Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Emptying the Nest and Parental Well-Being: An Analysis of National Panel Data

Author(s): Lynn White and John N. Edwards


Source: American Sociological Review , Apr., 1990, Vol. 55, No. 2 (Apr., 1990), pp. 235-
242
Published by: American Sociological Association

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/2095629

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/2095629?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

American Sociological Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend
access to American Sociological Review

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
EMPTYING THE NEST AND PARENTAL WELL-BEING:
AN ANALYSIS OF NATIONAL PANEL DATA*

LYNN WHITE JOHN N. EDWARDS


University of Nebraska-Lincoln Virginia Polytechnic University

Panel data from a national random sample are used to investigate the effects of children
leaving home on parental well-being. The "empty nest" is associated with significant
improvements in marital happiness for all parents, regardless of parent's or children's
characteristics. Overall life satisfaction improves significantly only under two conditions:
when there is frequent contact with nonresident children or when there were young teens
in the 1983 household. For both measures of parental well-being, the positive effects of
the empty nest appear to be strongest immediately after the children leave. These findings,
coupled with the high levels of post-launching contact, suggest that while parents
experience a modest post-launch honeymoon, the parental role remains important to
parental well-being.

T he effect of the child-leaving phase of the being and organized, functional behavior."
family life cycle on parental well-being According to this perspective, the more roles
has been the topic of much discussion and one has, the better off one is. This perspective
several influential studies, but empirical find- predicts that role loss will have negative effects
ings are sketchy. Most studies rely on cross- on psychological functioning, suggesting that
sectional data, and only two studies examine the child-leaving will be associated with decreases
change in parental well-being associated with in parental well-being. This prediction rests on
launching. We follow a national sample of 402 the questionable premise that launching one's
parents of older children over a four-year period children means exiting the parental role. Gener-
and compare changes in marital happiness and ally, scholars working in this tradition reject this
life satisfaction between those who did and did premise: respondents are counted as occupying
not empty their nest. We also examine whether a parental role if they have ever had children (cf.
the effects of the empty nest depend upon the Thoits 1983).
stressfulness of the parental role or amount of The role change perspective is associated
post-launch contact. with the work of Holmes and Rahe (1967).
Their work, which is the basis of a long research

PREVIOUS WORK tradition, suggests that any role change (whether


addition or deletion) will have negative effects
Theoretical Perspectives
on psychological and physical well-being. This
Three theoretical perspectives offer hypotheses perspective also predicts a negative effect of
about the effects of the empty nest: role identity emptying the nest. Because the negative effect
theories, role change theories, and role conflict of role change may be short-lived, it may not be
theories. observed in a four-year panel.
Role identity theories derive from the struc- Finally, a general perspective that we call role
tural school of symbolic interaction. Thoits stress argues that the effect of change depends
(1983, p. 183) argues, for example, that "Social on the degree of conflict and stress associated
identities provide actors with existential mean- with the role (Barnett and Baruch 1985). If there
ing and behavioral guidance, and that these is role strain or role conflict, loss of the role may
qualities are essential to psychological well- be beneficial. Because several studies find that
parenthood is a stressful role (McLanahan and
* This study was supported in part by Grant No. 5
Adams 1987), this perspective suggests that the
ROl AG4146 from the National Institute on Aging.
empty nest should lead to improvements in
The contributions of co-investigators Alan Booth and
David R. Johnson are gratefully acknowledged. The parental well-being.
paper also benefitted from the helpful comments of The sociological literature on launching has
anonymous ASR reviewers. generally used some form of role identity or role

American Sociological Review, 1990,Vol. 55 (April:235-242) 235

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
236 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

stress theory. The "empty nest syndrome" of marriages (Luckey and Bain 1970; Lauer and
early work is clearly a form of role identity Lauer 1986) suggests that people in poor mar-
theory: loss of a major role brings alienation, riages may hang on to their children longer. In
dissatisfaction, and loneliness (Phillips 1957). addition to these general problems of cross-
On the other hand, more recent commentators sectional research, the Glenn (1975) and Glenn
seem to assume that parenthood is a stressful and McLanahan (1982) studies are hampered by
role, and that its loss should bring relief (Glenn inexact comparisons: Because their data sets
1975; Miller and Myers-Walls 1983). only measured presence of children under 18,
they were forced to include childless families
and families with resident adult children in the
Empirical Findings
post-launch category.
Although two early clinical studies (Curlee 1969; Two studies examine change in parental well-
Bart 1972) report depression following the being associated with emptying the nest. Both
empty-nest period, most cross-sectional studiesreport generally positive effects of completing
of the general population reveal modest positive the launching process. Menaghan (1983), in a
outcomes (Rollins andFeldman 1970; Deutscher sample of 639 Chicagoans interviewed in 1972
1973; Glenn 1975; Harkins 1978; Campbell and 1976, reports the effects of the empty nest
1981). on two measures - an index of marital affec-
Several studies use the family life cycle ap- tion-fulfillment and an index of perceived eq-
proach of grouping a cross-sectional sample uity. Comparing 34 respondents whose young-
into four or five categories that correspond est (last?) child had left home with respondents
roughly to stages of family development. Most who were already empty nesters or who still had
of these studies report a U-shaped pattern in children in the home, Menaghan concludes that
which marital happiness is highest during the launching the youngest child results in signifi-
honeymoon stage, lowest when the children are cant improvement in perceived equity but not in
schoolage or teens, and higher when the chil- affection-fulfillment. Using a larger sample from
dren are older or gone (Rollins and Feldman the Panel Study of Income Dynamics, McLa-
1970; Rollins and Cannon 1974; Anderson, nahan and Sorensen (1985) investigate whether
Russell, and Schuman 1983). The effect is rather child-leaving changes self-satisfaction (Are you
small, however, and sometimes absent (Spanier, satisfied or dissatisfied with yourself?) between
Lewis, Cole 1975). 1968 and 1972. They find that on-time depar-
Studies by Glenn (1975) and Glenn and tures (i.e.,when parents were over 40) are asso-
McLanahan (1982), using national cross-sec- ciated with significant increases in women's
tional data, show that families with children inself satisfaction but not men's. Off-time depar-
the home are generally worse off in terms of tures (i.e., before the parent reached 40) are
global and marital satisfaction than families associated with decreased self-satisfaction for
without children. In support of the argument fathers. Because they used the departure of any
that parenthood is a generally stressful role, child rather than the completion of the launch-
Glenn and McLanahan found no group for whom ing process as their independent variable,
presence of children was positively correlated McLanahan and Sorensen may have underesti-
with marital satisfaction. mated the effect of emptying the nest on parental
These cross-sectional studies have several well-being.
drawbacks. In addition to the possibility that the These two panel studies are milestones in the
observed relationships are cohort effects (empty- assessment of the launching process. They pro-
nest parents come from earlier cohorts than vide the first evidence that change in parental
parents currently with children in the home), status is associated with positive change in
selection biases are possible (Spanieretal. 1975; parental well-being. Because neither study was
Schram 1979). A selection effect for presence designed to tap changes in parental well-being,
of children and lower happiness could occur in however, both use dependent variables that re-
two ways. First, children slow the divorce proc- late only indirectly to previous research.
ess suggesting an overrepresentation of unsatis-
factory marriages among families with children
The Issues
in the home (White, Booth, and Edwards 1986).
Second, the evidence that children are an impor- We examine the effects of entering and com-
tant source of satisfaction to people in poor pleting the launching process on two indicators

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
EMPTYING THE NEST 237

of parental well-being: marital happiness and tion on relationships with nonresident children
life satisfaction. We test the hypothesis that is available only for the 1988 wave, this study is
launching will have a positive effect on parental based on a comparison of the 1983-1988 panel.
well-being and that this effect will be stronger Analysis is restricted to the 402 respondents
for parents who experience the most stress from who met the following criteria: 1) interviewed
parenting. in 1983 and 1988; 2) marriage intact between
Previous studies suggest that mothers find 1983 and 1988; and 3) had at least one child 14
parenting more stressful than fathers (McLa- or older in the home in 1983.
nahan and Adams 1987), that parents argue
more with sons than daughters (Suitor and Pille-
Dependent Variables
mer 1988), and that stepchildren cause more
family tension than biological children (White To maximize comparability with previous lit-
and Booth 1985). In addition, we hypothesize erature, we use marital happiness and life satis-
that parenting will be more stressful when par- faction as dependent variables. Marital happi-
ents hold nontraditional gender roles or are ness is measured by an 1-item summed scale
highly educated. The latter variables have been with an alpha reliability of .87.2 The mean for
shown by Goldscheider and Goldscheider (1988) this scale, which has a possible range of 11 to 33,
to predict early departure of children from the was 29.2 in 1983 with a standard deviation of
parental home. Mother's employment and 4.0. Life satisfaction is measured by the stan-
number of children in the household are also dard single-item indicator: "Taking everything
included as indicators of parental stress. We test together, how would you say you are these
to see whether the effects of launching on paren- days? Would you say you are very happy (=3),
tal well-being are modified by these indicators pretty happy (=2), or not too happy? (=1)." In
of parental stress. We also include age of young- 1983, the mean on this variable was 2.3 and the
est child, which acts as a proxy for recency of standard deviation was .56. This item has been
emptying the nest as well as tapping amount of used in previous research (e.g., Glenn 1975).
active parenting required by resident children.
Finally, we test whether the effect of launch-
Independent Variables
ing depends upon contact with nonresident
children. If contact and obligations continue to To distinguish between entering and complet-
be high, launching may have little effect on ing the launching process, we use two measures.
parental well-being. EMPTY NEST is coded 1 if the respondent reports
no children in the household in 1988 and 0 if
there are children in the household in 1988. (All
STUDY DESIGN of the respondents had at least one child 14 or

Sample
was comparable to national distributions of married
This research is based on a national sample individuals under 55 on age, race, household size,
tenure, and region. Analysis of panel attrition over the
interviewed in 1980andagain in 1983 and 1988.
In 1980, telephone interviews were conducted following two waves demonstrates that the sample
remains representative of our target population, al-
with a random sample of 2,033 married indi-
though men, renters, and those with low education
viduals under the age of 55. In 1983, reinter-
were more likely to drop out of the sample. Probit
views were completed with 1,592 individuals of
analysis demonstrates that the probability of drop-
whom 1,331 were interviewed a third time in ping out of the sample is unrelated to marital happi-
1988. Careful analysis shows that the 1983 and ness scores on the prior interview. We feel confident
1988 panels are largely representative of the that, with appropriate controls for background fac-
nation's married couples.' Because informa- tors, the panel remains an effective tool for evaluating
factors associated with change in marital quality.
' In 1980, sample households were chosen using 2 The scale includes seven items asking about
random digit dialing procedures. A second random happiness with specific aspects of marriage (under-
procedure was used to select the respondent. Only standing, love, agreement, sexual relationship, tak-
married individuals under 55 were included in the ing care of things around the house, companionship,
sample. Interviews were completed with 65 percent and faithfulness) and four global satisfaction items
of those estimated to be eligible; among households (overall happiness of marriage, rating of own mar-
where an eligible respondent was contacted, the riage compared to others, strength of love for spouse,
completion rate was 76 percent. The 1980 sample and whether the marriage is getting better or worse).

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
238 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

older in the household in 1983.) Using this tion.3 Column 1 shows that having launched all
definition, 123 of the 402 respondents (31 per- of one's children is associated with a 1 .10-point
cent) launched all of their children by 1988. increase on the marital happiness scale. The
Nearly three-fourths (194) of those coded 0 on positive effect of the empty nest on marital
the empty nest variable entered the launching happiness but not on life satisfaction coincides
process and have at least one nonresident child with Glenn's (1975) finding that the correlation
in addition to the children still in the household. of empty nest with marital happiness is stronger
The dummy variable PARIAL LAUNCH identifies than with life satisfaction.
these parents who have started but not com- PARTIAL LAUNCH has no effect on either life
pleted the launching process. A comparison of satisfaction or marital happiness. A comparison
the effects of these two variables tells us more of the results for the two independent variables
precisely what stage of the launching process suggests that total absence of children rather
affects parental well-being. than simply older children or fewer children is
necessary before the launching process improves
marital happiness.
Background Variables

Gender-role traditionalism is measured by a 7-


Interaction Effects: Parental Stress
item Likert-type scale asking about normative
Indicators
behavior for men and women. The scale has an
alpha reliability of .71. Mother's employment isNext we examine whether the effects of the
a dummy variable (1 = employed more than 35 empty nest are general across all parents or
hours per week). Presence of stepchildren is whether they depend on extent of parental strain.
coded 1 if there are children of the respondent In or
the case of life satisfaction, we ask whether
his/her spouse in the household who are more more highly stressed parents experience im-
than one year older than the current marriage. provement following launch. We hypothesized
Boys is a dummy variable scored 1 if any of the that stress would be greater under the following
children in the household in 1983 were boys. conditions: respondent was female, more highly
educated, or reported nontraditional gender roles,
the number of children was larger, any of the
FINDINGS
children were boys or stepchildren, mother was
Because the dependent variables are continu- employed, or age of youngest child was low. All
ous, OLS regression is used. To assess whether stress indicators were drawn from the 1983 data.
changes in well-being are associated with child- The tests involved adding a multiplicative term
leaving, we regress the 1988 score for the de- (e.g., EMPTY NEST x gender-role traditionalism)
pendent variable on the 1983 score of the de- to the equations in columns I and 5. Because
pendent variable and the launching variables. EMPTY NEST rather than PARTIAL LAUNCH appears
The effective dependent variable is change in to be the relevant variable, the interactions were
well-being, i.e., that part of the 1988 score that restricted to the EMPTY NEST variable.
is not predicted from the 1983 score. Control Only one of the 16 tests of interaction was
variables include respondent's sex, age, educa- significant: the lower the age of youngest child,
tion, and gender-role traditionalism, the number the greater the improvement in marital happi-
of children in the household, whether any of the
ness following the empty nest (columns 2 and
children were boys, presence of stepchildren, 6). For life satisfaction, the term was also nega-
age of youngest child, and mother's employ- tive, but not significant (p =.09). The equation
ment status. predicts an improvement on the marital happi-

3 Because of inconsistencies in data availability


The Effect of Launching, 1983-1988
across the three waves, the full analysis cannot be
We first ask whether the launching process is replicated for the other panel components. A replica-
tion of the analysis in columns 1 and 5 of Table 1 for
associated with significant changes in life satis-
the 1980-1988 panel, however, produces very similar
faction or marital happiness between 1983 and
results. Using 517 individuals with children 10 or
1988. The results are presented in columns 1
older in 1980, the effect of EMPTY NEST on change in
and 5 of Table 1.
marital happiness was significant and positive (b =
EMPTY NEST has a significant positive effect 1.03, p = .009) and on life satisfaction was insignifi-
on marital happiness, but not on life satisfac- cant (b = .01, p = .84).

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
EMPTYING THE NEST 239

Table 1. OLS Regression Equations Showing the Effect of Child-Leaving on Change in Marital Happiness and Life
Satisfaction Between 1983 and 1988 (Unstandardized coefficients; standard errors in parentheses)

Marital Happiness, 1988 Life Satisfaction, 1988

Total Launchersa Total Launchersa

Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

PARTIAL LAUNCH .06 -.06 ---- ---- -.03 -.05 ---- ----
(.45) (.45) ---- ---- (.07) (.07)

EMPTY NEST 1.10* 3.83* 1.21 2.83 .04 .40 .05 .06
(.53) (1.48) (.42) (1.99) (.09) (.24) (.07) (.32)

EMPTY NEST x age youngest -. 16* -.15 -.02 -.03


(.08) (.09) (.01) (.01)

EMPTY NEST X CONTACTS ---- 1.02 ---- .52*


(1.48) ---- (.24)

Control Variables, 1983

Marital happiness .77** .77* .74** .74** ---- ---- ---- ----
(.04) (.04) (.05) (.05) ----

Life satisfaction ---- ---- ---- ---- .39** .39** .41** .42**
-- ---- ---- ---- (.05) (.05) (.06) (.05)

Sex .40 .40 .19 .22 -.12* -.12* -.13* -.12


(.35) (.34) (.39) (.39) (.06) (.06) (.06) (.06)

Education -.03 -.02 -.05 -.04 .02* .03* .02 .02*


(.07) (.07) (.07) (.07) (.01) (.01) (.01) (.01)

Age .01 .02 .01 .02 -.01 -.01 -.01 -.01


(.03) (.03) (.04) (.04) (.01) (.01) (.01) (.01)

Gender role -.09 -.10 -.07 -.08 -.00 -.00 -.00 -.00
traditionalism (.07) (.07) (.08) (.08) (.01) (.01) (.01) (.01)

Boys .12 .04 -.24 -.34 .07 .06 .05 .03


(.38) (.39) (.42) (.42) (.06) (.06) (.07) (.07)

Age of youngest child .00 .05 -.02 .03 .01 .02* .02 .03**
(.05) (.05) (.05) (.06) (.01) (.01) (.04) (.01)

Number of children .30 .39 .27 .34 .03 .04 .02 .04
in household (.21) (.21) (.23) (.23) (.03) (.03) (.04) (.04)

Mother's employment .36 .34 .10 .09 -.02 -.02 -.08 -.08
(.35) (.35) (.38) (.38) (.06) (.06) (.06) (.06)

Presence of stepchildren -.51 -.36 .05 .24 -.04 -.02 -.04 -.02
(.54) (.55) (.58) (.59) (.09) (.09) (.09) (.09)

CONTACT with nonresi- ---- ---- -.57 -.81 ---- ---- .08 -.02
dent children, 1988b ---- ---- (.56) (.62) ---- ---- (.09) (.10)

Intercept 6.01 5.26 8.02 7.34 1.32 1.22 1.39 1.28

R2 .50 .50 .48 .50 .17 .17 .21 .21

N 402 317 402 317

Up <.05 p < .01

a Includes empty nest parents and partial launch parents. PARTIAL LAUNCH variable is not defined for this group.
b CONTACT is defined only for respondents with nonresident children.

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
240 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

ness scale of 1.59 points for an empty nest children) not living with you take a lot of your
respondent whose youngest child in 1983 was time and energy, quite a bit, a little, or hardly
14, compared to a decrease in marital satisfac- any?" The second is a summary of questions
tion for respondents whose youngest resident asking, for each child living away from home,
child in 1983 was 24 or older. (The contingent "How many days has it been since you have seen
intercepts are 6.0 and 6.4 respectively.) or talked to (child)? CONTACT is a dummy vari-
Although one of 16 tests would be significant able indicating whether the respondent has seen
by chance alone, we are inclined to interpret this or talked to a nonresident child in the last three
result substantively. The greater relief from the days. The measures of post-launch contact are
exit of younger children may stem from the available only for the 3 17 respondents who have
greater demands that teenage children place on some or all of their children living away from
parents compared to young adults. Age of young-
home.
est child also stands as a rough proxy for recency Only a few parents (4 percent) reported that
of the empty nest stage: everything else equal, their nonresident children required a lot of time
the younger the youngest child, the more re- and energy. This variable had no main or joint
cently the empty nest is likely to have occurred. effects on either measure of parental well-being,
Viewed in this light, the positive impact of nor did it reduce the coefficients for EMPTY NEST
recency effectively repudiates role change the- (results not shown). Regardless of how much or
ory, which predicts a strong deleterious effect how little trouble nonresident children are, the
when the change is recent. Instead, these results empty nest is associated with an increase in
suggest the possibility of a modest post-launch marital happiness and unrelated to change in life
honeymoon stage. satisfaction.
Overall, this examination of possible interac- Contact with children had no main or joint
tion effects suggests that the empty nest is effects on marital happiness (columns 3 and 4),
associated with significant improvements in indicating that no matter how often the children
marital happiness for nearly all parents, but is dropped by or called, their nonresidence status
not associated with significant changes in life improved marital happiness. There was, how-
satisfaction. This finding supports the most ever, a significant joint effect of CONTACT and
general form of the stressful role hypothesis. EMPTY NEST on life satisfaction (column 8).
The general absence of interaction effects for Among parents with nonresident children, the
the marital happiness finding suggests that even multiplicative term for age of youngest child
the best of children of the most conventional and EMPTY NEST was also significant for life
satisfaction.
parents tend to be a source of strain in the marital
relationship. The one interaction effect, between The empty nest has a positive effect on life
EMPTY NEST and age of youngest child on mari- satisfaction (b = .52) when there is contact with
tal happiness, however, suggests that this bar- nonresident children, but a negative effect (b =
rier is stronger when the children are teenagers -.40) when there has not been recent contact.4
rather than young adults. Among parents with nonresident children (be-
fore as well as after the addition of the interac-
tion term for CONTACT), there is a significant
Post-Launch Relationships
interaction for age of youngest child similar to
One plausible reason for the relatively small that for marital happiness: the lower the age of
effect of emptying the nest found in this and the youngest child, the greater the improvement
previous studies is that children continue to be in life satisfaction following the empty nest.
very much a part of their parents' lives after they
When the youngest child in 1983 was 14, the
leave home. Among respondents with nonresi-
dent children, 88 percent reported that they had 4 The coefficient for EMPTY NEST reported in
seen or talked to one of their nonresident chil- column 8 is the effect of EMPTY NEST when both
dren in the three days prior to the interview and CONTACT and age of youngest child are zero. In order
80 percent had seen or talked to a child the to evaluate the effect of each interaction term sepa-
rately, this term was recalculated at the mean for the
previous day. Out of the house is rarely out of
other variable. For example, when age of youngest
sight, much less out of mind.
child is at its mean value (15.3), then the effect of
The 1988 data set includes two measures of
empty nest without CONTACT is -.40. Similarly, when
post-launch relationships. One is a Likert-type CONTACT is at its mean (.88), then the effect of the
measure that asks, "Do obligations to (child/ empty nest when age of youngest child is zero is .52.

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
EMPTYING THE NEST 241

empty nest entailed a .10-point increase in life parents are relieved to see an end to co-resi-
satisfaction; when the youngest child in 1983 dence. The general absence of interaction ef-
was older than 18, the effect of empty nest was fects suggests that the mere presence of chil-
actually negative. Again, a plausible explana- dren -regardless of number, sex, provenance,
tion is that the positive impact of the empty nest
parent's gender roles, and other indicators of
is strongest in the period immediately after the parental stress - creates a modest obstacle for
children leave. theirparents' marriage. These findings echo and
Although the positive conditional effect of extend the work of Glenn and McLanahan
contact contradicts our initial expectations, the ( 1982): all parents experience a modest increase
strength of parental attachment and identifica- in marital happiness following the empty nest.
tion with children makes it understandable that Launching teens produces greater improve-
the positive effects of emptying the nest depend ments in both marital happiness and life satis-
on not being estranged from one's children. The faction than the launching of older children.
finding of a positive, conditional effect of con- Because none of the other indicators of parental
tact with children supports the view that parent- stress modified the effect of the empty nest, we
hood is an important, identity-affirming role. are inclined to interpret this finding as a reflec-
Although actually living with one's teen-age tion of timing rather than stress. It suggests that
children may pose obstacles to the marital rela- the positive effect of the empty nest is strongest
tionship, it appears that life satisfaction depends in the period immediately after the children
on continued affirmation of the parental role. leave home.
These data also provide support for roleiden-
tity theory: The empty nest is associated with
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
significantly greater improvement in life satis-
This study adds to the literature on the family faction when the empty nest includes frequent
life cycle by using a relatively large national contact between parents and children. Although
panel to assess changes in marital happiness and parents respond positively to the end of co-
life satisfaction associated with children leav- residence, continuation of the parental role
ing home. appears to be important to parental well-being.
Emptying the nest is associated with signifi- Our data suggest that when the empty nest
cant improvements in marital happiness, re- results in the end of the parental role, indicated
gardless of parent's or children's characteris- by infrequent contact between parent and chil-
tics. The effect however, is significantly stronger
dren, life satisfaction is reduced. For most par-
when there had been young teens in the house- ents, however, the end of co-residence does not
hold. Having entered the launching stage (hav- end the parental role. Instead, most of these
ing launched some children, but not all) does not respondents see or talk to a nonresident child
affect marital happiness. daily.
Emptying the nest has no main effect on The end of co-residence has a generally posi-
changes in life satisfaction. Analysis of parents tive effect on parental well-being, resulting in a
who have launched some or all of their children modest post-launch honeymoon. A thoughtful
shows that the empty nest is associated with examination of parenthood in our society sug-
improvements in life satisfaction under two gests that any more substantial outcome is un-
conditions: when there is frequent contact with likely. First, 20-25 years of active parenthood
the nonresident children or there were young leave a mark on dyadic relationships and indi-
teens in the household in 1983. vidual personalities that cannot be eliminated
These findings directly contradict predictions simply because the children are gone. As Lee
from role change theory. Assuming that age of (1988) notes, the distinct gender roles associ-
youngest child in the household in 1983 stands ated with parenthood have a permanent effect
as a rough proxy for recency of launch, these on parents' lives and relationships. Second, the
findings suggest that the empty nest produces impact of the empty nest is reduced by the close
greater improvement in marital happiness and ties and frequent contact between parents and
life satisfaction immediately after the children children that suggests that separate residence is
leave. a relatively minor act of disengagement. As
The generally positive effects of the empty long as the normative, economic, and psycho-
nest on parental well-being support the hy- logical links between parents and children con-
tinue, it is unrealistic to expect that a simple
pothesis that parenting is a stressful role and that

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
242 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

change of residence will raise the burdens from Lauer, Robert and Jeanette Lauer. 1986. "Factors in
parental shoulders and enable them to gambol Long-Term Marriages." Journal of Marriage and
off into a carefree sunset. the Family 7:382-90.
Lee, Gary. 1988. "Marital Satisfaction in Later Life:
The Effect of Nonmarital Roles." Journal of
LYNN WHITE is Professor of Sociology at the Univer-
Marriage and the Family 50:775-83.
sity ofNebraska-Lincoln. Her research focuses on
Luckey, Eleanore and Joyce Bain. 1970. "Children:
the family over the life course.
A Factor in Marital Satisfaction." Journal of
JOHN N. EDWARDS is Professor of Sociology at Marriage and the Family 32:43-44.
Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University. McLanahan, Sara and Julia Adams. 1987. "Parent-
The present report represents one in a series of hood and Psychological Well-Being." Annual
papers dealing with marriage over the life course de- Review of Sociology 13:237-57.
rivedfrom a national longitudinal survey of married McLanahan, S. S. and A. B. Sorensen. 1985. "Life
women and men. He is also currently involved in a Events and Psychological Well-Being Over the
study in Bankok, Thailand, of the effects of household Life Course." Pp. 217-38 in Life Course Dynam-
crowding on family relations. ics, edited by G. H. Elder. Ithaca:Cornell Univer-
sity Press.
Menaghan, Elizabeth. 1983. "Marital Stress and
REFERENCES
Family Transitions: A Panel Analysis." Journal of
Anderson, S. A., C. S. Russell, and W. R. Schumm. Marriage and the Family 45:371-86.
1983. "Perceived Marital Quality and Life-Cycle Miller, Brent and Judith Myers-Walls. 1983. "Parent-
Categories: A Further Analysis." Journal of hood: Stresses and Coping Strategies." Pp. 54-73
Marriage and the Family 45:127-39. in Stress in the Family, Vol. I: Coping wibh Norma-
Barnett, R. C. and G. K. Baruch. 1985. "Women's tive Transitions, edited by H. McCubbin and C.
Involvement in Multiple Roles and Psychological Figley. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Distress." Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Phillips, B. S. 1957. "A Role Theory Approach to
chology 49:135-45. Adjustment in Old Age." American Sociological
Bart,P. B. 1972. "Depression in Middle-Age Women." Review 22:212-17.
Pp. 163-68 in Women in Sexist Society, edited by Rollins, Boyd and Kenneth Cannon. 1974. "Marital
V. Gomick and B. K. Moran. New York: The New Satisfaction Over the Family Life Cycle: A Re-
American Library. evaluation." Journal of Marriage and the Family
Campbell, A. 1981. The Sense of Well-Being in 356:271-83.
America. NewYork: McGraw-Hill. Rollins, Boyd and H. Feldman. 1970. "Marital Satis-
Curlee, J. 1969. "Alcoholism and the Empty-Nest." faction Over the Family Life Cycle." Journal of
Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic 33:165-71. Marriage and the Family 32:20-28.
Deutscher, I. 1973. "Socialization for Postparental Schram, R. W. 1979. "Marital Satisfaction Over the
Life." Pp. 510-17 in Love, Marriage, Family: A Family Life Cycle: A Critique and Proposal."
Developmental Approach, edited by M. Lasswell Journal of Marriage and the Family 41:7-12.
and T. Lasswell.Glenview: Scott, Foresman. Spanier, G., R. A. Lewis, and C. L. Cole. 1975.
Glenn, N. D. 1975. "Psychological Well-Being in the "Marital Adjustment Over the Life Cycle: The
Post-Parental Stage: Some Evidence from Na- Issue of Curvilinearity." Journal of Marriage and
tional Surveys." Journal of Marriage and the the Family 37:263-275.
Family 37:105-10. Suitor, Jill and Karl Pillemer. 1988. "Explaining
Glenn, Norval and Sara McLanahan. 1982. "Children Intergenerational Conflict when Adult Children
and Marital Happiness: A Further Specification of and Elderly Parents Live together." Journal of
the Relationship." Journal of Marriage and the Marriage and the Family 50:1037-47.
Family 44:63-72. Thoits, Peggy. 1983. "Multiple Identities and Psy-
Goldscheider, Frances and Calvin Goldscheider. 1988. chological Well-Being: A Reformulation and Test
"Family Structure and Conflict: Nest-Leaving of the Social Isolation Hypothesis." American
Expectations of Young Adults and Their Parents." Sociological Review 48:174-87.
Journal of Marriage and the Family 51:87-97. White, Lynn and Alan Booth. 1985. "The Quality and
Harkins, E. 1978. "Effects of Empty Nest Transition Stability of Remarriages: The Role of
on Self-Report of Psychological and Physical Well- Stepchildren." American Sociological Review
Being." Journal of Marriage and the Family 50:689-98.
40:549-56. White, Lynn, Alan Booth, and John Edwards. 1986.
Holmes, J. D. and R. H. Rahe, 1967. "The Social "Children and Marital Happiness: Why the Nega-
Readjustment Rating Scale." Journal of the Psy- tive Correlation?" Journal ofFamily Issues 7:131 -
chosomatic Research 11:213-18. 49.

This content downloaded from


180.179.211.228 on Fri, 08 Mar 2024 12:11:04 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like