Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 43

INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 1

What Does Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Look Like in Everyday Life?

Anh Tran1

Katharine H. Greenaway1

Joanne Kostopoulos2

Elise K. Kalokerinos1

1
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne
2
Deakin University

Word count = 8,313

Author Note

The authors were supported by an Australian Research Council Discovery Project awarded to the

second and fourth authors (DP190103072) and an Australian Research Council Future

Fellowship awarded to the second author (FT190100300). Corresponding author: Anh Tran,

Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Australia.

anh.tran@student.unimelb.edu.au
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 2

Abstract

How often do people intend to engage in interpersonal emotion regulation in day-to-day life?

Existing literature has focused primarily on asking about the strategies people use to regulate,

which means researchers have little understanding of how often people actually aim to engage in

regulation to begin with. To answer this foundational question, we conducted two studies using

daily diary (N = 171) and experience sampling methods (N = 239). We explored how often

people use others to regulate their own emotions, and how often they regulate others’ emotions.

Almost everyone engaged in interpersonal emotion regulation at least once over the course of a

week, primarily with the goal to help themselves or others feel better, rather than worse. In fact,

people regulated others’ emotions nearly twice as often as they turned to others to regulate their

own emotions, and put more conscious effort into regulating others’ emotions compared to their

own. Trait perceived efficacy of engaging in interpersonal emotion regulation was the most

consistent predictor of momentary regulation intentions. The medium of the interaction—in

person or digital—did not consistently predict momentary intentions or effort. Together, these

findings provide a foundational picture of the interpersonal emotion regulation landscape, and

lay the groundwork for future exploration into this emerging subfield of affective science.
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 3

What Does Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Look Like in Everyday Life?

When the going gets tough, we often turn to other people. Indeed, up to 96% of the time,

emotional experiences are shared with others (Rimé, 2009). The experience, expression, and

regulation of emotions rarely occurs in a vacuum, but instead occur most often with and through

other people (Parkinson & Manstead, 2015). As a result, affective science is beginning to

highlight the interpersonal nature of emotions (van Kleef, 2021). Relatedly, emotion regulation

—a deliberate process that involves influencing emotions in response to a mismatch between

desired and actual emotional states (Mauss & Tamir, 2014)—is increasingly studied as an

interpersonal process (Niven, 2017; Zaki & Williams, 2013).

A growing body of research has focused on mapping the strategies and outcomes of

interpersonal emotion regulation (e.g., Hofmann et al., 2016; Niven et al., 2011; Swerdlow &

Johnson, 2020). However, this focus leaves a key gap in the literature: researchers know little

about the processes which occur prior to strategy selection and implementation. There is a lack

of empirical understanding of how often people intend to engage in interpersonal emotion

regulation in the first place, what they want to feel when they do, how much effort they put in to

achieving these goals, and what predicts people’s regulation intentions and effort. Understanding

these initial steps of the process is key to laying strong foundations in the interpersonal emotion

regulation literature. Thus, the present research seeks to address these basic questions about how

interpersonal emotion regulation unfolds in daily life.

What Do We Know About Interpersonal Emotion Regulation?

Existing research delineates interpersonal emotion regulation into two classes: intrinsic,

whereby we influence our own emotions by turning to other people; and extrinsic, whereby we

influence other people’s emotions (Zaki & Williams, 2013). Both intrinsic and extrinsic
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 4

interpersonal emotion regulation are critical for wellbeing. They help build new relationships

(Niven et al., 2012), enhance the quality of existing relationships (Debrot et al., 2013), and

increase popularity (Niven et al., 2015). In contrast, interpersonal emotion dysregulation is

associated with higher levels of psychological distress (Hofmann et al., 2016), which manifests

in increased anxiety and depression (Marroquín, 2011; Mennin et al., 2005).

Apart from documenting its relational and emotional outcomes, research has focused

largely on the strategies people use to regulate their own and others’ emotions. Converging

evidence suggests that people may seek to feel better or to make others feel better through

emotional support, cognitive support, and social modelling (Hofmann et al., 2016; Swerdlow &

Johnson, 2020), or make others feel worse through hostility and inauthentic display of emotions

(Austin & O’Donnell, 2013; Swerdlow & Johnson, 2020). Research has also uncovered people’s

preferences to give and receive emotional support (Pauw et al., 2018), as well as how to optimize

relational and affective outcomes by combining emotional and cognitive strategies (Feng, 2009).

However, while this ever-growing body of research can help us answer questions about the ways

in which people engage in interpersonal emotion regulation, there is still little understanding of

how often people actually intend to engage in it. Examining regulation intention is therefore an

understudied, but necessary, precursor to understanding how interpersonal emotion regulation

processes unfold.

What Don’t We Know About Interpersonal Emotion Regulation?

Intentions and goals

By focusing on how people regulate, strategy-focused research tacitly assumes that

people intend to regulate in the first place. Indeed, a standard interpersonal emotion regulation

questionnaire might ask people to rate the degree to which they use certain strategies from ‘not at
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 5

all’ to ‘a great deal’ (e.g., Niven et al., 2011). However, research from both laboratory and daily

life indicates that people do not always choose to engage in emotion regulation—a necessary

precursor to choosing a given strategy (English et al., 2017; Sheppes, 2020; Suri et al., 2015). As

such, it is important to first understand the earlier processes that determine whether interpersonal

emotion regulation occurs. To do this, we turned to the dominant theoretical perspective in the

broader emotion regulation literature: the extended process model (Gross, 2015).

This model outlines three stages of emotion regulation: (a) identification (assessing if

regulation is required); (b) selection (choosing a regulation strategy); and (c) implementation

(executing the regulation strategy). In the identification stage, a person notices an emotion and

decides whether there is a discrepancy between actual and desired emotion states, at which point

they may want to engage in regulation, with an emotion regulation goal (i.e., desired directional

change in emotion state) in mind (Tamir, 2021). Having an emotion regulation intention and goal

is therefore crucial to initiate strategy selection and implementation (Tamir et al., 2020). Thus,

examining intentions (do people want to regulate) and goals (how do they want to feel when they

do) is an important first step to understand how, and when, emotion regulation processes begin.

Effort

Closely tied to the concept of a goal is effort, operationalized as the intensity of goal

pursuit (Tamir, 2021). Leading theoretical frameworks for both personal and interpersonal

emotion regulation assume that when individuals intend to regulate emotions, they will put in the

effort to do so (Gross, 2015; Zaki & Williams, 2013). However, as a motivated decision-making

process, emotion regulation is subject to the default effect, wherein individuals gravitate towards

the default option that requires the least action (Thaler & Sunstein, 2008). In the case of emotion

regulation, when given the choice between regulating and not regulating negative emotions in the
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 6

lab, people’s default is to not put in effort at all (Sheppes, 2020; Suri et al., 2015). Outside of the

lab, people also do not always engage in effortful emotion regulation (English et al., 2017).

These findings, while specific to personal emotion regulation, highlight the need to investigate

how much effort people actually invest in regulating their own and others’ emotions. Knowing

the amount of interpersonal emotion regulation effort people put in will help researchers refine

theoretical models and advance more practically relevant research, such as interventions that can

promote regulation effort and engagement.

Predicting intention and effort

To gain a more holistic understanding of regulation intentions and effort, it is not enough

to ask simply how often and how much, we also need to investigate the factors that predict these

processes. Extant literature highlights the importance of both trait and state variables in shaping

personal emotion regulation (Doré et al., 2016; English et al., 2017). In the current research, we

focus on some key trait and situational factors as potential predictors of interpersonal emotion

regulation engagement.

At the trait level, one possible starting point is to consider people’s trait interpersonal

emotion regulation tendencies. Evidence suggests that when individuals have a chronic tendency

to engage in intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation, they seek out others more following

emotional experiences; and when they believe doing so is effective, they benefit more from

receiving social support (Williams et al., 2018). Nevertheless, because these findings are based

on laboratory experimental designs, we still do not know if they translate to everyday behaviors.

In fact, recent work in the personal emotion regulation space has discovered that trait measures

do not always map well onto day-to-day engagement (Koval et al., in press). As such, we aim to

test the link between trait and state measures, by investigating whether trait tendency and
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 7

perceived efficacy of interpersonal emotion regulation predicts everyday intentions and effort

directed towards intrinsic and extrinsic forms of regulation.

At the situational level, the research spotlight has been on the interaction partner (English

et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2021). With the advent of digital communications, particularly during the

COVID-19 pandemic where face-to-face interactions are more difficult, people are socializing—

and regulating each other’s emotions—in increasingly comparable ways across media modalities

(Battaglini et al., 2021; Nguyen et al., 2020). This modality shift calls for an investigation into

whether individuals engage in interpersonal emotion regulation differently in-person compared

to digitally. We thus examine interaction medium as a predictor of everyday interpersonal

emotion regulation intentions and effort.

Measuring Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in Everyday Life

To gain a full picture of interpersonal emotion regulation, we need to capture the full

complexity and diversity of social interactions in which it occurs. Despite this need, most

research in this space has been based on global retrospective self-reports or laboratory

manipulations. While these methodologies have their own merits, they are perhaps not the best

suited to study the varied social interactions in which interpersonal emotion regulation occurs.

Retrospective self-reports may be susceptible to recall biases, which can affect ratings of

emotion intensity (Thomas & Diener, 1990). In addition, laboratory studies may lack ecological

validity. First, they only allow researchers to study few interactions, which do not reflect the full

range of social interactions that people have in daily life. Second, emotions induced in the lab

can be qualitatively and quantitatively different from emotions that occur in real life (Wilhelm &

Grossman, 2010), and thus in the lab it is not always the best place to study personally
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 8

meaningful and consequential emotional experiences which are critical for regulation processes

(Kuppens et al., 2022).

The limitations of traditional cross-sectional research methodologies can be addressed by

studying interpersonal emotion regulation in a more naturalistic setting. Ambulatory assessment

techniques, such as daily diary and experience sampling methodology (ESM), are well poised to

achieve this goal. With near-real time data collection that captures life as it is lived (Bolger et al.,

2003), these techniques can paint a more complete picture of what interpersonal emotion

regulation looks like in daily life.

Liu and colleagues (2021) were among the first to employ ESM to explore interpersonal

emotion regulation. Their research suggested that individuals share negative emotional

experiences with others roughly every other day, with the goal of seeking out emotion-oriented

and problem-oriented support. However, this work did not investigate regulation effort, nor did it

explicitly study regulation intention. Instead, it equated intention to the sharing of negative

emotional events and experiences, which does not always represent an attempt to regulate

emotions (Zaki & Williams, 2013). Our work aims to fill this gap by investigating regulation

intention as a deliberate process that individuals consciously intend to engage in (Niven, 2017;

Zaki & Williams, 2013), as well as the amount of effort they invest in regulation. Additionally,

the work by Liu and colleagues (2021) examined only the intrinsic side of interpersonal emotion

regulation, and focused only on negative events. This offers two further avenues for future work

to build on.

Negative events have been the focal point of emotion regulation research more broadly,

underpinned by the prohedonic assumption that regulatory attempts are centered around

downregulating negative emotions and upregulating positive emotions (Tamir, 2016).


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 9

Unsurprisingly, empirical evidence from personal emotion regulation research reveals that

people almost always hold affect-improving goals for themselves (English et al., 2017;

Kalokerinos et al., 2017) However, theoretical accounts propose that people can have affect-

improving or affect-worsening goals for both themselves and others, depending on what they are

trying to achieve (Niven, 2016; Niven et al., 2011; Tamir, 2016). While uncommon, affect-

worsening regulation does exist, although it has been investigated only rarely in the current

literature. As such, the research landscape is incomplete, with half the picture missing where (a)

extrinsic regulation and (b) affect-worsening goals should be. The current research provides

these missing pieces of the puzzle.

The Current Research

Interpersonal emotion regulation research is growing rapidly, but researchers have been

preoccupied with strategy selection and implementation, with little attention to the initial steps

underpinning these downstream processes. We sought to map the identification stage of

interpersonal emotion regulation using ambulatory assessment techniques, assessing both

intrinsic and extrinsic forms of interpersonal emotion regulation to answer four exploratory

research questions centered on intention and effort:

1. How often do people engage in intrinsic and extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation?

2. What emotion regulation goals do people have when they intend to regulate?

3. When they have a goal, how much effort do people invest to achieve that goal?

4. What factors predict people’s interpersonal emotion regulation intention and effort?

Specifically, we capitalized on the ability of ambulatory assessments to capture

contextual factors to examine interaction medium, as well as to test the link between trait
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 10

measures of intrinsic (Studies 1 and 2) and extrinsic (Study 2) interpersonal emotion regulation

in predicting state regulation engagement.

We examined these questions using daily diary (Study 1) and experience sampling

methods (ESM; Study 2) to capture interpersonal emotion regulation as it occurs in real life.

These two methods have strengths that complement each other. On the one hand, daily diary

methods allow researchers to focus on the most significant social interaction of the day, and

therefore examine interpersonal emotion regulation for events that are most salient to people. On

the other hand, ESM, which surveys participants multiple times a day, allows us to understand

interpersonal emotion regulation for events that are relatively more quotidian. Together, these

studies will provide a more ecologically valid picture of what interpersonal emotion regulation

looks like in everyday life.

Study 1

Methods

Participants

The final sample consisted of 1711 participants, aged 18 to 62 (M = 28.96, SD =

11.81). One hundred and thirty-five participants were women, 33 were men,

and 3 were non-binary, preferred to self-identify, or preferred not to say.

Roughly half were single, and half were in a relationship. 77% of participants

were Australian, 11% were Chinese, 4% were Indian, and the remaining 8% were of 15

different nationalities.

1
Fifty-five participants were screened from further participation or excluded prior to analysis for the following reasons (some
participants failed multiple criteria): 16 were located outside the specified recruitment location, 22 completed the baseline in less
than 10 minutes which indicated careless responding, 5 formally withdrew, 2 were under 18 years old, 6 did not complete any
daily diary, and 17 displayed suspicious behaviors which suggested they were likely bots. Specifically, they shared similar
emails, signed up to the study within seconds of one another, and their metadata indicated unfeasible geolocation changes with
every survey. In addition, 6 participants completed the baseline survey twice, so we kept only the most complete entry.
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 11

We aimed for a threshold of 200 participants in our sample, with a minimum viable

sample size of 150. This threshold number was determined by our available funding, and the

minimum number would allow us to detect a between-person correlation of .17 with 80% power

and alpha level of .05, per a sensitivity analysis conducted using G*Power 3.1.9.7 (Faul et al.,

2009).

Participants were recruited as a part of a larger project, in which residency in Victoria,

Australia was an inclusion criterion. However, this criterion was not pertinent to the current

study. Recruitment occurred through a combination of an undergraduate psychology research

participation program and community advertising. Reimbursement for both pools of participants

were dependent on their level of participation in the study (see https://osf.io/ydujv/ for the full

reimbursement scheme).

Design and procedure

The project received ethics approval from The University of Melbourne Office of

Research Ethics and Integrity (ethics approval number 2056479.1). It consisted of three parts: a

baseline survey on Day 1, a 7-day daily diary portion, and a follow-up survey on Day 9.

Community participants completed a screening survey beforehand to determine eligibility. No

variables were analyzed from the follow-up survey for the purposes of the current study. The full

list of measures, collected as part of the larger data collection project, can be found at

https://osf.io/ydujv/

Baseline survey. At 9am on the first day of the study, participants received a link to the

baseline survey on Qualtrics, which closed at 7pm. Participants were ineligible to continue with

the study if they completed the baseline in under 10 minutes or did not reside in Victoria.
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 12

Daily diary surveys. Eligible participants proceeded to the daily diary portion of the

study. For seven consecutive days, daily surveys were sent out via Qualtrics at 5pm and expired

at 11:59pm the same evening. Each survey contained 54 items (11 of which were relevant to the

current study) assessing participants’ most significant social interaction of the day. If participants

did not have any interaction that day, they instead answered questions about a recent significant

interaction, which were included for even branching and were not of interest for this study.

Participants completed on average six out of seven daily diaries, yielding a mean compliance rate

of 85.96% (SD = 22.45), and a total of 1,029 daily diaries. In the current study, we analyzed 989

diaries (96.11%) in which participants indicated they had had a social interaction during the day.

Of the 171 initial participants, we analyzed data from 170 participants who had at least one

social interaction over the course of a week. One day after completing the daily diary portion,

participants completed a follow-up survey on Qualtrics. Then, they were debriefed and

compensated for their participation.

Measures

Baseline Measures

Interpersonal Regulation Questionnaire. To measure participants’ chronic use of

intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation, we used the Interpersonal Regulation Questionnaire

(Williams et al., 2018). Participants indicated how much they agreed with each of 16 statements,

using a 7-point scale ranging from 1, strongly disagree to 7, strongly agree. This measure

yielded two dimensions: tendency to use intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation (e.g., “When

things are going well, I feel compelled to seek out other people”; α = .89), and perceived efficacy

of intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation (e.g., “I really appreciate having other people to

help me figure out my problem”; α = .86).


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 13

Daily Diary Measures

Social Interaction Medium. We asked participants to reflect on their most significant

interaction of the day. Social interaction was defined as a verbal exchange (e.g., in person, over

the phone, or using video chat services) or a written exchange (e.g., social media post, text

message) with another person that lasted more than 2 minutes. Participants then reported whether

this interaction occurred in-person, digitally, or both. Participants could also specify if the

interaction occurred in another medium not listed. For analyses involving interaction medium as

a predictor, we excluded responses that indicated interactions occurred both in person and

digitally, because it would be difficult to disentangle the potential effect of each distinct medium

on regulation intention and effort.

Emotion Regulation Intention. Reflecting on their most significant interaction,

participants indicated whether or not they tried to (1) use other people to influence their own

emotions (intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation), and (2) influence the emotions of others

(extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation).

Emotion Regulation Goals. Regardless of their intention to regulate, participants then

answered questions about regulation goals, by selecting one or multiple items that applied to

them from a list of five goals, adapted from Kalokerinos et al. (2017). Specifically, for both

intrinsic and extrinsic regulation, participants could indicate that they (1) had no goal, (2) wanted

to increase/maintain positive emotions, (3) wanted to decrease negative emotions, (4) wanted to

increase/maintain negative emotions, or (5) wanted to decrease positive emotions.

Emotion Regulation Effort. Three questions adapted from Gutentag et al. (under review)

asked participants how much effort participants invested in: (1) using other people to influence

their own emotions (intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation), and (2) influencing the emotions
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 14

of others (extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation). Participants responded using a slider scale

from 0, no effort at all, to 100, a lot of effort.

Data analytic strategy

We pre-registered our analysis plan at https://osf.io/ydujv/, after data collection and

preprocessing, but before any research questions were analyzed. Analyses were conducted using

RStudio (version 4.1.0). Emotion regulation intention responses were coded as 0, no and 1, yes.

In addition, we created a person-mean variable for intrinsic and extrinsic regulation, which

reflected the proportion of times a person intended to regulate.

Emotion regulation goal and effort analyses were conducted on observations where

participants intended to engage (1, yes) in the corresponding type of emotion regulation. Emotion

regulation goals were represented by a binary variable for each of the five response options for

each interpersonal emotion regulation category, coded as 0, did not endorse that option, or 1,

endorsed that option. On the very rare occasion when participants selected an emotion goal (e.g.,

increase/maintain positive emotions) and no goal simultaneously, we excluded that survey from

analysis, as their response constituted an intention conflict and/or noisy data.

We performed multilevel modelling using the package lme4 (Bates et al., 2015), fitting

two-level mixed effects models with measurement occasions (N = 989) nested within participants

(N = 170). All models included a random intercept for participant and random slopes for any

momentary predictors. All predictors at the person-level were grand-mean centered, which

involved subtracting each individual’s score from the sample’s mean score. This practice

allowed us to establish a meaningful zero point (Enders & Tofighi, 2007). The momentary-level

predictor interaction medium, which was categorical in nature, was entered uncentered to ease

interpretation of the effects (Nezlek, 2012).


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 15

We ran separate models predicting intrinsic and extrinsic interpersonal emotion

regulation. To predict intention, we ran six logistic models with binary outcome variables

(Models 1a-3b). To predict effort, we ran six generalized linear models with continuous outcome

variables (Models 4a-6b). Before interpreting the output of each model, we took a graphical

approach recommended by Fife (2020) to check model assumptions, using the R package sjPlot

(Lüdecke, 2020) to examine diagnostic plots. These checks revealed no assumption violations.

Results

Descriptive statistics for all variables in our analysis are presented in Table 1. Within-

and between-person correlations are in Supplementary Materials A.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics for Continuous Variables.

Study 1 (Daily Diary) Study 2 (ESM)

Variable M SDwithin SDbetween ICC α M SDwithin SDbetween ICC α

Momentary variables

Intrinsic intention 0.28 - 0.30 - - 0.25 - 0.23 - -

Extrinsic intention 0.45 - 0.32 - - 0.36 - 0.26 - -

Intrinsic effort 16.30 15.14 16.88 .35 - 45.20 16.35 17.71 .41 -

Extrinsic effort 26.63 20.36 20.69 .34 - 54.65 17.11 17.19 .38 -

Baseline variables

Intrinsic tendency 4.19 - 1.19 - .89 4.28 - 1.09 - .85

Intrinsic perceived efficacy 5.41 - 0.92 - .86 5.20 - 0.91 - .83

Extrinsic tendency - - - - - 3.67 - 0.72 - .86


Note. M = grand mean; SDwithin = within-person standard deviation; SDbetween = between-person standard deviation;
ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient. α = Cronbach's alpha. ICCs were not calculated for dichotomous variables
because there is no agreed upon method for their calculation.
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 16

Intentions

Slightly over half (64%) of participants turned to others to influence their own emotions

at least once during the study. On average, participants intended to engage in intrinsic

interpersonal emotion regulation 28% of the times they had an interaction.

In contrast, participants intended to engage in extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation

more often, doing so 45% of the times they had an interaction. Most participants engaged in this

type of regulation, with 81% trying to influence others’ emotions at least once during the study.

Goals

Of the 277 diaries in which participants reported intending to engage in intrinsic

interpersonal emotion regulation, almost all focused on affect-improving goals (see Table 2),

mostly by upregulating positive emotions rather than downregulating negative emotions.

Similarly, of the 455 diaries in which participants reported intending to engage in

extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation, almost all focused on affect-improving goals. Most

aimed to upregulate others’ positive emotions rather than downregulating their negative

emotions.

Table 2

Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Goals.

Study 1 (Daily Diary) Study 2 (ESM)


Intrinsic Extrinsic Intrinsic Extrinsic
Goals Frequency % Frequency % Frequency % Frequency %
Upregulating positive emotions 204 60.53 371 69.87 1675 78.27 1378 69.91
Upregulating negative emotions 0 0 9 1.69 67 3.13 47 2.38
Downregulating negative emotions 125 37.09 143 26.93 361 16.87 523 26.53
Downregulating positive emotions 8 2.37 8 1.51 37 1.73 23 1.17
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 17

Effort

The means, within-person standard deviations, between-person standard deviations, and

intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) of the emotion regulation effort types are presented in

Table 1. On average, participants spent roughly 10% less effort intrinsically regulating their own

emotions using others, compared to extrinsically regulating other people’s emotions. Average

ICCs suggested that roughly 34% of variance was between-person, and 66% was within-person.

This implies that effort fluctuates more within an individual than across different individuals.

Predictors of intention

Interaction medium. Two logistic models examined whether interaction medium (in

person vs. digital) predicted participants’ intention to engage in intrinsic (Model 1a) and extrinsic

(Model 1b) regulation. The results of all intention models are presented in Table 3. Interaction

medium was not a significant predictor of participants’ intention to engage in either intrinsic or

extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation. That is, participants were no more likely to engage in

interpersonal emotion regulation when they interacted in-person than they were digitally.

Intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. Two logistic models examined

trait intrinsic tendency as a grand-mean centered predictor of intrinsic (Model 2a) and extrinsic

(Model 2b) intentions. We found that trait intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency

positively predicted intention to engage in intrinsic and extrinsic interpersonal emotion

regulation. For each one-unit increase in trait intrinsic regulation tendency, participants were

1.37 times more likely to turn to others to regulate their own emotions, and 1.35 times more

likely to regulate others’ emotions in a given social interaction.

Intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation efficacy. We ran Models 3a and 3b in the

same fashion as Models 2a and 2b, this time with trait intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 18

efficacy as a grand-mean centered predictor. Results indicated that perceived efficacy

significantly predicted intention to engage in intrinsic and extrinsic interpersonal emotion

regulation. For a one-unit increase in trait intrinsic regulation efficacy, participants were 1.73

times more likely to turn to others to regulate their own emotions, and 1.71 times more likely to

regulate others’ emotions in a given social interaction.

Table 3

Results From Binary Logistic Analyses to Examine Predictors of Regulation Intention.

Model a: Intrinsic intention Model b: Extrinsic intention


Estimate OR Estimate OR
95% CI p 95% CI p
Parameters (SE) (SE) (SE) (SE)
Model 1: Interaction medium
-1.45 0.23 -0.37 0.68
Intercept 0.12, 0.44 <.001 0.45, 1.04 .073
(0.32) (0.08) (0.21) (0.14)
-0.02 0.98 -1.13 1.14
In-person interaction 0.50, 1.90 .949 0.75, 1.74 .539
(0.34) (0.33) (0.21) (0.24)
N / Observations 164 / 767 164 / 767
Model 2: IRQ tendency
-1.36 0.26 -0.24 0.79
Intercept 0.19, 0.35 <.001 0.62, 1.00 .054
(0.16) (0.04) (0.12) (0.10)
0.31 1.37 0.30 1.35
IRQ tendency (grand-mean centered) 1.06, 1.76 .016 1.10, 1.65 .004
(0.13) (0.18) (0.10) (0.14)
N / Observations 170 / 985 170 / 983
Model 3: IRQ efficacy
-1.36 0.26 -0.24 0.78
Intercept 0.19, 0.35 <.001 0.62, 1.00 .047
(0.16) (0.04) (0.12) (0.10)
0.54 1.73 0.54 1.71
IRQ efficacy (grand-mean centered) 1.22, 2.44 .002 1.30, 2.25 <.001
(0.18) (0.31) (0.14) (0.24)
N / Observations 170 / 985 170 / 983
Note. SE = Standard error; OR = odds ratio; CI = 95% confidence interval; N = number of participants. Significant
p-values are bolded.
Predictors of effort

Interaction medium. Models 4a and 4b examined interaction medium as a predictor of

intrinsic and extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation effort, respectively. Because of a singular

fit issue, we removed the random slope for interaction medium from Model 4a. The results of all
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 19

effort models are presented in Table 4. These revealed no significant relationship between

interaction medium and either type of effort.

Intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency and efficacy. We ran four models

to investigate whether trait intrinsic tendency (Models 5a and 5b) and perceived efficacy

(Models 6a and 6b) predicted intrinsic or extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation effort.

Neither trait tendency nor perceived efficacy significantly predicted how much effort participants

invested in interpersonal emotion regulation.

Table 4

Results From Generalized Linear Analyses to Examine Predictors of Regulation Effort.

Model a: Intrinsic effort Model b: Extrinsic effort


Parameters Estimate (SE) 95% CI p Estimate (SE) 95% CI p
Model 4: Interaction medium
Intercept 44.25 (2.65) 39.05, 49.44 <.001 53.16 (2.34) 48.58, 57.75 <.001
In-person interaction -2.23 (3.23) -8.57, 4.10 .491 -4.68 (2.50) -9.58, 0.21 .062
N / Observations 92 / 205 122 / 345
Model 5: IRQ tendency
Intercept 45.71 (1.96) 41.87, 49.54 <.001 50.95 (1.81) 47.41, 54.49 <.001
IRQ tendency (grand-mean centered) 0.13 (1.70) -3.21, 3.46 .940 0.61 (1.57) -2.47, 3.69 .697
N / Observations 108 / 277 137 / 455
Model 6: IRQ efficacy
Intercept 45.49 (1.97) 41.62, 49.36 <.001 50.73 (1.81) 47.18, 54.28 <.001
IRQ efficacy (grand-mean centered) 1.37 (2.46) -3.46, 6.19 .580 2.25 (2.21) -2.08, 6.57 .310
N / Observations 108 / 277 137 / 455
Note. SE = Standard error; CI = 95% confidence interval; N = number of participants. Significant p-values bolded.

Study 2

Study 1 found that almost every participant engaged in interpersonal emotion regulation

at least once over the course of a week, and wanted to make others feel better nearly twice as

often as they did themselves. Participants who engaged in intrinsic regulation more at the trait

level, and believed it to be more efficacious, were also more likely to engage in everyday

interpersonal emotion regulation, although they did not put in more effort. At the situational

level, interaction medium did not predict regulation intention or effort.


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 20

The purpose of Study 2 was to examine whether these findings replicated across

methodologies and samples. While both studies shared fundamentally similar research questions

and measures, Study 2 employed ESM for a more fine-grained approach in identifying patterns

of everyday interpersonal emotion regulation. Furthermore, to complement the trait measure of

intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation featured in Study 1 (Williams et al., 2018), we added a

trait measure of extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation (Niven et al., 2011).

Methods

Participants

The final sample consisted of 2392 participants, aged 18 to 79 (M = 29.74, SD =

10.85). 170 participants were women, 62 were men, and 6 were non-binary,

preferred to self-identify, or preferred not to say. Roughly 40% were single,

while 60% were in a relationship. In terms of nationality, 73% of participants

were Australian, 9% were Chinese, 2% were British, 2% were Indonesian, 2%

were Singaporean, and the remaining 12% were of 16 different nationalities.

Recruitment occurred through a combination of an undergraduate psychology research

participation program, as well as university community advertising. Reimbursement for both

pools of participants were dependent on their level of participation in the study.

We aimed to recruit a minimum of 200 participants. This target sample size was

determined based on an a priori power analysis conducted using the t-method for multilevel

models (Murayama et al., in press). Our goal was to achieve 80% power with an alpha level

of .05. We used a t-value of 2.50, which corresponded to a small effect size of d = .20.

2
Thirty-two participants were excluded prior to analysis for the following reasons (some participants failed multiple criteria): 9
failed the attention checks, 3 were ineligible to continue to the ESM portion, 1 experienced technical issues, 4 formally withdrew,
6 did not complete any ESM surveys, and 9 had no baseline data. In addition, 5 participants completed the baseline survey twice,
so we kept only the most complete entry.
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 21

Design and procedure

The data collection procedure for this study was pre-registered at https://osf.io/5ze6p.

The project received ethics approval from The University of Melbourne Office of Research

Ethics and Integrity (ethics approval number 21361). It comprised two parts: a baseline survey

on Day 1, followed by a 7-day experience sampling period with 7 ESM surveys and one end-of-

day survey per day. Community participants completed a screening survey beforehand to

determine eligibility. All variables of interest were in the baseline and ESM surveys. The full list

of measures, collected as part of the larger data collection project, can be found in the data

collection pre-registration.

Baseline survey. On Day 1 of the study, participants received an email with instructions

to download the SEMA3 mobile application (Koval et al., 2019), and a link to the baseline

survey on Qualtrics, which they had until 4pm to complete. Participants read the Plain Language

Statement and provided informed consent, before completing a battery of trait measures. After

participants completed the baseline measures, they watched a video about the study explaining

its purpose and how to complete specific items in the ESM survey, and a video about the

SEMA3 app. Comprehension checks were included after each video to ensure participants

understood the ESM protocol.

ESM surveys. The following day, eligible participants began the 7-day ESM period.

Each day, participants received notifications to complete 7 ESM surveys from 9:30am to

7:00pm, for a total of 49 ESM surveys. We used a mixed sampling scheme, with each survey

randomly scheduled within a fixed time window. These windows were evenly distributed across

the day. Participants had 30 minutes to complete each survey from when they received the

notification. ESM surveys occurred an average of 89.97 minutes apart (SD = 12.63). Participants
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 22

also completed one end-of-day survey each day, scheduled at 8:00pm and expired at 11:59pm

the same evening, although this survey was not relevant to the current project.

Each ESM survey contained either 29 or 26 items depending on participants’ responses to

certain items. Six items were relevant to this project. Participants reflected on a social interaction

they experienced since the previous survey, and reported on their emotions, as well any emotion

regulation that occurred during the interaction. If participants did not report an interaction, they

instead answered questions about their current emotional state, which were included for even

branching and were not of interest for this study.

To monitor compliance, all participants received email reminders to complete surveys on

days 2 and 5 of the ESM period. Overall compliance was 74.49% (SD = 19.79). Participants

completed on average 36 out of 49 ESM surveys, for a total of 8,678 surveys. In the current

study, we analyzed 5,534 surveys (63.77% of surveys) in which participants had a social

interaction since the previous survey.

Measures

Baseline Measures

Interpersonal Regulation Questionnaire. As in Study 1, we used the Interpersonal

Regulation Questionnaire (Williams et al., 2018) to measure participants’ chronic tendency to

use intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation (α = .85), and perceived efficacy of intrinsic

interpersonal emotion regulation (α = .83).

Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (EROS) Scale. We used the EROS scale (Niven

et al., 2011) to assess extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. Participants reported

the extent to which they regulated others’ emotions in the past two weeks on a scale from 1, not

at all to 5, a great deal. The measure yielded two subscales: extrinsic affect-improving (e.g., “I
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 23

made someone laugh to try to make them feel better”; α = .86), and extrinsic affect-worsening

(e.g., “I acted annoyed towards someone to try to make them feel worse”; α = .69).

In the current study, we focused on the extrinsic affect-improving subscale, henceforth

referred to as extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. The basis of this decision was

that there was low item endorsement for extrinsic affect-worsening (Niven et al., 2011), and

results from Study 1 indicated that extrinsic affect-worsening instances were extremely rare in

everyday life. Descriptive statistics and analyses involving the affect-worsening subscale are in

Supplementary Materials B and C.

ESM Measures

Social Interaction Medium. We asked participants to reflect on their most significant

interaction since the last survey. Social interaction was defined in the same way as Study 1.

Participants then reported whether this interaction occurred in-person, digitally, or in another

medium not specified. As in Study 1, we excluded responses that indicated interactions occurred

in an unspecified medium.

Emotion Regulation Goals. Goal items and response options were the same as Study 1.

Emotion Regulation Effort. Participants who indicated that they had a goal for intrinsic

and/or extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation were asked how much effort they put into

achieving said goal. The effort questions and response options were also the same as in Study 1.

Data analytic strategy

The analysis plan for this study was pre-registered at https://osf.io/pvnas/, after data

collection and preprocessing, but before any research questions were analyzed. Analyses were

conducted using RStudio (version 4.1.0). Following the recommendations of Geeraerts (2020)

regarding careless responding, we excluded prior to analysis any items that were responded to in
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 24

less than 650ms, as well as any surveys that had more than 50% of items responded to in under

this time. As a result, 233 items (0.8% of all relevant items), and 62 surveys (0.7% of all

surveys) were identified and replaced with missing data.

To examine intentions, we created three binary variables based on participants’ response

to the emotion regulation goal items. The decision to measure intention and goal using one item,

as opposed to two items in Study 1, was to reduce response burden in the ESM protocol. If

participants indicated that they did not try to regulate, intention was coded as 0. If they indicated

that they had one or more emotion regulation goals, intention was coded as 1. We then created

two person-mean intention variables from the binary intention variables in the same way as

Study 1. We analyzed the goal and effort variables using the same analytic strategy as Study 1.

Data for Study 2 followed a multilevel structure with measurement occasions (N = 5,534)

nested within participants (N = 239). We conducted all inferential analyses using multilevel

modelling with the package lme4 (Bates et al., 2015). In addition to the three predictors of

regulation intention and effort that were also presented in Study 1, Study 2 also examined

extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. This predictor was not included in our pre-

registered analysis plan. However, as with all analyses featured in the current paper, analyses

involving this predictor were exploratory in nature.

Similar to Study 1, we ran separate models for intrinsic and extrinsic outcome variables,

with eight logistic models to predict regulation intention (Models 1a-4b), and eight generalized

linear models to predict regulation effort (Models 5a-8b). Graphical checks of model

assumptions revealed no violations.


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 25

Results

Descriptive statistics for all variables in our analysis are presented in Table 2. Within-

and between-person correlations are in Supplementary Materials B.

Intentions

All 239 participants reported having had at least one social interaction during the study.

Of these participants, 82% intended to engage in intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation at

least once over the course of a week. On average, participants turned to others in the interaction

to influence their own emotions 25% of the times they had an interaction.

Slightly more (92%) participants intended to influence others’ emotions at least once in

the study. On average, extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation occurred 36% of the time.

Goals

Of the 1,278 surveys in which participants engaged in intrinsic interpersonal emotion

regulation, virtually all focused on affect-improving goals (see Table 2). As in Study 1, affect-

worsening goals were uncommon in everyday life.

Similarly, of the 1,924 surveys in which participants engaged in extrinsic interpersonal

emotion regulation, most goals also focused on improving others’ affect.

Effort

Descriptive statistics of the two effort types are presented in Table 1. On average,

participants invested roughly 10% more effort regulating other people’s emotions compared to

their own. The average ICC was around .39, suggesting—as in Study 1—that effort varied more

within-person than between people.


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 26

Predictors of intention

Interaction medium. We ran two separate logistic models to examine whether

interaction medium predicted participants’ intention to engage in intrinsic (Model 1a) and

extrinsic (Model 1b) interpersonal emotion regulation. The results of all models involving

intention are presented in Table 5. Once again, interaction medium did not significantly predict

intrinsic or extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation intention.

Intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. Models 2a and 2b examined trait

intrinsic tendency as a grand-mean centered predictor of emotion regulation intentions. These

showed that for a one-unit increase in trait intrinsic regulation tendency, participants were 1.28

times more likely more likely to turn to others to regulate their own emotions (intrinsic), but

were not more likely to regulate others’ emotions (extrinsic) in a given social interaction.

Intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation efficacy. Models 3a and 3b examined trait

intrinsic efficacy as a grand-mean centered predictor of emotion regulation intentions. These

showed that for a one-unit increase in trait intrinsic regulation efficacy, participants were 1.59

times more likely to turn to others to regulate their own emotions (intrinsic), and 1.24 times more

likely to regulate others’ emotions (extrinsic) in a given social interaction.

Extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. Models 4a and 4b examined trait

extrinsic tendency as a grand-mean centered predictor of emotion regulation intentions. These

showed that for a one-unit increase in trait extrinsic regulation tendency, participants were 1.47

times more likely more likely to turn to others to regulate their own emotions (intrinsic), but

were not more likely to regulate others’ emotions (extrinsic) in a given social interaction.
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 27

Table 5
Results From Binary Logistic Analyses to Examine Predictors of Regulation Intention
Model a: Intrinsic intention Model b: Extrinsic intention
Estimate OR Estimate OR
(SE) 95% CI p (SE) 95% CI p
Parameters (SE) (SE)
Model 1: Interaction medium
-1.56 0.21 -0.63 0.53
Intercept 0.17, 0.27 <.001 0.43, 0.65 <.001
(0.12) (0.03) (0.11) (0.06)
0.01 1.01 -0.17 0.84
In-person interaction 0.82, 1.26 .896 0.69, 1.03 .089
(0.11) (0.11) (0.10) (0.08)
N / Observations 239 / 5329 239 /5297
Model 2: IRQ tendency
-1.53 0.22 -0.73 0.48
Intercept 0.18, 0.26 <.001 0.40, 0.58 <.001
(0.10) (0.02) (0.09) (0.04)
0.24 1.28 0.09 1.09
IRQ tendency (grand-mean centered) 1.06, 1.53 .009 0.92, 1.29 .317
(0.09) (0.12) (0.09) (0.09)
N / Observations 239 / 5417 239 / 5385
Model 3: IRQ efficacy
-1.53 0.22 -0.73 0.48
Intercept 0.18, 0.26 <.001 0.40, 0.58 <.001
(0.10) (0.02) (0.09) (0.04)
0.47 1.59 0.22 1.24
IRQ efficacy (grand-mean centered) 1.29, 1.97 <.001 1.02, 1.52 .031
(0.11) (0.17) (0.10) (0.13)
N / Observations 239 / 5417 239 / 5385
Model 4: EROS
-1.53 0.22 -0.73 0.48
Intercept 0.18, 0.26 <.001 0.40, 0.58 <.001
(0.10) (0.02) (0.09) (0.04)
0.39 1.47 0.01 1.01
EROS (grand-mean centered) 1.11, 1.95 .007 0.79, 1.31 .914
(0.14) (0.21) (0.13) (0.13)
N / Observations 239 / 5417 239 / 5385
Note. SE = Standard error; OR = odds ratio; CI = 95% confidence interval; N = number of participants. Significant
p-values are bolded.

Predictors of effort

Interaction medium. Models 5a and 5b examined interaction medium as a predictor of

intrinsic and extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation effort respectively. Results from all

models predicting effort are presented in Table 6. There was no significant relationship between

interaction medium and intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation effort. However, in-person

interaction was associated with less effort invested in regulating others’ emotions compared to

digital interaction, a novel finding compared to Study 1.

Intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency and efficacy. We ran four models

to investigate whether trait intrinsic tendency (Models 6a and 6b) and efficacy (Models 7a and
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 28

7b) predicted intrinsic or extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation effort. As in Study 1, neither

trait tendency nor efficacy significantly predicted how much effort participants invested in

interpersonal emotion regulation.

Extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. Extrinsic interpersonal emotion

regulation tendency significantly predicted effort for both intrinsic (Model 8a) and extrinsic

regulation (Model 8b). Indeed, results showed participants who scored higher on trait extrinsic

regulation tendency invested roughly 5-6% more effort in intrinsic and extrinsic forms of

interpersonal emotion regulation than those who scored lower on this trait.

Table 6
Results From Generalized Linear Analyses to Examine Predictors of Regulation Effort
Model a: Intrinsic effort Model b: Extrinsic effort
Parameters Estimate (SE) 95% CI p Estimate (SE) 95% CI p
Model 5: Interaction medium
Intercept 47.10 (1.60) 43.96 , 50.23 <.001 57.80 (1.29) 55.26, 60.33 <.001
In-person interaction -1.65 (1.39) -4.38, 1.07 .237 -3.69 (1.18) -6.00, -1.39 .002
N / Observations 196 / 1248 221 / 1896
Model 6: IRQ tendency
Intercept 45.96 (1.27) 43.48, 48.45 <.001 55.48 (1.12) 53.29, 57.67 <.001
IRQ tendency (grand-mean centered) -0.12 (1.19) -2.45, 2.21 .919 0.65 (1.03) -1.37, 2.67 .530
N / Observations 197 / 1273 221 / 1938
Model 7: IRQ efficacy
Intercept 45.67 (1.26) 43.20, 48.13 <.001 55.38 (1.11) 53.21, 57.56 <.001
IRQ efficacy (grand-mean centered) 2.31 (1.41) -0.46, 5.07 .103 2.18 (1.23) -0.23, 4.59 0.078
N / Observations 197 / 1273 221 / 1938
Model 8: EROS
Intercept 45.53 (1.23) 43.11, 47.95 <.001 55.53 (1.08) 53.41, 57.64 <.001
EROS (grand-mean centered) 5.41 (1.79) 1.91, 8.92 .003 6.11 (1.54) 3.10, 9.12 <.001
N / Observations 197 / 1273 221 / 1938
Note. SE = Standard error; CI = 95% confidence interval; N = number of participants. Significant p-values are bolded.

General Discussion

Research on interpersonal emotion regulation is growing, but the spotlight has so far been

on the strategies people use to regulate, with little attention to the initial processes that precede

strategy selection and implementation. The present research contributes to mapping this

uncharted space. Using daily diary (Study 1) and experience sampling methodology (Study 2),
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 29

we conducted a comprehensive examination of interpersonal emotion regulation, investigating

both intrinsic and extrinsic forms of interpersonal emotion regulation, as well as affect-

improving and affect-worsening regulation goals. Our findings revealed that almost everyone

engaged in interpersonal emotion regulation, although at relatively low frequencies throughout

the week. Moreover, our research identified some overlap, but also some disconnects, between

trait and momentary measures of interpersonal emotion regulation constructs. In the next

sections, we revisit each of our initial research questions and discuss the implications of our

findings.

How Often Do People Engage in Interpersonal Emotion Regulation?

Almost everyone tried to regulate others’ emotions at least once in the sampling period of

a week, and they did so 36-45% of the time across their interactions. In contrast, fewer people

turned to others to regulate their own emotions, doing so 25-28% of the time. For Study 2, which

sampled participants seven times a day and thus allowed us to make more concrete judgment of

frequency, these percentages imply that intrinsic regulation occurred around once a day, as

opposed to nearly twice a day for extrinsic regulation.

Our findings suggest that when it came to emotions, people helped others more often than

turning to others for help. However, because support-seeking can happen at both conscious and

unconscious levels (Barbee et al., 1993), individuals may not always be aware that they are

turning to others for emotion regulation help in a social interaction. Extant theoretical

frameworks define interpersonal emotion regulation as a deliberate, conscious process (Niven,

2017; Zaki & Williams, 2013). This process was what our self-report measure captured, and thus

our findings on the frequency of intentions should not be generalized to the frequency of all

support-seeking and providing behaviors. Future work might do well to investigate more
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 30

unconscious forms of intrinsic support seeking, or to focus on contexts in which people are more

likely to reach out for support. Further, given the low frequencies of both intrinsic and extrinsic

regulation in everyday life, future researchers who wish to investigate everyday use of regulation

strategies might need to consider assessing people’s regulation intention first, to ensure that they

are capturing strategy use on occasions when people actually intend to regulate.

What Emotion Regulation Goals Do People Have When They Intend to Regulate?

The majority of emotion regulation goals focused on improving affect, primarily by

upregulating positive emotions. This pattern was consistent across intrinsic and extrinsic

interpersonal emotion regulation. The prevalence of goals to upregulate positive emotions was

somewhat surprising. Research often implicitly assumes that the crux of emotion regulation is to

downregulate negative emotions, such that emotion regulation strategies have become almost

synonymous with strategies to reduce negative emotions (see Webb et al., 2012 for a review).

Yet, our findings suggest that when people wanted to feel better, they more often wanted to

amplify the positive. It could be the case that striving to feel more positive is an approach goal,

whereas avoiding feeling negative is an avoidance goal. While both are central to wellbeing,

approach goals are easier to pursue and monitor, as well as potentially more useful in coping

with negative events (Tamir, 2021), and thus may hold more appeal than an avoidance goal

(Tamir & Diener, 2008).

Although affect-improving goals were most frequent, people endorsed affect-worsening

goals for others and for themselves 2-5% of the time. Research on affect-worsening is sparse,

likely stymied by the belief that it is a rare occurrence. Nevertheless, our findings suggest that

affect-worsening does exist in everyday life, and appears to be slightly more frequent for

interpersonal emotion regulation compared to 1.6% for personal emotion regulation (Kalokerinos
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 31

et al., 2017). Because the kind of goal people have can influence the strategies they choose to

achieve that goal (Millgram et al., 2019), investigating affect-worsening goals is a worthy

endeavor, as it can help researchers map out a more comprehensive taxonomy of emotion

regulation strategies, and better understand the downstream effects on regulation outcomes.

When People Have a Goal, How Much Effort Do They Invest to Achieve That Goal?

Across both studies, participants invested moderate levels of effort in achieving emotion

goals, scoring roughly 16 (Study 1) to 55 (Study 2) on a 100-point scale. They also spent

approximately 10% less effort in turning to others to regulate their own emotions, compared to

regulating others’ emotions. It is possible that since individuals could draw on intrapersonal

resources in addition to interpersonal resources when regulating their own emotions, intrinsic

regulation was relatively less effortful than extrinsic regulation. Furthermore, merely interacting

with others has been found to buffer against negative affect (Beckes & Coan, 2011). As a result,

people may benefit from this incidental interpersonal modulation, and thus need to put in less

conscious effort to seek help regulating their own emotions (Zaki & Williams, 2013).

What Predicts Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Intention and Effort?

Table 7 summarizes our findings across different predictors and outcome variables.
Interaction medium

There were no significant associations between interaction medium and regulation

intentions and effort. Across both studies, participants were no more likely to engage in

interpersonal emotion regulation when they interacted in-person than they were digitally, nor did

they consistently put in more effort in one medium compared to another.

To contextualize our findings, the studies collected data from September 2020 to August

2021, with every participant residing in Victoria, Australia. During this time, Victoria underwent
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 32

six stringent lockdowns due to the impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic, for a total of over 200

days under stay-at-home order by the end of the data collection period (Campbell, 2021). In

between lockdowns, restrictions were still enforced, including work-from-home and in-home-

visitor limit government directives that constrained face-to-face interactions. Victorians are thus

no strangers to digital communication replacing in-person interactions in every aspect of their

professional and personal lives. In a time when socialization and emotion regulation are

becoming more and more comparable across media (Battaglini et al., 2021; Nguyen et al., 2020),

our findings suggest that the medium of interaction—be it digital or in-person—did not influence

people’s intentions or effort to engage in interpersonal emotion regulation.

Trait predictors

We also investigated the overlap between trait measures of interpersonal emotion

regulation and daily measures of the same constructs. In particular, we tested whether trait

measures of intrinsic (Studies 1 and 2) and extrinsic (Study 2) interpersonal emotion regulation

predicted people’s momentary interpersonal emotion regulation behaviors in everyday

interactions.

Intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation perceived efficacy. Individuals who

believed intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation to be more efficacious—that is, more likely to

result in desired emotional outcomes—were more likely to intend to regulate their own and

others’ emotion, although they did not put in more effort to do so. High perceived efficacy of

intrinsic regulation, as measured by the IRQ scale (Williams et al., 2018), means individuals

have a positive attitude toward intrinsic regulation (i.e., turning to others is helpful), and

perceived behavioral control over intrinsic regulation (i.e., I can turn to others effectively for

help). Attitude and perceived behavioral control are two key predictors of behavior intention,
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 33

such that individuals are more like to intend to engage in a behavior if they have a positive

attitude towards it and believe they are capable to enacting it (Ajzen, 1991). Perhaps because the

IRQ perceived efficacy sub-scale captures both of these concepts, it was the most consistent

predictor of regulation intentions.

Intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. Trait tendency to engage in

intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation, as measured by the IRQ tendency sub-scale (Williams

et al., 2018), consistently predicted everyday intrinsic intention. This supports the measure’s

validity in mapping trait intrinsic tendency onto everyday intrinsic tendency. However, IRQ

tendency did not predict everyday regulation effort. As such, this measure appears to tap into

people’s decisions to regulate, but not necessarily how much effort they put into its

implementation. This could indicate that certain forms of interpersonal emotion regulation are

subject to the default effect, such that people take the path of least resistance by not investing

effort when it might be useful to do so (Sheppes, 2020; Suri et al., 2015; English et al., 2017). In

this case, what people generally reported doing (i.e., intention) did not always reflect how hard

they tried (i.e., effort). Further, inconsistency shown by the IRQ intrinsic tendency sub-scale in

predicting extrinsic constructs highlights the conceptual distinction between intrinsic and

extrinsic regulation, and calls for the use of separate but consistent trait measures to assess

intrinsic and extrinsic regulation concurrently.

Extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency. In Study 2, we included the

EROS scale to measure trait extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation tendency, which

predicted everyday intrinsic, but not extrinsic regulation intention. However, trait extrinsic

tendency was significantly associated with both intrinsic and extrinsic regulation effort, such that

individuals high in trait extrinsic tendency exerted more effort when regulating their own or
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 34

others’ emotions in everyday life. This significant association was in contrast with trait intrinsic

tendency, which was not associated with everyday regulation effort.

One possible explanation for this finding may involve the measures of these traits. The

EROS scale, which measures extrinsic tendency, asks about individuals’ specific behaviors

within the past two weeks, and therefore relies on episodic knowledge. Conversely, the IRQ

scale for intrinsic tendency asks about individuals’ broad tendency and beliefs without a

specified time scale, and therefore relies on semantic knowledge (Robinson & Clore, 2002).

These two measures may thus capture conceptually different things, and have different predictive

properties when it comes to momentary constructs. That is, if people bring to mind specific

instances of interpersonal emotion regulation when answering the EROS, these instances may be

more closely linked to the strategies they use—and effort they put into using—in everyday life.

In contrast, as a general measure of propensity, the IRQ tendency scale seems to more directly

map onto people’s intentions to intrinsically regulate, rather than the effort they invest once

having decided to regulate.

This finding raises methodological considerations for the broader emotion regulation

literature—time scale of measurement and consistency across measures, particularly for intrinsic

and extrinsic regulation which typically are not typically assessed together in the same study.

Future works that build on the wealth of existing trait measures should not only consider

assessing intrinsic and extrinsic regulation concurrently, but also incorporate related momentary

constructs in the measure validation stage (McMahon & Naragon-Gainey, 2020).


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 35

Table 7
Result Summary of Models Predicting Intention and Effort Across Studies

Outcomes
Predictors Intrinsic intention Extrinsic intention Intrinsic effort Extrinsic effort
Situational level
Interaction medium û û û ?
Trait level
Intrinsic interpersonal
emotion regulation ü ? û û
tendency
Intrinsic interpersonal
ü ü û û
emotion regulation efficacy
Extrinsic interpersonal
ü û ü ü
emotion regulation tendency
Note. üconsistently positive and significant associations; ûconsistently non-significant
associations; ? inconsistent results across analyses. Extrinsic interpersonal emotion
regulation tendency was included in Study 2 only.

Limitations and Future Directions

These studies are among the first to paint a holistic picture of interpersonal emotion

regulation in everyday life. Further, the findings advance understanding of regulation intention

and goals, which are the initial steps that precede strategy selection and implementation.

Nevertheless, our studies have limitations that future work can address.

First, because participants could only report one significant social interaction per survey,

which occurred roughly 24 hours apart in Study 1 and 1.5 hours in Study 2, the data likely do not

reflect the full extent of social interactions that people experienced. Indeed, signal-contingent

designs for ambulatory assessments, as employed in our studies, capture roughly 1.6 times fewer

social interactions than event-contingent designs, in which people can start a survey on their own

every time an interaction occurs (Himmelstein et al., 2019). Future works may also consider

adopting such a design, which allows researchers to target the more emotionally intense

interactions critical for regulation processes (Kuppens et al., 2022).

Additionally, interpersonal processes involve more than one individual, while our

investigation only looked at one side of the interaction. In the future, examining both sides of the
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 36

dyad would help researchers answer more questions, such as whether an extrinsic interpersonal

emotion regulation attempt is effective in changing others’ emotions, or whether both parties in

an intrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation attempt share the same emotion regulation goals.

Apart from addressing these limitations, future studies can also explore new avenues that

the present research has introduced. First, our investigation into how much effort people spend

invites a follow-up question of whether this effort pays off. Second, to gain a deeper

understanding of what is driving interpersonal emotion regulation, we suggest looking beyond

emotion regulation goals, into the higher-order motives these goals serve (Tamir et al., 2020).

Motives have implications on both goals and strategies people use to achieve those goals

(Millgram et al., 2019), yet have not been explored in detail in the interpersonal emotion

regulation space to date.

Conclusion

The present study was the first to examine intrinsic and extrinsic interpersonal emotion

regulation concurrently in everyday life. Using a combination of daily diary and experience

sampling methodology, our research provides insights into the frequency, goals, effort, and

predictors of interpersonal emotion regulation. Our findings suggest that people intend to engage

in interpersonal emotion regulation less, and invest less effort in doing so, than research has

tacitly assumed up to this point. Further, they begin the work of mapping methodological overlap

between trait and momentary measures of interpersonal emotion regulation constructs. As a

whole, our work paints a nuanced picture of the emerging interpersonal emotion regulation

landscape, and introduces new research opportunities to discover how people can interact more

effectively with one another in day-to-day life.


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 37

References

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 50(2). https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Austin, E. J., & O’Donnell, M. M. (2013). Development and preliminary validation of a scale to

assess managing the emotions of others. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(7), 834–839.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.005

Barbee, A. P., Cunningham, M. R., Winstead, B. A., Derlega, V. J., Gulley, M. R., Yankeelov, P. A.,

& Druen, P. B. (1993). Effects of Gender Role Expectations on the Social Support Process.

Journal of Social Issues, 49(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1993.tb01175.x

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using

lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Battaglini, A. M., Rnic, K., Tracy, A., Jopling, E., & LeMoult, J. (2021). Co-rumination across in-

person and digital communication: Associations with affect and relationship closeness in

adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2021.04.011

Beckes, L., & Coan, J. A. (2011). Social Baseline Theory: The Role of Social Proximity in Emotion

and Economy of Action. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(12).

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00400.x

Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary Methods: Capturing Life as it is Lived. Annual

Review of Psychology, 54(1). https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030

Campbell, D. (2021, October 24). Is Melbourne really the world's most locked-down city? ABC

News. https://www.abc.net.au/news/2021-10-25/fact-check-is-melbourne-most-locked-down-

city/100560172
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 38

Debrot, A., Schoebi, D., Perrez, M., & Horn, A. B. (2013). Touch as an Interpersonal Emotion

Regulation Process in Couples’ Daily Lives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(10),

1373–1385. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213497592

Doré, B. P., Silvers, J. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2016). Toward a Personalized Science of Emotion

Regulation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(4).

https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12240

Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel

models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12(2). https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-

989X.12.2.121

English, T., Lee, I. A., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2017). Emotion regulation strategy selection in

daily life: The role of social context and goals. Motivation and Emotion, 41(2).

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9597-z

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power

3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior research methods, 41(4), 1149–1160.

https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

Feng, B. (2009). Testing an Integrated Model of Advice Giving in Supportive Interactions. Human

Communication Research, 35(1), 115–129. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2008.01340.x

Fife, D. (2020). The Eight Steps of Data Analysis: A Graphical Framework to Promote Sound

Statistical Analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4).

https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620917333

Geeraerts, J. (2020, May 20). Investigating careless responding detection techniques in experience

sampling methods. Retrieved from https://osf.io/d798j/


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 39

Gross, J. J. (2015). The Extended Process Model of Emotion Regulation: Elaborations, Applications,

and Future Directions. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1).

https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2015.989751

Gutentag, T., Kalokerinos, E. K., Garrett, P. M., Millgram, Y., Sobel, R., & Tamir, M. (under

review). Motivational Strength in Emotion Regulation.

Himmelstein, P. H., Woods, W. C., & Wright, A. G. C. (2019). A comparison of signal- and event-

contingent ambulatory assessment of interpersonal behavior and affect in social situations.

Psychological Assessment, 31(7). https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000718

Hofmann, S. G., Carpenter, J. K., & Curtiss, J. (2016). Interpersonal Emotion Regulation

Questionnaire (IERQ): Scale Development and Psychometric Characteristics. Cognitive Therapy

and Research, 40(3). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9756-2

Kalokerinos, E. K., Tamir, M., & Kuppens, P. (2017). Instrumental motives in negative emotion

regulation in daily life: Frequency, consistency, and predictors. Emotion, 17(4).

https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000269

Koval, P., Hinton, J., Dozo, N., Gleeson, J., Alvarez, M., Harrison, A., Vu, D., Susanto, R.,

Jayaputera, G., & Sinnott, R. (2019). SEMA3: Smartphone ecological momentary assessment,

Version 3. [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://www.sema3.com

Koval, P., Kalokerinos, E. K., Greenaway, K. H., Medland, H., Kuppens, P., Nezlek, J. B., … Gross,

J. (in press). Emotion Regulation in Everyday Life: Mapping Global Self-Reports to Daily

Processes. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cav54

Kuppens, P., Dejonckheere, E., Kalokerinos, E. K., & Koval, P. (2022, January 10). Some

recommendations on the use of daily life methods in affective science.

https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/y4aqh
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 40

Liu, D. Y., Strube, M. J., & Thompson, R. J. (2021). Interpersonal Emotion Regulation: an

Experience Sampling Study. Affective Science. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-021-00044-y

Lüdecke, D. (2018). sjPlot: Data Visualization for Statistics in Social Science. R package version

2.4.1.9000, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sjPlot

Marroquín, B. (2011). Interpersonal emotion regulation as a mechanism of social support in

depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(8), 1276–1290.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.005

Mauss, I. B., & Tamir, M. (2014). Emotion goals: How their content, structure, and operation shape

emotion regulation. In Handbook of emotion regulation, 2nd ed. (pp. 361–375). The Guilford

Press.

McMahon, T. P., & Naragon-Gainey, K. (2020). Ecological validity of trait emotion regulation

strategy measures. Psychological Assessment, 32(8). https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000827

Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Preliminary evidence for an

emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

43(10), 1281–1310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.008

Millgram, Y., Sheppes, G., Kalokerinos, E. K., Kuppens, P., & Tamir, M. (2019). Do the ends dictate

the means in emotion regulation? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(1). https://

doi.org/10.1037/xge0000477

Murayama, K., Usami, S., & Sakaki, M. (in press). Summary-statistics-based power analysis: A new

and practical method to determine sample size for mixed-effects modelling. Psychological

Methods. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/6cer3

Nguyen, M. H., Gruber, J., Fuchs, J., Marler, W., Hunsaker, A., & Hargittai, E. (2020). Changes in

Digital Communication During the COVID-19 Global Pandemic: Implications for Digital
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 41

Inequality and Future Research. Social Media + Society, 6(3).

https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305120948255

Niven, K. (2016). Why do people engage in interpersonal emotion regulation at work?

Organizational Psychology Review, 6(4). https://doi.org/10.1177/2041386615612544

Niven, K. (2017). The four key characteristics of interpersonal emotion regulation. Current Opinion

in Psychology, 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.015

Niven, K., Totterdell, P., Stride, C. B., & Holman, D. (2011). Emotion Regulation of Others and Self

(EROS): The Development and Validation of a New Individual Difference Measure. Current

Psychology, 30(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-011-9099-9

Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2015). Current Emotion Research in Social Psychology:

Thinking About Emotions and Other People. Emotion Review, 7(4).

https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915590624

Pauw, L. S., Sauter, D. A., van Kleef, G. A., & Fischer, A. H. (2018). Sense or sensibility? Social

sharers’ evaluations of socio-affective vs. cognitive support in response to negative emotions.

Cognition and Emotion, 32(6), 1247–1264. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1400949

Riediger, M., Schmiedek, F., Wagner, G. G., & Lindenberger, U. (2009). Seeking Pleasure and

Seeking Pain: Differences in Prohedonic and Contra-Hedonic Motivation From Adolescence to

Old Age. Psychological Science, 20(12). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02473.x

Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion Elicits the Social Sharing of Emotion: Theory and Empirical Review.

Emotion Review, 1(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073908097189

Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Episodic and semantic knowledge in emotional self-report:

Evidence for two judgment processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1),

198–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.198
INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 42

Sheppes, G. (2020). Transcending the “good & bad” and “here & now” in emotion regulation:

Costs and benefits of strategies across regulatory stages.

https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2019.09.003

Suri, G., Whittaker, K., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Launching reappraisal: It’s less common than you

might think. Emotion, 15(1). https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000011

Swerdlow, B. A., & Johnson, S. L. (2020). The Interpersonal Regulation Interaction Scale (IRIS): A

multistudy investigation of receivers’ retrospective evaluations of interpersonal emotion

regulation interactions. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000927

Tamir, M. (2016). Why Do People Regulate Their Emotions? A Taxonomy of Motives in Emotion

Regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(3).

https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868315586325

Tamir, M. (2021). Effortful Emotion Regulation as a Unique Form of Cybernetic Control.

Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620922199

Tamir, M., & Diener, E. (2008). Approach-avoidance goals and well-being: One size does not fit all.

In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 415–428).

Psychology Press.

Tamir, M., Vishkin, A., & Gutentag, T. (2020). Emotion regulation is motivated. Emotion, 20(1).

https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000635

Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and

happiness. In Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Yale University

Press.

Thomas, D. L., & Diener, E. (1990). Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 59(2). https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.291


INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION IN EVERYDAY LIFE 43

van Kleef, G. A. (2021). Comment: Moving (Further) Beyond Private Experience: On the

Radicalization of the Social Approach to Emotions and the Emancipation of Verbal Emotional

Expressions. Emotion Review, 13(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073921991231

Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the

effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological

Bulletin, 138(4), 775–808. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027600

Wilhelm, F. H., & Grossman, P. (2010). Emotions beyond the laboratory: Theoretical fundaments,

study design, and analytic strategies for advanced ambulatory assessment. Biological

Psychology, 84(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.017

Williams, W. C., Morelli, S. A., Ong, D. C., & Zaki, J. (2018). Interpersonal emotion regulation:

Implications for affiliation, perceived support, relationships, and well-being. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 115(2). https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000132

Zaki, J., & Williams, W. C. (2013). Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emotion, 13(5). https://doi.org/

10.1037/a0033839

You might also like