Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 33

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/14187656

An Integrative Model for the Study of Developmental Competencies in


Minority Children

Article in Child Development · November 1996


DOI: 10.2307/1131600 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

2,440 8,342

7 authors, including:

Cynthia García Coll Keith Crnic


Brown University and University of Puerto Rico Arizona State University
197 PUBLICATIONS 14,266 CITATIONS 136 PUBLICATIONS 16,691 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Barbara Wasik
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
71 PUBLICATIONS 6,656 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Cynthia García Coll on 08 April 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


An Integrative Model for the Study of Developmental Competencies in Minority
Children

Cynthia García Coll; Gontran Lamberty; Renee Jenkins; Harriet Pipes McAdoo; Keith Crnic;
Barbara Hanna Wasik; Heidie Vázquez García

Child Development, Vol. 67, No. 5. (Oct., 1996), pp. 1891-1914.

Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199610%2967%3A5%3C1891%3AAIMFTS%3E2.0.CO%3B2-K

Child Development is currently published by Society for Research in Child Development.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained
prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in
the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/journals/srcd.html.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic
journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers,
and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take
advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

http://www.jstor.org
Mon Jan 28 16:00:24 2008
Theoretical Papers

An Integrative Model for the Study of


Developmental Competencies in Minority
Children

Cynthia Garcia Coll Keith Crnic


Brown University Pennsylvania State University

Gontran Lamberty Barbara Hanna Wasik


Maternal and Child Health Bureau University of North Carolina

Renee Jenkins Heidie Viizquez Garcia


Howard University Brown University

Harriet Pipes McAdoo


Michigan State University

G A R CCOLL,
~ CYNTHIA; LAMBERTY, GONTRAN; JENKINS, RENEE;MCADOO,HARRIET PIPES;CRNIC,
KEITH:WASIK,BARBARA HANNA:and VAzou~zG A R C ~HEIDIE. . A n Integrative Model for the
study'of ~ e v k l o n m e n t a l~ o m ~ k t e n c i in
e s Minority children. CHILDDEVELOPMENT, 1696, 67,
1891-1914. In thip article, a conceptual model for the study of child development in minority
populations in the United States is proposed. In support of the proposed model, this article
includes ( a ) a delineation and critical analysis of mainstream theoretical frameworks in relation
to their attention and applicability to the understanding of developmental processes in children
of color and of issues at the intersection of social class, culture, ethnicity, and race, and (b) a
description and evaluation of the conceptual frameworks that have guided the extant literature
on minority children and families. Based on the above considerations, an integrative conceptual
model of child development is presented, anchored within social stratification theory, emphasiz-
ing the importance of racism, prejudice, discrimination, oppression, and segregation on the de-
velopment of minority children and families.

This article proposes a conceptual ity populations. A conceptual framework


model for the study of child development i n that incornorates a n d e m ~ h a s i z e sessential
minority populations in t h e United States. factors for;nderstanding &e growth a n d de-
This work is t h e product of a multidiscipli- velopment of minority children a n d their
nary collaboration among the authors w h o families is critical to address omissions in
share a strong collective concern with the existing theoretical formulations and re-
absence of appropriate conceptual models or search. T h e proposed integrative model dif-
frameworks for conducting research that ad- fers from previous sociodevelopmental
dresses t h e diversity a n d strengths of minor- frameworks a n d models i n that it introduces

Preparation of this article was supported by cooperative agreement MCU-117007 between


the Maternal and Child Health Bureau (Title V, Social Security Act) and the National Center
for Education in Maternal and Child Health of Georgetown University. Portions of this paper
were presented at the Second National Head Start Research Conference, November 4-7, 1993,
in Washington, DC. We gratefully acknowledge the more than 75 scholars who contributed ideas
to this paper as part of study groups held in Boston, Washington, DC, and San Juan, Puerto Rico.
Special contributions were also made by Margaret B. Spencer, Mary Melo, Shelley Spisak,
Wendy K. K. Lam, and Katherine Magnuson. Reprint requests should be sent to Cynthia Garcia
Coll, Brown University, Department of Education, Box 1938, Providence, Rhode Island, 02912.
[Child Deoelopment, 1996,67,1891-1914. O 1996 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-392019616705-0007$01.001
1892 Child Development
considerations of both social position and so- tion and evaluation of the dominant concep-
cial stratification constructs at the core rather tual frameworks that have guided the extant
than at the periphery of a theoretical formu- literature on minority children and families
lation of children's development. and that exemplify the need for mainstream
models to incorporate and delineate other
A new model is needed because, tradi- important sources of influence. Based on the
tionally, the interaction of social class, cul- above considerations, an integrative concep-
ture, ethnicity, and race has not been in- tual model of child development is pre-
cluded at the core of mainstream theoretical sented. The model is anchored within social
formulations in the discipline of child devel- stratification theory (Attewell & Fitzgerald,
opment. For example, most of the prevalent 1980; Barber, 1957; Bendix & Lipset, 1966;
conce~tualframeworks do not emphasize Laumann, 1970; Tumin, 1967) and empha-
the social stratification system, or the social sizes the importance of racism, prejudice,
positions that comprise the scaffolding or discrimination, oppression, and segregation
structure of the system (i.e., social class, eth- to the development of minority children and
nicity, and race) and the processes and con- families.
sequences that these relative positions en-
gender for a child's development. This African-Americans and mainland Puerto
shortcoming is found even in most of the Ricans will be used as examples throughout
contextually based theoretical frameworks this article, although most of the issues
identified in the developmental literature as raised are generalizable to other ethnic and
organizational, transactional, and ecological. minority groups (Ogbu, 1981, 1987). Aside
from space considerations that preclude the
Accordingly, reviews of published re- inclusion of all minority groups, there are
search suggest a pattern of omission and ne- two main reasons for using these two groups
glect (Garcia Coll, 1990; McLoyd, 1990a; as examples. First, for most indicators of de-
McLoyd & Randolph, 1984, 1985). They in- velopment and social well-being (e.g., infant
dicate (1)the conspicuous absence of longi- mortality, low birthweight, teenage preg-
tudinal investigations on the normative de- nancy, and school drop-out), these two
velopment of minority children; (2) an groups occupy the most unfavorable posi-
emphasis on outcomes rather than on pro- tions in comparison to other minority groups
cess in what little research is being done on (Arcia, Keyes, & Gallagher, 1994; Becerra,
children of color; (3) a lack of attention to Hogue, Atrash, & PBrez, 1991; Collins &
intragroup variability and an emphasis on Shay, 1994; Mendoza, Glenn, Takata, &
between-group comparisons; (4) a disregard Martorell, 1994; Rodriguez, 1994; Ventura,
for the diversity inherent in some of the mi- 1994; Wise, Kotelchuck, Wilson, & Mills,
nority group categories in use; and (5) a min- 1985). Second, these two groups are exam-
imization of the effects of such social strati- ples of involuntary minorities (Ogbu, 1987)
fication derivatives as racism, prejudice, who were originally brought into the United
discrimination, and segregation on the de- States through slavery, conquest, or coloni-
velopment of minority children. These ex- zation. Although both groups have often
clusions and lack of attention to crucial as- been relegated to menial positions and de-
pects of the context in which children's nied integration into the mainstream, their
development takes place undermine a more contrasting social histories, languages, and
comprehensive scientific understanding of cultures provide two distinct reference
the minority child and raise questions about groups.
the validity of empirical knowledge about
children in general (McKinney, Abrams, This article has several recurrent
Terry, & Lerner, 1994). Moreover, they ham- themes. One is that neither early theorists
per our ability to intervene efficaciously and nor researchers have proposed a realistic un-
to lessen the deleterious effects of the less- derstanding of racial and ethnic groups in
than-optimal conditions experienced by the United States and their experiences
most minority and other low-income within a white, mainstream society. Certain
children. beliefs and practices such as ignoring minor-
ities, maintaining false stereotypes, andlor
This article includes (a) a critical analy- distorting their life styles have hindered the
sis of mainstream theoretical frameworks in understanding of different groups (Sue &
relation to the understanding of develop- Sue, 1990). Second, the expanded role that
mental processes in children of color and of family and kin networks play in develop-
issues at the intersection of social class, cul- mental processes for minority children may
ture, ethnicity, and race, and (b) a descrip- serve to protect them from economic hard-
Garcia Coll et al. 1893
ships and social and psychological sources influences often inhibit rather than facilitate
of oppression derived from their relative po- development. Indeed, as Spencer (1990)
sition in society (Harrison, Wilson, Pine, suggests, developmental adaptations of chil-
Chan, & Buriel, 1990; McAdoo, 1982). dren of color require insights not readily
Third, there is the need to more fully in- available from traditional paradigms.
clude contexts other than the family because
of their particular saliency in the develop- Early theoretical foundations of child
ment of minority children. Consequently, development generally failed to address is-
health and education should be viewed not sues related to life-course processes for chil-
pnly as important contributors to children's dren of color within a larger sociocultural
development, but also as outcomes in them- context (Sue & Sue, 1990). Though differ-
selves that share with other more tradition- ences in gender were frequently recognized
ally considered developmental outcomes as important by early researchers, race was
many of the same determinants and causal rarely considered (e.g., Goodenough & An-
sequencing. derson, 1931; Parten, 1932; Thomas, 1929).
When ethnic and racial factors were taken
Mainstream Theoretical Models: into account, they were typically incorpo-
Considerations of Race, Culture, rated within an evolutionary framework that
and Ethnicity in Child Development described differences between raciallethnic
groups rather than within groups and con-
Throughout Western history, develop- ceptualized these differences as evidence
mental theories have evolved toward a for either the genetic or the cultural inferior-
greater understanding of the child, pro- ity of ethniclracial groups relative to the
gressing from a lack of acknowledgment of white mainstream standard (see Darwin,
developmental differences between chil- 1859; de Gobineau, 1915; Galton, 1869; Jen-
dren and adults (see Locke, 169011913; sen, 1969; Reissman, 1962; Terman, 1916).
Rousseau, 176211938) toward recognition of Moreover, the focus on environmental in-
the unique characteristics of childhood and fluences on human learning and develop-
also of its complexity and diversity (see Dar- ment was limited primarily to immediate sit-
win, 1859; Erickson, 1950; Sears, Maccoby, uational events, and therefore sociocultural
& Levin, 1957; Vygotsky, 1978).These theo- factors were largely excluded from research
ries have not only influenced social values, efforts (e.g., Sears et al., 1957).
but have also been shaped by society's
views. These bidirectional influences are Mainstream developmental theories may
discernible in the treatment of race, ethnic nevertheless have an heuristic value in guid-
status, and gender, as well as other issues in ing the study of the normative develop-
child development (Young, 1974). mental process in children of color. Global
developmental theories can provide a gen-
There is no theoretical or empirical rea- eral framework for the development of more
son to assume that individual primary devel- specific predictive models. In particular, the
opmental processes operate differently for interplay of organizational (Cicchetti &
children of color than for Caucasian children Schneider-Rosen, 1986; Sroufe, 1979; Wer-
in Western society. Developmental pro- ner, 1948), transactional (Sameroff & Chan-
cesses (e.g., cognitive, affective, and social) dler, 1975; Sameroff & Fiese, 1990), life
probably emerge in similar fashion across ra- span (Lerner, 1989), and ecological theories
cially and ethnically diverse populations. (inclusive of person-process-context con-
However, developmental differentiation, cepts) (Bronfenbrenner, 1977, 1979; Bron-
beyond that related to constitutionally based fenbrenner & Crouter, 1983)has the capabil-
individual differences, is largely a function ity of addressing particular issues critical to
of the dynamic interaction between the developmental process in diverse popula-
child and both proximal and distal ecologies. tions of color, although both expansion and
As such, understanding the normal develop- greater specification are needed to realize
mental process of children of color requires this potential.
more explicit attention to the unique ecolog-
ical circumstances (e.g., the pervasive Although mainstream developmental
influence of racism) these children face. models could contribute to the conceptual-
Defining and integrating these unique eco- ization of developmental processes in chil-
logical circumstances that are not shared by dren of color (Slaughter-Defoe, Nakagawa,
Caucasian children becomes the basis for Takanishi, & Johnson, 1990), they have not
the formulation of theories of normal devel- met this promise. To date, these models
opment in children of color because their have been too narrowly defined and applied,
1894 Child Development
without elaborating those considerations work with which development of children
unique to populations of children of color of color can be best studied and understood.
(e.g., the culturally diverse physical and psy-
chological attributes of individual children Development in Children of Color:
of color, the contexts specific to their daily Dominant Conceptual Frameworks
experience, the racial and ethnic values that
influence their competencies, and the soci- The lack of attention to issues of race,
etal structures that limit them). Further, the ethnicity, and culture in developmental sci-
literature has proven to be more exclusive ence has resulted in a literature on minority
than inclusive. However, a call to become children and their families that concentrates
more inclusive does not represent a call to on explaining developmental deviations in
approach all populations from a similar comparison to white middle class popula-
view, nor does it imply that all populations tions rather than examining normative de-
can be understood from a single explanatory velopmental processes and outcomes. This
model. Indeed, a "one model fits all" ap- comparative paradigm has been exacerbated
proach exposes the well-documented dan- by a heavy reliance on two theoretical mod-
gers of race-comparative research (McLoyd els that have been used over the past 150
& Randolph, 1985). Further, it detracts from years to explain or describe differences
efforts to describe models that account for among ethniclracial groups. The genetically
intragroup variability in diverse populations deficient model (see Dunn, 1987; Herrn-
of color (McLoyd, 1990a). Given the vari- stein, 1971; Jensen, 1969; Shuey, 1966)
ability within populations of children of posits that differences in physical, intellec-
color (e.g., differing ethnicities, socioeco- tual, and psychological capacities between
nomic classes, or skin colors), these popula- races are innate. The culturally deficient
tions provide ideal samples with which to model (see Sears, 1975; Senn, 1975) con-
test our theories and the specific models to ceives of the "culturally deprived" as those
be developed. who lack the benefits and advantages of
white middle-class America and thus end up
Developing more inclusive models re- with developmental deficiencies and devi-
quires the rigorous specification and integra- ancies.
tion of contextual influences far beyond
what has been done to date in either ecologi- The conceptual and empirical litera-
cal theory or transactional theory. For exam- tures on Puerto Rican and African-American
ple, although cultural influence may well be children and their families have been
subsumed under the broad notion of mac- guided primarily by these deficiency models
rosystem influence, specific notions of cul- (Anastasi & Cordova, 1953; Anastasi & de
ture have been only marginally integrated Jesus, 1953; Armstrong, Achilles, & Sacks,
into ecological theory. Sameroff and Fiese 1935; Dunn, 1987; Glazer & Moynihan,
(1990) have incorporated culture more spe- 1963; Herrnstein, 1971; Jensen, 1969;
cifically within transactional theory by dis- Lewis, 1965; Miller, 1952; the Puerto Rican
cussing the importance of the cultural code Forum, 1964; the Puerto Rican Study, 1953-
as the primary regulator of family processes 1957, 1958; Sears, 1975; Senn, 1975). Ge-
that directly influence children's develop- netic and environmental deficiencies are
ment; however, these authors failed to spec- still considered to be the primary explana-
ify the mechanisms by which these distal tions for the poor school performance of Afri-
considerations shape development. can-American and Puerto Rican children
(Dunn, 1987; Ogbu, 1985). In addition, Afri-
In addition, social mechanisms such as can-American and Puerto Rican parents are
racism, discrimination, and prejudice have blamed for not transmitting the right educa-
not been routinely specified in analyses of tional values (i.e., white, middle-class com-
macrosystem influences, even though these petencies) to their children (see Bloom,
may well be the critical factors that underlie Davis, & Hess, 1965; Dunn, 1987; Passow,
the more commonly studied sociopsycholog- 1963).
ical aspects of developmental processes in
children of color. McAdoo (1992) has sug- Moreover, current research on African-
gested that ecological models may be appro- American and Puerto Rican children and
priate in describing environments of diverse their families continues to concentrate on
families and children, but only when they standard definitions of high risk and compe-
are extended to include societal racism, clas- tency. Although studies are addressing the
sism, and sexism. These mainstream theo- contributions of stress, poverty, and lack of
ries have not yet provided a specific frame- social support (rather than assuming genetic
Garcia Coll et al. 1895
or cultural deficits), the emphasis still re- conceptualization that reflects both the
mains on negative developmental outcomes. strengths and weaknesses in developmental
McLoyd (1990b) argues that the race- processes and competencies of these chil-
comparative research model encourages re- dren.
searchers to document how minority chil-
dren compare unfavorably with white An Integrative Model
children, and therefore concentrate on how
they are abnormal or incompetent. Similarly, We propose an integrative theoretical
Barbarin (1993) notes that most research on model that both incorporates and expands
African-American and Latino school-age current formulations of mainstream develop-
children has focused on aggression, delin- mental theoretical frameworks as well as
quency, attention deficits, and hyperactivity, culturally differentldiverse models. This
but it has not informed areas such as emo- model addresses two major considerations:
tional development or resiliency. (1)constructs salient only to populations of
color that contribute unique variance to their
In contrast to these deficiency models, developmental processes, and (2) constructs
there is a growing theoretical and empirical that are also relevant to the developmental
literature using culturally diverseldifference processes in other populations, but are dif-
models to guide the conceptualization and ferentiated on the basis of individual factors
investigation of minority children. The cul- that affect developmental processes. Figure
turally different model (see Boykin, 1978; 1 presents the model in schematic form,
Gibson, 1976; Ogbu, 1981; Ramirez & Cas- showing eight major constructs hypothe-
taneda, 1974) proposes that cultures and life- sized to influence developmental processes
styles different from the white middle-c1p.s for children of color.
mainstream are not pathological, deviant, or
deficient relative to the mainstream but We first provide a brief overview of the
rather legitimate and valuable in their own model as a framework for understanding the
right. This literature adapts existing con- relations among the eight constituent con-
cepts, measures, and diagnostic instruments structs. Of primary concern in this frame-
to the specific sociocultural contexts and cre- work is the preeminence of social position
ates new measures of competencies for these factors. These are attributes of individuals
populations. This body of work argues, for that societies use to stratify or place individ-
example, that by assuming that African- uals in the social hierarchy and that pertain
Americans and Puerto Ricans should have to children of color. These factors include
the same cultural imperatives (Cohen, 1971) (but are not limited to) race, social class, eth-
as middle-class whites, research is decontex- nicity, and gender. For example, included
tualizing these competencies from their cul- within considerations of race are factors such
tural, economic, and social realities (Ogbu, as skin color and racial features. These posi-
1985). In other words, the establishment and tion factors represent social addresses that
maintenance of white middle-class child- influence or create alternative develop-
rearing patterns as the standard for normal mental pathways to competence in these
development of intellectual, cognitive, and children. Further, these social position fac-
social competencies not only obscures cul- tors are not simply additive in their contribu-
tural differences in child rearing, but as- tions; rather, they have the potential to inter-
sumes that anything other than mainstream act in ways that magnify or diminish the
competencies are inferior. importance of the factors that follow (Bron-
fenbrenner & Crouter, 1983).
Although culturally diverseldifferent
models can capture the strengths within Although considered a primary con-
these populations, they have not yet ad- struct, social position does not directly affect
dressed in depth some of the critical factors developmental outcomes and the immediate
in the analyses of developmental competen- environments in which children of color
cies in children of color. These models in- grow. Rather, the effect of social position is
clude contextual variables but have yet to mediated through the pervasive social
fully articulate how variables such as racism, mechanisms of racism, prejudice, discrimi-
prejudice, discrimination, and other sources nation, and oppression. These factors, in
of oppression operate and influence devel- turn, create the segregated environments to
opmental outcomes. In addition, studies of which children of color and their families
children of color need to move from concep- are subjected. We hypothesize that the ef-
tualizing developmental outcome as either fects of racism on children of color operate
negative or positive to a more balanced through the creation of segregated contexts,
1896 Child Development

Temperament
Health S u u s
Biological Facton
Physical Charactenstic

Social Position Variables

Soclal Class
Ethn~caty
1
hotin@ahiMtlng
EovimruwnCI
Schoals
Neighborhoods
Health Care
Trad~uons&
Cultural Legacia
Economic & Palttical
H~sloria
Developmen@
Compete&
Cogntlive

Emotional
Linguistic
Bicularalism
Coping w~thRacism

Segregation
Res~dcntlal
FamUy
Smcture & Roles
Famlly Values. Beltefs & Goals
Ractal Socnal~zauon
Soclaconomlc Status

FIG. 1.-Integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority chil-
dren.

as opposed to a simple direct pathway of in- children's experience through social interac-
fluence. Segregation must be considered as tions in specific inhibiting and promoting
multifaceted, combining residential, eco- environments. Through these social interac-
nomic, and social and psychological dimen- tions the influence of the macrosystem fac-
sions. These elements create unique condi- tors (derived from the social stratification
tions that more directly influence individual system in place) directly affects develop-
developmental processes of children of mental processes in children of color.
color. The interplay of these three major de-
rivatives of social stratification (social posi- Inhibiting and promoting environments
tion, racism, and segregation) creates the in turn directly influence the adaptive cul-
unique conditions faced by children of color tures that are created in response to chil-
and affects the nature of the developmental dren's and families' experience within these
processes that operate and the eventual com- environments. An adaptive culture involves
petencies that result. These are "nonshared" a social system defined by sets of goals, val-
experiences with mainstream populations ues, and attitudes that differs from the domi-
and define the unique pathways of develop- nant culture. The adaptive culture evolves
ment for children of color. from a combination of both historical forces
and current demands. As Boykin and Toms
Moreover, we propose that segregation explain, "though a Black cultural orientation
(residential, economic, and social and psy- might be present, Blacks must still possess
chological) directly influences the various distinctly pragmatic and expedient ways of
inhibiting and promoting environments that responding to racially and economically
children of color experience. Within these problematic life circumstances" (Boykin &
multiple environments (e.g., the school, the Toms, 1985, p. 44).
neighborhood, and other institutions such as
health care) children are directly affected by Both inhibitinglpromoting environ-
the macrosystem factors. Although previ- ments and the adaptive culture directly in-
ously we have posited that racism and its fluence the nature of specific individual fam-
concomitant processes provide a macrosys- ily processes (the day-to-day interactions
tem context that indirectly influences the na- and experiences) and interact with the chil-
ture of the opportunities available to chil- dren's biological, constitutional, and psycho-
dren of color, processes such as racism, logical characteristics. Children are not sim-
discrimination, and prejudice directly affect ply passive recipients of their experience;
Garcia Coll et al. 1897
rather, they influence their family processes priately measure these demographic char-
and contribute to their own socialization. Fi- acteristics in studies is being addressed,
nally, children's developmental competen- their article highlights the difficulties that
cies emerge as a direct function of individual researchers have had defining and measur-
contributions of adaptive culture, family pro- ing these constructs.
cesses, and the child's own characteristics
operating through the interactions among Race.-There are two definitions of the
these systems of influence. Each of the mod- concept of race: the biological and the social.
els' constructs are discussed in more detail UNESCO's Statement on Race summarizes
in the following sections. the biological definition as " . . . a group or
population characterized by some concen-
Social Position Variables tration, relative as to frequency and distribu-
A fundamental assumption of this model tion, of hereditary particles (genes) or physi-
is that developmental outcome is profoundly cal characters, which appear, fluctuate, and
affected by the individual's social position often disappear in the course of time by rea-
derived from the social stratification system son of geographic andlor cultural isolation"
of any given society. Although no standard (Montague, 1972). Even at the biological
definition of social stratification exists, most level, there is great controversy about the
definitions denote a process that sorts indi- validity and origin of the concept of race
viduals into a hierarchy of groups based on (Yee, Fairchild, Weizmann, & Wyatt, 1993).
their imputed relative worth, utility, or im-
portance to the society in which they live As socially defined, the construct of race
(Tumin, 1967). constitutes a way of classifying individuals
and groups in the context of daily living,
Three assumptions are embedded in the usually on the basis of externally visible
concept of social stratification: (a) a social physical characteristics (Simpson & Yinger,
position such as social class, ethnicity, or 1953). The classification that ensues implic-
race carries with it varying degrees of segre- itly or explicitly attributes an inferior/supe-
gation in the spatial, physical, social, and rior social position to the group or individual
psychological environments; (b) the degree in question. This attribution of social posi-
of social mobility is a consequence of one's tion is often institutionalized, either for-
relative social position; and (c) individuals mally or informally, and guides to varying
develop a hierarchical attribution system degrees public as well as private behavior.
that consists of attitudes and beliefs about The social valuation of race and ethnicity
the self, as well as the persons both above evolves in response to changes in historical
and below on the social ladder. and economic circumstances.
Theoretically, the bases for imputing Skin color, hair texture, and facial char-
relative worth, utility, or importance can be acteristics are often used as indicators of
any personal or group attribute. The critical
prerequisite is that the characteristic or attri- race. This has been true especially when
considering the racial identity and social po-
bute is defined by the society as desirable1 sition of individuals with multiracial or bira-
undesirable and consequently merits differ-
ential rewards, such as access to scarce re- cial backgrounds. Historically in U.S. cul-
sources (Tumin, 1967). In the United States, ture, there has been a conceptual racial
race, social class, and ethnicity are the most dichotomy. Children of mixed ancestry are
important attributes on which our society is categorized as either white or black, de-
stratified. These categories are overlapping, pending on their features, and those who are
with resultant additive and multiplicative ef- considered black are relegated to an inferior
social. status (Hirschfeld, 1995; Massey,
fects, depending on the degree to which an
individual child occupies specific combina- 1993). Hirschfeld (1995) has demonstrated
tions of these social positions (e.g., a child that by the end of their grade school years
who is poor, dark skinned, and Puerto Ri- children expect that a child with one black
can). Gender further compounds the adver- parent will have a black external anatomy.
sities faced (Krieger, Rowley, Herman, Moreover, these children's thinking was
Avery, & Phillips, 1993). specific to racial categorization and asym-
metrical, in that they did not believe a child
A recent article by Entwisle and Astone would have brown hair if they had one par-
(1994) emphasizes the difficulty that re- ent with brown hair and one with blond hair,
searchers have had incorporating these so- nor did they predict that dark hair or skin
cial constructs in developmental research. dominated in animals. Therefore, racial cate-
Although the need to include and appro- gorization is a cultural construct sustained
1898 Child Development
by beliefs about the inheritability of identity conditions of life (e.g., occupation, educa-
which are passed on from generation to gen- tion) have an important influence on peo-
eration. ple's behavior, social class position, and
Moreover, the darker the skin color the value orientation and therefore in the trans-
more social penalties accrue (West, 1993). mission of these values to their children.
This situation exists across all race and eth- Ethnicity.-The term ethnicity is used
nic groupings within the United States, and principally to convey cultural distinctness
derives from . . . the salience of race in the
"
deriving mostly from national origin, lan-
United States . . . a n d . . . the unique status of guage, religion, or a combination thereof
African ancestry" (Massey & Denton, 1988). (Broom & Selznick, 1970; Harrison, Serafica,
Differences in innate abilities, poverty, and & McAdoo, 1984; Morris, 1968). Because it
culture are frequently imputed to be the is socially acquired, this cultural dis-
causes of racial and ethnic differentials in tinctness is subject to change over time
health and development to avoid acknowl- through the process of acculturation and as-
edging that racism may be the culprit (Ogbu, similation, unless other social barriers such
1991a). as race prevent full participation in the so-
Social class.-As used most often today, ciety.
the term social class refers to a unit of social As in the concepts of social class and
stratification in which individuals are classi- race, ethnic identification for the most part
fied on the basis of economic considerations includes valuation and attribution of infe-
(Gordon, 1963). Such classification implic- riorlsuperior social status, and it changes as
itly or explicitly posits commonalities in psy- a function of historical processes. For exam-
chosocial and cultural characteristics among ple, Masson and Verkuyten (1993) found that
the classified individuals arising from their ethnic identity was highly correlated with
unit's economic rank in the overall social in-group formation and preference. In addi-
structure (Tumin, 1967). tion, they found that more frequent cross-
More recent conceptualizations of social ethnic contact was associated with a de-
class include multiple levels, which are in- crease in in-group preference and formation,
dividual, household, and neighborhood, suggesting that in-group preferences and
both independently and in their interactions orientations may shift as a function of expo-
(Krieger et al., 1993). Research also suggests sure and contact with out-groups. However,
that age plays a significant role in how social while it has been widely noted that in-
class affects the self-esteem of individuals. tergroup contact lessens discrimination and
For example, Rosenberg and Pearlin (1978) hostility, negative intergroup contact can
suggest that social class is achieved by also reinforce hostility and prejudice (Mas-
adults and assigned to children. Conse- son & Verkuyten, 1993).
quently, they argue that the principles of
Gender.-As with race, there are both
self-esteem formation in children, adoles- biological and social aspects of the definition
cents, and adults are affected by social class of gender. One refers to the biological pri-
differently as each age group organizes their mary and secondary characteristics per-
"social class" experiences differently. Simi- taining to the ability to reproduce. Socially
larly, Wiltfang and Scarbecz (1990) found
defined gender involves the culture-bound
that adolescents' self-esteem was signifi- conventions about the appropriate roles and
cantly related to their own achievement behaviors for, as well as relations between,
rather than their parents' social class. Yet, women and men (Krieger et al., 1993). Gen-
they also found that some dimensions of pa- der relations, as part of society's social strati-
rental social class, such as measures of fication system, are supported by assump-
neighborhood unemployment, had a nega- tions of the innate superiority of men over
tive effect on adolescents' self-esteem.
women and the subsequent subordination of
Social class has also been examined as women by men. For minority children, their
it relates to values and orientation. For ex- expected role in society as a function of their
ample, Kohn (1977) suggests that social class gender also influences access to resources,
variables influence parental values and social interactions, and expectations, and
child-rearing practices, thereby influencing will consequently influence their develop-
how children develop and acquire the mental outcomes. For example, Stevenson,
knowledge and skills to cope with change Chen, and Uttal (1990) found that African-
and the conditions of life. Furthermore, American girls received significantly higher
Kohn posits that systematic differences in scores for comprehension at first grade and
Garcia Coll et al. 1899
computation at third grade when compared treated equitably, and what they deserve.
to boys. In addition, Spencer, Dobbs, and Behaviorally this is manifested in a set of
Swanson (1988) reported that poverty has acts (such as voting against minority candi-
more detrimental effects for black boys that dates or opposing affirmative action) that are
it does for black girls. These findings may rationalized on a nonracial basis but that op-
help explain why black boys and girls differ erate to maintain the racial status quo. Thus,
academically. The social valuation of gen- while in the past racism was rooted in legal
der, therefore, also evolves with changes in barriers that prevented minority children
economic and historical circumstances and from having access to the same educational
can lead to differential developmental out- institutions as white children, today minor-
comes within particular groups. ity children are faced with more subtle forms
of educational racism such as "low teacher
Social Stratijication Mechanisms expectations and attitudes, clinical defini-
Classification in terms of social position tions of black academic problems, testing
does not itself explain how these variables and tracking, biased curriculum and text-
eventually influence developmental out- books, and socializing into lower expecta-
come. Four macrosystem-level mechanisms tions and inferiorjobs" (Ogbu, 1991a, p. 269).
mediate between an individual's social posi-
tion and various other contexts that may Prejudice.-Prejudice refers to the pre-
more directly affect developmental out- conceived judgment or opinion made about
come: (a) racism, (b)prejudice, (c) discrimi- a person or a group based on social position
nation, and (d) oppression. variables, and it is usually accompanied by
an unreasonable predilection or objection.
Racism.-Racism refers to the pervasive Prejudice is operationalized by the auto-
and systematic assumptions of the inherent matic attribution of certain (often pejorative)
superiority of certain races, and consequent characteristics to a person regardless of
discrimination against other races. Racism whether that person indeed has these partic-
occurs when the ideology and concepts of ular characteristics (Feagin, 1992). Even
social stratification are actualized and per- when evidence is presented that a specific
sons are deemed to be more or less inferior attribute may not be applicable, a prejudiced
solely on the basis of their membership in a person does not incorporate this new infor-
racial group (Essed, 1991; Montague, 1942). mation into the perception of the other indi-
In Western societies, the preferred group is vidual or into their conceptual framework
Caucasoid. Persons who are descendants of (Duckitt, 1992). Under such informational
all other racial groups are placed into an in- conditions, the person is seen as an excep-
ferior class status. Whereas an individual's tion to the ongoing prejudicial rule of status.
position can be modified or buffered through Prejudice is likely based on a complex inter-
the acquisition of wealth or higher social sta- action of the attributes of race, social class,
tus, it can never be completely changed. ethnicity, and gender.
Racism is considered to be the primary
mechanism of influence within this categori- Children, even at the preschool level,
zation system, as it refers to different social make judgments about people based on eth-
attitudes and treatment based solely on race. nic, racial, and social categories and also
In contrast, prejudice, discrimination, and identify themselves as members of particu-
oppression may be experienced not only as lar groups, compete for resources, and segre-
a function of race, but also on the basis of gate themselves based on social and physi-
social class, ethnicity, and gender. cal characteristics (see Garcia Coll &
VBzquez Garcia, 1995a).While these are nor-
Racism also can vary in its expression mative processes, it is not necessary that
from institutionalized racism to symbolic they result in prejudicial attitudes against
racism (McConahay & Hough, 1976). Re- children of color. Garcia Coll and Vfizquez
search on symbolic racism demonstrates that Garcia (1995a) argue that certain environ-
racism in the United States has not actually mental conditions and socialization patterns,
declined but is now expressed in a more such as de-emphasizing in-grouplout-group
subtle and complex manner (Duckitt, 1992; distinctions, providing positive models, and
Frey & Gaertner, 1986). While majority indi- reducing social distance, can contribute to
viduals may increasingly support the princi- reducing the development of prejudicial at-
ple of racial or ethnic equality, they still titudes in children.
maintain a set of moral abstractions and atti-
tudinal predispositions concerning how mi- Discrimination.-Discrimination has
norities ought to act, whether or not they are been defined as "any behavior which denies
1900 Child Development
individuals or groups of people equality of other salient mediator between social posi-
treatment which they may wish" (Stroebe & tion variables and developmental outcome
Insko, 1989, elaborated from Allport, 1954, is segregation. Segregation refers to the sys-
p. 50). As a manifestation of prejudice, indi- tematic separation of groups and individuals
viduals are discriminated against on the ba- based on attributions made in regard to their
sis of preconceived notions or stereotypes. social position (Taeuber & Taeuber, 1965).
The systematic inclusion or exclusion of Separation and segregation are ongoing facts
members of a specific group can also stem of life in the United States. No longer by law
from social distance or social desirability. or d e jure, but d e facto, segregation contin-
Discrimination may be subtle as well as ues to be pervasive in today's society (Fea-
overt. Discrimination not only limits the gin, 1992). The proposed model denotes
quantity of resources available to a particular three types of segregation that can affect the
group but also places limits on their access family's and the child's ongoing transactions
to those resources. with the environment: (a) residential, (b)
economic, (c) social and psychological.
One of the most detrimental forms of
discrimination today is employment dis- Residential.-The most pervasive form
crimination. Although this form of discrimi- of segregation in the United States is resi-
nation was made unlawful by the Civil dential. Where a family lives determines
Rights Act of 1964, there are still occupa- many of the elements of the environment
tional fields in which women and minorities that will either promote or inhibit the child's
are paid less than their white male counter- development (Blau, 1981). Barriers in the
parts (Haberfeld & Shenav, 1990). In addi- housing market, which create residential
tion, several studies have shown that dis- segregation, have been perceived to have a
crimination persists in hiring practices, and direct and constraining effect on the re-
therefore minorities have a harder time than sources available to a population within a
whites securing employment (Coontz, 1992; segregated area (Massey & Bitterman, 1985;
Shulman, 1990; Turner, Fix, & Struyk, Santiago, 1992). Despite the mitigation of
1991). Bowman's (1990) national survey of legal barriers, members of the same ethnic
young black adults portrays a group of indi- and racial groups still tend to cluster in spe-
viduals whose hope and expectations have cific residential areas because of social posi-
been eroded by their own experiences in tion variables and stratification mechanisms
and observations of the job market. As Bow- (e.g., ethnic and racial discrimination that
man explained, "chronic job search strain does not allow families to freely live in any
and related psychosocial problems among area that they can afford) (Massey & Bit-
black youth do not occur at one point in terman, 1985). The higher concentration of
time; they evolve from past student-role ex- poor blacks than poor whites in impover-
pectations and adaptations to pressing labor ished neighborhoods suggests that black
market barriers" (Bowman, 1990, p. 99). children are more likely to live in dangerous
neighborhoods and thus more likely to
Oppression.-Oppression is the system- be exposed to violence, inadequate hous-
atic use of power or authority to treat others ing, and other environmental health risks
unjustly. Oppression is an omnipresent at- (Krieger et al., 1993).
mosphere that exists when a group of per-
sons find that they are in a devalued posi- Economic.-Socioeconomic status of
tion, a position from which they can not the family is another major determinant of
escape (Turner & Singleton, 1978). There is segregation. The persistence of employment
a pervasive element of oppression that discrimination in both salary levels and hir-
comes from the acting out of a racial or eth- ing practices has contributed to economic
nic classification. Research has shown that segregation of minority populations. Eco-
the psychoIogical internalization of the de- nomic segregation is critical to children's de-
valued status and feelings of oppression can velopment because families with higher
tend to limit the mobility of families and in- economic status have greater access to the
dividuals and lead to the adoption of deni- resources that enhance the development of
grating views and judgments both about their children's competencies than families
themselves and others in their racial and of lower economic status. However, even an
ethnic group (Essed, 1991; Feagin, 1989). upwardly mobile minority family with an in-
creased income will not have accumulated
Segregation available resources from previous genera-
Aside from the ideological social strati- tions that often are necessary for a family to
fication mechanisms described above, an- meet emergency needs (Landry, 1980). In
Garcia Coll et al. 1901
addition, families are also forced into form- tween old-fashioned racism and modern or
ing protective groups because of differential symbolic racism captures these different
education and occupational opportunities types of segregation (McConahay & Hough,
that are available under segregation (Mc- 1976). Although negative attitudes about the
Adoo, 1995; McAdoo & Villarruel, in prepa- desirability of segregation, miscegenation
ration). laws, or the innate intelligence of people of
color are less prevalent in modern society
Employment discrimination and subse- than they were earlier, negative attitudes
quent economic segregation are particularly and beliefs continue to affect the non-white
troublesome given the detrimental effects of population. For example, the notions that
poverty on children's development (see blacks are getting more money or attention
Huston, McLoyd, & Garcia Coll, 1994b).For than they deserve and that blacks do not
example, Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, and Kleba- have the right to push themselves into situa-
nov (1994) found that children, who lived in tions where they are not wanted are cur-
poverty, even only occasionally, demon- rently prevalent (McConahay, Hardee, &
strated lower IQ scores and more internaliz- Batts, 1981). These attitudes can provide the
ing behavior than children who had never foundation for subtle forms of racism, preju-
experienced poverty. Furthermore, eco- dice, and discrimination that promote social
nomic hardship heightens a parent's psycho- and psychological segregation.
logical strain, which in turn creates a ten-
dency for parents to be erratic, punitive, and PromotinglInhibiting Environments
nonsupportive of their children (McLoyd, Children are exposed to similar settings
1990a). This type of parenting has been across cultures, ethnic groups, and socioeco-
found to be a critical contributor to the socio- nomic backgrounds (i.e., schools, neighbor-
emotional problems of children. hoods, and health care settings). Yet the
structure, function, and relative importance
Social and psychological.-Residential of these systems for the development of
and economic segregation provides the competenck vary according to cultural in-
background for social and psychological seg- fluences. values. and goals (Sameroff &
regation. Social and psychological segrega- Fiese, 1990). within these settings, mac-
tion occurs when families and children of rosystem variables such as poverty and seg-
color are not ~ermittedaccess to im~ortant regation become operationalized in these
social and emotional resources as a result of children's lives (Bronfenbrenner & Crouter,
social stratification mechanisms. Social and 1983). It is necessary both to specify how
emotional isolation among groups only these environmental factors affect children
serves to widen the gap between them. As of color and to consider the relevance of
the emotional intensity of discord between these "mainstream" environments to the re-
these groups escalates, their separation in- alities, experiences, and necessities of these
creases, fostering feelings of fear and dis- particular populations. In the case of minor-
trust (Harry, 1992). Social and psychological ity families and children, there is the need
disconnection is compounded by the cul- to evaluate how the environments of these
tural, racial, and ethnic characteristics of the children can either promote, inhibit, or si-
groups that initially separated them from multaneously inhibit and promote their de-
one another (Harry, 1992). For the most part, velopmental competencies.
minority families of diverse heritages find Inhibiting environments can result from
themselves living in different worlds from a limitation in resources (e.g., inadequate
each other, as well as from mainstream soci- health care). These resource deficits create
ety. By remaining separate, groups are un- conditions that do not facilitate and might
able to find commonalities between their actually undermine the development of
cultures and their experiences. This situa- competencies in children. In addition, in-
tion enhances the power positions of those hibiting environments can result from an in-
who control environmental resources congruence between the expectations, goals,
(Harry, 1992). and values of the child and the family and
Social and psychological segregation those held in a particular environment. For
also can occur within environments that are example, familial and cultural values can
residentially and economically integrated. conflict with school ideologies, thereby in-
The experience of middle-class African- fluencing the child's school performance
Americans in predominantly white commu- (Harry, 1992).
nities clearly exemplifies these mechanisms Promoting environments, on the other
(Tatum, 1987). The distinction made be- hand, can result not only from an appropriate
1902 Child Development
number and quality of resources (e.g., neigh- other important variables such as the role of
borhoods that can adequately respond to so- administrators and school boards. the
cial, emotional, and economic needs) but school's policies on staffing and tracking,
also from the compatibility between the two and the extent to which the school promotes
sets of systemic variables. In other words, an parental involvement (Harry, 1992; Sleeter
environment that would be considered in- & Grant, 1987).
hibiting (e.g., such as school segregation)
can become a promoting environment for a There is some evidence that compatibil-
child when that setting is supportive of the ities between school and culture have fortu-
developmental outcome of children and pre- itous effects on student achievement and
pares them to deal with the societal de- school satisfaction (Tharp, 1989). Tharp's
mands imposed by prejudice or discrimina- (1989) review of studies revealed that the
tion. Rosenberg (1979) asserts that a child's sociocultural compatibility of classroom in-
self-esteem is affected by whether he or she struction and children's natal-culture pat-
operates in a dissonant or consonant envi- terns could lead to an improvement in learn-
ronment. In a consonant environment, ing. The studies surveyed included factors
which can be segregated but supportive, not such as social organization, participation
only is a minority child protected from the structures, speech patterns, and the contex-
prejudice of the majority world, but the envi- tualization of instruction. For example, a
ronment and norms are more comprehensi- study by Mohatt and Erickson (cited in
ble and congenial and the social reference Tharp, 1989) found that when Anglo teach-
group is appropriate. In a dissonant context, ers did not employ a Native American's
the minority child faces greater discrimina- rhythm and patterns of speech during their
tion, an unfamiliar environment and norms, instruction with Native American children,
and a less appropriate reference group, and "a more disorganized and less efficient pat-
consequently is more apt to suffer from a tern of interaction as well as a lower level
lower sense of self-esteem. or rapport between teacher and students re-
sulted" (Tharp, 1989, p. 352).
Among the multiple environments that Delgado-Gaitan's work (1994) also illus-
can be both promoting and inhibiting to chil- trates how the cultural gap between schools
dren of color, we will highlight three: the and homes can be bridged for more effective
school, the neighborhood, and the health schooling experiences. She examined how
care system. Mexican-American immigrant parents in
Carpenteria, California, changed their child-
Schools.-Children enter school with a rearing practices after participating in a
rich background that includes the child's Latino parent/community program to help
unique characteristics, family characteris- families address school-related issues. She
tics, and community characteristics. This found that the parent support group was ef-
background influences the child's ability to fective in two ways. First, the group enabled
learn and develop within the context of the Mexican-American parents to learn how to
school setting. School variables that can in- convey to the schools their interest in bilin-
fluence child behavior can be viewed as a gual programs and other curriculums. Sec-
set of nested environments: (a) the school ond, it helped them socialize their children
district or system (including organizational to meet the expectations of the school. Al-
and instructional philosophies, policies, and though both fifit-generation and immigrant
procedures); ( b ) the individual schools families verballv communicated to their
(which includes school personnel and re- children, who participated in the
sources); (c) and the individual classrooms program spoke to their children with the in-
(which includes child, teacher, and peer tent of encouraging specific verbal skills, ex-
characteristics and classroom structure, cur- pressive language, and critical thinking,
riculum, and instructional strategies) (Wasik, which would help them academically.
1992). Each of these nested environments
can be inhibiting, promoting, or both. Very Neighborhoods.-Neighborhoods also
little systematic research has been done to are a crucial component of children's devel-
address how these different school variables opment. Barbarin and Soler (1993) suggest
influence the social and academic compe- that the persistent prevalence of anxiety-
tence of children of color. Most literature based behaviors (problems with concentra-
discussing issues of multicultural education tion, restlessness, and noncompliance) in Af-
focuses on the individual classroom teachers rican-American adolescents is based in an
(Sleeter & Grant, 1987). However, an analy- anxiety response to environmental stresses,
sis of multicultural education should include particularly the concern for personal safety.
Garcia Coll et al. 1903
However, while the tendency for many the individual's health status, which is partly
is to label poor and ghetto neighborhoods as a reflection of adequate health care re-
inhibiting environments and categorize mid- sources. By this we mean a system with rea-
dle-class communities as promoting envi- sonable accessibility that can screen for dis-
ronments for the development of children of eases or conditions that would impede
color, these environments can be both inhib- normal development and that can respond
iting and promoting. Children develop sub- appropriately to injury, infection, or health
sistence tasks and acquire instrumental com- problems to avoid further complication or
petencies according to their surroundings disability.
'(Ogbu, 1981). Although a child who grows
up in a poor, all Puerto Rican neighborhood Research has shown that underutiliza-
might not have the availability or access to tion of health services among minority fami-
adequate resources (i.e., health care or good lies, especially preventive care, is due to so-
schools), the community can still encourage cioeconomic, linguistic, and cultural barriers
and provide the child with the instrumental (Anderson, Giachello, & Aday, 1986; Boyce
competencies needed to be successful out- et al., 1986; Chavez, Cornelius, & Jones,
side of that community. Conversely, while a 1986). Guendelman & Schwalbe (1986) com-
middle-class environment can provide a mi- pared rates of Hispanic, black, and non-
nority child with some sort of economic sta- Hispanic whites' contact with physicians
bility, better schools, and access to adequate during a 1 year period and found that the
health services, it might not buffer the ef- rates of health care utilization were signifi-
fects of prejudice, racism, and discrimina- cantly different. They found that Hispanic
tion to which the child might be exposed children were least likely to have had con-
within as well as outside of the community tact with a doctor, followed by black chil-
(Rodriguez, 1975; Tatum, 1987). Barnes dren and then non-Hispanic white children
(1991) argues that a sense of belonging in an (68.3%, 72.2%, and 78.5%, respectively).
ethnic or racial community can filter out the This occurred despite the fact that Hispanic
harmful effects of mainstream society, either families were more likely to report that their
by rejecting harmful messages or trans- children had only fair or poor health.
forming them so they are harmless. Initial In addition, the presence or absence of
data analysis of a study conducted by Barnes family stressors and social networks operates
in four schools in Dayton, Ohio, showed that to enhance or diminish the likelihood that a
parental involvement in the black commu- child can take advantage of available health
nity and black consciousness were associ- resources. Traditional sociocultural prac-
ated positively with a more positive self- tices can operate as buffering mechanisms
concept in their children. Neighborhoods against negative health conditions (Dowling
need to be examined not only on the basis of & Fisher, 1987; Swenson, Erickson, Eh-
the external resources available (i.e., better linger, Swaney, & Carlson, 1986). A child's
housing, better schools, good health care), environment can also operate to inhibit or
but also on the internal resources of the com- promote better health status. Health risk
munity that may support or interfere with a conditions (i.e., an area of high crime or vio-
child's social, academic, and psychological lence, unsafe playgrounds, older housing
competencies. with lead-based paints or hazardous waste)
The ability of predominantly African- can expose children of color to environmen-
American neighborhoods to provide eco- tal influences that can have negative conse-
nomic and social support for residents is a quences for developmental outcomes
strong theme in Tatum's (1987) qualitative (Krieger et al., 1993).
study of African-American families living in The most frequently noted health risk
a predominantly white community. Eight of for children is lead poisoning. Between 1988
the 10 sets of parents interviewed lamented and 1991, one in four black children younger
that their children were not a part of a black than age 6 were affected by elevated levels
peer group, and four of the families actively of lead in their bloodstream, which can re-
sought to create a black peer group for their sult from inhaling lead dust or from paint
children. Tatum's work suggests that black chips (Sherman, 1994). Children in poverty
families see a link between having black are at particular risk for lead exposure. High
friends and the establishment of a positive levels of lead in childhood have caused
racial identity, an important source of pro- hearing loss, stunted growth, damage to
tection from white racism. blood production and kidney development,
Health care environments.-The devel- and poor vitamin D metabolism (Sherman,
opment of competencies also is affected by 1994). Health care environments can be im-
1904 Child Development
portant influences in the development of nial Spanish, and African influences, as well
competencies in children of color, affecting as influences from the United States). Boy-
use of health care services, adoption of kin (1983) has specified nine interrelated but
health-promoting life-styles, exposure to en- distinct dimensions of black culture that are
vironmental risk conditions, and ultimately incorporated into the socialization processes
health status. of African-American families, including an
emphasis toward the affective-feeling do-
Adaptive Culture
Social stratification deriving from preju- main and an enhanced responsiveness to
dice, discrimination, racism, or segregation variability. These dimensions, according to
and the differential access to critical re- Boykin, are an integral part of black culture
sources such as good schools, employment, and permeate the child-rearing processes of
African-American families.
and health care influence families and chil-
dren of color to develop goals, values, atti- Economic and political history.-Ad-
tudes, and behaviors that set them apart from aptation is also rooted in the economic
the dominant culture. This adaptive culture and political events that have shaped
is the product of the group's collective his- the group's collective history. For African-
tory (cultural, political, and economic) and Americans, the system of enslavement and
current contextual demands posed by the its subsequent economic and political rami-
promoting and inhibiting environments. fications have had tremendous conse-
Work by Ogbu (1981, 1985) and Boykin quences for adaptation (Herskovits, 1930;
McAdoo, 1993). For Puerto Ricans, the his-
(Boykin, 1983; Boykin & Toms, 1985),
among others, contributes to the conceptual- tory of colonization from Spain and the
United States has been postulated as a major
ization of adaptive culture. Ogbu (1981) de-
contributor to psychosocial development
scribes a series of subsistence tasks and sur-
(e.g., Garcia Coll & Vhzquez Garcia, 199513).
vival strategies that are developed in
response to the environmental demands of Migration and acculturation pat-
the inner city, such as scarcity of jobs, dead- terns.-A third influence is the migration
end peripheral and unstable jobs, and low and acculturation patterns of a group. These
wages and little social credit as measured by patterns include the initial reasons for mi-
the values of the larger society. Boykin and gration (i.e., slavery and seasonal employ-
Toms (1985)postulate that African-American ment), length of the settlement of the partic-
families must negotiate three different ular community, and the ease of back and
realms of experience: the mainstream, the forth, or circular, migration. Patterns of ac-
minority, and the black cultural experience. culturation are also important, including the
These different survival strategies and ease of acculturation and the community's
realms of experience are integrated into the ideal of biculturalism versus assimilation to
development of an adaptive African- the mainstream culture. Minority parents re-
American culture. siding in the United States must decide what
aspects of ethnic parenting they wish to re-
These responses largely reflect cultur-
tain and those they wish to relinquish in fa-
ally defined coping mechanisms to the de- vor of the dominant culture's parental val-
mands placed by the promoting and inhib- ues, attitudes, and practices. Subsequently,
iting environments. A good example is the parental acculturation levels might have an
development of kinships or extended social impact on parenting styles by influencing
networks to cope with the demands of child- developmental expectations, mother infant
care and employment. At the collective interactions, feeding and caregiving prac-
level, three sources of historical processes tices, and the role of the extended family
are operating to influence the development (Garcia Coll, Meyer, & Brillon, 1995). For
of these responses: traditions and other cul- example, Gutierrez and Sameroff (1990)
tural legacies, economic and political found that bicultural Mexican-American
events, and migration and acculturation pat- mothers' levels of acculturation were associ-
terns. ated with their perceptions of child develop-
Traditions and cultural legacies.-This ment. Mothers who were more acculturated
first source of adaptation refers to traditions had more complex conceptualizations of
and other cultural legacies that have been their child's development and therefore
part of the group's collective history for gen- could better understand their children's be-
erations (i.e., for Puerto Ricans, internalized havior than mothers who were less accultu-
and externalized expressions of a mixed cul- rated or monocultural. They suggest that a
tural heritage that incorporates Taino, colo- bicultural living circumstance may enable a
Garcia Coll et al. 1905
mother to more easily understand and differ- While the range of potential characteristics
entiate multiple influences on a child's de- to consider is wide and shares some similari-
velopment and thus enhance her parenting ties with those of majority culture children,
skills. Similarly, Rueschenberg and Buriel a number of others might be considered par-
(1989) found that Mexican-American fami- ticularly relevant for children of color.
lies became more acculturated they became
increasingly involved with formal social Age and temperamental characteristics
support services outside the family. are obvious considerations. Few studies ex-
plicitly assess temperamental characteristics
These collective forces of migration and of children of color; however, previous find-
acculturation provide the background from ings suggest that African-American and
which adaptive responses are generated at Puerto Rican children differ from Caucasian
both the family and the community level. infants on Brazelton indices assessed neona-
Cohort effects need to be considered when tally (Garcia Coll, Sepkoski, & Lester, 1981).
assessing the impact of migration and accul- Temperament also has been incorporated
turation, as each are ongoing processes and into previous models of the connections be-
can be experienced as much by recent immi- tween poverty and African-American chil-
grants as by those living in the United States dren's socioemotional functioning (McLoyd,
who move from an ethnic neighborhood to 1990a). Although important, such consider-
one that is predominantly white (Garcia ations are less unique to children of color
Coll, Meyer, & Brillon, 1995). In addition, than other potential factors such as health,
the distinction made by Ogbu (1991b) be- maturational timing, and racial features.
tween voluntary and involuntary minorities
might be important in that involuntary mi- There are clear indications that the
norities might be less likely to embrace the health status of children of color is less satis-
dominant culture than voluntary minorities. factory than is the health status of majority
Finally, the consequences of these historical culture children (see Garcia Coll, 1990).
processes should be recognized as relevant Rates of prematurity in both African-
for both dominant and minority groups. American and Puerto Rican populations are
higher than in Caucasian populations,
Current contextual demands.-More thereby creating greater developmental risk
contemporary and immediate sources of in- (see Vohr & Garcia Coll, 1988). Further, Af-
fluence also contribute to the development rican-American infants have been found to
of an adaptive culture. Rates of unemploy- have a greater incidence of iron deficiency,
ment, neighborhood safety, and other as- which has been associated with decreased
pects of the promotinglinhibiting environ- attention, greater fatigue, and impaired per-
ments are current influences on children and formance on measures of cognitive ability
families (Krieger et al., 1993). Also operating (Carter, 1983; Webb & Oski, 1974).
at this level are racism, prejudice, and other
ideological stratification mechanisms that Biological factors may have specific sig-
are transmitted through systems of formal nificance for children of color. Garcia Coll
education, the media, and interpersonal in- (1990) reviewed a number of studies that
teractions. demonstrate differences in neonatal behav-
Thus, the adaptive culture is the prod- ior in various populations of infants of color.
uct of the group's prior collective history Lester and Brazelton (1982) suggested that
(cultural, political, and economic) and the neonatal behavior may be a phenotype that
expresses the complex relations among ge-
contextual demands placed by the promot- netic endowment, intrauterine environment,
ing and inhibiting environments. and mothers' obstetrical history, and these
Child Characteristics characteristics can elicit specific adaptive
The developmental principle that chil- behaviors from the caregiving environment
dren are not passive recipients of environ- and contribute to the practices and expecta-
mental influences is no less the case for chil- tions of the culture. In a different develop-
dren of color, such as African-Americans and mental period, biology, as manifested in
Puerto Ricans, than for any other groups. In maturational timing during adolescence,
the proposed model, child characteristics may be important. Spencer, Dobbs, and
-~ l a. va kev role, both in how thev are influ- Swanson (1988) note that African-American
enced byVprombtingand inhibiting environ- adolescent boys tend to be taller and heavier
ments and a d a ~ t i v cultures
e and in how thev that their Caucasian counterparts and may
influence family functioning and the erne;- mature earlier as well. These authors sug-
gence of developmental competencies. gest that early maturation and increased size
1906 Child Development
are risk factors for social and academic com- African-American, Puerto Rican, and other
petence. They posit that larger, early matur- minority populations have a high percentage
ing African-American boys may b e per- of single heads of households (Center for the
ceived as a threat in their environment and Study of Social Policy, 1986; Vizquez Nut-
therefore receive less positive support from tall, 1979), but the reliance on extended kin
the contexts in which they interact, which in by some of these family systems might pro-
turn affects the development of compe- vide enough resources to meet the child's
tencies. needs.
Physical characteristics, such as racial Reliance on the social support of ex-
features and skin color, also may affect mi- tended kin is maintained as part of the adap-
nority children's development, although the tive culture because of cultural patterns that
impact of such characteristics has not been have been brought from other lands (Har-
fully studied. Spencer and Markstrom- rison et al., 1990). These patterns have been
Adams (1990) have suggested that skin color maintained because of the economic and
may affect development of identity and so- discriminatory environments of the societies
cial relationships in African-American chil- in which these groups live (McAdoo, 1982).
dren, although they note that this possibility These families are not able to rely on main-
is seldom addressed either empirically or stream institutions to provide assistance and
conceptually. Lightness of skin color is per- help to meet the differential developmental
ceived as more desirable, even within needs of family members.
groups of African-American and Puerto Ri- The roles that individuals play in the
can children, and is associated with greater family are based on culturally defined gen-
opportunity (Russell, Wilson, & Hall, 1992). der, parental, and age-appropriate expecta-
Gender has been discussed in previous tions as well as the family structure (i.e., the
sections of this model, but also has relevance persons living in the home). In families of
as an individual characteristic. Spencer, color, there can be flexibility of roles as an
Dobbs, and Swanson (1988) showed that adaptive response to cultural and societal
poverty appears to have more detrimental ef- pressures, as well as adherence to traditional
fects on black boys than black girls, although culturally defined roles (McAdoo, 1993). In-
the extent to which this finding is culturally teractions between family members, there-
unique is unclear. Nevertheless, docu- fore, will be reflective of the adaptive cul-
mented gender differences in various com- ture and the stressors and pressures that the
petencies (Patterson, Kupersmidt, & Vaden, family must cope with.
1990) suggest that the impact of gender must
be taken into account in multivariate models Family values, beliefs, and goals.-Fam-
ily values, beliefs, and goals embody the ele-
of developmental competence.
ments that are held dear and important to
Familu family members. A growing literature docu-
norit it^ families tend to have certain ments the differences between ethnic
characteristics that differentiate them from and minority groups in child-rearing val-
mainstream families and that affect family ues, beliefs, and goals (McAdoo, 1993).
processes in very profound ways. Among These beliefs are rooted in cultural and reli-
these characteristics we will highlight the gious traditions that can be traced to the
following: the structure and roles of the fam- countries of origin (Boykin & Toms, 1985;
ily; family beliefs, values, and goals; racial Fitzpatrick, 1988; Garcia Coll, 1990; Garcia
socialization; and socioeconomic status and Coll, Meyer, & Brillon, 1995; Harrison et al.,
resources. 1990; Ogbu, 1981) and the adaptive culture,
as well as the unique experiences of parents.
Structure and roles of the family.-The
These factors will not only determine the
presence in the family of at least one or more basis of the behavior displayed during fam-
persons who unconditionally love a child
ily interactions, but will also influence fam-
(Bronfenbrenner, 1979) is considered a min- ily structure and roles and therefore interac-
imum requirement for the development of
tions with other members of the extended
competencies in children. In families of family.
color, there is a tendency for a more integral
use of persons other than the birth parents Racial socialization.-Positioning fami-
to perform some of the tasks of parenting, lies and children of color within their envi-
through the support of extended family ronment requires incorporating the ways in
members, familism, and fictive kin (friends which minority families cope with racism
who become as close as kin) (Garcia Coll, and discrimination in the confines of family
1990; Harrison et al., 1990; McAdoo, 1982). life (Cross, 1992; McAdoo, 1993). Families
Garcia Coll et al. 1907
of color face chronic stressors when they at- child at any one point in time and the devel-
tempt to socialize their children and simulta- opinglemerging skills that children bring to
neously protect them from the negative ef- the multiple ecologies in which they exist.
fects of racism, segregation, and the Nevertheless, it is critical to determine and
resulting inhibiting environments (Peters, appropriately measure the emergence and
1985). salience of specific skills that define the
functional or adaptive competence of chil-
Racial socialization has been postulated dren of color in specific developmental pe-
as an important aspect of family processes riods. According to organizational models,
within minority populations from diverse so- development in children of color cannot be
cioeconomic backgrounds (Branch & New- judged solely in relation to a specific "stan-
combe, 1986; Garcia Coll, Meyer, & Brillon, dard ~ O I T I " applied to all children, but must
1995; Hale-Benson, 1982; Renne, 1970; Ta- be considered within the context of specific
tum, 1987; Thornton, Chatters, Taylor, & Al- ecological circumstances. Further, within-
len, 1990). Children of color must learn to group variability in ecological niches and
function in both white and black realms developmental outcome in children of color
(Boykin & Toms, 1985; Cross, 1991; Thorn- is as great as is the between-group variabil-
ton et al., 1990). Parents teach their children ity that necessitates the development of this
how to cope with the demands of a society model.
that devalues their heritage, race, and cul-
ture (McAdoo, 1992).While children of color The evaluation of meaningful develop-
may demonstrate preferences for being mental competencies will continue to in-
members of the dominant culture, parents volve important traditional skill areas such
have the task of ensuring that their children as cognitive, social, emotional, and linguistic
maintain a positive view of their ethnic and skills. However, functional outcome mea-
racial group. In fact, Brigham (1974) found sures must also recognize manifestations of
that African-American children were more these skills that reflect competent adaptation
likely than white children to attribute posi- to circumstances created by social stratifica-
tive traits to their own group. The impor- tion, the effects of racism and its concomi-
tance of racial socialization is emphasized by tant processes, and the influence of segrega-
findings that relate these practices to the tion on the nature of the environments faced
child's motivation, achievement, prospects by children of color. For example, assertive
for upward mobility, and racial attitudes behavior with members of the peer group
(Bowman & Howard, 1985; Branch & New- may be more appropriate in some contexts
combe, 1980, 1986). for African-American children (Heath, 1989;
Rotheram-Borus & Phinney, 1990), and the
Socioeconomic statuslresources.-The lack of eye contact with authority figures is
socioeconomic resources available to fami- an appropriate social response by Puerto Ri-
lies of color and how they are used also are can children (Fitzpatrick & Travieso, 1980).
important influences on the developmental
competencies of minority children (see Hus- Notions of competence also must be ex-
ton, McLoyd, & Garcia Coll, 199413). While panded to include a broader range of adap-
measures of socioeconomic status can be tive responses beyond the traditional areas
good indicators of social class variables, they of concern and to incorporate additional and
fail to pick up within-group nuances. For ex- alternative abilities, such as the child's abil-
ample, a first-generation middle-class family ity to function in two or more different cul-
of color may react differently to a financial tures, to cope with racism, subtle and overt
crisis than a fourth-generation middle-class discrimination, and social and psychological
family, with greater uncertainty about socio- segregation. Both culture-specific and bicul-
economic status. Given the fact that most mi- tural competencies are needed to promote
nority families experience higher, more ex- these children's development. Children
treme, and more long-lasting poverty (U.S. must learn the codes that are appropriate to
Bureau of the Census, 1991), the conse- both cultures if they are to master the activi-
quences of differences in socioeconomic sta- ties that are called upon in each (LaFrom-
tus on the development of competencies in boise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993). Szapocz-
children of color need further attention nik and colleagues (Szapocznik, Kurtines, &
(Huston, McLoyd, & Garcia Coll, 1994a). Fernandez, 1980; Szapocznik, Santisteban,
Kurtines, Perez-Vidal, & Hervis, 1984)
Developmental Competencies found that Hispanic children with bicultural
Developmental competencies represent skills were less likely to experience school
the "outcome" portion of the model and re- and family conflicts or to become involved
flect both the functional competencies of a in illegal drug use. Children of color also
1908 Child Development
must effectively cope with racism and its de- example, the present analysis implies that
rivatives and maintain a strong sense of self bilingual education should foster the devel-
despite multiple threats. Parents who are opment of balanced bilingualism so that the
successful in preparing their children for children develop mastery in two languages
these tasks thus can be considered to be ef- and the family is not negatively affected by
fective teachers of a culture who foster the the creation of a linguistic gap between fam-
adaptive racial socialization of their children ily members. Alternatively, bilingual educa-
(see Garcia Coll, Meyer, Brillon, 1995). tion for children can be paired with English
Competency for children of color in- as a Second Language instruction for family
volves a wide range and multiple levels of members so that, as the child's communica-
abilities that are intertwined and cannot be tive skills evolve toward greater mastery in
defined by any one single index, indicator, a second language, so do other family
or measure. The adaptive competency of members'.
children of color at any one point in time is, The third challenge is to recognize that
in turn, an important influence on the subse- we cannot continue to waste human talent
quent ecological processes that will con- because of outdated racialtethnic conceptu-
tinue to affect the developmental course of alizations. The competence and productivity
these children. The process is recursive and of minority populations are crucial to our
operates at multiple levels, but is most rep- collective well-being (McLoyd, 1990a). In-
resented in reciprocal influences on the deed, people of color from various national
child's psychological and social segregation, origins will be close to a majority in the
promotingtinhibiting environments, and United States (Exter, 1992) early in the next
family processes. century. The proposed model and the conse-
quent research and social policies can con-
Conclusions tribute to a necessary change in societal
views of children's development and the
Both historically and in the present, characteristics of racial and ethnic groups.
mainstream developmental sciences have
not emphasized the unique normative pro- References
cesses among minority children and their
families or the development of competen- Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice.
cies within these populations. This omission Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
reflects, in part, the inability of existing theo- Anastasi, A., & Cordova, F. (1953). Some effects
retical models to address critical aspects of of bilingualism upon the intelligence test per-
the environment that have profound influ- formance of Puerto Rican children in New
ences in the developmental processes in York City. Journal of Education Psychology,
these populations. By placing these influ- 44(1), 1-19.
ences (social position, racism and its deriva- Anastasi, A., & de Jesbs, C. (1953). Language de-
tives, and segregation) at the core rather than velopment and nonverbal IQ of Puerto Rican
at the periphery of a causal framework and preschool children in New York City. lournal
by specifying how they influence more im- of Abnormal and Social ~ s ~ c h o l o g48(3),
i,
mediate settings, the proposed model is a 357-366.
Anderson, R. M., Giachello, A. L., & Aday, L. A.
heuristic guide to research in the develop-
(1986).Access of Hispanics to health care and
ment of competencies in children of color.
cuts in services: A state-of-the-art overview.
Patterns of family interaction can then be
Public Health Reports, 101, 238-252.
conceptualized as a reflection of an adaptive
Arcia, E., Keyes, L., & Gallagher, J. J. (1994).Indi-
culture-a mix of history, traditions, and
cators of developmental and functional status
adaptive responses to present contextual de-
of Mexican-American and Puerto Rican chil-
mands-and not solely as individual pat-
dren. Deuelopmental and Behavioral Pediat-
terns of interactions.
rics, 15(1), 27-33.
Several challenges are posed by this Armstrong, C. P., Achilles, E. M., & Sacks, M. J.
model. The first is to identify the alternative (1935). Reactions of Puerto Rican children in
competencies in children of color that are New York City to psychological tests. Report
not measured by traditional assessment of the Special Commission on Immigration
tools, not only in the realms of established and Naturalization, New York State Chamber
developmental competencies but in areas of of Commerce.
bicultural adaptation and coping with rac- Attewell, P., & Fitzgerald, R. (1980). Comparing
ism. The second is to analyze the implica- stratification theories. American Sociological
tions for social policy and interventions. For Reuiew, 45,325-328.
1910 Child Development
can Journal of Epidemiology, 139(2), avoidance of inappropriate interracial behav-
184-192. ior: A strategy that perpetuates a nonpreju-
Coontz, S. (1992). The way we never were. New diced self-image. Journal of Personality and
York: Basic. Social Psychology, 50, 1083-1090.
Cross, W. (1991). Shades of black: Diversity in Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius: An inquiry
African-American identity. Philadelphia: into its laws and consequences. London:
Temple University Press. Macmillan.
Cross, W. (1992). Shades of blackness. Philadel- Garcia Coll, C. (1990). Developmental outcome of
phia: Temple University Press. minority infants: A process-oriented look into
Darwin, C. R. (1859). O n the origin of species by our beginnings. Child Development, 61,
natural selection. London: J. Murray. 270-289.
de Gobineau, A. (1915). The inequality of human Garcia Coll, C. T., Meyer, E. C., & Brillon, L.
races. New York: Putman. (1995). Ethnic and minority parenting. In M.
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1994). Socializing young chil- Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol.
dren in Mexican-American families: An inter- 2. Biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 189-
generational perspective. In P. M. Greenfield 209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
& R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross cultural roots Garcia Coll, C. T., Sepkoski, C., & Lester, B. M.
of minority child development (pp. 55-86). (1981). Cultural and biomedical correlates of
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. neonatal behavior. Developmental Psychobi-
Dowling, P. T., & Fisher, M. (1987). Maternal fac- ology, 14, 147-154.
tors and low birth weight infants: A compari- Garcia Coll, C. T., & VAzquez Garcia, H. A.
son of Blacks with Mexican-Americans.Jour- (1995a). Developmental processes and their
nal of Family Practice, 25, 153-158. influence on interethnic and interracial rela-
Duckitt, J. (1992). Psychology and prejudice. tions. In W. D. Hawley & A. W. Jackson
American Psychologist, 47(10), 1182-1193. (Eds.), Toward a common destiny (pp. 103-
Duncan, J. D., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, 130). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
P. D. (1994). Economic deprivation and early Garcia Coll, C. T., & Vkzquez Garcia, H. A.
childhood development. Child Development, (1995b). Hispanic children and their families:
65,296-318. On a different track from the very beginning.
Dunn, L. M. (1987). Bilingual Hispanic children In H. E. Fitzgerald, B. M. Lester, & B. Zuck-
on the U.S. mainland: A review of research erman (Eds.), Children of poverty: Research,
on their cognitive, linguistic and scholastic health care, and policy issues (pp. 57-83).
development. Circle Pines, MN: American New York: Garland.
Guidance Service. Gibson, M. A. (1976). Approaches to multicultural
Enhvisle, D. R., & Astone, N. M. (1994). Some education in the United States: Some con-
practical guidelines for measuring youth's cepts and assumptions. Anthropology and
racelethnicity and socioeconomic status. Education Quarterly, 7, 7-18.
Child Development, 65, 1521-1540. Glazer, N., & Moynihan, D. P. (1963).Beyond the
Erickson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. melting pot. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press and
New York: Norton. Harvard University Press.
Essed, P. (1991). Understanding everyday racism. Goodenough, F. L., &Anderson, J. E. (1931). Ex-
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. perimental child study. New York: Century.
Exter, T. G. (1992). Demographic forecasts: Gordon, M. M. (1963). Social class i n American
Blacks to 2010. American Demographics, sociology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
14(12), 63. Guendelman, S., & Schwalbe, J. (1986). Medical
Feagin, J. R. (1989). Racial and ethnic relations utilization by Hispanic children. Medical
(3d ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Care, 26(10), 925-940.
Feagin, J. R. (1992). Racial and ethnic relations. Gutierrez, J., & Sarneroff, A. (1990). Determinants
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. of complexity in Mexican-American mothers'
Fitzpatrick, J. P. (1988). The Puerto Rican family. conceptions of child development. Child De-
In C. H. Mindel & R. W. Habenstein (Eds.), velopment, 61,384-394.
Ethnic families in America: Patterns and Haberfeld, Y., & Shenav, Y. (1990). Are women
variations (pp. 192-217). New York: and blacks closing the gap? Salary discrimina-
Elsevier. tion in American science during the 1970s
Fitzpatrick, J. P., & Travieso, L. (1980). The and 1980s. industrial and Labor Relations
Puerto Rican family: Its role in cultural transi- Review, 44(1), 68-82.
tion. In M. D. Fantini & R. Ckrdenas (Eds.), Hale-Benson, J. E. (1982). Black children: Their
Parenting i n a multicultural society (pp. 103- roots, culture and learning styles. Baltimore:
113). New York: Longrnan. Johns Hopkins Press.
Frey, D., & Gaertner, S. (1986). Helping and the Harrison, A. O., Serafica, F., & McAdoo, H. (1984).
Garcia Coll et al. 1911
Ethnic families of color. In R. D. Parke (Ed.), H. Stevenson & D. Wagner (Eds.), Cultural
The family: Review of child development re- perspectives on child development (pp. 20-
search (Vol. 7 , pp. 329-371). Chicago: Uni- 53). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
versity of Chicago Press. Lewis, 0. (1965). La uida. New York: Vintage.
Harrison, A. O., Wilson, M. N., Pine, C. J., Chan, Locke, J. (1913). Some thoughts concerning edu-
S. Q., & Buriel, R. (1990). Family ecologies of cation: Sections 38 and 40. London: Cam-
ethnic minority children. Child Develop- bridge University Press. (Original work pub-
ment, 61,347-362. lished 1690).
Harry, B. (1992). Restructuring the participation Massey, D. S. (1993). Latinos, poverty, and the
of African-American parents in special educa- underclass: A new agenda for research. His-
tion. Exceptional Children, 59(2), 123-131. panic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 15(4),
Heath, S. B. (1989). Oral and literate traditions 449-475.
among black Americans living in poverty. Massey, D. S., & Bitterman, B. (1985). Explaining
American Psychologist, 44(2), 367-373. the paradox of Puerto Rican segregation. So-
Herrnstein, R. (1971, September). IQ. Atlantic cial Forces, 64(2), 306-331.
Monthly, 43-64. Massey, D. S., & Denton, N. A. (1988). Suburbani-
Herskovits, M. (1930). The Negro in the New zation and segregation in U.S. metropolitan
World. American Anthropologist, 32, 145- areas. American Journal of Sociology, 94,
155. 592-626.
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1995). The inheritability of Masson, C. N., & Verkuyten, M. (1993).Prejudice,
identity: Children's understanding of the cul- ethnic identity, contact and ethnic group pref-
tural biology of race. Child Development, 66, erences among Dutch young adolescents.
1418-1437. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23(2),
Huston, A. C., McLoyd, V. C., & Garcia Coll, C. 156-168.
(1994a). Children and poverty: Issues in con- McAdoo, H. P. (1982). Stress absorbing systems
temporary research. Child Development, 65, in black families. Family Relations, 31,
275-282. 379-488.
Huston, A. C., McLoyd, V. C., & Garcia Coll, C. McAdoo, H. (1992).Reaffirming African-American
(Guest Eds.). (199413). Children and poverty families and our identities. Psychology Dis-
[Special issue]. Child Development, 65(2). course, 23(3). (Excerpted from the Distin-
Jensen, A. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and guished Psychologist Address, Aug. 17, 1991,
school achievement? Harvard Educational New Orleans).
Review, 39, 1-123. McAdoo, H. (1993).The social cultural contexts of
Kohn, M. L. (1977). Class and conformity. Chi- ecological developmental family models. In
cago: University of Chicago Press. P. G. Boss, W. J. Doherty, R. LaRossa, W. R.
Krieger, N., Rowley, D. L., Herman, A. A., Avery, Shumm, & S. K. Steinmetz (Eds.), Sourcebook
B., & Phillips, M. T. (1993).Racial differences of family theories and methods: A contextual
in preterm delivery-Developing a new re- approach (pp. 298-301). New York: Plenum.
search paradigm. American Journal of Pre- McAdoo, H. P. (1995). African-American families:
ventive Medicine, 9(Suppl. 6), 82-122. Strength and realities. In H. I. McCubbin,
LaFromboise, T., Coleman, H. L. K., & Gerton, J. E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. A. Fu-
(1993). Psychological impact of biculturalism: bell (Eds.), Resiliency and ethnic minority
Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, families: African American families (Vol. 2,
114(3), 395-412. pp. 17-30). Madison: University of Wis-
Landry, L. B. (1980). The social and economic ad- consin.
equacy of the black middle class. In J. R. McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V.
Washington, Jr. (Ed.), Dilemmas of the new (1981). Has racism declined in America?Jour-
black middle class. University of Pennsylva- nal of ConfEict Resolution, 25(4), 563-579.
nia Afro-American Studies Symposium Pro- McConahay, J. B., & Hough, J. C. (1976). Sym-
ceedings. bolic racism. Journal of Social Issues, 32(2),
Laumann, E. 0.(Ed.).(1970). Social stratij?cation 23-45.
research and theory for the 1970's. New York: McKinney, M., Abrams, L. A,, Terry, P. A., & Ler-
Bobs-Merrill. ner, R. M. (1994). Child development re-
Lerner, R. M. (1989). Individual development and search and the poor children of America: A
the family system: A life-span perspective. In call for a developmental contextual approach
K. Kreppner & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Family to research and outreach. Family G Consumer
systems and life-span development (pp. 15- Sciencs Research Journal, 23,26-42.
31). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McLoyd, V. C. (1990a). The impact of economic
Lester, B. M., & Brazelton, T. B. (1982). Cross- hardship on Black families and children: Psy-
cultural assessment of neonatal behavior. In chological distress, parenting, and socioemo-
1912 Child Development
tional development. Child Development, 61, Patterson, C. J., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Vaden, N. A.
311-346. (1990). Income level, gender, ethnicity, and
McLoyd, V. C. (1990b). Minority children: Intro- household composition as predictors of chil-
duction to the special issue. Child Develop- dren's school-based competence. Child De-
ment, 61,263-266. velopment, 61,485-494.
McLoyd, V. C., & Randolph, S. (1984). The con- Peters, M. F. (1985). Racial socialization of young
duct and publication of research on Afro- Black children. In H. McAdoo & J. McAdoo
American children: A content analysis. Hu- (Eds.), Black children: Social, educational,
man Development, 27,65-75. and parental environments (pp. 159-173).
McLoyd, V. C., & Randolph, S. (1985). Secular Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
trends in the study of Afro-American chil- Puerto Rican Forum. (1964). A study of poverty
dren: A review of Child Development. In conditions i n the New York Puerto Rican
A. B. Smuts & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), History and community. New York: Puerto Rican Forum,
research in child development. Monographs Inc.
of the Society for Research i n Child Develop- Puerto Rican Study 1953-1957. (1958).New York:
ment, 50(4-5, Serial No. 211). New York City Board of Education.
Mendoza, F., Glenn, S., Takata, S., & Martorell, Ramirez, M., & Castaneda, A. (1974).Cultural de-
R. (1994). In G. Lamberty & C. Garcia Coll mocracy, bicognitive development, and edu-
(Eds.),Puerto Rican women and children: Is- cation. New York: Academic Press.
sues i n health, growth, and development. Reissman, F. (1962). The culturally deprived
New York: Plenum. child. New York: Harper & Row.
Miller, H. (1952). New York City's Puerto Rican Renne, K. (1970). Correlates of dissatisfaction in
pupils: A problem of acculturation. School marriage. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
and Society, 76(1967), 129-132. ily, 32, 54-67.
Montague, A. (1942). Man's most dangerous Rodriguez, C. (1975). A cost benefit analysis of
myth: The fallacy of race. New York: Colum- subjective factors affecting assimilation:
bia University Press. Puerto Ricans. Ethnicity, 2, 66-80.
Montague, A. (1972). Statement of race (3d ed.). Rodriguez, C. (1994). A summary of Puerto Rican
New York: Oxford University Press. migration to the United States. In G. Lam-
Morris, H. S. (1968). Ethnic groups. In D. Sill berty & C. Garcia Coll (Eds.), Puerto Rican
(Ed.), International encyclopedia of the so- women and children: Issues i n health,
cial sciences (Vol. 15, p. 167). New York: Con- growth, and development (pp. 11-28). New
well, Collier & MacMillian. York: Plenum.
Ogbu, J. U. (1981). Origins of human competence: Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. Mala-
A cultural-ecological perspective. Child De- bar, FL: Krieger.
velopment, 52,413-429. Rosenberg, M., & Pearlin, L. (1978). Social class
Ogbu, J. U. (1985). A cultural ecology of compe- and self esteem among children and adults.
tence among inner-city blacks. In M. B. Spen- American Journal of Sociology, 84(1), 53-77.
cer, G. K. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.),Be- Rotheram-Borus, M. J., & Phinney, J. S. (1990).
ginnings: The social and affective Patterns of social expectation among black
development of black children. Hillsdale, NJ: and Mexican-American children. Child De-
Erlbaum. velopment, 61,542-556.
Ogbu, J. U. (1987). Variability in minority school Rousseau, J. J. (1938). Emile: Or concerning edu-
performance: A problem in search of and ex- cation: Book 2. New York: Dutton, 1938.
planation. Anthropology and Education Cited in P. H. Mussen, J. J. Conger, & J. Ka-
Quarterly, 18, 312-334. gan (Eds.), Child development and personal-
Ogbu, J. U. (1991a). Low school performance as ity (2d ed., p. 9). New York: Harper & Row.
an adaptation: The case of blacks in Stockton, (Original work published 1762).
California. In M. A. Gibson & J. U. Ogbu Rueschenberg, E., & Buriel, R. (1989). Mexican
(Eds.), Minority schooling and status (pp. family functioning and acculturation: A fam-
249-285). New York: Garland. ily system perspective. Hispanic Journal of
Ogbu, J. U. (1991b). Minority coping responses Behavioral Sciences, 11(3),232-244.
and school experience. Journal of Psychohis- Russell, K., Wilson, M., & Hall, R. (1992). The
tory, 18(4), 433-456. color complex: The politics of skin color
Parten, M. B. (1932). Social participation among among African-Americans. New York: Har-
pre-school children.Journa1 of Abnormal and court Brace Jovanovich.
Social Psychology, 27,243-269. Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproduc-
Passow, H. A. (Ed.). (1963). Education i n de- tive risk and the continuum of caretaking ca-
pressed areas. New York: Teachers College, sualty. In F. D. Horowitz, E. M. Hethering-
Columbia University. ton, & S. Scarr-Salapatek (Eds.), Review
Garcia Coll et al. 1913
of child development research (Vol. 4, pp. white, and Hispanic children. Child Deuelop-
187-244). Chicago: University of Chicago ment, 61,508-523.
Press. Stroebe, W., & Insko, C. A. (1989). Stereotype,
Sameroff, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (1990). Transac- prejudice and discrimination: Changing con-
tional regulation and early intervention. In ceptions in theory and research. In D. Bar-
S, J. Meisels & J. P. Shonkoff (Eds.), Hand- Tal, C. F. Graumann, A. W. Kruglanski, & W.
book of early childhood intervention (pp. Stroebe (Eds.), Stereotyping and prejudice:
119-191). New York: Cambridge University Changing conceptions (pp. 3-34). New York:
Press. Springer-Verlag.
Santiago, A. (1992). Patterns of Puerto Rican seg- Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (1990). Counseling the cul-
regation and mobility. Hispanic Journal of turally different. New York: Wiley.
Behauioral Sciences, 14(1), 107-133. Swenson, I., Erickson, D., Ehlinger, E., Swaney,
Sears, R. R. (1975). Ancients revisited: A history S., & Carlson, G. (1986). Birthweight, apgar
of child development. Reuiew of Child Deuel- scores, labor and delivery complications and
opment Research (Vol. 5, pp. 1-73). Chicago: prenatal characteristics of Southeast Asian ad-
University of Chicago Press. olescents and older mothers. Adolescence, 21,
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). 711-722.
Patterns of child rearing. Evanston, IL: Row Szapocznik, J., Kurtines, W., & Fernandez, T.
& Peterson. (1980). Bicultural involvement and adjust-
Senn, M. (1975). Insights on the child develop- ment in Hispanic-American youths. Znterna-
ment movement. Monographs of the Society tional Journal of Intercultural Relations, 4,
for Research in Child Deuelopment, 40(3-4, 353-365.
Serial No. 161). Szapocznik, J., Santisteban, D., Kurtines, W.,
Sherman, A. (1994). Wasting America's future: Perez-Vidal, A., & Hervis, 0. (1984). Bicul-
The Children's Defense Fund report on the tural effectiveness training: A treatment inter-
costs of child poverty. Boston: Beacon. vention for enhancing intercultural adjust-
Shuey, A. (1966). The testing of Negro intelli- ment in Cuban American families. Hispanic
gence. New York: Social Science Press. Journal of Behauioral Sciences, 6, 317-344.
Shulman, S. (1990). The causes of black poverty: Taeuber, K. E., & Taeuber, A. F. (1965). Negroes
Evidence and interpretation. Journal of Eco- in cities: Residential segregation and neigh-
nomic Issues, 24(4), 995-1016. borhood change. Chicago: Aldine.
Simpson, G. E., & Yinger, M. J. (1953).Racial and Tatum, B. (1987). Assimilation blues. Westport,
cultural minorities. New York: Harper & CT: Greenwood.
Brothers. Terman, L. M. (1916). The measurement of intelli-
Slaughter-Defoe, D. T., Nakagawa, K., Takanishi, gence: An explanation of and a complete
R., & Johnson, D. J. (1990). Toward cultural1 guide for the use of the Stanford reuision and
ecological perspectives on schooling and extension of the- Binet-~ihonIntelligence
achievement in African- and Asian-American Scale. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
children. Child Deuelopment, 61,363-383. Tharp, R. G. (1989). Psychocultural variables and
Sleeter, C. E., & Grant, C. A. (1987).An analysis of constants: Effects on teaching and learning in
multicultural education in the United States. schools. American Psychologist, 44,349-359.
Harvard Educational Reuiew, 57,421-444. Thomas, D. S. (1929). Some new techniques for
Spencer, M. B. (1990). Development of minority studying social behavior. Child deuelopment
children: An introduction. Child Deuelop- monographs (No. 1). New York: Teachers
ment, 61,267-269. College, Columbia University.
Spencer, M. B., Dobbs, B., & Swanson, D. P. Thornton, M. C., Chatters, L. M., Taylor, R. J.,
(1988). Afro-American adolescents: Adapta- & Allen, W. R. (1990). Sociodemographic and
tional processes and socioeconomic diversity environmental correlates of racial socializa-
in behavioral outcomes. Journal of Adoles- tion by black parents. Child Deuelopment, 61,
cence, 11, 117-137. 401-409.
Spencer, M. B., & Markstrom-Adams, C. (1990). Tumin, M. M. (1967). Social stratijication. Engle-
Identity processes among racial and ethnic wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
minority children in America. Child Develop- Turner, J. H., & Singleton, R. (1978). A theory of
ment, 61,290-310. ethnic oppression: Toward a reintegration of
Sroufe, L. (1979). The coherence of individual de- cultural and structural concepts in ethnic rela-
velopment: Early care, attachment and subse- tions theory. Social Forces, 56(4), 1001-1018.
quent developmental issues. American Psy- Turner, M., Fix, M., & Struyk, R. J. (1991) Oppor-
chologist, 34(10), 834-841. tunities denied, opportunities diminished:
Stevenson, H. W., Chen, C., & Uttal, D. H. (1990). Discrimination in hiring. Washington, DC:
Beliefs and achievement: A study of black, Urban Institute Press.
1914 Child Development
U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1991). Poverty in the sium conducted at the School Readiness: Sci-
United States. Current Population Reports: entific Perspectives National Conference,
Consumer Income, Series P-60, No. 175. Columbia, MD.
Washington, DC: Government Printing Webb, T. E., & Oski, F. A. (1974). Behavioral sta-
Office. tus of young adolescents with iron deficiency
Vhzquez Nuttall, E. (1979). The support system anemia.Journa1 of Special Education, 8, 153.
and coping patterns of the female Puerto Ri- Werner, H. (1948). The comparative psychology
can single parent.Journa1 of Non-White Con- of mental development. New York: Harper &
cerns, 7(3), 128-137. Row.
Ventura, S. J. (1994). Demographic and health West, C. (1993). Race matters. Boston: Beacon.
characteristics of Puerto Rican mothers and Wiltfang, G. L., & Scarbecz, M. (1990).Social class
their babies. In G. Lamberty & C. Garcia Coll and adolescents' self-esteem: Another look.
(Eds.), Puerto Rican women and children: 1s- Social Psychology Quarterly, 53(2), 174-183.
sues in health, growth, and development (pp. Wise, P. H., Kotelchuck, M., Wilson, M. L., &
71-84). New York: Plenum. Mills, M. (1985, August). Racial and socioeco-
Vohr, B. R., & Garcia Coll, C. T. (1988).Follow-up nomic disparities in childhood mortality in
studies of high-risk low-birthweight infants. Boston. New England Journal of Medicine,
In H. E. Fitzgerald, B. M. Lester, & M. W. 313(6), 360-366.
Yogman (Eds.), Theory -and research in be- Yee, A. H., Fairchild, H. H., Weizmann, F., & Wy-
havioral pediatrics (Vol. 4, pp. 1-65). New att, G. E. (1993). Addressing psychology's
York: Plenum. problem with race. American Psychologist,
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cam- 48(11), 1132-1140.
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Young, V. H. (1974). A black American socializa-
Wasik, B. H. (1992, January). The context of the tion pattern. American Ethnologist, 1(2),
classroom and children's learning. In J. Bel- 405-413.
sky (Chair), The context of schooling. Sympo-
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 1 of 7 -

You have printed the following article:


An Integrative Model for the Study of Developmental Competencies in Minority Children
Cynthia García Coll; Gontran Lamberty; Renee Jenkins; Harriet Pipes McAdoo; Keith Crnic;
Barbara Hanna Wasik; Heidie Vázquez García
Child Development, Vol. 67, No. 5. (Oct., 1996), pp. 1891-1914.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199610%2967%3A5%3C1891%3AAIMFTS%3E2.0.CO%3B2-K

This article references the following linked citations. If you are trying to access articles from an
off-campus location, you may be required to first logon via your library web site to access JSTOR. Please
visit your library's website or contact a librarian to learn about options for remote access to JSTOR.

References

Comparing Stratification Theories


Paul Attewell; Robert Fitzgerald
American Sociological Review, Vol. 45, No. 2. (Apr., 1980), pp. 325-328.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0003-1224%28198004%2945%3A2%3C325%3ACST%3E2.0.CO%3B2-I

Psychological/Behavioral Verve in Academic/Task Performance: Pre-Theoretical


Considerations
A. Wade Boykin
The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 47, No. 4. (Autumn, 1978), pp. 343-354.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0022-2984%28197823%2947%3A4%3C343%3APVIAPP%3E2.0.CO%3B2-8

Racial Attitude Development among Young Black Children as a Function of Parental


Attitudes: A Longitudinal and Cross-Sectional Study
Curtis W. Branch; Nora Newcombe
Child Development, Vol. 57, No. 3. (Jun., 1986), pp. 712-721.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28198606%2957%3A3%3C712%3ARADAYB%3E2.0.CO%3B2-Z
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 2 of 7 -

Economic Deprivation and Early Childhood Development


Greg J. Duncan; Jeanne Brooks-Gunn; Pamela Kato Klebanov
Child Development, Vol. 65, No. 2, Children and Poverty. (Apr., 1994), pp. 296-318.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199404%2965%3A2%3C296%3AEDAECD%3E2.0.CO%3B2-Y

Some Practical Guidelines for Measuring Youth's Race/Ethnicity and Socioeconomic Status
Doris R. Entwisle; Nan Marie Astone
Child Development, Vol. 65, No. 6. (Dec., 1994), pp. 1521-1540.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199412%2965%3A6%3C1521%3ASPGFMY%3E2.0.CO%3B2-3

Developmental Outcome of Minority Infants: A Process-Oriented Look into Our Beginnings


Cynthia T. Garcia Coll
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 270-289.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C270%3ADOOMIA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-W

Approaches to Multicultural Education in the United States: Some Concepts and Assumptions
Margaret Alison Gibson
Anthropology & Education Quarterly, Vol. 7, No. 4, Anthropological Perspectives on
Multi-Cultural Education. (Nov., 1976), pp. 7-18.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0161-7761%28197611%297%3A4%3C7%3AATMEIT%3E2.0.CO%3B2-H

Determinants of Complexity in Mexican-American and Anglo-American Mothers'


Conceptions of Child Development
Jeannie Gutierrez; Arnold Sameroff
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 384-394.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C384%3ADOCIMA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-7

Are Women and Blacks Closing the Gap? Salary Discrimination in American Science During
the 1970s and 1980s
Yitchak Haberfeld; Yehouda Shenhav
Industrial and Labor Relations Review, Vol. 44, No. 1. (Oct., 1990), pp. 68-82.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0019-7939%28199010%2944%3A1%3C68%3AAWABCT%3E2.0.CO%3B2-N
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 3 of 7 -

Family Ecologies of Ethnic Minority Children


Algea O. Harrison; Melvin N. Wilson; Charles J. Pine; Samuel Q. Chan; Raymond Buriel
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 347-362.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C347%3AFEOEMC%3E2.0.CO%3B2-1

The Negro in the New World: The Statement of a Problem


Melville J. Herskovits
American Anthropologist, New Series, Vol. 32, No. 1. (Jan. - Mar., 1930), pp. 145-155.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0002-7294%28193001%2F03%292%3A32%3A1%3C145%3ATNITNW%3E2.0.CO%3B2-F

The Inheritability of Identity: Children's Understanding of the Cultural Biology of Race


Lawrence A. Hirschfeld
Child Development, Vol. 66, No. 5. (Oct., 1995), pp. 1418-1437.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199510%2966%3A5%3C1418%3ATIOICU%3E2.0.CO%3B2-Q

Children and Poverty: Issues in Contemporary Research


Aletha C. Huston; Vonnie C. McLoyd; Cynthia Garcia Coll
Child Development, Vol. 65, No. 2, Children and Poverty. (Apr., 1994), pp. 275-282.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199404%2965%3A2%3C275%3ACAPIIC%3E2.0.CO%3B2-M

Children and Poverty: Issues in Contemporary Research


Aletha C. Huston; Vonnie C. McLoyd; Cynthia Garcia Coll
Child Development, Vol. 65, No. 2, Children and Poverty. (Apr., 1994), pp. 275-282.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199404%2965%3A2%3C275%3ACAPIIC%3E2.0.CO%3B2-M

Explaining the Paradox of Puerto Rican Segregation


Douglas S. Massey; Brooks Bitterman
Social Forces, Vol. 64, No. 2. (Dec., 1985), pp. 306-331.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-7732%28198512%2964%3A2%3C306%3AETPOPR%3E2.0.CO%3B2-F
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 4 of 7 -

Suburbanization and Segregation in U.S. Metropolitan Areas


Douglas S. Massey; Nancy A. Denton
The American Journal of Sociology, Vol. 94, No. 3. (Nov., 1988), pp. 592-626.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0002-9602%28198811%2994%3A3%3C592%3ASASIUM%3E2.0.CO%3B2-R

Stress Absorbing Systems in Black Families


Harriette P. McAdoo
Family Relations, Vol. 31, No. 4. (Oct., 1982), pp. 479-488.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0197-6664%28198210%2931%3A4%3C479%3ASASIBF%3E2.0.CO%3B2-X

Has Racism Declined in America? It Depends on Who Is Asking and What Is Asked
John B. McConahay; Betty B. Hardee; Valerie Batts
The Journal of Conflict Resolution, Vol. 25, No. 4. (Dec., 1981), pp. 563-579.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0022-0027%28198112%2925%3A4%3C563%3AHRDIAI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-P

The Impact of Economic Hardship on Black Families and Children: Psychological Distress,
Parenting, and Socioemotional Development
Vonnie C. McLoyd
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 311-346.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C311%3ATIOEHO%3E2.0.CO%3B2-X

Minority Children: Introduction to the Special Issue


Vonnie C. McLoyd
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 263-266.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C263%3AMCITTS%3E2.0.CO%3B2-G

Secular Trends in the Study of Afro-American Children: A Review of "Child Development",


1936-1980
Vonnie C. Mc Loyd; Suzanne M. Randolph
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 50, No. 4/5, History and
Research in Child Development. (1985), pp. 78-92.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-976X%281985%2950%3A4%2F5%3C78%3ASTITSO%3E2.0.CO%3B2-A
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 5 of 7 -

Origins of Human Competence: A Cultural-Ecological Perspective


John U. Ogbu
Child Development, Vol. 52, No. 2. (Jun., 1981), pp. 413-429.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28198106%2952%3A2%3C413%3AOOHCAC%3E2.0.CO%3B2-U

Variability in Minority School Performance: A Problem in Search of an Explanation


John U. Ogbu
Anthropology & Education Quarterly, Vol. 18, No. 4, Explaining the School Performance of
Minority Students. (Dec., 1987), pp. 312-334.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0161-7761%28198712%2918%3A4%3C312%3AVIMSPA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-7

Income Level, Gender, Ethnicity, and Household Composition as Predictors of Children's


School-Based Competence
Charlotte J. Patterson; Janis B. Kupersmidt; Nancy A. Vaden
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 485-494.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C485%3AILGEAH%3E2.0.CO%3B2-8

Correlates of Dissatisfaction in Marriage


Karen S. Renne
Journal of Marriage and the Family, Vol. 32, No. 1. (Feb., 1970), pp. 54-67.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0022-2445%28197002%2932%3A1%3C54%3ACODIM%3E2.0.CO%3B2-Z

Social Class and Self-Esteem Among Children and Adults


Morris Rosenberg; Leonard I. Pearlin
The American Journal of Sociology, Vol. 84, No. 1. (Jul., 1978), pp. 53-77.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0002-9602%28197807%2984%3A1%3C53%3ASCASAC%3E2.0.CO%3B2-W

Patterns of Social Expectations among Black and Mexican-American Children


Mary Jane Rotheram-Borus; Jean S. Phinney
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 542-556.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C542%3APOSEAB%3E2.0.CO%3B2-C
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 6 of 7 -

Insights on the Child Development Movement in the United States


Milton J. E. Senn; William Kessen; L. J. Borstelmann; G. Stanley Hall; John Dewey; Caroline
Zachry; Lawrence K. Frank; Robert S. Woodworth; John B. Watson; Arnold Gesell; Lewis M.
Terman; Sigmund Freud; Kurt Lewin; Jean Piaget
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 40, No. 3/4, Insights on the
Child Development Movement in the United States. (Aug., 1975), pp. 1-107.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-976X%28197508%2940%3A3%2F4%3C1%3AIOTCDM%3E2.0.CO%3B2-2

Toward Cultural/Ecological Perspectives on Schooling and Achievement in African- and


Asian-American Children
Diana T. Slaughter-Defoe; Kathryn Nakagawa; Ruby Takanishi; Deborah J. Johnson
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 363-383.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C363%3ATCPOSA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-Y

Development of Minority Children: An Introduction


Margaret Beale Spencer
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 267-269.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C267%3ADOMCAI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-7

Identity Processes among Racial and Ethnic Minority Children in America


Margaret Beale Spencer; Carol Markstrom-Adams
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 290-310.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C290%3AIPARAE%3E2.0.CO%3B2-D

Beliefs and Achievement: A Study of Black, White, and Hispanic Children


Harold W. Stevenson; Chuansheng Chen; David H. Uttal
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 508-523.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C508%3ABAAASO%3E2.0.CO%3B2-A
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 7 of 7 -

Sociodemographic and Environmental Correlates of Racial Socialization by Black Parents


Michael C. Thornton; Linda M. Chatters; Robert Joseph Taylor; Walter R. Allen
Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 2, Special Issue on Minority Children. (Apr., 1990), pp. 401-409.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199004%2961%3A2%3C401%3ASAECOR%3E2.0.CO%3B2-N

A Theory of Ethnic Oppression: Toward a Reintegration of Cultural and Structural Concepts


in Ethnic Relations Theory
Jonathan H. Turner; Royce Singleton, Jr.
Social Forces, Vol. 56, No. 4. (Jun., 1978), pp. 1001-1018.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-7732%28197806%2956%3A4%3C1001%3AATOEOT%3E2.0.CO%3B2-L

A Black American Socialization Pattern


Virginia Heyer Young
American Ethnologist, Vol. 1, No. 2. (May, 1974), pp. 405-413.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0094-0496%28197405%291%3A2%3C405%3AABASP%3E2.0.CO%3B2-Q

View publication stats

You might also like