Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Seizure: European Journal of Epilepsy 86 (2021) 147–151

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Seizure: European Journal of Epilepsy


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/seizure

Dr. Strangelove demystified: Disconnection of hand and language


dominance explains alien-hand syndrome after corpus callosotomy
Christoph Helmstaedter a, *, Lázló Solymosi b, Martin Kurthen c, Shahan Momjian d,
Karl Schaller d
a
Department of Epileptology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
b
Department of Neuroradiology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
c
Swiss Epilepsy Clinic Lengg, Zürich, Switzerland
d
Department of Neurosurgery, Geneva University Medical Center & Faculty of Medicine, Geneva, Switzerland

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Background: Alien hand syndrome (AHS) is a disabling condition in which one hand behaves in a way that the
Callosotomy person finds "alien". This feeling of alienation is related to the occurrence of movements of the respective hand
Alien hand performed without or against conscious intention. Most information on AHS stems from single case observations
Hemisphere dominance
in patients with frontal, callosal, or parietal brain damage.
Methods: Retrospective analysis of distinctive clinical features of three out of 18 epilepsy patients who developed
AHS with antagonistic movements of the left hand after corpus callosotomy (CC) (one anterior, two complete) for
the control of epileptic seizures, particularly epileptic drop attacks (EDA).
Results: Remarkably, these three patients, two men and one woman, displayed atypical language dominance with
a bilateral, left more than right hemisphere language representation in intracarotidal amobarbital testing before
surgery. The overall additional distinctive feature of the target patients was genuine left-handedness, with
writing retrained to right-handedness in two patients. After surgery the left hands became alien. The problem
was permanent, despite strategies for compensation.
Conclusion: From this observation we suggest that under the conditions of dissociation of language and motor
dominance, loss of both intentional control of contralateral action and physiological inhibition of antagonistic
movements lead to post-callosotomy alien-hand-like motor phenomena. The dissociation pattern posing this risk
seems rare but needs to be considered when evaluating candidates for callosotomy.

1. Introduction Meanwhile different subsyndromes are subsumed under "AHS", which,


in what follows, will refer to as alien-hand-like motor phenomena
Many clinical neuroscientists are familiar with the character of Dr. (AHLMP) [4–7].
Strangelove in the iconic Stanley Kubrick movie of 1964. During his Frontal, callosal, and posterior lesions are discussed as being causal
audition in the war room of the US government Strangelove’s right arm for AHS [7,8]. However, as variable as the underlying lesions are the
jumps up repeatedly for the “Hitlergruss”, while his left hand can hardly clinical manifestations of AHS. Risk factors for the occurrence of AHS,
cope to prevent these seemingly purposeful but involuntary movements. and particularly AHLMP, remain unknown. There is ongoing discussion
Later, this was coined to signify the presence of an alien hand syndrome as to whether a split brain harbors a split consciousness [9].
(AHS) in Dr. Strangelove by the referring literature [1]. Here we focus on callosal AHLMP with characteristic antagonistic
In a clinical context, the phenomenon of a hand (mostly the left, but behavior of the alien non-dominant hand. While AHLMP-variants have
in Strangelove the right) behaving in a contextual but unintended been identified as consequences of callosotomy [4,5] patterns of hemi­
manner, and seemingly detached of any voluntary control was described spheric specialization have not been addressed. Callosal disconnection
first by the German neurologist-psychiatrist Kurt Goldstein in the early phenomena gained major interest in the context of Sperry’s Nobel prize
20th century [2]. In the early 1970ies the term AHS was introduced [3]. winning split-brain observations and experiments in cats, monkeys, and

* Corresponding author at: Department of Epileptology, University Hospital Bonn, Building 83, Venusberg - Campus 1, 53127 Bonn, Germany.
E-mail addresses: C.Helmstaedter@uni-bonn.de, Christoph.Helmstaedter@ukbonn.de (C. Helmstaedter).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.seizure.2021.02.013
Received 19 January 2021; Received in revised form 10 February 2021; Accepted 11 February 2021
Available online 16 February 2021
1059-1311/© 2021 British Epilepsy Association. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This article is made available under the Elsevier license
(http://www.elsevier.com/open-access/userlicense/1.0/).
C. Helmstaedter et al. Seizure: European Journal of Epilepsy 86 (2021) 147–151

Table 1
Pre- and post-operative clinical and neuropsychological characteristics of all n = 18 patients, who underwent CC at the Epilepsy Center.
No-AHS AHS

N (%) 15 (83%) 3 (17%)


gender m/f (f%) 7/15 47% 1/3 33%
age (yrs.) 20 ± 9 30 ± 9
Education/IQ
- average 1 7% 1 33%
- borderline 2 13% 1 67%
- impaired 8 53% /
- severely impaired 4 27% /
Handedness
- right 13 86% /
- left 1 1 (writing with left)
- ambidextrous 1 /
- left retrained to right / 2 (writing with right)
age at epilepsy onset 8.6 ± 2 6.8 ± 5
Type of epilepsy/etiology:
- Lennox 5 33% 1
- cryptogenic 4 27% 1
- perinatal lesion 1
- migration disorder 1 1
- tuberous sclerosis 1
- idiopathic 1
- cryptogenic frontal 1
- post meningitis 1
Seizures:
- generalized & drop attacks 15 (100%) 3 (100%)
IAT language dominance:
- left dominant 10 (67%) (9 right-handed, one ambidextrous) /
- left > right dominant 3 (20%) (3 right-handed) 3 (100%) (3 genuine left handed)
- bilateral / /
- right dominant 2 (13%) (one right-handed, one left-handed) /
Callosotomy:
- anterior 7 47% 1 33%
- posterior 6 40% / /
- complete 2 13% 2 67%
4.1 ± 4 3.6 ± 3
Follow-Up interval (yrs.)
range: 1− 14 range: 2− 7
Postcallosotomy seizure control:
- drop attack free 7 53% 1 33%
- free of other seizures 1 7% / /
Cognitive development:
- improved 1 7% / /
- unchanged 8 53% 1 33%
- worsened 6 40% 2 67%

later in man, who contributed a lot to the understanding of the understand the mechanism of postoperative AHS observed in three pa­
higher-order, cognitive capacities of the non-dominant right hemisphere tients by the re-evaluation of the presurgical process and of the surgical
and the role of the corpus callosum in interhemispheric communication procedure in our epileptic patients, who underwent callosotomy for
[10]. treatment of their epileptic seizures and EDA.
The present report is not intended to explain alien hand in general.
We aim, however, to understand AHS in terms of a functional anatom­ 2.1. Patients
ical approach in a subset of patients who underwent corpus callosotomy
(CC) for otherwise intractable epilepsy (EDA). Most importantly, the We retrospectively report a series of 18 patients in whom CC – either
question of the determinants of AHS can be addressed more adequately anterior 2/3, or single-step complete, or two-step complete – was per­
when (atypical) functional hemisphere dominance patterns are taken formed (Table 1) in the years 1988–2000 at the Epilepsy Center in Bonn.
into consideration.
Microsurgical anterior 2/3 callosotomy and/or complete callos­ 2.2. Language dominance assessment
otomy are established disconnective epilepsy surgical techniques for the
treatment of otherwise intractable epilepsies with rapid bihemispheric All patients underwent intracarotidal amobarbital testing of lan­
seizure spread leading to epileptic drop attacks (EDA). Since discon­ guage dominance, the so called WADA Test, according to J. Wada who
nection symptoms may increase with the amount of corpus callosum first described this method in 1949 [14,15]. Language dominance was
transected some surgeons recommend starting anterior, others posterior assessed by consecutive anesthesia of the left and right hemisphere at
[11,12]. Notably, disconnection at the level of the SMA may cause AHS consecutive days assessing the following functions: series repetition,
[13]. comprehension of commands and questions, motor responses to com­
mands, naming, repetition, reading, spontaneous speech. Following
2. Methods and materials analyses of large series of patients who underwent WADA tests in Bonn,
Germany, a dominance index was routinely being calculated ranging
The goal of the present clinical study is to identify risk factors and to from –1 to 1, with scores -1 to -0.85 and 0.85 to 1 representing complete

148
C. Helmstaedter et al. Seizure: European Journal of Epilepsy 86 (2021) 147–151

right and left dominance, -0.5 to -0.85 and 0.5 to 0.85 representing WADA test indicated bilateral language organization with left hemi­
incomplete right or left dominance and -0.5 to 0.5 representing bilateral sphere predominance. In the left WADA expressive and receptive lan­
language dominance. Neuropsychological examinations, as far as a guage functions were partly possible. In the right WADA she was
standardized assessment was possible, at that time made use of the tests dysarthric, paraphasic and with impaired spontaneous speech and
described in the respective publications. [16,17]. Handedness, which, repetition performance. She underwent anterior 2/3 CC at age 40. After
apart from language dominance, is important for what follows, was surgery she experienced reversible lack of initiation, sensory aphasia,
categorized according the Oldfield Edinburgh inventory [18]. left-sided ataxia, and became right-handed since that time. Postoperative
neuropsychology indicated lasting impairments in attention and exec­
3. Results utive functions, verbal memory, and verbal comprehension. Over
twenty-year follow-up: No more drop attacks and completely seizure-
Out of the eighteen patients, n = 15 did not develop AHS, and three free under Lamotrigine, Valproate and Topiramate. After surgery she
did. As it can be taken from the table the three patients who developed couldn’t write any longer with her left hand and had to relearn writing
AHS shared two distinctive features compared to those who did not. First right-handed. Drawing and painting were continued to be performed
the three patients displayed atypical language dominance with left more with the left hand. Starting after surgery and continuing ever since, she
than right hemisphere language representation in intracarotidal experienced involuntary movements in actions which were intended
amobarbital testing before surgery. The overall additional distinctive with the right hand and were executed with the left hand. In part the left
feature of the target patients, however, was genuine left-handedness, hand performed antagonistic actions, i.e. if she wants to take the light
with writing retrained to right-handedness in two patients. After sur­ sweater from the wardrobe with her right hand, the left takes the woolen
gery the left hands became alien. pullover instead. Hence, she presented with agonistic dyspraxia and
intermanual conflict. No signs of independent apraxia.
3.1. Male patient BS
3.3. Male patient FB
Unremarkable birth and early childhood. Normal school and
apprenticeship as gardener. As child left-handed, however forced to learn to Unremarkable early childhood. As child left-handed. Was then forced
use the right hand for writing and became ambidextrous. First seizure at 8 to learn to use right hand. Normal performance at primary and then basic
years. From age 15 until epilepsy surgery at the age of 22 years, high school. First tonic-clonic seizure at 11 years. Became seizure free
increasing frequency of up to daily seizures of three different types with with AED until age 15, when he had his first drop attack. Drop attacks
or without backward falls. Failed mono- and polytherapies before sur­ increased in frequency, and apprenticeship as office assistant was
gery. Neuropsychology showed an average IQ (IQ ¼ 91). The WADA stopped. No complete seizure control with double and triple AED
test indicated bilateral language lateralization, with left predominance. medication. Neuropsychology showed IQ at lower limit of the norm (IQ
Tachistoscopy indicating left more than right hemisphere capacity for ¼ 85). The WADA test indicated bilateral language lateralization, with
language processing. After a first anterior 2/3 CC at age 22 in 1994 the left sided predominance. Expressive language was mostly left repre­
patient continued to have almost daily drop attacks, with no AHS noted. sented, i.e. not possible in the left but right WADA, receptive language
Completion of the CC one year later controlled the drop attacks, but a functions were bilaterally, i.e. present during the left and right WADA.
disconnection syndrome with AHS (left hand antagonizing and interfering Complete CC was performed at age 27. Postoperatively, this patient had
with right hand continuously) was evident. When he put on his pants he serious problems with writing and drawing with the right hand.
needed to repeat this 5–15 times in order to get it up as intended with the Throughout the ensuing seven years the patient suffered from a discon­
right hand instead of down with the left hand, thus presenting dia­ nection syndrome with AHLMP: after surgery the non-dominant left hand
gonistic dyspraxia, however no signs of independent apraxia. Neuro­ increasingly started to antagonize the dominant right hand. He dis­
psychologically he improved in regard to language-related functions. He played intermanual conflict or diagonistic dypsraxia, respectively. As for
went back to work as a gardener, but some antagonisms persisted as he the seizure situation he experienced repeated EDA.
described that during his work the right hand graps something to put it
at a certain place and that this is often directly reversed by the left hand. 4. Discussion
Apart from left-right confusions (e.g. turning left versus right when
walking), the patient had general difficulties in decision-making. He, The present study reveals common and specific features associated
different from the other two cases, called his left hand the evil hand and with AHS in terms of anatagonistic actions in three out of 18 patients,
he argued loudly with the hand to stop its interference. Follow-up who underwent callosotomy for the control of their drop attacks. The
consultations revealed persistent control of drop attacks. AHS per­ principal rationale of CC is to prevent rapid interhemispheric spread of
sisted, but over time he developed defensive strategies to avoid AHS seizure activity leading to tonic or atonic falls – taking into account that
being too incapacitating in daily life, i.e. putting the left hand in the disconnecting the two hemispheres may have disabling complications
pocket when standing, or sitting on the hand. which however are outweighted by the control of the drop attacks.
In this regard we made the observation that all three patients were Split-brain phenomena after callosotomy were detected and
sitting on their left hand in order to prevent interference while being described early, leading to fascinating knowledge about the human
neuropsychologically evaluated. brain’s hemispheric functional organization and the decisive role of the
Corpus callosum’s involvement in interhemispheric communication and
3.2. Female patient MH inhibition. AHS after callosotomy is a serious complication, which ren­
ders identification of risk factors is highly desirable.
Unremarkable birth and early childhood. Left-handed, not retrained to We identified specific hemispheric dominance patterns as risk factors
write with the right hand. Head injury at 5 years. As of 7th year devel­ for AHS after callosal lesions. The Wada test assesses the functioning
opment of pyknoleptic absences and drop attacks (50–100/d), which versus inactivation of one hemisphere by consecutive separate anaes­
improved under AED. Between 20–40 years of age she experienced an thetization of the left and right hemispheres [14]. The procedure sim­
increasing frequence of psychomotor seizures and Grand Mal seizures. ulates unilateral surgical adamage and reveals contralateral
At the time of extensive evaluation, three different types of seizures were reserve-capacities for compensation and recovery [19]. In our patients
recorded: psychomotor seizures (2–4/d), drop attacks (3–4/week), the test was performed to prevent disconnection of language systems,
secondary generalized seizures (4/week). Neuropsychology showed a such as in crossed language dominance [20], and to prevent the
mild mental retardation despite an IQ within the norm (IQ ¼ 90). The disconnection of language and hand-dominance resulting for example in

149
C. Helmstaedter et al. Seizure: European Journal of Epilepsy 86 (2021) 147–151

agraphia [21].
Unknown at that time was the outcome of callosotomy in a
dominance-pattern with bilateral language-dominance with a predom­
inance in the left hemisphere and a right-hemispherical dominance for
handedness, i.e. genuine left-handedness, retrained to right-handedness
in two of the three patients. Screening the clinical and surgical features
of our patient series, the disconnection of two hemispheres, which were
differentially dominant for language and motor functions appears the
plausible and distinctive feature that differentiates patients who devel­
oped AHS from those who did not. In our three AHS patients, the left
hemisphere was dominant, but not exclusively for language functions,
while the right hemisphere had some capacities for language and was
dominant for motor functions. It is likely that under this condition the
right hemisphere has enough capabilities for own actions in intentional
activities, which due to callosal disconnection cannot be inhibited by the Fig. 1. Distribution and functional connections (+) and inhibitions (-) of left >
left hemisphere, which intends to initiate and lead the individual’s ac­ right hemisphere language dominance and right hemisphere motor dominance
tions. Normally, the language-dominant (mostly left) hemisphere, due to (MH), handedness having been retrained in patients BS and FB. The post-
its digital and sequential information processing mode, is the one which callosotomy (CC) patterns indicate the disconnection of interhemispheric
organizes skillful action. Apraxia is a common problem, apart from communication and inhibition, leaving the two hemispheres with opposite
aphasia, in left-hemispherically damaged patients. After callosotomy, within hemisphere distributions of language and motor control.
the leading left hemisphere in the three AHS-patients was left over with The scheme also explains why patient MH, who had not been forced to write
right-handed had to learn writing with the right hand after callosotomy.
an inferior action control for the right hand. The two patients, who had
been retrained to write with their right hand could continue to use their
right hand for writing, one more successful than the other. The female
patient, however, who was not trained to write with the right hand As for Dr. Strangelove, it seems reasonable that as an act of artistic
became agraphic for the left hand and had to learn writing with the freedom the spectacular split-brain research of that era was associated
right. She continued drawing and painting with her left hand, in line with the prototypical involuntary right arm “Sieg Heil” Hitlergruß in the
with the right hemisphere’s visual spatial processing capabilities. In all movie. Scientifically, Dr. Strangelove’s right hand AHS might be clas­
three patients the right hemisphere, controlling the genuine dominant sified as one of rare cases of right alien hands [27], which are not
left hand became capable to act on its own, presumably because of behaving antagonistic, and which would fit to Feinbergs frontal right
additional inferior left-hemispherical practical capacities with access to (dominant) hand AHS [26]. Another interpretation, which was probably
the motor system. Before callosotomy such independent actions were not indended but which would fit better to non-dominant AHS could be
most likely suppressed and inhibited via callosal communication and an explanation according to which the right hand’s action was intended
inhibition of action control and motor execution (see schematic figure). and the left alien hand tried to revert this action.
It seems as though a complete dissociation results in a clean "division of Finally, for clinical practice, a dissociated hemispehere dominance
labor" between the hemispheres, while partial dissociation tends to pattern of bilateral language and genuine left handedness (right hemi­
bring about conflicting processing efforts. sphere motor dominance) should raise a red flag in candidates for cal­
Why callosal disconnection results in antagonistic and not just some losotomy. We, on the basic of our finding, would not recommend
otherwise dissociated motor action requires additional explanations. callosotomy in such cases but since we didn’t find comparable cases
The right hemisphere in AHS appears to have no independent intention reported in the literature knowledge of any callosotomy patients with
or scheme for action and follows the action design chosen by the the same dominance pattern not resulting in AHS would surely be of
language-dominant left hemisphere to account for the respective situa­ major interest.
tional affordance. However, the alien hand actions are antagonistic
rather than simply a copy of the intended action program or supportive Declaration of authorship
in some way.
Precise motor planning requires bilateral hemispherical activiation, C. Helmstaedter: data collection from patient files, writing, inter­
and it includes the inhibition of antagonistic movements [7]. Activities pretation, figure.
are represented in mental mirror images as indicated for example by the K. Schaller: data collection from patient files, writing, interpretation,
phenomenon of mirror writing. Mirror movements are ‘involuntary figure.
movements of one body part that mirror the voluntary movement of the M. Kurthen: writing, interpretation.
contralateral homologous body part’ [22]. Alteration of transcallosal S. Momjian: literature search, interpretation.
projections inhibiting controlaterally generated ipsilateral movements is L. Solymosi: literature search, collection of MR reports and surgical
discussed as one explanation for mirror movements [23]. Detachment of reports.
the dominant hand from proper motor planning with subsequent lack of
suppression of contralateral mirror and antagonistic movements, Declaration of Competing Interest
through CC or other damage of the corpus callosum, may explain the
clinical manifestation of antagonistic AHS of the left side [24,25]. Dr. Helmstaedter reports grants from ERN EpiCare, personal fees
One of the male patients, who was retrained to right-handedness, from Eisai, personal fees from UCB, personal fees from GW, other from
developed AHS only after completion of the callosotomy. Following Seizure Elsevier, personal fees from Precisis, outside the submitted
what has been described as the anterior motor form of AHS, or the pure work; In addition, Dr. Helmstaedter has a patent EpiTrack licensed to
callosal form, the disconnection of the motor system including premotor UCB, a patent EpiTrack licensed to Eisai, a patent NeuroCogFX licensed
areas and SMA, which are involved in motor programming, execution to Eisai, and a patent EpiTrack licensed to Precisis.
and feedback-guided adjustment, would suffice to explain AHS [7,26]. The authors report no declarations of interest.
The relevant mechanism may be a dissociation of language and motor
dominance and a loss of both intentional control of contralateral action
and physiological inhibition of antagonistic movements (see Fig. 1).

150
C. Helmstaedter et al. Seizure: European Journal of Epilepsy 86 (2021) 147–151

References [15] Wada J, Rasmussen T. Intracarotid injection of sodium amytal for the lateralization
of cerebral speech dominance. J Neurosurg 1960;2007(106):1117–33.
[16] Kurthen M, Helmstaedter C, Linke DB, Hufnagel A, Elger CE, Schramm J.
[1] Biran I, Chatterjee A. Alien hand syndrome. Arch Neurol 2004;61:292–4.
Quantitative and qualitative evaluation of patterns of cerebral language dominance
[2] Goldstein K. Zur Lehre von der motorischen Apraxie. J. Psychol. Neurol. 1908;11:
- an amobarbital study. Brain Lang 1994;46:536–64.
169–87.
[17] Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M, Linke DB, Elger CE. Patterns of language dominance in
[3] Brion S, Jedynak CP. Disorders of interhemispheric transfer (callosal disonnection).
focal left and right hemisphere epilepsies: relation to MRI findings, EEG, sex, and
3 cases of tumor of the corpus callosum. The strange hand sign. Rev Neurol (Paris)
age at onset of epilepsy. Brain Cogn 1997;33:135–50.
1972;126:257–66.
[18] Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.
[4] Bogen JE. Does cognition in the disconnected right hemisphere require right
Neuropsychologia 1971;9:97–113.
hemisphere possession of language? Brain Lang 1997;57:12–21.
[19] Kurthen M, Helmstaedter C, Linke DB, Hufnagel A, Elger CE, Schramm J.
[5] Akelaitis AJ. Studies on the Corpus callosum. 4. Diagonistic dyspraxia in epileptics
Quantitative and qualitative evaluation of patterns of cerebral language
following partial and complete section of the Corpus callosum. Am J Psychiatry
dominance. An amobarbital study. Brain Lang 1994;46:536–64.
1945;101:594–9.
[20] Kurthen M, Helmstaedter C, Linke DB, Solymosi L, Elger CE, Schramm J.
[6] Goldberg G, Mayer NH, Toglia JU. Medial frontal cortex infarction and the alien
Interhemispheric dissociation of expressive and receptive language functions in
hand sign. Arch Neurol 1981;38:683–6.
patients with complex-partial seizures: an amobarbital study. Brain Lang 1992;43:
[7] Aboitiz F, Carrasco X, Schroter C, Zaidel D, Zaidel E, Lavados M. The alien hand
694–712.
syndrome: classification of forms reported and discussion of a new condition.
[21] Sass KJ, Novelly RA, Spencer DD, Spencer SS. Postcallosotomy language
Neurol Sci 2003;24:252–7.
impairments in patients with crossed cerebral dominance. J Neurosurg 1990;72:
[8] Hassan A, Josephs KA. Alien hand syndrome. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2016;16:
85–90.
73.
[22] Woods BT, Teuber HL. Mirror movements after childhood hemiparesis. Neurology
[9] de Haan EHF, Corballis PM, Hillyard SA, Marzi CA, Seth A, Lamme VAF, et al. Split-
1978;28:1152–7.
brain: what we know now and why this is important for understanding
[23] Riddell M, Kuo HC, Zewdie E, Kirton A. Mirror movements in children with
consciousness. Neuropsychol Rev 2020;30:224–33.
unilateral cerebral palsy due to perinatal stroke: clinical correlates of plasticity
[10] Pearce JMS. Frcp the “split brain” and Roger Wolcott Sperry (1913-1994). Rev
reorganization. Dev Med Child Neurol 2019;61:943–9.
Neurol (Paris) 2019;175:217–20.
[24] Brainin M, Seiser A, Matz K. The mirror world of motor inhibition: the alien hand
[11] Thohar Arifin M, Muttaqin Z, Bakhtiar Y, Andar E, Priambada D, Kurnia H, et al.
syndrome in chronic stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:246–52.
Seizure outcomes in patients with complete versus anterior corpus callosotomy:
[25] Haberling IS, Corballis PM, Corballis MC. Language, gesture, and handedness:
analysis of outcome. Int J Gen Med 2020;13:105–10.
evidence for independent lateralized networks. Cortex 2016;82:72–85.
[12] Jalilian L, Limbrick DD, Steger-May K, Johnston J, Powers AK, Smyth MD.
[26] Feinberg TE, Schindler RJ, Flanagan NG, Haber LD. Two alien hand syndromes.
Complete versus anterior two-thirds corpus callosotomy in children: analysis of
Neurology 1992;42:19–24.
outcome. J Neurosurg Pediatr 2010;6:257–66.
[27] Schaefer M, Denke C, Apostolova I, Heinze HJ, Galazky I. A case of right alien hand
[13] Basu AP, Taylor PN, Lowther E, Forsyth EO, Blamire AM, Forsyth RJ. Structural
syndrome coexisting with right-sided tactile extinction. Front Hum Neurosci 2016;
connectivity in a paediatric case of anarchic hand syndrome. BMC Neurol 2015;15:
10:105.
234.
[14] Wada J. A new method for the determination of the side of cerebral speech
dominance. A preliminary report on the intracarotid injection of sodium amytal in
man. Med Biol Tokyo 1949;14:221–2.

151

You might also like