Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 69

Developmental Psychology:Infancy and

Childhood 5th Edition David R. Shaffer


Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmass.com/product/developmental-psychologyinfancy-and-childhood-5th-
edition-david-r-shaffer/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Developmental Psychology: Childhood and Adolescence –


Ebook PDF Version

https://ebookmass.com/product/developmental-psychology-childhood-
and-adolescence-ebook-pdf-version/

Developmental Psychology, 2e (UK Higher Education


Psychology Psychology) 2nd Edition Patrick Leman

https://ebookmass.com/product/developmental-psychology-2e-uk-
higher-education-psychology-psychology-2nd-edition-patrick-leman/

Theories of Developmental Psychology 6th Edition,


(Ebook PDF)

https://ebookmass.com/product/theories-of-developmental-
psychology-6th-edition-ebook-pdf/

Developmental Research Methods 5th Edition, (Ebook PDF)

https://ebookmass.com/product/developmental-research-methods-5th-
edition-ebook-pdf/
Developmental Psychology: The Growth of Mind and
Behavior (Ebook PDF)

https://ebookmass.com/product/developmental-psychology-the-
growth-of-mind-and-behavior-ebook-pdf/

The Developmental Psychology of Personal Identity


Massimo Marraffa

https://ebookmass.com/product/the-developmental-psychology-of-
personal-identity-massimo-marraffa/

Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics, 5th Edition Heidi


M. Feldman

https://ebookmass.com/product/developmental-behavioral-
pediatrics-5th-edition-heidi-m-feldman/

Estatística Aplicada a Administração e Economia 5th


Edition David R. Anderson

https://ebookmass.com/product/estatistica-aplicada-a-
administracao-e-economia-5th-edition-david-r-anderson/

Complete Mathematics for Cambridge IGCSE (R) 5th


Revised edition Edition David Rayner

https://ebookmass.com/product/complete-mathematics-for-cambridge-
igcse-r-5th-revised-edition-edition-david-rayner/
Fifth
Canadian
Edition

Infancy and
Childhood
Shaffer
Kipp
Wood
Willoughby
Roberts
Gottardo
Krettenauer
Lee
Newton
bowdenimages/Getty Images
MindTap empowers students.
Personalized content in an easy-to-use interface
helps you achieve better grades.

The new MindTap Mobile App allows The MindTap Reader lets you highlight
for learning anytime, anywhere with and take notes online, right within the
flashcards, quizzes and notifications. pages, and easily reference them later.

nelson.com/mindtap
Developmental
Psychology
Infancy and
Childhood
fifth canadian edition

David R. Shaffer
University of Georgia

Katherine Kipp
University of Georgia

Eileen Wood
Wilfrid Laurier University

Teena Willoughby
Brock University

Kim P. Roberts
Wilfrid Laurier University

Alexandra Gottardo
Wilfrid Laurier University

Tobias Krettenauer
Wilfrid Laurier University

Joanne Lee
Wilfrid Laurier University

Nicky Newton
Wilfrid Laurier University
This is an electronic version of the print textbook. Due to electronic rights restrictions,
some third party content may be suppressed. The publisher reserves the right to remove content
from this title at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. For valuable information
on pricing, previous editions, changes to current editions, and alternate formats, please visit
www.nelson.com to search by ISBN#, author, title, or keyword for materials in your areas
of interest.
Developmental Psychology, Fifth
Canadian Edition
by David R. Shaffer, Katherine Kipp,
Eileen Wood, Teena Willoughby,
Kim P. Roberts, Alexandra Gottardo,
Tobias Krettenauer, Joanne Lee, and
Nicky Newton

VP, Product Solutions, K–20: Production Service: Interior Design Modifications:


Claudine O’Donnell MPS Limited Sharon Lucas Creative
Senior Publisher, Digital and Print Copy Editor: Cover Design:
Content: Valerie Adams John Montgomery
Lenore Taylor-Atkins
Proofreader: Cover Image:
Senior Marketing Manager: MPS Limited SensorSpot/Getty
Claire Varley
Indexer: Compositor:
Content Manager: MPS Limited MPS Limited
Liisa Kelly
Design Director:
Photo and Permissions Researcher: Ken Phipps
Carrie McGregor
Higher Education Design PM:
Senior Production Project Manager: Pamela Johnston
Imoinda Romain

CoPyRIgHt © 2020, 2013 by Nelson All RIgHtS RESERVED. No part of Library and Archives Canada
Education Ltd. this work covered by the copyright Cataloguing in Publication
herein may be reproduced,
Adapted from Developmental transcribed, or used in any form Shaffer, David R. (David Reed),
Psychology, Ninth Edition, by David or by any means—graphic, author
R. Shaffer and Katherine Kipp, electronic, or mechanical, including Developmental psychology :
published by Wadsworth. Copyright photocopying, recording, taping, infancy and childhood / David
©2014 by Cengage Learning. Web distribution, or information R. Shaffer (University of Georgia),
storage and retrieval systems— Katherine Kipp (University of
Printed and bound in Canada Georgia), Eileen Wood (Wilfrid
1 2 3 4 22 21 20 19 without the written permission of
the publisher. Laurier University), Teena
For more information contact Willoughby (Brock University), Kim P.
Nelson Education Ltd., For permission to use material Roberts, Alexandra Gottardo, Tobias
1120 Birchmount Road, Toronto, from this text or product, Krettenauer, Joanne Lee, Nicky
Ontario, M1K 5G4. Or you can visit submit all requests online at Newton. — Fifth Canadian edition.
our Internet site at nelson.com cengage.com/permissions.
Further questions about Includes bibliographical references
Cognero and Full-Circle Assessment permissions can be emailed to and index.
are registered trademarks of permissionrequest@cengage.com Issued in print and electronic
Madeira Station LLC. formats.
Every effort has been made to ISBN 978-0-17-687397-4
trace ownership of all copyrighted (softcover).—ISBN 978-0-17-687607-4
material and to secure permission (PDF)
from copyright holders. In the event
of any question arising as to the use 1. Child psychology—Textbooks.
of any material, we will be pleased 2. Adolescent psychology—
to make the necessary corrections in Textbooks. 3. Textbooks. I. Title.
future printings. BF721.S4688 2019 155.4
C2018-906422-6
C2018-906423-4

ISBN-13: 978-0-17-687397-4
ISBN-10: 0-17-687397-X
Brief Contents
Preface xiii

PArt I theory AnD reseArCh In the DeveLoPmentAL sCIenCes


Chapter 1 Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its research strategies 1
Chapter 2 theories of human Development 36

PArt II FounDAtIons oF DeveLoPment


Chapter 3 hereditary Influences on Development 68
Chapter 4 Prenatal Development 98
Chapter 5 Birth and the newborn’s readiness for Life 123
Chapter 6 Physical Development: the Brain, Body, motor skills, and the Beginnings
of sexual Development 145

PArt III LAnguAge, LeArnIng, AnD CognItIve DeveLoPment


Chapter 7 early Cognitive Foundations: sensation, Perception, and Learning 178
Chapter 8 Cognitive Development: Piaget’s theory, Case’s neo-Piagetian theory,
and vygotsky’s sociocultural viewpoint 218
Chapter 9 Cognitive Development: Information-Processing Perspectives
and Connectionism 264
Chapter 10 Intelligence: measuring mental Performance 307
Chapter 11 Development of Language and Communication skills 348

PArt Iv soCIAL AnD PersonALIty DeveLoPment


Chapter 12 emotional Development, temperament, and Attachment 394
Chapter 13 Development of the self and social Cognition 429
Chapter 14 sex Differences and similarities, and gender-role Development 463
Chapter 15 moral Development and Aggression 498

PArt v the eCoLogy oF DeveLoPment


Chapter 16 the Family 538
Chapter 17 Beyond the Family Context: Peers, schools, and media 576

glossary g-1
references r-1
name Index I-1
subject Index I-23

NEL iii
Contents
Preface xiii Learning Theories 42
Watson’s Behaviourism 42
Skinner’s Operant Learning Theory 42
Bandura’s Cognitive Social Learning
PArt I Theory 43
theory AnD reseArCh In the n Box 2.1 FoCus on reseArCh: An Example of
Observational Learning 44
DeveLoPmentAL sCIenCes Contributions and Criticisms
of Learning Theories 45
ChAPter 1
Cognitive-Developmental Theories 47
Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its
Piaget’s View of Intelligence
research strategies 1 and Intellectual Growth 47
Introduction to Developmental Psychology 1 Contributions and Criticisms
What Is Development? 2 of Piaget’s Viewpoint 48
The Scientific Study of Development and Its Origins 6 Sociocultural Theories 49
Early Philosophical Perspectives on Childhood 6 Contributions and Criticisms
Children as Subjects of Study: Baby Biographies of Vygotsky’s Viewpoint 50
and Diaries 7 Information-Processing Theories 50
Research Methods in Developmental Psychology 8 Contributions and Criticisms of the
Gathering Data: Basic Fact-Finding Strategies 8 Information-Processing Viewpoint 51
Detecting Relationships: Correlational and Ethological and Evolutionary Theories 52
Experimental Designs 16 Assumptions of Classical Ethology 52
Research Strategies in Developmental Psychology 23 Ethology and Human Development 52
Research Designs for Studying Development 23 Evolutionary Theory 53
Ethical Considerations in Developmental Research 30 Contributions and Criticisms of the Ethological
What Makes a Research Study Ethical and Evolutionary Viewpoints 54
or Unethical? 31 Ecological Systems Theory 55
Postscript: On Becoming a Wise User Contexts for Development 55
of Developmental Research 32 Family and the Ecological Systems Theory 57
Summary 33 Contributions and Criticisms
of the Ecological Systems Theory 58
Key Terms 35
Themes in the Study of Human Development:
Questions and Controversies 59
ChAPter 2
The Nature/Nurture Issue 59
theories of human Development 36
The Active/Passive Issue 60
The Nature of Scientific Theories 36 The Continuity/Discontinuity Issue 60
Psychoanalytic Theories 38 The Holistic Nature of Development Issue 61
Freud’s Psychosexual Theory 38 Theories and World Views 62
Contributions and Criticisms of Freud’s Theory 39 The Developmental Systems View 63
Erikson’s Theory of Psychosocial Development 40
Summary 65
Contributions and Criticisms of Erikson’s Theory 40
Psychoanalytic Theory beyond Freud and Erikson 41 Key Terms 66

NEL v
vi Contents

PArt II The Period of the Embryo 101


The Period of the Fetus 101
FounDAtIons oF DeveLoPment Environmental Influences on Prenatal
ChAPter 3 Development 105
hereditary Influences on Development 68 Teratogens 105
Principles of Hereditary Transmission 68 n Box 4.1 DeveLoPmentAL Issues: Teratogenic
The Genetic Material 69 Effects of Sexually Transmitted Diseases/
Growth of the Zygote and Production Infections 108
of Body Cells 70 n Box 4.2 the InsIDe trACk: Joanne Rovet /
Germlines 70 Kelly Nash 112
Multiple Births 71 Environmental Hazards 114
Male or Female? 72 Maternal Characteristics 116
What Do Genes Do? 72 Prevention of Birth Defects 120
How Important Are Environmental Applying Developmental Themes
Influences? 73 to Prenatal Development 120
How Do Phenotypes Develop Summary 122
through Genotypes? 73
Key Terms 122
n Box 3.1 DeveLoPmentAL Issues: Examples of
Dominant and Recessive Traits in Human
Heredity 75 ChAPter 5
Polygenic Inheritance 76
Birth and the newborn’s readiness
The Role of Epigenetics 76
for Life 123
Hereditary Disorders 77
Childbirth and the Perinatal Environment 123
Chromosomal Abnormalities 78
The Birth Process 123
Genetic Abnormalities 80
The Baby’s Experience 124
Predicting, Detecting, and Treating Hereditary
Labour and Delivery Medication 126
Disorders 81
The Social Environment Surrounding
n Box 3.2 Current ControversIes: Ethical Issues
Birth 128
Surrounding Treatments for Hereditary
n Box 5.1 the InsIDe trACk: Ann Bigelow 130
Disorders 84
Birth Complications 130
Hereditary Influences on Behaviour 86
Anoxia 130
Methods of Studying Hereditary Influences 86
Low Birth Weight 131
Estimating the Contribution of Genes
and Environment 86 Reproductive Risk and Capacity for
Recovery 134
Hereditary Contributions to Personality 89
Applying Developmental Themes to Birth 135
Hereditary Contributions to Behaviour Disorders
and Mental Illness 89 The Newborn’s Readiness for Life 136
Theories of Heredity and Environment Interactions Newborn Reflexes 137
in Development 90 Infant States 138
The Canalization Principle 90 Developmental Changes in Infant States 139
The Range-of-Reaction Principle 91 n Box 5.2 DeveLoPmentAL Issues: Sudden Infant
From Genotype to Environment 91 Death Syndrome 140
n Box 5.3 APPLyIng DeveLoPmentAL reseArCh:
How Do Genotype/Environment Interactions
Influence Development? 92 Methods of Soothing a Fussy Baby 142

Contributions and Criticisms of the Summary 143


Behavioural Genetics Approach 94 Key Terms 144
Applying Developmental Themes to Hereditary
Influences on Development 95 ChAPter 6
Summary 95 Physical Development: the Brain,
Key Terms 97 Body, motor skills, and the Beginnings
of sexual Development 145
ChAPter 4 An Overview of Maturation and Growth 146
Prenatal Development 98 Changes in Height and Weight 146
From Conception to Birth 99 Changes in Body Proportions 147
The Period of the Zygote 99 Skeletal Development 148

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Contents vii

Muscular Development 148 n Box 7.1 FoCus on reseArCh: Causes and


Variations in Physical Development 148 Consequences of Hearing Loss 184
Development of the Brain 149 Taste and Smell 185
Neural Development and Plasticity 149 Touch, Temperature, and Pain 186
n Box 6.1 the InsIDe trACk: Dr. Sid Segalowitz 150
Vision 187
n Box 6.2 the InsIDe trACk: Bryan Kolb 152 Visual Perception in Infancy 189
Brain Differentiation and Growth 153 Perception of Patterns and Forms 189
n Box 7.2 the InsIDe trACk: Daphne Maurer 192
Motor Development 156
Basic Trends in Locomotor Development 157 Perception of Three-Dimensional Space 193
Fine Motor Development 160 Intermodal Perception 197
Psychological Implications of Early Motor Are the Senses Integrated at Birth? 197
Development 162 Development of Intermodal Perception 198
Beyond Infancy: Motor Development Explaining Intermodal Perception 199
in Childhood 163 Infant Perception in Perspective—And a
n Box 6.3 APPLyIng reseArCh to your LIFe: Look Ahead 200
Exercise: The Key to a Healthy Childhood 164 Perceptual Learning in Childhood: Gibson’s
The Onset of Puberty: The Early Beginnings of the Differentiation Theory 200
Physical Transition from Child to Adolescent 165 Cultural Influences on Perception 201
Sexual Maturation in Girls 165 Basic Learning Processes 203
Sexual Maturation in Boys 166 Habituation: Early Evidence of Information
Individual Differences in and Sexual Maturation: Processing and Memory 203
Early Onset 166 Classical Conditioning 204
Secular Trends—Are We Maturing Earlier? 166 Operant (or Instrumental) Conditioning 206
Does Timing of Puberty Matter? 167 Operant Conditioning in Infancy 208
Causes and Correlates of Physical Development 168 Observational Learning 210
Biological Mechanisms 168 n Box 7.3 APPLyIng DeveLoPmentAL reseArCh:
Environmental Influences 169 Corporal Punishment— Cultural Ideals and
Applying Developmental Themes to Physical Alternatives 211
Development 173 Applying Developmental Themes to Infant
Summary 175 Development, Perception, and Learning 214

Key Terms 176 Summary 215


Key Terms 217

PArt III ChAPter 8


LAnguAge, LeArnIng, AnD Cognitive Development: Piaget’s theory, Case’s
CognItIve DeveLoPment neo-Piagetian theory, and vygotsky’s sociocultural
viewpoint 218
ChAPter 7 Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development 219
early Cognitive Foundations: sensation, Perception, What Is Intelligence? 219
and Learning 178 How We Gain Knowledge: Cognitive Schemes
Early Controversies about Sensory and Perceptual and Cognitive Processes 220
Development 179 Piaget’s Stages of Cognitive Development 221
Nature versus Nurture 179 The Sensorimotor Stage (Birth to 2 Years) 221
Enrichment versus Differentiation 179 n Box 8.1 DeveLoPmentAL Issues: Why Infants
Research Methods Used to Study the Infant’s Sensory Know More about Objects than Piaget
and Perceptual Experiences 180 Assumed 227
The Preference Method 180 The Preoperational Stage (2 to 7 Years) and the
Emergence of Symbolic Thought 230
The Habituation Method 180
n Box 8.2 DeveLoPmentAL Issues: Play Is Serious
The High-Amplitude Sucking Method 181
Business 232
The Evoked Potentials Method 182
n Box 8.3 the InsIDe trACk: Kang Lee 240
Brain Imaging Techniques 182
n Box 8.4 FoCus on reseArCh: Is Theory of Mind
Infant Sensory Capabilities 183 Biologically Programmed? The Special Case
Hearing 183 of Autism Spectrum Disorder 241

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
viii Contents

The Concrete-Operational Stage Cognitive Inhibition: Dismissing


(7 to 11 Years) 242 Irrelevant Information 281
The Formal-Operational Stage Meta-attention: What Do Children Know about
(11 to 12 Years and Beyond) 244 Attention? 282
An Evaluation of Piaget’s Theory 246 Alternative Models of Memory: Fuzzy Traces
Piaget’s Contributions 246 and Scripts 283
Challenges to Piaget 247 Fuzzy-Trace Theory 283
Case’s Neo-Piagetian Theory 248 Schemas 284

Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Perspective 250 The Development of Event Memory 286


The Role of Culture in Intellectual Origins of Event Memory 286
Development 250 n Box 9.3 applyIng researCh to your lIfe:
The Social Origins of Early Cognitive What Happened to Our Early Childhood
Competencies 252 Memories? 287
Implications for Education 255 The Social Construction of Autobiographical
Memories 288
The Role of Language in Cognitive
Development 256 Summing Up 289
Vygotsky in Perspective: Summary Children as Eyewitnesses 290
and Evaluation 257 How Suggestible Are Child Witnesses? 291
Applying Developmental Themes to Piaget’s Implications for Legal Testimony 292
and Vygotsky’s Theories 260 The Development of Analogical Reasoning 292
Summing Up 261 The Development of Number and Arithmetic
Summary 261 Skills 294
Counting and Arithmetic Strategies 294
Key Terms 262
n Box 9.4 the InsIDe traCk: Jeff Bisanz 296
Evaluating the Information-Processing
Chapter 9
Perspective 299
Cognitive Development: Information-processing n Box 9.5 applyIng researCh to your lIfe: Some
perspectives and Connectionism 264 Educational Implications of Research on Attention
Information Flow and the Multistore Model 265 and Memory 300
Cognitive Processes and the Multistore Connectionist Approaches to Cognitive
Model 266 Development 301
Developmental Differences in “Hardware”: The Origins of Connectionism 301
Information-Processing Capacity 267 How Are Networks Created? 301
Development of the Short-Term Store 267 How Connectionist Networks Work 302
Knowledge Base and Memory Development 269 Connectionism and Development 302

Developmental Differences in “Software”: Applying Developmental Themes to Information-


Strategies 270 Processing Perspectives 303
Rehearsal 271 Summary 304
Organization 272 Key Terms 306
Elaboration 272
Production and Utilization Deficiencies 273
Chapter 10
Multiple Strategy and Variable Strategy Use 275
Intelligence: Measuring Mental performance 307
The Development of Metacognition and Executive
What Is Intelligence? 308
Control Processes 276
Psychometric Views of Intelligence 308
Knowledge and Reasoning 277
The Modern Information-Processing
n Box 9.1 the InsIDe traCk: Ori Friedman 278
Viewpoint 312
Retention and the Development of Attention 279 Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelligences 314
Changes in Attention Span 279
How Is Intelligence Measured? 315
n Box 9.2 the InsIDe traCk: Kimberly Schonert-
The Wechsler Scales 316
Reichl 280
n Box 10.1 Cultural InfluenCes: Making American
Development of Planful Attentional
Tests Valid in Canada 317
Strategies 280
n Box 10.2 the InsIDe traCk: Donald H. Saklofske 318
Selective Attention: Ignoring Information That Is
Clearly Irrelevant 281 Distribution of IQ Scores 318

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Contents ix

Group Tests of Mental Performance 319 Producing Sounds: The Infant’s Prelinguistic
Newer Approaches to Intelligence Testing 319 Vocalizations 362
Assessing Infant Intelligence 319 What Do Prelinguistic Infants Know about Language
and Communication? 363
What Do Intelligence Tests Predict? 322
IQ as a Predictor of Scholastic Achievement 322 The Holophrastic Period: One Word at a Time 365
IQ as a Predictor of Health, Adjustment, and Life Early Semantics: Building a Vocabulary 365
Satisfaction 323 n Box 11.2 the InsIDe trACk: Janet F. Werker 366
Factors That Influence IQ Scores 326 Attaching Meaning to Words 367
The Evidence for Heredity 326 When a Word Is More than a Word 371
The Evidence for Environment 327 The Telegraphic Period: From Holophrases to Simple
Sentences 371
Social and Cultural Correlates of Intellectual
Performance 328 A Semantic Analysis of Telegraphic
Speech 372
Home Environment and IQ 328
The Pragmatics of Early Speech 373
Social Class, Culture, Race, and Ethnic Differences
in IQ 331 n Box 11.3 APPLyIng reseArCh to your LIFe:
Learning a Gestural Language 374
Why Do Groups Differ in Intellectual
Performance? 332 Language Learning during the Preschool
Period 375
Improving Cognitive Performance through
Compensatory Education 337 Grammatical Development 376
Long-Term Follow-Ups 338 Semantic Development 378
The Importance of Parental Involvement 338 Development of Pragmatics
and Communication Skills 379
The Importance of Intervening Early 339
n Box 10.3 APPLyIng DeveLoPmentAL reseArCh: Language Learning during Middle Childhood 380
An Effective Compensatory Intervention Later Syntactic Development 381
for Families 339 Semantics and Metalinguistic Awareness 381
Creativity and Special Talents 340 Further Development of
What Is Creativity? 340 Communication Skills 382
The Psychometric Perspective 341 Bilingualism: Learning More than One Language 385
The Multicomponent (or Confluence) n Box 11.4 the InsIDe trACk: Johanne Paradis 387
Perspective 341 n Box 11.5 the InsIDe trACk: Ellen Bialystok 388
Sternberg and Lubart’s Investment Theory 342 Applying Developmental Themes to Language
Applying Developmental Themes to Intelligence Acquisition 390
and Creativity 345 Summary 391
Summary 345 Key Terms 392
Key Terms 347

ChAPter 11
PArt Iv
Development of Language and Communication
skills 348 soCIAL AnD PersonALIty
The Five Components of Language 349 DeveLoPment
Phonology 349
Morphology 349 ChAPter 12
Semantics 349 emotional Development, temperament,
Syntax 350 and Attachment 394
Pragmatics 350
Emotional Development 394
Theories of Language Development 350 Displaying Emotions: The Development
The Learning (or Empiricist) Perspective 351 (and Control) of Emotional
The Nativist Perspective 352 Expressions 395
n Box 11.1 FoCus on reseArCh: On the “Invention” of Recognizing and Interpreting Emotions 399
Language by Children 355 Emotions and Early Social Development 401
The Interactionist Perspective 356 Temperament and Development 402
The Prelinguistic Period: Before Language 361 Hereditary and Environmental Influences on
Early Reactions to Speech 361 Temperament 403

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
x Contents

Stability of Temperament 405 ChAPter 14


Early Temperamental Profiles sex Differences and similarities, and gender-role
and Later Development 406 Development 463
n Box 12.1 the InsIDe trACk: Elizabeth J. Hayden 406
Defining Sex and Gender 464
Attachment and Development 409
Categorizing Males and Females: Gender-Role
Attachments as Reciprocal Relationships 409
Standards 464
How Do Infants Become Attached? 410
n Box 14.1 DeveLoPmentAL Issues: What Traits
Individual Differences in Attachment Characterize Males and Females? 466
Quality 414
Factors That Influence Attachment Security 418 Some Facts and Fictions about Sex Differences 466
Attachment and Later Development 421 Actual Psychological Differences between
the Sexes 466
n Box 12.2 the InsIDe trACk: Ellen Moss 424
Cultural Myths 469
Applying Developmental Themes to Emotional Do Cultural Myths Contribute to Sex Differences in
Development, Temperament, and Attachment 425 Ability (and Vocational Opportunity)? 470
Summary 426 Developmental Trends in Gender Typing 472
Key Terms 427 Development of Gender Identity 472
Development of Gender-Role Stereotypes 473
ChAPter 13 n Box 14.2 the InsIDe trACk: Lisa Serbin 474
Development of the self and social Cognition 429 Development of Gender-Typed Behaviour 475
Development of the Self-Concept 429 Theories of Gender Typing and Gender-Role
Self-Differentiation in Infancy 430 Development 478
Self-Recognition in Infancy 430 Evolutionary Theory 478
Contributors to Self-Recognition 432 A Biosocial Overview of Gender Differentiation and
Social and Emotional Consequences Development 479
of Self-Recognition 433 n Box 14.3 FoCus on reseArCh: Is Biology Destiny? 482
“Who Am I?” Responses of Preschool Social-Learning Theory 484
Children 434 Kohlberg’s Cognitive-Developmental Theory 486
Conceptions of Self in Middle Childhood 434 Gender Schema Theory 487
Who Am I to Be? Identity as an Extension An Integrative Theory 488
of Self-Concept 436 Psychological Androgyny: A Prescription
Self-Esteem: The Evaluative Component for the 21st Century? 489
of Self 436 Do Androgynous People Really Exist? 490
Origins and Development of Self-Esteem 436 Are There Advantages to Being Androgynous? 491
n Box 13.1 the InsIDe trACk: Shelley Hymel 439 Applications: On Changing Gender-Role Attitudes
n Box 13.2 the InsIDe trACk: Joanne and Behaviour 492
Cummings 439 n Box 14.4 APPLyIng reseArCh to your LIFe:
Social Contributors to Self-Esteem 440 Combating Gender Stereotypes with Cognitive
Interventions 494
Development of Achievement Motivation and
Academic Self-Concept 442 Applying Developmental Themes to Sex Differences
Early Origins of Achievement Motivation 442 and Gender-Role Development 495
Achievement Motivation during Summary 495
Middle Childhood 443 Key Terms 496
Beyond Achievement Motivation: Development of
Achievement Attributions 447
ChAPter 15
The Other Side of Social Cognition: Knowing about moral Development and Aggression 498
Others 452
Defining the Moral Domain 499
Age Trends in Person Perception 452
Moral Foundations Theory 499
n Box 13.3 DeveLoPmentAL Issues: Racial
Turiel’s Social Domain Theory 500
Categorization and Racism in Young Children 453
The Anthropologist View 501
Theories of Social-Cognitive Development 455
Evolutionary Roots of Morality in Young
Applying Developmental Themes to the Development
Children 502
of the Self and Social Cognition 459
Empathy and Compassion 502
Summary 460 Social Preference for Helpfulness 503
Key Terms 461 Prosocial Helping 503

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Contents xi

Beyond Nature— Cultural Influences on Sympathy n Box 16.1 the InsIDe trACk: Hildy Ross 552
and Prosocial Behaviour 504 Positive Contributions of Sibling
Rule Internalization in the Context of Close Relationships 553
Relationships 505 Characteristics of Only Children 554
Positive Reinforcement in the Context Diversity in Family Life 555
of Close Relationships 506 Adoptive Families 556
n Box 15.1 APPLyIng reseArCh to your LIFe: How Donor Insemination (DI) Families 556
Should I Discipline My Children? 509 Gay and Lesbian Families 557
Social-Modelling Influences 510 Family Conflict and Divorce 558
Moral Reasoning Development 511 Remarriage and Blended Families 560
Piaget’s Theory of Moral Development 511 n Box 16.2 APPLyIng DeveLoPmentAL reseArCh:
Updates to Piaget’s Theory 513 Smoothing the Rocky Road to Recovery
Kohlberg’s Theory of Moral Development 516 from a Divorce 561
Empirical Support for Kohlberg’s Theory 519 Down the Hidden Side of Family Life: The Problem
Criticisms of Kohlberg’s Approach 522 of Child Abuse 564
The Development of Aggression 524 How Prevalent Is Child Abuse
and Neglect? 564
Aggressive Behaviour in Infancy and Childhood 525
Is There a Typical Profile of an Abuser? 565
Aggressiveness as a Trait: How Stable Is It? 526 Who Is Abused? 566
Sex Differences in Aggressive Behaviour 527 Consequences of Abuse and Neglect 568
Aggression as a Behavioural Problem 528 n Box 16.3 the InsIDe trACk: David Wolfe 569
Dodge’s Social Information-Processing Theory of How Can We Reduce the Prevalence
Aggression 528 of Abuse and Neglect? What Works? 570
n Box 15.2 the InsIDe trACk: Tina Malti 530 Applying Developmental Themes to Family Life,
Social and Cultural and Influences on Aggression 530 Parenting, and Siblings 572
Coercive Home Environments: Breeding Grounds for Summary 573
Aggression 531
Key Terms 574
Methods of Controlling Aggression
in Young Children 532
ChAPter 17
Applying Developmental Themes to Moral
Beyond the Family Context: Peers, schools,
Development and Aggression 535
and media 576
Summary 535
Peers as Agents of Socialization 576
Key Terms 536 Who Is a Peer and What Functions
Do Peers Serve? 577
The Development of Peer Sociability 578
PArt v Peer Acceptance and Popularity 582
Children and Their Friends 585
the eCoLogy oF DeveLoPment n Box 17.1 the InsIDe trACk: William Bukowski 585
n Box 17.2 DeveLoPmentAL Issues:
ChAPter 16
A Longitudinal Analysis of the Benefits
the Family 538 of Chumships 588
Understanding the Family 538 n Box 17.3 APPLyIng DeveLoPmentAL reseArCh:
The Family as a Social System 539 On Improving the Social Skills of Unpopular
Families Are Developing Systems 540 Children 589
Parental Socialization during Childhood 543 Parents and Peers as Influence Agents 590
Two Major Dimensions of Parenting 544 Increasing Conformity to Peers 590
Four Patterns of Parenting 544 School as a Socialization Agent 591
Social Class and Ethnic Variations How Well Educated Are Our Children?
in Child Rearing 548 A Cross-Cultural Comparison 591
The Influence of Siblings and Sibling n Box 17.4 APPLyIng reseArCh to your LIFe:
Relationships 551 Should Preschoolers Attend
Changes in the Family System School? 592
when a New Baby Arrives 551 Determinants of Effective Schooling 596
Sibling Relationships over the Course Do Our Schools Meet the Needs
of Childhood 551 of All of Our Children? 599

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
xii Contents

Effects of Media Technologies on Summary 611


Child Development 602 Key Terms 613
Development of Media Literacy 603
Some Potentially Undesirable Effects of Screen glossary g-1
Media Technologies 604
references r-1
Some Desirable Developmental Outcomes from
Screen Media Technologies 606 name Index I-1
subject Index I-23
Computer Technologies and Cognitive Development 608
The Added Impact of the Internet 608
Applying Developmental Themes to Extrafamilial
Contextual Forces in Child Development 610

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Preface

You will note that this fifth Canadian edition of Developmental Psychology: Infancy and
Childhood differs significantly from its predecessors. The revised title reflects a change in
focus, and the list of authors reflects a collaborative effort to create a timely, meaningful,
and accurate developmental textbook for Canadian students. Although this text has
always been written by multiple authors, the fifth Canadian edition is unique as it reflects
the expertise of seven Canadian developmental researchers (along with two original
authors from the United States) working together to create a comprehensive yet stream-
lined text that reflects the many domains of developmental psychology. In fact, the order
of authorship among the Canadian contributors was decided randomly as all contributed
equally to this edition. This edition is also unique in its focus. Where earlier editions
examined development through childhood and adolescence, this edition has been stream-
lined to focus on childhood (starting from before birth up until about 12 years of age). In
making this transition, we were able to further refine, update, and elaborate on the
information presented in our fourth edition. Importantly, given the time between the
fourth and fifth editions, considerable revisions were made to accommodate the fast pace
of change in the research literature. We have continued to couch the content in terms of
issues faced by Canadians, but we have also incorporated research from a range of coun-
tries to provide as complete a picture as possible.
Like the previous edition, this text provides an overview of child development that
reflects the best theories, research, and practical advice that developmentalists have to
offer. It is a substantive developmental text that we hope is interesting, accurate, up to
date, and written in clear, concise language that both introductory and more advanced
students can easily understand. We believe that a good text should talk to rather than at
its readers. It should anticipate their interests, questions, and concerns. It should treat
them as active participants in the learning process. It should stress the processes that
underlie developmental change, so that students come away from the course with a firm
understanding of the causes and complexities of development. We wanted this text to
challenge students to think about the fascinating process of human development, to
share in the excitement of our dynamic discipline, and to acquire a knowledge of devel-
opmental principles that will serve them well in their roles as parents, teachers, nurses,
childcare workers, pediatricians, psychologists, nurses, academics, or in any other capacity
in which they may one day influence the lives of developing individuals.

Philosophy
Certain philosophical views underlie any systematic treatment of a field as broad as child
development. The philosophy that guided the construction of this text can be summa-
rized as follows.

NEL xiii
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
xiv Preface

We Believe in Theoretical Eclecticism


Many theories have contributed to our knowledge about child development, and this
theoretical diversity is a strength rather than a weakness. Some theories may do a better
job than others of explaining particular aspects of development, but we see time and
again that different theories emphasize different aspects of development and that knowledge of
many theories is necessary to explain the course and complexities of human develop-
ment. So this book does not attempt to convince its readers that any one theoretical
viewpoint is best. The behaviouristic, cognitive-developmental, ecological, sociocultural,
information processing, ethological, behavioural, and genetic/biological viewpoints (as
well as several less encompassing theories that address selected aspects of development)
are all treated with respect.

The Best Information about Human Development


Comes from Systematic Research
To teach this course effectively, one must convince students of the value of theory
and systematic research. Although there are many ways to achieve that objective,
this text discusses the many methodological approaches that researchers use to test
their theories and answer questions about developing children. Care has been taken
to explain why there is no one best method for studying developing individuals and
why our most reliable findings are those that can be replicated using a variety
of methods.

We Believe in a Strong Process Orientation


In recent years, investigators have become increasingly concerned about identifying and
understanding developmental processes—the biological and environmental factors that
cause us to change—and this book reflects that concern. We believe that students are
more likely to remember what develops, and when, if they know and understand the
reasons that these developments take place.

We Believe in a Strong Contextual Orientation


One important lesson that developmentalists have learned is that children live in histor-
ical eras and sociocultural contexts that affect every aspect of their development. We
have highlighted these contextual influences in two ways. First, cross-cultural comparisons
are discussed in various places throughout the text. Cross-cultural research helps us see
how human beings can be so much alike and, at the same time, so different from one
another. Second, the impacts of such immediate contextual influences as our families,
neighbourhoods, schools, and peer groups are considered throughout the first 15 chap-
ters as we discuss each aspect of human development, and again in the final two chapters
as important topics in their own right.

Human Development Is a Holistic Process


Although individual researchers may concentrate on particular topics such as physical
development, cognitive development, or the development of moral reasoning, develop-
ment is not piecemeal but holistic: human beings are at once physical, cognitive, social,
and emotional creatures, and each of these components of self depends, in part, on
changes taking place in other areas of development. This holistic perspective is empha-
sized throughout the text.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Preface xv

A Developmental Text Should Be a Resource


Book for Students— One That Reflects Current
Knowledge
This text cites many new studies and new programs of research—many are “hot off the
press” (see The Inside Track boxes)—to ensure that our coverage represents the most
up-to-date understanding of topics in developmental psychology. However, we have
avoided the tendency to ignore older research simply because it is older. Many of the
classics of our discipline are featured prominently throughout the text to illustrate impor-
tant breakthroughs and to show how our knowledge about development builds on earlier
findings and insights.

organization
There are two traditional ways of presenting human development. In the chronological,
or “ages and stages,” approach, the coverage begins at conception and proceeds through
the lifespan, using ages or chronological periods as the organizing principle. By contrast,
the topical approach is organized around areas of development and follows each from its
origins to its mature forms. Each of these presentations has its advantages and disadvan-
tages. On the one hand, a chronological focus highlights the holistic character of devel-
opment but may obscure the links between early and later events within each
developmental domain. On the other hand, a topical approach highlights developmental
sequences and processes but at the risk of failing to convey that development is holistic
in nature.
This book is organized topically to focus on developmental processes and to provide
the student with an uninterrupted view of the sequences of change that children experi-
ence within each developmental domain. In our opinion, this topical approach best
allows the reader to appreciate the flow of development—the systematic, and often truly
dramatic transformations that take place over the course of childhood, as well as the
developmental continuities that make each individual a reflection of his or her past self.
At the same time, we consider it essential to paint a holistic portrait of the developing
person. To accomplish this aim, we have stressed the fundamental interplay among bio-
logical, cognitive, social, and cultural influences for each and every aspect of development.
So, even though this text is organized topically, students will not lose sight of the whole
person and the holistic character of human development.

Content
We made an effort to retain in this edition those qualities of earlier editions that students
and professors say they like. One such quality is the division of the book into five parts.
n■ Part I: Theory and Research in the Development Sciences. This first part
presents an orientation to the discipline and the tools of the trade, including a
thorough discussion and illustration of research methodologies in Chapter 1
and a succinct review of the major theories of human development in Chapter 2.
These chapters illustrate why research methods and theories are important to
an understanding of human development. The coverage also analyzes the con-
tributions and the limitations of each research method and each major theory.
n■ Part II: Foundations of Development. Chapters 3 to 6 address foundations of
development strongly influenced by biological factors. Chapter 3 focuses on
hereditary contributions to human development and illustrates how genes and
environments interact to influence most human characteristics. Chapters 4 and
5 focus on prenatal development and on the many prenatal and perinatal

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
xvi Preface

environmental factors that influence a newborn’s health and readiness for


adapting to the world outside the womb. Chapter 6 is devoted to physical
growth, including the development of the brain and motor skills.
n■ Part III: Language, Learning, and Cognitive Development. The five chapters
of Part III address the many theories and the voluminous research pertaining to
the development of language, learning capabilities, and intellectual develop-
ment. Chapter 7 begins exploring the growth of perceptual and learning capa-
bilities—two crucial cognitive foundations for many other aspects of
development. Chapter 8 is devoted to major viewpoints of intellectual growth
including Piaget’s cognitive-developmental theory, Case’s neo-Piagetian theory,
and Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory. These theories are covered in detail, for
each is important to understanding the social, emotional, and language develop-
ments that are covered in later chapters. Chapter 9 explores the information-
processing viewpoint and connectionist models. The application of
information-processing research in everyday contexts is covered through topics
such as reasoning and mathematics. Chapter 10 focuses on individual differences
in intellectual performance. Here we review the intelligence testing movement,
the many factors that influence children’s IQ scores, and the merits of compen-
satory interventions designed to improve intellectual performance. The chapter
concludes with a discussion of creative abilities and their development. Chapter 11
explores the fascinating topic of language development and addresses a number
of intriguing questions such as, Do children acquire language more easily than
adults? Is sign language a true language? Does bilingualism promote or inhibit
linguistic proficiency and cognitive development?
n■ Part IV: Social and Personality Development. The next four chapters focus on
crucial aspects of social and personality development. Chapter 12 examines the
process of emotional development, the developmental significance of individual
differences in temperament, and the growth and implications for later develop-
ment of the emotional attachments that children form with their close compan-
ions. Chapter 13, on the self, traces the development of the self-concept and
children’s emerging sense of self-esteem, and the growth of social cognition and
interpersonal understanding. Chapter 14 focuses on sex differences and similari-
ties and on how biological factors, social forces, and intellectual growth can
interact to steer males and females toward different gender roles. The chapter
also examines the utility (or lack thereof ) of traditional gender roles and dis-
cusses ways in which we might be more successful at combating unfounded
gender stereotypes. Chapter 15 examines interrelated aspects of social develop-
ment that people often consider when making judgments about one’s character:
moral development and aggression.
n■ Part V: The Ecology of Development. The final section of the text concen-
trates on the settings, or contexts, in which people develop—the ecology of
development. Chapter 16 is devoted to family influences, focusing on the
functions that families serve, patterns of child rearing that foster adaptive or
maladaptive outcomes, the impacts of siblings on developing children, and
the effects of family diversities and family transitions on child development.
Chapter 17 concludes the text with an in-depth examination of three extra-
familial influences on developing children: peers, schools, and the impact
of media.

new to the Fifth Canadian edition


One of the most challenging tasks we face when writing a Canadian edition is under-
standing what defines a Canadian text. What should a Canadian text in developmental
psychology look like compared to the American texts we are accustomed to seeing?
Although, for the most part, Canadian and American researchers investigate

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Preface xvii

developmental psychology using the same theories, philosophies, designs, methods, and
analyses, we acknowledge that there are differences—some obvious, some not—in how
research is conducted.
One of the obvious areas of difference between the two cultures is that of legal
issues. Canadians and Americans have different legislation and therefore different
expectations on such basic issues as health care, parental leave, acceptable discipline,
and the right to appropriate education. There are more subtle differences, too, that
also need to be addressed. For example, though both countries promote diversity,
they do so in different ways. That Canadians embrace diversity is especially evident
in the way we respond to questions of language. Like many other countries in the
world, Canada supports two languages officially but also supports many heritage
languages through an array of publicly funded venues. The study of language,
second-language learning, and bilingualism in particular are areas of research in
which Canadians are at the forefront.
Throughout the text, we present issues like these that are important to Canadians.
The fifth edition has been thoroughly updated and revised to reflect the ever-changing
field of developmental psychology as well as to provide updated information on current
Canadian research.
As noted at the outset, one the largest changes between previous editions and
the present text is the focus. The present text focuses on child development, whereas
earlier editions targeted both childhood and adolescence. Although some develop-
mental theories and findings relate to both groups, a growing body of research indi-
cates unique developmental issues in these two stages of development. As a result,
whole courses are now offered for each of child and adolescent development. Thus,
the present text focuses on child development with some material extending into
early adolescence, as the division between childhood and adolescence is not so
clear-cut in all domains or for every individual. Some changes that cut across all
chapters include
n■ organization of material, with material being condensed, relocated, and pre-
sented in forms that allow easier comprehension of the main ideas;
n■ updates to existing research programs in the introduction of new research pro-
grams and studies in the text and highlighted in The Inside Track boxes to
summarize current research being conducted by prominent Canadian develop-
mentalists;
n■ the use of specific examples to highlight research findings and provide applica-
tions to real-life situations; and
n■ significant and thorough updates to the research and theory to reflect current
thinking in developmental psychology.
In addition to these general changes that affect all chapters, numerous specific
changes have been made in each chapter. The following provides some examples to dem-
onstrate particular changes within each chapter.

Chapter 1: Introduction to Developmental


Psychology and Its Research Strategies
n■ Added and updated discussions, materials, and research on the following topics:
●■ baby diaries (added information about Clara and William Stern)

●■ psychophysiological methods (added eye tracking)

●■ ethical standards for conducting research (Tri-Council policies)

●■ different notions of developmental stability (absolute and positional

stability)
n■ Streamlined content to make it more accessible by deleting non-essential con-
tent and by reorganizing subsections to improve flow.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
xviii Preface

Chapter 2: Theories of Human Development


n■ Updated information regarding theories and theorists and augmented these
updates with photos.
n■ Updated information on sociocultural theories.
n■ Introduced and discussed developmental systems view and Gottlieb’s model of
co-active developmental systems.

Chapter 3: Hereditary Influences on Development


n■ To ensure clarity for students, the information on heritability was removed from
the Current Controversies box and embedded in the text.
n■ Added paragraph in Developmental Issues box explaining the concept that
“recessive” doesn’t mean “rare” and “dominant” doesn’t mean “ordinary.”
n■ Reduced section on hereditary disorders.
n■ Expanded recent advances in epigenetics and other ways genes are changed.

Chapter 4: Prenatal Development


n■ Introduced current research on prenatal exposure to HIV, including the impor-
tance of differentiating experiences in developed versus developing countries.
n■ Updated prevalence statistics throughout.
n■ Updated section on prenatal exposure to alcohol, including our current under-
standing of FASD.
n■ Updated section on illicit drugs and highlighted the impact of opioids as well as
including a summary of the effects of cocaine.

Chapter 5: Birth and the Newborn’s


Readiness for Life
n■ New The Inside Track box to highlight infant–parent mirroring.
n■ Revised birthing environments section to capture diversity of choices.
n■ Updated information related to postpartum stresses and depression.
n■ Updated statistics on sudden infant death syndrome in Canada.

Chapter 6: Physical Development: The Brain, Body,


Motor Skills, and the Beginnings of Sexual
Development
n■ Updated research on physical and motor changes.
n■ New Applying Research to Your Life box to explain the importance of physical
activity for children.
n■ Revised discussion of sexual development to focus on early onset of puberty in
childhood.

Chapter 7: Early Cognitive Foundations: Sensation,


Perception, and Learning
n■ Revised section on research methods to include brain imaging techniques.
n■ Updated statistics on otitis media and its impact on language, cognition, and
social development of children with recurring infections.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Preface xix

n■ Added information on the cues newborns use to recognize faces and how pain
is experienced by newborns.
n■ Revised section on the development of depth perception regarding why infants
avoid the drop-off in the visual cliff experiment.
n■ Updated statistics on corporal punishment of children and highlighting of its
impact on children’s aggression even in cultures that accept the practice.

Chapter 8: Cognitive Development: Piaget’s


Theory, Case’s Neo-Piagetian Theory, and
Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Viewpoint
n■ Revised opening vignette to reflect a childhood example.
n■ Added information on physical reasoning system and explanation-based learning.
n■ New The Inside Track box featuring the research of Kang Lee.

Chapter 9: Cognitive Development: Information-


Processing Perspectives and Connectionism
n■ Rearranged sections to make the flow among theories smoother.
n■ Streamlined and updated section on mathematics to make this application seam-
lessly follow the theory.
n■ Expanded and elaborated material related to metacognition.
n■ Updated research and examples.
n■ New The Inside Track box on preschoolers’ reasoning.
n■ New The Inside Track box on mindfulness and academic success.

Chapter 10: Intelligence: Measuring Mental


Performance
n■ Revised and updated opening vignette.
n■ Added PASS theory of intelligence.
n■ Updated statistics.
n■ Revised and updated The Inside Track box on David Saklofske’s work to reflect
a new focus on emotional intelligence.
n■ Updated information on adoptions and the role of genetics versus environment.
n■ Updated section on intellectual disabilities to reflect the DSM-5.

Chapter 11: Development of Language and


Communication Skills
n■ Revised section on the optimal age of linguistic exposure to attain native profi-
ciency in second language learning by examining international adopted children.
n■ Introduced statistical learning in early speech perception and word learning.
n■ New Canadian statistics on immigrants and languages used.
n■ New The Inside Track box on Johanne Paradis’s work on syntax and mor-
phology in second-language learners.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
xx Preface

Chapter 12: Emotional Development,


Temperament, and Attachment
n■ Reorganized and streamlined discussion on attachment.
n■ Reorganized and streamlined discussion on emotions.
n■ Expanded discussion on temperament.
n■ Two new The Inside Track boxes highlighting the work of Ellen Moss and
Elizabeth Hayden.
n■ Expanded and updated references throughout the chapter.

Chapter 13: Development of the Self


and Social Cognition
n■ Revised discussion of identity.
n■ Expanded discussion of cultural differences in self-concept.
n■ Expanded discussion of self-esteem.
n■ Expanded and updated discussion of achievement motivation.
n■ Expanded and updated references.
n■ Two new The Inside Track boxes highlighting the work of Shelley Hymel and
Joanne Cummings.

Chapter 14: Sex Differences and Similarities,


and Gender-Role Development
n■ Expanded discussion of evolutionary theory.
n■ Updated Focus on Research box dealing with the Bruce/Brenda story.
n■ Expanded discussion of marketing and media influence on observational
learning of gender roles.
n■ Updated discussion of gender schema theory.
n■ Expanded and updated references throughout.

Chapter 15: Moral Development and Aggression


n■ Chapter restructured and sections on moral development and prosocial develop-
ment merged to reduce redundancy.
n■ Section on definition of moral domain expanded.
n■ Evolutionary perspectives introduced.
n■ Discussion of new research on infants’ social preferences, helping behaviour,
and lying.
n■ New The Inside Track box highlighting Tina Malti’s research.
n■ Research on bullying removed to reduce overlap with other chapters and make
content more pertinent for children’s development.

Chapter 16: The Family


n■ Updated references and Canadian statistics throughout chapter.
n■ Updated The Inside Track boxes.
n■ New discussion of the so-called “tiger parenting” style.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Preface xxi

n■ Updated statistics and discussion on child abuse and neglect including practices
of immigrant families.
n■ Added information on “grooming.”

Chapter 17: Beyond the Family Context: Peers,


Schools, and Media Technologies
n■ Clarified the different forms of social inhibition (e.g., shyness versus withdrawing).
n■ Added information about how culture, intergenerational support, and tempera-
ment affects children’s behaviour and school success.
n■ Added more recent school models.
n■ Significant changes in technology have resulted in a different landscape for
children in today’s technological world. Conditions leading to change and the
impact of screen media technologies are introduced and elaborated upon.
n■ Introduced intergenerational contributions to schooling.
n■ Added information about how parents use technologies to monitor children’s
behaviour.

Writing style
Our goal has been to write a book that speaks directly to its readers and treats them
as active participants in an ongoing discussion. We have tried to be relatively informal
and down to earth in our writing style and to rely heavily on questions, thought
problems, concept checks, and a number of other exercises to stimulate students’
interest and involvement. Most of the chapters were “pretested” on our own faculty
and students, who identified parts that weren’t clear to them and suggested several
of the concrete examples, analogies, and occasional anecdotes that we’ve used when
introducing and explaining complex ideas. So, with the valuable assistance of our
student and peer critics, we have attempted to prepare a text that is substantive and
challenging yet reads more like a dialogue or a story than like an encyclopedia.

special Features
Among the more important features that are included to encourage student interest and
involvement and make the material easier to learn are the following:
n■ Introductions and chapter summaries. A brief introductory section at the
beginning of each chapter provides the student with a preview of what will be
covered.
n■ Concept Checks located at strategic points within each chapter give stu-
dents opportunities to review as they progress through the chapter. Concept
Checks include multiple-choice, fill-in-the-blank, essay, and scenario-based
questions. Answers to all Concept Checks can be found at the end of each
chapter.
n■ Summary sections at the end of each chapter present bulleted statements
organized by major chapter section, summarizing the key points of each
section.
n■ Running glossary, key term lists, and comprehensive end-of-book glossary.
A running glossary provides on-the-spot definitions of boldface key terms as
they appear in the text. At the end of each chapter is a list of key terms that
appeared in the narrative, as well as the page number on which each term is

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
xxii Preface

defined. A complete glossary of key terms for the entire text appears at the end
of the book. The number of the page where each term is first introduced is
included in the glossary.
n■ Boxes. Each chapter contains boxes that highlight important research, ideas,
processes, issues, or applications. The aim of these boxes is to permit a closer
or more personal examination of selected topics while stimulating the reader
to think about the questions, controversies, practices, and policies under dis-
cussion. For example, The Inside Track boxes highlight current Canadian
research and researchers; Applying Research to Your Life gives students useful
tools for becoming wise consumers of the research; Focus on Research boxes
discuss classic and recent studies that illuminate the topics and issues of devel-
opment; and Applying Developmental Themes sections highlight the book’s
four core developmental themes (nature/nurture, active/passive, continuity/
discontinuity, and the holistic nature of development), showing students how
chapter topics apply to these themes. Developmental Issues boxes highlight
applied questions important to development. All of these boxes are carefully
woven into the chapter narrative and were selected to reinforce central themes
in the text.
n■ The Inside Track boxes. An exciting feature of this text is The Inside Track.
These boxes highlight one or two recent studies or a program of research
conducted by researchers at Canadian universities. In writing these features,
we had the opportunity to communicate directly with almost all of the
researchers represented. You will notice that the research captures the state-
of-the-art work being conducted by Canadian researchers. Together, The
Inside Track boxes identify the extraordinary array of research being con-
ducted at Canadian universities. Although we have added new researchers in
this edition, these boxes feature only a few of the many individuals whose
work is making an impact on our understanding of Developmental Issues.
We would have liked to include many others but were limited, in some
cases, by space restrictions and, in others, by the advanced level of research,
which was beyond the scope of an introductory textbook. We have, how-
ever, integrated other important Canadian contributions throughout the
body of the text. In many cases, university affiliations are provided with the
researchers’ names to let students know where the researcher is located.
Where affiliations are left out, we felt they impeded the flow of the text or
compromised the clarity of the presentation. Overall, students will have an
opportunity to become acquainted with Canadian research and gain a better
understanding of how it fits within the big picture in developmental psy-
chology. In this fifth edition, we added new The Inside Track boxes, as well
as revising and updating the existing ones.
n■ Illustrations. Photographs, tables, figures, and chronological tables are
used extensively to review important developmental ideas and milestones.
All visual aids, including the occasional cartoon, were selected to illustrate
important principles and concepts and thereby enhance the educational
goals of the text.
n■ Critical-thinking questions. The What Do You Think? feature is designed to
encourage students to think about current controversies and/or to apply what
they have learned in formulating their own reasoned position on developmen-
tally significant issues. Any and all of these questions may serve as excellent
springboards for class discussion.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Preface xxiii

supplementary Aids and Ancillaries


Instructor Resources
The Nelson Education Teaching Advantage (NETA) program delivers research-
based instructor resources that promote student engagement and higher-order
thinking to enable the success of Canadian students and educators. Visit Nelson
Education’s Inspired Instruction website at www.nelson.com/inspired/ to find out
more about NETA.
The following instructor resources have been created for Developmental Psychology:
Infancy and Childhood, Fifth Canadian Edition. Access these ultimate tools for customizing
lectures and presentations at www.nelson.com/instructor.

netA test Bank


This resource was updated for this edition by Nancy Ogden of Mount Royal University.
It includes over 100 multiple-choice questions written according to NETA guidelines for
effective construction and development of higher-order questions. Also included is a
range of short-answer and essay questions for each chapter.
The NETA Test Bank is available in a new, cloud-based platform. Nelson Testing
Powered by Cognero® is a secure online testing system that allows instructors to author,
edit, and manage test bank content from anywhere Internet access is available. No special
installations or downloads are needed, and the desktop-inspired interface, with its drop-
down menus and familiar, intuitive tools, allows instructors to create and manage tests
with ease. Multiple test versions can be created in an instant, and content can be imported
or exported into other systems. Tests can be delivered from a learning management
system, the classroom, or wherever an instructor chooses. Nelson Testing Powered by
Cognero for Developmental Psychology: Infancy and Childhood, Fifth Canadian Edition, can
be accessed through www.nelson.com/instructor.

netA PowerPoint
Microsoft® PowerPoint® lecture slides for every chapter have been updated for this edi-
tion by Nancy Ogden of Mount Royal University. There is an average of 30 slides per
chapter, many featuring key figures, tables, and photographs from Developmental
Psychology: Infancy and Childhood, Fifth Canadian Edition. NETA principles of clear design
and engaging content have been incorporated throughout, making it simple for instruc-
tors to customize the deck for their courses.

Image Library
This resource consists of digital copies of figures, short tables, and photographs used in
the book. Instructors may use these jpegs to customize the NETA PowerPoint or create
their own PowerPoint presentations. An image library key describes the images and lists
the codes under which the jpegs are saved. Codes normally reflect the chapter number
(e.g., C01 for Chapter 1), the figure or photo number (e.g., F15 for Figure 15), and the
page in the textbook. C01-F15-pg26 corresponds to Figure 1-15 on page 26.

mindtap
Off ering personalized paths of dynamic assignments and applications, MindTap is a
digital learning solution that turns cookie-cutter into cutting-edge, apathy into engage-
ment, and memorizers into higher-level thinkers. MindTap enables students to analyze
and apply chapter concepts within relevant assignments and allows instructors to mea-
sure skills and promote better outcomes with ease. A fully online learning solution,

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
xxiv Preface

MindTap combines all student learning tools—readings, multimedia, activities, and


assessments—into a single Learning Path that guides the student through the curric-
ulum. Instructors personalize the experience by customizing the presentation of these
learning tools to their students, even seamlessly introducing their own content into the
Learning Path. Questions in the MindTap have been revised for this edition by Nancy
Ogden of Mount Royal University.

Student Ancillaries
mindtap
Stay organized and efficient with MindTap—a single destination with all the course mate-
rial and study aids you need to succeed. Built-in apps leverage social media and the latest
learning technology. For example:
n■ ReadSpeaker will read the text to you.
n■ Flashcards are pre-populated to provide you with a jump start for review—or
you can create your own.
n■ You can highlight text and make notes in your MindTap Reader. Your notes will
flow into Evernote, the electronic notebook app that you can access anywhere
when it’s time to study for the exam.
n■ Self-quizzing allows you to assess your understanding.
Visit www.nelson.com/student to start using MindTap. Enter the Online Access
Code from the card included with your text. If a code card is not provided, you can pur-
chase instant access at NELSONbrain.com.

Acknowledgments
As is always the case with projects as large as this one, there are many, many individuals
whose assistance was invaluable in the planning and production of the book. We would
like to acknowledge several researchers who helped to ensure the most up-to-date mate-
rial and understandings in the literature were present in this text. First, the updates to the
Principles of Hereditary Transmission section were greatly dependent on the eye-opening
and patient explanations from Dr. Sash Damjanovski from Western University, London,
Ontario. The research regarding early screen media relied heavily on research conducted
as part of the dissertations of two newly graduated doctoral students from Wilfrid
Laurier University, Dr. Domenica De Pasquale and Dr. Karin Archer. Dr. Marc Joanisse
of the University of Western Ontario deserves a special note of thanks for his assistance
in constructing the section on connectionism in Chapter 9.
Second, we would like to acknowledge the tremendous contributions of each
Canadian researcher highlighted in this text. Many not only shared their most important
pieces of work and took the time to summarize or review our summaries to make sure
information was accurate, but they also hunted down photographs so that we would be
able to show you the person behind the research.
Third, the quality of any volume in human development depends to a large extent
on the quality of the prepublication reviews from developmentalists around the world.
Many colleagues (including several dozen or so interested and unpaid volunteers) have
influenced this book by contributing constructive criticism, useful suggestions, refer-
ences, and a whole lot of encouragement. Each of those experts has helped to make the
final product a better one, and we thank them all.
The reviewers of the first through fifth Canadian editions include Scott Adler, York
University; Alisa Almas, University of Toronto; Tsasha Awong, Ryerson University; Lilly
Both, University of New Brunswick, Saint John; Elizabeth Bowering, Mount Saint
Vincent University; Anne Bowker, Carleton University; Mary Courage, Memorial
University; Jason Daniels, University of Alberta; Amy De Jaeger, University of Manitoba;

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Preface xxv

Nancy Digdon, Grant MacEwan College; Helen Doan, York University; Margaret Forgie,
University of Lethbridge; Deb Glebe, Wilfrid Laurier University; Darcy Hallet, University
of British Columbia; Barbara Hodkin, Mount Saint Vincent University; Gretchen Hess,
University of Alberta; Nina Howe, Concordia University; Jacqueline Kampman,
Thompson Rivers University; Jane Ledingham, University of Ottawa; Elizabeth Levin,
Laurentian University; Kathleen McKim-Dawes, University of New Brunswick; Mowei
Liu, Trent University; Kim MacLean, St. Francis Xavier University; Sandra Martin-Chang,
Mount Allison University; Laura Melnyk Gribble, University of Western Ontario; Colleen
McQuarrie, University of Prince Edward Island; Gene Ouellette, Mount Allison
University; Shelley Parlow, Carleton University; Alissa Pencer, Dalhousie University;
Carole Peterson, Memorial University of Newfoundland; Michael Pratt, Wilfrid Laurier
University; Marjorie Rabiau, McGill University; William Roberts, University College of
the Cariboo; Bruce Ryan, University of Guelph; Louis Schmidt, McMaster University;
Thomas Shultz, McGill University; Tanya Spencer, Lakehead University; Caroline
Sullivan, University of Ottawa; Kara Thompson, St. Francis Xavier University; Connie
Varnhagen, University of Alberta; Anthony Volk, Brock University; Tara Vongpaisal,
MacEwan University; Sally Walters, Capilano University; Gillian Wark, Simon Fraser
University; and Susan Weir, University of Regina. We would like to thank these reviewers
for helping to guide many of the changes we made to our book.
Finally, many other people have contributed their professionalism and skills to the
development and production of the fifth Canadian edition of this text. We are especially
grateful to Lenore Taylor-Atkins, Publisher for this project; to Liisa Kelly, who served as
Content Manager; to Imoinda Romain, Senior Production Project Manager; to John
Montgomery, who designed the book’s cover; to freelance copy editor Valerie Adams for
her skill, efficiency, tenacity, and attention to detail in copy editing; to MPS Limited for
proofreading and for page-formatting expertise; and to Claire Varley, Marketing Manager.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Sladic/iStock

1
Introduction to
Developmental Psychology
and Its Research Strategies

L
et’s begin this book with a question. Why did you choose to enroll in
a course on human development? For many of you majoring in psy-
chology, family studies, elementary education, or nursing, this class is
required. Expectant parents may take the course in order to learn more
about babies and children. Occasionally, people choose the course seeking to
answer specific questions about their own behaviour or that of a friend or family
member. Whatever your reasons, at one time or another you have probably
been curious about one or more aspects of human development. For example,
■■ What does the world smell, feel, sound and look like to newborn in-
fants? How do they make any sense of their new surroundings?
■■ When do infants first recognize their mothers? their fathers? them-
selves (in a mirror)?
■■ Why do many 1-year-olds seem so attached to their mothers and wary
of strangers?
■■ Foreign languages are difficult to follow if we merely listen to people
conversing in them. Yet, infants and toddlers will acquire their native
language without any formal instruction. How is this possible? Is lan-
guage learning easier for children than for adults? Is a child in a bilin-
gual home at a disadvantage?
■■ Why do you remember so little about the first two or three years of
your life?
■■ Why are some people friendly and outgoing, while others are shy and
reserved? How does the home environment influence an individual’s
personality?
■■ Why is it that all humans turn out similar in many ways and, at the
same time, so different from one another?

Introduction to Developmental
Psychology
The aim of this book is to seek answers for these and many other fascinating
questions about developing persons by reviewing the theories, methods, discov-
eries, and many practical accomplishments of the modern developmental
NEL 1
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
2 Part One | Theory and Research in the Developmental Sciences

sciences. This introductory chapter lays the groundwork for the remainder of the book
by addressing important issues about the nature of human development and how knowl-
edge about development is gained. What does it mean to say that people “develop” over
time? How is your experience of development different from that of developing persons
in past eras or in other cultures? Why are scientific studies of human development impor-
tant? What strategies, or research methods, do scientists use to study the development of
children and adolescents? Let’s begin by considering the nature of development.

What Is Development?
development Development refers to systematic changes in the individual that occur between concep-
systematic continuities and changes tion (when the father’s sperm penetrates the mother’s ovum, creating a new organism)
in the individual over the course of
and death. By describing changes as “systematic,” we imply that they are orderly, pat-
life.
terned, and relatively enduring, so temporary mood swings and other transitory changes
developmental continuities in our appearances, thoughts, and behaviours are therefore excluded. The complement
ways in which we remain stable over
of change is continuity, or ways in which we remain the same. Change cannot be prop-
time or continue to reflect our past.
erly understood without understanding the ways we remain the same and continue to
developmental psychology reflect our past. Thus, the developmental process entails both continuity and change.
branch of psychology devoted to If development represents the continuities and changes an individual experiences
identifying and explaining the from “womb to tomb,” developmental sciences refers to the study of these phenomena and
continuities and changes that
is a multidisciplinary enterprise. Although developmental psychology is the largest of
individuals display over time.
these disciplines, many biologists, sociologists, anthropologists, educators, physicians,
developmentalist and even historians share an interest in developmental continuity and change, and have
any scholar, regardless of discipline
contributed in important ways to our understanding of both human and animal develop-
(e.g., psychologist, biologist,
sociologist, anthropologist, educator), ment. Because the science of development is multidisciplinary, we use the term develop-
who seeks to understand the mentalist to refer to any scholar—regardless of discipline—who seeks to understand the
developmental process. developmental process.

Why Do We Develop?
To grasp the meaning of development, we must understand two important processes
maturation that underlie developmental change: maturation and learning. Maturation refers to the
developmental changes in the body or biological unfolding of the individual according to species-typical biological inheritance
behaviour that result from the aging
and an individual person’s biological inheritance. Just as seeds become mature plants,
process rather than from learning,
injury, illness, or some other life assuming that they receive adequate moisture and nourishment, human beings grow
experience. within the womb. Beyond the womb, the human maturational (or species-typical) bio-
logical program calls for us to become capable of walking and uttering our first mean-
ingful words at about 1 year of age, to reach sexual maturity between ages 11 and 15, and
then to age and die on a roughly similar schedule. Maturation is partly responsible for
psychological changes such as our increasing ability to concentrate, solve problems, and
understand another person’s thoughts or feelings. So one reason that we humans are so
similar in many important respects is that our common species heredity guides all of us
through many of the same developmental changes at about the same points in our lives.
learning The second critical developmental process is learning—the process through which
relatively permanent change in our experiences produce relatively permanent changes in our feelings, thoughts, and behav-
behaviour (or behavioural potential) iours. Let’s consider a very simple example. Although a certain degree of physical matura-
that results from one’s experiences or
practice.
tion is necessary before an elementary school child can become reasonably proficient at
dribbling a basketball, careful instruction and many, many hours of practice are essential
if this child is ever to approximate the ball-handling skills of a professional basketball
player. Many of our abilities and habits do not simply unfold as part of maturation; we
often learn to feel, think, and behave in new ways from our observations of and interac-
tions with parents, teachers, and other important people in our lives, as well as from
events that we experience. This means that we change in response to our environments—
particularly in response to the actions and reactions of the people around us.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Chapter 1 | Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its Research Strategies 3

Of course, most developmental changes are the


product of both maturation and learning. Take language
development as an example. Infants’ brains are wired to
learn language, and the regular process of language acqui-
sition seems like a maturational process. However, expo-
sure to a language environment is critical for children to
learn to talk. Children learn different mother tongues
Matthew Bingley/CBC Licensing

depending on their language environment. Moreover,


when children learn a language, they do not just parrot
what they hear from others. Instead, they actively seek to
make sense of it, and extract the sounds, meanings, and
rules that govern our everyday language use. Thus, lan-
Despite the common assumption that highly successful children are
guage development as any other developmental process,
“naturally gifted,” the special skills they display require an enormous requires an active individual. Children actively contribute
amount of practice. For example, 7-year-old Vanya Virami’s victory as a to their own development. They do so by interpreting a
spelling bee champion in 2016 required ability, practice, and hard work. given situation differently, by triggering different reactions
from others, and by selecting different environments.
Consider an aggressive child in comparison to a child who is rather shy and withdrawn.
Both children experience the same environment (e.g., the classroom) differently, they
evoke different reactions in others (e.g., peers and teachers), and they choose different
friends. If children were not active individuals, their development would not happen. Thus,
development is always a joint function of maturation, learning, and the active individual.

What Goals Do Developmentalists Pursue?


Three major goals of the developmental sciences are to describe, to explain, and to opti-
mize development (Baltes, Reese, & Lipsitt, 1980). In pursuing the goal of description,
human developmentalists carefully observe the behaviour of people of different ages,
seeking to specify how people change over time. Although there are typical pathways of
development that virtually all people follow, no two persons are exactly alike. Even iden-
tical twins raised in the same home, to some extent, display different interests, abilities,
and behaviours. Thus, to adequately describe development, it is necessary to focus both
normative development on typical patterns of change (or normative development) and on individual variations
developmental changes that in patterns of change (or ideographic development). So developmentalists seek to
characterize most or all members of a understand the important ways that developing humans resemble each other and how
species; typical patterns of
development.
they are likely to differ as they proceed through life.
Description provides us with the “facts” about development, but it is only the starting
ideographic development point. Developmentalists next seek to explain the changes they have observed. In pur-
individual variations in the rate,
extent, or direction of development.
suing this goal of explanation, developmentalists hope to determine why people develop
as they typically do and why some people develop differently from others. Explanation
centres both on normative changes within individuals and on variations in development
between individuals. As we will see throughout the text, it is often easier to describe devel-
opment than to conclusively explain how it occurs.
Finally, developmentalists hope to optimize development by applying what they have
learned in attempts to help people develop in positive directions. This is a practical side
to the study of human development that has led to such breakthroughs as ways to:
■■ Promote stronger affectional ties between fussy, unresponsive infants and their
frustrated parents;
■■ Assist children with learning difficulties to succeed at school; and
■■ Help socially unskilled children prevent the emotional difficulties that could re-
sult from having no close friends and being rejected by peers.
The goal of optimization often appears to be clear and unproblematic. After all,
having children learn how to walk, talk, read, and count, and to have friends and to be
happy would be endorsed by most of us. However, sometimes it is less clear what quali-
fies as optimal development. Take gender roles and social behaviour as an example. Is it

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
4 Part One | Theory and Research in the Developmental Sciences

better to expect males to be more dominant and females to be more nurturing? Should
children be taught to tell the truth at all times? Any definition of optimal development
relies on cultural and societal values. Thus, the goal of optimization depends on the
values developmentalists share with the larger cultural context of which they are part.
Optimization goals may increasingly influence research agendas as developmental-
ists show greater interest in solving real problems and communicating the practical impli-
cations of their findings to the public and policymakers (APA Presidential Task Force on
Evidence-Based Practice, 2006; Kratochwill, 2007; Lerner, 2012; McCall & Groark, 2000;
Schoenwald et al., 2008). Yet this heavier focus on applied issues in no way implies that
traditional descriptive and explanatory goals are any less important, because optimization
goals often cannot be achieved until researchers have adequately described normal and
idiopathic pathways of development and their causes (Schwebel, Plumert, & Pick, 2000).

The Nature of Development


Now that we have defined development and talked very briefly about the goals that devel-
opmentalists pursue, let’s consider some of the conclusions they have drawn about the
nature of development.

A Continual and Lifelong Process. Although no one can specify precisely what
adulthood holds in store from even the most meticulous examination of a person’s child-
hood, developmentalists have learned that the first 12 years are extremely important for
setting the stage for adolescence and adulthood. Obviously, you are not the same person
you were at age 10 or even at age 15. You have probably grown somewhat, acquired new
academic skills, and developed very different interests and aspirations from those you had
in Grade 5 or in high school. And the path of such developmental change stretches ever
onward, through middle age and beyond, culminating in the final change that occurs
when we die. In sum, human development is best described as a continual and lifelong
process. The one constant is change, and the changes that occur at each major phase of
life can have important implications for the future. In this textbook the focus is on devel-
opment in infancy and childhood, which are the age periods where development is most
obvious, profound and fast. However, even when focusing on children, we should keep
in mind that development never stops.
Table 1.1 presents a chronological overview of the life span as developmentalists see
it. Our focus in this text is on development during the first four periods of life—prenatal
development, infancy and toddlerhood, preschool, and middle childhood. By examining

TABLE 1.1 A Chronological Overview of Human Development


Period of Life Approximate Age Range
1. Prenatal period Conception to birth
2. Infancy Birth to 18 months old
3. Toddler period 18 months to 3 years
4. Preschool period 3 to 5 years of age
5. Middle childhood 5 to 12 or so years of age (until the onset of puberty)
6. Adolescence 12 or so to 20 years of age (many developmentalists define the end of adolescence
as the point at which the individual begins to work and is reasonably independent
© Cengage Learning 2014

of parental sanctions)
7. Young adulthood 20 to 40 years of age
8. Middle age 40 to 65 years of age
9. Old age 65 years of age or older

Note: The age ranges listed here are approximate and may not apply to any particular individual. For example, a few
10-year-olds have experienced puberty and are properly classified as adolescents. Some adolescents are fully self-
supporting, with children of their own, and are best classified as young adults.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Chapter 1 | Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its Research Strategies 5

how children develop, we will learn about ourselves and the determinants of our behav-
iour. Our survey will also provide some insight as to why no two individuals are ever
exactly alike. Our survey won’t provide answers to every important question you may
have about developing children. The study of human development is still a relatively
young discipline with many unresolved issues. But as we proceed, it should become quite
clear that developmentalists have provided an enormous amount of very practical infor-
mation about infants and children that can help us to become better educators, child/
adolescent practitioners, and parents.

A Holistic Process. It was once fashionable to divide developmentalists into three


camps: (1) those who studied physical growth and development, including bodily changes
and the sequencing of motor skills; (2) those who studied cognitive aspects of develop-
ment, including perception, language, learning, and thinking; and (3) those who concen-
trated on psychosocial aspects of development, including emotions, personality, and the
growth of interpersonal relationships. Today we know that this classification is misleading,
for researchers who work in any of these areas have found that changes in one aspect of
development have important implications for other aspects. Let’s consider an example.
What determines a person’s popularity with peers? If you were to say that social
skills are important, you would be right. Social skills such as warmth, friendliness, and
willingness to cooperate are characteristics that popular children typically display. Yet
there is much more to popularity than meets the eye. We now have some indication that
the age at which a child reaches puberty, an important milestone in physical develop-
ment, can have an effect on social life. For example, decrements in social relationships
may occur for those who reach puberty early relative to peers than those who reach
puberty later (Downing & Bellis, 2009; Mendle et al., 2012; Warren & Yu, 2015). Children
who do well in school also tend to be more popular with their peers than children who
perform somewhat less well in school.
We see, then, that popularity depends not only on the growth of social skills but also
on various aspects of both cognitive and physical development. As this example illustrates,
holistic perspective development is not piecemeal but holistic—humans are physical, cognitive, and social
unified view of the developmental beings, and each of these components of self depends, in part, on changes taking place in
process that emphasizes the other areas of development. Many researchers now incorporate this holistic theme into
important interrelationships among
the physical, mental, social, and
their theories and research. For example, in reviewing the literature on sex differences in
emotional aspects of human science and mathematics, Halpern and her colleagues (Halpern et al., 2007) adopted a
development. biopsychosocial approach in which they considered all aspects of the child in understanding
sex differences and similarities. The holistic perspective is one of the dominant themes of
human development today and a perspective around which this book is organized.

plasticity Plasticity. Plasticity refers to a capacity for change in response to positive or negative
capacity for change; a developmental life experiences. Although we have described development as a continual and lifelong
state that has the potential to be process and noted that past events often have implications for the future, developmental-
shaped by experience.
ists know that the course of development can change abruptly if important aspects of a
person’s life change. For example, aggressive children who are disliked by their peers
often improve their social status after learning and practising the social skills that popular
children display (Bierman, 2004; Mize & Ladd, 1990; Shure, 1989). It is indeed fortunate
that human development is so plastic, for children who have horrible starts can often be
helped to overcome their deficiencies.

Historical/Cultural Context. No single portrait of development is accurate for all


cultures, social classes, or racial and ethnic groups. Each culture, subculture, and social
class transmits a particular pattern of beliefs, values, customs, and skills to its younger
generations, and the content of this cultural socialization has a strong influence on the
attributes and competencies that individuals display. Development is also influenced by
societal changes: historical events such as wars, technological breakthroughs such as the
increasing portability and powerfulness of computers, and social causes such as

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
6 Part One | Theory and Research in the Developmental Sciences

environmentalism. Each generation develops in its own way, and each generation changes
the world for succeeding generations. So we should not automatically assume that devel-
opmental patterns observed in North American or European children (the most heavily
studied populations) are optimal, or even that they characterize persons developing in
other eras or cultural settings. Only by adopting a historical/cultural perspective can we
fully appreciate the richness and diversity of human development.

The Scientific Study of Development and Its Origins


Early Philosophical Perspectives on
Childhood
original sin According to Hermann Ebbinghaus (1908), one of the founders of experimental psy-
idea that children are inherently chology in the late 19th century, psychology is a scientific discipline with “a long past
negative creatures who must be but short history.” This characterization is still applicable to developmental psychology
taught to rechannel their selfish
more than a century later. Scholars have been pondering the nature of children’s
interests into socially acceptable
outlets. development and best practices for child rearing for centuries. Greek philosophy is
rich with answers. With the advent of developmental psychology as an independent
innate purity
field of inquiry in the late 19th century, a new way of approaching these questions
idea that infants are born with an
intuitive sense of right and wrong was established that relied more on empirical observation than philosophical specula-
that is often misdirected by the tion. Still, the empirical study of children’s development inevitably rests on philo-
demands and restrictions of society. sophical world views with a long past. How does the child become a mature adult? Is
tabula rasa the child passively shaped by culture and society? Or, does the child actively construct
the idea that the mind of an infant is beliefs and values on the basis of everyday social interactions with the physical and
a “blank slate” and that all social environment?
knowledge, abilities, behaviours, and Thomas Hobbes’s (1651/1904) doctrine of original sin held that children are
motives are acquired through
inherently selfish egoists who must be restrained by society, whereas Jean Jacques
experience.
Rousseau’s (1762/1955) doctrine of innate purity maintained that children are born
with an intuitive sense of right and wrong that society often corrupts. These two view-
points clearly differ in their implications for child rearing. Proponents of original sin
argued that parents must actively control their egoistic children; the innate purists
argued that parents should give their children freedom to follow their inherently posi-
tive inclinations.
Another influential view on children and child rearing was suggested by John Locke
(1690/1913), who believed that the mind of an infant is a tabula rasa, or “blank slate,”
and that children have no inborn tendencies. In other words, children are neither inher-
ently good nor inherently bad, and how they turn out depends entirely on their worldly
experiences. Locke argued in favour of disciplined child rearing to ensure that children
would develop good habits and acquire few bad ones.
GFX

These philosophers also differed on the question of children’s participation in their


own development. Hobbes maintained that children must learn to rechannel their natu-
rally selfish interests into socially acceptable outlets; in this sense, they are passive sub-
jects to be moulded by parents. Locke, too, believed that the child’s role is passive
because the mind of an infant is a blank slate on which experience writes its lessons. But
a strikingly different view was proposed by Rousseau, who believed that children are
actively involved in the shaping of their own intellects and personalities. In Rousseau’s
words, the child is not a “passive recipient of the tutor’s instruction” but a “busy, testing,
motivated explorer. The active searching child, setting his own problems, stands in
marked contrast to the receptive one . . . on whom society fixes its stamp” (quoted in
Kessen, 1965, p. 75).
Clearly, these philosophers had some interesting ideas about children and child
GFX

rearing. But how could anyone decide whether their views were correct? Unfortunately,
the philosophers collected no objective data to back their pronouncements, and the few
observations they did make were limited and unsystematic. Can you anticipate the next
step in the evolution of the developmental sciences?
NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Chapter 1 | Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its Research Strategies 7

Children as Subjects of Study: Baby Biographies


and Diaries
The first glimmering of a systematic study of children can be traced to the late 19th
century. This was a period in which investigators from a variety of academic backgrounds
began to observe the development of their own children and to publish these data in
baby biography works known as baby biographies.
a detailed record of an infant’s An early and influential baby biographer was Charles Darwin, who made daily
growth and development over a records of the early development of his son (Darwin, 1877; and see Charlesworth, 1992).
period of time.
Darwin’s curiosity about child development stemmed from his theory of evolution. Quite
simply, he believed that young, untrained infants share many characteristics with their
nonhuman ancestors, and he advanced the (now discredited) idea that the development
of the individual child retraces the entire evolutionary history of the species, thereby
illustrating the “descent of man.” So Darwin and many of his contemporaries viewed the
baby biography as a means of answering questions about our evolutionary past.
A few decades after Darwin, Clara and William Stern kept one of the first systematic
diaries on the psychological development of their three children, Hilde, Günther, and
Eva, born in 1900, 1902, and 1904, respectively. A major publication based on these diaries
was Clara and William Stern’s book Die Kindersprache [Children’s talk], which became a
classic in the language acquisition literature (Stern & Stern, 1907).
Two to three decades later, Jean Piaget, a famous Swiss psychologist, observed and
recorded the development of his three children, Jacqueline, Lucienne, and Laurent. He
used these observations to develop more fine-grained methods for studying the develop-
ment of logical thinking in children. His theories revolutionized our understanding of
children’s cognitive development.

The diaries William and Clara Stern recorded for their children led to
the publication of Die Kindersprache [Children’s talk], which became a
classic in the language acquisition literature.

This is the first page of the diary of Clara and William


Stern’s firstborn child, Hilde, born April 7, 1900, at 2 a.m.
This page describes Hilde’s behaviour (e.g., sucking her
thumb), sensitivity to various sounds and light, as well as
the characteristic vowel patterns of her crying.
NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
8 Part One | Theory and Research in the Developmental Sciences

These three famous examples demonstrate how keeping diaries of their own children
was an important step for researchers to establish developmental psychology as an empir-
ical field of inquiry. It allowed them to contrast their theories and ideas about children’s
development with empirical observations. To be sure, many of these observations may
not meet the more rigorous requirements of the scientific method expected today. Parent
researchers may not be entirely objective about their own children. They also may have
let their assumptions about the nature of development bias their observations so that they
“found” what they were looking for. Finally, each baby biography was based on a single or
small number of children—and often the child of a distinguished individual. Conclusions
based on a single case or limited sample may not hold true for other children.
Despite these shortcomings, baby biographies and diaries were a step in the right
direction. The fact that eminent scientists were writing about developing children implied
that human development was a topic worthy of scientific scrutiny.

Research Methods in Developmental Psychology


Modern developmental psychology is appropriately labelled a scientific enterprise because
scientific method those who study development have adopted the scientific method, which guides their
the use of objective and replicable attempts at reaching the three goals of describing, explaining, and optimizing development.
methods to gather data for the There is nothing mysterious about the scientific method. It refers to the use of objective
purpose of testing a theory or
hypothesis. It dictates that, above all,
and replicable methods to gather data for the purpose of testing a theory or hypothesis.
investigators must be objective and Although the word theory is an imposing term, theories are something that everybody has.
must allow their data to decide the If we were to ask you why males and females appear to be very different as adults when
merits of their thinking. they seem so very similar as infants, you would undoubtedly have some opinions on the
theory issue. Your answer would state or at least reflect your own underlying theory of the devel-
a set of concepts and propositions opment of sex differences. A theory is a set of concepts and propositions that describe and
designed to organize, describe, and explain some aspect of experience. In the field of psychology, theories help us to describe
explain an existing set of and explain various patterns of behaviour. Good theories have the ability to predict future
observations.
events. These theoretical predictions, or hypotheses, are then tested by collecting data.
hypothesis It is important that the method used to collect data is objective. By this we mean that
a theoretical prediction about some everyone who examines the data will come to the same conclusions; that is, it is not a
aspect of experience.
subjective opinion. It is also important that the method be replicable, meaning that every
time the method is used, it results in the same data and conclusions. Thus, the scientific
method dictates that, above all, investigators must be objective and must allow their data
to decide the merits of their thinking.
In earlier eras, when social philosophers such as Hobbes, Locke, and Rousseau were
presenting their views on children and child rearing, their largely unsubstantiated claims
were often accepted as fact. People assumed that great minds always had great insights.
Very few individuals questioned the word of well-known scholars because the scientific
method was not yet a widely accepted criterion for evaluating knowledge.
The intent here is not to criticize the early social philosophers. However, great minds
WHAT DO YOU THINK? ? may on occasion produce miserable ideas that can do a great deal of harm if those ideas
What might you say to a person are uncritically accepted and influence the way people are treated. The scientific method,
who rejects an established then, is a valuable safeguard that helps to protect the scientific community and society at
finding by saying, “It didn’t large against flawed reasoning (Machado & Silva, 2007). Protection is provided by the
happen that way for my child”? practice of evaluating the merits of various theoretical pronouncements against the objec-
If this parent’s recollection is tive record, rather than simply relying on the academic, political, or social credibility of the
accurate, does this invalidate the
theorist. Of course, this also means that the theorist whose ideas are being evaluated must
finding?
be equally objective and willing to discard pet notions when there is evidence against them.

Gathering Data: Basic Fact-Finding Strategies


No matter what aspect of development we hope to study—be it the perceptual capabili-
ties of newborn infants, the growth of friendships among elementary school children, or
the reasons some adolescents begin to use drugs—we must find ways to measure what

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Chapter 1 | Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its Research Strategies 9

interests us. Today, researchers are fortunate in having many tried-and-true procedures
they can use to measure behaviour and to test their hypotheses about human develop-
ment. But regardless of the technique employed, scientifically useful measures must
reliability always display two important qualities: reliability and validity.
the extent to which a measuring A measure is reliable if it yields consistent information over time and across observers.
instrument yields consistent results, Suppose you go into a classroom and record the number of times each child behaves
both over time and across observers.
aggressively toward others, but your research assistant, using the same scheme to observe
validity the same children, does not agree with your measurements. Or you measure each child’s
the extent to which a measuring aggressiveness one week but come up with very different aggressiveness scores while
instrument accurately reflects what
the researchers intended to measure.
applying the same measure to the same children a week later. Clearly, your observational
measure of aggression is unreliable because it yields highly inconsistent information. To
be reliable and thus useful for scientific purposes, your measure would have to produce
comparable estimates of children’s aggression from independent observers (interrater
reliability) and yield similar scores for individual children from one testing to another
shortly thereafter (test-retest reliability).
A measure is valid if it measures what it is supposed to measure. An instrument must
be reliable before it can possibly be valid. Yet reliability, by itself, does not guarantee
validity (Miller, 1997). For example, a highly reliable observational scheme intended as a
measure of children’s aggression may provide grossly overinflated estimates of aggres-
sive behaviour if the investigator simply classifies all acts of physical force as examples of
aggression. What the researcher has failed to recognize is that much high-intensity
behaviour may simply represent enjoyable forms of rough-and-tumble play without
harmful or aggressive intent. Researchers must demonstrate they are measuring the
attribute they say they are measuring before we can have much faith in the data they col-
lect or the conclusions they reach.
Keeping in mind the importance of establishing the reliability and validity of mea-
sures, let us consider some of the different ways in which aspects of human development
might be measured.

Self-Report Methods
Three common procedures developmentalists use to gather information and test hypoth-
eses are interviews, questionnaires (including psychological tests), and the clinical
method. Although these approaches are similar in that each asks participants to answer
questions posed by the investigator, they differ in the extent to which the investigator
treats individual participants alike.

Interviews and Questionnaires. Researchers who opt for interview or question-


naire techniques ask the child, or the child’s parents, a series of questions pertaining to
such aspects of development as the child’s behaviour, feelings, beliefs, or characteristic
methods of thinking. Collecting data via a questionnaire (and most psychological tests)
simply involves putting questions in written or electronic formats and asking participants
to respond to them, whereas interviews require participants to respond orally to the
structured interview or investigator’s queries. If the procedure is a structured interview or structured ques-
structured questionnaire tionnaire, all who participate in the study are asked the same questions in the same
a technique in which all participants
order. The purpose of this standardized or structured format is to treat each person alike
are asked the same questions in
precisely the same order so that the so that the responses of different participants can be compared.
responses of different participants Interviews and questionnaires have some very real shortcomings, as when they are
can be compared. used with very young children. Although some accommodations can be made—such as
using variations of smiley-faces as a rating scale instead of numbers or words (Egan,
Santos, & Bloom, 2007)—neither approach can be used with very young children, who
cannot read or comprehend speech very well. Investigators must also hope that the
answers they receive are honest and accurate and are not merely attempts by respondents
to present themselves in a favourable or socially desirable way. Clearly, inaccurate or
untruthful responses lead to erroneous conclusions. Investigators must also be careful to
ensure that participants of all ages interpret questions in the same way; otherwise, the

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
10 Part One | Theory and Research in the Developmental Sciences

age trends observed in the study may reflect differences in children’s ability to compre-
hend and communicate rather than real underlying changes in their feelings, thoughts,
or behaviours. Finally, researchers who interview both developing children and their
parents (or teachers) may have trouble determining which set of reports is more accurate
if the children’s descriptions of their own behaviours differ from those of the other infor-
mants (Hussong, Zucker, Wong, Fitzgerald, & Puttler, 2005).
Despite these potential shortcomings, structured interviews and questionnaires can
be excellent methods of obtaining large amounts of useful information in a short time.
Both approaches are particularly useful when the investigator emphasizes to partici-
pants that their responses will be confidential and/or challenges them to report exactly
what they know about an issue, thereby maximizing the likelihood of a truthful or
accurate answer.

clinical method The Clinical Method. The clinical method is very similar to the interview technique.
a type of interview in which a The investigator is usually interested in testing a hypothesis by presenting the research
participant’s response to each
participant with a task or stimulus of some sort and then inviting a response. After the
successive question (or problem)
determines what the investigator will participant responds, the investigator typically asks a second question or introduces a
ask next. new task to clarify the participant’s original answer. Although participants are often
asked the same questions initially, each participant’s answer determines what he or she is
asked next. Thus, the clinical method is a flexible approach that considers each partici-
pant to be unique.
Jean Piaget did not only observe his own children but relied extensively on the clin-
ical method to study children’s moral reasoning and intellectual development. The data
from Piaget’s research are largely protocol records of his interactions with individual
children. Here is a small sample from Piaget’s work (1932/1965, p. 140) on the develop-
ment of moral reasoning, which shows that this young child thinks about lying in a very
different way than adults do:
Do you know what a lie is?—It’s when you say what isn’t true.—Is 2 1 2 5 5 a lie?—
Yes, it’s a lie.—Why?—Because it isn’t right.—Did the boy who said 2 1 2 5 5 know
it wasn’t right or did he make a mistake?—He made a mistake.—Then if he made a
mistake, did he tell a lie or not?—Yes, he told a lie.
Like structured interviews, clinical methods are often useful for gathering large
amounts of information in relatively brief periods. This strategy’s flexibility is also an
advantage; by asking follow-up questions that are tailored to the participant’s original
answers, it is often possible to obtain a rich under-
standing of the meaning of those answers.
However, the flexibility of the clinical method is
also a potential shortcoming. It may be difficult, if
not impossible, to directly compare the answers of
participants who are asked different questions.
Furthermore, tailoring one’s questions to the par-
ticipant’s responses raises the possibility that the
examiner’s pre-existing theoretical biases may
affect the particular follow-up questions asked and
the interpretations provided. Because conclusions
drawn from the clinical method depend, in part,
on the investigator’s subjective interpretations, it is
always desirable to verify these insights using other
research techniques.
© image100/Corbis

Observational Methods
Investigators using the clinical method. All participants are asked the same Often researchers prefer to observe people’s
questions at first, but each participant’s answers to these initial questions behaviour directly rather than asking them ques-
determine what the researcher will ask next. tions about it. One method that many

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Chapter 1 | Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its Research Strategies 11

naturalistic observation developmentalists favour is naturalistic observation—observing people in their


a method in which the scientist tests
common, everyday (i.e., natural) surroundings (Pellegrini, 1996). To observe children,
hypotheses by observing people as this usually means going into homes, schools, malls, public parks, and playgrounds and
they engage in everyday activities in
carefully recording what they do. Rarely will investigators try to record every event
their natural habitats (e.g., at home,
at school, or on the playground). that occurs; they are usually testing a specific hypothesis about one type of behaviour,
such as cooperation or aggression, and will focus their attention and data collection
exclusively on acts of this kind. One strength of naturalistic observation is the ease
with which it can be applied to infants and toddlers, who often cannot be studied
through methods that demand verbal skills. A second strength of naturalistic observa-
tion is that it illustrates how people actually behave in everyday life (Willems &
Alexander, 1982).
However, naturalistic observation also has its limitations. First, some behaviours
occur so infrequently (e.g., heroic rescues) or are so socially undesirable (e.g., morally
reprehensible behaviours) that they are unlikely to be witnessed by an unknown observer
in the natural environment. Second, many events are usually happening at the same time
in a natural setting, and any (or some combination) of them may affect people’s behav-
iour. This makes it difficult to pinpoint the causes of participants’ actions or of any
developmental trends in behaviour. Finally, the mere presence of an observer can some-
times make people behave differently than they otherwise would. Children may “show
off ” when they have an audience, whereas parents may be on their best behaviour,
showing a strong reluctance, for example, to berate a misbehaving child as they normally
observer influence might. For these reasons, researchers often attempt to minimize observer influence by
tendency of participants to react to (1) videotaping their participants from a concealed location or (2) spending time in the
an observer’s presence by behaving in setting before collecting their “real” data so that the individuals they are observing will
unusual ways.
grow accustomed to their presence and behave more naturally.
Mary Haskett and Janet Kistner (1991) conducted an excellent piece of naturalistic
observation to compare the social behaviours of nonabused preschoolers with those of
daycare classmates identified by child protection agencies as having been physically abused
by their parents. The investigators first defined examples of the behaviours they wished
to record—both desirable behaviours, such as appropriate social initiations and positive
play, and undesirable behaviours, such as aggression and negative verbalizations. They then
time-sampling monitored 14 abused and 14 nonabused preschool children as the children mingled with
a procedure in which the investigator
peers in a play area of a daycare facility. Observations were made using a time-sampling
records the frequencies with which
individuals display particular procedure; each child was observed during three 10-minute play sessions on three dif-
behaviours during the brief time ferent days. To minimize their influence on the play activities, observers stood outside the
intervals that each is observed. play area while making their observations.
The results were disturbing. Abused chil-
dren initiated fewer social interactions than their
nonabused classmates and were somewhat
socially withdrawn. And when they did interact
with playmates, the abused youngsters displayed
more aggressive acts and other negative behav-
iours than did their nonabused companions.
Indeed, nonabused children often blatantly
ignored the positive social initiations of an
abused child, as if they did not want to get
involved with him or her.
Tragically, Haskett and Kistner’s observational
study, which was subsequently supported through
survey research (Anthonysamy & Zimmer-
Gembeck, 2007), shows that abused children are
Joanna Cotton

unattractive playmates who are likely to be disliked


and even rejected by peers. But as is almost always
the case in naturalistic observational research, it is
Children’s tendency to perform for observers is one of the problems that difficult to pinpoint the exact cause of these find-
researchers must overcome when using the method of naturalistic observation. ings. Did the negative behaviours of abused

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
12 Part One | Theory and Research in the Developmental Sciences

children cause their peers to reject them? Or did peer rejection cause the abused children
to display negative behaviours? Either possibility or another could account for Haskett and
Kistner’s results.
How might observational researchers study unusual or undesirable behaviours
that they are unlikely to observe in the natural environment? One way is to conduct
structured observation structured observations in the laboratory. In a structured observational study, each
an observational method in which the participant is exposed to a setting that might cue the behaviour in question and is then
investigator cues the behaviour of
surreptitiously observed (via a hidden camera or through a one-way mirror) to see if he
interest and observes participants’
responses in a laboratory. or she performs the behaviour. For example, Leon Kuczynski from the University of
Guelph (1983) got children to promise to help him with a boring task and then left them
alone to work in a room where attractive toys were present. This procedure enabled
Kuczynski to determine whether children would break a promise to work when they
thought there was no one present to observe their transgression. Kuczynski found that
some of the children did break the promise to work so they could play with the toys,
whereas others continued with the work even when they thought no one was watching.
Aside from being a most feasible way of studying behaviours that occur infrequently
or are not openly displayed in the natural environment, structured observations also
ensure that every participant in the sample is exposed to the same eliciting stimuli and has
an equal opportunity to perform the target behaviour—circumstances that are not always
true in the natural environment. Of course, the major disadvantage of structured obser-
vation is that participants may not always respond in a contrived laboratory setting as
they would in everyday life.
In an interesting example of structured observation, Tronick and his colleagues
(Tronick et al., 2005) studied the interaction between 4-month-olds and their mothers,
with a specific interest in how the mother–infant interactions of babies prenatally exposed
to cocaine compared to those of nonexposed infants. To find out, they brought 695
mother–infant pairs into a laboratory setting, 236 of whom had been exposed to cocaine
prenatally. Cameras were positioned so that both the infant’s face and the mother’s face
were videotaped for three two-minute periods. During the first two minutes, mother and
child were allowed to interact normally. During the second period, the mother was
instructed to present a “still face” to the infant; that is, she was told not to laugh, smile,
talk to, or touch the infant. During the third two-minute period, the mother was to
resume normal interaction with her child. This face-to-face still-face procedure allowed
the researcher to observe the interactions of interest in a little over six minutes, rather
than travelling to 695 different homes and waiting for hours and hours for the behaviours
to occur.
As Tronick and colleagues suspected, the interaction patterns of the cocaine-exposed
mother–infant pairs were different from those of the nonexposed pairs. For the most
part, the cocaine-exposed infants and their mothers did not appear to be engaged in the
kind of social interaction that facilitates both social and cognitive development in later
months. Previous research suggests that the quality of caregiver–infant interactions is
extremely important to the healthy social and cognitive development of very young chil-
dren (Ainsworth, 1979, 1989). Positive, synchronized interactions provide the infant with
the foundation for forming other positive, supportive relationships later on in life. Such
relationships also enable the child to investigate objects and the rest of the world without
excessive fear (Bowlby, 1973, 1988).

Case Studies
case study Any or all of the methods we have discussed—structured interviews, questionnaires,
a research method in which the clinical method, and behavioural observations—can be used to compile a detailed por-
investigator gathers extensive trait of a single individual’s development through the case study method. In preparing
information about the life of an
an individualized record, or case, the investigator typically seeks many kinds of informa-
individual and then tests
developmental hypotheses by tion about the participant, such as his or her family background, socioeconomic status,
analyzing the events of the person’s health records, academic or work history, and performance on psychological tests. Much
life history. of the information included in any case history comes from interviews with and

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Chapter 1 | Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its Research Strategies 13

observations of the individual, although the questions asked and observations made are
typically not standardized and may vary considerably from case to case. Case studies may
also be used to describe groups.
Although many developmentalists have used case studies to great advantage, there
are major drawbacks to this approach. For example, it is often difficult to directly com-
pare subjects who have been asked different questions, taken different tests, and been
observed under different circumstances. Case studies may also lack generalizability; that
is, conclusions drawn from the experiences of the small number of individuals studied
may simply not apply to most people. Theories posited after studying children sampled
from a large city in the United States may not apply to children in Canada or Finland or
Southeast Asia. For these reasons, any conclusions drawn from case studies should always
be verified through the use of other research techniques.

Ethnography
ethnography Ethnography—a form of participant observation often used in the field of anthropology—
method in which the researcher seeks is becoming increasingly popular among researchers who hope to understand the effects of
to understand the unique values,
culture on developing children and adolescents. To collect their data, ethnographers often
traditions, and social processes of a
culture or subculture by living with live for periods of months or even years within the cultural or subcultural community they
its members and making extensive are studying. The data they collect are typically diverse and extensive, consisting largely of
observations and notes. naturalistic observations, notes made from conversations with members of the culture, and
interpretations of these events. These data are eventually used to compile a detailed portrait
of the cultural community and draw conclusions about how the community’s unique
values and traditions influence aspects of the development of its children and adolescents.
Detailed ethnographic portraits of a culture or subculture that arise from close and
enduring contact with members of the community can lead to a richer understanding of
that community’s traditions and values than is possible through a small number of visits,
in which outsiders make limited observations and conduct a few interviews (LeVine et al.,
1994). Extensive cultural or subcultural descriptions are particularly useful to investiga-
tors hoping to understand cultural conflicts and other developmental challenges faced by
minority children and adolescents in diverse multicultural societies (Segal, 1991; see also
Patel, Power, & Bhavnagri, 1996). But despite these clear strengths, ethnography is a
highly subjective method because researchers’ own cultural values and theoretical biases
can cause them to misinterpret what they have experienced. In addition, ethnographic
conclusions pertain only to the culture or subculture studied and cannot be assumed to
generalize to other contexts or social groups.
An example of ethnographic research was conducted by Posada and colleagues
(Posada, Carbonell, Alzate, & Plata, 2004). Because the various questionnaires and behav-
ioural coding schemes typically used to assess caregiver–infant interactions were devel-
oped in studies using Caucasian middle-class participants from industrialized countries,
Posada and colleagues chose ethnographic methods to assess mother–infant interactions
in middle- to lower-middle-class families in Bogotá, Colombia. They then compared the
results derived from observations made in the Colombian households to results derived
using previously developed assessments.
In a traditionally ethnographic manner, observers made eight to nine two-hour,
unstructured visits to 27 Colombian homes. During the visits, mothers were told to carry
on with their daily routines, behaving as they normally would. The observers interacted
with the families naturally. After each visit, they transcribed their observations. Repeat
visits were conducted by the same observer.
From the observers’ transcripts, 10 domains of maternal caregiving were identified.
Using an inductive approach, two of the researchers and an ethnographic expert reviewed
the transcripts. On first pass, they identified major caregiving themes. Then they reviewed
the transcripts in more detail, focusing on specifying the major domains and identifying
subdomains. In this way they were able to develop a set of culture-sensitive scales that
could be used alongside previously developed measures in order to assess the universality
of infant-sensitive maternal care.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
14 Part One | Theory and Research in the Developmental Sciences

The 10 scales of maternal sensitivity derived


from the observations included domains such as
promptness of response, enjoyment of interaction,
interactive smoothness, and quality of physical con-
tact. Results from the ethnographically derived
Colombian scales were highly consistent with
results from measures previously developed for
Caucasian middle-class and upper-middle-class fam-
Courtesy of the Shuar Health and Life History Project

ilies, lending credence to the notion that sensitive


caregiving behaviours are similar across cultures
and socioeconomic circumstances, at least within
the first few years of an infant’s life.
Another example of ethnological research
comes from the work of Gregory Bryant and Clark
Barrett (Bryant & Barrett, 2007). They observed and
interacted with the Shuar people, a culture of
hunter-horticulturalists living in the South American
rainforest who had little experience with people
Researchers attempt to understand cultural knowledge, values, and influences from industrialized countries. Bryant and Barrett
on the Shuar people by living within the community and participating in found evidence that Shuar adults were able to recog-
community life. nize infant-directed speech and even tell the differ-
ence between various intentions of speech (e.g., prohibitions, attention, approval)
in English, a language with which they have no experience. This exciting finding dem-
onstrated a universality in infant-directed speech that was not known before because all
previous research had been conducted with speakers from industrialized nations.

Psychophysiological Methods
psychophysiological methods Increasingly, developmentalists have turned to psychophysiological methods—
methods that measure the techniques that measure the relationship between physiological responses and
relationships between physiological behaviour—to explore the biological underpinnings of children’s perceptual, cogni-
processes and aspects of children’s
physical, cognitive, social, or
tive, and emotional responses. Psychophysiological methods are particularly useful
emotional behaviour/development. for interpreting the mental and emotional experiences of infants and toddlers, who
are unable to report such events (Bornstein, 1992).
Heart rate is an involuntary physiological response that is highly sensitive to psycho-
logical experiences. Compared to their normal resting, or baseline, levels, infants who are
carefully attending to an interesting stimulus may show a decrease in heart rate, those
who are uninterested in the stimulus may show no heart rate change, and others who are
afraid of or angered by the stimulus may show a heart rate increase (Campos, Bertenthal,
& Kermoian, 1992).
Measures of brain function are also very useful for assessing psychological state. For
example, electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings of brain wave activity can be obtained
by attaching electrodes to the scalp. Because different patterns of EEG activity charac-
terize different arousal states, such as sleep, drowsiness, and alertness, investigators can
track these patterns and determine how sleep cycles and other states of arousal change
with age. Novel stimuli or events also produce short-term changes in EEG activity. So an
investigator who hopes to test the limits of infant sensory capabilities can present novel
sights and sounds and look for changes in brain waves (called event-related potentials, or
ERPs) to determine whether these stimuli have been detected, or even discriminated,
because two stimuli sensed as “different” will produce different patterns of brain activity
(Bornstein, 1992). Researchers have used ERPs to explore infants’ reactions to others’
displays of emotions, finding that 7-month-olds attend more to facial displays of negative
rather than positive (or neutral) emotions (Leppanen, Moulson, Vogel-Farley, & Nelson,
2007), and that 12-month-olds are more inclined to use negative rather than positive (or
neutral) facial expressions as a guide for how they should be feeling or behaving in new
and uncertain situations (Carver & Vaccaro, 2007). More recently, technological advances

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Chapter 1 | Introduction to Developmental Psychology and Its Research Strategies 15

have made it possible to observe the brain “in action.” Using MRI (magnetic resonance
imaging) and fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) technology, researchers can
compare pictures taken before a subject engages in an activity and during the activity to
see which areas of the brain were activated.
A highly specialized observational technique that qualifies as a psychophysiological
method is eye-tracking. Eye-trackers are small video cameras that track a person’s eye
movement and allow researchers to precisely determine what stimulus in the visual field
the person is looking at. Eye-trackers have been mostly used in combination with com-
puter experiments where the visual field is limited by a computer screen. However, they
also can be used as wearable devices when participants freely roam around in a research
lab. Many aspects of children’s perceptual, cognitive, and socioemotional development
have been successfully studied using eye-trackers as gaze direction and looking times
reveal important aspects of children’s information processing. Through the use of eye-
trackers, researchers can find out where children seek information and how they form
expectations while watching a series of events. This is particularly useful in research with
younger children who have limited verbal abilities.
It is a common finding in developmental psychology that verbal methods underes-
timate the cognitive abilities of children relative to nonverbal methods. Take, for
instance, the famous false-belief tasks. In this task, children watch two puppets enacting
the following script: a girl (or boy depending on the participant’s sex) is coming home
from grocery shopping with her mother. At the store, the girl got a chocolate bar. The
girl does not want to eat the chocolate right away, so she puts it in the green kitchen
cabinet. The girl then goes outdoors to play with a friend. While the girl is playing out-
side the house, the mother needs some chocolate for baking a cake. She takes a few bits
from the girl’s chocolate and puts it in the blue kitchen cabinet. The girl comes back
from playing with her friend. She is hungry and wants to eat some of her chocolate.
Where does the girl look for the chocolate bar? In the green or the blue kitchen cabinet?
When asked this question verbally, 3- to 5-year-old children commonly respond “blue,”
which indicates that they do not understand that others can hold false beliefs that are
different from their own. However, using eye-tracking methods, Southgate, Senju, and
Csibra (2007) found that even 2-year-olds gaze at the right location while giving the
wrong verbal response. Thus, children’s gaze reveals cognitive abilities that are not
present in their verbal reports.
Psychophysiological states of parents can also be examined in investigations of chil-
dren’s development. For example, the hormone oxytocin is thought to play a role in
human attachment and social relationships. Feldman and her colleagues measured oxy-
tocin levels in pregnant women across their pregnancies and after the birth of their chil-
dren (Feldman, Weller, Zagoory-Sharon, & Levine, 2007). They found that the hormone
levels across pregnancy predicted behavioural measures of bonding between the mothers
and their babies after birth. Psychophysiological measures can also be used with older
children to assess aspects of development. As one example, blood pressure and cortisol
levels have been found in adolescence to be accurate measures of chronic stress that is
empirically related to chronic childhood poverty (Evans & Kim, 2007).
Though very useful, psychophysiological responses are far from perfect indicators of
psychological states. Even though an infant’s heart rate or brain wave activity may indi-
cate that he or she is attending to a stimulus, it is often difficult to determine exactly
which aspect of that stimulus (shape, colour, etc.) has captured the infant’s attention.
Furthermore, changes in physiological responses often reflect mood swings, fatigue,
hunger, or even negative reactions to the physiological recording equipment, rather than
a change in the infant’s attention to a stimulus or emotional reactions to it. For these
reasons, physiological responses are more likely to be valid indications of psychological
experiences when participants (particularly very young ones) are initially calm, alert, and
contented.
Table 1.2 provides a brief review of the data-gathering methods that we have exam-
ined thus far. In the sections that follow, we will consider how investigators might design
their research to test hypotheses and detect developmental continuities and changes.

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
16 Part One | Theory and Research in the Developmental Sciences

TABLE 1.2 Strengths and Limitations of Seven Common Research Methods


Method Strengths Limitations
Self-reports
Interviews and Relatively quick way to gather much information; standardized Data collected may be inaccurate or less than completely
questionnaires format allows the investigator to make direct comparisons honest, or may reflect variations in respondents’ verbal skills
between data provided by different participants. and ability to understand questions.
Clinical method Flexible methodology that treats subjects as unique individuals; Conclusions drawn may be unreliable in that participants are
freedom to probe can be an aid in ensuring that the participant not all treated alike; flexible probes depend, in part, on the
understands the meaning of the questions asked. investigator’s subjective interpretations of the participant’s
responses; can be used only with highly verbal participants.
Observational methods
Naturalistic observation Allows study of behaviour as it actually occurs in the natural Observed behaviours may be influenced by observer’s presence;
environment. unusual or undesirable behaviours are unlikely to be observed
during the periods when observations are made.
Structured observation Offers a standardized environment that provides every child Contrived observations may not always capture the ways
an opportunity to perform target behaviour; excellent way to children behave in the natural environment.
observe infrequent or socially undesirable acts.
Case studies Very broad method that considers many sources of data when Kind of data collected often differs from case to case and may
drawing inferences and conclusions about individual be inaccurate or less than honest; conclusions drawn from indi-
participants. vidual cases are subjective and may not apply to other people.
Ethnography Provides a richer description of cultural beliefs, values, and Conclusions may be biased by the investigator’s values and

© Cengage Learning 2014


traditions than is possible in brief observational or interview theoretical viewpoints; results cannot be generalized beyond
studies. the groups and settings that were studied.
Psychophysiological Useful for assessing biological underpinnings of development Cannot indicate with certainty what participants sense or feel;
methods and identifying the perceptions, thoughts, and emotions of many factors other than the one being studied can produce a
infants and toddlers, who cannot report them verbally. similar physiological response.

Detecting Relationships: Correlational and


Experimental Designs
Once researchers have decided what they want to study, they must devise a research plan,
or design, that permits them to identify relationships among events and behaviours and
to specify the causes of these relationships. Here we consider the two general research
designs that investigators might employ: correlational and experimental designs.

The Correlational Design


correlational design In a correlational design, the investigator gathers information to determine whether
a type of research design that two or more variables of interest are meaningfully related. If the researcher is testing a
indicates the strength of associations specific hypothesis (rather than conducting preliminary descriptive or exploratory
among variables; though correlated
variables are systematically related,
research), he or she will be checking to see whether these variables are related as the
these relationships are not necessarily hypothesis specifies they should be. No attempts are made to structure or manipulate the
causal. participants’ environment in any way. Instead, correlational researchers take people as
they find them—already “manipulated” by natural life experiences—and try to deter-
mine whether variations in people’s life experiences are associated with differences in
their behaviours or patterns of development.
To illustrate the correlational approach to hypothesis testing, let’s work with a
simple theory specifying that youngsters learn a lot from watching television and are apt
to imitate the actions of the characters they observe. One hypothesis we might derive
from this theory is that the more frequently children observe TV characters who display
violent and aggressive acts, the more inclined they will be to behave aggressively toward
their own playmates. After selecting a sample of children to study, our next step in testing
our hypothesis is to measure the two variables that we think are related. To assess chil-
dren’s exposure to violent themes on television, we might use the interview or natural-
istic observational methods to determine what each child watches and then count the

NEL
Copyright 2020 Nelson Education Ltd. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content
may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Nelson Education reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
equipment.
"And this," said Arden as Walt vanished into the refrigerator once
again, "will be known as the first time Walt Franks ever spent so
much time in here without a drink!"
"Time," said Joe, "will tell."

Half way from Lincoln Head to Canalopsis, Barney Carroll was


examining a calendar. "Christmas," he said absently.
Christine Baler stretched slender arms. "Yeah," she drawled, "and on
Mars."
Her brother Jim smiled. "Rather be elsewhere?"
"Uh-huh," she said.
"On Terra, where Christmas originated? Where Christmas trees
adorn every home, and the street corners are loaded with Santa
Clauses? Where—"
"Christmas is a time for joy," said Christine. "Also to the average
party, Christmas means snow, wassail, and friends dropping in. Me,
I'm acclimated—almost—to this chilly Martian climate. Cold weather
has no charm for your little sister, James."
"Oh," said Barney.
"Oh," echoed Jim, winking at his side-kick.
"Don't you 'Oh' me," snorted Christine.
"Oh?" repeated Barney. "Okay, woman, we get it. Instead of the cold
and the storm you'd prefer a nice warm climate like Venus?"
"It might be fun," she said evasively.
"Or even better," said Jim Baler to Barney Carroll, "we might visit
Venus Equilateral."
Christine's evasive manner died. "Now," she said, "you've come up
with a bright idea!"
Barney chuckled. "Jim," he said, "call Walt Franks and ask him if he
has a girl for us?"
"He has quite a stock in his little black book," remarked Jim.
"We'll drop in quietly, surprise-like," announced Christine. "And if
there's any little black book, I'll see that you two Martian wolves
divide 'em evenly."
"Walt is going to hate us for this," chuckled Jim. "Accessories to the
fact of his lost bachelorhood. Okay, Chris, pack and we'll—"
"Pack nothing," laughed Christine. "I've packed. For all three of us.
All we need is our furs until we get to Canalopsis. Then," she added
happily, "we can dress in light clothing. I'm beginning to hate cold
weather."
"How about passage?" asked Barney. "Or did you—"
Christine nodded. "The Martian Girl leaves Canalopsis in about three
hours. We pause at Mojave, Terra, for six hours, and thence to
Venus Equilateral on the special trip that takes Christmas stuff out
there."
Jim Baler shrugged. "I think we've been jockeyed," he said. "Come
on, Barney, needs must when a woman drives."
"The quotation pertains to the devil," objected Barney.
"No difference," said Jim, and then he ducked the pillow that
Christine threw at him.
A half hour later they were heading for Canalopsis.

"Walt?" smiled Arden. "Oh, sure, Walt's fine."


"Then—?"
"Yeah," added Barney good naturedly, "do we find 'em in Joe's or
elsewhere?"
"The Joe-Section of the engineering has been completed," said
Arden with a grin. "They nearly drove Joe nuts for about a week."
"What were they doing?" asked Jim. "Building an electronically-
operated Martini?"
"When I tell you, you won't believe me," said Arden. "But they've
been living in Joe's refrigerator."
"Refrigerator?" gasped Christine.
"Just like a gang of unhung hams," said Arden. "But they're out now."
"Well! That's good."
Arden paused in front of three doors on the residence level near her
apartment and Jim, Christine, and Barney each put their traveling
bags inside. Then Arden led them high into the station where they
came to a huge bulkhead in which was a heavy door.
Arden opened the door and an icy blast came out.
"Jeepers!" exploded Christine.
"Hey! Ice-men!" called Arden.
From the inside of the vast room came Don, Walt, and Wes. They
were clad in heavy furs and thick gloves, Channing was carrying a
small pair of cutters that looked a bit ridiculous in the great glove.
"Well, holy rockets!" exploded Channing, "what gives?"
"Merry pre-Christmas," said Jim. Don whipped off a glove and Jim
wrung his hand unmercifully. Wes Farrell greeted Barney Carroll
jovially, while Walt Franks stood foolishly and gaped at Christine
Baler.
Christine looked the heavy clothing over and shook her head. "And I
came here to be warm," she said. "Come out from behind that fur,
Walt Franks. I know you!"
"What is going on?" asked Barney.
"It all started in Joe's refrigerator," said Wes. "We found that the cold
had crystallized a bit of metal in the compressor. We discovered that
it was radiating one of the super-frequencies of the crystal-alloy
level. When warm it didn't. So we've set up this super-cooler to make
checks on it. Looks big."
Channing waved toward the door. "We've got the ultimate in super-
coolers in there," he said. "Remember the principle of the sun-power
tube—that it will drain power out of anything that it is attuned to?
Well, we're draining the latent heat-energy out of that room with a
power-beam tube—actually we're transmitting it across space to
Pluto."
"Pluto?"
"Uh-huh. In effect, it is like trying to warm Pluto from the energy
contained in that room. Obviously we aren't going to melt much of
the solid-frozen atmosphere of Pluto nor create a warm and
habitable planet of it. We can run the temperature down to darned
near Nothing Kelvin without doing much of anything to Pluto."
"We're below the black-body temperature of Mars right now," said
Walt. "And the gadgetry is working so much better that we're going to
run it down to as far as we can get it."
"What do you hope to find?" asked Barney.
"Why, it looks as though we can make a set of crystals that will
permit instantaneous communication from one to the other."
"Sounds good."
"Looks good, so far," said Channing. "Want to see it?"
Christine looked at the thermometer set in the face of the door. She
turned back to the others and shook her head vehemently.
"Not for all the ice in Siberia," she said fervently.
Walt brightened. "How about some ice in a glass," he said.
"For medicinal purposes only," agreed Barney. "It's been deadly cold
on Mars—about a quart and a half of sheer and utter cold."
"Been cold in there, too," said Don. "Arden, you're out of luck—
you've stayed out of the cold."
"You try to freeze me out of this session," said Arden, "and you'll find
that I have the coldest shoulder in the Solar System."
As the party from Mars left the platform of the spacecraft that was
poised on the Landing Stage of Venus Equilateral, another landing
was made. This landing came from the same ship, but unlike the
arrival of the Balers and Barney Carroll, the later landing was
unseen, unknown, and unwanted.
Mark Kingman had been a stowaway.
Now, most stowaways are apprehended because success in such a
venture is difficult. To properly stow away, it is calculated that more
than the nominal cost of the trip must be spent in planning and
preparation. Also, there is the most difficult of all problems—that of
stepping blithely ashore under the watchful eye of purser or authority
whose business it is to see that all the passengers who embarked
ultimately disembark—no more and no less; plus or minus zero. (It is
considered that an infant born aboard ship is a legal passenger and
not a stowaway. This is a magnanimity on the part of the
transportation companies who understand that they might have
difficulty in persuading any court that the will exists to defraud the
company of rightful revenues, etc.) (A death and burial at sea is also
ignored; the transportation company has already collected for a full
fare!)
But Mark Kingman had done it. He had come aboard in a large
packing case, labelled:
CERTIFIED UNIQUES!
(Identium Protected)
Under NO Circumstances
will
DUPLICATOR
or
MATTER TRANSMITTER
Be Tolerated
With magnificent sophistry, Kingman was within the letter of the law
that did not permit false representation of contained merchandise.
For he, a human being, was a certified unique, he having never been
under the beam of the integrator scanning beam of the matter
duplicator or transmitter. Nor had any other living human, for that
matter. The identium protection was insurance on all such cargoes; it
prevented some overly—or underly bright clerk from slipping the
package into a duplicator to make shipping easier. Identium
exploded rather violently under the impact of the scanner beam, it
will be recalled.
Along with Kingman was a small battery-powered duplicator, and a
set of recordings. The duplicator produced fresh air as needed,
water, food, and even books, games, and puzzles for solitary
entertainment. Waste material went into the matter bank, proving the
earlier statements that with a well-equipped duplicator and a set of
recordings, any man can establish a completely closed system that
will be valid for any length of time desired.
When the ship landed, Kingman tossed all the loose material into the
duplicator and reduced it to nonhomogeneous matter in the matter
bank. Then he turned the duplicator-beam against the side-wall of
the huge box and watched the side-wall disappear into the machine.
He stepped out through the opening, which was calculated to miss
the concealed plates of identium installed to prevent just this very
thing. Kingman, of course, had planned it that way.
Once outside, Kingman set the duplicator on the deck between other
cases and snapped the switch. The scanner beam produced books
from Kingman's own library which he packed in the case. Then by
reversing the direction of depth scan without changing the vertical or
horizontal travel, Kingman effected a completed reversal of the
restoration. The side of the packing case was re-established from
the inside out, from the original recording which, of course, was
made from the other side. It re-formed perfectly, leaving no seam.
Kingman went down an unused shaft to the bottom of the ship,
where he drilled down with the duplicator through the bottom of the
ship where it stood upon the Landing Stage. Down through the stage
he went and into a between-deck volume that was filled with girders.
He re-set the duplicator and replaced landing stage and the ship's
hull.
By the time that the party had adjourned to Joe's, Mark Kingman
was high in the Relay Station, near the center line and a full mile and
a half from the Landing Stage. He was not far from the vast room
that once contained a lush growth of Martin Sawgrass, used before
the advent of the duplicator, for the purification of the atmosphere in
Venus Equilateral.
He was reasonably safe. He knew that the former vast storages of
food and supply were no longer present, and that being the case,
few people would be coming up to this out of the way place almost a
third of a mile above the outer radius of the station where the
personnel of Venus Equilateral lived and worked.
He started his duplicator and produced a newly charged battery first.
He tossed the old one into the matter bank. He'd have preferred a
solar energy tube, but he was not too certain of Sol's position from
there and so he had to forego that.
Then he used the duplicator to produce a larger duplicator, and that
duplicator to make a truly vast one. The smaller numbers he shoved
into the larger one.
From the huge duplicator, Kingman made great energy-beam tubes
and the equipment to run them. Taking his time, Kingman set them
up and adjusted them carefully.
He pressed the starting button.
Then a complete connection was established between an area high
in the station but a good many thousand feet away—and on the
other side of the central axis—through the energy-beam tubes, and a
very distant receptor tube on the planet Pluto.
"This," punned Kingman, "will freeze 'em out!"
His final act before relaxing completely was to have the huge
duplicator build a small but comfortable house, complete with
furniture and an efficient heating plant. Then he settled down to wait
for developments.

"So what brings you out to Venus Equilateral?" asked Don.


"Christmas," said Barney. "We—Christine—thought that it might be
nice to spend Christmas with old friends in a climate less violent than
Mars."
"Well, we're all tickled pink," nodded Arden.
"Frankly," grinned Jim Baler, "my charming sister has set her sights
on your bachelor playmate."
"I think it is mutual," said Arden. "After all, Walt has had a lot of
business to tend to on Mars. He used to use the beams to conduct
business—in fact he still does most of it by communications when it
isn't Mars—but give him three ten-thousandths of an excuse and
he's heading for Canalopsis."
"I noted with interest that Christine was quite willing to help him
work."
"Fat lot of work they'll accomplish."
"Speaking of work, Wes, what goes on right now in this deal?"
"We've just set up a modulator," said Wes. "I'm modulating the
current since the magnetic field is supplied by a permanent magnet
and the monochromatic light comes from an ion arc. Using varying
light seems to widen the response band with a loss in transmission
intensity. This way, you see, all the energy going into the crystal is
transmitted on a single band, which is of course a matter of
concentrated transmission."
"That sounds sensible. Also, if this gets to sounding practical, it is
quite simple to establish and maintain a high-charge permanent
magnet field, and also a monochromatic light from a continuous gas-
arc. Easier, I'd say, than making ammeters all read alike."
"Utopia," said Wes Farrell, "is where you can use any handy meter
and find it within one tenth of one percent of any other—including the
Interplanetary Standard."
Channing observed that Utopia was far from achieved. Then he said:
"You've got the Thomas gents out in a ship with another crystal set-
up?"
"Anopheles," said Farrell. "Will shortly head for Mars with the other
half of the gear in another refrigerated compartment. If this proves
practical, Pluto is going to become useful."
Arden nodded absently. "I've always claimed that there is a practical
use for everything."
Channing opened his mouth to say something and had it neatly
plugged by Arden's small hand. "No, you don't," she said. "We've all
heard that one."
"Which one?" asked Farrell.
"The one about the navel being a fine place to hold the salt when
you're eating celery in bed," said Arden. Channing removed Arden's
hand from his mouth and placed it in hers. "You done it," he told her
ungrammatically. "For which I'll not tell you what Walt and Christine
are doing right now."
Arden's attempt to say, "Pooh. I know," was thoroughly stifled and it
came out as a muffled mumble.
Channing turned to Wes and asked: "Have any good theories on this
thing?"
Farrell nodded. "I noted that the energy entering the crystal was not
dissipated as heat. Yet there was quite a bit of energy going in, and I
wanted to know where it was going. Apparently the energy going in
to the crystal will only enter under the influence of a magnetic field.
Changing the field strength of the magnet changes the band, for the
transmission to the similar crystal ceases until the other one has had
its magnetic field reduced in synchronous amount. Also, no energy is
taken by the crystal unless there is an attuned crystal. The power
just generates heat, then, as should be normal.
"So," said Wes thoughtfully, "the propagation of this communicable
medium is powered by the energy going into the crystal. Crystals
tend to vibrate in sympathy with one another; hitting one with a light
hammer will make the other one ring, and vice versa. I've tried it with
three of them, and it makes a complete three-way hookup. As soon
as Chuck and Freddie Thomas get out a good way, we'll be able to
estimate the velocity of propagation, though I think it is the same as
that other alloy-transmission band we've been using."
Channing grinned. "The speed of light, squared?"
Farrell winced. That argument was still going on, whether or not you
could square a velocity. "We'll know," he said quietly.
The loudspeaker above Farrell's desk hissed slightly, and the voice
of Freddie Thomas came in: "I'm about to trust my precious life once
more to the tender care of the hare-brained piloting of my semi-idiot
brother. Any last words you'd like to have uttered?"
Wes picked up a microphone and said: "Nothing that will bear
transmission under the rules. If there's anything I want to tell you, I'll
call you on this—and if this doesn't work, we'll try the standard.
They're on your course?"
"On the button all the way—they tell me."
"Well, if you jiggle any, call us," said Farrell, "either on the standard
space phone or this coupled-crystal set-up."
Channing grinned. "So it has a name?"
Freddie laughed. "We never did settle on a name for the driver-
radiation communication system. So we're starting this one off right.
It's the Coupled-Crystal Communicator. For short, 'Seesee', see?"
Channing returned the laugh. "Seeseesee, or Seesee, understand?"
Chuck Thomas chimed in. "My semi-moronic brother will delay this
take-off if he doesn't sharpen up," he said. "What he means is:
Seesee, get it?"
"I get it," replied Channing.
And they did get it. Hour followed hour and day followed day from
take-off to turnover, where there was no Doppler effect even though
the velocity of the ship was fiercely high.
The hours fled by in a working flurry of tests and experiments and
almost-constant talk between the arrowing ship and Venus
Equilateral....

"It doesn't add up," complained Walt Franks.


Christine looked up from her book and waited.
"Something's more'n we bargained for," he said.
"What?" asked Christine.
"Why, that area we're chilling off is cooling far too fast."
"I should think that would be an advantage," said Christine.
"Maybe—and maybe not," said Walt. "The big thing is that things
should behave according to rules. When they do not, then's when
people make discoveries that lead to new rules."
"That I don't follow," said Christine.
"Well, in this case we know to several decimal places the heat
equivalent of electrical energy. Three thousand, four hundred
thirteen kilowatt hours equals one B.T.U.—a British Thermal Unit.
We know the quantity of electrical power—the number of kilowatts—
being coursed through the tubes en route to Pluto. We know by
calculation just how many calories of heat there are in the area we're
cooling off—and therefore we can calculate the time it will take to
reduce the temperature of that area a given number of degrees
centigrade. We're about double."
"And—you were starting to explain something different," said
Christine.
"Oh—yes. Well, for a number of years—several thousand, in fact—it
was taught that a heavy mass falls faster than a light mass. Then
Galileo tossed rocks off of the Tower at Pisa and showed that a small
stone and a large stone fall equally fast. That was a case where
definitely provable evidence was at variance with the rules. They
couldn't revise the actuality, so they had to revise the rules."
"I see. And now because that area is cooling off much faster than
anticipated, you anticipate that something is not behaving according
to the rules?"
"Bright girl," chuckled Walt.
"Thank you, kind sir," laughed Christine. "But remember that I was
raised in a bright family."
"Come on," said Walt. "We're going to investigate."
"In that cold room?" asked Christine with some concern.
Walt nodded. "You'll get used to it," he said absently, collecting a few
instruments.
"Look, Walt," said Christine in a scathing tone, "I'm used to it! That's
why I came to Venus Equilateral from Mars. Remember?"
Walt looked at her, wondering. But Christine wore a smile that took
most of the sting out of her words.
"Lead on, Walt. I can take a bit of chill. In fact," she said with a half-
smile, "under the proper circumstances, a bit of chill is fun."
Walt finished collecting his equipment and packed it into two carrying
cases. Then from a closet, he took electrically warmed clothing,
helped Christine into hers, climbed into his own, and then they took
the long trek along corridors and up elevators to the cold room.
"It's cold even here," said Christine.
"The room leaks bad," said Walt. "Wes Farrell's hobby these days is
making synthetic elements on the duplicator—he uses a filter to get
a mono-atomic pattern and then heterodynes the resulting signal to
atomic patterns above the transuranic system. But in all of Wes
Farrell's playing at making synthetic transuranic elements, he hasn't
come up with anything like a good heat insulator yet. We did toy with
the idea of hermetically sealing in a double wall and piping some of
the vacuum of interstellar space in there. But it was too vast a
project. So we let some heat leak and to hell with it."
Christine shuddered. "I've never really appreciated the fact that
Venus Equilateral is really just a big steel capsule immersed in the
vacuum of interplanetary space," she said. "It's so much like a town
on Terra."
"Inside, that is," grinned Walt. "There's a nice queasy thrill awaiting
you when first you stand in an observation blister made of
plastiglass."
"Why?" she asked.
"Because first you are terrified because you are standing on a
bubble that is eminently transparent and looking down beneath your
feet, you see the stars in the sky. You know that 'down' to the
working and residence section of the station is actually 'out and
away' from the axis of the station, since it revolves about the long
axis to provide a simulated gravity plus gyroscopic action to stabilize
the beam-stage and pointers. Well, when you go down—and again
'Down' is a relative term meaning the direction of gravitic thrust—into
one of the blisters, your mind is appalled at the fact that your feet are
pressing against something that your eyes have always told you is
'up'. The stars. And then you realize that between you and the
awesome void of space in just that thin glass.
"You end up," he grinned, "being very careful about banging your
heels on the floor of the station for about a week."
"Well thanks for the preparation," said Christine.
"You'll still go through it," he told her. "But just remember that
anybody on the other side of the station, standing in a similar blister
a mile 'above' your head, is standing feet 'upward' with respect to
you. But he, too, is being thrown out and away by centrifugal force."
Walt put his equipment down and rummaged through it. He selected
a supersensitive thermocouple and bridge and fixed the couple to
one of the fixtures in the room. He balanced the bridge after the
swinging needle came to a halt—when the thermocouple junction
had assumed the temperature of the fixture. "Now," he said, "we'll
read that at the end of a half hour and we'll then calculate the caloric
out-go and balance it against the kilowatts heading out through the
energy beam."
"And in the meantime?" asked Christine.
"In the meantime, we measure the electrical constants to within an
inch of their lives," he told her. "I've got a couple of real fancy meters
here—this one that I'm hooking across the original wattmeter in the
circuit measures the wattages in the region between one hundred
thousand kilowatts and one hundred ten thousand kilowatts.
Designed especially as a high-level meter."
Walt clipped the portable meters in place and made recordings.
Finally he nodded. "Right on the button," he said. "Just what the
meters should read."
The crystal began to vibrate faintly, and Walt mentioned that either
Wes Farrell was calling Freddie Thomas or vice versa. "Can't hear it
very well," complained Walt, "because Wes has the amplifiers
downstairs, both incoming amplifier from the dynamic pick-up—we
had to give up the standard crystal because it is expected to get cold
enough to make the crystal too brittle—-to the modulating
equipment. The monitor-speaker is outside—we haven't been in
here enough to make use of it since our first tries."
Walt took a look at the bridge on the thermocouple and nodded
vaguely. He killed more time by showing Christine the huge tube that
drained the latent heat out of the room and hurled it across the solar
system to Pluto.
"Y'know," he grinned as a thought struck him, "I think we've licked
the Channing Layer that so neatly foiled Mark Kingman and Terran
Electric on that solar power project."
"Yes?"
"Sure," he said. "All we do is set up a real beam-input device on the
moon, for instance, and then use a batch of these things to draw the
power from there."
"But how about the formation of ozone?"
"That'll have to be checked," said Walt. "For Pluto hasn't got a
Channing Layer, of course, and our station out there is no criterion.
But you note there is no smell of ozone in here. That leads me to
think that w've given Terran Electric the runaround once more. Funny
thing about Kingman, if someone gave him this development, he'd
never think of reversing it to bring energy in."
"From what I know of the man," said Christine, "he'd not think of
reversing, but he would think of perverting."
"Christine!" shouted Walt.
"Huh?" asked the bewildered girl.
"You may have had your thought for the week!"
Walt tried a bit of Indian War Dance but failed because the pseudo-
gravitic force was too light to hold him down. They were too close to
the axis for full force.
"But I don't understand."
Walt laughed hugely and hugged her. Christine was lissome in the
curve of his arm as she relaxed against him.
Walt looked down at her for what seemed to be a long time while the
stream of highly-technical thinking and deduction gave way to a
series of more fundamental thoughts. Then he added his other arm
to the embrace, and Christine turned to face him. He kissed her
gently; experimentally—and discovered instead of resistance there
was co-operation. His kiss became fervent and Christine's lips
parted beneath his.
Some minutes later, Christine leaned back in his arms and smiled at
him affectionately. "I was wondering if you'd ever get around to that,"
she said softly.
Walt grinned. "Have I been had?"
"I had Jim pack the all-white shotgun," she told him.
"Shucks, why not just have him threaten to sit on me?" asked Walt.
He kissed her again.
"Now," she said after an appropriate and pleasant interval, "just what
was my 'thought for the week'?"
"Kingman," he said, his forehead creasing in a frown.
"Kingman?"
"We've no corner on brains," said Walt. "Anybody tinkering with
these energy tubes might easily devise the same thing. Kingman's
immediate thought would be to freeze us out, I betcha."
Walt kissed her again and then let her go, "Let's do some juggling
with figures," he said.
"What kind of?"
"The Laws of Probability aided by a bit of sheer guesswork and
some shrewd evaluation of the barrister's mind."
Christine smiled. "You can speak plainer than that," she said.
"I know," he replied, reaching for his bag of gear, "but there's a lady
present."
"You forget that the lady thought of it," Christine pointed out. "So let's
go and find the—barrister."
"It ought to show, though," observed Walt. "And yet, my lady, we can
check whether there has been cross-duggery at the skull-roads by
making a brief observation along here somewhere."
"How?"
"Well, about fifty yards up this corridor there is a wall-thermostat."
"You think that if Kingman were trying to chill-off the place, he'd have
bollixed the thermostats so they can't heat up the place and
compensate?"
Walt nodded. "He'd do it, not knowing that we had all the near-by
circuits shut off for our own experiment, no doubt."
"You don't suppose Kingman knew about this idea and decided to
add to the general effect?" asked Christine.
Walt shook his head. "He'd assume that someone would be rambling
up here off and on to look at the works. He'd automatically choose
another place if he thought we had this one under observation."
Walt stopped at the thermostat and with a screwdriver he removed
the face of the instrument. He reached down into his tool-pocket and
took out a long, slender pair of tweezers. He probed in the depths of
the thermostat and come out with a tiny square of paper.
He held it up for Christine to see.
"Stickum on one side held it until the contacts closed," he said.
"Then it made a darned good insulator. Betcha this slip of paper
came from Terran Electric!"
"Now what?" asked Christine.
"I'm going to call Don," said Walt. "Iffen and providen we can find a
live jack." He took a handset from his kit of tools and plugged it into
the jack below the thermostat. He jiggled a tiny switch and pressed a
little red button, and after a full three minutes, he said "Damn," under
his breath and dropped the handset back into his tool kit.
"Nobody's paying much attention to the telephone from this section
of Venus Equilateral any more," he said. "There's a live one in the
cold room, though. Let's take a look around first."
"Which way?"
Walt thought for a moment. "We set the cold room about one-third of
the way from the North End because it was as far from the rest of the
station's operating and living section as possible while
commensurable with being reasonably close to the labs," he said.
"We're not very far—perhaps a hundred yards from the axis. We're
about a mile from the North End.
"Now, if I were Kingman, I'd set up shop in some place as far from
the operating section as possible commensurable with an out of the
way place—and definitely far from the laboratories. Then I'd select a
place as far from me as I could get without too much danger of
having the effect detected."
Christine nodded. "If Venus Equilateral were a cube, you'd take one
corner and chill off the opposite corner."
"Venus Equilateral is a cylinder, and the skin is filled with people.
However, you can set up an equation in differential calculus that will
give you two spots as far from one another as possible with the least
danger of detection from the ends or skin of a cylinder. The answer
will give you two toroidal volumes located inside of the cylinder. You
set your workshop in one and start the chill-off in the other—and
right across the center from you."
"And?" prompted Christine with a smile.
"We used the same equation to locate the least dangerous place.
Predicated on the theory that if the personnel need be protected
from the danger area as much as the danger area need be
concealed from people, we can assume the use of the same
constants. Now, since by sheer coincidence Markus the Kingman
selected a spot in the toroid that we also selected, it narrows our
search considerably."
"In other words, we chase down the length of the station, cross the
axis, and knock on Kingman's door."
"Right," said Walt.
And being firmly convinced that mixing pleasure with business often
makes the business less objectionable, Walt kissed Christine once
more before they started toward the place where they expected to
find their troublemaker.

"About here," said Walt, looking up at a smooth bulkhead.


"How are we going to find him?" asked Christine. The corridor was
long and die-straight, but both walls were sheer for thirty feet and
unbroken.
"Look, I guess," said Walt, uncertainly. "I'm not too familiar with this
section of the station. When I was first here—many years ago—I
spent a lot of spare time roaming and exploring these seldom-used
corridors. But my Boy Scout hatchet wouldn't cut trail-blazes on the
steel walls."
He laughed a bit thoughtfully, and then he put his hands to his
mouth, cupping them like a megaphone, and he yelled:
"Hey! Kingman! We're on to you!"
"But what good will that do?" asked Christine doubtfully.
"Might scare him into action," said Walt. "Easiest way to shoot
pa'tridge is to flush it into the open. Otherwise you might walk over a
nest and never see it. I—Holy Grease!"
A four foot section of the wall beside them flashed into nothingness
with neither sound nor light nor motion. It just disappeared. And as
they goggled at the vacant square, an ugly round circle glinted in the
light and a sourly-familiar voice invited them in—or else!
"Well," said Walt Franks, exhaling deeply. "If it isn't Our Legal Lamp
himself!"
Kingman nodded snappishly. "You were looking for me?"
"We were."
"It's too bad you found me," said Kingman.
"It was just a matter of time before you dropped all pretense of being
thinly legal," said Walt scathingly. "Give you credit, Kingman, for
conducting yourself as close to the line without stepping over for a
long time. But now you can add breaking and entering to kidnaping
to whatever other crimes you have committed."
Kingman smiled in a superior manner. "I might," he said suavely,
"add murder. There would be no corpus delicti if both of you were fed
into the duplicator."
"You can't record a human being," said Walt.
"Don't be stupid," said Kingman. "Who said anything about making a
record?"
Walt admitted that this was so.
Then Kingman snapped the switch on the duplicator and the wall
was re-established. Then he forced Christine to tie Walt, after which
he tied Christine and then checked and added to Walt's bonds from

You might also like