Burnout Artigo

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Studies in Educational Evaluation 60 (2019) 199–204

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Studies in Educational Evaluation


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/stueduc

Relations between academic performance, student engagement and student T


burnout: A cross-lagged analysis of a two-wave study

Ramona Paloș, Laurenţiu P. Maricuţoiu , Iuliana Costea
Department of Psychology, West University of Timișoara, Romania

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Academic performance and student well-being are two desired outcomes of any learning and instruction process.
Academic performance The purpose of the present study was to test the temporal order of the relationships between academic per-
Student engagement formance and two forms of student well-being (i.e., student engagement and burnout) in a specific context. 142
Student burnout psychology undergraduate students completed self-reported measures of student engagement and student
Cross-lagged relations
burnout in two waves, during a semester. Also, we included their grades from two intermediate evaluations. Path
analysis models suggested that academic grades could be considered antecedents of student engagement and
student burnout, while the well-being measures cannot be considered antecedents of the academic performance.
These findings provide additional knowledge regarding the relationships between academic well-being and
academic performance.

1. Introduction argued that school is a central element of adolescents’ and youths’ lives,
and defined well-being in relation to the school context (Tuominen-
The performance of an education system is currently defined in Soini, Salmela-Aro, & Niemivirta, 2012). In this framework, student
terms of academic achievement and student well-being (OECD, 2017), engagement and student burnout are positive and negative facets of
and the existence of both characteristics describes high-performing well-being that are connected to school activities (e.g., studying, at-
education (Lindfors, Minkkinen, Rimpela, & Hotulainen, 2018). In his tending classes). Research studies (e.g., Lindfors, Minkkinen, Rimpelä,
meta-synthesis, Wilder (2014) showed that the measurement of the & Hotulainen, 2018; Upadyaya & Salmela-Aro, 2013) that investigated
academic performance generally involves the assessment of student the relationships between student engagement, student burnout, and
knowledge (i.e., grades), but it can also include non-standardized academic performance used scales for academic engagement (Schaufeli,
achievement tests (e.g., teacher ratings of student academic behaviors Martínez et al., 2002; Schaufeli, Salanova et al., 2002) or scales for
and attitudes), homework completion rate, student study habits and student burnout (Schaufeli, Martínez et al., 2002; Schaufeli, Salanova
skills, student conduct. From a general perspective, subjective well- et al., 2002).
being describes how individuals generally evaluate their lives (Diener, In a recent meta-analysis, Bücker, Nuraydin, Simonsmeier,
1984). In an occupational context, employee well-being includes four Schneider, and Luhmann, (2018) concluded that the correlation be-
types of variables as follows: work engagement, happiness at work, tween academic performance and student well-being is small to
workaholism, and burnout (Bakker & Oerlemans, 2011). medium and that there is a need for more evidence regarding the re-
In an occupational context, two dimensions are identified as being ciprocal relationships between student performance and student well-
related to well-being: (1) activation, ranging from exhaustion to vigor, being. In this vein, Ng, Huebner, and Hills, (2015) reported reciprocal
and (2) identification, ranging from cynicism to dedication. Hence, relationships between life satisfaction and academic performance. Also,
burnout is characterized by a combination of exhaustion (low activa- Steinmayr, Crede, McElvany, and Wirthwein, (2016) concluded that
tion) and cynicism (low identification), whereas engagement is char- academic performance is an antecedent of life satisfaction (i.e., the
acterized by vigor (high activation) and dedication (high identification) cognitive component of subjective well-being, which is strongly asso-
(Schaufeli, Martínez, Pinto, Salanova, & Bakker, 2002; Schaufeli, ciated with high engagement and low burnout), but the components of
Salanova, González-Romá, & Bakker, 2002). subjective well-being did not predict later changes in students’ aca-
The researchers that used this perspective in educational settings demic performance.


Corresponding author at: West University of Timișoara, Department of Psychology, 4 Vasile Pârvan Blvd., Room 303, 300223, Timișoara, Romania.
E-mail address: laurentiu.maricutoiu@e-uvt.ro (L.P. Maricuţoiu).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2019.01.005
Received 8 September 2018; Received in revised form 14 January 2019; Accepted 18 January 2019
0191-491X/ © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
R. Paloș et al. Studies in Educational Evaluation 60 (2019) 199–204

Recent research studies that used Bakker and Oerlemans (2011) and (2) an European perspective based on the Bakker and Oerlemans
perspective on well-being in the educational context (i.e., student en- (2011) perspective, which defines engagement as a positive, fulfilling,
gagement, student burnout) also had difficulties in establishing a clear work-related state of mind characterized by vigor (or energy), dedica-
answer regarding the reciprocal relationships between academic per- tion, and absorption. In the present study, we adopted the European
formance and subjective well-being. Some researchers argued that perspective described by Upadyaya and Salmela-Aro (2013). In this
student engagement is an antecedent of academic performance (Seibert, vein, to be academically engaged means to be willing to invest effort in
Bauer, May, & Fincham, 2017 Study 1; Salanova, Schaufeli, Martínez, & your study work, to have high levels of energy and mental resilience
Bresó, 2010; van Beek, Kranenburg, Taris, & Schaufeli, 2013). These while studying (vigor); to be characterized by a sense of significance,
studies had some shortcomings, as follows. Some studies used a cross- enthusiasm, inspiration, pride, and challenge (dedication); to be fully
sectional research design and assumed that student engagement (van concentrated and happily engrossed in your study work (absorption)
Beek et al. (2013) or student burnout (Seibert et al., 2017, Study 1) are (Schaufeli, Martínez et al., 2002). Also, students’ engagement has a
antecedents of academic performance. Salanova et al. (2010) used a positive impact on their adjustment in the academic context and on
longitudinal design to test the temporal order using cross-lagged cor- their overall success (Upadyaya & Salmela-Aro, 2013).
relation analyses and reported significant results for one possible order There is an inconsistencyregarding the relationship between student
(i.e., student engagement as the antecedent of academic performance) engagement and academic performance. For instance, previous re-
without analyzing the other possible order (i.e., academic performance search reported that student engagement is positively related to student
as an antecedent for student engagement). performance (Fiorilli, De Stasio, Di Chiacchio, Pepe, & Salmela-Aro,
Based on the evidence presented above, we can assume that re- 2017; Schaufeli, Martínez et al., 2002; Schlenker, Schlenker, &
searchers favor the idea that subjective well-being is an antecedent of Schlenker, 2013) or predicts academic performance (van Beek et al.,
academic performance, and that the possibility of having a two-way 2013). Interestingly, Wang, Chow, Hofkens, and Salmela-Aro, (2015)
temporal order relation is generally overlooked. Therefore, the purpose reported that the correlation between student engagement in the 9th
of the present research paper was to fill this gap and to test both pos- grade and their GPA in 11th grade (r = .19) is smaller than the corre-
sible temporal relations. Using cross-lagged analyses, we are particu- lation between GPA in the 9th grade and the engagement in the 11th
larly interested in investigating a possible time order between academic grade (r = .46). This result suggests that it is more likely for GPA to be
performance and student engagement, and between academic perfor- the antecedent of student engagement, not the other way around.
mance and student burnout. Previous research recruited their students
from different faculties and studies (social and behavioral sciences, 1.3. Student burnout
chemistry, engineering, law, etc.). The present study was conducted
only with Psychology students involved in Educational Psychology Considered as an erosion of student engagement (Fiorilli et al.,
courses and can improve our understanding of the relationships be- 2017; Schaufeli, Martínez et al., 2002; Zhang, Gan, & Cham, 2007),
tween academic performance and student well-being variables in a student burnout refers to feeling exhausted because of study demands
particular educational context. Also, this study investigates whether the (emotional exhaustion), having a cynical and detached attitude toward
outcomes in a specific academic area are different from the outcomes one’s study (depersonalization, or cynicism), and feeling incompetent as a
found in a sample with mixed academic areas. Consequently, our results student, having the tendency to evaluate the educational context ne-
should provide support for interventions focused on well-being (as an gatively (low personal accomplishment) (Schaufeli, Martínez et al.,
antecedent for academic performance) or for interventions focused on 2002). Because the exhaustion and the depersonalization components
academic performance (as an antecedent for student well-being) in a are strongly correlated, researchers suggested that a general factor –
specific academic environment. core burnout, should be used in research studies (Green, Walkey, &
Taylor, 1991; Schaufeli, 2003).
1.1. Academic performance The relationship of student burnout with performance is incon-
sistent. On the one hand, some studies reported that academic perfor-
Academic performance is a central issue of educational practice mance is negatively related to student burnout (May, Bauer, & Fincham,
(Mahimuang, 2005). Researchers have linked academic performance to 2015; Schaufeli, Martínez et al., 2002). Students with a high level of
some behavioral, psychological, and social outcomes (Chen, 2005). academic burnout obtain lower levels of academic performance (Fiorilli
There is a consensus regarding the impact of academic performance et al., 2017). Also, students’ burnout affects their capacity to cope with
upon school adaptation or social integration (Rienties, Beausaert, events of school life and has a positive effect on depressive symptoms
Grohnert, Niemantsverdriet, & Kommers, 2012), future outcomes (Fiorilli et al., 2017). Similarly, Salmela-Aro, Kiuru, Leskinen, and
(Chen, 2005), or future jobs performance (Sonny & Sherman, 1989). Nurmi, (2009) reported a negative association between academic per-
Converging empirical evidence suggests that students who perform well formance and the cynicism toward the meaning of school and sense of
academically tend to experience positive schooling (Marks, 2010), po- inadequacy at school experienced by adolescents. Also, Salmela-Aro
sitive adjustment, life satisfaction (Crede, Wirthwein, McElvany, & and Read (2017) showed that cynicism and inadequacy increase gra-
Steinmayr, 2015) and general well-being (Nordlander & Stensöta, dually with the number of years of study, in the case of university
2014). In turn, academic performance and motivation are influenced by students. On the other hand, Salanova et al. (2010) found no significant
the feedback that students receive in the learning process (Denton, association between student burnout and future academic performance.
2014; Fishbach, Zhang, & Koo, 2009). Feedback helps them to know
how far or how close are by their goals, to increase their self-efficacy 2. The present study
beliefs (Beatson, Berg, & Smith, 2018), and to adjust their effort in
studying for an exam or in pursuing academic goals (Carrillo-de-la- The idea that knowing own performance (i.e., performance feed-
Peña et al., 2009). back) can be an antecedent for student well-being is not new (Beatson
et al., 2018; Fishbach, Eyal, & Finkelstein, 2010). The performance
1.2. Student engagement feedback received by students produce specific emotions and moods
(positive or negative), making them feel good or bad, and consequently
Upadyaya and Salmela-Aro (2013) delimitate between two ap- influencing their behavior in response to feedback (Fishbach et al.,
proaches regarding student engagement: (1) an American perspective, 2010). For example, the positive mood can cause disengagement when
where engagement is defined as a multidimensional construct which students interpreted it as a signal that they have done enough, or it can
includes students’ behavior, emotions, and cognitions while studying; increase goal engagement when is interpreted as a signal that they like

200
R. Paloș et al. Studies in Educational Evaluation 60 (2019) 199–204

the task (Fishbach et al., 2010). 3.3. Measures


Therefore, the objective of the present research study was to in-
vestigate the evidence regarding the temporal relations between aca- The academic performance measure was the student's grades on
demic grades and the two forms of student well-being (i.e., student Educational Psychology, on each of the two evaluations (i.e., T1 - the
engagement and student burnout) in a specific academic context (i.e., 7th and T2 - 14th week of the semester), and the correlation between
Psychology educational courses). Because previous research studies did these two measurement moments was .62. In the Romanian educational
not provide converging evidence regarding the order of these variables, system, grading scale runs from 1 to 10. The grades 5 (lowest) to 10
we formulated the following hypotheses: (highest) are all passing grades, whereas for grades below 5 indicate
failed exams.
H1. There is a reciprocal relation between academic grades and student
Regarding the well-being measures, we choose the educational
engagement.
versions of two scales that are most popular in the occupational health
H2. There is a reciprocal relation between academic grades and student domain. For the two questionnaires, their Romanian versions were
burnout. successfully used in previous studies on Romanian students’ samples
(Sulea, van Beek, Sârbescu, Vîrga, & Schaufeli, 2015).
Student engagement was assessed with 17-items Utrecht Work
3. Method Engagement Scale – Student version (UWES-S, Schaufeli, Martínez
et al., 2002). Although the questionnaire has three dimensions (i.e.,
Every Department of Psychology in Romania has its entrance re- vigor, dedication, and absorption), we have chosen to assess engage-
quirements. Usually, the main criterion of student selection is the mean ment as a composite score because of the strong correlations (i.e., va-
of baccalaureate exam, which account in various ratio. Because the lues above .70) between these scales. Items were scored on a 7-point
competition for study places is high, every faculty can add its criteria. In scale (0–never; 6–always), and the larger scores indicate higher levels
our university, students who want to follow courses of Psychology of engagement. The reliability of the scale was α = .93 in both mea-
specialization have to meet two criteria: the mean of baccalaureate surement moments. The items and instructions were contextualized to
exam (which account for 67% from the entrance mean), and a test be relevant to the Educational Psychology course (e.g., Time flies when I
which assesses their linguistic and cognitive competencies. This test am studying Educational Psychology).
accounts for 33% of the admission mean. There are 250 places per year, Student burnout was measured with 15-items Maslach Burnout
and only 50 of them are without tuition fees. The rate of dropout after Inventory – student survey (MBI-SS, Schaufeli, Martínez et al., 2002),
the first university year is almost 30%. adapted for educational context. For this study, the items and instruc-
tions were contextualized to be relevant to the Educational Psychology
course (e.g., Studying or attending a class of Educational Psychology is
3.1. Participants
really a strain for me). Following the approach suggested by Green et al.
(1991), we computed a general score for core burnout using the items
162 Romanian psychology freshman students signed up for this
from the exhaustion and depersonalization scales. All items are scored
research, but 20 of them failed to complete the questionnaires in both
on a 7-point frequency rating scale ranging from 0 (never) to 6 (always),
measurement moments. Regarding their initial levels of engagement
and the internal consistency alphas are .76 (at T1) and .81 (at T2).
and burnout, the 20 students (15 female) that dropped out of the study
were not significantly different from the students that were included in
3.4. Data analysis
this study. Therefore, the final research sample consisted of 142 first-
year Psychology students (76.06% females), who received extra credits
We used structural equation modeling to analyze the latent struc-
for their participation. The age of the participant ranged from 19 to 55
ture of our outcomes. Firstly, we tested two alternative measurement
years (M = 21.34; SD = 5.37).
models to assess whether the common variance of student engagement
and student burnout can be specified i) in twolatent factors, one for
3.2. Procedure each measurement moment; or ii) in two latent factors, one for each
measure. In the first structural model, we specified a latent factor for
All participants were enrolled in a compulsory semester course in the variables measured in T1, another latent factor for the variables
Educational Psychology. A semester has 14th weeks and the assessment measured in T2, and we allowed these two latent factors to correlate.
is summative (i.e., at the end of the semester). For this research, stu- The fit indices of this measurement model were poor (CFI = .69,
dents had a midterm evaluation (T1, in the 7h week of the semester) RMSEA = .64, SRMR = .13); therefore we concluded that engagement
from the first seventh lectures content and another final evaluation (T2, and burnout could not be seen as measures of a common latent variable
in the 14th week of the semester) from the other seventh lectures (e.g., general student well-being). In the second structural model, we
content. All participants were informed regarding the results of the T1 specified a latent factor for both measurement moments of student
in the 8th week of the semester. Therefore, they were aware of their engagement, and another latent factor for both measurement moments
performance in T1 when they prepared for the T2 evaluation. The of student burnout. The fit indices of this model were also poor
students that failed the midterm evaluation (T1, the grade was less than (CFI = .85, RMSEA = .25, SRMR = .09) For this reason, we treated
5) were not allowed to participate to the final evaluation (T2 – this was engagement and burnout separately in our analyses.
the reason why 20 of them did not complete the second measurement). Next, we used path analysis to investigate the cross-lagged relations.
The exam consisted of a mix of multiple-choice items and situational We used an approach similar to the one used previously by Hakanen,
problems. The final grade for the Educational Psychology exam was the Schaufeli, and Ahola, (2008). We estimated the fit of four path analysis
average of the two assessments. Participants completed paper-and- models: the autonomy model (i.e., auto-correlations from T1 to T2), the
pencil measures at two moments, before each evaluation. Before grades as predictor (i.e. the autonomy model, and the grades at T1
starting filling in the questionnaires, the participants had to read an predicting all measures in T2), the engagement as predictor (i.e. the
introduction where the aims and the procedure of the study were ex- autonomy model, and engagement at T1 predicting all measures in T2),
plained. Their answers indicated their agreement to be part of the and the burnout as predictor (i.e. the autonomy model, and burnout at
study. Although the time between the two assessments seems to be T1 predicting all measures at T2).
short, previous research (Ouweneel, Le Blanc, & Schaufeli, 2011) re- We conducted all our analyses in the R package lavaan (Rosseel,
ported significant cross-lagged effects using a 4-week interval. 2012), using the maximum likelihood estimation method. Regarding

201
R. Paloș et al. Studies in Educational Evaluation 60 (2019) 199–204

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and correlations between study variables.
min max m SD Gender Age Grade1 SE T1 SB T1 Grade2 SE T2 SB T2

Gender — −.215* .171* .106 −.116 .187* .05 −.204 *


Age 19 55 21.34 5.36 — −.013 .131 −.059 −.058 .067 −.039
Grade1 5 10 7.62 1.27 — .037 −.058 .618*** .157 −.173 *
SE T1 1.76 6.94 4.37 1.08 .93 −.375*** .085 .678 *** −.315 ***
SB T1 1.10 5.40 3.09 0.94 .76 −.004 −.231 ** .623 ***
Grade2 3.75 10 7.28 1.64 — .119 −.069
SE T2 1 6.52 4.37 1.02 .93 −.347 ***
SB T2 1.20 5.70 3.16 0.93 .81

Note. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001 Gender (1-male, 2-female); SE T1 = Student engagement at T1, SB T1 = Student burnout at T1, SE T2 = Student
engagement at T2, SB T2 = Student burnout at T2. Internal consistency indices (i.e., Cronbach’s alphas) are presented in italics on the diagonal.

model fit, we used the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis that high academic performance can be seen as an antecedent of high
index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and student engagement and low student burnout. In conclusion, our results
the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Hu and Bentler did not support the hypotheses of this research study, which anticipated
(1999) recommended that acceptable models should have CFI and TLI reciprocal relations between academic grades and student engagement
values above .95 indicate good fit, RMSEA values below .06, and the (Hypothesis 1), or reciprocal relations between academic grades and
SRMR values should be less than .08 to consider the model as appro- student burnout (Hypothesis 2).
priate. We compared the alternative models using the Δχ² statistical test The model that assumed that grades are antecedents of student
for nested models. Furthermore, we conducted post-hoc statistical engagement and burnout is presented in Fig. 1. In this model, the high
power analyses (Preacher & Coffman, 2006) to investigate the ability to grades at T1 have an incremental effect on high student engagement
detect significant differences between our models at a .05 alpha level. (B = .106, SE = .049, p = .029, β = .13) and on low student burnout
(B = -.102, SE = .048, p = .034, β = -.14), beyond the auto-regression
4. Results effects of each variable.

4.1. Correlations between study variables


5. Discussion
Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and the zero-order corre-
lation matrix of the variables included in this study, at the two mea- This research aimed to investigate the possible temporal order be-
surement moments. The results of the first academic evaluation (stu- tween academic performance (i.e., student grades) and two forms of
dents’ grades) correlated significantly with student engagement at T2 (r student well-being: student engagement and student burnout in a spe-
(140) = .16, p = .06) and student burnout at T2 (r(140) = −.17, cific academic context (i.e., Psychology educational courses). Previous
p = .04). The second evaluation of academic performance (students’ research studies, which included students from various academic areas,
grades) did not correlate with engagement or burnout at any moment. did not provide a clear direction for this temporal order. Therefore we
tested both possible relations: grades as a consequence of student well-
being, and student well-being as a consequence of academic perfor-
4.2. Model comparisons
mance (i.e., grades).
One central finding of this study is that high academic grades can be
We presented the fit indices for all models in Table 2. Although the
considered an antecedent for high student engagement and low student
values of most fit indices indicated good fit in the case of all our models,
burnout. Although some cross-sectional research studies reported that
the RMSEA index had indicated acceptable fit only for the model that
increased burnout is associated with smaller GPA (e.g., May et al.,
assumed that grades are an antecedent of student engagement and
2015), our results are convergent with the findings reported by
student burnout.
Salanova et al. (2010), who found that burnout has no significant effect
Model comparison analyses indicated that the autonomy model is
on future performance. In our alternative model, burnout was not a
significantly inferior only to the model that assumed grades as ante-
predictor of future student grades. However, when we tested the op-
cedents of engagement and well-being (Δχ²(2) = 7.323, p = .026;
posite temporal relation, our results showed that high academic per-
ΔRMSEA = .077, statistical power = .996 for detecting RMSEA differ-
formance (i.e., grades) could predict future low student burnout. Si-
ences a .05 alpha level). Our results suggested that the autonomy model
milarly, Salmela-Aro et al. (2009) found that adolescents experienced
does not have superior fit as compared with the model that assumed
more cynicism toward the meaning of school and more sense of in-
student engagement as antecedent of all T2 variables (Δχ²(2) = 2.673,
adequacy at school, as they had lower GPA and lower school engage-
p = .263), or as compared with the model that assumed student
ment.
burnout is an antecedent of all T2 variables (Δχ²(2) = 0.336, p = .845).
Also, previous studies showed that student engagement positively
Taken together, the results of the model comparison analyses suggested
predicts student grades (Schlenker et al., 2013; van Beek et al., 2013).
Likewise, Salanova et al. (2010) reported that engagement has a sig-
Table 2
Fit indices of the four models tested.
nificant incremental effect on student grades. In our analyses, the value
of the regression coefficient is very similar to the one reported by
Model χ² (df) CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR Salanova et al. (2010), but our smaller sample size did not provide
(with 90% confidence
interval)
sufficient statistical power to reach statistical significance. However, we
had found significant results when we tested the alternative temporal
Autonomy model 11.070 (6) .980 .950 .077 (.000–.147) .058 direction: high student grades significantly predicted future high student
Grades as antecedent 3.747 (4) .999 .999 .001 (.000–.123) .027 engagement. In a similar vein, Fishbach et al. (2009) suggested that
Engagement as 8.397 (4) .983 .935 .088 (.000–.172) .048
students will focus on their academic work if they get a high mark on an
antecedent
Burnout as antecedent 10.734 (4) .973 .900 .109 (.031–.190) .061 exam and interpret this is the result of their commitment. Conse-
quently, students are inclined to give up to their academic work if the

202
R. Paloș et al. Studies in Educational Evaluation 60 (2019) 199–204

Fig. 1. Standardized coefficients of the model that assumed grades are antecedents of engagement and burnout.

mark is low. To sum up, our results showed that student grades could From a theoretical perspective, our findings are important because they
lead to future student burnout and significantly predict future student provide additional knowledge regarding the direction of the relations
engagement. between academic well-being and academic performance in a particular
academic context. Therefore, more research studies in different aca-
5.1. Limitations and future directions demic areas are needed to clarify these divergent findings.
From a practical perspective, the results are useful especially for
This study has some limitations that should be taken into con- instructors and educational practices. For example, if teachers are
sideration. Firstly, we didn’t take into account the way students inter- aware of the factors that influence students’ engagement and burnout
pret and relate to the feedback they got from the first evaluation. We levels, they can design an optimal learning environment to support
already know that feedback makes people feel good or bad (Fishbach students’ needs. Moreover, to prevent emotional exhaustion and cynical
et al., 2010) and influences their motivation to reach a goal (Fishbach attitudes towards learning, instructors could help students to manage
et al., 2009). Consequently, although we know that grades are the first their effort in dealing with learning demands and could adjust their
feedback that teachers gave to students, we don’t know if the academic workloads (Noh, Shin, & Lee, 2013). Also, our students had some feed-
grades or the student interpretation of the feedback are causes for back regarding their academic performance because they knew their
student burnout and engagement. Therefore, a future direction for re- academic grades two weeks before they completed the wellbeing scales
search should investigate the feedback effect to see if and how it for the second measurement moment. Previous research pointed out
mediates the relation between grades and academic well-being. that the absence of feedback regarding their academic performance
A second possible limitation is the fact that we only had two mea- (Schaufeli, Martínez et al., 2002) or poor performance feedback from
surement moments for a specific discipline (i.e., Educational the teacher (Carrillo-de-la- Peña et al., 2009) may be related to the
Psychology), over the course of one semester. But beyond this, a strong cynical attitude of students towards study (i.e., depersonalization) .
point of this study is precisely the longitudinal analysis over the se- Therefore, teachers should pay more attention to the evaluation process
mester. At this moment, it is unclear whether the effects presented in and how they offer performance feedback no matter their educational
this paper can be linked with academic grades in other disciplines from area.
this semester. Because some of our outcomes were different from pre-
vious studies, to get a clearer image on the relationships between Acknowledgments
academic performance and student well-being variables future research
should investigate whether such effects can be observed from one dis- The first and the second authors contributed equally; order of au-
cipline to another (also compulsory and optional), or from one semester thorship is arbitrary. This work was partially supported by a grant from
to another. the Romanian Ministry of Education, CNCS – UEFISCDI, project number
Finally, the structure of the sample (i.e., the imbalance between 108BM/2017. This organization had no role in the design and im-
men and women, size) might be a possible limitation of this research plementation of the study.
study. Although we did not find significant differences between male
and female students regarding engagement, we found some marginally References
significant gender differences regarding the grades in both measure-
ment moments and the level of burnout in the second measurement Bakker, A. B., & Oerlemans, W. (2011). Subjective well-being in organizations. In K. S.
Cameron, & G. M. Spreitzer (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of positive organizational
moment. Although these differences are marginally significant scholarship (pp. 178–189). New York: Oxford University Press.
(p > .01), we encourage future research studies to include gender Beatson, N. J., Berg, D. A., & Smith, J. K. (2018). The impact of mastery feedback on
differences in the analysis of the relationships between academic per- undergraduate students’ self-efficacy beliefs. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 59,
58–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2018.03.002.
formance and student well-being. Because of the limited number of the
Bücker, S., Nuraydin, S., Simonsmeier, B. A., Schneider, M., & Luhmann, M. (2018).
students involved, our findings should be considered with caution. Subjective well-being and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Moreover, some of the results presented in this paper are marginally Research in Personality, 74, 83–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.02.007.
significant and, therefore, more research studies are needed to confirm Carrillo-de-la- Peña, M. T., Baillès, E., Caseras, X., Martínez, A., Ortet, G., & Pérez, J.
(2009). Formative assessment and academic achievement in pre-graduate students of
our findings. health sciences. Advances in Health Sciences Education: Theory and Practice, 14, 61–67.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-007-9086-y.
Chen, J. J.-L. (2005). Relation of academic support from parents, teachers, and peers to
6. Conclusions Hong Kong adolescents’ academic achievement: The mediating role of academic
engagement. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 131(2), 77–127.
The fact that this study is a longitudinal analysis over a semester it https://doi.org/10.3200/MONO.131.2.77-127.
Crede, J., Wirthwein, L., McElvany, N., & Steinmayr, R. (2015). Adolescents’ academic
allowed us to compare both directions of the temporal order directly
achievement and life satisfaction: The role of parents’ education. Frontiers in
and to emphasize not only the effect of well-being on academic per- Psychology, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00052.
formance but also the effect of academic performance on well-being. Denton, D. W. (2014). Using screen capture feedback to improve academic performance.

203
R. Paloș et al. Studies in Educational Evaluation 60 (2019) 199–204

TechTrends, 58(6), 51–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11528-014-0803-0. Salanova, M., Schaufeli, W. B., Martínez, I., & Bresó, E. (2010). How obstacles and fa-
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575. https://doi. cilitators predict academic performance: The mediating role of study burnout and
org/10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542. engagement. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 23, 53–70. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Fiorilli, C., De Stasio, S., Di Chiacchio, C., Pepe, A., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2017). School 10615800802609965.
burnout, depressive symptoms and engagement: Their combined effect on student Salmela-Aro, K., & Read, S. (2017). Study engagement and burnout profiles among
achievement. International Journal of Educational Research, 84, 1–12. https://doi.org/ Finnish higher education students. Burnout Research, 7, 21–28. https://doi.org/10.
10.1016/j.ijer.2017.04.001. 1016/j.burn.2017.11.001.
Fishbach, A., Zhang, Y., & Koo, M. (2009). The dynamics of self-regulation. European Salmela-Aro, K., Kiuru, N., Leskinen, E., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2009). School-Burnout inventory
Review of Social Psychology, 20, 315–344. https://doi.org/10.1080/ (SBI). Reliability and validity. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(1),
10463280903275375. 48–57. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.25.1.48.
Fishbach, A., Eyal, T., & Finkelstein, S. R. (2010). How positive and negative feedback Schaufeli, W. B. (2003). Past performance and future perspectives of burnout research. SA
motivate goal pursuit. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(8), 517–530. Journal of Industrial Psychology, 29(4), 1–15.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00285.x. Schaufeli, W. B., Martínez, I. M., Pinto, A. M., Salanova, M., & Bakker, A. B. (2002).
Green, D. E., Walkey, F. H., & Taylor, A. J. W. (1991). The three factor structure of the Burnout and engagement in university students. A cross-national study. Journal of
Maslach Burnout Inventory. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6, 453–472. Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33(5), 464–481. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Hakanen, J. J., Schaufeli, W. B., & Ahola, K. (2008). The job demands-resources model: A 0022022102033005003.
three-year cross-lagged study of burnout, depression, commitment, and work en- Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-Romá, V., & Bakker, A. B. (2002). The mea-
gagement. Work and Stress, 22, 224–241. https://doi.org/10.1080/ surement of engagement and burnout: A two sample confirmatory factor analytic
02678370802379432. approach. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 71–92. https://doi.org/10.1023/
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure A:1015630930326.
analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, Schlenker, B. R., Schlenker, P. A., & Schlenker, K. A. (2013). Antecedents of academic
6, 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118. engagement and the implications for college grades. Learning and Individual
Lindfors, P., Minkkinen, J., Rimpelä, A., & Hotulainen, R. (2018). Family and school Differences, 27, 75–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.06.014.
social capital, school burnout and academic achievement: A multilevel longitudinal Seibert, G. S., Bauer, K. N., May, R. W., & Fincham, F. D. (2017). Emotion regulation and
analysis among Finnish pupils. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 23, academic underperformance: The role of school burnout. Learning and Individual
368–381. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2017.1389758. Differences, 60, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.10.001.
Mahimuang, S. (2005). Factors influencing academic achievement and improvement: A Sonny, A. S., & Sherman, T. A. (1989). Employee education and job performance: does
value-added approach. Educational Research for Policy and Practice, 4, 13–26. https:// education matter? Public Personnel Management, 18(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/
doi.org/10.1007/s10671-005-0677-1. 009102608901800101.
Marks, G. N. (2010). What aspects of schooling are important? School effects on tertiary Steinmayr, R., Crede, J., McElvany, N., & Wirthwein, L. (2016). Subjective well-being,
entrance performance. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 21(3), 267–287. test anxiety, academic achievement: Testing for reciprocal effects. Frontiers in
https://doi.org/10.1080/09243451003694364. Psychology, 6, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01994 1994.
May, R. W., Bauer, K. N., & Fincham, F. D. (2015). School burnout: Diminished academic Sulea, C., van Beek, I., Sârbescu, P., Vîrga, D., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2015). Engagement,
and cognitive performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 42, 126–131. https:// boredom, and burnout among students: Basic need satisfaction matters more than
doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.07.015. personality traits. Learning and Individual Differences, 42, 132–138. https://doi.org/
Ng, Z. J., Huebner, S. E., & Hills, K. J. (2015). Life satisfaction and academic performance 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.08.018.
in early adolescents: Evidence for reciprocal association. Journal of School Psychology, Tuominen-Soini, H., Salmela-Aro, K., & Niemivirta, M. (2012). Achievement goal or-
53, 479–491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2015.09.004. ientations and academic well-being across the transition to upper secondary educa-
Noh, H., Shin, H., & Lee, S. M. (2013). Developmental process of academic burnout tion. Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 290–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
among Korean middle school students. Learning and Individual Differences, 28, 82–89. lindif.2012.01.002.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.09.014. Upadyaya, K., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2013). Development of school engagement in associa-
Nordlander, E., & Stensöta, H. O. (2014). Grades – For better or worse? The interplay of tion with academic success and well-being in varying social context. A review of
school performance and subjective well-being among boys and girls. Child Indicators empirical research. European Psychologist, 18(2), 136–147. https://doi.org/10.1027//
Research, 7, 861–879. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-014-9233-y. 1016-9040/a000143.
OECD (2017). PISA 2015 results (Volume III): Students’ well-being. Paris: OECD Publishing. van Beek, I., Kranenburg, I. C., Taris, T. W., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2013). BIS- and BAS-
Ouweneel, E., Le Blanc, P. M., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2011). Flourishing students: A long- activation and study outcomes: A mediation study. Personality and Individual
itudinal study on positive emotions, personal resources, and study engagement. The Differences, 55, 474–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.04.013.
Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 142–153. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2011. Wang, M.-T., Chow, A., Hofkens, T., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2015). The trajectories of student
558847. emotional engagement and school burnout with academic and psychological devel-
Preacher, K. J., & Coffman, D. L. (2006). Computing power and minimum sample size for opment: Findings from Finnish adolescents. Learning and Instruction, 36, 57–65.
RMSEA [Computer software]. May, Available fromhttp://quantpsy.org/. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.11.004.
Rienties, B., Beausaert, S., Grohnert, T., Niemantsverdriet, S., & Kommers, P. (2012). Wilder, S. (2014). Effects of parental involvement on academic achievement: A meta-
Understanding academic performance of international students: The role of ethnicity, synthesis. Educational Review, 66(3), 377–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131911.
academic and social integration. Higher Education, 63, 685–700. https://doi.org/10. 2013.780009.
1007/s10734-011-9468-1. Zhang, Y., Gan, Y., & Cham, H. (2007). Perfectionism, academic burnout and engagement
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling and more. among Chinese college students: A structural equation modelling analysis. Personality
Version 0.5–12 (BETA). Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 1–36. https://doi.org/10. and Individual Differences, 43, 1529–1540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.
18637/jss.v048.i02. 010.

204

You might also like