Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 48

Thalamus Modulates Consciousness

via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex


Michelle J. Redinbaugh & Jessica M.
Phillips & Niranjan A. Kambi & Sounak
Mohanta & Samantha Andryk &
Gaven L. Dooley & Mohsen Afrasiabi &
Aeyal Raz & Yuri B. Saalmann
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmass.com/product/thalamus-modulates-consciousness-via-layer-specific
-control-of-cortex-michelle-j-redinbaugh-jessica-m-phillips-niranjan-a-kambi-sounak-m
ohanta-samantha-andryk-gaven-l/
More products digital (pdf, epub, mobi) instant
download maybe you interests ...

Elsevier Weekblad - Week 26 - 2022 Gebruiker

https://ebookmass.com/product/elsevier-weekblad-
week-26-2022-gebruiker/

Jock Seeks Geek: The Holidates Series Book #26 Jill


Brashear

https://ebookmass.com/product/jock-seeks-geek-the-holidates-
series-book-26-jill-brashear/

The New York Review of Books – N. 09, May 26 2022


Various Authors

https://ebookmass.com/product/the-new-york-review-of-
books-n-09-may-26-2022-various-authors/

Calculate with Confidence, 8e (Oct 26,


2021)_(0323696953)_(Elsevier) 8th Edition Morris Rn
Bsn Ma Lnc

https://ebookmass.com/product/calculate-with-
confidence-8e-oct-26-2021_0323696953_elsevier-8th-edition-morris-
rn-bsn-ma-lnc/
Destination C1 & C2 : grammar & vocabulary with answer
key 18 [printing]. Edition Malcolm Mann

https://ebookmass.com/product/destination-c1-c2-grammar-
vocabulary-with-answer-key-18-printing-edition-malcolm-mann/

1 st International Congress and Exhibition on


Sustainability in Music, Art, Textile and Fashion
(ICESMATF 2023) January, 26-27 Madrid, Spain Exhibition
Book 1st Edition Tatiana Lissa
https://ebookmass.com/product/1-st-international-congress-and-
exhibition-on-sustainability-in-music-art-textile-and-fashion-
icesmatf-2023-january-26-27-madrid-spain-exhibition-book-1st-
edition-tatiana-lissa/

Drugs and Drug Policy: The Control of Consciousness


Alteration (NULL)

https://ebookmass.com/product/drugs-and-drug-policy-the-control-
of-consciousness-alteration-null/

The Roots of Normativity Raz

https://ebookmass.com/product/the-roots-of-normativity-raz/

Application of Control Volume Based Finite Element


Method (CVFEM) for Nanofluid Flow and Heat Transfer 1st
Edition Mohsen Sheikholeslami

https://ebookmass.com/product/application-of-control-volume-
based-finite-element-method-cvfem-for-nanofluid-flow-and-heat-
transfer-1st-edition-mohsen-sheikholeslami/
Report

Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-


Specific Control of Cortex
Highlights Authors
d Central lateral thalamic stimulation arouses macaques from Michelle J. Redinbaugh,
stable anesthesia Jessica M. Phillips,
Niranjan A. Kambi, ...,
d Thalamic and deep-layer cortical spiking correlate with Mohsen Afrasiabi, Aeyal Raz,
consciousness level Yuri B. Saalmann
d Consciousness depends on feedforward, feedback, and
intracolumnar signaling
Correspondence
mredinbaugh@wisc.edu (M.J.R.),
d Pathway-specific signaling operates at alpha and gamma saalmann@wisc.edu (Y.B.S.)
during consciousness
In Brief
Redinbaugh et al. demonstrate neural
correlates of consciousness, consistent
in wakefulness and stimulation-induced
arousal, which break down during
anesthesia and sleep. The authors
conclude that activity in central lateral
thalamus and cortical deep layers is vital
for consciousness, influencing
feedforward, feedback, and
intracolumnar processes.

Redinbaugh et al., 2020, Neuron 106, 1–10


April 8, 2020 ª 2020 Elsevier Inc.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005
Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

Neuron

Report

Thalamus Modulates Consciousness


via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex
Michelle J. Redinbaugh,1,* Jessica M. Phillips,1 Niranjan A. Kambi,1 Sounak Mohanta,1 Samantha Andryk,1
Gaven L. Dooley,1 Mohsen Afrasiabi,1 Aeyal Raz,2,3,5 and Yuri B. Saalmann1,4,5,6,*
1Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA
2Department of Anesthesiology, Rambam Health Care Campus, Haifa 3109601, Israel
3Ruth and Bruce Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion – Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa 3200003, Israel
4Wisconsin National Primate Research Center, Madison, WI 53715, USA
5These authors contributed equally
6Lead Contact

*Correspondence: mredinbaugh@wisc.edu (M.J.R.), saalmann@wisc.edu (Y.B.S.)


https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

SUMMARY feedforward and feedback pathways and intracolumnar


communication within a cortical area.
Functional MRI and electrophysiology studies sug- Feedforward pathways carry sensory information from super-
gest that consciousness depends on large-scale ficial layers to superficial and middle layers of higher-order
thalamocortical and corticocortical interactions. cortical areas, whereas feedback pathways carry priorities and
However, it is unclear how neurons in different predictions from deep layers to either superficial or deep layers
cortical layers and circuits contribute. We simulta- of lower-order cortical areas (Markov et al., 2014; Mejias et al.,
2016). Reports of the contribution of feedforward (Maksimow
neously recorded from central lateral thalamus
et al., 2014; Sanders et al., 2018; van Vugt et al., 2018) and
(CL) and across layers of the frontoparietal
feedback (Boly et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2009, 2013; Raz et al.,
cortex in awake, sleeping, and anesthetized 2014; Uhrig et al., 2018) pathways to consciousness have varied.
macaques. We found that neurons in thalamus However, previous studies did not have the spatial resolution
and deep cortical layers are most sensitive to to determine transmission along paths between specific
changes in consciousness level, consistent across layers—in particular, whether feedback paths to superficial or
different anesthetic agents and sleep. Deep-layer deep cortical layers, or both, contribute to consciousness.
activity is sustained by interactions with CL. Available evidence suggests that changes in the level of
Consciousness also depends on deep-layer neu- consciousness differentially influences activity in cortical
rons providing feedback to superficial layers layers. Non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep reduces spiking
(not to deep layers), suggesting that long-range activity in deep layers of the primary visual cortex (V1) in mice
and cats (Livingstone and Hubel, 1981; Senzai et al., 2019), as
feedback and intracolumnar signaling are impor-
well as interactions between the superficial and deep layers
tant. To show causality, we stimulated CL in
of V1 (Senzai et al., 2019). NREM sleep in mice (Funk et al.,
anesthetized macaques and effectively restored 2016) and isoflurane anesthesia in ferrets (Sellers et al., 2013)
arousal and wake-like neural processing. This also changes local field potentials (LFPs) differentially across
effect was location and frequency specific. Our layers. It is unclear how changes in consciousness influence
findings suggest layer-specific thalamocortical individual neurons across layers, and their interaction, in the
correlates of consciousness and inform how tar- higher-order cortex.
geted deep brain stimulation can alleviate disor- Reciprocally connected with the higher-order cortex, higher-
ders of consciousness. order thalamic areas facilitate cortical communication (Saal-
mann et al., 2012; Schmitt et al., 2017; Theyel et al., 2010) and
could thus play a role in modulating corticocortical interaction
INTRODUCTION across different conscious states. Changes in consciousness
level broadly influence thalamic activity and thalamocortical
Consciousness is the capacity to experience one’s environment interactions (Baker et al., 2014; Contreras et al., 1997; Jones,
and internal states. The minimal mechanisms that are sufficient 2009; Llinás et al., 1998). However, the central lateral thalamus
for this experience, the neural correlates of consciousness (CL) may have a special relation to consciousness. CL damage
(NCC), are much debated (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Fris- is linked to disorders of consciousness (Schiff, 2008). Anatomi-
ton, 2010; Lamme, 2006; Oizumi et al., 2014). Nonetheless, ma- cally, CL receives input from the brainstem reticular activating
jor theories of consciousness emphasize the importance of system. It also projects to superficial layers and reciprocally
recurrent activity and interaction between neurons. This can connects with deep layers of the frontoparietal cortex (Kaufman
take the form of communication between brain areas along and Rosenquist, 1985; Purpura and Schiff, 1997; Towns et al.,

Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020 ª 2020 Elsevier Inc. 1


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

1990). Thus, CL is well positioned to influence information flow to zero in on NCCs, identified here as activity differences be-
between cortical layers and areas. Deep brain stimulation of tween wakefulness and anesthesia, which are selectively
the central thalamus increased responsiveness in a minimally restored by effective (arousal score R 3, n = 55, mean = 4.70,
conscious patient (Schiff et al., 2007), and central thalamic SD = 1.70) relative to ineffective (arousal score < 3, n = 171,
stimulation improved the performance of a vigilance task in mean = 0.77, SD = 0.74) 50-Hz stimulations.
healthy macaques (Baker et al., 2016). Although thalamocortical
mechanisms of such effects have been proposed (Jones, 2009; Consciousness Level Modulated Spike Rate and Timing
Llinás et al., 1998; Purpura and Schiff, 1997), experimental in Deep Cortical Layers and CL
evidence is lacking. Based on its connectivity, we hypothesized We recorded 845 neurons across 3 brain areas (FEF, LIP, CL)
that CL influences feedforward, feedback, and intracolumnar during 4 states (wake, sleep, isoflurane, propofol; Figure 2).
cortical processes to regulate information flow, and thus, Wake and anesthesia data derived from separate sessions,
consciousness. whereas the same neurons yielded sleep and wake data. This
To resolve the contributions of corticocortical and thalamo- included a subset of CL neurons with a relatively high firing
cortical paths to the NCC, we used linear multi-electrode rate (40–50 Hz; Figures S2E and S2F), similar to neurons re-
arrays to record spikes and LFPs simultaneously from the ported in cats (Glenn and Steriade, 1982; Steriade et al., 1993).
right frontal eye field (FEF); the lateral intraparietal area (LIP; Thalamic neurons showed state-dependent spike rate and
frontoparietal areas implicated in awareness) (Wardak et al., bursting activity (Figures 2C and 2D). CL neurons recorded
2002, 2006); and interconnected CL in two macaques during during anesthesia (T = 4.67, n = 282, p = 3.0 3 105) and
task-free wakefulness, NREM sleep, and general anesthesia NREM sleep (F = 16.40, n = 83, p = 0.001) had significantly
(isoflurane, propofol). After characterizing thalamocortical lower spike rates than during wakefulness. Isoflurane and
network activity under anesthesia, we electrically stimulated propofol effects were not significantly different (Figure S2A).
the thalamus, reliably inducing arousal. We report layer-specific Relative to wakefulness, CL neurons also increased bursting
NCC in frontoparietal cortex and cortico-CL pathways. during anesthesia (Figures 2D and S2G; T = 2.27, n = 172, p =
0.024) and sleep (F = 7.11, n = 121, p = 0.0095).
RESULTS We localized cortical neurons to superficial, middle, or
deep layers using current source density (CSD) responses
Gamma-Frequency CL Stimulation Increased to sounds in the passive oddball paradigm (Figures 2A and
Consciousness Level 2B). Only deep neurons showed state-dependent activity
After maintaining stable anesthesia (arousal score 0–1) for at (Figures 2E–2H). Firing rates during sleep were significantly
least 2 h while recording neural activity, we evaluated the lower than wakefulness; the state-by-layer interaction was
signs of arousal before, during, and after stimulation using a significant in both FEF (F = 15.17, n = 101, p = 0.008) and LIP
customized scale similar to clinical metrics (STAR Methods) (F = 7.70, n = 98, p = 0.031). Similarly, firing rates during
and performed statistical analyses using general linear models anesthesia were lower than during wakefulness; state-by-layer
(STAR Methods; Tables S1–S4). Across 261 stimulation blocks, interactions in FEF (T = 3.05, n = 281, p = 0.013) and LIP (T =
thalamic stimulation significantly increased arousal relative to 3.79, n = 282, p = 0.001) were significant. Only deep neurons
pre- (F = 119.28, n = 261, p < 1.0 3 1010) and post-conditions increased bursting during anesthesia, evidenced by significant
(F = 124.64, n = 261, p < 1.0 3 1010), even when accounting state-by-layer interaction (Figure 2E; T = 2.12, n = 285, p =
for differences in dose and anesthetic (Figures 1A, 1B, and 0.035). Isoflurane and propofol yielded similar results (Figures
S1A–S1C). Behavioral changes (Figure 1A) were time locked to S2B–S2D). Effective 50-Hz thalamic stimulation countered
stimulation: monkeys opened their eyes with wake-like anesthesia effects in deep cortical layers of the LIP (Figures 2I–
occasional blinks, performed full reaches and withdrawals 2K); the 4-way interaction of stimulation epoch, effectiveness,
with forelimbs (ipsi- or contralateral), made facial and body layer, and area was significant (F = 5.19, n = 167, p = 0.023).
movements, showed increased reactivity (palpebral reflex, Overall, states with higher consciousness levels (stimulation-
toe-pinch withdrawal), and had altered vital signs (respiration induced arousal, wakefulness) showed increased deep cortical
rate, heart rate). The reconstruction of electrode tracks and thalamic activities, suggesting a role in the NCC.
placed effective stimulations (arousal score R 3) near the center
of CL (Figures 1C–1F). Euclidian proximity of the stimulation Intracolumnar Interactions Showed Layer-Specific NCC
array to CL significantly predicted changes in arousal (Figures To measure state-related changes in thalamocortical and
S1G–S1I; T = 3.39, n = 225, p = 0.00082); when systematically corticocortical communication, we calculated power and coher-
varying array depth, proximity to the CL center showed a ence using bipolar derivatized LFPs. We combined data
significant quadratic relation with arousal (T = 2.92, n = 225, across anesthetics, as effects were qualitatively similar (Figures
p = 0.00393; Figures 1F and S1D–S1F). Effective stimulation S2H–S2O and S4A–S4H). We focus here on intracolumnar
sites remained so on separate recording days and with different changes, particularly coherence within superficial layers,
anesthetics (Figure 1G). Stimulation effectiveness depended deep layers, and between superficial and deep layers of the
on frequency (Figures 1G and 1H). At effective sites, only same cortical area. Coherence changed markedly between
50-Hz stimulations reliably increased arousal (T = 3.91, n = 44, wakefulness and anesthesia; anesthesia increased delta coher-
p = 0.00035). These results show that CL stimulation can ence (<4 Hz) and reduced alpha (8–15 Hz) and low gamma
rouse animals from stable, anesthetized states. This allowed us coherence (30–60 Hz; Figures 3A and 3B; Table S1 for complete

2 Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

Figure 1. Gamma-Frequency CL Stimulation Increased Consciousness Level


(A) Example behavioral and neural recordings during 50-Hz stimulation (arousal score 5).
(B) Population mean arousal score (±SE) before, during, and after stimulations for both monkeys (circles show individual stimulation events).
(C) Coronal section of right hemisphere 8 mm anterior to interaural line (A8). The arrow shows the electrode.
(D) Magnified view of the thalamus.
(E) Stimulation sites in monkey R (n = 90) collapsed along the A-P axis. Circles represent the middle contact in the stimulation array; diameter scales with induced
arousal.
(F) Stimulation-induced arousal change (score during stim–pre) as a function of the dorsal-ventral distance from CL center. Symbols show stimulation events by
monkey; the red curve shows the quadratic fit (±SE).
(G) Example of stimulation series for different frequencies during propofol (left) and isoflurane (right) at the same site 22.5 mm ventral to the cortical surface.
(H) Population mean arousal change (±SE of point estimate) for different stimulation frequencies at effective and ineffective sites from both monkeys.

statistics). Wake-anesthesia differences were consistent be- and 3F; Table S1), suggesting altered processing in cortical
tween different layers of FEF and LIP (Figures 3C–3H) and microcircuits.
qualitatively similar to differences between wakefulness and Effective 50-Hz thalamic stimulation increased intracolumnar
NREM sleep (Figure S2). Notably, coherence between superficial coherence differentially across frequency bands and layers
and deep layers of both cortical areas showed substantial (Figures 3K–3P; Table S1 for complete statistics). Coherence
decreases in all higher-frequency (>4 Hz) communication during within superficial layers was increased for effective compared
anesthesia (T R 10.05, n = 8,725, p < 1.0 3 1010; Figures 3E to ineffective stimulations at low gamma (Figures 3K and 3L),

Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020 3


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

Figure 2. Consciousness Level Modulated Spike Rate and Timing in Deep Cortical Layers and CL
(A) Example of sound-aligned evoked potentials from linear multi-electrode array in FEF. Tone onset at 0 s. Bold lines show inverse CSD (iCSD)-defined middle layers.
(B) Sound-aligned iCSD corresponding to (A).
(C and D) Population CL spike rate (±SE) (C) and CL burst index (±SE) (D) for anesthesia (blue), sleep (teal), and wake (orange) states; *p < 0.05.
(E) Cortical (FEF and LIP) burst index (±SE) in superficial (S), middle (M), and deep (D) layers for wakefulness (orange) and anesthesia (blue).
(F–H) Superficial (F), middle (G), and deep (H) layer spike rates (±SE) in FEF and LIP across states.
(I–K) Superficial (I), middle (J), and deep (K) spike rates (±SE) in FEF and LIP during effective stimulation (SE, red), ineffective stimulation (SI, gray), and pre-
stimulation (P, blue).

showing a significant interaction between stimulation and coherence increased at delta and decreased at all higher fre-
effectiveness (T = 5.24, n = 2,387, p = 1.8 3 106). Within deep quencies across putative feedforward and feedback pathways
layers (Figures 3O and 3P), similar interactions show that effec- (|T| R 6.04, N R 4,030, p % 1.6 3109; Table S3 for complete
tive stimulations selectively increased coherence at theta (4– statistics). We found qualitatively similar but smaller effects
8 Hz, T = 9.04, n = 2,183, p < 1.0 3 1010) and alpha (T = during NREM sleep (Figures S4C, S4F, and S4I) and for spike-
11.79, n = 2,183, p < 1.0 3 1010) frequencies. Important for in- field coherence (Figures S4J–S4L; Table S3 for complete statis-
tracolumnar processing, superficial and deep layers showed tics). Coherence between thalamus and either superficial or
broadband coherence increases selective for effective stimula- deep cortical layers decreased across all frequency bands dur-
tions at the same frequencies hindered by general anesthesia ing anesthesia (Figures 4I–4L; Table S4 for complete statistics).
(bands > 4 Hz; T R 4.83, n = 2,631, p % 1.3 3 106; see Table Thalamocortical spike-field coherence showed similar effects
S1). Note that power changes during thalamic stimulation did (Figures S4M–S4R; Table S4 for complete statistics). These
not correlate with coherence changes (Figures S3K–S3P; Table results show that anesthesia decreases broadband thalamo-
S2 for complete statistics), nor did power changes reflect behav- cortical and corticocortical coherence.
ioral arousal during stimulation. Thalamic stimulation isolated specific interactions between
cortical areas important for consciousness (Figure 4E). Effec-
Thalamocortical and Corticocortical Interactions tive stimulations resulted in targeted restoration of frontoparie-
Showed Pathway-Specific NCC tal coherence in putative feedforward and feedback pathways
We next focus on anatomically motivated interactions across (Figures 4F–4H). Coherence between superficial layers of
the frontoparietal cortex. We measured coherence between LIP and superficial and middle FEF substantially reduced at
the origin and termination of putative feedforward (superficial delta (T = 4.05, n = 2,799, p = 8.6 3 104), and increased
LIP-superficial and middle FEF) and two feedback (deep FEF-su- at alpha (T = 6.87, n = 2,799, p = 1.46 3 1010), low gamma
perficial LIP or deep FEF-deep LIP) pathways (Markov et al., (T = 4.45, n = 2,799, p = 1.50 3 104), and high gamma
2014; Mejias et al., 2016). We also examined state-dependent (60–90 Hz; T = 3.03, n = 2,799, p = 0.027), for effective more
effects on thalamocortical coherence (CL-superficial or CL- than ineffective stimulations, as shown by significant interac-
deep cortical layers) (Molinari et al., 1994; Towns et al., 1990). tions (Figure 4F). There was also a significant interaction for
Communication between cortical areas showed significant coherence between deep layers of FEF and superficial LIP at
changes during general anesthesia (Figure 4A). Corticocortical alpha (Figure 4G; T = 3.97, n = 1,617, p = 0.001), showing

4 Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

Figure 3. Intracolumnar Interactions Show Layer-Specific NCC


(A and B) Population FEF (A) and LIP (B) coherence with 95% confidence intervals for wakefulness and anesthesia. Average of all contact pairs across layers. The
gray lines show significant differences between spectra (Holm’s corrected t tests).
(C–H) Population coherence difference between wakefulness and anesthesia. Positive when wake > anesthesia. The error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals
of t tests at each frequency. The gray shading shows effects consistent between state (wake versus anesthesia) and thalamic stimulation (effective versus
ineffective in K–P) results. Average of all contact pairs for and between superficial (C) FEF and (D) LIP; superficial and deep (E) FEF and (F) LIP; and deep (G) FEF
and (H) LIP.
(I and J) Population FEF (I) and LIP (J) coherence with 95% confidence intervals under anesthesia before and during effective stimulation. Average across all
layers.
(K–P) Population coherence difference (stim–pre) with 95% confidence intervals for effective and ineffective stimulations. Positive when stim > pre. Average of all
contact pairs for and between superficial (K) FEF and (L) LIP; superficial and deep (M) FEF and (N) LIP; and deep (O) FEF and (P) LIP.

substantial increases in alpha coherence specific to effective ical circuit dynamics contribute to the NCC (Figure 4M).
stimulations. While coherence between FEF and LIP deep We link consciousness to increased spiking activity in
cortical layers did generally increase with stimulation, no inter- deep cortical layers and CL, which is consistent with cat and
actions were significant (Figure 4H; Table S3 for complete sta- rodent studies of V1 (Livingstone and Hubel, 1981; Senzai
tistics). Overall, more-conscious states showed increased et al., 2019) and CL (Glenn and Steriade, 1982) comparing
alpha and gamma coherence in feedforward pathways as wakefulness and NREM sleep. This spiking activity is likely
well as alpha coherence in the feedback pathways originating sustained through reciprocal deep-layer cortex-CL connec-
in deep layers and terminating in superficial layers of the tions, because reduced deep cortical layer and CL spiking
lower-order area. coincided with reduced functional connectivity between
CL and cortex, and CL stimulation increased deep cortical
DISCUSSION layer spiking.
The deep cortical layers are anatomically positioned to
Circuit-Level Mechanism for Consciousness and drive feedback to superficial layers in lower-order areas, and
Anesthesia to influence feedforward pathways via interactions with super-
Our results suggest that specific feedforward and feedback ficial layers in the same cortical column. CL, with projections
corticocortical path as well as intracolumnar and thalamocort- both to superficial and deep cortical layers, can modulate

Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020 5


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

Figure 4. Thalamocortical and Corticocortical Interactions Show Pathway-Specific NCC


(A) Population coherence with 95% confidence intervals for all paired contacts between FEF and LIP during wakefulness and anesthesia. The gray lines show
significant differences between spectra (Holm’s corrected t tests).
(B–D) Population average coherence difference: wake–anesthesia. The error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals of t tests at each frequency. The gray
shading shows effects consistent between state and thalamic stimulation (in F–H) results. (B) Superficial LIP and superficial and middle FEF; (C) deep FEF and
superficial LIP; and (D) deep FEF and deep LIP.
(E) Population coherence between FEF and LIP with 95% confidence intervals for all paired contacts under anesthesia, before and during effective stimulation.
(F–H) Population average coherence difference, stim–pre, with 95% confidence intervals for effective and ineffective stimulations. (F) Superficial LIP and su-
perficial and middle FEF; (G) deep FEF and superficial LIP; and (H) deep FEF and deep LIP.
(I and J) Population thalamocortical coherence with 95% confidence intervals for wake and anesthesia across all paired CL-FEF (I) and CL-LIP (J) contacts.
(K and L) Population average thalamocortical coherence difference, wake–anesthesia, with 95% confidence intervals of t tests. CL-superficial and CL-deep
layers for (K) FEF and (L) LIP.
(M) Schematic showing pathways and predominant frequencies contributing to NCC. Yellow shading where spiking changes with consciousness level. FF,
feedforward; FB, feedback; TC, thalamocortical.

intracolumnar and cross-area interactions (Purpura and Schiff, common target of anesthetic drugs, particularly those with
1997). These interactions operated at alpha and gamma fre- actions on the GABA-A receptor.
quencies during consciousness, whereas general anesthesia
and sleep reduced activity in deep cortical layers and CL, Specificity of Effective Thalamic Stimulation Points to
thus reducing alpha and gamma frequency communication Minimal and Sufficient Mechanisms for Consciousness
within and between cortical areas. Reactivation of this CL- Our results suggest that CL has a special role to play in con-
deep cortical layer loop with gamma-frequency stimulation sciousness, as stimulations were most effective when
reinstated wake-like cortical dynamics and increased the con- centered on CL as opposed to neighboring thalamic areas,
sciousness level, overcoming two separate anesthetics with such as the mediodorsal (MD) and centromedian (CM) nuclei.
different molecular targets. Overall, our study provides Thus, the unique properties of CL have important implications
empirical evidence for a circuit-level mechanism of conscious- for consciousness. CL projects to superficial and deep layers
ness with special emphasis on the reciprocal interaction of the frontal and the parietal cortex (Kaufman and Rosen-
between CL and deep cortical layers, which may serve as a quist, 1985; Purpura and Schiff, 1997; Towns et al., 1990).

6 Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

This projection pattern differs substantially from the MD, which Long-Range Frontoparietal Interaction Contributes to
largely projects to the prefrontal cortex and has different Consciousness, Not Just Report
laminar distribution (Erickson and Lewis, 2004; Giguere and Frontal cortical activity, in studies of consciousness requiring
Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Ray and Price, 1993). While cholinergic behavioral reports, may reflect the report rather than the level
stimulation of frontal cortex has proven sufficient for behav- of consciousness (Tsuchiya et al., 2015). Our study did not
ioral arousal in rodents (Pal et al., 2018), prefrontal stimulation require report (although it cannot be ruled out completely that
via the MD proved less effective in our macaques. This sug- observed activity reflects an underlying process related to re-
gests that prefrontal activation alone, via the MD, is insuffi- porting). Furthermore, to rule out sensorimotor differences
cient for consciousness. CL also has a prominent projection between different states of consciousness, we performed re-
to the striatum, which could contribute to consciousness. cordings in a quiet, dark room and only analyzed data from
However, the CM, with a stronger projection to the striatum epochs when the monkey’s eyes were stable, thereby control-
but limited cortical projections (Smith et al., 2004), was less ling for sensory stimulation and eye movements. Under these
effective than CL stimulation at inducing consciousness. This conditions, we found frontoparietal interaction featured during
suggests that activation of the direct thalamo-striatal path is wakefulness and stimulation-induced arousal, appearing
insufficient for consciousness. Our results further support rapidly after the onset of thalamic stimulation and generally
coordinated activity across the CL and the frontoparietal cor- dissipating just as quickly after stimulation ended in anesthe-
tex as NCC. tized monkeys. This suggests that frontoparietal communica-
tion may not simply be related to report, but rather contributes
Validating NCC with Multiple Conscious and to the conscious experience. Because feedback projections
Unconscious States to distant cortical areas preferentially terminate in superficial
Changes in consciousness coincide with state changes across layers (whereas feedback to adjacent areas preferentially
sleep and wake cycles, traumatic brain injury, or exposure to a terminate in deep layers) (Markov et al., 2014), our finding that
wide range of anesthetic agents with different molecular tar- feedback to superficial layers correlated with the level of con-
gets. As such, it can be difficult to distinguish NCC from neural sciousness may suggest that long-range feedback projections
effects specific to one of these states, such as effects of atten- are vital for consciousness.
tion, trauma, or a particular pharmacological agent. In this
study, we show NCC in a thalamocortical system that are (1) Implications for Clinical Interventions in Disorders of
similarly perturbed in natural (sleep) and two induced states of Consciousness
unconsciousness with different pharmacological agents (iso- Interventions using clinical deep brain stimulation (DBS) elec-
flurane or propofol), and (2) validated in two states of con- trodes in humans (Schiff et al., 2007) and monkeys (Baker
sciousness, normal wakefulness, and stimulation-induced et al., 2016) and bipolar stimulating electrodes in rodents
arousal during continuous anesthetic administration (Figures 3 (Mair and Hembrook, 2008; Shirvalkar et al., 2006) have shown
and 4, gray shading). This shows that our results are neither arousal modulation with stimulation frequencies between 100
drug specific nor purely reflecting an unnatural state of con- and 200 Hz. In comparison, using a linear multi-electrode
sciousness. Rather, our results show consistent NCC and point array with a small electrode contact size, we found that CL stim-
to a common circuit-level target of general anesthetics. ulation most effectively induced arousal at 50 Hz (cf. with 2, 10,
and 200 Hz). CL is an elongated nucleus along the dorsal-
Beyond Delta: Path-Specific Indicators of Level of ventral axis, 3–4 mm in extent in macaques. By using linear
Consciousness multi-electrode arrays (simultaneously stimulating 16 contacts,
Since the early studies of consciousness, electroencephalo- with 200-mm inter-contact spacing), we were able to stimulate
gram (EEG) delta activity has been considered critical for sleep across the dorsal-ventral extent of CL at small, regularly spaced
staging and monitoring depth of anesthesia. Our results intervals in a relatively precise way. This configuration led to the
show that delta activity, while prevalent in unconscious states greatest behavioral effects (Figures 1F and S1D–S1F), demon-
(isoflurane, propofol, and sleep), differs between the frontal strating clear spatial specificity. While our study did not test the
and the parietal cortex, as well as across cortical layers, and full range of clinically relevant stimulation frequencies, it is inter-
is not itself a predictor of behavioral arousal (Figures S3A– esting to note that 50-Hz stimulation in our study, and the more
S3H). Furthermore, we found that while delta coherence spatially refined 40- and 100-Hz optogenetic stimulation in a
was anticorrelated with consciousness in feedforward path- rat sleep study (Liu et al., 2015), modulated arousal. These
ways, possibly demonstrating a mechanism for disconnection, frequencies match the activity patterns of the subset of CL
there was no such relation for feedback pathways (Figures 4F– neurons with a high firing rate during wakefulness, reported in
4H). If delta oscillations do facilitate corticocortical discon- Figures S2E and S2F and previous work (Glenn and Steriade,
nection, our findings suggest that they do so in a pathway- 1982; Steriade et al., 1993). Mimicking the wakeful firing
specific manner, in which feedforward processes are readily rate of these neurons during anesthetic administration may
disrupted by the increased prevalence of delta activity relative partially explain the increased efficacy of gamma stimulation
to feedback. Thus, delta oscillations may play a larger role in our data. In light of these results, it may be possible to opti-
in disconnecting the brain from the external sensory environ- mize clinical DBS to better reflect the desired neural dynamics
ment than in preventing internal and top-down generated of affected thalamocortical circuits to help alleviate disorders
experiences. of consciousness.

Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020 7


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

STAR+METHODS REFERENCES

Baker, R., Gent, T.C., Yang, Q., Parker, S., Vyssotski, A.L., Wisden, W.,
Detailed methods are provided in the online version of this paper
Brickley, S.G., and Franks, N.P. (2014). Altered activity in the central medial
and include the following: thalamus precedes changes in the neocortex during transitions into both sleep
and propofol anesthesia. J. Neurosci. 34, 13326–13335.
d KEY RESOURCES TABLE
Baker, J.L., Ryou, J.W., Wei, X.F., Butson, C.R., Schiff, N.D., and Purpura, K.P.
d LEAD CONTACT AND MATERIALS AVAILABILITY
(2016). Robust modulation of arousal regulation, performance, and frontostria-
d EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS tal activity through central thalamic deep brain stimulation in healthy
d METHOD DETAILS nonhuman primates. J. Neurophysiol. 116, 2383–2404.
B Neuroimaging Bokil, H., Purpura, K., Schoffelen, J.M., Thomson, D., and Mitra, P. (2007).
B Surgery Comparing spectra and coherences for groups of unequal size. J. Neurosci.
B Behavioral tasks and sensory stimuli Methods 159, 337–345.
B Arousal scoring Bokil, H., Andrews, P., Kulkarni, J.E., Mehta, S., and Mitra, P.P. (2010).
B Electrophysiological recording and stimulation Chronux: a platform for analyzing neural signals. J. Neurosci. Methods 192,
B Electrode array localization 146–151.
B Anesthesia experiments Bollimunta, A., Chen, Y., Schroeder, C.E., and Ding, M. (2008). Neuronal
B Awake experiments mechanisms of cortical alpha oscillations in awake-behaving macaques.
J. Neurosci. 28, 9976–9988.
B Sleep
B Neural data preprocessing Boly, M., Garrido, M.I., Gosseries, O., Bruno, M.A., Boveroux, P., Schnakers,
C., Massimini, M., Litvak, V., Laureys, S., and Friston, K. (2011). Preserved
B Spike rate
feedforward but impaired top-down processes in the vegetative state.
B Spike timing
Science 332, 858–862.
B Current source density (CSD)
Bruce, C.J., Goldberg, M.E., Bushnell, M.C., and Stanton, G.B. (1985). Primate
B Power frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically
B Coherence evoked eye movements. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 714–734.
d QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS Caruso, V.C., Pages, D.S., Sommer, M.A., and Groh, J.M. (2016). Similar prev-
B General approach alence and magnitude of auditory-evoked and visually evoked activity in the
B Stimulation effects frontal eye fields: implications for multisensory motor control.
B Spike rate effects J. Neurophysiol. 115, 3162–3173.
B Bursting effects Chen, X., Zirnsak, M., and Moore, T. (2018). Dissonant Representations of
B Power and coherence effects Visual Space in Prefrontal Cortex during Eye Movements. Cell Rep. 22,
2039–2052.
d DATA AND CODE AVAILABILITY
Cohen, Y.E., Russ, B.E., and Gifford, G.W., 3rd (2005). Auditory processing in
the posterior parietal cortex. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 4, 218–231.
SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION Contreras, D., Destexhe, A., Sejnowski, T.J., and Steriade, M. (1997).
Spatiotemporal patterns of spindle oscillations in cortex and thalamus.
Supplemental Information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j. J. Neurosci. 17, 1179–1196.
neuron.2020.01.005.
Dehaene, S., and Changeux, J.P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical ap-
proaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70, 200–227.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Erez, Y., Tischler, H., Moran, A., and Bar-Gad, I. (2010). Generalized frame-
work for stimulus artifact removal. J. Neurosci. Methods 191, 45–59.
We thank M.I. Banks, C.W. Berridge, R.A. Pearce, R.D. Sanders, B.M. Krause, Erickson, S.L., and Lewis, D.A. (2004). Cortical connections of the lateral me-
and C. Murphy for useful comments on the manuscript. This work was sup- diodorsal thalamus in cynomolgus monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 473, 107–127.
ported by NIH grants R01MH110311 and P51OD011106, BSF grant 201732,
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat. Rev.
and a WNPRC pilot grant.
Neurosci. 11, 127–138.
Funk, C.M., Honjoh, S., Rodriguez, A.V., Cirelli, C., and Tononi, G. (2016). Local
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS Slow Waves in Superficial Layers of Primary Cortical Areas during REM Sleep.
Curr. Biol. 26, 396–403.
M.J.R., J.M.P., N.A.K., S.M., A.R., and Y.B.S. performed the research; M.J.R.,
Gifford, G.W., 3rd, and Cohen, Y.E. (2005). Spatial and non-spatial auditory
S.A., G.L.D., M.A., and Y.B.S. analyzed the data; M.J.R., A.R., and Y.B.S.
processing in the lateral intraparietal area. Exp. Brain Res. 162, 509–512.
wrote the paper; M.J.R., J.M.P., N.A.K., S.M., S.A., G.L.D., M.A., A.R., and
Y.B.S. edited the paper. Giguere, M., and Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1988). Mediodorsal nucleus: areal,
laminar, and tangential distribution of afferents and efferents in the frontal
lobe of rhesus monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 277, 195–213.
DECLARATION OF INTERESTS Glenn, L.L., and Steriade, M. (1982). Discharge rate and excitability of cortically
projecting intralaminar thalamic neurons during waking and sleep states.
A.R. is a consultant for and receives funding from Medtronic. The other authors
J. Neurosci. 2, 1387–1404.
declare no competing interests.
Grunewald, A., Linden, J.F., and Andersen, R.A. (1999). Responses to auditory
Received: November 19, 2019 stimuli in macaque lateral intraparietal area. I. Effects of training.
Revised: December 26, 2019 J. Neurophysiol. 82, 330–342.
Accepted: January 7, 2020 Haegens, S., Barczak, A., Musacchia, G., Lipton, M.L., Mehta, A.D., Lakatos,
Published: February 12, 2020 P., and Schroeder, C.E. (2015). Laminar Profile and Physiology of the a Rhythm

8 Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

in Primary Visual, Auditory, and Somatosensory Regions of Neocortex. Pettersen, K.H., Devor, A., Ulbert, I., Dale, A.M., and Einevoll, G.T. (2006).
J. Neurosci. 35, 14341–14352. Current-source density estimation based on inversion of electrostatic forward
Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E., Woolrich, M.W., and Smith, solution: effects of finite extent of neuronal activity and conductivity disconti-
S.M. (2012). Fsl. Neuroimage 62, 782–790. nuities. J. Neurosci. Methods 154, 116–133.

Jones, E.G. (2009). Synchrony in the interconnected circuitry of the thalamus Pigarev, I.N., Saalmann, Y.B., and Vidyasagar, T.R. (2009). A minimally inva-
and cerebral cortex. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1157, 10–23. sive and reversible system for chronic recordings from multiple brain sites in
macaque monkeys. J. Neurosci. Methods 181, 151–158.
Katzner, S., Nauhaus, I., Benucci, A., Bonin, V., Ringach, D.L., and Carandini,
M. (2009). Local origin of field potentials in visual cortex. Neuron 61, 35–41. Purpura, K., and Schiff, N.D. (1997). The Thalamic Intralaminar Nuclei: A Role in
Visual Awareness. Neuroscientist 3, 8.
Kaufman, E.F., and Rosenquist, A.C. (1985). Efferent projections of the
thalamic intralaminar nuclei in the cat. Brain Res. 335, 257–279. Ray, J.P., and Price, J.L. (1993). The organization of projections from the me-
diodorsal nucleus of the thalamus to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in ma-
Lacey, C.J., Bolam, J.P., and Magill, P.J. (2007). Novel and distinct operational caque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 337, 1–31.
principles of intralaminar thalamic neurons and their striatal projections.
Raz, A., Grady, S.M., Krause, B.M., Uhlrich, D.J., Manning, K.A., and Banks,
J. Neurosci. 27, 4374–4384.
M.I. (2014). Preferential effect of isoflurane on top-down vs. bottom-up path-
Lamme, V.A. (2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends ways in sensory cortex. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 191.
Cogn. Sci. 10, 494–501.
Romanski, L.M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., and
Lee, U., Kim, S., Noh, G.J., Choi, B.M., Hwang, E., and Mashour, G.A. (2009). Rauschecker, J.P. (1999). Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple
The directionality and functional organization of frontoparietal connectivity domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 1131–1136.
during consciousness and anesthesia in humans. Conscious. Cogn. 18,
Saalmann, Y.B., Pigarev, I.N., and Vidyasagar, T.R. (2007). Neural mecha-
1069–1078.
nisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant loca-
Lee, U., Ku, S., Noh, G., Baek, S., Choi, B., and Mashour, G.A. (2013). tions. Science 316, 1612–1615.
Disruption of frontal-parietal communication by ketamine, propofol, and sevo-
Saalmann, Y.B., Pinsk, M.A., Wang, L., Li, X., and Kastner, S. (2012). The pul-
flurane. Anesthesiology 118, 1264–1275.
vinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on
Linden, J.F., Grunewald, A., and Andersen, R.A. (1999). Responses to auditory attention demands. Science 337, 753–756.
stimuli in macaque lateral intraparietal area. II. Behavioral modulation.
Saleem, K.S., and Logothetis, N.K. (2007). A Combined MRI and Histology
J. Neurophysiol. 82, 343–358.
Atlas of the Rhesus Monkey Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates
Liu, J., Lee, H.J., Weitz, A.J., Fang, Z., Lin, P., Choy, M., Fisher, R., Pinskiy, V., (Academic Press).
Tolpygo, A., Mitra, P., et al. (2015). Frequency-selective control of cortical and
Sanders, R.D., Banks, M.I., Darracq, M., Moran, R., Sleigh, J., Gosseries, O.,
subcortical networks by central thalamus. eLife 4, e09215.
Bonhomme, V., Brichant, J.F., Rosanova, M., Raz, A., et al. (2018). Propofol-
Livingstone, M.S., and Hubel, D.H. (1981). Effects of sleep and arousal on the induced unresponsiveness is associated with impaired feedforward connec-
processing of visual information in the cat. Nature 291, 554–561. tivity in cortical hierarchy. Br. J. Anaesth. 121, 1084–1096.
Llinás, R., Ribary, U., Contreras, D., and Pedroarena, C. (1998). The neuronal Schall, J.D., Morel, A., King, D.J., and Bullier, J. (1995). Topography of visual
basis for consciousness. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 353, cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segre-
1841–1849. gation of processing streams. J. Neurosci. 15, 4464–4487.
Maimon, G., and Assad, J.A. (2009). Beyond Poisson: increased spike-time Schiff, N.D. (2008). Central thalamic contributions to arousal regulation and
regularity across primate parietal cortex. Neuron 62, 426–440. neurological disorders of consciousness. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1129, 105–118.
Mair, R.G., and Hembrook, J.R. (2008). Memory enhancement with event- Schiff, N.D., Giacino, J.T., Kalmar, K., Victor, J.D., Baker, K., Gerber, M., Fritz,
related stimulation of the rostral intralaminar thalamic nuclei. J. Neurosci. 28, B., Eisenberg, B., Biondi, T., O’Connor, J., et al. (2007). Behavioural improve-
14293–14300. ments with thalamic stimulation after severe traumatic brain injury. Nature 448,
Maksimow, A., Silfverhuth, M., Långsjö, J., Kaskinoro, K., Georgiadis, S., 600–603.
Ja€a
€skela
€inen, S., and Scheinin, H. (2014). Directional connectivity between Schmitt, L.I., Wimmer, R.D., Nakajima, M., Happ, M., Mofakham, S., and
frontal and posterior brain regions is altered with increasing concentrations Halassa, M.M. (2017). Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains
of propofol. PLoS One 9, e113616. attentional control. Nature 545, 219–223.
Markov, N.T., Vezoli, J., Chameau, P., Falchier, A., Quilodran, R., Huissoud, C., Schroeder, C.E., Mehta, A.D., and Givre, S.J. (1998). A spatiotemporal profile
Lamy, C., Misery, P., Giroud, P., Ullman, S., et al. (2014). Anatomy of hierarchy: of visual system activation revealed by current source density analysis in the
feedforward and feedback pathways in macaque visual cortex. J. Comp. awake macaque. Cereb. Cortex 8, 575–592.
Neurol. 522, 225–259.
Sellers, K.K., Bennett, D.V., Hutt, A., and Fröhlich, F. (2013). Anesthesia differ-
Mejias, J.F., Murray, J.D., Kennedy, H., and Wang, X.J. (2016). Feedforward entially modulates spontaneous network dynamics by cortical area and layer.
and feedback frequency-dependent interactions in a large-scale laminar J. Neurophysiol. 110, 2739–2751.
network of the primate cortex. Sci. Adv. 2, e1601335. Senzai, Y., Fernandez-Ruiz, A., and Buzsaki, G. (2019). Layer-Specific
Mitra, P., and Bokil, H. (2007). Observed Brain Dynamics (Oxford Physiological Features and Interlaminar Interactions in the Primary Visual
University Press). Cortex of the Mouse. Neuron 101, 500–513.e5.
Molinari, M., Leggio, M.G., Dell’Anna, M.E., Giannetti, S., and Macchi, G. Shirvalkar, P., Seth, M., Schiff, N.D., and Herrera, D.G. (2006). Cognitive
(1994). Chemical compartmentation and relationships between calcium-bind- enhancement with central thalamic electrical stimulation. Proc. Natl. Acad.
ing protein immunoreactivity and layer-specific cortical caudate-projecting Sci. USA 103, 17007–17012.
cells in the anterior intralaminar nuclei of the cat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 6, 299–312. Smith, Y., Raju, D.V., Pare, J.F., and Sidibe, M. (2004). The thalamostriatal sys-
Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L., and Tononi, G. (2014). From the phenomenology to tem: a highly specific network of the basal ganglia circuitry. Trends Neurosci.
the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0. PLoS 27, 520–527.
Comput. Biol. 10, e1003588. Steriade, M., Curró Dossi, R., and Contreras, D. (1993). Electrophysiological
Pal, D., Dean, J.G., Liu, T., Li, D., Watson, C.J., Hudetz, A.G., and Mashour, properties of intralaminar thalamocortical cells discharging rhythmic (approx-
G.A. (2018). Differential Role of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices in Controlling imately 40 HZ) spike-bursts at approximately 1000 HZ during waking and rapid
Level of Consciousness. Curr. Biol. 28, 2145–2152.e5. eye movement sleep. Neuroscience 56, 1–9.

Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020 9


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

Theyel, B.B., Llano, D.A., and Sherman, S.M. (2010). The corticothalamo- Uhrig, L., Sitt, J.D., Jacob, A., Tasserie, J., Barttfeld, P., Dupont, M., Dehaene,
cortical circuit drives higher-order cortex in the mouse. Nat. Neurosci. S., and Jarraya, B. (2018). Resting-state Dynamics as a Cortical Signature of
13, 84–88. Anesthesia in Monkeys. Anesthesiology 129, 942–958.
van Vugt, B., Dagnino, B., Vartak, D., Safaai, H., Panzeri, S., Dehaene, S., and
Towns, L.C., Tigges, J., and Tigges, M. (1990). Termination of thalamic intra-
Roelfsema, P.R. (2018). The threshold for conscious report: signal loss and
laminar nuclei afferents in visual cortex of squirrel monkey. Vis. Neurosci. 5,
response bias in visual and frontal cortex. Science 360, 537–542.
151–154.
Wardak, C., Olivier, E., and Duhamel, J.R. (2002). Saccadic target selection
Trongnetrpunya, A., Nandi, B., Kang, D., Kocsis, B., Schroeder, C.E., and deficits after lateral intraparietal area inactivation in monkeys. J. Neurosci.
Ding, M. (2016). Assessing Granger Causality in Electrophysiological Data: 22, 9877–9884.
Removing the Adverse Effects of Common Signals via Bipolar Derivations.
Wardak, C., Ibos, G., Duhamel, J.R., and Olivier, E. (2006). Contribution of the
Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9, 189.
monkey frontal eye field to covert visual attention. J. Neurosci. 26, 4228–4235.
€ssle, S., and Lamme, V.A.F. (2015). No-Report
Tsuchiya, N., Wilke, M., Fra Womelsdorf, T., Ardid, S., Everling, S., and Valiante, T.A. (2014). Burst firing
Paradigms: Extracting the True Neural Correlates of Consciousness. Trends synchronizes prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex during attentional con-
Cogn. Sci. 19, 757–770. trol. Curr. Biol. 24, 2613–2621.

10 Neuron 106, 1–10, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

STAR+METHODS

KEY RESOURCES TABLE

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER


Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins
Isoflurane, USP Piramal Enterprises Limited NDC 66794-013-25
PropoFlo 28 (Propofol) Zoetis Inc. NDC 54771-4944-1
KETAVED (KETAMINE HCL 100MG) Vedco Inc. NDC 50989-996-06
Experimental Models: Organisms/Strains
Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Wisconsin National Primate https://www.primate.wisc.edu/
Research Center (WNPRC)
Software and Algorithms
OmniPlex Server v 1.14.1 Plexon https://plexon.com/products/omniplex-software/
PlexContron v 1.14.1 Plexon https://plexon.com/products/omniplex-software/
Plexon Stim-2 v 2.3.0.0 Plexon https://plexon.com/products/plexstim-electrical-
stimulator-2-system/
Presentation v 19.0 Neurobehavioral Systems https://www.neurobs.com/
Logitech Webcam Software v 2.51 Logitech https://support.logi.com/hc/en-us/articles/
360024691274–Downloads-Webcam-C260
Fsl v 1.6 Oxford University Innovation https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsldownloads_registration
Fsleyes v 0.22.6 Oxford University Innovation https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsldownloads_registration
XQuartz v 2.7.11 X.Org Foundation https://www.xquartz.org/
Offline Sorter v4 Plexon https://plexon.com/products/offline-sorter/
MATLAB R2015b MathWorks https://www.mathworks.com/products.html?
s_tid=gn_ps
Chronux v 2.11 chronux http://chronux.org/
SARGE toolbox v 1.5.4 IBG Lab https://www.ibglab.org/sarge
CSDplotter v 0.1.1 Klas Pettersen https://github.com/espenhgn/CSDplotter
(klas.pettersen@umb.no.)
R v 3.2.2 R Foundation for https://www.r-project.org/
Statistical Computing
RStudio v 1.1.463 R Studio https://rstudio.com/
Movavi Video Converter v 7.3.0 Movavi https://www.movavi.com/videoconvertermac/?
device=c&gclid=Cj0KCQiAtrnuBRDXARIsABiN-
7BytoiOxcGD8lyR3Fvs1MGc3zWwA0UisQaA1
DdujFHHS_q1fbZk6WwaAlnlEALw_wcB
Elmedia video player v 7.6 Elmedia Player https://www.elmedia-video-player.com/
Other
Macaque Stimulated Arousal Index This paper STAR Methods, Email (mredinbaugh@wisc.edu)

LEAD CONTACT AND MATERIALS AVAILABILITY

This study did not generate new unique reagents. Further information and requests regarding resources, equipment, and experi-
mental methods should be directed to, and will be fulfilled by, the Lead Contact, Yuri B. Saalmann (saalmann@wisc.edu).

EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS

We acquired data from two male monkeys (Macaca mulatta, 4.3-5.5 years old, 7.63-10.30 kg body weight). Animal daily needs main-
tained by experimenters and husbandry staff at the Wisconsin National Primate Research Center (WNPRC), where animals
were housed. Animal health was monitored by veterinarians at the WNPRC. The University of Wisconsin-Madison Institutional
Animal Care and Use Committee approved all procedures, which conformed to the National Institutes of Health Guide for the
Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020 e1


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

METHOD DETAILS

Neuroimaging
We performed structural imaging on anesthetized monkeys using the GE MR750 3T scanner (GE Healthcare, Waukesha WI). At
the start of each scan session, we pre-medicated the monkey with ketamine (up to 20 mg/kg body weight) and atropine sulfate
(0.03-0.06 mg/kg), prior to intubation. We then administered isoflurane (1%–3% on 1 L/min O2 flow) to the monkey, with a semi-
open breathing circuit and spontaneous respiration, to maintain general anesthesia for the duration of the session. We monitored
the monkey’s vital signs (expired carbon dioxide, respiration rate, oxygen saturation, pulse rate, temperature) using an MR-compat-
ible pulse oximeter and rectal thermometer.
We acquired a high-resolution structural brain image prior to the implant surgery, to delineate thalamocortical regions of interest
(ROIs), and, after craniotomy, additional scans of electrodes in situ to confirm electrode positioning. For these three dimensional
T1-weighted structural images, we used an inversion-recovery prepared gradient echo sequence with the following parameters:
FOV = 128 mm2; matrix = 256 3 256; no. of slices = 166; 0.5 mm isotropic; TR = 9.68 ms; TE = 4.192 ms; flip angle = 12 ; inversion
time (TI) = 450 ms). To generate the high-quality structural image, we collected 6-10 T1-weighted structural images and calculated
the average image for each monkey using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) (Jenkinson et al., 2012). To localize electrodes, we aver-
aged two structural images of electrodes in situ.

Surgery
We induced anesthesia with ketamine (up to 20 mg/kg body weight, i.m.) and maintained general anesthesia during aseptic surgical
procedures with isoflurane (1%–2%). We used 12 ceramic skull screws and dental acrylic to affix head implants on monkeys. We
drilled 2.5 mm craniotomies in the frontal and parietal bones within a customized plastic recording chamber, providing access to
our three thalamocortical ROIs in the right hemisphere: frontal eye field (FEF), lateral intraparietal area (LIP), and central lateral
thalamic nucleus (CL). We derived craniotomy coordinates from the high-quality T1-weighted structural images acquired prior to
the surgery. We fitted each craniotomy with a conical plastic guide tube filled with bone wax (guide tube prefabricated using
model of skull based on T1-weighted structural images) (Pigarev et al., 2009; Saalmann et al., 2007, 2012) through which linear
electrode arrays traversed. We also inserted two titanium skull screws within the recording chamber, one from which to record
the EEG and one to serve as a reference. The head implant included a head post and, on the implant left and right sides, four hollow
slots (two on each side) into which rods fitted, allowing head immobilization during electrophysiological recordings.

Behavioral tasks and sensory stimuli


To compare electrophysiological data between different states of consciousness, we needed to acquire data under similar behavioral
and sensory conditions for awake and anesthetized monkeys. Thus, we acquired electrophysiological data from both awake and
anesthetized monkeys during a passive auditory oddball paradigm as well as during ‘‘resting state’’ (in which no sensory stimuli
were presented). The passive auditory oddball paradigm was useful because it does not require a behavioral response, does not
require open eyes, and auditory stimuli have been shown to elicit neuronal responses from FEF (Caruso et al., 2016; Romanski
et al., 1999; Schall et al., 1995) and LIP (Cohen et al., 2005; Gifford and Cohen, 2005; Grunewald et al., 1999; Linden et al., 1999),
allowing sound-aligned current source density analyses. Additionally, as controls in the awake monkeys, we acquired electrophys-
iology data during a fixation task, and during the passive auditory oddball paradigm while the monkey maintained fixation (oddball
paradigm run concurrently with fixation task; see ‘‘Awake Experiments’’ section). All electrophysiological recordings occurred in a
quiet, dark room.
In the passive auditory oddball paradigm, the sequence of auditory tones (200 ms duration, with 800 ± 100 ms jitter between each
tone) comprised 80% standard tones (0.9 kHz frequency) and 20% deviant/oddball tones (1 kHz frequency). At least the first four
stimuli of a sequence (3 min duration for anesthesia; 6 min duration for wake) were standard tones, and two sequential tones could
not be deviant stimuli, otherwise the tone order was pseudorandom within the constraint of the overall 80/20 standard-to-deviant
ratio. We presented tones using two speakers placed 35 cm from each ear under anesthesia and 80 cm from each ear during wake-
fulness (sound level at each ear was about 75 dB SPL for both states).
In the fixation task, the monkey needed to fixate a central fixation point (dim gray circle of diameter 0.42 degrees of visual angle
on black background) on the monitor screen located 57 cm away. The monkey received a small volume (0.18-0.22 mL) of juice every
2.2-3.5 s while maintaining fixation within a 3 3 3 degree of visual angle window, centered on the fixation point. When the monkey’s
gaze left the fixation window, he would typically re-establish central fixation quickly, to again receive juice every 2.2-3.5 s
while fixating. To encourage long fixations, we doubled the juice volume if fixation persisted beyond 10 s. We only analyzed
electrophysiological data during stable eye epochs (eye position remained fixed for at least 1 s). This applied to all wake-state
data (resting, oddball paradigm and fixation task).
For awake experiments, we monitored monkeys’ eye position using a video-based eye tracker (500 Hz sampling rate). For
anesthesia experiments, we monitored eyes using a digital video camera (capturing 30 frames per second) and used MATLAB to
analyze luminance contrast in a window tightly bounding the eye image. The contrast differentiated closed eyes (i.e., relatively

e2 Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

homogeneous high luminance eyelid shade) and thalamic stimulation-induced eye openings (i.e., dark pupil and iris contrasting
against sclera), as shown in Figure 1A; and visual inspection of the eye video verified the timing of eye openings/closings derived
from the contrast analysis.

Arousal scoring
We developed an arousal index based on clinical arousal scales to measure the behavioral effects of electrical stimulation. The
arousal index incorporated five main indicators of arousal, with each indicator scored 0, 1 or 2, and the sum of the scores of the
five indicators yielding the arousal index (range 0-10). The five indicators are:

1) limb/face movements (0 = nothing; 1 = small movement or increased EMG with no clear movement; 2 = full reach or withdrawal)
2) oral signs (0 = nothing; 1 = small mouth/jaw/tongue movements; 2 = full jaw openings/closures, with multiple repetitions)
3) body movements (0 = nothing; 1 = small torso movement or swallowing; 2 = large full torso movement)
4) eye movements/openings (0 = nothing; 1 = eyelid flutters/small blinks or increased eye movements; 2 = full eye opening with
occasional blinks)
5) vital signs (0 = no change, i.e., difference of < 10% respiration rate (RR), < 5% heart rate (HR); 1 = difference of > 10% RR, > 5%
HR; 2 = at least 20% change in either RR or HR, or at least 10% change in both RR and HR; compared to baseline 30 s prior to
stimulation).

A veterinarian at the Wisconsin National Primate Research Center, a clinical anesthesiologist, and five other primate electrophys-
iologists observed the electrical stimulation effects during anesthesia experiments. Using observations recorded at the time of
stimulation experiments as well as offline review of videos and EMG data (filtered 30-450 Hz, full-wave rectified, then filtered
5-100 Hz to extract the envelope), we scored arousal level prior to, during, and after all stimulation events. A typical stimulation block
consisted of three stimulation event repetitions (one minute each) within a seven minute recording period at a given site, using the
same stimulation frequency, current, polarity, duration, anesthetic and dose. We defined stimulation event epochs from the onset
to offset of pulses, i.e., from 1-2 minutes, 3-4 minutes, and 5-6 minutes of a seven minute block. The time between two stimulation
epochs was split equally into post- and pre-stimulation epochs (see Figure 1A for an example). The pre-stimulation, during stimula-
tion and post-stimulation arousal index for a block reflected the maximum possible score across the repetitions (repetitions largely
produced the same score within each epoch type). Prior to electrical stimulations (except for a rare few instances testing the valence
of different stimulation frequencies), the arousal index was 0 or 1. This could be differentiated from stimulation events inducing
an arousal index of 3 or more by all observers. Thus, we defined effective stimulation events as those inducing an arousal index
of 3 or more, whereas ineffective stimulation events had an arousal index of 0-2. The behavioral indices used to compute the arousal
score are based on behaviors exhibited by individuals recovering from general anesthesia or disorders of consciousness. Stimula-
tion-induced arousal scores reflect different (parallel) progressions along the recovery sequence.

Electrophysiological recording and stimulation


FEF and LIP electrodes had either 16 or 24 contacts, and CL electrodes had 24 contacts (MicroProbes). These platinum/iridium
electrode contacts had a diameter of 12.5 mm, and 200 mm spacing between contacts. The impedance of contacts on recording
electrodes was typically 0.8-1 MU. We also measured the EEG using titanium skull screws located above dorsal frontoparietal cortex
and, in anesthetized experiments, the EMG using a hypodermic needle (30G) in the forearm. We recorded electrode signals (filtered
0.1-7,500 Hz, amplified and sampled at 40 kHz) using a preamplifier with a high input impedance headstage and OmniPlex data
acquisition system controlled by PlexControl software.
We electrically stimulated using 24-contact electrode arrays that had previously been used several times as recording electrodes
(and now had lower impedance). In early stimulation trials, we titrated current (tested 100-300 mA, but because 100-200 mA induced
arousal, there were only a small number of > 200 mA cases), polarity of first phase of biphasic pulse (negative- or positive-going first
phase), number of electrode contacts simultaneously stimulated (tested 1, 4, 8 and 16 contacts), and stimulation duration (15-60 s).
For subsequent electrical stimulations, we simultaneously stimulated via 16 electrode contacts (16 most ventral contacts), with
400 ms bi-phasic pulses of 200 mA, for a total of 60 s stimulation duration for any given stimulation event (experiments included
multiple stimulation events). We typically performed three stimulation events at a given frequency within a stimulation block for
reproducibility, with a recovery time of at least the stimulation event duration between repetitions, i.e., stimulations from 1-2 minutes,
3-4 minutes, and 5-6 minutes of a seven minute block. In our analyses, we included all stimulation data with currents from
100-200 mA. Stimulation event duration, ranging from 15-60 s, did not influence arousal indices, so we included all durations in
our analyses.

Electrode array localization


We acquired T1-weighted structural images with electrodes held in situ by the customized guide tubes (Pigarev et al., 2009; Saal-
mann et al., 2007, 2012). While the actual electrode is not visible in the images, a susceptibility ‘‘shadow’’ artifact appears
along the length of the electrode with a width of approximately one voxel (0.5 mm3, either side of the electrode). We targeted elec-
trodes to thalamocortical ROIs based on the individual monkey’s structural images, using a stereotaxic atlas as a general reference

Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020 e3


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

(Saleem and Logothetis, 2007). We re-positioned electrodes as necessary and re-acquired T1-weighted structural scans until elec-
trodes were in their desired locations in the thalamus and cortex. Offline, we registered (6 degrees of freedom) the images with elec-
trodes in situ to the high-quality structural image acquired prior to surgery. Using measurements of electrode depth during imaging
and recording sessions as well as the image of electrodes in situ, we reconstructed recording and stimulation sites along electrode
tracks. Thalamic stimulation sites, specifically the eighth electrode contact of the 16 contacts simultaneously used for electrical stim-
ulation (i.e., middle of stimulating array), are shown on one coronal slice (sites collapsed across the anterior-posterior axis) in Figure 1
(monkey R).
We further validated the localization of recording sites in our three thalamocortical ROIs using functional criteria. We confirmed
the FEF ROI in an initial experiment using electrical stimulation at the frontal recording site, i.e., low currents (< 100 mA) elicited
eye movements (Bruce et al., 1985). In the LIP ROI during awake experiments, a large number of neurons showed the classical
response characteristic of peri-saccadic activity. In the CL ROI, we found a subset of neurons with high firing rates (around
40-50 Hz) in the awake state, consistent with a CL locus (Glenn and Steriade, 1982; Steriade et al., 1993).
With the aim of positioning electrode contacts in all cortical layers in FEF and LIP, we used depth measurements derived from
structural images to initially position electrode arrays across FEF and LIP layers (24 contacts with 200 mm spacing between contacts
corresponds to a 4.6 mm span, and 16 contacts correspond to a 3 mm span, which generally allows for contacts in superficial, middle
and deep cortical layers for tracks near perpendicular to the cortical surface or with moderate angles from perpendicular). We further
adjusted electrode position to maximize the number of contacts showing single-unit or multi-unit spiking activity, and we visualized
evoked potentials to auditory tones, with middle layers showing earliest response. We then used current source density (CSD)
analysis to attribute contacts to superficial, middle and deep cortical layers (see section ‘‘Current Source Density [CSD]’’ below).
We performed post-mortem histology to reconstruct electrode tracks in one monkey (in addition to the reconstructions using
structural MRI and electrode depth measurements in both monkeys). After fixing the brain in 10% neutral buffered formalin, the right
hemisphere was cut into approximately 5 mm thick coronal sections, embedded in paraffin, then thinly sectioned (8 mm). Around
ROIs, we stained sections with Hematoxylin and Eosin, and visualized sections under a microscope to confirm electrode tracks
through our ROIs.
We recorded 282 CL neurons, 281 FEF neurons and 282 LIP neurons in total. For CL, there were 181 neurons during anesthesia;
101 neurons during wakefulness; and 83 neurons during sleep. For FEF superficial, middle and deep layers, there were respectively
48, 33 and 91 neurons during anesthesia; 37, 22 and 50 neurons during wakefulness; and 37, 22 and 42 neurons during sleep. For LIP
superficial, middle and deep layers, there were respectively 38, 34 and 91 neurons during anesthesia; 36, 10 and 73 neurons during
wakefulness; and 24, 9 and 65 neurons during sleep. Neurons recorded during sleep were also recorded during the wake state.
Neurons recorded during anesthesia were recorded in different sessions from neurons recorded during wakefulness/sleep.

Anesthesia experiments
We used either isoflurane (9 sessions: 5 for Monkey R, 4 for Monkey W) or propofol (9 sessions: 4 for Monkey R, 5 for Monkey W) in
anesthesia experiments, to ensure that results were not drug-specific, instead reflecting general mechanisms of anesthesia/con-
sciousness. The duration of each anesthesia experimental session was 10-12 hours. We induced anesthesia with ketamine (up to
20 mg/kg body weight, i.m.), then intubated the monkey and inserted an intravenous catheter(s) for fluid and drug administration.
We maintained general anesthesia in spontaneously respiring monkeys with isoflurane (0.8%–1.5% on 1 L/min O2 flow) or propofol
(0.17-0.33 mg/kg/min i.v.), and a clinical anesthesiologist (A.R.) oversaw stable conditions throughout. We categorized doses as
lower (isoflurane < 1%; propofol < 0.23 mg/kg/min), medium (isoflurane 1%–1.19%; propofol 0.23-0.26 mg/kg/min) and higher
(isoflurane R 1.2%; propofol R 0.27 mg/kg/min) within the aforementioned ranges for statistical purposes (see ‘‘Quantification
and Statistical Analysis’’ section). We positioned monkeys in the prone position within a modified stereotaxic apparatus atop a sur-
gical table, with the monkey’s head immobilized by four rods (attached to the stereotaxic device) that slid into the implant hollows. We
maintained the monkey’s temperature using a forced-air warming system and monitored vitals (end tidal carbon dioxide, respiration
rate, oxygen saturation, heart rate, blood pressure and rectal temperature).
Each experimental session had two parts: the first part involved simultaneous recordings from FEF, LIP and CL (recordings started
at least two hours after anesthetic induction and ketamine administration), and the second part involved electrical stimulation of CL
during simultaneous recordings from FEF and LIP without changing the anesthetic regimen. We independently positioned linear
multielectrode arrays in each ROI, and allowed arrays to settle for 30 minutes prior to starting recordings. Microdrives coupled to
an adaptor system allowed different approach angles for each ROI. For both parts of experiments, we interleaved resting state
epochs and the passive auditory oddball paradigm. During the first part of the experimental session, we performed neural recordings
at a number of different anesthetic levels, adapting the dose to reflect a range of clinically relevant anesthetic depths, e.g., 1%, 1.1%,
1.25% and/or 1.5% isoflurane, or 0.2, 0.225, 0.25 and/or 0.3 mg/kg/min propofol, allowing dosing changes to stabilize before starting
the next block of recordings (typically at least 30 minutes). During the second part of the experiment, we either electrically stimulated
using the linear multielectrode array existing in the thalamus or replaced it with another array inserted along the same trajectory to
the same depth. We first stimulated thalamic sites at a frequency of 50 Hz. If this did not induce arousal, then we moved the stim-
ulating electrode to a new depth in the thalamus in steps of 0.5-1 mm dorsal or ventral along the electrode track, until stimulation
induced arousal. When 50 Hz stimulation induced arousal, we tested additional stimulation frequencies, i.e., 2, 10 or 200 Hz, or further
depths (mapping the area of effect). The order of stimulation frequencies generally followed one of two patterns: 50 Hz alternating

e4 Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

with one of the other stimulation frequencies; or multiple repetitions of a particular stimulation frequency, followed by multiple
repetitions of a different stimulation frequency.
In early experiments, we tested thalamic stimulations at different anesthetic doses between 0.8%–1.3% for isoflurane and
between 0.17-0.3 mg/kg/min for propofol. We observed thalamic stimulation-induced arousal for all but the highest doses
(i.e., 1.3% isoflurane and 0.3 mg/kg/min propofol). In subsequent isoflurane experiments, we used doses between 0.8 – 1.25%
(M = 1.04, SD = 0.11) during thalamic stimulation, and in propofol experiments, we used doses between 0.17-0.28 mg/kg/min
(M = 0.23, SD = 0.03). Data for all doses were included in analyses and controlled for statistically (see ‘‘Quantification and Statistical
Analysis’’ section).
As an additional control, we also separately stimulated the FEF and LIP using the same stimulation parameters as those used in the
thalamus (10 or 50 Hz). FEF or LIP stimulation alone did not induce arousal. Stimulating both areas would have required considerable
piloting and additional experimentation, and was thus beyond the scope of this study.

Awake experiments
We performed 40 awake experimental sessions (18 for monkey R; 22 for monkey W), each session usually of 2-4 hours duration. Mon-
keys sat upright in a primate chair with their head immobilized using the head post and/or four rods that slid into the hollow slots in the
head implant. Awake experiments were split into two types (similar to the two parts of anesthesia experimental sessions); those with
and without thalamic stimulation. Experiments without stimulation involved simultaneous recordings from FEF, LIP and CL across
multiple blocks of all task conditions. Stimulation experimental sessions involved electrical stimulation of CL, at different frequencies,
during simultaneous recordings from FEF and LIP across all task conditions. During each type of experiment, we interleaved
task conditions involving reward (fixation and oddball fixation) with those not involving rewards (resting state and passive oddball).
The specific task order was varied randomly across different experimental sessions.
For electrical stimulation, we pseudorandomly applied stimulation blocks of different frequencies, i.e., 10, 50 and 200 Hz. Because
electrical stimulation of the thalamus at 50 Hz frequency (or other frequencies) in awake monkeys did not elicit any movements (as
observed during effective stimulation events in anesthesia experiments), it is unlikely that the effects of 50 Hz stimulation of CL in
anesthetized monkeys simply reflected direct effects on the motor system. Rather, it supports the finding that 50 Hz stimulation
effects reflected increased arousal.
We performed neural recordings from brain areas implicated in awareness. Because these areas are also involved in selective
attention and oculomotor function, we aimed to ensure differences between wake and anesthesia results were not related to atten-
tional or saccadic processes. To this end, within the wake state, we compared recordings during the fixation task to resting state, as
well as recordings during the passive oddball with fixation to that without fixation. For each condition (fixation task, resting state,
oddball with and without fixation), we analyzed epochs (at least 1 s in duration) in which the monkey’s eye position was stable, as
verified using the eye tracker. These analyses showed neural data from compared conditions to be qualitatively similar. Considering
these controls, to keep wake and anesthesia conditions as similar as possible, we compared wake and anesthesia data collected
during conditions in which there were no task demands, i.e., the resting state and passive oddball conditions (not the fixation
task or the oddball with fixation) in the dark.

Sleep
During awake experiments, monkeys at times would fall asleep, particularly during conditions not involving rewards, such as the
resting state. Online, we identified non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep using the following criteria: increased delta (1-4 Hz) activity
in EEG (compared with wake); extended eye closure (recording times when eyes closed and re-opened, to compare with semi-auto-
matic detection offline); preceding period of drowsiness indicated by slow drooping/closing of eyelids; stop in fixation task perfor-
mance (if current task is fixation task); and no overt body movement. Offline, we identified NREM sleep periods using EEG and
eye tracker data. We bandpass filtered (1-4 Hz; Butterworth, order 6) EEG data and applied the Hilbert transform, to calculate the
instantaneous delta-band amplitude. From the resulting time series, we detected times of relatively high delta amplitude using
thresholds titrated for each recording session, because the mean delta amplitude and standard deviation could vary depending
on the recording session and total sleep time. For each session, we selected the threshold as the number of standard deviations
from the mean delta amplitude that produced a total sleep time estimate that closely resembled the expected sleep time based
on online NREM identification, as well as the offline calculation of the total time when the monkey’s eyes were closed (using the re-
corded eye tracker time series data). Offline NREM sleep identification and time stamping then involved automated detection
of extended epochs across the recording session when both the monkey’s eyes were closed and delta amplitude was above
threshold. These offline NREM sleep detections were similar to manual online detections, and proved reliable for different recording
sessions and monkeys.
The identified sleep epochs corresponded to early phases of NREM sleep (N1 or N2, i.e., light sleep). Thus, monkeys were not at
the same depth of unconsciousness during sleep as they were during general anesthesia in our study. This notwithstanding, we
included the spike rate data during early NREM sleep, as this allowed us to compare the influence of conscious and less-conscious
states on the same subset of neurons (n = 282) recorded in both wakefulness and sleep. This further substantiated our comparison of
spiking activity between the awake and anesthetized states, activity recorded from two different samples of neurons from the
same ROIs (maintenance of stable anesthesia up to 12 hours required recordings to take place in a surgical suite, whereas awake

Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020 e5


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

recordings took place in the behavioral lab). Because local field potentials (LFPs) reflect combined activity from a considerably larger
volume (compared with single-neuron activity) (Katzner et al., 2009), LFPs recorded at different times, i.e., awake and during anes-
thesia, are more readily compared. Nonetheless, we include early NREM sleep LFP data as well, to further substantiate the altered
connectivity during anesthesia (although a complete account of sleep influence on our thalamocortical recordings is beyond the
scope of this study).

Neural data preprocessing


We defined data segments of 1 s duration (akin to trials) for analysis. In the awake state, we first determined stable eye epochs (to
match eye behavior between conscious and unconscious states), i.e., epochs starting 200 ms after a saccade and ending 200 ms
before the next saccade. Next, we divided stable eye epochs into non-overlapping 1 s windows. In the anesthetized and non-REM
sleep states (when eyes are closed), we divided all data in each of these states into non-overlapping 1 s windows.
We lowpass filtered data to 250 Hz for LFPs (Butterworth, order 6, zero-phase filter). Next, we linearly detrended LFPs, then ex-
tracted artifacts from LFP data, by removing significant sine waves using the Chronux function rmlinesc. Individual electrode con-
tacts with signal amplitude greater than 5 standard deviations from the mean were excluded from analysis. For power and coherence
analyses, we further calculated bipolar derivations of LFPs, i.e., the difference between two adjacent electrode contacts (excluding
contacts that had been removed due to noise), to minimize any possible effects of a common reference and volume conduction
(Bollimunta et al., 2008; Haegens et al., 2015; Trongnetrpunya et al., 2016).
We bandpass filtered data 250-5,000 Hz for spiking activity (Butterworth, order 4, zero-phase filter) and sorted spikes using Plexon
Offline Sorter software. Initial spike detection involved thresholding data at > 3 standard deviations away from the mean. We then
used principal components analysis to extract features of the spike shapes. Finally, we used the T-distribution expectation maximi-
zation algorithm to identify clusters of spikes with similar features.
For neural data during electrical stimulation, there was a brief artifact caused by the applied current. To remove this artifact, we first
excised a 1 ms window around the artifact, then linearly interpolated across this window. Next, we used the Chronux function rmli-
nesc to remove any significant sine waves at the stimulation frequency (we also performed artifact removal using the SARGE toolbox
(Erez et al., 2010), which yielded qualitatively similar results).

Spike rate
We calculated the average spike rate in 1 s windows (during stable eye epochs) for each neuron, in the awake, sleep and/or anes-
thetized states. We divided anesthetized state data into electrical stimulation and no stimulation windows. For electrical stimulation
data, we calculated the spike rate during the stretches of data unaffected by the stimulation-induced artifact.

Spike timing
For each neuron, we generated interspike interval (ISI) histograms (1 ms bin width), from which we derived an index of burst firing
propensity in the awake and anesthetized states (Senzai et al., 2019). We excluded neurons with very low spike rate (< 1 Hz) from
the burst index analysis, as their ISI histograms had too few samples). For thalamic neurons, the burst index equaled the proportion
of spikes occurring within 2-8 ms (sum of spikes in the 2-8 ms bins of the ISI histogram divided by the total number of spikes; Fig-
ure 2D); we also calculated indices for 2-5, 2-10 and 2-15 ms bins (for qualitatively similar results). Because ISIs in CL neuronal
bursts have been reported to commonly range up to 6 ms (lengthening with increasing burst size) (Lacey et al., 2007), we selected
the next accommodating window size, 2-8 ms. For cortical neurons, the burst index equaled the proportion of spikes occurring
within 2-15 ms (sum of spikes in the 2-15 ms bins of the ISI histogram divided by the total number of spikes; Figure 2E); we also calcu-
lated indices for 2-10, 2-20 and 2-30 ms bins. Although it has been reported that LIP neurons have a low tendency to burst in the wake
state (Maimon and Assad, 2009), we still measured changes in spiking regularity across different states, by using a relatively
larger window, 2-15 ms (cf. CL), still applicable for frontal cortex (Womelsdorf et al., 2014), to allow comparisons between
cortical areas.

Current source density (CSD)


We localized electrode contacts to superficial, middle or deep cortical layers based on inverse CSD analyses (Pettersen et al., 2006).
To do this, we used the CSDplotter toolbox for MATLAB (https://github.com/espenhgn/CSDplotter; dt = 1 ms, cortical conductivity
value = 0.4 S/m, diameter = 0.5 mm) for calculating the inverse CSD in response to auditory tones in the passive oddball paradigm.
Linear multi-electrode arrays measure the LFP, f, at N different cortical depths/electrode contacts along the z axis with spacing h.
The standard CSD, Cst, is estimated from the LFPs using the second spatial derivative.
ðfðz + hÞ  2fðzÞ + fðz  hÞÞ
Cst ðzÞ =
h2
b where F is the vector containing the N measure-
LFPs can also be estimated from given CSDs, represented in matrix form as F = F C,
b
ments of f, C is the vector containing the estimated CSDs, and F is an NxN matrix derived from the electrostatic forward calculation
of LFPs from known current sources. The inverse CSD method uses the inverse of F to estimate the CSD, i.e., Cb = F 1 F. For the step

e6 Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

inverse CSD method (Pettersen et al., 2006) used here, it is assumed that the CSD is stepwise constant between electrode contacts,
so the sources are extended cylindrical boxes with radius R and height h. In this case, F is given by:

qffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi 
h
Zzi + 2
1  0
ðzj  z0 Þ + R2  zj  z  dz
2 0
Fji =
2s
zi h2

where s is the electrical conductivity tensor, and f(zj) is the potential measured at position zj at the cylinder center axis due to a cylin-
drical current box with CSD, Ci, around the electrode position zi. The inverse CSD method offers advantages over the standard CSD.
The inverse CSD method estimates the CSD around all N electrode contacts, whereas the standard CSD method yields estimates
around N-2 contacts. Further, the standard CSD requires equidistant contacts, whereas the inverse CSD method does not, which is
advantageous when data from a noisy contact may need to be excluded. We used the step inverse CSD method, because it may
perform better than the delta-source CSD method as electrode contact spacing increases, and the spline CSD method can be more
sensitive to spatial noise, e.g., from gain differences between electrode contacts or from an excluded contact (Pettersen et al., 2006).
We identified the early current sink in response to auditory stimulation and designated the bottom of the sink as the bottom of the
middle layers (around boundary between layers 4 and 5). We included the electrode contact at the bottom of the middle layers and the
two more superficial contacts as the middle layers. Electrode contacts in FEF or LIP superficial to the middle layers were designated
as being in the superficial layers, whereas FEF or LIP contacts deeper than the middle layers were designated as being in the deep
layers. Layer assignments were cross-referenced to reconstructions of the recording sites along the electrode track (based on mea-
surements of electrode depth as well as the image of electrodes in situ) as well as to single-unit or multi-unit spiking activity, which
helped delineate the border between gray and white matter. We excluded from analysis contacts that were found to be located
outside the ROI.
Previous studies generated CSD data in FEF (Chen et al., 2018) and LIP (Schroeder et al., 1998) using visual stimulation (FEF: 1
degree of visual angle square at 60% contrast; LIP: diffuse light). Our CSD profiles generated with auditory stimulation were roughly
consistent with these previous studies of FEF and LIP in so far as sensory stimulation elicited early sinks in middle layers (which would
be predicted based on auditory stimulation activating middle cortical layers relatively early).
We also performed CSD analyses, in the case of resting state recordings, using LFP signals aligned to the trough of delta-band
oscillations recorded from the electrode contact with the highest delta power (i.e., this contact served as the phase index) (Bollimunta
et al., 2008; Funk et al., 2016; Haegens et al., 2015). These delta phase-realigned CSDs showed differences across cortical layers
which helped verify that probe positions remained stable across recording blocks that did not include auditory stimuli of the passive
oddball paradigm.

Power
We calculated power in 1 s windows (stable eye epochs) for every bipolar-derived LFP, using multi-taper methods (5 Slepian taper
functions, time bandwidth product of 3, averaging over windows/trials) with the Chronux data analysis toolbox for MATLAB (http://
chronux.org/) (Bokil et al., 2007, 2010; Mitra and Bokil, 2007). Noisy trials, samples with amplitudes that exceeded 4 standard de-
viations from the mean, were removed. Sinusoidal noise, especially at stimulation frequencies and 60 Hz, was removed using notch
filters or the Chronux function rmlinesc. There were an unequal number of windows per condition, due to differences in data length
and number of stable eye epochs. Because the number of time windows (or trials) affects the power estimate (S(f)), we bias-corrected
power values (Bokil et al., 2007). The bias-corrected power spectrum, B(f), is given by:
v  v 
0 0
BðfÞ = logðSðfÞÞ  j + ln
2 2
where n0 = 2*K*N, where K is the number of tapers (5) and N is the number of time windows. To obtain population values, we pooled
the bias-corrected power estimates for the awake state and again for the anesthetized state (separately for the no stimulation, effec-
tive stimulation and ineffective stimulation conditions).

Coherence
We calculated coherence using multi-taper methods (5 Slepian taper functions, time bandwidth product of 3) with the Chronux
toolbox. Noisy trials, samples with amplitudes that exceeded 4 standard deviations from the mean, were removed. Sinusoidal noise,
especially at stimulation frequencies and 60 Hz, was removed using notch filters or the Chronux function rmlinesc. We used the
coherence measure to study the temporal relationship between LFPs, or between spikes and LFPs, within and between the thalamus,
FEF and LIP. The coherence is given by:
S12 ðfÞ
CðfÞ =
OðS11 ðfÞS22 ðfÞÞ
where S(f) is the spectrum with subscripts 1 and 2 referring to the simultaneously recorded spike/LFP at one site and LFP at another
site. The coherence is normalized between 0 and 1, so it can be averaged across different pairs of time series. For each paired

Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020 e7


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

recording, we calculated the coherence in 1 s windows during which the monkey’s eyes were stable. There were an unequal
number of windows per condition, due to differences in data length and number of stable eye epochs. Because the number of
time windows (or trials) affects the coherence estimate, we bias-corrected/transformed coherence values (Bokil et al., 2007). The
transformed coherence, T(f), is given by:
1
T ðf Þ = tanh1 ðCðf ÞÞ 
ðv0  2Þ
where n0 is the degrees of freedom; for our multi-taper estimates, n0 = 2*K*N, where K is the number of tapers (5) and N is the number
of time windows. To obtain population values, we pooled the transformed coherence estimates for the awake state and again for the
anesthetized state (separately for the no stimulation, effective stimulation and ineffective stimulation conditions).
To ensure that changes in coherence did not simply reflect changes in power at given frequency bands, we investigated the rela-
tionship between our power and coherence results. While we did find that anesthesia increased delta power and decreased power at
higher frequencies for all cortical areas, power changes during thalamic stimulation were broadband and typically smaller for effec-
tive relative to ineffective stimulations (unlike coherence changes; Figure S3; Table S2). This poor correlation between arousal and
power during stimulation suggests that power is unlikely to be driving stimulation-induced changes in coherence, and is not a key
component of the NCC.

QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

General approach
We performed statistical analyses using general linear models (GLMs) in R via RStudio, regressing the relevant dependent variable on
all independent variables, interactions, and covariates (Models 1-21 below). We used linear models (LM in R) for effects that varied
between all other effects, yielding T statistics for each estimated slope (b parameter). Effects that varied within other effects of interest
were estimated using linear mixed effect models (LMER in R), yielding F statistics, or after computing difference scores with linear
models, yielding T statistics. Random effects of LMER models are represented as gamma parameters, and all simple and main ef-
fects are presented as beta parameters, where the slope for the effect of interest is b1. P values stemming from the same family of
statistical tests (models intended to describe the same effect in different populations) were controlled for multiple comparisons using
Holm’s correction.
To compare doses between anesthetics, we separated doses into lower (1), medium (0), and higher (1) dose groups within the
experimental range used for both anesthetic agents. For isoflurane, lower doses were < 1%, medium were R 1% and < 1.2%, and
higher doses were R 1.2%. For propofol, lower doses were < 0.23 mg/kg/min, medium were R 0.23 and < 0.27 mg/kg/min, and
higher doses were R 0.27 mg/kg/min. This allowed us to use coded dose (DoseCode) as a covariate independent of anesthetic.
To contrast stimulation effectiveness, we coded stimulations producing arousal R 3 as effective (1) and those producing
arousal < 3 as ineffective (0). This allowed us to compare neural dynamics across stimulations that reflected clear changes in the level
of consciousness while controlling for changes that may be induced only by introduction of thalamic current, which was the same for
ineffective and effective stimulations.
To limit the number of multiple comparisons across frequency, we averaged power and coherence across canonical frequency
bands: delta = 0-4 Hz, theta = 4-8 Hz, alpha = 8-15 Hz, beta = 15-30 Hz, low gamma = 30-60 Hz and high gamma = 60-90 Hz. As
a control for possible artifacts, we also averaged more selectively within the low gamma (across 30-47 and 53–57 Hz) and high
gamma (63-90 Hz) bands, so as not to include data at 50 Hz, the frequency of thalamic stimulation, and 60 Hz, the frequency of power
line noise, producing similar results.

Stimulation effects
To test the general effect of thalamic stimulation on arousal (Figure 1B), we regressed arousal score within stimulation blocks on the
peri-stimulation epoch (pre, stimulation, post), including dose and anesthetic as covariates. Peri-stimulation epoch (StimEpochF)
was dummy coded as a factor referenced to the epoch with stimulation, anesthetic was coded as a centered dichotomous variable
(isoflurane = 0.5, propofol = 0.5), and dose was treated as DoseCode. We included random slopes only for stimulation epochs, as
dose and anesthetic remained constant within a given stimulation event. Significantly negative b1 shows that outside of stimulation,
arousal score is lower even controlling for the effects of dose and anesthetic.
ArousalScore  b0 + b1  StimEpochF + b2  DoseCode + b3  Anes (Model 1)
To ensure the effects of stimulation were not being driven or modulated by dose, we repeated this model using DoseCode as an
interactant, but the interaction was not significant (Figures S1A–S1C).
To test the effect of dorsal-ventral (D-V) proximity to CL of the stimulation array on arousal (Figure 1F), we regressed the
stimulation arousal difference (stim – pre) on the linear and quadratic components of the D-V proximity to CL of the centermost
contact of each stimulation array, including dose, anesthetic, and variation of placement along the medial-lateral (M-L) axis as
covariates. D-V distance (D-Vdist) was coded as the distance from the centermost contact of the stimulation array to center of
CL in the D-V plane in each monkey, dose was coded as DoseCode, anesthetic (Anes) was coded as a dichotomous variable

e8 Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

(isoflurane = 0.5, propofol = 0.5), and M-L distance (M-Ldist) was coded as the linear distance from the centermost contact in the
stimulation array to the center of CL in the M-L plane for each animal. Significantly negative b1 shows that moving more dorsal or more
ventral from the center of CL decreases the arousal induced by stimulation above and beyond the effects of anesthetic, dose, and
M-L variation.
ArousalDiff ðstim  preÞ  b0 + b1  D  Vdist2 + b2  D  Vdist + b3  DoseCode + b4  M  Ldist + b5  Anes (Model 2)
To test the effect of Euclidian distance from the center of CL on arousal (Figures S1G–S1I), we regressed arousal difference (stim –
pre) on the Euclidian distance (Distance) including dose, anesthetic, and monkey as covariates. Importantly, we included monkey as
a covariate in this model to control for general differences between the monkeys in terms of the size and shape of their anatomy in M-L
and D-V planes (main source of variation in our electrode track locations). Euclidian distance was calculated as the length of a vector
from the center of CL to the centermost contact of each stimulation array. Dose was coded as DoseCode, anesthetic (Anes) was
coded as a centered, dichotomous variable (isoflurane = 0.5, propofol = 0.5), and monkey (Animal) was coded as a centered,
dichotomous variable (monkey R = 0.5, monkey W = 0.5). Significantly negative b1 shows that moving further from the center of
CL in any M-L/D-V direction decreases the arousal induced by stimulation above and beyond variation contributed by dose, anes-
thetic, or monkey.
ArousalDiffðstim  preÞ  b0 + b1  Distance + b2  DoseCode + b3  Anes + b4  Animal (Model 3)
To test the relative effectiveness of stimulation frequency on arousal (Figure 1H), we regressed arousal difference (stim – pre) on stim-
ulation frequency, for all case-matched examples in which stimulations occurred at the same site, at multiple frequencies, and at
least one stimulation had been effective (arousal score R 3). Because only 50 Hz stimulations reliably increased arousal (Figure 1H;
error bars did not include 0), we coded stimulation frequency (StimFreq) as a dichotomous variable, where stimulations were either at
50 Hz (0.5) or not at 50 Hz (0.5). Dose (DoseCode) was coded as a factor reflecting lower, medium and higher doses within our
experimental range, and anesthetic (Anes) was coded as a centered dichotomous variable (isoflurane = 0.5, propofol = 0.5). Signif-
icantly positive b1 shows that, as monkeys go from pre to stimulation conditions, arousal increases more when stimulations are at
50 Hz above all other frequencies, even controlling for differences in dose and anesthetic between stimulation blocks.
ArousalDiffðstim  preÞ  b0 + b1  StimFreqB + b2  DoseCode + b3  Anes (Model 4)

Spike rate effects


For non-stimulation data, we limited all comparisons to resting state and anesthesia conditions without auditory stimuli. To test the
effect of sleep on thalamic spike rate (Figure 2C), we regressed spike rate within neuron on state (wake versus sleep). State was
coded as a dichotomous variable (wake = 0, sleep = 1). A random intercept and slope for state was included by neuron. Significant
negative b1 shows that after neurons transition from wake to sleep, spike rate tends to decrease.
SpikeRate  b0 + b1  State + gneuron  ð1 + StateÞ (Model 5)
In the cortex (Figures 2F–2H), we used a similar model but included the layer from which the neuron was recorded (SpikeLayer) as an
interaction. SpikeLayer was dummy coded as a factor referenced to the deep cortical layers. A random intercept and slope for state
was included by neuron. No random slope was included for spike layer as it could not vary within neuron. Finding significant positive
b1 for the interaction of state and layer shows that the decrease in spike rate predicted by the state change is less for superficial rela-
tive to deep cortical layers controlling for variation in the middle layer spike rate. This model was used separately for neurons found in
FEF and LIP (and controlled for multiple comparisons).
SpikeRate  b0 + b1  State  SpikeLayer + b2  State + b3  SpikeLayer + gneuron  ð1 + StateÞ (Model 6)
To test the effect of anesthesia on thalamic spike rate (Figure 2C), we regressed spike rate between neuron on state (wake versus
anesthesia). State was coded as a dichotomous variable (wake = 0, anesthesia = 1). Significant b1 shows that neurons recorded dur-
ing anesthesia had lower spike rates relative to wakefulness.
SpikeRate  b0 + b1  State (Model 7)
In the cortex (Figures 2F–2H), we used a similar model but included the layer from which the neuron was recorded (SpikeLayer) as an
interaction. SpikeLayer was dummy coded as a factor referenced to the deep cortical layers. Finding significant positive b1 for the
interaction of state and layer shows that the decrease in spike rate predicted by the state change is less for superficial relative to deep
cortical layers controlling for variation in the middle layer spike rate. This model was used separately for units found in FEF and LIP.
SpikeRate  b0 + b1  State  SpikeLayer + b2  State + b3  SpikeLayer (Model 8)
To ensure that effects were consistent between anesthetics, we compared the spike rate for each type of neuron (superficial, middle,
deep cortical, or thalamic) separately across anesthetic states (isoflurane versus propofol; Figures S2A–S2D) including dose and
cortical area as covariates. State was dummy coded as a dichotomous variable (propofol = 0, isoflurane = 1). Cortical area (Area)
was coded as a centered dichotomous variable (FEF = 0.5, LIP = 0.5) and dose was coded as DoseCode. Negative b1 shows

Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020 e9


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

that spikes recorded during isoflurane have lower spike rate than those recorded under propofol (though none were significant after
controlling for multiple comparisons).
SpikeRate  b0 + b1  StateF + b2  Area + b3  DoseCode (Model 9)
For stimulation data, we analyzed passive auditory oddball paradigm data in addition to resting state data. To test the effect of
thalamic stimulation (50 Hz) on cortical spike rate (Figures 1I–1K), we regressed spike rate within stimulation block on the 4-way inter-
action between peri-stimulation epoch (pre versus stim), cortical layer (superficial versus deep), stimulation effect (effective versus
ineffective), and cortical area (FEF versus LIP), including dose, anesthetic and task as covariates. Peri-stimulation epoch (StimEpoch)
was coded as a dichotomous variable (pre = 1, stim = 0), spike layer (SpikeLayer) was coded as a dichotomous variable (superfi-
cial = 0, deep = 1), stimulation effectiveness (StimEffect) was coded as a dichotomous variable (ineffective = 0, effective = 1), and
cortical area (Area) was coded as a centered dichotomous variable (FEF = 0.5, LIP = 0.5). In addition, dose was coded as Dose-
Code, and anesthetic (isoflurane = 0.5, propofol = 0.5) and task (resting state = 0.5, passive oddball = 0.5) were coded as a
centered, dichotomous variables. A random intercept and slope for stimulation epoch was included by stimulation block (stimID),
as this was the only variable which changed within a given stimulation block. A significantly positive b1*for the 4-way interaction
shows that effective stimulation increases spike rate more for deep layers in LIP than any other condition, controlling for differences
in dose, anesthetic, and task conditions.
SpikeRate  b0 + b1  StimEpoch  SpikeLayer  StimEffect  Area + b2  StimEpoch  StimEffect  Area +
b3  StimEpoch  SpikeLayer  Area + b4  StimEpoch  SpikeLayer  StmEffect +
b5  SpikeLayer  StimEffect  Area + b6  StimEpoch  SpikeLayer + b7  StimEpoch  StimEffect +
(Model 10)
b8  StimEpoch  Area + b9  SpikeLayer  StimEffect + b10  SpikeLayer  Area + b11  StimEffect  Area +
b12  StimEpoch + b13  SpikeLayer + b14  StimEffect + b15  Area + b16  DoseCode + b17  Anes + b18  task +
gstimlD  ð1 + StimEpochÞ

Bursting effects
To test the effect of sleep on thalamic bursting (Figure 2D), we regressed bursting index within neuron (BI2-8; derived from the 2-8 ms
bins of the ISI histogram) on state (wake versus sleep), including spike rate as a covariate (as spike rate tended to change with state in
thalamic neurons and could influence the burst index). State was coded as a dichotomous variable (wake = 0.5, sleep = 0.5).
Because the relationship between bursting changes and spike rate was largely linear within neuron, spike rate was coded as a contin-
uous variable (total spikes/total time). We included a random slope for state and spike rate by neuron. A significant positive b1 indi-
cates that after transitions from wakefulness into NREM sleep, thalamic neurons increase bursting, controlling for changes in
spike rate.
Bl28  b0 + b1 State + b2 SpikeRate + gneuron  ðState + SpikeRateÞ (Model 11)

To test the effect of anesthesia on thalamic bursting (Figure 2D), we regressed bursting index (BI2-8; derived from the 2-8 ms bins of
the ISI histogram) between neuron on state (wake versus anesthesia), including spike rate as a covariate (as spike rate tended to
change with state in thalamic cells and could influence the burst index). State was coded as a dichotomous variable (wake = 0, anes-
thesia = 1). As the relationship between bursting changes and spike rate were not reliably linear between neurons, spike rate was log
transformed (SpikeRateL) and coded as a continuous variable (ln(total spikes/total time)). A significant positive b1 indicates that neu-
rons recorded during anesthesia have higher burst index than wakefulness, controlling for differences in spike rate.
Bl28  b0 + b1  State + b2  SpikeRateL (Model 12)
To test the effect of anesthesia on cortical bursting (Figure 2E), we regressed bursting index (BI2-15; derived from the 2-15 ms bins of
the ISI histogram) between neuron on the interaction of state (wake versus anesthesia) and spike layer, including spike rate and
cortical brain area as covariates. State (wake = 0, anesthesia = 1) and spike layer (superficial = 0, deep = 1) were coded as dichot-
omous variables. As both cortical areas yielded similar results, we combined data across the cortex, and included cortical area as a
centered, dichotomous covariate (FEF = 0.5, LIP = 0.5). Because the relationship between bursting changes and spike rate were not
reliably linear between cells, spike rate was log transformed (SpikeRateL) and coded as a continuous variable (ln(total spikes/total
time)). Significant positive b1 for the state and layer interaction indicates that the increased bursting during anesthesia is larger for
deep relative to superficial neurons, controlling for differences in spike rate and cortical area.
Bl215  b0 + b1  State  SpikeLayer + b2  State + b3  SpikeLayer + b4  SpikeRateL + b5  Area (Model 13)

Power and coherence effects


For the purpose of graphical representation, we computed 95% confidence intervals for power and coherence spectra using the
ttest function in MATLAB (reflecting 95% confidence intervals of the mean) for all spectra and paired difference scores (wake – sleep,
stim – pre). Similarly, we used the ttest2 function to produce confidence intervals for unpaired difference scores (wake – anesthesia).

e10 Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

To illustrate regions of significant difference across frequencies between states and stimulation conditions, we computed t tests
between spectra using the ttest2 command in MATLAB at each frequency. P values were controlled for multiple comparisons using
Holm’s correction.
We limited all non-stimulation comparisons to resting state and anesthesia conditions without auditory stimuli. To test the effects of
anesthetics on power and coherence (Figures S2, S3, and S4), we regressed power (S), coherence (C), and spike-field coherence
(spikeFC) averaged across different frequency bands (delta, alpha, theta, beta, low gamma and high gamma) and isolated to different
electrode contact pairs of interest (in the case of coherence, e.g., isolated to superficial-deep contact pairs within a cortical area, or
deep FEF-deep LIP contact pairs), on state (wake versus anesthesia). For coherence estimates within or between layers of the same
cortical layer, we included cortical area as a covariate. Thalamocortical comparisons were performed separately for each cortical
area, and thus did not need this covariate. Similarly, cross-area corticocortical coherence, which was always computed between
FEF and LIP, did not include this covariate. State was coded as a dichotomous variable (anesthesia = 1, wake = 2), and cortical
area (Area) was coded as a centered, dichotomous variable (FEF = 0.5, LIP = 0.5) where applicable. Because spike-field coherence
was calculated between individual neurons and derivatized LFPs, we included a random intercept by neuron (this inclusion changed
neither the direction nor significance of the effects). Significant positive b1 parameters show frequency bands with increased power,
coherence, or spike-field coherence during wakefulness relative to anesthesia. Significant negative b1 parameters show frequency
bands with decreased power, coherence, or spike-field coherence during wakefulness relative to anesthesia.
Intracolumnar and thalamocortical:
Cðd;q;a;b;gl;ghÞ  b0 + b1  State + b2  Area Table S1 (Model 14)

Sðd;q;a;b;gl;ghÞ  b0 + b1  State + b2  Area Table S2 (Model 15)

Cross-area corticocortical:
Cðd;q;a;b;gl;ghÞ  b0 + b1  State Tables S3 and S4 (Model 16)

Sðd;q;a;b;gl;ghÞ  b0 + b1  State Table S2 (Model 17)

spikeFCðd;q;a;b;gl;ghÞ  b0 + b1  State + gneuron  ð1Þ Tables S3 and S4 (Model 18)

We limited all stimulation comparisons to anesthesia conditions without auditory stimuli where the stimulation frequency was 50 Hz
and within the effective current range (120 - 200 mA). To test the effects of 50 Hz thalamic stimulation on power and coherence (Fig-
ures 3K–3P, 4F–4H, and S3K–S3P), we regressed change in power (S) and coherence (C) (stim – pre) averaged across different
frequency bands (delta, alpha, theta, beta, low gamma, high gamma) and isolated to different electrode contact pairs of interest
(in the case of coherence), on stimulation effectiveness (effective versus ineffective) including anesthetic and dose as covariates.
For coherence estimates within or between layers of the same cortical layer, we included cortical area as a covariate. Cross-area
corticocortical coherence, which was always computed between FEF and LIP, did not include this covariate. Anesthetic (Anes; iso-
flurane = 0.5, propofol = 0.5) and cortical area (Area; FEF = 0.5, LIP = 0.5), where applicable, were coded as centered, dichoto-
mous variables. We coded dose as DoseCode. Significant positive b1 parameters show an interaction with stimulation epoch, where
positive changes in power or coherence at the given frequency band are significantly larger for effective relative to ineffective
stimulations. Significant negative b1 parameters show changes in power or coherence at the given frequency band that are signif-
icantly smaller for effective relative to ineffective stimulations. It was possible to get negative interactions even if power or coherence
still increased during stimulation relative to the pre epoch.
Intracolumnar:
CDiffðd;q;a;b;gl;ghÞ ðstim  preÞ  b0 + b1  StimEffect + b2  DoseCode + b3  Anes + b4  Area Table S1 (Model 19)

SDiffðd;q;a;b;gl;ghÞ ðstim  preÞ  b0 + b1  StimEffect + b2  DoseCode + b3  Anes + b4  Area Table S2 (Model 20)

Cross-area corticocortical:
CDiffðd;q;a;b;gl;ghÞ ðstim  preÞ  b0 + b1  StimEffect + b2  DoseCode + b3  Anes Table S3 (Model 21)

We considered effects consistent between (a) wake versus anesthesia and (b) effective versus ineffective stimulation comparisons
(Figures 3 and 4, gray shading) if both the StimEpoch effect from stimulation models (b0; stim – pre) were consistent in direction

Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020 e11


Please cite this article in press as: Redinbaugh et al., Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex, Neuron (2020), https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.005

and significance to the beta parameter for the state effect in non-stimulation models (b1*State; wake – anesthesia). Such a finding
indicates that the changes following stimulation-induced arousal are in the same direction as those found in the wake state over
anesthesia. Additionally, the interaction term for stimulation data (b1*StimEffect) had to be significant, indicating that similarities
were limited to the effective stimulation condition, and thus driven by arousal and not applied thalamic current in itself.

DATA AND CODE AVAILABILITY

All data and code available upon reasonable request. Requests should be made to, and will be fulfilled by, the Lead Contact, Yuri B.
Saalmann (saalmann@wisc.edu).

e12 Neuron 106, 1–10.e1–e12, April 8, 2020


Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
The Chief continued: “Yesterday he spoke of Europe, which
would intervene if we did not moderate our demands. But I replied, ‘If
you speak to me of Europe I shall speak to you of Napoleon.’ He
would not believe that they had anything to fear from him. I proved
the contrary to him, however. He should remember the plebiscite and
the peasantry, together with the officers and soldiers. It was only
under the Emperor that the Guards could again have the position
which they formerly occupied; and with a little cleverness it could not
be difficult for Napoleon to win over 100,000 soldiers among the
prisoners in Germany. We should then only have to arm them and let
them cross the frontier, and France would be his once more. If they
would concede good conditions of peace we might even put up with
one of the Orleans, though we knew that that would mean another
war within two or three years. If not, we should have to interfere,
which we had avoided doing up to the present, and they would have
to take Napoleon back again. That, after all, must have produced a
certain effect upon him, as, to-day, just as he was going to talk about
Europe again, he suddenly broke off and said, ‘Excuse me.’ For the
rest, I like him very well. He is at least highly intelligent, has good
manners, and is an excellent story-teller. Besides, I often pity him, for
he is in an extremely awkward position. But all that can’t help him in
the least.”
With regard to the war indemnity, the Chief said: “Thiers insisted
that fifteen hundred million francs was the maximum, as it was
incredible how much the war had cost them. And in addition to that
everything supplied to them was of bad quality. If a soldier only
slipped and fell down, his trousers went to pieces, the cloth was so
wretched. It was the same with the shoes which had pasteboard
soles, and also with the rifles, particularly those from America.” I
replied: “But just imagine, you are suddenly pounced upon by a man
who wants to thrash you, and after defending yourself and getting
the better of him, you demand compensation—what would you say if
he asked you to bear in mind how much he had had to pay for the
stick with which he had intended to beat you, and how worthless the
stick had proved to be? However there is a very wide margin
between fifteen hundred and six thousand millions.”
The conversation then lost itself—I can no longer remember how
—in the depths of the Polish forests and marshes, turning for a while
on the large solitary farm houses in those districts and upon
colonisation in the “backwoods of the east.” The Chief said:
“Formerly when so many things were going wrong—even in private
affairs—I often thought that if the worst came to the worst I would
take my last thousand thalers and buy one of those farms out there
and set up as a farmer. But things turned out differently.”
Later on, diplomatic reports were again discussed, and the Chief,
who seems in general to have a poor opinion of them said: “For the
most part, they are just paper smeared with ink. The worst of it is
that they are so lengthy. In Bernstorff’s case, for instance, when he
sends a ream of paper filled with stale newspaper extracts—why,
one gets accustomed to it! But when some one else writes at
interminable length, and as a rule there is nothing in it, one becomes
exasperated. As for using them some day as material for history,
nothing of any value will be found in them. I believe the archives are
open to the public at the end of thirty years—but it might be done
much sooner. Even the despatches which do contain information are
scarcely intelligible to those who do not know the people and their
relations to each other. In thirty years time who will know what sort of
a man the writer himself was, how he looked at things, and how his
individuality affected the manner in which he presented them? And
who has really an intimate knowledge of the people mentioned in his
reports? One must know what Gortschakoff, or Gladstone, or
Granville had in his own mind when making the statements reported
in the despatch. It is easier to find out something from the
newspapers, of which indeed governments also make use, and in
which they frequently say much more clearly what they want. But
that also requires a knowledge of the circumstances. The most
important points, however, are always dealt with in private letters and
confidential communications, also verbal ones, and these are not
included in the archives.
“The Emperor of Russia, for instance, is on the whole very
friendly to us—from tradition, for family reasons, and so on—and
also the Grand Duchesse Hélène, who influences him and watches
him on our behalf. The Empress, on the other hand, is not our friend.
But that is only to be ascertained through confidential channels and
not officially.”
Thursday, February 23rd.—We retain Metz, but not Belfort. It has
been practically decided that a portion of our army shall enter Paris.
And I write the following intimation for the Moniteur:—
“The arrogance with which the Parisian press insults and abuses
the victorious German army that stands outside the gates of the
capital has been frequently stigmatised by us as it deserves. We
have likewise pointed out that the occupation of Paris by our troops
would be the most effectual means of putting an end to this sort of
insolence. At the present moment these lies and calumnies and
provocations know no bounds. For instance, the Figaro of the 21st of
February, in a feuilleton entitled ‘Les Prussiens en France,’ and
signed Alfred d’Aunay, charges German officers and the Germans in
general with the most disgraceful conduct such as theft and pillage.
We learn that these proceedings, which we forbear to characterise,
have entirely frustrated the efforts made by the Parisian negotiators
to prevent the German army entering into Paris. We are positively
assured that the entry of the German forces into the French capital
will take place immediately after the expiration of the armistice.”
Friday, February 24th.—Thiers and Favre were here from 1 to
5.30 p.m. After they left, the Duc de Mouchy and the Comte de
Gobineau were announced. The object of their visit was to complain
of the oppressive action of the German Prefect at Beauvais, who is
apparently rather harsh, or at least not very conciliatory or indulgent.
The Chief came to dinner in plain clothes for the first time during the
war. Is this a sign that peace has been concluded? He again
complained that when he went to see the King, the Grand Dukes,
“with their feminine curiosity, pestered him with questions.” With
regard to the deputation from Beauvais, Hatzfeldt said that Mouchy
and Gobineau were both sensible men and Conservatives, and that
our Prefect, Schwarzkoppen, bullied them and the other notables of
the town and neighbourhood in an unpardonable way. Amongst
other things, two days before the expiration of the term on which a
contribution of two millions was to be paid, they brought him a million
and a half and said that the balance would follow shortly, whereupon
he told them brutally that he was there for the purpose of ruining
them and meant to do so, and he threatened to have them locked up
in order to “coerce” them, which was not in the least necessary. The
Chief was very angry and called Schwarzkoppen a “blockhead.”
Saturday, February 25th.—Unpleasant news has again been
received from Bavaria. Werther (who, it is true, is described by
Bucher as unreliable and a visionary) writes that Count Holnstein
regards the condition of King Lewis with very great anxiety. Prince
Adalbert, who combines “the Wittelsbach haughtiness with Jesuitry,”
is inciting him against us. He asserts that he signed the treaties
under pressure. Before every Court dinner and even before every
audience he drinks large quantities of the strongest wines, and then
says the most extraordinary things to every one without distinction of
persons. He wants to abdicate and leave the crown to his brother
Otto, who, however, has no wish for it, and he is always inquiring
about deadly poisons, &c. The Ultramontanes are aware of all this,
and their candidate for the Reichstag, Prince Luitpold, is also their
candidate for the throne, and they mean to get him chosen in spite of
Prince Otto’s claims.
Wednesday, March 1st.—In the morning I crossed the bridge of
boats at Suresnes to the Bois de Boulogne where, from the half-
ruined stand on the racecourse, I saw the Emperor review the troops
before they marched into Paris.
We were joined at dinner by Mittnacht, and the Würtemberg
Minister, von Wächter, who was formerly attached to the Embassy in
Paris, and while there did his utmost against Prussia. The Chief said
he had ridden in to Paris, and was recognised by the populace, but
there was no demonstration against him. He rode up to one man
who looked particularly vicious, and asked him for a light, which he
willingly gave.
The Chancellor afterwards took occasion once more to speak his
mind out on the obtrusiveness of certain princely personages. “They
are like flies,” he said, “there is no getting rid of them. But Weimar is
the worst of the lot. He said to me to-day, ‘Please tell me where did
you disappear to so quickly yesterday? I should have been glad to
put some further questions to you.’ I replied, ‘That was exactly it,
your Royal Highness. I had business to do, and could not enter into
a lengthy conversation.’ He fancies that the whole world has been
created merely for his sake, for his amusement, the improvement of
his education, and the satisfaction of his curiosity, which is insatiable,
and he has absolutely no tact.” Somebody observed that as a rule
when he talks he does not think of what he says, but rather repeats
phrases that he has learnt by rote. Mittnacht told another story about
this august personage. “Some one was introduced to him: ‘Ah, very
pleased indeed, I have heard so much to your credit. Let me see,
what was it I heard?’”
Thursday, March 2nd.—Favre arrived this morning at 7.30 a.m.,
and wished to be shown in to the Chief. Wollmann declined to wake
him, however, at which the Parisian Excellency was very indignant.
Favre wanted to inform the Chancellor of the news he had received
during the night that the National Assembly at Bordeaux had ratified
the preliminaries of peace, and thereupon to ask that Paris and the
forts on the left bank of the Seine should be evacuated. This request
was submitted in a letter which he left behind him.
Sunday, March 5th.—We leave to-morrow, first going to Lagny
and thence to Metz. The Chief is present at dinner. The conversation
first turned upon our landlady, Madame Jesse, who put in an
appearance either to-day or yesterday and made a variety of
complaints to the Minister as to the damage we are supposed to
have done to her property. He replied that was the way in war,
particularly when people deserted their homes. Besides she had
reasons to be thankful that she had got off so easily. The little table
on which the Treaty of Peace was signed is to be taken with us to
Germany. Taglioni, who is to remain behind a few days with the King,
is instructed to have it replaced by an exactly similar piece of
furniture. In speaking of the preparations for our departure the Chief
says: “Kühnel thinks we ought not to travel by night, as Lorraine will
be haunted, and they might lay something on the rails.” I replied,
“Then I will travel incognito as the Duke of Coburg. Nobody owes
him a grudge. He is regarded as perfectly innocent—and with
justice.”
Monday, March 6th.—A lovely morning. Thrushes and finches
warble the signal for our departure. At 1 o’clock the carriages get
under way, and with light hearts we drive off towards the gate that we
entered five months ago, and passed Villa Coublay, Villeneuve Saint
Georges, Charenton, and La Fasanerie to Lagny, where we take up
our quarters for the night.
We leave here next day by a special train for Metz, where we
arrive late at night. We put up at an hotel, while the Chief stays with
Count Henckel at the Prefecture. Next morning we stroll through the
town, visit the cathedral, and survey the neighbourhood from the
bastions of the fortress. Shortly before 11 o’clock we are again in the
train, and travel by Saarbrücken and Kreuznach to Mainz, and
thence to Frankfurt.
The Chief has an enthusiastic reception everywhere along the
line and particularly at Saarbrücken and Mainz. Frankfurt is the only
exception. We arrive there at a late hour, and start again in the night.
At 7.30 on the following morning we reach Berlin, after exactly seven
months’ absence. All things considered, everything has been done
during those seven months which it was possible to do.
FOOTNOTES
[1] Strictly speaking, almost complete, as some passages
must still be omitted for the present.
[2] The despatch was understood to contain a sentence to the
effect that Rome should take care not to challenge Europe, and
that whatever the Church might say, the Austrian Courts of Justice
would not allow themselves to be influenced into according any
indulgence towards those who broke the laws or instigated others
to do so.
[3] At that time it had only been accepted by the Committee of
the House of Commons,—without any important amendments
however, and its adoption on a third reading was assured. It is
true, objections were raised. Gladstone very characteristically
observed that the law now only empowered the Administration to
proceed against incitements to treasonable action; it was,
however, necessary to provide for the punishment of attempts by
the press to create a “treasonable state of mind” amongst the
people. The sole concession made by the Government was that
the threatened measures should not be put into execution until
warning (once only) had been given.
[4] The loyal Hanoverian circles did not tell the truth in this
matter. Stoffel’s reports were, on the whole, good, and he himself
was a man of respectable character.
[5] Not quite correct, according to a subsequent statement of
the Minister’s and Count Bill’s own account.
[6] Louis de Condé was treacherously murdered on the 12th of
March, 1569, after the engagement at Jarnac, just as he had
delivered up his sword to an officer of the royal army, being shot
by one Montesquieu, a captain of the Guards.
[7] These particulars are worked up into the preceding chapter.
[8] In presence of later events he can hardly have expressed
himself in this way.
[9] The Würtemberger was Von Reinhard, and the
Darmstadter Von Munch-Bellinghausen, both determined
opponents of Prussia.
[10] Compare this passage with the speech delivered by
Bismarck in the United Diet on the 15th of June, 1847. On that
occasion he said, “I am of opinion that the conception of the
Christian state is as old as the so-called Holy Roman Empire, as
old as all the European States, and that it is exactly the ground in
which those States have struck deep roots; and further, that each
State that wishes to secure its own permanence, or even if it
merely desires to prove its right to existence, must act upon
religious principles. The words ‘By the grace of God,’ which
Christian rulers add to their names, are for me no mere empty
sound. On the contrary, I recognise in them the confession that
Princes desire to wield the sceptre with which God has invested
them in accordance with His Will.” Certain remarks made by the
Chancellor in his speech of the 9th of October, 1878, during the
debate on the Anti-Socialist Bill, should also be remembered in
this connection. He said, inter alia: “If I had come to believe as
these men (the Social Democrats) do—yes, I live a full and busy
life and am in opulent circumstances—but that would not be
sufficient to make me wish to live another day if I had not, in the
words of the poet, ‘an Gott und bessere Zukunft Glauben’ (faith in
God and a better future).”
[11] It was a report from Mohl, originally intended, for his
Government at Carlsruhe, which was communicated to the Chief,
under whose instructions extracts therefrom were utilised in the
press.
[12] At that time Secretary of State in the Foreign Office. He
was not a Catholic.
[13] Bucher afterwards told me that the Chancellor was
affected both by the superstition respecting the number thirteen
and that relating to Friday. Other diplomats, as, for instance, the
French, seem to entertain the same objection both to the number
and the day. The following anecdote, which I was assured was
perfectly genuine, may serve as an example. After the
negotiations respecting the duty payable by ships passing through
the Sound had been completed, it was arranged that the treaty
containing the terms agreed upon should be signed at
Copenhagen on the 13th of March, 1587. It turned out that the
day thus chosen was not only the thirteenth of the month, but was
also a Friday, and that there were thirteen Plenipotentiaries to
sign the document. “A threefold misfortune!” exclaimed the French
Ambassador Dotezac. To his delight, however, the addition of the
signatures was postponed for some days owing to difficulties
occasioned by the difference in the rate of exchange of Danish
and Prussian thalers. The number of representatives still caused
him so much anxiety, however, that it made him ill, and it was only
on the decease of the Hanoverian Plenipotentiary a few weeks
later that the French Ambassador and the other signatories of the
treaty felt that they were no longer in danger of sudden death.
[14] Walker, the English Kutusow of Count Bismarck-Bohlen,
H. B. M.’s Military Plenipotentiary at headquarters, was not held in
much estimation by the Chancellor and his entourage.
[15] These suspicions, though fully justified by appearances,
were subsequently shown to be for the greater part unfounded,
except that there was inadequate provision for the requirements
of the wounded. I reproduce the episode as evidence of the
Minister’s usual humane feeling and love of justice.
[16] A reference to the popular Thuringian ballad of “The
Landgrave and the Smith.”
[17] His greeting to those who brought him the news of his
election as Emperor while he was netting birds in the forest.
[18] Thun, Rechberg and Prokesch held in succession the
position of Austrian Minister to the Bundestag.
[19] The communication referred to is a letter by Thomas
Carlyle published in The Times of November 18, in which it
occupied two and a half columns. The passages quoted by Dr.
Busch are here reproduced from the original:—
“The question for the Germans, in this crisis, is not one of
‘magnanimity,’ of ‘heroic pity and forgiveness to a fallen foe,’ but
of solid prudence and practical consideration what the fallen foe
will, in all likelihood, do when once on his feet again. Written on
her memory, in a distinctly instructive manner, Germany has an
experience of 400 years on this point; of which on the English
memory, if it ever was recorded there, there is now little or no
trace visible.... No nation ever had so bad a neighbour as
Germany has had in France for the last 400 years; bad in all
manner of ways; insolent, rapacious, insatiable, unappeasable,
continually aggressive.... Germany, I do clearly believe, would be
a foolish nation not to think of raising up some secure boundary
fence between herself and such a neighbour now that she has the
chance. There is no law of nature that I know of, no Heavens Act
of Parliament whereby France, alone of terrestrial beings, shall
not restore any portion of her plundered goods when the owners
they were wrenched from have an opportunity upon them.... The
French complain dreadfully of threatened ‘loss of honour’; and
lamentable bystanders plead earnestly, ‘Don’t dishonour France;
leave poor France’s honour bright.’ But will it save the honour of
France to refuse paying for the glass she has voluntarily broken in
her neighbour’s windows. The attack upon the windows was her
dishonour. Signally disgraceful to any nation was her late assault
on Germany; equally signal has been the ignominy of its
execution on the part of France. The honour of France can be
saved only by the deep repentance of France, and by the serious
determination never to do so again—to do the reverse of so for
ever henceforth.... For the present, I must say, France looks more
and more delirious, miserable, blamable, pitiable and even
contemptible. She refuses to see the facts that are lying palpably
before her face, and the penalties she has brought upon herself.
A France scattered into anarchic ruin, without recognisable head;
head, or chief, indistinguishable from feet, or rabble; Ministers
flying up in balloons ballasted with nothing else but outrageous
public lies, proclamations of victories that were creatures of the
fancy; a Government subsisting altogether on mendacity, willing
that horrid bloodshed should continue and increase rather than
that they, beautiful Republican creatures, should cease to have
the guidance of it; I know not when and where there was seen a
nation so covering itself with dishonour.... The quantity of
conscious mendacity that France, official and other, has
perpetrated latterly, especially since July last, is something
wonderful and fearful. And, alas! perhaps even that is small
compared to the self-delusion and unconscious mendacity long
prevalent among the French.... To me at times the mournfullest
symptom in France is the figure its ‘men of genius,’ its highest
literary speakers, who should be prophets and seers to it, make at
present, and, indeed, for a generation back have been making. It
is evidently their belief that new celestial wisdom is radiating out
of France upon all the other overshadowed nations; that France is
the new Mount Zion of the universe; and that all this sad, sordid,
semi-delirious, and, in good part, infernal stuff which French
literature has been preaching to us for the last fifty years is a
veritable new Gospel out of Heaven, pregnant with blessedness
for all the sons of men.... I believe Bismarck (sic) will get his
Alsace and what he wants of Lorraine, and likewise that it will do
him, and us, and all the world, and even France itself by and by, a
great deal of good.... (Bismarck) in fact seems to me to be striving
with strong faculty, by patient, grand and successful steps,
towards an object beneficial to Germans and to all other men.
That noble, patient, deep, and solid Germany should be at length
welded into a nation and become Queen of the Continent, instead
of vapouring, vain-glorious, gesticulating, quarrelsome, restless
and over-sensitive France, seems to me the hopefullest public
fact that has occurred in my time.”—The Translator.
[20] The King.
[21] The Crown Prince.
END OF VOL. I.

RICHARD CLAY AND SONS, LIMITED, LONDON AND BUNGAY.


Transcriber's Notes
The following changes have been made to the
text as printed. In cases of doubt, recourse has
been had to the original German work
(Tagebuchblätter).
1. Obvious typographical errors have been
corrected.
2. Errors in use of quote marks and other
punctuation have been corrected.
3. In cases of inconsistent spelling of German
and French names, the spelling used in the original
language has been preferred. Examples include
changing Frankfort to Frankfurt, Mayence to Mainz,
Rheims to Reims, Delbruck to Delbrück. However,
where the English text is consistent in spelling, that
spelling has been retained (Cologne, Munich,
Jahrbuecher).
4. Where a word is used repeatedly in the same
way, hyphenation has been made consistent,
preferring the form most often used in the printed
work, or failing that the more usual form in general
use at the time of publication.
5. Page 113: the result would not been has
been changed to the result would not have been.
*** END OF THE PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK BISMARCK ***

Updated editions will replace the previous one—the old editions


will be renamed.

Creating the works from print editions not protected by U.S.


copyright law means that no one owns a United States copyright
in these works, so the Foundation (and you!) can copy and
distribute it in the United States without permission and without
paying copyright royalties. Special rules, set forth in the General
Terms of Use part of this license, apply to copying and
distributing Project Gutenberg™ electronic works to protect the
PROJECT GUTENBERG™ concept and trademark. Project
Gutenberg is a registered trademark, and may not be used if
you charge for an eBook, except by following the terms of the
trademark license, including paying royalties for use of the
Project Gutenberg trademark. If you do not charge anything for
copies of this eBook, complying with the trademark license is
very easy. You may use this eBook for nearly any purpose such
as creation of derivative works, reports, performances and
research. Project Gutenberg eBooks may be modified and
printed and given away—you may do practically ANYTHING in
the United States with eBooks not protected by U.S. copyright
law. Redistribution is subject to the trademark license, especially
commercial redistribution.

START: FULL LICENSE


THE FULL PROJECT GUTENBERG LICENSE
PLEASE READ THIS BEFORE YOU DISTRIBUTE OR USE THIS WORK

To protect the Project Gutenberg™ mission of promoting the


free distribution of electronic works, by using or distributing this
work (or any other work associated in any way with the phrase
“Project Gutenberg”), you agree to comply with all the terms of
the Full Project Gutenberg™ License available with this file or
online at www.gutenberg.org/license.

Section 1. General Terms of Use and


Redistributing Project Gutenberg™
electronic works
1.A. By reading or using any part of this Project Gutenberg™
electronic work, you indicate that you have read, understand,
agree to and accept all the terms of this license and intellectual
property (trademark/copyright) agreement. If you do not agree to
abide by all the terms of this agreement, you must cease using
and return or destroy all copies of Project Gutenberg™
electronic works in your possession. If you paid a fee for
obtaining a copy of or access to a Project Gutenberg™
electronic work and you do not agree to be bound by the terms
of this agreement, you may obtain a refund from the person or
entity to whom you paid the fee as set forth in paragraph 1.E.8.

1.B. “Project Gutenberg” is a registered trademark. It may only


be used on or associated in any way with an electronic work by
people who agree to be bound by the terms of this agreement.
There are a few things that you can do with most Project
Gutenberg™ electronic works even without complying with the
full terms of this agreement. See paragraph 1.C below. There
are a lot of things you can do with Project Gutenberg™
electronic works if you follow the terms of this agreement and
help preserve free future access to Project Gutenberg™
electronic works. See paragraph 1.E below.
1.C. The Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation (“the
Foundation” or PGLAF), owns a compilation copyright in the
collection of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works. Nearly all the
individual works in the collection are in the public domain in the
United States. If an individual work is unprotected by copyright
law in the United States and you are located in the United
States, we do not claim a right to prevent you from copying,
distributing, performing, displaying or creating derivative works
based on the work as long as all references to Project
Gutenberg are removed. Of course, we hope that you will
support the Project Gutenberg™ mission of promoting free
access to electronic works by freely sharing Project
Gutenberg™ works in compliance with the terms of this
agreement for keeping the Project Gutenberg™ name
associated with the work. You can easily comply with the terms
of this agreement by keeping this work in the same format with
its attached full Project Gutenberg™ License when you share it
without charge with others.

1.D. The copyright laws of the place where you are located also
govern what you can do with this work. Copyright laws in most
countries are in a constant state of change. If you are outside
the United States, check the laws of your country in addition to
the terms of this agreement before downloading, copying,
displaying, performing, distributing or creating derivative works
based on this work or any other Project Gutenberg™ work. The
Foundation makes no representations concerning the copyright
status of any work in any country other than the United States.

1.E. Unless you have removed all references to Project


Gutenberg:

1.E.1. The following sentence, with active links to, or other


immediate access to, the full Project Gutenberg™ License must
appear prominently whenever any copy of a Project
Gutenberg™ work (any work on which the phrase “Project
Gutenberg” appears, or with which the phrase “Project
Gutenberg” is associated) is accessed, displayed, performed,
viewed, copied or distributed:

This eBook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United


States and most other parts of the world at no cost and with
almost no restrictions whatsoever. You may copy it, give it
away or re-use it under the terms of the Project Gutenberg
License included with this eBook or online at
www.gutenberg.org. If you are not located in the United
States, you will have to check the laws of the country where
you are located before using this eBook.

1.E.2. If an individual Project Gutenberg™ electronic work is


derived from texts not protected by U.S. copyright law (does not
contain a notice indicating that it is posted with permission of the
copyright holder), the work can be copied and distributed to
anyone in the United States without paying any fees or charges.
If you are redistributing or providing access to a work with the
phrase “Project Gutenberg” associated with or appearing on the
work, you must comply either with the requirements of
paragraphs 1.E.1 through 1.E.7 or obtain permission for the use
of the work and the Project Gutenberg™ trademark as set forth
in paragraphs 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.

1.E.3. If an individual Project Gutenberg™ electronic work is


posted with the permission of the copyright holder, your use and
distribution must comply with both paragraphs 1.E.1 through
1.E.7 and any additional terms imposed by the copyright holder.
Additional terms will be linked to the Project Gutenberg™
License for all works posted with the permission of the copyright
holder found at the beginning of this work.

1.E.4. Do not unlink or detach or remove the full Project


Gutenberg™ License terms from this work, or any files
containing a part of this work or any other work associated with
Project Gutenberg™.

You might also like