Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Journal of Public Administration Research And Theory, 2018, 1–17

doi:10.1093/jopart/muy008
Article

Article

“They Are Underpaid and Understaffed”: How


Clients Interpret Encounters with Street-Level
Bureaucrats
Carolyn Y. Barnes, Julia Henly

Duke University, University of Chicago

Abstract
Scholars have explored the nature and consequences of administrative burden but less is known
about how citizens interpret costly encounters with the state. This qualitative study of 85 child
care subsidy recipients applies attribution theory from psychology to illustrate how clients de-
velop causal explanations for administrative burden. The findings show that clients either blamed
negative experiences on bureaucrats—viewing workers as in control of their behavior, or the bur-
eaucracy—blaming factors related to the subsidy system. In rare instances, clients viewed the
bureaucracy as intentionally discouraging claims. We observed some variation by race/ethnicity
and study sites. Examining clients’ causal explanations of administrative burden helps clarify how
clients’ interpretation of costly bureaucratic encounters influences future claims, their perceptions
of the state, and their political participation.

Public administration scholars have revisited the study This literature focuses on the valence, nature, and
of administrative burden, or citizens’ onerous experi- broader effects of administrative burden, but less is
ences with policy implementation (Burden et al. 2012). known about how citizens interpret administrative bur-
Studies have demonstrated how administrative bur- den. We argue that it is not just what citizens experience
den affects bureaucrats who implement policy (Burden when interacting with public bureaucracies; it is also
et al. 2012) and clients who apply for and receive ben- how they interpret those experiences that may influence
efits (Heinrich 2015). Moynihan, Herd, and Harvey whether or not they claim benefits and their perception
(2014) have conceptualized administrative burden as of the state and political rights as citizens (Soss, 2005).
learning, psychological, and compliance costs that can While qualitative studies of bureaucratic encounters
undermine the effectiveness of policy, and negatively suggest that clients hold nuanced views of bureaucrats
shape perceptions of the government. and bureaucracies (e.g., Adams, Snyder, and Sandfort
2002; Soss 1999a; Watkins-Hayes 2011), few studies
explain how clients interpret costly encounters, how
The authors gratefully acknowledge funding provided by the Office of they ascribe responsibility for their quality, or the con-
Planning, Research and Evaluation in the Administration for Children ditions under which distinct attributions emerge. We
and Families, US Department of Health and Human Services under argue that a client’s decision to seek future public ben-
grant 90YE0133. The contents of this article are solely the responsibility efits depends in part on her interpretation of who (or
of the authors and do not represent the official views or endorsement
of the funding agency. The authors are also grateful to the study
what) controls the quality of a bureaucratic encounter.
participants for contributing their valuable insights and time to this We address these questions through a qualitative
project. Special thanks to Merete Monrad and Jennifer Mosley, and study of a means-tested, child care subsidy program.
three anonymous reviewers, for helpful comments on an earlier draft. Serving more than 1.4 million children a month (U.S.
Address correspondence to the author at carolyn.barnes@duke.edu. DHHS 2015), the federal Child Care Development

© The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Public Management Research Association. 1
All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
2 Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx

Fund (CCDF) is the primary source of government- cannot bear the burden of costly encounters. Modeling
funded child care assistance for low-income working how clients make sense of onerous experiences—how
families in the United States (Lynch 2014). Like other they arrive at causal explanations for costly encoun-
public programs, child care subsidies aim to promote ters—may help explain why costs deter some clients
work and the economic “self-sufficiency” of families. and not others. These insights can clarify how admin-
Child care subsidies have received little attention in the istrative burden will affect client claiming behavior
public bureaucracy literature. and can inform policy interventions that aim increase
The interpretive approach we take reflects the lived program access. Finally, integrating client causal attri-
experiences of study respondents (Schwartz-Shea and butions into theories of bureaucratic encounters may
Yanow 2013). To understand clients’ causal explana- inform understanding of the influence of public poli-
tions for negative bureaucratic encounters, we draw cies on citizens’ political participation (Moynihan and
from the administrative burden literature and attribu- Soss 2014). Whom clients view as responsible for oner-
tion theory—a theory that explains how individuals ous experiences may shape their views of government
use information to arrive at a causal explanation for and political behavior.
some behavior or event (Fiske and Taylor 1991; Heider We begin by reviewing research on administrative
1958; Jones and Davis 1965; Kelley 1967; Weiner burden and bureaucratic encounters. We then sum-
2000). We adapt a concept we call “attributions of marize key concepts from attribution theory and turn
control”—whether claimants view outcomes as under to our empirical case: bureaucratic encounters with
their control, under the control of bureaucrats, or the child care subsidy programs in four regions: two
due to external factors to explore how claimants de- in Illinois and two in New York. Using qualitative
velop perceptions of who is responsible for the bur- interviews with 85 clients, we examine administra-
eaucratic encounters. Attribution theory acknowledges tive burden and attributions of control within clients’
the valence and costs of bureaucratic encounters—a evaluations of program experiences, asking whom or
familiar topic of administrative burden research— what clients view as responsible for negative and posi-
and incorporates the capacity of clients, workers, and tive bureaucratic encounters. We leverage differences
bureaucracies to control bureaucratic encounters, im- in subsidy administration across the four study sites
portant concepts in prominent public administration and race/ethnic differences in attributions to explore
frameworks. the conditions under which distinct attributional pat-
We contribute to the study of administrative burden terns emerge. We discuss attributions of control as a
by conceptualizing key elements of clients’ interpretive conceptual link that can help public administration
process—illustrating what clients do when interpreting scholars understand how subjective interpretations of
burdensome encounters (develop causal explanations) costly interactions with government can shape citizens’
and whom claimants view as responsible for their willingness to claim benefits and participate politically.
costs (distinct objects of ascription). We argue that at-
tribution theory from psychology provides a cognitive
model for this dynamic that can supplement research Bureaucratic Encounters: Theoretical and
on bureaucratic encounters and clarify the relationship Empirical Background
between claiming behavior and political participation. Qualitative studies of administrative burden suggest
Extending attribution theory to the study of admin- that clients hold nuanced interpretations of burden-
istrative burden also opens the black box of client some bureaucratic encounters. For example, Soss’s
interpretation in ways that may reconcile conflicting (1999a) study of welfare application encounters sug-
insights in existing theory and the gaps between theory gests that negative experiences do not always lead to
and empirical realities. For example, the administrative dissatisfaction with welfare claims. He finds a “paradox
burden perspective and related frameworks cast claim- of subordination” across client responses whereby cli-
ing behavior as a function of client resources relative ents express both satisfaction with claims and dissatis-
to the costs of claiming benefits, but conflict on how faction with caseworker treatment. Soss surmises that
costs deter claims. The street-level bureaucracy frame- the high stakes of application processes and clients’
work and power-dependence theory suggest that those low expectations of caseworkers lead clients to ration-
with the least resources claim benefits despite burden- alize that experiences could be worse.
some experiences because they lack alternatives. In Soss (1999a) finds that clients of the Aid to Families
contrast, administrative burden studies frame costs as with Dependent Children (AFDC) program evaluate
deterring the most disadvantaged because they cannot workers and agencies separately, rather than having a
afford to pay the high costs of claims. Empirical stud- generally negative or positive view of their bureaucratic
ies are inconclusive on this point—and demonstrate encounters. Similarly, Watkins-Hayes’s (2011 study of
how costs deter both the well resourced and those who racial representation in welfare bureaucracies suggests

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx 3

that clients sometimes recognize that the constraints and Harvey 2014, 5). Finally, compliance costs reflect
workers face (e.g., time burdens, paper work, limited the burden of completing applications, providing docu-
resources) can confine their interactions with workers mentation, and responding to the demands of agency
to brief untailored service experiences. Although this staff (Moynihan, Herd, and Harvey 2014, 4).
finding is not central to Watkins-Hayes’s analysis, it Other related frameworks also construct bureau-
suggests that clients may recognize obstacles limiting cratic encounters in terms of costs and claimants’
bureaucrats’ agency when evaluating encounters. resources, without explaining how clients differentiate
Adams , Snyder, and Sandfort’s (2002) qualitative workers from bureaucracies or ascribe responsibility
study of clients and staff in the child care subsidy pro- for negative encounters. For example, Lipsky’s (1980,
gram show that some but not all subsidy clients blame 27–9) street-level bureaucracy framework describes
caseworkers for negative experiences such as losing bureaucratic encounters as negative and the realities
paper work, failing to return phone calls, and provid- of frontline work—poor training, high caseloads, and
ing incorrect information about the subsidy program. ambiguous performance standards—as compromis-
Similarly, Snyder Bernstein, and Koralek (2006) find ing workers’ discretion and service quality. On client
that child care subsidy clients distinguished responsive resources, Lipsky (1980, 54–6) does argue that clients
and helpful caseworkers from negligent workers who do not necessarily hold workers accountable for poor
rarely answered phone calls or were considered “slow” experiences because of the high demand for services and
in paper processing. the limited alternatives for assistance. Nevertheless, his
While pointing to varied interpretations of bureau- argument emphasizes costly bureaucratic encounters
cratic encounters, these child care subsidy studies do and clients’ limited capacity to improve experiences.
not provide a theoretical explanation for the observed Lipsky’s aim is not to elucidate how clients evaluate
variation. And the administrative burden literature has workers and agencies differently or to delineate the fac-
yet to offer a theory to explain how clients develop tors that contribute to the quality of agency encounters.
causal explanations for onerous claiming experiences, Hasenfeld’s social policy application of power-
which we argue could help explain variation and fur- dependence theory introduces variation in client evalu-
ther the explanatory power of the administrative ations (Hasenfeld 1985; Hasenfeld, Rafferty, and Zald
burden concept. Rather, the administrative burden 1987), but it is also not intended to explain nuanced
literature focuses on the punitive nature of worker views resulting from clients’ subjective interpretation
discretion, describing bureaucratic encounters in of bureaucratic encounters. This framework ties the
means-tested programs as largely negative and costly valence of client evaluations to power and resources.
(Kahn, Katz, and Gutek 1976). Caseworkers use their Clients with more power and resources have access
discretion in ways that subvert policy objectives and to less stigmatizing bureaucracies, such as entitlement
increase the costs of claims to clients (Brodkin 2007; programs (Hasenfeld 1985, 625; Hasenfeld, Rafferty,
Lipsky 1980; Soss 2000): “street-level bureaucrats” and Zald 1987; Schneider and Ingram 1993). These
can increase the burden of bureaucratic encounters clients also expect favorable claiming outcomes, per-
through “personal abuse, neglectful treatment, and ceive themselves as having influence over officials,
inconvenience” (Lipsky 1980, 56). Mounting empir- and have positive evaluations of their encounters
ical evidence suggests that the 1990s welfare reform (Hasenfeld, Rafferty, and Zald 1987, 631). In con-
further shifted the cost of claims to clients (Brodkin trast, clients of means-tested programs receive vague
and Majmundar 2010; Soss, Fording, and Schram information about benefit access, face administrative
2011) and incentivized punitive behavior toward cli- hurdles in securing services, and are subject to the
ents, especially African American and Latino appli- high discretionary authority of caseworkers (Auyero
cants (Brodkin and Madjumar 2010; Soss, Fording, 2012; Danziger 2010; Soss 2001, 2005; Soss, Fording,
and Schram 2011). and Schram 2011; Ybarra 2011). Scarce alternatives
In one important contribution to the administra- limit clients’ influence over agency workers. Indeed,
tive burden literature, Moynihan, Herd, and Harvey Auyero’s (2012) ethnographic work of welfare offices
(2014) developed a typology of costs that consti- in Buenos Aires illustrates the high levels of bureau-
tutes administrative burden and are expected to deter cratic power over clients who must learn to patiently
claims (Brodkin and Madjumar 2010). Learning costs comply with arbitrary claiming processes to secure
reflect the burden of gaining knowledge about pro- benefits. As a result, clients of means-tested programs
gram availability, eligibility requirements, and access. “come to expect less, believe they have no influence
Psychological costs refer to the stigma and stress over officials, and face greater risk of having their
of program experiences, including interactions that requests denied” and are less likely than beneficiaries
“violate basic psychological needs of autonomy and of universal entitlements to claim benefits (Hasenfeld,
respectful treatment” (Soss 1999a; Moynihan, Herd, Rafferty, and Zald 1987, 631).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
4 Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx

An emphasis on resources and costs in the power- or a lengthy boarding time to poorly trained person-
dependence framework obscures aspects of inter- nel (external and controllable) (Weiner 2000, 384).
pretive process that may shape how clients understand On purchasing behavior, how consumers ascribe
bureaucratic encounters. It assumes that clients in responsibility and controllability can shape whether
means-tested programs—because of their limited they complain, retaliate, or even boycott a product
resources—uniformly view caseworkers as powerful (Weiner 2000, 385–6).
agents. The question of whether and how clients dis- We argue that these components of attribution
tinguish bureaucrats from bureaucracies or distinctly theory can theoretically enrich the administrative
ascribe responsibility for program experiences to one burden literature by clarifying a client’s interpretive
or the other remains unanswered. process when evaluating program experiences. The
We argue that attribution theory—when extended to theory describes what claimants do to interpret
administrative burden—can explain how clients make negative bureaucratic encounters by suggesting that
sense of costly bureaucratic encounters. Attribution they seek to identify the cause of their experiences
theory seeks to explain how individuals arrive at a (Heider 1958; Taylor and Fiske 1991; Weiner 2000).
causal explanation for some behavior or event (Fiske Furthermore, the theory helps identify what clients
and Taylor 1991). The theory originates in psycholo- consider when evaluating bureaucratic encounters by
gist Heider’s (1958) early attribution work, which sug- differentiating objects of ascription—distinguishing
gests that we seek to explain someone’s behavior by whether individuals blame themselves, others (e.g.,
discerning her intentions. We either attribute an indi- bureaucrats), or external factors (e.g., bureaucracies).
vidual’s behavior to personal attribution (personality, Finally, the concept of “controllability” sheds light on
motivation, ability, and effort) or use external factors how clients assess their autonomy in claiming pro-
to make situational attributions for behavior. cesses and the autonomy of bureaucrats and the larger
A rich literature in psychology has elaborated on bureaucracy, which aligns with the concept of “power
Heider’s early model. Weiner (1985) suggests that differentials” emphasized by existing frameworks (i.e.,
attributions occur along the dimensions of locus of Hasenfeld 1987; Lipsky 1980).
control, stability, and controllability. Locus of control An attribution theory of how clients interpret bur-
refers to whether we attribute an outcome to internal eaucratic encounters can explain variation in client
or external causes. For example, we might perceive views and how their interpretation impacts claiming
success as a result of our own effort (internal locus behavior. Clients may assess the quality of encoun-
of control) or rather attributes it to luck or the diffi- ters by their ability to contact caseworkers. One cli-
culty of the task (external locus of control). Stability ent may interpret busy phone lines and unreturned
reflects whether we view the cause as fixed (an indi- messages as the result of a resource-constrained
vidual’s ability) or unstable (motivation or effort). agency—blaming the agency or the broader welfare
Finally, perceptions of controllability refer to whether bureaucracy—and viewing the worker as having lim-
we view ourselves as having control over the cause of ited control over outcomes. This client may choose
outcomes. to excuse the worker’s inaccessibility and persist in
Of central relevance to the current case, Weiner’s claiming benefits despite the negative experience.
(2000) later work applies these causal dimensions to In contrast, another client may interpret limited
marketing. He integrates the stability and control- caseworker accessibility as the deliberate effort of
lability dimensions to explain consumer satisfaction, a hostile or apathetic welfare bureaucrat to curtail
judgments of responsibility, and future purchas- successful claims. This client views the locus of con-
ing behavior. For consumers, attributions “exert trol to be with the worker, who is perceived as hav-
their influence after a product-related outcome ing high controllability over the outcome. This client
and prior to the next choice” (Weiner 2000, 383). might choose to forgo benefits because he or she
During this period, consumers evaluate whether a views the worker as intentionally deterring claims.
product-related outcome (e.g., service at the dry Or the client might persist in his or her efforts but
cleaners) aligns with their expectations. Consumers pursue a different claiming strategy that increases
blame negative experiences on whomever they per- the chances of dealing with an alternate (and ideally
ceive as both responsible and in control of out- more sympathetic) worker. This example illustrates
comes. Consumers generally attribute dissatisfaction how the concept of controllability can account for
to external causes but distinguish causes as either different views of the same negative program experi-
uncontrollable or controllable. Weiner illustrates this ence and why some negative encounters deter cli-
point by using the example of a poor flight experi- ents from future welfare claims, while other negative
ence in which a passenger may attribute a flight experiences do not—informing claiming strategies
delay to a snowstorm (external and uncontrollable) instead.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx 5

Given the potential implications of clients’ causal the form of a co-payment to the provider that is based
explanations for administrative burden, we use the on family income and household size. The remainder is
empirical case of the child care subsidy program to paid to the provider by the state.
explore how clients integrate notions of responsibil- Compared to means-tested cash welfare programs,
ity and controllability of outcomes when interpret- child care subsidies have received less attention in stud-
ing bureaucratic encounters. We build on existing ies of administrative burden. As previously noted, the
frameworks to incorporate attribution theory into few existing child care subsidy studies suggest clients
the analysis. Our goal is to examine the attributional experience onerous claiming processes similar to those
dimensions of client encounters with the subsidy pro- observed in AFDC and TANF studies. Still, we might
gram, rather than test the validity of attribution the- expect more variation in client experience with child
ory as an explanation of the claiming behavior of care subsidies because the program serves both TANF
welfare recipients. and non-TANF families and targets the working poor,
a less stigmatized group than nonworking welfare
recipients.
Empirical Case: The Child Care Subsidy
The Illinois and New York communities under study
Program
in this investigation include two suburban counties in
The CCDF promotes parental employment and chil- New York (Nassau and Westchester), an urban county
dren’s wellbeing by reducing the child care expenses in northern Illinois (Cook County), and a seven-county
of low-income families. The CCDF program was mixed urban–rural region in southwestern Illinois. In
born out of the 1996 Personal Responsibility and addition to their geographic diversity, the four study
Work Opportunity Reconciliation Act, the welfare re- sites differ in subsidy eligibility rules, administration,
form law that replaced the former AFDC entitlement and staffing models. For example, eligibility standards,
program with time-limited Temporary Assistance for parent co-payments, and other subsidy policy features
Needy Families (TANF) and instituted work require- are set at the county level in New York. County so-
ments as a condition of TANF receipt for most pro- cial service departments administer the program
gram beneficiaries (Lynch 2014). Although TANF (Professional Development Program 2013). In con-
time limits and work requirements received the most trast, in Illinois the program eligibility requirements
public attention, the child care changes in the 1996 and policies regarding implementation are determined
law were significant. Three AFDC-related child care at the state level; the Illinois Department of Human
programs were consolidated into one block grant and Services contracts with nonprofit community-based
the Child Care Development Block Grant (CCDBG) Child Care Resource and Referral (CCR&R) agen-
of 1990 was amended and reauthorized. The 1996 cies to administer the subsidy program for distinct
welfare law authorized that the funds for these geographic regions (Illinois Department of Human
various child care programs be combined at the state Services 2016). These differences between the struc-
level and that states disperse all funds in accordance tures of the New York and Illinois programs may influ-
with CCDBG rules. States were also permitted to shift ence the attributions of responsibility that clients form
a portion of TANF funds to CCDF. The subsidy pro- regarding their bureaucratic encounters. Compared to
gram reimburses a range of provider types: licensed Illinois clients, New York clients may be more likely
and license-exempted providers in centers and homes, to view caseworkers as closely aligned with the bur-
including relative caregivers in many states and eaucratic system given that New York caseworkers are
territories. county government employees and services are admin-
As with TANF, states and territories were given con- istered out of local government welfare offices. We
siderable latitude over the design and administrative reason that this may increase the likelihood that clients
structure of their CCDF programs (Blau 2003). For view caseworkers as government agents, reducing the
example, the federal government allows eligibility to chance that New York clients will excuse caseworkers
extend up to a maximum of 85% of the state median for negative experiences. In contrast, to the extent that
income (SMI), although states can and do adopt income Illinois subsidy clients are aware that the resource and
eligibility limits lower than the federal maximum— referral agency administering the program is distinct
between 40% and 65% of SMI (or between 125% and from the government and does not dictate program
200% of federal poverty line, FPL) (Matthews et al. rules, funding, and application procedures, they may
2015). At the time of the current study, income eligi- be less likely to hold workers accountable for nega-
bility was 200% FPL in the two New York sites and tive encounters. Moreover, because Illinois CCR&Rs
185% FPL in the two Illinois sites under investigation. do not confine their service to the administration of
In most states, including New York and Illinois, par- means-tested programs—they also assist families
ents are required to make a financial contribution in across income groups in finding child care—Illinois

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
6 Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx

clients may view the agency as less stigmatizing than of this study, took place an average of 22 months after
do New York clients, who must claim benefits from families entered the child care assistance program.
county welfare offices. In this scenario, Illinois sub-
sidy clients may be more likely to attribute negative Sample
encounters to the government bureaucracy rather than Sample selection for the qualitative component was
the resource and referral agency or the caseworker. purposive. Our goal was not to achieve a representa-
The two New York counties and the southwestern tive sample of survey respondents but rather to iden-
Illinois region use a caseworker model to administer tify a sample that was diverse on key demographic,
the program. Caseworkers are assigned a caseload of employment, and program characteristics. Interview
families and are responsible for overseeing and pro- participants represented diversity in race/ethnicity and
cessing application and recertification paper work and immigrant status, employment characteristics, family
any changes to the family’s case. In contrast, in Cook size, and child care setting (e.g., centers, licensed family
County, the large urban site in northern Illinois, a case- child care homes, and legally exempt relatives and
worker model is not used. Rather, subsidy eligibility nonrelative providers.) Participants also displayed di-
workers in this region serve clients across the system, verse subsidy trajectories: some stayed in the program
handling in a first-come, first-serve fashion clients’ continuously, some exited but returned within a few
application and redetermination paper work and all months, and some exited without returning during the
other client contacts with the agency. Thus, we might study period. This sample diversity provides a range of
expect that New York and southwestern Illinois clients bureaucratic encounters for us to examine. (See sample
have more opportunity to develop an individualized descriptives, Appendix table A1.)
relationship with a caseworker, which could result in
greater differentiation of bureaucrat from bureaucracy. Procedures
The implications of this for the formation of causal After obtaining informed consent, a pair of interviewers
attributions of caseworker responsibility are unclear. conducted in-person interviews with respondents, last-
More data about caseworker behavior and the con- ing an average of 90 min (range: 60–120 min). A $40
ditions shaping it could feed into a person-blaming cash incentive was provided. Eighty-four of the 85 inter-
scenario or, rather, help the client understand the con- views were audio-recorded and transcribed (16 were
straints that limit caseworker agency. The implications conducted in Spanish and translated into English.) One
for clients from northern Illinois who do not have a participant declined to be audio-recorded; in this case,
personal caseworker are also unclear. These clients detailed notes replaced a verbatim transcription. Field
may experience a greater number of workers overall. If notes and analysis memos were written on completion
their interactions across diverse caseworkers are over- of each interview (Ryan and Bernard 2000; Weiss 1994).
whelmingly negative, the uniformity of experience may
lead to the formation of system-level attributions for Interview Schedule
caseworker behavior. With more varied experiences, The semi-structured interview protocol covered a
however, they may conclude that caseworkers have range of topics including child care subsidy, employ-
agency to shape encounter quality and attribute re- ment, and child care use patterns and the challenges
sponsibility—whether encounters are positive or nega- participants faced using the subsidy program. In add-
tive—to the caseworker. ition, the interviews included information on partici-
pants’ experiences with applying for and recertifying
subsidy eligibility, interactions with the subsidy system
Methods and its staff, and suggestions for program improve-
Analyses are based on 85 qualitative interviews with ment. The interview schedule included suggested ques-
child care subsidy recipients in four regions. The data tions and probes, but interviewers were trained to ask
are drawn from a project entitled “Determinants of questions in a flexible and conversational fashion.
Subsidy Stability and Continuity of Child Care,” a
multi-method study of new entrants into child care Analysis
assistance in 2011–2012 who used the subsidy for at We followed an interpretive approach to analysis in
least one child younger than age 5. The broader study which we sought to understand the significance and
includes three components: an analysis of child care meaning of subsidy interactions from the perspective of
payment records, a telephone survey of a random sam- participants (Haverland and Yanow 2012; Schwartz-
ple of recipients, and a qualitative interview compo- Shea and Yanow 2013). The analytic approach was
nent that included a subset of survey respondents. The both deductive—informed by literature on adminis-
85 interviews (46 in Illinois and 39 in New York) that trative burden, street-level bureaucracy, and attribu-
make up the qualitative component, which is the focus tion theory—and inductive—allowing for emergent

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx 7

concepts from interview text to inform the analytic they were gleaned from interviews with clients. It is of
story (Haverland and Yanow 2012). All transcripts, course impossible to know whether or not a particular
memos, and field notes were analyzed using a qualita- client view is a valid interpretation of what transpired
tive software package, NVIVO-10. We started the ana- or if the source of a client’s attribution is or is not ac-
lysis process with a line-by-line reading of transcripts curate. Such is not the goal of the analysis as we aimed
to which we applied a priori codes reflecting key study to understand the interpretive process of a client and
topics and drawing from existing research to initially not the actual motives or behaviors of a caseworker.
organize the data. We then allowed additional catego-
ries to emerge through an in vivo coding process and
Results
refined them through an iterative comparison of cli-
ent responses (Miles and Huberman 1994; Ryan and We organized our study findings around three key re-
Bernard 2000). We double-coded every fifth interview; search questions: What is the overall valance of clients’
reliability was over 90%. We constructed unit-by-code evaluations of their experiences (positive, negative,
matrices to help identify patterns across participants or neutral)? Do clients distinguish bureaucrats from
and across evaluation types (Ryan and Bernard 2000). bureaucracies in making these assessments? What
The analysis included an examination of respond- causal explanations (attributions of control) do clients
ents’ evaluations of the subsidy program and their provide for the basis of their evaluations? Throughout,
experiences with it, their descriptions of application we highlight site differences and differences by partici-
and recertification processes, caseworker interactions, pant racial/ethnic characteristics.
and recommendations for improving the subsidy pro-
gram. We categorized respondents’ assessments of sub- Valence of Bureaucrat Encounters
sidy staff and encounters by their negative and positive Onerous claiming processes are prevalent in our
valences. For each interview, we assessed whether it study—63 of the 85 subsidy clients interviewed (73%)
was possible to determine if the negative, positive, reported negative encounters. Interviews indicated that
or neutral evaluation was made in regard to the sub- most respondents from Westchester (n = 22) and all
sidy staff person (bureaucrat) or the subsidy program/ of the subsidy clients interviewed in Nassau (n = 17)
system (bureaucracy) or both. reported negative experiences with the subsidy pro-
We then examined these evaluations further to gram. In contrast, respondents from our Illinois sites
assess whether the evaluation revealed a causal ex- reported more varied valences of subsidy experiences.
planation for the experience. Attributions of “bur- Of the Cook County respondents who reported contact
eaucrat control” were coded when respondents made with the subsidy office, 6 reported positive experiences
claims suggesting that subsidy staff had control over (22%) and 20 (74.1%) reported negative experiences
the quality of encounters with clients. Attributions of with the subsidy program.1 In southwestern Illinois,
“system control” reflected statements suggesting that half of the respondents who reported contact with
the office, agency, or policy affected the quality of the program (n = 8) reported positive experiences.
subsidy encounters. For example, we coded instances Negative experiences were the norm among respond-
when respondents described workers as hard to reach ents regardless of racial/ethnic identity. However,
because of heavy caseloads as system control because African American respondents2 were even more likely
the participant was expressing a view that the system than Latino and White respondents to report negative
was influencing how staff members were able to re- experiences. Of the 41 African Americans interviewed,
spond to client phone calls. In contrast, attributions 33 (80%) reported negative subsidy experiences. Only
were coded as bureaucrat control when negative expe- two reported positive experiences; the remaining six
riences were understood to be the intent of subsidy African Americans reported limited or no contact with
staff members. For example, if in explaining that she the subsidy office. Sixteen of the 24 Latino respond-
had difficulties reaching agency workers, a respondent ents (66%) reported negative experiences, 5 reported
remarked that her worker (or workers generally) inten- positive experiences, and 3 reported limited or no con-
tionally avoided phone calls, her response was coded tact with subsidy workers. Most White clients (n = 12,
as bureaucrat control. Categories on controllability 66.6%) also reported negative subsidy experiences,
were nearly mutually exclusive—only five interviews
included a mix of bureaucrat and system attributions.
However, these responses were still disproportionally
one or the other, and coded according to the most
1 Nine respondents reported limited or no contact with the subsidy
dominant attribution. office.
We have reported all evaluations and attributions of 2 Two respondents who are counted as African American identified as
control as the perceptions of the respondents because mixed race.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
8 Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx

only two reported positive experiences, and three views of the system, noting the complex claiming pro-
reported limited or no contact. cesses and glitches in the administration of the subsidy
When asked to assess their experiences, clients used program. Of the 55 respondents who reported nega-
three evaluative criteria that aligned with Moynihan tive experiences with subsidy bureaucrats, 21 posi-
et al.’s concept of compliance and psychological costs. tively characterized the subsidy system, for example, as
On compliance costs, our findings support previous a “huge help,” and 34 negatively described the system,
studies that find lengthy application processes and for example, as under resourced and cumbersome to
long waits in welfare offices (Auyero 2012; Hays 2003; navigate.
Soss 1999a). Clients evaluated their experiences by the These results provide support for the claim of
timeliness of the bureaucratic processes and the acces- some past authors (e.g., Soss 1999a; Watkins-Hayes
sibility of caseworkers. They complained of late pro- 2011) that clients hold differentiated views of subsidy
vider payments and delayed eligibility decisions, which encounters. But beyond that, the analyses that follow
respondents viewed as 30 days to several months fol- suggest that this two-dimensional system of evalu-
lowing an application submission. Accessibility con- ation does not by itself reflect clients’ attributions of
cerns referred to clients’ challenges with contacting responsibility. As we show, holding a negative view of
the benefits office—whether a personal caseworker or the subsidy bureaucracy or a subsidy caseworker does
another staff person—which could slow the claiming not necessarily indicate that a client holds the system
process. Without the help of office personnel, paper or a particular worker accountable for the negative
work could be incomplete or completed incorrectly. On experience. In fact, we observed distinct variation in
psychological costs, caseworker demeanor—whether how study participants view bureaucrat control over
workers were polite and helpful—emerged as a theme encounters—whether experiences are the responsi-
in client evaluations. Negative comments described bility of the worker or the system—even when clients
workers as “rude” and unwilling to answer questions. share similar affective evaluations of the encounter it-
We elaborate on these criteria further in our discussion self. Drawing on insights from attribution theory, we
of the main study results. next explore this variation in how clients use percep-
tions of “controllability” to interpret their encounters
Do Clients Distinguish Bureaucrats from with the subsidy program.
Bureaucracies?
A few studies of AFDC and TANF clients suggest that
clients differentiate bureaucracies from bureaucrats Who Is Responsible? The Bureaucrat or the
in their evaluations (e.g., Soss 1999a; Watkins-Hayes Bureaucracy?
2011). We also found this to be the case for the majority Responsibility for Positive Encounters
of study respondents (Appendix, table A2). Twelve of The subgroup of respondents who reported positive
the 85 respondents did not report a sufficient degree or neutral interactions with subsidy staff (n = 11) was
of interaction with the subsidy office to allow classifi- exclusively in Illinois (and primarily in southwest-
cation; but for 73 respondents, we were able to clas- ern Illinois). This small group of respondents gen-
sify them into one of four groups. The table illustrates erally expressed satisfaction with workers and the
that almost half (46%, n = 34) of these 73 respondents subsidy system, describing the claiming processes as
reported negative views of both the system (e.g., bur- “straightforward” and workers as responsive, “fast,”
eaucracy) and its workers (e.g., bureaucrats) and 15% “friendly,” and “helpful.” When attributional views
(n = 11) reported positive or neutral views of both. emerged, respondents gave credit to the worker for
Twenty-nine percent (n = 21) of these 73 respondents the positive experience. For example, Sarah described
evaluated the worker negatively but held benign or the claiming process as “easy” and attributed her
positive views of the system, whereas only 10% (n = 7) experience to a worker who was “just really good.”
held negative views of the system but positive or neu- She explains,
tral views of their worker. Just, my personal caseworker—I know other
Thus, our qualitative interviews suggest that even people don’t have it that good, they say—but
when respondents hold similar views of bureaucrats, my personal caseworker was awesome. Because
they nevertheless can vary in their views of the bureau- I had never done it and she knew what situation
cracy. Likewise, when they share views of the bureau- I was in. And she gave me a list of people. I mean,
cracy, they may deviate in their views of bureaucrats. she was just really, really good.
For example, 18 respondents reported positive or
neutral views of their workers, but only 11 of these Unlike Sarah, it was difficult to discern respondents’
commented positively or were neutral about the sub- causal attributions for positive encounters in the ma-
sidy system. The other seven reported having negative jority of cases.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx 9

Responsibility for Negative Encounters Constrained bureaucrat attributions were somewhat


Respondents’ descriptions of their negative encounters more prominent among Whites than among African
almost always included an attribution of responsi- Americans and Latinos. Whereas 66% (8 of 12) of
bility. We identified three types of attributions which White respondents who reported a negative experience
we label “constrained bureaucrats,” “autonomous also reported viewing workers as constrained by the
bureaucrats,” and “hostile bureaucracy.” Appendix system, less than half of African American (46.8%, 15
table A3 provides a distribution of study participants of 32) and Latino (43.8%, 7 of 16) respondents with
across these attributional types by study site and race/ negative evaluations ascribed responsibility for their
ethnicity. negative encounters to the system.
Some respondents viewed workers as the “con- The participants with constrained bureaucrat
strained bureaucrats” described by the street-level attributions excused caseworkers for their negative
bureaucracy framework (Lipsky 1980), in which the encounters and blamed poor experiences on large
subsidy system’s resource deficits hampered bureau- caseloads, limited staff, and limited resources. For ex-
crats’ abilities to offer high-quality service experiences. ample, Danielle, from one of the New York counties,
In these cases, the experience with the caseworker was had difficulty reaching her caseworker and described
negative but was not perceived as within her control, the staff as “impossible to get in touch with,” dismis-
or as her fault. sive, and “harsh on help.” However, Danielle did not
Other respondents adopted an “autonomous bur- blame the caseworker. Rather, workers were unavail-
eaucrat” attribution and emphasized that workers do able and “not nice” because they were “overworked
have discretionary authority to provide prompt and and underpaid”:
pleasant service experiences. Rather than constrain-
ing implementation, these clients viewed the subsidy They’re very impossible … if paper work needs to
system as establishing procedures that allowed bureau- go through, or if it’s not on the computer, they’re
crats to produce successful claims. They viewed the very—They are very … harsh on help. They don’t
system as defining clear professional roles for workers really get back to you. They’re not nice. They’re
and sufficient resources to fulfill these roles. Within this just very overworked and underpaid, and they
frame, clients reasoned that unsuccessful claims must have too many cases.
be a product of workers’ deliberate negligence. These Danielle further attributed caseworker behavior to
study participants seemed to believe that bureaucrats county-level budget cuts. Because of these cuts, the
control the claiming process in how they choose to subsidy office was short staffed, which decreased
fulfill their professional roles. When workers use their worker accessibility and further delayed the applica-
discretionary authority in ways that obstruct claims, tion process:
negative encounters result.
Study participants who adopted a “hostile bureau- Just like the county needs more workers. Because
cracy” attribution described the subsidy system as inten- I think that’s what a lot of the problem is …. Like
tionally discouraging claims. Not only was the system I think that’s why it took so long in the begin-
responsible for their negative experience with the claiming ning. Because if I remember correctly, when we
process, but according to this view, it was also designed to started, that’s when like the county did all the
be difficult as a means of deterring program use. lay-offs and like half the building was termi-
nated. But, other than that, I mean, considering
that there’s not that many people working in that
Constrained Bureaucrat
department, like, I understand that’s why it takes
Of the 63 respondents who reported negative expe- so long to get stuff done, but I mean, yeah. That
riences with either the subsidy worker or the system, would be the only thing. Because I’m sure the
nearly half (n = 32) attributed these experiences to caseworkers are stressed out about getting every-
external system-level factors they considered beyond thing done on time too. And I think that’s why
the worker’s control. The worker was a “constrained they don’t return phone calls because there’s just
bureaucrat” with minimal power to improve upon not enough hours in the day to do everything.
a faulty system. Twenty-six of the 32 participants But, like I said, I’m still waiting for them to call
reported negative experiences with both the subsidy me back. It’s been like two weeks.
program and subsidy staff. Most attributions that fit
the constrained bureaucrat category were in the New Stephanie, a southwestern Illinois subsidy client, also
York counties (n = 18, 56%), but this was the most attributed poor caseworker interactions to system fac-
common attribution for negative experiences in Cook tors. She waited 30 days for an eligibility decision from
county as well (12 out of 18 respondents). her worker, but Stephanie did not view her worker as

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
10 Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx

intentionally delaying her application. Instead, she Brittany’s recommendations to improve the pro-
understood the delay to be due to a heavy caseload: gram also reflect her view that the subsidy bureau-
cracy influences service quality. To shorten lengthy
She, the lady who handles it—I believe her name
office waits, the system should increase the capacity
is [Caseworker’s name]—and she’s just so over-
of workers to see clients by adding more offices—
loaded. She I mean, I think one time she said she
what she calls “centers.” She explains, “I probably
had like 1200 cases on her desk or something like
say more centers or bigger centers …. Yeah, so prob-
that …. And that’s what took so long.
ably either more centers or … maybe more people to
Stephanie also reported that large caseloads prevented help out.”
her caseworker from answering and returning phone
calls. After faxing her application to the subsidy office, Autonomous Bureaucrat
Stephanie called her caseworker “nonstop” to make A third (n = 19) of the 63 respondents who reported
sure the office processed her application. But reaching negative subsidy encounters attributed responsibil-
her caseworker was like trying to find a “needle in a ity to the subsidy staff. We categorized these 19 par-
haystack”: ticipants as having an attribution of “autonomous
bureaucrat” because rather than viewing workers
You had to fax it, or you could mail it, which as constrained agents, they reported that workers
would take forever …. So, good old library here had the ability to exercise significant control over
has a fax machine. [Laughter] …. And then called service delivery. When clients did not receive polite
nonstop … was like a needle in a haystack trying and prompt assistance, subsidy staff members were
to get a hold of her. It’s either you get her voice- understood to be at fault. Indeed, many participants
mail and you can leave a message, out of all the in this group said workers purposely complicated
thousands of messages she’s already got on there. claiming processes by losing paper work and ignoring
Or her answering machine was full, and you just phone calls.
had to keep calling back. So it was like one of Fifteen of these 19 respondents were from the two
those things that it was very frustrating. There New York counties, Westchester (n = 8) and Nassau
was people before me, so I have to just basically (n = 7). In most cases (14 of 19), these participants
wait your turn. Which is understandable. It’s just positively evaluated the subsidy system but reported
like, well, I kind of need it now. negative experiences with workers. Only four respond-
Although frustrated, Stephanie explained her negative ents in this group negatively evaluated both the subsidy
program and the subsidy staff. Across the four sites,
experiences by emphasizing the high demand for ser-
very few White respondents (n = 2) attributed negative
vices and the limited control that the caseworker had
program experiences to caseworkers, whereas Latino
over the process. She must “wait her turn” because of
and African American respondents disproportionately
the pressing needs of other clients.
held this perspective. Seventeen of the 19 respondents
In 4 of these 32 cases, clients actually reported
reporting autonomous attributions were Latino (n = 7)
positive experiences with subsidy staff despite nega-
or African American (n = 10). Within the subgroups
tive assessments of the subsidy system (e.g., positive
of African American and Latino respondents, the
worker/negative bureaucracy). These clients com-
autonomous bureaucrat attribution was also common.
mented on the professionalism of workers despite
Among the 32 African American respondents report-
their limited power to respond to system problems.
ing negative experiences, 10 (31%) viewed workers as
Brittany’s reflections on a visit to a Cook County sub-
responsible. For Latino clients, 7 of the 16 (43.7%) cli-
sidy office illustrate this perspective. She waited for
ents who reported negative subsidy experiences made
3 h to receive help with the application, attributing
this attribution.
the delay to crowded office conditions. She expressed
Gabriela’s comments illustrate the autonomous bur-
satisfaction with subsidy workers, noting their calm
eaucrat perspective. When asked to describe her expe-
and helpful demeanor:
riences with the CCR&R agency in Cook County, she
They helped. No attitudes. They was pretty, remarked, “it’s a wonderful program, but I’m going to
I would think, you know, since there were so tell you, they have terrible personnel there.” Gabriela
many people there … they didn’t get … into it visited the office after calling several times about a re-
with one another and the employees kept their cent denial letter. She believed that the inaccessible and
composure. Nobody was acting upset or attitude- dismissive behavior of staff was to blame for her long
y …. Nobody was upset. They were pretty cool. and unproductive office visit:

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx 11

They’re terrible. You walk in. I had to go bring described the subsidy system as having rules and pro-
some papers because I wasn’t … I walk in there cedures that guide claiming processes and stated that
taking the papers and I’m waiting in line. Of her worker’s negligence warranted additional account-
course, she’s on the phone and I’m waiting and ability. She recommended that caseworkers should face
standing there. Then suddenly she’s on the phone performance reviews that evaluate the promptness of
because she’s playing cards. She’s playing 21 on their responses and eligibility decisions. As it stands,
the computer. And I’m waiting and waiting and she believed that workers face few consequences for
she says, “What can I do for you? Do you have poor service delivery and do not “make the effort” to
an appointment?” I said, “No. I’m supposed to help clients:
come and bring these forms.” “Do you have cop-
Like the caseworkers have to have … they have
ies of them?” I said, “I did make copies just to
a deadline of having something in by the end of
keep.” Yeah, “Because we don’t do copies here.”
a month. And then once that month is over, then
I said, “yes, I do.” “Then why are you here? You
it’s having something else. Instead of that, I think
drop that in the mailbox that’s downstairs.” I’m
it would work better if the whole process has to
serious. I said, “Well, thank you for your help.”
be completed in a month, or it’s your butt on the
And I walk away. But she was playing 21 on that
line, as the caseworker. Because that’s your job.
computer. Okay, seriously.
And, you know, they would actually make the
According to Gabriela, crowded office conditions were effort of picking up the phone instead of send-
not the reason for her extended wait. Rather, the case- ing a piece of mail or waiting three weeks to do
worker’s behavior contributed to the delay. She illus- that. Or make an appointment for the person to
trated this point in her emphasis on the worker’s choice come in.
to play a computer game instead of providing attentive
Clients such as Gina viewed the subsidy system as part
service. From her perspective, the worker chose to be
of a bureaucratic system that was designed to help.
unhelpful although it was in her power to help.
Yet, bureaucrats could jeopardize successful claims by
Gina’s comments about her southwestern Illinois
failing to fulfill their professional responsibilities to
caseworker also demonstrated her view of an autono-
clients.
mous bureaucrat with some control over the claim-
Similarly, Marcus from Westchester County under-
ing process. She viewed the subsidy system as setting
stands his negative experiences to be a result of dif-
guidelines for timely processing of client paper work
ficult caseworkers who intentionally burden the
and blamed her caseworker for delaying her subsidy
client throughout the claiming process. As a client of
claim. According to Gina, her worker complicated the
the TANF program, Marcus interacts with both the
application process, was distrusting, and “tried to pick
Department of Social Services TANF unit and the child
apart everything.” Gina’s worker required her to ex-
care subsidy unit. He describes his interactions with
tensively verify household composition, income, and
child care subsidy staff as a “headache.” Workers in the
work schedules and often requested these documents
subsidy office are “nasty” and “make it more difficult”
at the last minute. She described how her worker’s be-
for him. Marcus suspects that the taxing application
havior delayed her eligibility by 3 months:
process and troublesome child care subsidy staff reflect
Like, the application wasn’t complicated, but the the deliberate efforts of subsidy staff to discourage
woman, my caseworker was impossible to get a successful claims. He draws this conclusion by con-
hold of. And she tried to pick apart everything trasting what he experiences as the helpful behavior
I said. And when I said, because she asked if, who of a county supervisor in the child care subsidy pro-
the father was and I said, “Oh, that’s my fiancé,” gram to the frontline caseworkers at the local office.
and she said, “Oh, well, you have to …. So you’re To Marcus, the workers at the local office are unham-
living together?” I said, “Well, not right now, pered by broader organization and policy constraints.
no.” And then she made me actually verify, have The responsiveness of the county subsidy commis-
like three forms of where I lived, prove where sioner—despite his extensive duties—proves this point
I lived, and then come up with three forms to for Marcus. He reasons that caseworkers have fewer
prove where he lived. And then after that she like responsibilities than the county supervisor, yet they de-
waited until the deadline. So this is three months liberately choose not to return his phone calls:
later.
You can call … the head dude, head guy …. He
In addition to the worker’s lengthening the applica- gets it done when you call him. Because they’ll
tion process, Gina also emphasized her unwilling- do little slick stuff, lie about certain stuff …
ness to meet deadlines set by the subsidy system. She You got a fire under their butt, then you—Or,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
12 Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx

they just give you a runaround and you know Hostile Bureaucracy
they lying …. Just making me do this, I guess, to Four of the 63 clients who reported negative experi-
deter people from staying on it or whatever, or ences with the subsidy program described a system
being on it …. Now, this guy, the head dude, if that was designed to intentionally discourage claims—
you call him in the morning, by the afternoon, a category of attributions that we distinguish from
he calls you back, and he’s running the whole both the autonomous bureaucrat perspective and the
thing, so if he has time to take out to call you constrained bureaucrat view. These clients described a
back, why can’t you? And, he’s the big—So, system that was under-resourced, complicated, and/or
I know there are plenty of complaints, paper inefficient (similar to the constrained bureaucrat cat-
work he got to do … I don’t understand how egory), but they went beyond this to argue that the
this guy … how he has time to call you back, system was deliberately set up to fail them. These four
check his messages, call you back, and take care respondents evaluated both the subsidy program and
of the problem? …. And, you all can’t do it? It its workers negatively; three of the four were from
can’t be that busy. New York. Marina, a Westchester County respond-
ent, viewed workers as constrained by large caseloads
Central to Marcus’s critical rationale is the belief that and excused inaccessible staff, although she reported
caseworkers have the power to choose how to manage her experience as negative—“they’re overwhelmed,
their professional responsibilities and subsequent ser- maybe they have too many people and can’t do it
vice experience for client. He elaborates on this author- with everyone …. But when I call they never pick up.”
ity when he distinguishes the quality of caseworkers by Importantly, Marina went further in her explanation
their willingness to “do [their] job.” Marcus believes of responsibility than respondents in the constrained
that “nasty” or “crazy” caseworkers “don’t want to do bureaucrat category. She claimed that the program
nothing.” These workers opt to underperform and evade used deliberate strategies to deter claims. As she put it,
work tasks. In contrast, a “nice caseworker” wants to the subsidy program “wants you to lose the services.”
“do her job.” Nice caseworkers choose to fulfill pro- Marina ultimately compared her subsidy experiences
fessional responsibilities, which for Marcus means negatively against those for other means-tested pro-
responding to client concerns with polite and helpful grams. She reported that workers from the Food Stamps
assistance. and Medicaid programs assisted clients throughout the
Not all autonomous bureaucrat attributions are application process. Workers from these programs also
negative. In rare instances, clients who reported posi- called to remind clients about recertification. In contrast,
tive experiences with workers emphasized a worker’s she commented that subsidy workers requested paper
capacity to control the quality of the experience—des- work at the last minute, behavior Marina viewed as the
pite a cumbersome bureaucracy. While recognizing the system’s way of discouraging claims. She explained:
flaws of the subsidy system, these clients also consid-
ered workers as having the power to intervene to make When you go to recertify the health insurance, it’s
a poor system function better. For instance, Sabrina, really good, now, I have to, a lot, because since
a Cook County respondent, described the application I get Food stamps, I get help from Child Care, the
process as a complicated “paper trail” and attributed difference is huge, and health insurance. Health
the delay in an initial eligibility decision to the exten- insurance and food stamps are hard too; it’s dif-
sive paper work required for the application. Sabrina ficult but health insurance is easy because if you
communicated with the subsidy office to confirm the get confused or you forget something, they call;
receipt of her application materials. When she asked they say look, can I put this here, and I say yes,
staff about the reason for the delay, she found they of course or whatever question, come to certain
were helpful and prompt: place, we will personally help you with the paper
work. They’re always willing to help; they always
I just called over the phone and told them that want to help. They don’t want you to lose the
we had sent the work in, you know; see if they support. Child Care Subsidy doesn’t want you to
had got it that second time. [Interviewer 1: Okay. have help. That’s how I feel. They want you to
And how was your experience talking to them? lose the services, because there is a lot of time to
Were they helpful?] Yeah. They basically told fill out the paper work and to wait until the last
me, yeah, they had got it that time and they were week to tell me something is missing. I`m talking
submitting everything; they would get back to us, about always, there is always a problem, so, to be
you know? And they pretty much got back to us more like the health care, they call, they remind
in a prompt time after that. you, whatever, come to the office, we will help
you fill it out, to be more human with that.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx 13

Mandy, also a Westchester County respondent, viewed own power relative to that of agencies and workers
the subsidy system as one that discourages claims as (Hasenfeld 1987), they also develop perceptions about
well. While Marina excused workers for being in- who controls the quality of their bureaucratic encoun-
accessible, Mandy viewed frontline workers as deliber- ters—namely, the caseworker or the system. Our pur-
ately complicating claiming processes. She commented suit of clients’ causal explanations of their child care
on the difficulties of getting assistance from subsidy subsidy experiences extends the study of administrative
staff: “When you try to call and speak to somebody, burden to inform understanding of how clients’ inter-
you never get through to speak to anybody. You don’t pretation of bureaucratic encounters may influence
know the status on anything. And when you do go up claiming behavior and political participation.
there, they just are not willing to work with you or Our findings replicate previous welfare studies that
answer any questions.” Ultimately, she relied on the have described clients’ views of bureaucratic encoun-
help of a supervisor to process claims, suggesting that ters with means-tested programs as overwhelmingly
she did recognize some potential for staff agency to get negative. Amid this generally negative backdrop, some
around the hostile bureaucracy. Yet, we include Mandy study participants differentiated bureaucrats from
in the hostile bureaucracy category because she later bureaucracies, reporting positive experiences with the
elaborated her view that the subsidy system—by de- subsidy bureaucracy but negative assessments of sub-
sign—deters claims. She suggested that its complex sidy workers and vice versa. Of particular relevance to
application forms and bureaucratic processes were our argument, we also found that clients’ interpreta-
intended to intimidate prospective claimants. These tions of encounters reflected causal attributions of re-
barriers reflected, in her mind, the deterrent tactics sponsibility for a negative (or positive) experience—an
of “social services,” which have a “history of sending aspect of clients’ interpretive processes not systemat-
people back.” Consequently, clients must have “their ically explored by previous research on bureaucratic
ducks in a row” to claim benefits: encounters. Specifically, our findings suggest that even
when clients have similar evaluations of experiences,
It was a very lengthy application process, but
they vary in whom they view as responsible. In one
I prepared for it. [Interviewer 1 agrees] You really
view, clients hold workers responsible—emphasizing
have to have your ducks in a row when you go
the bureaucrat’s capacity to control service quality.
down to social services, because they are able to
Bureaucrats are autonomous actors who deliberately
review the application as long as you put their
increase the costs of claiming to deter applicants. In
name on it, but that doesn’t mean that it’ll be
the other view, clients perceive workers as constrained
approved … I provided everything, so there wasn’t
bureaucrats, whose behavior toward them is influ-
a real reason not to approve the application … so-
enced by the bureaucracy’s insufficient resources.
cial services has a history of sending people back,
Such diminished resources, from this perspective, limit
sort of kind of like extending the process. It was a
bureaucrats’ capacity to provide high-quality service.
13–14-page application in and of itself. So, I just
Finally, a small group focuses on bureaucracies (not
included everything they needed, and then some.
bureaucrats) as hostile institutions that are intention-
ally set up to deter successful claims through compli-
cated and punitive administrative processes.
Conclusion Negative experiences were common regardless of
This qualitative study of client experiences with the race/ethnicity, but African Americans disproportion-
child care subsidy program begins to clarify how clients ately reported negative encounters with both the system
interpret administrative burden. We move beyond the and workers and tended to attribute responsibility to
distinction clients make between workers and bureau- the caseworker. In contrast, White respondents were
cracies to examine the causal explanations clients de- more likely to interpret their negative experiences to be
velop for burdensome claiming processes. By focusing the fault of the system—which they viewed as compli-
on how clients interpret their bureaucratic encounters cated and under resourced. In some ways, the preva-
we mean to make no judgments as to whether the lence of attributing poor experiences to caseworkers’
respondents are accurate or inaccurate in their assess- intentional behavior among minority respondents sup-
ments of who or what is truly responsible for their expe- ports findings of previous studies that suggest Black
riences. Rather, we suppose that as a predictor of future and Latino welfare clients face more punitive actions
claiming behavior, perceptions—right or wrong—are (Soss, Fording, and Schram 2011), aloof “arms-length”
likely what matter most and therefore worthy of study. interactions with bureaucrats, and although less conclu-
To explain this aspect of clients’ interpretive processes, sive instances where workers raise the costs of claims
we apply the concept of “controllability” from attri- (Maynard-Moody and Musheno 2003). Our find-
bution theory and argue that while clients assess their ing that African Americans and Latinos tend to view

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
14 Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx

negative experiences as the fault of the worker extends vis-à-vis other factors such as employment experiences,
these insights to include how clients of color interpret child care availability, or children’s needs in explaining
the valence of encounters and workers’ practices. While claiming behavior. Instead, our theory-building aims
Black and Latino respondents did not necessarily report are oriented toward generating plausible concepts
feeling discriminated against, they did disproportion- and hypotheses about clients’ interpretive processes to
ately view workers as responsible for negative experi- explore in future research (Small 2009). Our sample is
ences and intentionally increasing the costs of claiming. well suited for this purpose and allows for an analysis
Given our small and non-representative sample, we that theoretically deepens the literature and raises new
note these differences by race/ethnicity with appropriate questions about bureaucratic encounters.
caution.3 First, understanding clients’ attributions reveals
We also discuss observed site differences with cau- important insight about how clients interpret the costs
tion, not only because of sample size limitations but of claiming benefits beyond a descriptive valence of
because the four study sites are heterogeneous in ways the burden. As the marketing application of attribu-
beyond their approach to program administration. tion theory demonstrates, consumer satisfaction alone
Participants in the New York counties showed both does not predict future behavior. Rather decisions to
more negative encounters with the subsidy office and purchase a service again in part stem from consum-
were more likely to attribute responsibility to subsidy ers’ nuanced views about who controls the quality of
workers (i.e., autonomous bureaucrat attributions) the service experience. In the same way, the valence
than participants in the Illinois counties. It is unclear of claimants’ evaluations of bureaucratic encoun-
why this would be, but perhaps the CCR&R agen- ters alone may not predict future claiming behavior.
cies that administered the subsidy program in Illinois Future claims may reflect the claimants’ understanding
were understood to be distinct from the government, regarding who controls the quality of their encounters
whereas in New York, the county social service agen- with bureaucracies. We do not argue that claiming
cies and by association their workers were viewed means-tested benefits is analogous to consumer pur-
as part of the system and with limited discretionary chasing behavior. Indeed, both Lipsky’s (1980) street-
powers. It is also the case, however, that autonomous level bureaucracy framework and Hasenfeld’s (1987)
bureaucrat attributions were made more frequently power-dependency application suggest otherwise—
by participants in southwestern Illinois than in Cook material needs and limited alternatives for assistance
County. This suggests that perhaps a caseworker model can drive claims despite onerous processes (Lipsky
(which was used in southwestern Illinois as well as the 1980). However, our findings suggest future research
two New York counties to administer the subsidy pro- might examine whether and how client attributions
gram) resulted in greater familiarity—and by extension influence approaches to claiming behavior.
blaming—of a particular caseworker for negative treat- Future quantitative studies with large, representa-
ment. Because clients interacted with multiple workers tive samples can also explore the relationship between
about their case in Cook County, they may have been attributions and claiming strategies. Some studies
more apt to develop a broad appraisal of the system as suggest that the burdens and the arbitrary nature of
influencing worker behavior, especially if the majority claiming outcomes teach clients to be subordinate and
of their worker encounters were negative ones. submissive to the wills of agency workers and bureau-
Because the 85 clients interviewed were from four cracies (Auyero 2012). Still, our findings point to a
unique policy contexts—and within each context more complex interpretation of administrative burden
their selection was nonrandom—their experiences that may not lead to passivity. Whether and how clients
are not representative of subsidy clients generally or exercise agency to claim benefits may depend on who
within these four communities. Thus, generalization clients blame for negative experiences. Clients may
of findings beyond this sample would be inappropri- use one set of strategies to circumvent an autonomous
ate. Furthermore, our relatively small sample does not bureaucrat and another set of strategies to navigate a
allow us to explore how distinct attributions might complex under-resourced bureaucracy. In this sense,
relate to clients’ subsequent claiming efforts, or the attributions may help explain who remains complicit
relative importance of these attributional processes and passive and who exercises agency to secure better
quality service experiences and benefits.
3 We do not have information on the racial or ethnic characteristics of Future research may also benefit from examin-
subsidy staff. Thus, it is impossible to determine whether differences ing the features of policy administration that lead to
in client experiences vary by the race/ethnicity of their worker or distinct attributions. We find that clients are more
the match between the worker and the client. Evidence suggests,
however, that Black and Latino clients experiences negative treatment
likely to attribute negative encounters to caseworkers
regardless of the workers’ racial/ethnic identity (Soss, Fording, and in counties that use caseworker models. Studies can
Schram 2011; Watkins-Hayes 2009). formally test whether differences in intake processes

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx 15

and program administration are associated with con- Dias, Janice Johnson, and Steven Maynard-Moody. 2007. For-profit welfare:
Contracts, conflicts, and the performance paradox. Journal of Public
strained, autonomous, or hostile attributions. If attri-
Administration Research and Theory 17:189–211.
butions shape claiming efforts, these insights can direct Fiske, Susan T., and Shelley E. Taylor. 1991. Social cognition, 2nd ed. McGraw-
the focus of policy interventions to improve participa- Hill Series in Social Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company.
tion outcomes. Hasenfeld, Yeheskel. 1985. Citizens’ encounters with welfare state bureaucra-
Of further importance to political science, studies cies. The Social Service Review 59:622–35.
———. 1987. Power in social work practice. The Social Service Review
can explore how clients’ attributions of controllability
61:469–83.
affect their broader exercise of political rights. Some Hasenfeld, Yeheskel, Jane A. Rafferty, and Mayer N. Zald. 1987. The wel-
scholarship suggests that policies influence citizens’ fare state, citizenship, and bureaucratic encounters. Annual Review of
political participation (e.g., Campbell 2012; Mettler Sociology 13:387–415.
and Soss 2004; Pierson 1993). Citizens infer their Haverland, Markus, and Dvora Yanow. 2012. A hitchhiker’s guide to the pub-
political standing and the government’s responsive- lic administration research universe: Surviving conversations on method-
ologies and methods. Public Administration Review 72:401–8.
ness from their experiences with public bureaucracies Hays, Sharon. 2003. Flat broke with children: Women in the age of welfare
(Schneider and Ingram 1993; Soss 1999b). For low- reform. New York: Oxford Univ. Press.
income citizens, the design of welfare programs teaches Heider, Fritz. 1958. The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York:
recipients negative lessons about the government that Wiley.
dampen political participation (Bruch, Ferree, and Illinois Department of Human Services. 2016. What is IDHS CCAP? www.
chicagomanualofstyle.org/tools_citationguide.html (accessed November
Soss 2010; Soss 1999b). Client attributions regarding
8, 2016).
the quality of their experiences may offer interpretive Jones, Edward E., and Keith E. Davis. 1965. From acts to dispositions: The
causal mechanisms to explain the relationship between attribution process in person perception. Advances in Experimental Social
program experiences and clients’ political behavior. Psychology 2:219–66.
Clients who view workers as responsible for negative Kahn, Robert L., Daniel Katz, and Barbara Gutek. 1976. Bureaucratic encoun-
ters—an evaluation of government services. The Journal of Applied
experiences may draw different conclusions about the
Behavioral Science 12:178–98.
government from those who attribute burdensome Kelley, Harold H. 1967. Attribution theory in social psychology. In Nebraska
experiences to bureaucracies. Future research can take Symposium on motivation, ed. D. Levine. Lincoln: Univ.of Nebraska Press.
up this hypothesis. Lipsky, Michael. 1980. Street-level bureaucrats. New York: Russell Sage
Foundation.
Lynch, Karen E. 2014. The child care and development block grant: Background
References and funding. Congressional Research Service Report RL30785. http://
Adams, Gina, Kathleen Snyder, and Jodi Sandfort. 2002. Getting and retaining greenbook.waysandmeans.house.gov/sites/greenbook.waysandmeans.
child care assistance: How policy and practice influence parents’ experi- house.gov/files/RL30785_gb.pdf (accessed September 29, 2016).
ences. Accessing the New Federalism 55:1–119. Matthews, Hannah, Karen Schulman, Julie Vogtman, Christine Johnson-
Auyero, Javier. 2012. Patients of the state: The politics of waiting in Argentina. Staub, and Helen Blank. 2015. Implementing the child care and develop-
Durham, NC: Duke Univ. Press. ment block grant reauthorization: A guide for states. Center for Law and
Blau, David. 2003. Child care subsidy programs. In Means-tested transfer pro- Social Policy. www.clasp.org/resources-and-publications/publication-1/
grams in the United States, ed. Robert Moffitt, 443–516. Chicago: Univ. ccdbg-guide-for-state-final.pdf
of Chicago Press. Maynard-Moody, Steven Williams, and Michael Craig Musheno. 2003. Cops,
Brodkin, Evelyn Z., Carolyn Fuqua, and Elaine Waxman. 2005. Accessing teachers, counselors: Stories from the front lines of public service. Ann
the safety net: Administrative barriers to public benefits in metropolitan Arbor: Univ. of Michigan Press.
Chicago. Chicago: Sargent Shriver Center on Poverty Law. Mettler, Suzanne, and Joe Soss. 2004. The consequences of public policy
Brodkin, Evelyn Z., and Malay Majmundar. 2010. Administrative exclusion: for democratic citizenship: Bridging policy studies and mass politics.
Organizations and the hidden costs of welfare claiming. Journal of Public Perspectives on Politics 2:55–73.
Administration Research and Theory 20:827–48. Miles, Matthew B., and A. Michael Huberman. 1994. Qualitative data ana-
Bruch, Sarah, Myra M. Ferree, and Joe Soss. 2010. From policy to polity: lysis: An expanded sourcebook. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Democracy, paternalism and the incorporation of disadvantaged citizens. Moynihan, Donald, Pamela Herd, and Hope Harvey. 2014. Administrative
American Sociological Review 75:205–26. burden: Learning, psychological, and compliance costs in citizen-state
Burden, Barry, David Canon, Kenneth Mayer, and Donald Moynihan. 2012. interactions. Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory
The effect of administrative burden on bureaucratic perception of poli- 25:43–69.
cies: Evidence from election administration. Public Administration Review Pierson, Paul. 1993. When effect becomes cause: Policy feedback and political
72:741–51. change. World Politics 45:595–628.
Campbell, Andrea L. 2012. Policy makes mass politics. Annual Review of Professional Development Program. 2013. Child care subsidy training agenda.
Political Science 15:333–51. Albany: Rockefeller College.
———. 2015. Basics of qualitative research. Washington, DC: Sage Ryan, Gery W., and H. Russel Bernard. 2000. Data management and ana-
Publications. lysis methods. In Handbook of qualitative research, 2nd ed., eds. Norman
Danziger, Sandra K. 2010. The decline of cash welfare and implications for Densin and Yvonna Lincoln, 769–802. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
social policy and poverty. Annual Review of Sociology 36:523–45. Publications.
Davis, Elizabeth E., Caroline Krafft, and Nicole D. Forry. 2016. The role of Schneider, Anne, and Helen Ingram. 1993. Social construction of target popu-
policy and practice in short spells of child care subsidy participation. lations: Implications for politics and policy. American Political Science
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 20:1–20. Review 87:334–47.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
16 Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx

Schram, Sanford F., Joe Soss, Richard C. Fording, and Linda Houser. 2009. U. S. Department of Health and Human Services. 2015. Characteristics of
Deciding to discipline: Race, choice, and punishment at the frontlines of families served by child care and development fund (CCDF) based on
welfare reform. American Sociological Review 74:398–422. preliminary FY 2014 data. Washington, DC: Office of Child Care, U.S.
Schwartz-Shea, Peregrine, and Dvora Yanow. 2013. Interpretive research Department of Health and Human Services. www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/
design: Concepts and processes. New York: Routledge. occ/resource/characteristics-of-families-served-by-child-care-and-develop-
Small, Mario Luis. 2009. How many cases do I need: On science and the logic ment-fund-ccdf (accessed September 29, 2016).
of case selection in field-based research. Ethnography 10:5–38. Watkins-Hayes, Celeste. 2009. The new welfare bureaucrats: Entanglements
Snyder, Kathleen, Sara Bernstein, and Robin Koralek. 2006. Parents’ per- of race, class, and policy reform. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press.
spectives on child care subsidies and moving from welfare to work. ———. 2011. Race, respect, and red tape: Inside the black box of racially rep-
Washington, DC: The Urban Institute. resentative bureaucracies. Journal of Public Administration Research and
Soss, Joe. 1999a. Welfare application encounters subordination, satisfaction, Theory 21:i233–51.
and the puzzle of client evaluations. Administration & Society 31:50–94. Weiner, Bernard. 1985. An attributional theory of achievement motivation and
———. 1999b. Lessons of welfare: Policy design, political learning, and polit- emotion. Psychological Review 92:548–73.
ical action. American Political Science Review 93:363–80. ———. 2000. Attributional thoughts about consumer behavior. Journal of
———. 2000. Unwanted claims. Ann Arbor: Univ. of Michigan Press. Consumer Research 27:382–87.
———. 2005. Making clients and citizens: Welfare policy as a source of status, Weiss, Robert S. 1994. Learning from strangers: The art and method of quali-
belief, and action. In Deserving and entitled: Social constructions and pub- tative interview studies. New York: The Free Press
lic policy, eds. Anne L. Schneider and Helen M. Ingram, 291–328. Albany: Ybarra, Marci A. 2011. Should I stay or should I go? Why welfare applicants
State Univ. of New York Press. leave the extended welfare application process. Journal of Sociology and
Soss, Joe, Richard Fording, and Sanford F. Schram. 2011. The organization of Social Welfare 38:55–76.
discipline: From performance management to perversity and punishment.
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 21:i203–32.

Appendix

Table A1. Characteristics of Qualitative Sample (n = 85)

Race/ethnicity
Black 39 45.9%
White 18 21.2%
Latino 26 30.6%
County
Nassau 18 21.1%
Westchester 21 24.5%
Cook County 35 41.1%
Southwestern Illinois 11 12.9%
Works at nonstandard times 56 65.9%
Two or more jobs since program entry 51 60.0%
Immigrant 23 27.1%
Interviewed in Spanish 16 18.8%
Multiple subsidized children at program entry 34 40.0%
Type of subsidized provider at program entrya
Center-based care 44 51.8%
Family child care 29 34.1%
Legally exempt relative care 13 15.3%
Legally exempt nonrelative care 6 7.1%
a
Six respondents reported using multiple subsidized providers at program entry. Total N of subsidized providers is 92.

Table A2. Subsidy Evaluations by Subsidy Bureaucracy and Bureaucrats (N = 85)

Positive/Neutral Bureaucracy Negative Bureaucracy Total

Positive/neutral bureaucrat 11 7 18
Negative bureaucrat 21 34 55
Total 32 41 73

Note: Twelve clients reported limited interactions with the subsidy office or workers; responses not classified on table.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018
Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 2018, Vol. xx, No. xx 17

Table A3. Attributions by Site and Race

Autonomous (n = 19) Constrained (n = 32) Hostile (n = 4) None (n = 8)

Race
Black 10 15 2 5
White 2 8 1 1
Latino 7 7 1 1
Other 1
Westchester 8 10 2 2
Black 2 6 1 2
White 1 1
Latino 5 3 1
Other
Nassau 7 8 1 1
Black 6 2 1 1
White 1 3
Latino 2
Other 1
Cook county 3 12 1 4
Black 2 6 3
White 0 3 1 1
Latino 1 3
Other
Southwest IL 1 2 0 1
Black 1
Latino
White 1 1 1
Other
Total 19 32 4 8

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpart/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jopart/muy008/4885362


by University of Durham user
on 24 February 2018

You might also like