The Role of The Media in The Fear of Crime - A Qualitative Study in The Portuguese Context

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Original Article

Criminal Justice Review


1-18

The Role of the Media in the © 2022 Georgia State University

Fear of Crime: A Qualitative Article reuse guidelines:


sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/07340168221088570
Study in the Portuguese journals.sagepub.com/home/cjr

Context

Cynthia Silva1 and Inês Guedes2,3

Abstract
The present study, using a Portuguese sample, is aimed at understanding how the media shapes the
fear of crime experiences. Concretely, we explored how individuals interpret the content transmit-
ted by the media and how these perceptions influence the fear of crime contextually situated. It was
also our aim to explore if the area of residence (rural and urban), as well as certain sociodemo-
graphic characteristics (such as gender), were important in the way meanings are attributed to
the media in experiences of insecurity. In total, 20 semi-structured interviews were undertaken
(n = 12 women; 13 from rural areas and 7 from urban areas). Among the relevant results found
in the present study, one of the conclusions we can highlight is that it is not possible to establish
a clear relationship between media consumption and insecurity feelings. Indeed, media do not totally
shape the fear of crime experiences, as a panoply of meanings has emerged, which makes their influ-
ence differential. Thus, aspects such as the sensationalism, the location of the reported crime, the
realism of the news and the proximity to the event are examples of factors that emerged with great
relevance in this investigation. In short, this study allowed the authors to reach a depth and diversity
of meanings of insecurity, through several approaches that were the basis for their understanding
and confirms the relevance of studying this theme from experiences and discourses using qualitative
research.

Keywords
media, fear of crime, qualitative approach, interviews, meanings, experiences

1
School of Criminology, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
2
Interdisciplinary Research Centre on Crime, Justice and Security (CJS) School of Criminology, University of Porto, Porto,
Portugal
3
CEJEA, Lusiada University, Norte, Portugal

Corresponding Author:
Inês Guedes, Faculdade de Direito da Universidade do Porto, Rua dos Bragas 223, 4050-123 Porto, Portugal.
Email: iguedes@direito.up.pt
2 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

Introduction
We have seen a huge spread of information about crime in the news, such as on social networks,
blogs or other sources, appearing either in the form of text or images (Srinivasa & Thilagam,
2019). On television, for example, there are two main reasons why crime is increasingly prevalent:
first, crime is linked to fear, a basic element of the entertainment format; and, second, crime and vio-
lence are clearly easy topics to cover (Soola, 2007), and are faster, cheaper and more efficient topics
for television stations (Altheide, 2003). Fear of crime, broadly defined, is a multidimensional phe-
nomenon that includes emotional, cognitive and behavioral manifestations to crime or symbols asso-
ciated with crime (e.g., Gabriel & Greve, 2003). There have been diverse explanations to address
why certain groups or places fear more than others. These explanations are nested into individual
(such as sex or gender), contextual (for instance, lack of street lighting or simply darkness) and
social models (such as collective efficacy). Among them, previous literature has focused on the rela-
tionship between fear of crime and the media, using both qualitative and quantitative methodologies
(Cashmore, 2014; Chadee, 2001; Chadee & Ditton, 2005). In 2004, after a review on the linkage
between fear of crime and the media (newspaper and television consumption), Ditton and colleagues
found that 41% of the studies found a positive relationship between those variables, but 59% did not.
Later, Chadee and Ditton (2005) focusing on Trinidad, stated that that “the results clearly indicate
that there is no relationship between fear of crime and the media” (p. 329).
Nevertheless, according to some authors, such as Leal (2010, p. 397), the mass media “have
become a powerful vehicle for transmitting messages essential to the cohesion of individuals and
the structuring of their own identity”. Thus, if the media play an informative and entertainment
role, and if part of its agenda is made up of crime-related news and content, it may be relevant to
understand, through qualitative methodologies, how these messages are received and interpreted
and what role they will play in the fear of crime. Therefore, in the present study, qualitative in
nature, we used semi-structured interviews looking to explore these meanings in order to understand
how the place where individuals live (rural or urban) and the relationships they establish in this
context influence their perception of the media and the role of news in the fear of crime.
Specifically, in the first place, (a) this study aims to analyze the consumption habits of media and
social media, namely the type of sources and content most consumed. In other words, it aims to
explore the perceptions about the role that the media plays in society and about the way news are
transmitted, as well as the factors that influence these meanings; (b) secondly, this study explores
the consequences of the consumption of media in the fear of crime (in its different manifestations);
(c) thirdly, it is intended to investigate what meanings are attributed to (in) security and the way in
which it is perceived and experienced from the physical, social and fear contexts; and finally, (d) it
seeks to explore the role that direct and indirect victimization experiences have in the fear of crime.
Regarding the structure through which this article will be developed, firstly the crucial concepts
for exploring the theme will be defined, followed by the main prominent theories of literature, with
some later considerations about the fear of crime, and further on, about the media. Next, the meth-
odology of the present study will be described in detail. To conclude, the main results will be pre-
sented and discussed, ending with a reflection and implications of the study.

Theoretical Background
Definition of Fear of Crime
The scientific community has been following two different understandings when referring to the
concept of fear of crime (Vandeviver, 2011). One the one hand, fear of crime is conceived in a
restricted way, corresponding to the emotional dimension to crime or symbols associated with it
Silva and Guedes 3

(e.g., Ferraro & LaGrange, 1987). Other authors (e.g., Gabriel & Greve, 2003; Madriz, 1997) inter-
pret fear of crime as a multidimensional phenomenon encompassing three different components: the
emotional one (fear of crime in the strict sense), the cognitive (perceived risk of victimization) and
the behavioral dimension (avoidance, protective and self-defense behaviors).
Fear of crime is narrowly defined as “an emotional response of dread or anxiety to crime or to
symbols that a person associates with crime” (Ferraro 1995, p. 4). This dimension is different
from a perception of the environment (Warr, 2000) and includes a set of various emotions
towards the possibility of victimization (fear, worry and anxiety) (Jackson & Gouseti, 2012). In
turn, the perceived risk of victimization is an assessment of personal threat or the perceived likeli-
hood of being victimized. Therefore, individuals make estimates or judgements about the possibility
of victimization (Ferraro & LaGrange, 1987). We argue that there is a symbiotic relationship between
fear and perceived risk: while fear of crime is influenced by risk judgments, fear also affects such
judgment. For that reason, several authors have pointed out that both components must be analyzed
simultaneously in the same studies (Rader, 2004; Rader et al., 2007). Nevertheless, as a negative
emotional reaction generated by crime or its associated symbols, fear is conceptually distinct from
judgments (risks) or concerns (values) about crime (Ferraro & LaGrange, 1987). Furthermore,
even as a result of cognitive processing, fear per se is not a belief, attitude or assessment. On the
contrary, it is a feeling of alarm at the perception or expectation of danger (Warr, 2000). Indeed,
there are at least two likely reactions when people believe that crime is a problem. Naturally, one
reaction is fear, and another is restrictive behavior - which implies adjustments to routine activities,
apparently to reduce this same risk (Ferraro, 1996; Miethe, 1995). The behavioral dimension of fear
manifests in the frequency with which individuals fortify their homes against possible invasion,
avoid contact with certain people or use self-defense arms (Guedes et al., 2018). These behaviors
have been aggregated in different categories, namely the avoidance, protective and defensive.
Finally, while fear of crime might be conceived as an experiential fear – a transitory and short-
lived experience, generated by an external stimulus – it can also be understood as an expressive
fear. Here, fear is built through the knowledge and individual’s perceptions about their community
or broader social-cultural concerns (Farrall et al., 2009).
In fact, one of the premises for the study of fear of crime is that it operates at different levels of
meaning and awareness, constantly emerging and reacting to direct personal experiences, knowledge
about the experiences of others and mediated sources of information, and to broader narratives about
anxieties related to “the way society is today” (Lupton & Tulloch, 1999, p. 521). The attempts to
understand the dynamics underlying the fear of crime have led to numerous empirical and theoretical
developments. Some studies examine the effects of structural characteristics, such as crime rates,
social disorganization and ethnicity, on fear; others argue that those who experience victimization,
directly or indirectly through the experiences communicated in their interpersonal relationships,
manifest a greater sense of fear than those who do not (Liska & Baccaglini, 1990). Other authors
focus on individual characteristics such as sex, age, socioeconomic status, education (Hale, 1996)
and, more recently, personality (Guedes et al., 2018). We briefly overview a set of relevant results
related to the main explanations of fear of crime.

Explanations of Fear of Crime


In order to understand the phenomenon of fear of crime, sociodemographic characteristics appear in
the literature, which include sex, age, ethnicity and economic status (e.g., Pain, 2001; Rader et al.,
2007). People may feel especially vulnerable to the risk of crime because they are likely to be
targets of victimization, because they feel that the impact of crime would be particularly serious
and also, they are unable to control whether or not crime occurs (Farrall et al., 2007). Some individ-
uals may associate a type of crime with particularly serious consequences, since they feel particularly
4 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

vulnerable to the criminal event (Warr, 1987). Moreover, the internalization of female frailty (e.g.,
derived from apparently inferior physical abilities) and the vision of the body as an easy target for
certain crimes such as rape (messages that can be socially constructed, including by the media),
can put women in a high state of fear and lead them to adopt more security and protection measures
(Campbell, 2005). We can distinguish, on one hand, the physical vulnerability defined by the assess-
ment of one’s physical strength and personal competence in the case of aggression, which is often
used to explain the increased fear of crime levels among women. On the other hand, social vulner-
ability is influenced by the social network, personal financial situation and access to material and
social resources, related to the higher levels of fear among disadvantaged ethnic and socio-economic
minorities (Franklin et al., 2008; Skogan & Maxfield, 1981).
Focusing attention on another set of variables - the physical and social environmental contexts -
there is an understanding that indicates that people’s responses to crime are based on the experiences
of everyday life. For that reason, it is essential to investigate the fear of crime by exploring the
dynamic contexts in which the fear of crime is generated and experienced (Lupton & Tulloch,
1999). Fear can arise when people see the erosion of the values that keep public behavior under
control - such as neighborhood connections that create security feelings - and is an expression of
the most everyday social concerns that can generate similar patterns of emotional and behavioral
responses. Incivilities become a symbol of decadent communities (communities where incivilities
and lack of connection between neighbors are identified) and can produce fear of crime, even
with relatively low rates of criminality (Hale, 1996). Incivilities can be differentiated according to
two categories - social and physical ones (LaGrange et al., 1992). The first concerns disruptive
behaviors, namely drinking in public spaces, begging, the presence of gangs, among others. The
latter refer to disordered environments, such as the visualization of abandoned cars, destroyed prop-
erties, garbage and empty or deteriorated houses (Franklin et al., 2008). On the other hand, residents
in areas of high crime, where the spirit of cohesion in the community and involvement is high, may
be less afraid. Faced with a scenario of physical deterioration and social disorder, perceptions of risk
and assessments of the consequences of victimization are also affected by people’s feelings about
their neighborhood and the support network they feel or not on the part of the community (Hale,
1996). In conclusion, individuals’ fear of crime can be shaped by the organization of the environ-
ment, public social behaviors, the period of the day and also by social integration, meaning, the
sense of belonging and connection of a person in their local environment (Franklin et al., 2008).
Fear can also depend on the location of individual such as the fact of living in more rural or urban
contexts. As an example, Little et al. (2005) reveal that the social relations that characterize rural
areas are presented as stronger and more durable compared to the individualism of urban society.
In addition, the harmonious, close and authentic social relations attributed to the rural community
are strongly linked to feelings of security. The security of the rural environment is seen in part as
a result of its separation from the city, so comparisons between the “safe” rural and the “dangerous”
city are increasingly reflected in the representation of the urban as a threat to the rural (idem).
Lastly, considering also the experiences of victimization and its relevance in the investigation of
fear of crime, the several studies that worked on its relationships have shown mixed results. (Evans
and Fletcher, 2000; Hale, 1996; Hill et al., 1985). For instance, Hanslmaier (2013) found significant
differences in fear of crime levels considering the victimization experiences. In fact, those who were
victimized during the previous two years reported a level of fear significantly higher compared to
individuals without this recent victimization experience. On the other hands, people who have
never been victims are more likely to fear the occurrence of crimes (e.g., Wilcox et al., 2006). To
clarify the different meanings of this relationship, other authors justify that more relevant than
direct victimization is experiencing victimization indirectly (e.g., having a family member, friend,
neighbor or community member who was a victim) which can effectively have a substantial
impact on fear crime (e.g., Rader et al., 2007). Through sharing the experiences of those who
Silva and Guedes 5

have been the target of crimes, people are subject to ‘vicarious victimization’ (Hale, 1996, p. 108).
The perception of crime can be more strongly shaped by the character of the addictive crime infor-
mation than by personal experiences. Finally, it should be noted that fear of crime also involves
issues of representation and meaning, and therefore it is vital to understand the character and uses
of the mass media in conjunction with other sources of meaning and within the local context
(Lupton & Tulloch, 1999).

Mass Media and Fear of Crime


As referred before, individuals form their beliefs about real levels of crime through two main
sources: direct observation of reality and, on the other hand, through indirect channels such as tele-
vision, internet, newspapers and radio (Mastrorocco & Minale, 2018), the last source to be
explored below.
Diverse theories that help to understand the role of the media consumption in the fear of crime
have been developed. Therefore, in the next lines these theories will be briefly outlined. One of the
most established theories is Cultivation Theory conceived by George Gerbner in the 1960s. The
empirical studies by Gerbner and Gross (1976) demonstrated that frequent television viewers
(those who watch television for more than four hours a day) are more likely to feel that they
live in a violent society due to the fact that television exaggerates so much severity as the risk
of criminal victimization, portraying the world as a “scary medium” (Reiner, 2002). Thus, the
greater the consumption, the greater the likelihood that consumers’ perceptions of the real world
will align with what is represented in the media. Several studies have attempted to test the
theory of cultivation, among which Dolliver et al. (2018) and Boda and Szabó (2011), and both
found a strong connection between consumption and fear of crime. This result may indicate that
the vision presented by the participants echoes the picture shown by the media, which portrays
life as dangerous and unpredictable. Nevertheless, the relevance and empirical support of the cul-
tivation theory has been mixed (e.g., Potter, 2014). For instance, a meta-analysis employed by
Shanahan and Morgan (1999) found that the television exposure predicted only 1% of the variation
in the cultivation indicators.
An interesting alternative to this theory as an explanation for the fear of public crime is that people
use their personal experience or the experience of others on their social networks to decide whether or
not they should be concerned with crime (Romer et al., 2003; Sacco, 1995). According to the
Substitution Thesis, “greater exposure to exaggerated representations of crime on television, in the
absence of other more direct sources of information, can lead to assimilation of high perceptions
of the occurrence of crime” (Weaver & Wakshlag, 1986, pp. 153–154; Romer et al., 2003). This
theory is based on the framework of cultivation, but additionally takes into account the characteristics
of the public, maintaining that media stories related to crime cause greater fear among individuals
without experience of personal victimization (Weaver & Wakshlag, 1986; Weitzer & Kubrin,
2004). In the study by Intravia et al. (2017) there was a positive and significant relationship
between consumption and their fear of crime, for individuals who reported feeling “very safe”
walking alone in their neighborhood at night.
Contrary to the previous approach, the Resonance Thesis states that the media will increase fear
when the content is consistent with personal experience (Weitzer & Kubrin, 2004). Thus, the media
reinforces what individuals experience in the real world. As Eschholz (1997, p. 47) states: “[individ-
uals] living in areas with a high level of criminality may be particularly sensitive to crime on televi-
sion due to their direct knowledge of the crime problem in their neighborhoods”. This approach is
confirmed in the study by Doob and Macdonald (1979), where residents in high-crime urban
areas watched more television and, in general, tended to watch more violent content and to show
higher levels of fear.
6 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

In summary, when research into television’s relationship with fear began, Gerbner and Gross
(1976) argued that television programing contained a relatively homogenous ideological message
that “cultivated” a more or less generic view of the world. Since then, other perspectives have
emerged to designate that the TV/fear relationship can vary substantially by the specific content
of television programing and by the specific sociocultural composition of its viewers, acting in
harmony with other social factors that may control the presence, extent and meaning of the effects
of the media (Gunter, 2008). It was then started to assess whether the “media effects” would be con-
sistent or variable by the characteristics of the audience (Eschholz et al., 2003), realizing that both the
social reality of each one and the sociodemographic characteristics, namely age and gender are
important factors in shaping these experiences, which indicates that these factors must therefore
affect the resonance of images transmitted by the media and their ability to cultivate an individual’s
view of the world (Cohen & Weimann, 2000).

Factors and Processes Involved in the Meanings Attributed to the Media


In order to understand how consumers are affected by the media, it is useful to explore what makes
individuals view media content (Zillman & Wakshlag, 1985). Furthermore, the way in which indi-
viduals perceive the mode of transmission and the content of the news, together with the interpreta-
tion that each person makes of the facts portrayed in the information product, are equally relevant
aspects for a better understanding of the role of the media in the fear of crime. Furthermore, individ-
uals may also be subconsciously influenced by hegemonic interpretations of the content of news and
the facts portrayed in the information product.
The framing of news by the media, namely the sensationalism involved, is one of the elements
suggested as crucial to the level of fear of crime in the public (Warr, 1994, 2000). In fact, the way
in which the media represent crime often includes “sensationalist” aspects, which focus on neg-
ative images of crime in communities (Heath & Gilbert, 1996). Associated with the view of news
as sensationalists is also the perception of them as realistic. Potter (1986, p. 161) states that
viewers “… who believe that television content is real are more likely to be influenced by it
than viewers who believe that content is fictional”. Both the sensationalism and the realism asso-
ciated with the transmitted content are elements often related with other factor - the perceived
credibility of the source -, which can play an important role in the fear of crime (Rogers,
2005). Thus, if the source is perceived as less credible, the information, although it may be reliable
itself, is likely to be discredited regarding the implications on insecurity, leading people to “label”
it and declare it less relevant.
Other factors that influence the effects of mass media presentations on fear of crime are also the
identification of the reader with those involved in the episode portrayed, such as the association with
the same social group (e.g., the same nationality for the reader) and for one of the parties involved
(Weber, 2014). On the same line, knowing the victim, especially when living in the same area, can
have a much more immediate impact than hearing about a crime in a distant area (and being an
unknown/random victim) (Hale, 1996). Additionally, the location of the reported crime (Heath &
Gilbert, 1996; Koomen et al., 2000) also influences the levels of fear. As an example, in the study
by Heath (1984) it was found that the reports on crimes, although apparently “bizarre”, about
cities other than those of residence of the participants in his study, reassured them and reduced
their fear.
In conclusion, individuals bring to the interpretation of the experience their own predispositions
and the content learned on their social networks, which influence what the news about crime means to
them. These predispositions can include personal experiences with crime or violence, perceptions of
the credibility of the media or the extension of the previous concern with personal security (Sacco,
1995).
Silva and Guedes 7

Method
Sample
For the constitution of the sample, two criteria of access to individuals were taken into consideration.
On the one hand, theoretical sampling was used, in which the criteria that define its nature were based
both on the literature and on the research questions previously mentioned (Mason, 2002). In addition
to the aforementioned sampling, intentional sampling was also used, since there was an active search
in the choice of participants in view of the objectives and research questions, as well as accessibility
in accessing them (Coyne, 1997). Twenty individuals were invited to participate, having been ini-
tially contacted by telephone, and were intentionally chosen according to the aforementioned criteria,
with no refusal of participants. Moreover, these respondents were asked to indicate other potential
participants (snow-ball sampling), who were then contacted by researchers.
In total, 20 individuals were interviewed, of which 12 were female and 8 were male. During the sam-
pling process, we sought to ensure as much heterogeneity as possible, based on key variables such as
age, gender and area of residence (rural and urban), in order to obtain a greater diversity of perspectives.
Concerning the area of residence, thirteen individuals were from two Portuguese rural areas, and seven
were residents from an urban city in Portugal. Regarding age, it is possible to observe four main groups:
nine individuals aged between 20 and 25 years old, four individuals aged between 26 and 45 years old,
five individuals between 46 and 55 years old and two individuals between 56 and 60 years old. The
professional occupancy was also diverse across sample. In fact, seven were students and the remaining
thirteen had different jobs in various areas such as law, engineering, home care, mechanics, among
others. After conducting the interviews, it was possible to distinguish that eleven individuals had
direct victimization experiences and nine had no past victimization experience. Among those who
revealed to have had an experience of victimization (in the attempted and / or effective way), five indi-
viduals reported having been victims of theft, robbery or attempted robbery, two of sexual harassment,
three of threats to physical integrity or persecution and one of physical aggression.

Methodological Procedures
The individuals that compose the sample were interviewed between December 2018 and January
2019. First, the objectives of the study were briefly explained, trying to avoid any bias in the expla-
nation of the same. Respondents were asked about their willingness to participate in the investigation,
were informed about the issues of anonymity and confidentiality, as well as the audio recording and
the possibility of withdrawing at any time, without prejudice of any kind. After clarification, inter-
views were scheduled, taking place in the participants’ homes or in public spaces, as it was impos-
sible to implement the first hypothesis. These had an average duration of 30–60 min.

Interview Script and Analysis


To achieve the specified objectives, a qualitative approach was used in the form of semi-structured
interviews, which provides the discovery of rich data and patterns that have not been studied. The
interviews were based on a script previously developed according to the objectives that we intended
to explore, in order to boost the discourse, and at same time allowing the opening to the emergence of
new topics, as long as they are related to the research theme. The guide was divided into three fun-
damental parts: (a) main dimensions, (b) sub-dimensions and (c) proposal of questions. Within the
central dimensions, we explored (i) the meanings associated with (in) security; (ii) social contexts
and fear figures; (iii) victimization experiences; (iv) media consumption habits; (v) the perceptions
about the media and (vi) the consequences of the consumption of the media. For each general
8 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

category, some important subcategories or topics to be explored have been defined, as well as some
questions have been suggested.
Regarding the content of the script, we planned to ask around 30 questions, varying between open
and semi-closed questions. These were some examples of the questions: “In your perspective, what
distinguishes a “safe” environment from an “unsafe” environment?”, “Could you tell me about places
close to your area of residence where you like to be?”, “What aspects of these places make you feel
safe?”, “What kind of news draw your attention?”, “What characteristics must a piece of news have
in order for you to trust and take it seriously?”, “What do you feel when you see or hear news about a
crime that took place in Portugal?” and “Now imagine that it is a news story about a crime that took
place in your area of residence. What aspects of the news would make you most worried/fearful?”.
In this study, the data analysis procedure followed the Thematic Analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006)
starting with the transcription of the interviews. The audio recording of the interviews, at this stage,
prevented the loss of information. The next step was to read the transcripts of the interviews and to
build synopses that allowed, at first, to draw some conclusions, classify ideas into categories and
identify potential relationships between them. Then, during the readings and in conjunction with
the research questions, codes were created, constituting the stage called codification. After the
codes were created, the excerpts from the interviews were associated with those, some of which
were repeated, as they were suitable for more than one code. This whole process - called thematic
analysis - involved data reduction by identifying patterns, describing and classifying data into
themes or categories that allow for a broader interpretation of the data. These categories denote infor-
mation units composed of situations, events and instances that place the participants’ experiences in
the context under analysis (Lindseth & Norberg, 2004).

Results
Media Consumption Habits and Perceptions
Starting with the central topic of this study, we sought, within the study of the habits of media con-
sumption, to unveil the type of media most viewed, distinguishing between the use of traditional
means of communication such as radio, television and newspapers, and the use of social media
like the internet, and social networks. Interestingly, it was found that all individuals who used tradi-
tional media exclusively lived in rural areas and were aged between 51 and 60 years. This result is in
line with the general understanding of the literature that social media, namely the internet, are rapidly
becoming an important means of communication, especially among younger individuals (Intravia
et al., 2017; Roche et al., 2016), while traditional means of accessibility in rural areas make their (tra-
ditional media) use more frequent. The following discourse reflect the distance of individual from the
rural areas to the internet (namely, social networks):

“I am already a person who is not twenty years old (laughs), I come from a time when the internet is not
for me, the internet started for my children… I am not even aware of it, I do not have Facebook and etc…”
(Female, 52 years old, rural area)

For a better understanding of the role of the media in the daily lives of the sample, we also
explored how the interviewees of this study perceived the media in the transmission of news. In
the analysis of the discourses, we found similar perceptions in most of the sample - an absence of
credibility associated with a series of factors, especially sensationalism. This was clearly expressed
in the discourses of the interviewees, justified, on the one hand, by an extreme intrusion of private
life, with excessively detailed news and, on the other hand, by a strong language and visual
content used by the media. Accordingly, Sacco (1995) argues that, when it comes to crime-related
Silva and Guedes 9

news, individuals tend to show some skepticism, interpreting and contrasting what they see in the
media with their experiences and experiences of their interpersonal networks. Therefore, individuals
of the present study reported strategies such as the crossing of sources (i.e., consulting more than one
source regarding the same news) as a “defense mechanism” for possible false news and the impor-
tance of scientific basis, official sources and research to counter common sense that is easily trans-
mitted. Let’s see some examples in the following excerpts:

“I think they are sometimes broadcast in a slightly sensational way… I think they sometimes take excerpts
from interviews to make headlines about it and I think they use certain words or certain very strong
images to take very radical positions…”
(Female, 22 years old, urban area)

“I think a good way to prove the truth is to try to readily cross sources and different means and see what
can be taken from there, and what is a lie, or what has been exaggerated and so on.”
(Male, 28 years old, rural area)

Hierarchization of Means of Communication


One of the important conclusions that emerged through individual’s discourses was the spontaneous
way in which a large part of the sample hierarchized the means they choose to inform themselves.
This hierarchy has a key criterion that assess the credibility that is attributed to the means, as the
most legitimate to inform about the facts. For example, for four respondents (n = 4), the news
watched on television serve to confirm the veracity of the news that are disseminated by other
means. A possible interpretation for this result may be the official and reliable character that individ-
uals attribute to the television news, increasing confidence in this mean, for instance, highlighted by a
woman of the sample:

“I think that there are certain sources that by themselves already give us some loyalty … for example, we
consider the newscast… this news are a bit more real and that is transmitted by people to whom we
already give some legitimacy…”
(Female, 20 years old, rural area)

In the study of McCracken (2011), the role of credibility in the frequency of consumption
from different means is emphasized. In fact, the results of the study revealed, concerned to
local television news, a relevant relationship between the frequency with which local television
news is consumed and their perception of credibility. On the other hand, in the current study, and
regarding the opinion that the sample reveals about social media (internet and social networks),
the perception of the existence of false news was highlighted. Indeed, for all users and non-users,
it is considered as a mean of communication, but not so much as a mean of preferential
information.

“The internet in my case is not a mean that I use a lot for news because I think we’re going a bit too far… I
don’t think it’s very credible (…) there are a lot of false news, people misrepresent the facts…”
(Male, 25 years old, rural area)

Meanings of Fear Considering the Nature and Extent of News


Throughout the conversations, it was also possible to suggest that the fear of crime may addition-
ally depend on the nature and extent of local news and media content. Accordingly, Jackson (2011)
10 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

refers that, when hearing about specific episodes of crime, which are brought home in a very close
way - as if that could happen to you - individuals can then develop personalized representations of
the risk of crime, through perception of serious consequences, lack of control and high probability
of occurrence. In the study by Eschholz and colleagues (2003), the local violent crimes portrayed in
the news for example, although less frequent, indicated a greater fear of crime, due to the combi-
nation of the proximity and the realism of the episodes. Therefore, local crime news may be asso-
ciated with a higher level of fear of crime for local residents compared to non-local crime news, and
may even lead to these people commenting more on this news on their social media (e.g., on
Twitter) (Kounadi et al., 2015). Accordingly, in the present investigation, for the majority of the
sample (n = 16) it was found that the area of occurrence of the reported episode plays a major
role, at least in the greater perception of risk. Here are two examples that shows this perception
of risk:

“… in fact, only when it happens very close do we probably start to think that that reality that only hap-
pened to others is getting closer.”
(Male, 48 years old, rural area)

“The local news also makes me more afraid (…) As I am a very negative person, I am sure that the first
thing I would think was: “well if this arrived here, it will certainly happen to me!” So it would have a lot of
implications for my life and I would be even more concerned.”
(Female, 22 years old, urban area)

In addition to the relevance we found in relation to the location of the reported crime, the identi-
fication characteristics (n = 4) emerged as important, interpreted as knowing the victim and/or the
offender and spontaneously arose as enhancers of a greater perception of risk and fear of crime,
as it is a closer reality. Interestingly, this aspect was mentioned by individuals only from rural
areas, and this result can be understood under the perspective that considers the importance of
social ties and integration in the community - characteristics mostly associated with rural areas in
the literature (e.g., Little et al., 2005; Riger et al., 1981). These social ties can be relevant in the
role that knowing the victim and/or the offender might have in the fear of crime, as these two indi-
viduals point out:

“If I know the victim or the criminal, it makes me feel more afraid, it is a reality closer to me…”
(Female, 55 years old, rural area)

“These things that a person hears, regarding other merchants, or other customers, in this case my
brother… a person knows that and ends up being also afraid that the same thing may happen to you…”
(Female, 52 years old, rural area)

Finally, for nine individuals of the sample (n = 9), the type of crime reported has different mean-
ings, often when it comes to the crimes they fear most. At this point, terrorism stood out as a type of
crime feared for its “worldwide” character and for being “increasingly present”. It is therefore rea-
sonable to assume that the media plays a significant role in fear of terrorism, compared to other
types of crime, since it is a rare phenomenon for a large part of the areas of residence, serving the
media as the source of information more accessible about the topic (Nellis & Savage, 2012). We
now show an excerpt on the above:

“World terrorism organizations are what scared me the most (…) in the new year I was in the Aliados
[famous square in the city of Porto] and I was talking to the person who I was with me… this would be
Silva and Guedes 11

a spectacular place to attack (…) for terrorist acts there is no defense possible… just be in the wrong place, at
the wrong time… and it is more worrying in that respect… ordinary people they can’t do anything.”
(Female, 22 years old, urban area)

Experiences and Meanings of Fear Beyond Media


When expressing the meanings of insecurity, gender differences are revealed. On the one hand,
in women, we find a tendency to describe a constant distrust and discomfort in certain environ-
ments or situations, such as unknown places, places dimly illuminated, isolated or marked by
conflicts.

“Feeling afraid is walking on the street and feeling that we are being persecuted, feeling that someone is
coming towards us, someone who is looking at us strangely…”
(Female, 52 years old, rural area)

On the other hand, for men, insecurity results in a feeling of lack of control in circumstances that they
describe as threatening. This result may fit in with what Day and colleagues (2013) observed in their
study, where male subjects identified their apprehension as a feeling of being “out of control”. These
individuals revealed the importance of constant attention to the surrounding environment, that is, sur-
veillance, which allows the anticipation of possible confrontations, similar to what men reported in the
present study. These different ideas were expressed, among others, in the examples below:

“A safe environment is being among people who … I know who are close to me (…) I like going to places
I already know who works and who frequents them … I like it to feel like a family environment.”
(Male, 56 years old, rural area)

“One way to feel safe personally is to avoid situations of conflict, discussion, situations that I know can
have bad consequences for me.”

(Male, 45 years old, rural area)

Throughout the conversations, it was also possible to highlight another difference in the expres-
sions of insecurity for men and women. In general, women expressed greater feelings of “fear” or
states of “alertness”. In males, there was a reluctance to pronounce the expression “fear” in the expo-
sure they made of their insecurities. In the same vein, the results of the qualitative studies by Madriz
(1997) and Guedes (2016) found the evidence of greater suppression of the expression “fear” in
males and the avoidance of confrontations where they feel they can lose control. These findings
may be justified in line with what we saw earlier in the literature framework - men tend to be less
willing to admit to others their concerns and their perception of vulnerability, choosing to omit
their real levels of fear (Koomen et al., 2000; Sutton & Farrall, 2004). These results can then be
explained by a question of “social desirability”, associated with a status and self-representation
that men seek to maintain. For example, Sutton and Farrall (2004) found in their study that men
are more concerned with distorting their responses for reasons of self-representation reported
lower levels of fear. The next example highlights this type of discourse:

“(…) we are careful to close the doors, of course we guarantee that we have lighting around the house, but
this is almost the normal procedure, it is not a matter of fear, it is almost a mandatory thing. I’m not afraid,
I still don’t see the need here to feel fear.”
(Male, 48 years old, rural area)
12 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

Finally, there seems to be no gender differences in this study taking into consideration the four
(two men and two women) individual’s perception of importance of the place of residence as a
source of security, when individuals are from the rural environment. There is agreement as to
their perception of the area where they live as calm, peaceful, where there is trust in the people
who live in it and, as such, the concerns they accuse are of a mostly economic and family nature.

“I feel safe because I also live in a rural environment, I have no security issues, or I have never had it to
this day. I can’t say there are reasons to feel insecure…”
(Female, 55 years old, rural area)

“I have other concerns that I find more relevant, my family, work, health (…)”

(Male, 60 years old, rural area)

Discussion
The present study aimed at explore the interpretations and meanings attributed to the media and its
role on fear of crime, unraveling possible gender and residential areas differences. For that, 20 qual-
itative interviews were undertaken to a diversified Portuguese sample. In general, two main results in
accordance with the previous literature were found: firstly, the almost exclusive use of traditional
media (e.g., television news) by individuals from the rural environment (Intravia et al., 2017;
Roche et al., 2016) and, secondly, that individuals have the general idea that the media are sensation-
alist and that they should be suspicious of their content (Heath & Gilbert, 1996). For example,
Koomen and colleagues (2000) found that the effect of a newspaper article on the fear of theft,
fear of crime in general and concern about theft as a social problem depended on the credibility
of the mean. Similarly, in the study by Zhu et al., (2018), readers of the non-sensationalist press
had a higher perception of the crime than those who preferred the more sensationalist newspapers.
In addition, individuals with greater awareness also showed less fear of crime. The study results
thus indicate that readers of a less sensationalist press are more likely to be more aware of the
“big picture” of reality. Consequently, and based on this detail, it becomes more likely that these
readers will move on to a more objective analysis, thus harboring less fear. In the study by
Purcell et al., (2010) around 49% of individuals said they preferred to receive direct and objective
news, while 31% preferred sources that shared their point of view.
While exploring the perceptions of the sample concerning the social media, the view of the exis-
tence of fake news stood out. The same was verified in the study by Intravia et al. (2017), for
example, in which individuals showed to be aware of the circulation of false information on the
Internet. Therefore, in the present study, it was observed that individuals use other means to
obtain reliable information about crime, along with the study by Hollis et al. (2017) where when
assessing how much people acquire their information about crime on social networks and on the
internet, they found that traditional media sources and official crime rates in the city are more impor-
tant than information from social media sources.
This study also confirms the notion that the fear of crime may depend additionally on the nature
and extent of local news and media content. This can be justified by the fact that individuals use local
crime news as a reference point to understand the environment around them (Yanich, 2001). In this
regard, Näsi and colleagues’ study (2021) reflects the fact that a large part of people are not passive
consumers of crime news, that is, individuals can actively seek this content. Moreover, for this
sample, local news, compared to others, produces the greatest risk perception, triggers the adoption
of diverse security measures and increases fear of crime. Moreover, it can be argued that the role of
the media in fear of crime depends on the characteristics of individuals, place of residence and
Silva and Guedes 13

experiences of victimization (direct and indirect). Therefore, our results are aligned with Banks
(2005)’s ideas that for each home the consumption, interpretation and “effects” of the reported
crime should be considered within the local context of each one. Thus, in this study, both the
Resonance Thesis and the Substitution Thesis help to explain the differential impact of the media.
Notwithstanding the results described above, there are studies that believe that the influence of the
media is secondary rather than direct, and that it is difficult to find direct causal effects of the media
on fear of crime (Elmas, 2021). In Debraelet and colleagues’ study (2021), for example, greater news
consumption did not always result in more fear of terrorism, suggesting that the power of opinion
formation through the media should be relativized, as only limited correlations were found to specific
types of media (e.g., television news exposure and more fear of terrorism). Thus, the media are a
dynamic social institution whose effect on fear of crime will remain difficult to fully demonstrate.
That is, the degree and way in which public fears are influenced by the media have been studied,
but open questions remain (Perkins & Taylor, 2002).
Beyond the media, in general, the entire sample transported their meanings of insecurity to a phys-
ical, social and/or temporal context, through clues or signs that could be perceived as threatening. In
females, a tendency to describe a constant distrust and discomfort in certain environments was found.
For men, insecurity caused a feeling of lack of control in circumstances they describe as threatening.
In terms of the environment, people often consider their area of residence or even the house itself as a
safe place because both denote a sense of ownership of the space (Lupton, 1999). In this study, indi-
viduals from rural areas were safer in their areas of residence. Within the area of residence, tranquility
and good relations with the neighborhood – highlighted as an essential informal social control mech-
anism – were also factors that emerged as associated with the security they attribute to the environ-
ment in which they live. In the words of Swatt and colleagues (2013), it creates the physical and
social context for interactions among neighborhood residents that help shape notions of “commu-
nity” and individuals’ perceptions of their broader social space. Therefore, we can consider that
regarding the view of the place of residence as a source of security, residents of rural areas do not
have gender has an influence. Here, the focus is on people’s assessments of the extent to which
crime and disorder are real problems for their community, and in several studies, individuals consider
their home zone to be safer compared to others (Skogan, 1999).
As factors of insecurity, the sample reported fear of the unknown, physical disorders, conflicts and
the night. In Lupton’s (1999) study for example, for most people who felt insecure, the night was a
particularly “unsafe” period, because space was not as visible and darkness brought with it a sense of
threat. Therefore, this feeling induces a response pattern that focuses on avoiding threatening places.
Notwithstanding the relevance of the results discussed above, it is important to emphasize the
main limitation of the study, namely the use of a small sample size (n = 20). The issue of generaliz-
ability in qualitative research has been highly debated (Maxwell, 2020; Polit & Beck, 2010). Even
though researchers are interested in providing a contextualized understanding of the human experi-
ences and feelings through particular cases, not many of them are in accordance with the importance
of generalization of the results. Strategies such as replication of studies, employing larger sample
sizes and mixed-methods have been suggested to increase the capacity of generalization.
Therefore, the results of the present study must be read in light of the specific context where it
was developed and further investigation is needed to confirm our assumptions.

Conclusions
This qualitative study presents a set of conclusions and implications that must be highlighted. First, in
spite of the incontestable importance of the internet as a means of communication, especially among
younger people, the traditional media in rural environments continue to be relevant source of infor-
mation and more frequently used. Second, this research suggests the need to deeply explore the issue
14 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

of credibility of the news in general, which seems to be related to aspects such as sensationalism. The
rapidly increase of fake news (mainly noticed in social media) produces the need to cross sources by
individuals as a defense mechanism. Accordingly, we conclude that people hierarchize the media
means based on credibility – for instance, news watched on television confirm the veracity of
news read on social networks. Important conclusions are also drawn when analyzing how the
media shapes fear of crime experiences. In fact, the present study suggests the area of occurrence
of the episodes disseminated by the news plays a major role. Concretely, knowing the victim and/
or the offender and the location of the news were aspects that differentially influenced l aspects
that influenced fear of crime and risk perceptions. Finally, and consistent with many previous
studies, the vulnerability hypothesis may be adequate to understand higher fear of crime in
women reported by our sample. It is argued that the effects of the media will be greater among indi-
viduals who perceive themselves as defenseless in the face of criminal victimization, when women
and the elderly are often mentioned in this position. The same was evidenced mostly in individuals
who had experienced past direct victimization.
Finally, it should be noted that frequent communication about crime facilitates preventive behav-
ior, in turn, reaching individuals in general, and not just those who have suffered any type of victim-
ization. In this study, the use of preventive measures emerged as a routine behavior and when faced
with the possibility of a crime in the area where they live - through news - the main consequence
announced by the subjects was the reinforcement of defensive behaviors and / or protection and
avoidance.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests


The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/
or publication of this article.

Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
article.

ORCID iD
Inês Guedes https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4804-9394

References
Altheide, D. L. (2003). Mass media, crime, and the discourse of fear. The Hedgehog Review, 5(3), 9–26.
Banks, M. (2005). Spaces of (in) security: Media and fear of crime in a local context. Crime, Media, Culture,
1(2), 169–187. https://doi.org/10.1177/1741659005054020
Boda, Z., & Szabó, G. (2011). The media and attitudes towards crime and the justice system: A qualitative
approach. European Journal of Criminology, 8(4), 329–342. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F147737081141
1455
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative research in psychology,
3(2), 77–101.
Campbell, A. (2005). Keeping the ‘lady’safe: The regulation of femininity through crime prevention literature.
Critical Criminology, 13(2), 119–140. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10612-005-2390-z
Cashmore, J. (2014). The ‘fear of crime-media feedback’ cycle. Internet Journal of Criminology, 1, 1–19.
Chadee, D. (2001). Fear of crime and the media: From perceptions to reality. Criminal Justice Matters, 43(1),
10–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/09627250108552955
Chadee, D., & Ditton, J. (2005). Fear of crime and the media: Assessing the lack of relationship. Crime, Media,
Culture, 1(3), 322–332. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1741659005057644
Silva and Guedes 15

Cohen, J., & Weimann, G. (2000). Cultivation revisited: Some genres have some effects on some viewers.
Communication Reports, 13(2), 99–114. https://doi.org/10.1080/08934210009367728
Coyne, I. T. (1997). Sampling in qualitative research. Purposeful and theoretical sampling; merging or clear
boundaries? Journal of Advanced Nursing, 26(3), 623–630. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2648.1997.
t01-25-00999.x
Day, K., Stump, C., & Carreon, D. (2003). Confrontation and loss of control: Masculinity and men’s fear in
public space. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23(3), 311–322. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-
4944(03)00024-0
Debrael, M., d’Haenens, L., De Cock, R., & De Coninck, D. (2021). Media use, fear of terrorism, and attitudes
towards immigrants and refugees: Young people and adults compared. International Communication
Gazette, 83(2), 148–168. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1748048519869476
Dolliver, M. J., Kenney, J. L., Reid, L. W., & Prohaska, A. (2018). Examining the relationship between media
consumption, fear of crime, and support for controversial criminal justice policies using a nationally repre-
sentative sample. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 34(4), 399–420. https://doi.org/10.1177%
2F1043986218787734
Doob, A. N., & Macdonald, G. E. (1979). Television viewing and fear of victimization: Is the relationship
causal? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(2), 170. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/
0022-3514.37.2.170
Elmas, M. S. (2021). Perceived risk of terrorism, indirect victimization, and individual-level determinants of fear
of terrorism. Security Journal, 34(3), 498–524. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41284-020-00242-6
Eschholz, S. (1997). The media and fear of crime: A survey of the research. University of Florida. Journal of
Law and Public Policy, 9, 37–59.
Eschholz, S., Chiricos, T., & Gertz, M. (2003). Television and fear of crime: Program types, audience traits, and
the mediating effect of perceived neighborhood racial composition. Social Problems, 50(3), 395–415. https://
doi.org/10.1525/sp.2003.50.3.395
Evans, D., & Fletcher, M. (2000). Fear of crime: Testing alternative hypotheses. Applied Geography, 20(4),
395–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0143-6228(00)00011-4
Farrall, S., Gray, E., & Jackson, J. (2007). Theorising the fear of crime: The cultural and social significance of
insecurities about crime. Experience & Expression in the Fear of Crime. Working Paper, (5). https://doi.org/
10.2139/ssrn.1012393
Farrall, S. D., Jackson, J., & Gray, E. (2009). Social order and the fear of crime in contemporary times. Oxford
University Press.
Ferraro, K. F. (1995). Fear of crime: Interpreting victimization risk. SUNY press.
Ferraro, K. F. (1996). Women’s fear of victimization: Shadow of sexual assault? Social Forces, 75(2), 667–690.
https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/75.2.667
Ferraro, K. F., & LaGrange, R. (1987). The measurement of fear of crime. Sociological Inquiry, 57(1), 70–97.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-682X.1987.tb01181.x
Franklin, T. W., Franklin, C. A., & Fearn, N. E. (2008). A multilevel analysis of the vulnerability, disorder, and
social integration models of fear of crime. Social Justice Research, 21(2), 204–227. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11211-008-0069-9
Gabriel, U., & Greve, W. (2003). The psychology of fear of crime. Conceptual and methodological perspectives.
British Journal of Criminology, 43(3), 600–614. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/43.3.600
Gerbner, G., & Gross, L. (1976). Living with television: The violence profile. Journal of Communication, 26(2),
172–199. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1976.tb01397.x
Guedes, I. M. E. (2016). Medo do Crime: Emergência, Reações Emocionais e Dircursos. Contributos para a
Utilização de Multi-Metodologias [Doctoral dissertation]. Universidade do Porto.
Guedes, I. M. E. S., Domingos, S. P. A., & Cardoso, C. S. (2018). Fear of crime, personality and trait emotions:
An empirical study. European Journal of Criminology, 15(6), 658–679. https://doi.org/10.1177%
2F1477370817749500
16 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

Gunter, B. (2008). Media violence: Is there a case for causality? American Behavioral Scientist, 51(8), 1061–
1122. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0002764207312007
Hale, C. (1996). Fear of crime: A review of the literature. International Review of Victimology, 4(2), 79–150.
https://doi.org/10.1177%2F026975809600400201
Hanslmaier, M. (2013). Crime, fear and subjective well-being: How victimization and street crime affect fear
and life satisfaction. European Journal of Criminology, 10(5), 515–533. https://doi.org/10.1177%
2F1477370812474545
Heath, L. (1984). Impact of newspaper crime reports on fear of crime: Multimethodological investigation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(2), 263–276. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.2.263
Heath, L., & Gilbert, K. (1996). Mass media and fear of crime. American Behavioral Scientist, 39(4), 379–386.
Hill, G. D., Howell, F. M., & Driver, E. T. (1985). Gender, fear and protective handgun ownership. Criminology:
An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23(3), 541–552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1985.tb00353.x
Hollis, M. E., Downey, S., del Carmen, A., & Dobbs, R. R. (2017). The relationship between media portrayals
and crime: Perceptions of fear of crime among citizens. Crime Prevention and Community Safety, 19(1), 46–
60. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41300-017-0015-6
Intravia, J., Wolff, K. T., Paez, R., & Gibbs, B. R. (2017). Investigating the relationship between social media
consumption and fear of crime: A partial analysis of mostly young adults. Computers in Human Behavior,
77, 158–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.047
Jackson, J. (2011). Revisiting risk sensitivity in the fear of crime. Journal of research in crime and delinquency,
48(4), 513–537.
Jackson, J., & Gouseti, I. (2012). Fear of crime: An entry to the encyclopedia of theoretical criminology. In
Miller, J. M. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of theoretical criminology (pp. 1594–1602). Wiley-Blackwell.
Koomen, W., Visser, M., & Stapel, D. A. (2000). The credibility of newspapers and fear of crime. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 30(5), 921–934. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02503.x
Kounadi, O., Lampoltshammer, T. J., Groff, E., Sitko, I., & Leitner, M. (2015). Exploring Twitter to analyze the
public’s reaction patterns to recently reported homicides in London. PloS one, 10(3), e0121848.
LaGrange, R. L., Ferraro, K. F., & Supancic, M. (1992). Perceived risk and fear of crime: Role of social and
physical incivilities. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 29(3), 311–334. https://doi.org/10.
1177%2F0022427892029003004
Leal, P. (2010). O sentimento de insegurança na discursividade sobre o crime. Sociologias, 12(23), 394–427.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1517-45222010000100014
Lindseth, A., & Norberg, A. (2004). A phenomenological hermeneutical method for researching lived experi-
ence. Scandinavian journal of caring sciences, 18(2), 145–153.
Liska, A. E., & Baccaglini, W. (1990). Feeling safe by comparison: Crime in the newspapers. Social Problems,
37(3), 360–374. https://doi.org/10.2307/800748
Little, J., Panelli, R., & Kraack, A. (2005). Women’s fear of crime: A rural perspective. Journal of Rural Studies,
21(2), 151–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrurstud.2005.02.001
Lupton, D. (1999). Dangerous places and the unpredictable stranger: Constructions of fear of
crime. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 32(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1177%
2F000486589903200102
Lupton, D., & Tulloch, J. (1999). Theorizing fear of crime: Beyond the rational/irrational opposition. The British
Journal of Sociology, 50(3), 507–523. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-4446.1999.00507.x
Madriz, E. (1997). Nothing bad happens to good girls: Fear of crime in women’s lives. University of California
Press.
Mason, J. (2002). Qualitative researching (2nd Edition). Sage Publications.
Mastrorocco, N., & Minale, L. (2018). News media and crime perceptions: Evidence from a natural experiment.
Journal of Public Economics, 165, 230–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2018.07.002
Maxwell, J. (2020). Why qualitative methods are necessary for generalization. American Psychological
Association, 8(1), 111–118. https://doi.org/10.1037/qup0000173
Silva and Guedes 17

McCracken, B. (2011). Are new media credible? A multidimensional approach to measuring news consumers’
credibility and bias perceptions and the frequency of news consumption [Thesis Dissertation]. Rochester
Institute of Technology.
Miethe, T. D. (1995). Fear and withdrawal from urban life. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and
Social Science, 539(1), 14–27. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0002716295539001002
Näsi, M., Tanskanen, M., Kivivuori, J., Haara, P., & Reunanen, E. (2021). Crime news consumption and fear of
violence: the role of traditional media, social media, and alternative information sources. Crime &
Delinquency, 67(4), 574–600. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0011128720922539
Nellis, A. M., & Savage, J. (2012). Does watching the news affect fear of terrorism? The importance of media expo-
sure on terrorism fear. Crime & Delinquency, 58(5), 748–768. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0011128712452961
Pain, R. (2001). Gender, race, age and fear in the city. Urban Studies, 38(5–6), 899–913. https://doi.org/10.
1080%2F00420980120046590
Perkins, D. D., & Taylor, R. B. (2002). Ecological assessments of community disorder: Their relationship to fear
of crime and theoretical implications. In Ecological research to promote social change (pp. 127–170).
Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0565-5_6
Polit, D., & Beck, C. (2010). Generalization in quantitative and qualitative research: Myths and strategies.
International Journal of Nursing Studies, 47(11), 1451–1458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2010.06.004
Potter, W. J. (1986). Perceived reality and the cultivation hypothesis. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic
Media, 30(2), 159–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838158609386617
Potter, W. J. (2014). A critical analysis of cultivation theory. Journal of Communication, 64(6), 1015–1036.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12128
Purcell, K., Rainie, L., Mitchell, A., Rosenstiel, T., & Olmstead, K. (2010). Understanding the participatory
news consumer. Pew Internet and American Life Project, 1, 19–21.
Rader, N. E. (2004). The threat of victimization: A theoretical reconceptualization of fear of crime. Sociological
Spectrum, 24(6), 689–704. https://doi.org/10.1080/02732170490467936
Rader, N. E., May, D. C., & Goodrum, S. (2007). An empirical assessment of the “threat of victimization:”
Considering fear of crime, perceived risk, avoidance, and defensive behaviors. Sociological Spectrum,
27(5), 475–505. https://doi.org/10.1080/02732170701434591
Reiner, R. (2002). Media made criminality: The representation of crime in the mass media. In Reiner, Robert,
Maguire, Mike, & Morgan, Rod, (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of criminology (pp. 302–340). Oxford
University Press
Riger, S., LeBailly, R. K., & Gordon, M. T. (1981). Community ties and urbanites’ fear of crime: An ecological inves-
tigation. American Journal of Community Psychology, 9(6), 653–665. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00896247
Roche, S. P., Pickett, J. T., & Gertz, M. (2016). The scary world of online news? Internet news exposure
and public attitudes toward crime and justice. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 32(2), 215–236.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-015-9261-x
Rogers, T. (2005). Towards an analytical framework on fear of crime and its relationship to print media report-
age. Sheffield Online Papers in Social Research, 8.
Romer, D., Jamieson, K. H., & Aday, S. (2003). Television news and the cultivation of fear of crime. Journal of
Communication, 53(1), 88–104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb03007.x
Sacco, V. F. (1995). Media constructions of crime. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social
Science, 539(1), 141–154. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0002716295539001011
Shanahan, J., & Morgan, M. (1999). Television and its viewers: Cultivation theory and research. Cambridge
University Press.
Skogan, W. G. (1999, July). Measuring what matters: Crime, disorder, and fear. In Measuring what matters:
Proceedings from the Policing Research Institute meetings (pp. 37–53). National Institute of Justice
Washington, DC.
Skogan, W. G., & Maxfield, M. G. (1981). Coping with crime: Individual and neighborhood reactions. Sage
Publications.
18 Criminal Justice Review 0(0)

Soola, E. O. (2007). Coverage of crime and violence in Nigerian media: Matters arising. Global Media
Journal-African Edition, 1(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.5789/1-1-43
Srinivasa, K., & Thilagam, P. S. (2019). Crime base: Towards building a knowledge base for crime entities and
their relationships from online news papers. Information Processing & Management, 56(6), 102059. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2019.102059
Sutton, R. M., & Farrall, S. (2004). Gender, socially desirable responding and the fear of crime: Are women
really more anxious about crime? British Journal of Criminology, 45(2), 212–224. https://doi.org/10.1093/
bjc/azh084
Swatt, M. L., Varano, S. P., Uchida, C. D., & Solomon, S. E. (2013). Fear of crime, incivilities, and collective
efficacy in four Miami neighborhoods. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jcrimjus.2012.09.004
Vandeviver, C. (2011). Fear of crime in the EU-15 and Hungary: assessing the impact of vulnerability, victim-
ization and incivilities model on the fear of crime in a European cross-national context [Master Thesis].
Hogeschool-Universiteit Brussel.
Warr, M. (1987). Fear of victimization and sensitivity to risk. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 3(1), 29–46.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01065199
Warr, M. (1994). Public perceptions and reactions to violent offending and victimisation. In A. Reiss, & J. Roth
(Eds.), Understanding and preventing violence, volume 4: Consequences and control (pp. 1–66). National
Academy Press.
Warr, M. (2000). Fear of crime in the United States: Avenues for research and policy. Criminal Justice, 4(4),
451–489.
Weaver, J., & Wakshlag, J. (1986). Perceived vulnerability to crime, criminal victimization experience, and tele-
vision viewing. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 30(2), 141–158. https://doi.org/10.1080/
08838158609386616
Weber, P. (2014). Discussions in the comments section: Factors influencing participation and interactivity in
online newspapers’ reader comments. New Media & Society, 16(6), 941–957. https://doi.org/10.1177%
2F1461444813495165
Weitzer, R., & Kubrin, C. E. (2004). Breaking news: How local TV news and real-world conditions affect fear of
crime. Justice Quarterly, 21(3), 497–520. https://doi.org/10.1080/07418820400095881
Wilcox, P., May, D. C., & Roberts, S. D. (2006). Student weapon possession and the “fear and victimization
hypothesis”: Unraveling the temporal order. Justice Quarterly, 23(4), 502–529. https://doi.org/10.1080/
07418820600985362
Yanich, D. (2001). Location, location, location: Urban and suburban crime on local TV news. Journal of Urban
Affairs, 23(3–4), 221–241. https://doi.org/10.1111/0735-2166.00086
Zhu, R., Krever, R., & Choi, A. S. K. (2018). The impact of newspaper reports on fear of violent crime in Hong
Kong. Newspaper Research Journal, 0739532918806889. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0739532918806889
Zillmann, D., & Wakshlag, J. (1985). Fear of victimization and the appeal of crime drama. In D. Zillman, & J.
Bryant (Eds.), Selective exposure to communication (pp. 141–156). Erlbaum.

Author Biographies
Cynthia Silva is Degree and Master in Criminology from the Faculty of Law of the University of Porto.
Postgraduate in Crisis Intervention. Works at the Portuguese Association for Victim Support (APAV) as a
Project Officer in the field of violence prevention. Develops research in the areas of security, cybercrime and
victimology.

Inês Guedes is Master and PhD in Criminology from the Faculty of Law of the University of Porto (FDUP).
Assistant Professor at the same University. She is a founding and integrated member of the Center for
Interdisciplinary Research-Crime, Justice and Security (CJS) of the FDUP and a collaborating member of the
Center for Legal, Economic and Environmental Studies (CEJEA).

You might also like