to learn motor and interpersonal skills by sed-
tion from screen time to developmental scores remained con- entary looking at screens.
sistent throughout toddlerhood. Notably, we found a bidirec- Our study demonstrated an obverse association from lower
tional association between TV/DVD screen time and ASQ-3 scores only in the communication domain at age 1 year
developmental scores in the communication domain from age to more screen time at age 2 years and a similar association with
1 to 2 years. Additionally, we observed negative associations a stronger effect from age 2 years to 3 years. The factors that
between TV/DVD screen time at age 2 years and the develop- may contribute to increased screen time include low devel-
mental scores in gross motor, fine motor, and personal-social opment of communication skills, an environment that does not
domains at age 3 years. A negative association between the de- promote communication development, and a child’s prefer-
velopmental score at age 2 years and screen time at age 3 years ence for the environment. The association between lower
was observed in the communication domain. No significant ASQ-3 scores and subsequent increases in screen time was par-
interaction by child’s sex or maternal psychological distress was ticularly evident among mothers experiencing mental health
observed. In addition, developmental scores at age 2 years were challenges. These findings underscore the importance of so-
negatively linked to screen time at age 3 years in children with cial support for mothers who may rely on media to manage
maternal psychological distress. their child’s behavior.

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online September 18, 2023 E5

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 10/16/2023


Research Original Investigation Screen Time and Developmental Performance Among Children at 1-3 Years of Age

environment.49 Third, promoting social networks that allow


Table 4. Predictors of Television/DVD Screen Time
and Child Development Among 53 085 Children children to have high-quality interactions with their fami-
lies is important. Active social interaction in the neighbor-
Standardized estimate (95% CI)
hood may promote healthy behaviors, such as reducing
Development
Predictor Screen time (ASQ-3 score) screen time and increasing physical activity levels.50
Maternal age −0.03 (−0.04 to −0.11 (−0.12 to
−0.02) −0.10)
Maternal education −0.11 (−0.12 to 0.02 (0.01 to Strengths and Limitations
−0.10) 0.03) The strength of this study is that it provides insights into the
Household income (≥¥4 million −0.05 (−0.06 to 0.04 (0.03 to directional association between TV/DVD screen time and de-
[≥US $27 863] vs <¥4 million −0.04) 0.05)
[<US $27 863]) velopmental milestones in typically developing young chil-
Maternal psychological distress 0.05 (0.04 to −0.09 (−0.10 to dren. It was based on a large birth cohort dataset representa-
(yes vs no)a 0.06) −0.08)
tive of the Japanese population and provides reliable results.51
Elder sibling (yes vs no) −0.19 (−0.20 to 0.14 (0.13 to
−0.18) 0.15) However, it had some limitations. First, we assessed the de-
Attending childcare facility −0.28 (−0.29 to 0.16 (0.15 to velopmental milestones and screen time based on parental re-
(yes vs no) −0.27) 0.17) ports, which is suitable for collecting data from large popula-
Reading to child −0.13 (−0.14 to 0.18 (0.17 to tions; however, it may have led to reporting bias. Second, there
−0.11) 0.19)
is a single-informant bias, ie, most of the respondents to the
Going outdoors −0.04 (−0.05 to 0.07 (0.06 to
−0.03) 0.08) questionnaire were mothers because they were the primary
Sleep time −0.03 (−0.04 to 0.05 (0.04 to caregivers of their children. These biases may weaken the gen-
−0.02) 0.05)
eralizability of the findings. Third, advances in media tech-
Child’s sex (girls vs boys) 0 (−0.01 to 0.24 (0.23 to
0.02) 0.25) nology may have changed the situation of children’s screen
2
R for the random intercept 0.14 0.15 viewing because children are increasingly exposed to por-
table media screens other than TVs and DVDs from an early
Abbreviation: ASQ-3, Ages and Stages Questionnaires, third edition.
a age. According to a 2016 Japanese survey, smartphone, tab-
Maternal psychological distress was assessed by the Kessler Psychological
Distress Scale Japanese version33 at 1 year post partum. Participants were let, and portable gaming device use among children aged 1 to
considered to be distressed if their score was 5 or greater. 3 years ranged from 2% to 32%.52 In a 2017 US survey, chil-
dren aged 2 to 4 years spent 1 hour a day on both TV and mo-
Frequent reading to the child, attending childcare, and hav- bile devices.39 Information on mobile device usage from age
ing an elder sibling(s) were particularly associated with higher 1 year was not available for this study, which may underesti-
developmental scores and lesser screen time. A similar, albeit mate total screen time. Fourth, the media content available to
weaker, association was observed for taking children out- children is becoming increasingly diverse, and they may be ex-
doors. This finding supports the existing literature41-48 by sug- posed to content intended for adults. Further research is war-
gesting that parent-child verbal interaction, communication ranted on the effects of various screens and different types of
with others, and social play are effective in promoting devel- content on children’s development.
opment for young children, besides being associated with lesser
screen time.
This study highlights several practical recommenda-
tions. First, emphasis should be placed on having opportu-
Conclusions
nities for face-to-face interaction with family members and In this study, TV/DVD screen time was associated with lower
other children rather than relying on screens from an early subsequent developmental performance in young children.
age to enhance children’s development. Second, pediatri- Screen media is prevalent in children’s environments, thus ne-
cians and health care professionals should help each family cessitating tailored media plans for each family for the better
develop a feasible media plan by making recommendations development of their child. In addition, parents experiencing
and introducing social support tools because parental atti- emotional distress require support in reducing their chil-
tudes toward media use significantly effect the home media dren’s exposure to screens from an early age.

ARTICLE INFORMATION Center for Child Health and Development, Tokyo, All authors.
Accepted for Publication: July 24, 2023. Japan (Mezawa); Department of Nutrition and Drafting of the manuscript: Yamamoto, Mezawa.
Metabolic Medicine, Center for Preventive Medical Critical review of the manuscript for important
Published Online: September 18, 2023. Sciences, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan (Sakurai); intellectual content: All authors.
doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2023.3643 Department of Bioenvironmental Medicine, Statistical analysis: Yamamoto, Mezawa.
Open Access: This is an open access article Graduate School of Medicine, Chiba University, Administrative, technical, or material support:
distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. Chiba, Japan (Mori). Mezawa, Sakurai.
© 2023 Yamamoto M et al. JAMA Pediatrics. Author Contributions: Dr Yamamoto had full Study supervision: Mezawa, Mori.
Author Affiliations: Department of Sustainable access to all of the data in the study and takes Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Health Science, Center for Preventive Medical responsibility for the integrity of the data and the Funding/Support: This study was funded by the
Sciences, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan accuracy of the data analysis. Japanese Ministry of the Environment.
(Yamamoto, Mori); Medical Support Center for Study concept and design: Yamamoto, Mezawa.
Japan Environment and Children’s Study, National Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data:

E6 JAMA Pediatrics Published online September 18, 2023 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 10/16/2023


Screen Time and Developmental Performance Among Children at 1-3 Years of Age Original Investigation Research

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funder had no meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665-675. 25. Adani S, Cepanec M. Sex differences in early
role in the design and conduct of the study; doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0327 communication development: behavioral and
collection, management, analysis, and 11. Zhao J, Yu Z, Sun X, et al. Association between neurobiological indicators of more vulnerable
interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or screen time trajectory and early childhood communication system development in boys. Croat
approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit development in children in China. JAMA Pediatr. Med J. 2019;60(2):141-149. doi:10.3325/cmj.2019.
the manuscript for publication. 2022;176(8):768-775. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics. 60.141
Group Information: A complete list of the 2022.1630 26. Fenske SJ, Liu J, Chen H, et al. Sex differences
members of the Japan Environment and Children’s 12. McArthur BA, Hentges R, Christakis DA, in resting state functional connectivity across the
Study Group appears in Supplement 2. McDonald S, Tough S, Madigan S. Cumulative social first two years of life. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2023;60:
Disclaimer: The findings and conclusions of this risk and child screen use: the role of child 101235. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2023.101235
article are solely the responsibility of the authors temperament. J Pediatr Psychol. 2022;47(2):171-179. 27. Kushima M, Kojima R, Shinohara R, et al; Japan
and do not represent the official views of the doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsab087 Environment and Children’s Study Group.
Japanese Ministry of the Environment. 13. Thompson AL, Adair LS, Bentley ME. Maternal Association between screen time exposure in
Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 3. characteristics and perception of temperament children at 1 year of age and autism spectrum
associated with infant TV exposure. Pediatrics. disorder at 3 years of age: the Japan Environment
Additional Contributions: We express our and Children’s Study. JAMA Pediatr. 2022;176(4):
gratitude to all Japan Environment and Children’s 2013;131(2):e390-e397. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-1224
384-391. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.5778
Study participants, their families, and the 14. Sugawara M, Matsumoto S, Murohashi H, Sakai
cooperating health care professionals. A, Isshiki N. Trajectories of early television contact 28. Slobodin O, Heffler KF, Davidovitch M. Screen
in Japan: relationship with preschoolers’ media and autism spectrum disorder: a systematic
REFERENCES externalizing problems. J Child Media. 2015;9(4): literature review. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2019;40(4):
453-471. doi:10.1080/17482798.2015.1089298 303-311. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000654
1. Slomski A. Chronic mental health issues in
children now loom larger than physical problems. 15. Radesky JS, Silverstein M, Zuckerman B, 29. Kawamoto T, Nitta H, Murata K, et al; Working
JAMA. 2012;308(3):223-225. doi:10.1001/jama.2012. Christakis DA. Infant self-regulation and early Group of the Epidemiological Research for
6951 childhood media exposure. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5): Children’s Environmental Health. Rationale and
e1172-e1178. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-2367 study design of the Japan environment and
2. Houtrow AJ, Larson K, Olson LM, Newacheck children’s study (JECS). BMC Public Health. 2014;14:
PW, Halfon N. Changing trends of childhood 16. Radesky JS, Peacock-Chambers E, Zuckerman 25. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-14-25
disability, 2001-2011. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):530- B, Silverstein M. Use of mobile technology to calm
538. doi:10.1542/peds.2014-0594 upset children: associations with social-emotional 30. Vandenbroucke JP, von Elm E, Altman DG,
development. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(4):397-399. et al; STROBE Initiative. Strengthening the
3. Strasburger VC, Hogan MJ, Mulligan DA; Council Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology
on Communications and Media. Children, doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4260
(STROBE): explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med.
adolescents, and the media. Pediatrics. 2013;132(5): 17. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough 2007;4(10):e297. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0040297
958-961. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-2656 S. Association between screen time and children’s
performance on a developmental screening test. 31. Squires J, Twombly E, Bricker D, Potter L. ASQ-3
4. American Academy of Pediatrics. American User’s Guide. Brookes Publishing; 2009.
Academy of Pediatrics announces new JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(3):244-250. doi:10.1001/
recommendations for children’s media use. jamapediatrics.2018.5056 32. Mezawa H, Aoki S, Nakayama SF, et al.
Accessed July 18, 2023. https://dnr.vnr1.com/wp- 18. Neville RD, McArthur BA, Eirich R, Lakes KD, Psychometric profile of the ages and stages
content/uploads/2016/10/PR-Media-Use-FINAL- Madigan S. Bidirectional associations between questionnaires, Japanese translation. Pediatr Int.
101216.pdf screen time and children’s externalizing and 2019;61(11):1086-1095. doi:10.1111/ped.13990

5. World Health Organization. Guidelines on internalizing behaviors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 33. Furukawa TA, Kawakami N, Saitoh M, et al. The
physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for 2021;62(12):1475-1484. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13425 performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and
children under 5 years of age. Accessed July 18, 19. Nelson CA, Bosquet M. Neurobiology of fetal K10 in the World Mental Health Survey Japan. Int J
2022. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/ and infant development: implications for infant Methods Psychiatr Res. 2008;17(3):152-158. doi:10.
311664 mental health. In: Zeanah CH, ed. Handbook of 1002/mpr.257

6. McArthur BA, Volkova V, Tomopoulos S, Infant Mental Health. 2nd ed. Guilford Press; 2000: 34. Hamaker EL, Kuiper RM, Grasman RPPP.
Madigan S. Global prevalence of meeting screen 37-59. A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychol
time guidelines among children 5 years and 20. Johnson MH. Functional brain development in Methods. 2015;20(1):102-116. doi:10.1037/a0038889
younger: a systematic review and meta-analysis. humans. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2(7):475-483. doi: 35. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes
JAMA Pediatr. 2022;176(4):373-383. doi:10.1001/ 10.1038/35081509 to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ
jamapediatrics.2021.6386 21. Delagneau G, Twilhaar ES, Testa R, van Veen S, Modeling. 2007;14(3):464-504. doi:10.1080/
7. Aishworiya R, Magiati I, Phua D, et al. Are there Anderson P. Association between prenatal maternal 10705510701301834
bidirectional influences between screen time anxiety and/or stress and offspring’s cognitive 36. Mulder JD, Hamaker EL. Three extensions of
exposure and social behavioral traits in young functioning: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2023;94 the random intercept cross-lagged panel model.
children? J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2022;43(6):362-369. (3):779-801. doi:10.1111/cdev.13885 Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 2021;28(4):638-648.
doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000001069 22. Madigan S, Oatley H, Racine N, et al. A doi:10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738
8. Alroqi H, Serratrice L, Cameron-Faulkner T. The meta-analysis of maternal prenatal depression and 37. Orth U, Meier LL, Bühler JL, et al. Effect size
association between screen media quantity, anxiety on child socioemotional development. J Am guidelines for cross-lagged effects. Psychol Methods.
content, and context and language development. Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;57(9):645- Published online June 23, 2022. doi:10.1037/
J Child Lang. 2022;27:1-29. doi:10.1017/ 657.e8. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.012 met0000499
S0305000922000265 23. Kingston D, McDonald S, Austin MP, Tough S. 38. Statistics and Information Department. Outline
9. Eirich R, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, McGuinness C, Association between prenatal and postnatal of the fourth longitudinal survey of newborns in the
Christakis DA, Madigan S. Association of screen psychological distress and toddler cognitive 21st century. Accessed July 18, 2023. https://www.
time with internalizing and externalizing behavior development: a systematic review. PLoS One. 2015; mhlw.go.jp/english/database/db-hw/
problems in children 12 years or younger: 10(5):e0126929. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0126929 newborns4th/index.html
a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA 24. Weissman MM. Children of depressed 39. Common Sense. The Common Sense census:
Psychiatry. 2022;79(5):393-405. doi:10.1001/ parents—a public health opportunity. JAMA media use by kids age zero to eight. Accessed July
jamapsychiatry.2022.0155 Psychiatry. 2016;73(3):197-198. doi:10.1001/ 18, 2023. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/
10. Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, jamapsychiatry.2015.2967 sites/default/files/research/report/csm_
Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and zerotoeight_fullreport_release_2.pdf
child language skills: a systematic review and

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online September 18, 2023 E7

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 10/16/2023


Research Original Investigation Screen Time and Developmental Performance Among Children at 1-3 Years of Age

40. Law EC, Han MX, Lai Z, et al. Associations Pediatrics. 2021;147(6):e2020011429. doi:10.1542/ 49. Lauricella AR, Wartella E, Rideout VJ. Young
between infant screen use, electroencephalog- peds.2020-011429 children’s screen time: the complex role of parent
raphy markers, and cognitive outcomes. JAMA 45. Moreno MA, Furtner F, Rivara FP. Advice for and child factors. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2015;36:11-17.
Pediatr. 2023;177(3):311-318. doi:10.1001/ patients: reading to children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2014.12.001
jamapediatrics.2022.5674 Med. 2012;166(11):1080. doi:10.1001/2013. 50. Baldwin J, Arundell L, Hnatiuk JA. Associations
41. Brown A; Council on Communications and jamapediatrics.412 between the neighbourhood social environment
Media. Media use by children younger than 2 years. 46. Radesky J, Christakis D; Council on and preschool children’s physical activity and
Pediatrics. 2011;128(5):1040-1045. doi:10.1542/peds. Communications and Media. Media and young screen time. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1065.
2011-1753 minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20162591. doi:10. doi:10.1186/s12889-022-13493-2
42. Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, et al. 1542/peds.2016-2591 51. Michikawa T, Nitta H, Nakayama SF, et al; Japan
Audible television and decreased adult words, 47. Schmidt ME, Pempek TA, Kirkorian HL, Lund Environment and Children’s Study Group. The
infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: AF, Anderson DR. The effects of background Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS):
a population-based study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. television on the toy play behavior of very young a preliminary report on selected characteristics of
2009;163(6):554-558. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics. children. Child Dev. 2008;79(4):1137-1151. doi:10. approximately 10 000 pregnant women recruited
2009.61 1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01180.x during the first year of the study. J Epidemiol. 2015;
43. Zimmerman FJ, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, et al. 25(6):452-458. doi:10.2188/jea.JE20140186
48. Sugiyama M, Tsuchiya KJ, Okubo Y, et al.
Teaching by listening: the importance of adult-child Outdoor play as a mitigating factor in the 52. Cabinet Office. Heisei 28 Survey on Internet
conversations to language development. Pediatrics. association between screen time for young children usage environment among youth. Accessed July 18,
2009;124(1):342-349. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-2267 and neurodevelopmental outcomes. JAMA Pediatr. 2023. https://www8.cao.go.jp/youth/youth-harm/
44. McArthur BA, Browne D, McDonald S, Tough S, 2023;177(3):303-310. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics. chousa/h28/net-jittai_child/pdf-index.html
Madigan S. Longitudinal associations between 2022.5356
screen use and reading in preschool-aged children.

E8 JAMA Pediatrics Published online September 18, 2023 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 10/16/2023

You might also like