Cloninger (2009)

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Evolution of human brain functions: the functional

structure of human consciousness

C. Robert Cloninger

The functional structure of self-aware consciousness in human beings is described based


on the evolution of human brain functions. Prior work on heritable temperament and
character traits is extended to account for the quantum-like and holographic properties (i.e.
parts elicit wholes) of self-aware consciousness. Cladistic analysis is used to identify the
succession of ancestors leading to human beings. The functional capacities that emerge
along this lineage of ancestors are described. The ecological context in which each clado-
genesis occurred is described to illustrate the shifting balance of evolution as a complex
adaptive system. Comparative neuroanatomy is reviewed to identify the brain structures and
networks that emerged coincident with the emergent brain functions. Individual differences
in human temperament traits were well developed in the common ancestor shared by reptiles
and humans. Neocortical development in mammals proceeded in five major transitions: from
early reptiles to early mammals, early primates, simians, early Homo, and modern Homo
sapiens. These transitions provide the foundation for human self-awareness related to sexu-
ality, materiality, emotionality, intellectuality, and spirituality, respectively. The functional struc-
ture of human self-aware consciousness is concerned with the regulation of five planes of
being: sexuality, materiality, emotionality, intellectuality, and spirituality. Each plane elaborates
neocortical functions organized around one of the five special senses. The interactions
among these five planes gives rise to a 5 × 5 matrix of subplanes, which are functions that
coarsely describe the focus of neocortical regulation. Each of these 25 neocortical functions
regulates each of five basic motives or drives that can be measured as temperaments or
basic emotions related to fear, anger, disgust, surprise, and happiness/sadness. The result-
ing 5 × 5 × 5 matrix of human characteristics provides a general and testable model of the
functional structure of human consciousness that includes personality, physicality, emotion-
ality, cognition, and spirituality in a unified developmental framework.
Key words: cognition, emotionality, human characteristics, personality, thinking.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 2009; 43:994–1006

Human beings are perennially curious about their inner Regarding scope, there are many different theories that
nature and seek to understand what motivates their focus on sexuality, physicality, emotionality, sociality,
desires, actions, feelings, thoughts, and values. The character, cognition, or spirituality [1]. Some broad
study of human characteristics has led to many insightful theories try to serve as a coherent theory of theories
theories and useful approaches, but each suffers from (i.e. meta-theory), as did Freud, Piaget, and Maslow [2].
serious limitations regarding its scope and the qualitative Each still focused on a particular aspect of being, such
properties of its form, aetiology, and dynamics. as sexuality, cognition, or spirituality. Separate theories
for personality, cognition, and spirituality can each be
C. Robert Cloninger, MD, Wallace Renard Professor of Psychiatry, Genetics,
and Psychology
useful, but the divisions are actually artificial and impair
Washington University School of Medicine, 660 South Euclid Avenue,
understanding in serious ways. Health is an integrated
St Louis, MO 63110, USA. Email clon@wustl.edu state of physical, emotional, social, cognitive, and
Received 7 August 2009; accepted 7 August 2009. spiritual well-being [3,4].

© 2009 The Royal Australian and New Zealand College of Psychiatrists


Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016
C.R. CLONINGER 995

Regarding qualitative properties and form, most as part of the rich innate endowment of human beings
models of human characteristics fail to recognize the [15]. He argued that without such universal structure we
unique properties of different planes of being. ‘Planes’ are would not be able to communicate and respect one
defined as distinct levels of existence, thought, or develop- another as we do [16]. There may still be substantial dif-
ment [5]. Emotions cannot be fully described or explained ferences among individuals in the expression of these
by sexual or material mechanisms. Symbolic communica- universal functions, which provides diversity in adaptive
tion cannot be fully explained in terms of emotional proc- capacities for possible ecological change [17,18].
esses. Spirituality cannot be adequately explained by Finally, a general model of human consciousness needs to
intellectual judgments or rules of moral conduct. provide a meta-theory for development, including the matu-
Even within the cognitive domain, there is often failure to ration and integration of sexual, physical, emotional, intel-
attend to the qualitatively distinct properties of the multiple lectual, and spiritual aspects of being [19–21]. Lifespan
systems of learning and memory that are observed in human psychology is an effort to understand development from the
beings [6]. For example, associative conditioning varies quan- perspective of multiple disciplines in an integrated fashion
titatively according to the strength of habits (Table 1). [22]. It recognizes biological and cultural influences on
Aesthetic appreciation of beauty, however, is a qualitative development, but lacks a coherent model of the functional
experience. Self-aware consciousness is always subjective and structure of human beings. Development is a complex adap-
holographic in form (i.e. self-aware perceptions are holistic tive process that cannot be specified by linear stage models,
and personal), so information about beauty cannot be specified such as those of Freud, Erikson, Piaget, or Kohlberg. Human
by any amount of quantitative or objective detail [7]. consciousness is holographic, and so its development must
No models of neurobiology or personality provide an be a quantum-like adaptive process, much like the develop-
adequate explanation of subjectivity (i.e. self-aware con- ment of fractals [7].
sciousness). There are several promising neurobiological Unfortunately, there is no consensus at present regarding
insights for explaining different components of conscious- the functional structure [23] or the developmental paths
ness [8–13]. These individual components of consciousness [22,24] of basic human characteristics, such as personality,
need to be integrated in a unified general model recognizing emotionality, or cognition. Likewise, there is no consensus
the holographic nature of self-aware consciousness. regarding the structure of psychopathology and other dysfunc-
Personality and emotionality have other properties that tional human characteristics. Most psychiatrists recognize that
must be satisfied by a general model of consciousness. current categorical systems of classification are seriously
Personality and emotionality have a universal structure flawed, but do not know with what to replace them. It is impos-
shared by all human beings, as emphasized by Darwin and sible to build an adequate science of health and well-being
supported by cross-cultural studies of facial expression without understanding how normal adaptive functions are
by Ekman [14]. Likewise, Chomsky suggested that there organized [7,25]. Hence a general model of the functional
is a universal grammar for the development of language structure of human consciousness is imperative for under-
standing well-functioning and ill-functioning human beings.
Darwin’s anniversary is a propitious time for the develop-
Table 1. Distinctive properties of three systems
ment of a general model of the functional structure of human
of learning and memory in human beings
beings. As Darwin recognized, human evolutionary history
Qualitative provides a solid scientific basis for describing the functional
System Form of learning properties
structure of consciousness. As he asked (C Notebook, p. 166,
Procedural Habits and skills Prelogical around May 1838), ‘Why is thought being a secretion of the
Emotion-laden brain, more wonderful than gravity a property of matter? It
Quantitative is our arrogance…our admiration of ourselves’ [26].
(variable strength
Not self-aware
Methods
Semantic Facts and Logical
propositions Algorithmic Several recent scientific advances have now converged to permit the
Hierarchical
preliminary description of a general model of human consciousness
Not self-aware
that can be tested and refined by subsequent research. First, cladistic
Autonoetic Intuitions and Self-aware analysis is used to identify the succession of ancestors leading to
narratives Holistic human beings based on molecular phylogenetics [27,28]. A clade is a
Biographical
group of organisms that share a common ancestor. Recent advances in
Creative and freely
willed molecular phylogenetics have clarified the succession of ancestors
leading to human beings from the earliest life forms [29–33].

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


996 FUNCTIONAL STRUCTURE OF HUMAN CONSCIOUSNESS

The functional capacities that emerge along this line of ancestors descendants of the common ancestor shared with human beings. All
are described. The ecological context in which each cladogenesis life forms share DNA as the mechanism of genetic inheritance, going
occurred is described to understand the influences on evolution, as back to the emergence of the first life forms on earth 4 billion years
illustrated by the co-evolution of angiosperms and the ancestors of ago. The ancestral lineage leading to humans includes the first eukary-
primates [32,34,35] and the importance of hunting animals for meat otes, craniates, and amniotes, thereby leading to the common ancestor
for increasing body and brain size of early Homo [36]. These observa- shared by reptiles and mammals. Among mammals, the line continues
tions illustrate the shifting balance of evolution as a complex adaptive from the earliest non-placental mammals to tree shrews and then the
system [37]. proto-primates called plesiadapiforms. The tree shrews were small,
Comparative neuroanatomy is reviewed to identify the brain struc- nocturnal, placental mammals who spent little time in maternal care.
tures and networks that emerged coincident with the emergent brain Their young developed quickly and they are free to spend most of their
functions. Detailed neuroanatomical studies have been conducted that time having sex and eating. The pen-tailed tree shrew, Ptilocerus iowii,
were guided by cladistic analysis to focus on the ancestral line leading is found in Indonesia, Malaysia, and Thailand. It is thought to be the
to human beings [38,39]. Other studies identify which clades have spe- closest living example of the common ancestor of primates [33]. It
cific brain structures and networks, such as mirror neurons [40] or von consumes fermented nectar regularly with 3.8% alcohol without getting
Economo neurons [8]. excessively intoxicated [59].
The primate-like mammals called Plesiadapiforms, such as Car-
polestes simpsoni, are known from the late Palaeocene epoch
Results of Wyoming. They had a grasping foot like primates, including
an opposable toe and a nail rather than a claw. It could probably grasp
What evolutionary transitions led to humans? with its hands as well. As a result, it was well-adapted to move in the
terminal branches of fruit-bearing trees that flourished at that time
Evolution is a complex adaptive process in which multiple genetic [32,35].
and environmental events are constantly interacting, shifting the Among primates, the line continues through ancestors in com-
balance of reproductive fitness from situation to situation and mon with prosimians, simians, then great apes. Prosimians are
time to time [37]. As a result, genetic influences on personality and typically nocturnal and solitary foragers, whereas simians (monkeys
other human characteristics show extensive gene–gene and gene– and apes) are typically diurnal and active in social groups most of
environmental interactions [7,41,42]. Consequently, it is impor- the time [48,49,60]. The great apes show warm emotional expressions
tant to follow the thread of evolution leading to humans as it and affectivity, including ventral hugging, in addition to more com-
unfolded in response to the unrepeatable ecological complexities plex imitation learning, more flexible dominance hierarchies, and
of the past. communication with concrete symbols such as sign language
The timeline of major transitions in brain system structure and [49,61].
function in human evolution is summarized in Tables 2–4. The transi- Among the hominoids, the line continues through ancestors of
tions represent the emergence of clades, usually with some living Australopiths to early Homo. The details of the lineage are intensively

Table 2. Timeline of major transitions in nervous system structure and function in human evolution:
cells to reptiles
Functional change
Time period (mya) Animal clade no. Animal group [Reference] Living examples

15 000 – – Origin of universe Big Bang


4540 – – Formation of Earth Planet Earth
4000 – – Prokaryotic cellular life with DNA Archaea, Bacteria
900 Vendian 0 Choanoflagellates Sperm-like unicellular ancestor of Monosiga
eukaryotes, including animals,
plants, fungi [31]
505 Cambrian 1 Craniates Jawless fish without vertebrae; Hagfish
Bilaterally symmetrical chordates
with heads and neural crests [43]
480 Ordovician 2 Vertebrates Vertebrae, taste buds, genome Lampreys, bony fish,
duplicated [44] amphibia
315 Carboniferous 3 Amniotes Adapted to reproducing and living Reptiles
on land [45]

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


C.R. CLONINGER 997

Table 3. Timeline of major transitions in nervous system structure and function in human
evolution: mammals to apes
Animal Functional change
Time period (mya) clade no. Ancestral group [Reference] Living examples

220 Permian 4 Non-placental mammals Neocortical control of sexuality, Monotremes, marsupials


warm, milk-feeding [27]
125–65 Cretaceous 5 Placental mammals Live births, minimal maternal care, Pen-tailed tree shrew
pleasure-moderating [33]
65–55 Palaeocene 6 Plesiadapiforms Primate-like with more maternal (fossil only)
care and varied diet [32,35,46,47]
47 Eocene 7 Prosimian primates Solitary nocturnal foragers, agile Lemurs, lorises, tarsiers
with forward-facing eyes [48]
40 Oligocene 8 Simian primates Diurnal and social with neocortical Monkeys, gibbons
control of emotions [40,49,50,51]
15 Miocene 9 Great apes Neocortical control of sociality and Orangutans, gorillas,
imitative learning [51] chimps

Table 4. Timeline of major transitions in nervous system structure and function in human evolution:
early Homo to modern Homo sapiens
Time period (mya) Animal clade no. Animal group Functional change [Reference] Living examples

2.5–1.6 9/10? Homo habilis ‘Handy’ tool-maker, australopith-like (Fossil only)


Pliocene–Pleistocene size and proportions, brain
approx. 650 cm3 [52–54]
1.8–0.3 10 Homo erectus Larger body and brain, prominent (Fossil only)
Pleistocene (Ice Age) parietal area for language,
migrated to Eurasia, brain
approx. 880 cm3 [55–57]
0.35–0.04 11 Neanderthals, ancient Larger brain volume than modern (Fossil only)
Palaeolithic Homo sapiens Homo, possibly interbred with
other Homo [53]
0.2–present 12 Homo sapiens sapiens Self-aware with art, science, spiritu- Modern Homo
ality [55,58]

debated, but the functional and structural changes are fairly clear even What were the circumstances of adaptive
when the precise transitional form remains uncertain. Australop- change?
ithecines had well-developed erect bipedal walking, as was shown in
the fossilized footprints preserved at Laetoli in Africa,. The footprints Every transition in the ancestral lineage leading to modern humans
preserve evidence of one holding hands with a younger child while a involved adapting to a novel ecological challenge. Some of the major
second adult followed, stepping precisely in the tracks of the first adult adaptive forces playing out in human evolution are summarized in
[55]. Homo habilis is classified as the earliest human species on the Table 5. Every transition was associated with different challenges that
basis of an average brain volume of >600 cm3 and development of the extant species had to adapt to or face extinction. Mass extinctions
brain regions that support language functions in modern humans. occurred often, shifting the balance of dominance from one life form to
Some argue, however, that it should be grouped with Australopiths, another [63,75,76]. Animals co-evolved with plants, just as both plants
not Homo, because its body was similar to that of an Australopith and animals had to adapt to the changing climate and tectonic shifts.
[52]. It is clear that between Australopiths and Homo erectus there The adaptive challenge helps to recognize the functional shifts that
was a revolutionary increase in the size of the brain and body of occurred at each transition leading to modern human beings. Both
human beings [62]. This increase required a new way of obtaining mammals and dinosaurs emerged around the same time during the
nutrients to support the greater energy consumption of a larger body Jurassic period. The earliest mammals were small nocturnal animals
and brain. that ate insects and avoided the large plant-eating dinosaurs. Then

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


998
Table 5. Geologic timeline of coincident events in human evolution

Time period Emerging Climatic and Bacterial and Animal competition/extinction events
(mya) ancestral group tectonic events plant events [References]

650–543 0 Choanoflagellates Before Vendian, methane and other Cyanobacteria produce oxygen as RNA and DNA formed in chemical soup of
Vendian gases fill atmosphere, complex waste product, creating an oceans. Increasing oxygen level retards
chemicals form in oceans. oxidizing atmosphere. Plants form anaerobic life and stimulates aerobic life.
by symbiosis of cyanobacteria to Bacteria, archaea, and simple soft-bodied
form chloroblasts excreting organisms live entirely in ocean. First fossil
oxygen, and animals cells by animals appear [31,63]. Mass extinction of
symbiosis with mitochondria soft-bodied biota at end of Vendian allowed
consuming oxygen. diversification of fauna with shells.
543–490 1 Craniates Oxygen accumulated enough by end Primitive algae and seaweeds Cambrian explosion of multicellular life forms.
Cambrian of Vendian and early Cambrian to present. Primitive plants evolve Most major groups of animals appear in fossil
form ozone layer blocking ultraviolet from green algae and move onto records. Trilobites are the dominant animals
radiation and permitting land to be land. Fungi also may have aided initially, but were reduced by mass extinctions
colonized. Mass extinctions may colonization of land by symbiosis. [63].
have been related to glacial cooling
or oxygen depletion in the oceans.

490–354 2 Vertebrate Climate is variable. Most land was in True plants emerge in water and on Rapid evolution of fish in water and wingless
Ordovician– southern supercontinent Gondwana. land, and dense forests emerge insects on land. Ordovician and Devonian
Devonian When it shifted to South Pole, during the Devonian period. There extinctions eliminate most species, leaving
massive glaciers formed and were arid periods during the early sharks as the top predators in oceans.
sealevel dropped, possibly leading Devonian, followed by more
to mass extinctions in Late temperate conditions allowing
Ordovician. more plant growth.
354–290 3 Amniotes Climate was mostly warm, humid and Great forests of fern-like seeding Sharks are top predators in ocean, tetrapods
Carboniferous tropical initially. It then cooled with plants flourish and dominate land, move to land, and first amniotes emerge. The
glaciations in south in Gondwana. herbivores grow large to accom- amniote egg protects the developing fetus from
Sealevels fell all over the world due modate large guts needed to desiccation on land, and reptiles thrive.
to southern glaciation. Oxygen digest nutrient-poor food.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


FUNCTIONAL STRUCTURE OF HUMAN CONSCIOUSNESS

levels in atmosphere around 35% Extensive plant life does not


(compared to 21% today). The large decay and forms future coal
continent Pangaea begins to form. deposits.
290–144 4 Non-placental Initially there was an ice age followed Gymnosperm plants flourish, replac- 95% of species go extinct at end of Permian,
Permian– mammals by warming. Much of land was com- ing fern forests. Conifers appear and especially marine invertebrates. This allows
Jurassic bined in one large continent of Pan- spread inland and up mountains, survivors to diversify. Reptiles take over from
gaea, much of which was arid. There and later diversify. Southern hemi- amphibians on land. Mammal-like reptiles lose
were alternating warming and cool- sphere dominated by large seeding dominance, and large plant-eating dinosaurs
ing periods. ferns dominate Jurassic. Mammals emerge approx.
same time as dinosaurs and survive as small,
prolific nocturnal animals feeding on insects
and avoiding reptiles.
144–65 5 Placental Pangaea is now split up in separate Initially angiosperm flowering plants Extinction event eradicates more than half of all
Cretaceous mammals continents. Generally a warm climate are abundant, with pollen spread by animals, including nearly all dinosaurs [64].
with high sealevels. Meteorite impact insects. Then lack of sunlight killed Archaic mammals rapidly diversify and
in Yucatan occluded sunlight for long much plant life and large herbivores. become dominant vertebrates.
period at 65.5 mya, leading to
extinction of dinosaurs except for
ancestors of birds.
65–55 6 Primate-like Initially climate was temperate, cooler Angiosperms flourish producing fruit in Primate-like nocturnal mammals become agile in
Palaeocene mammals and drier than the Cretaceous. Then terminal branches of trees, grasping and feeding on fruits and insects in
epoch it began to warm as Eocene stimulating agility and varied diet. trees [34,35,46].
approached.
55–34 7 Prosimians Global warm and moist climate Tropical rainforests spread from pole Hothouse effect favoured smaller mammals like
Eocene develops from pole to pole. Hot- to pole, displacing other plants. prosimians with relatively large surface areas
house effect on plants and animals. Primates appear in Asia and [65].
Australia separates from Antarctica America.
and current changes led to global
cooling in later Eocene.
34–24 8 Simians Climate gets colder for 7 million years, Grasses evolve among the Monkeys have higher metabolic rates than
Oligocene making daytime activity desirable and angiosperms, grasslands begin to prosimians, allowing for larger bodies and
competition for food greater. dominate, tropical broad-leaf forests brains, stimulating shift to diurnal and so cial
regress to equatorial belt. Primates activities [66]. Diurnal elephants with trunks
appear in Africa for first time [63]. and raptors such as hawks also appear.
C.R. CLONINGER

24–5.3 9 Great apes Climate is initially warmer than Wetland forests decline, temperate Silica-rich grasses cause worldwide extinction of
Miocene Oligocene and then becomes cooler grasses using C4 photosynthesis most large herbivores so grasslands safer for
and drier, facilitating the expansion of expand that assimilate CO2 apes [67,68].
grasslands in continental interiors. efficiently but are richer in silica
Asia and Africa have contact and
animals migrate.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


5.3–1.8 9 Australopiths Climates continue to get cooler and Cooling and drying spurs spread of Distribution of primates restricted by forests
Pliocene drier, similar to modern seasonally temperate climate grasslands and retreating. Wide expanses of open grassland
variable climates. Glaciation begins open savannas with further regression provide advantage for emergent bipedal
toward end of period. of wetland forests. Australopiths but walking and opportunistically consuming
not most other primates adapt to grasses or meat of grazing animals [69–71].
eating grasses or opportunistic
scavenging meat of grazing animals.

(Continued)
999
1000 FUNCTIONAL STRUCTURE OF HUMAN CONSCIOUSNESS

65.5 million years ago a large asteroid struck the Yucatan and the

[36,72,73]. Extinction of most large mammals,


abundant. Human diet required meat protein,

fishing to support larger body and brain size


fat, and essential nutrients from hunting and

northward out of Africa if capable. By end of


resulting dust is thought to have occluded sunlight for so long that the

non-domesticated species are threatened by


Pleistocene, humans have spread through
Animal competition/extinction events

Agriculture and husbandry permitted human


herbivorous dinosaurs were extinguished, except for the ancestors of

domesticated food sources [72,74]. Many


sabre-toothed cats, bison, and birds are
Large land mammals such as mammoths,

allowed humans and others to spread

population expansion with more stable


birds [63,64].
The extinction of the dinosaurs allowed mammals to diversify and
they rapidly became the dominant land animals. The emergence of
[References]

fruit-bearing trees provided the circumstance in which sexually prolific


mammals were selected for greater agility, taste discrimination, and
maternal care [34,35]. Extant prosimians are like living fossils of

most of the world.


these Palaeocene ancestors of human beings except that they are true

human impact.
primates with forward-directed eyes. The prosimians thrived as noc-
turnal solitary foragers during most of the hothouse period of the
Eocene, in which the earth was covered with tropical forest between
55 and 45 million years ago.
The global climate began to cool during the late Eocene. Vegetation
changed in response to climate. As competition for resources increased,
diurnal animals emerged, including elephants with trunks, diurnal raptors
source for large body and brains of

structures adapted for omnivorous

during this relatively stable period


areas for food supply. Wild plants

such as eagles and hawks, and also the first simians. Simians are typically
did not provide adequate energy

Plant life has not changed much


diet with substantial reliance on
Glaciation caused retreat of most
plant and animal life to warmer

shifting available foods for diet.


Homo. Homo teeth and cranial

Agriculture is introduced, sharply

diurnal and social, which gave them advantages in energy consumption.


Simians have a higher rate of oxygen consumption than prosimians [66],
Bacterial and
plant events

so being active during the day and foraging in social groups confers an
energy advantage that supported larger bodies and brains. Some prosimi-
ans did later adapt to diurnal living, but only one group of simians, the
(Continued)

until recently.

owl monkey, reverted to nocturnal existence.


When grasslands become widespread, replacing forest, most large
herbivores became extinct. In contrast, Australopiths adapted well to
meat.

the grasslands. They walked upright and also consumed grass or the
meat of grass-grazers, giving them an advantage over apes that did not
Table 5.

adapt for grass consumption [69–71].


30% of world at its peak. Temperate

Ice age ended, warm habitable zones


Great Ice Age, recurrent glaciation, in

Early humans developed larger bodies and brains, which required a


open grasslands remain in some

extend with fairly stable climates

means of richer nutrition. Hunting, fishing, tool-making, and use of fire


all contributed to the supplementation of their high-fibre diets by early
tectonic events

humans with meat for adequate nutrition during the Palaeolithic Ice
Climatic and

Age [36,72]. They had the intelligence to migrate out of Africa and
colonize areas throughout most of the world during the Pleistocene
epoch. The end of the Ice Age made the development of agriculture
feasible, providing a more reliable source of nutrition for modern
regions.

Homo sapiens.

What brain structures emerged coincident with


the functional changes?
habilis to ancient
ancestral group

10/11 Early Homo

12 Modern Homo
Homo sapiens
Emerging

The transitions described in Tables 2–5 can be summarized as the


sapiens

emergence of specific aspects of consciousness, as detailed in Table 6.


The five major transitions are described more briefly in Table 7
because they are the crux of each functional development. In reptiles
central regulation of brain functions is organized in the hypothalamus.
0.10–Holocene

Sensory information is first processed in the basal forebrain of before


Pleistocene
Time period

being relayed to the thalamus and dorsal cortex of reptiles. The dorsal
1.8 –0.10

epoch

cortex and thalamus of reptiles receive sensory input, but do not recip-
(mya)

rocate with output that could modulate the hypothalamus [83].


A multi-layered neocortex first emerges in early mammals, and there is

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


Table 6. Evolution of major brain functions in human evolution
Clade Emergent functions Emergent structures

1 Craniates Animals with skulls, all associated with emergence of the neural crest Neural crest derivatives: skull, brain with five components and cranial nerves
for development of central nervous system. Also all have 1–10 (except for eye muscle nerves 3,4,6, which may have been
ectodermal placodes for development of paired organs for smell, secondarily lost by hagfish). All have peripheral nervous systems, endocrine
hearing, and vision in the head. tissues, in addition to paired organs for smell, hearing and vision in head.
2 Vertebrates Fish and amphibia show well-developed associative conditioning. Cranial nerves for eyes (3,4,6) are present typically [43].
Whole genome duplicated. Amphibia still reproduce in water.
3 Amniotes Adaptations for tetrapods to live and reproduce on land, breathing Reptile brain is centrally regulated by hypothalamus without thalamocortical
oxygen, and amnion to protect developing fetus. feedback to or control of hypothalamus. Dorsal cortex is single layer of
pyramidal neurons.
4 Non-placental All mammals are warm-blooded, have skin with hair and glands, Neocortex emerges with six layers and allows cortical control of sexual
mammals including milk-producing glands to feed young. Early mammals are copulation. All special senses represented neocortically, but most of
typically sexually prolific, a trait shared with rabbits and others. neocortex processes touch with no separate motor areas.
5 Tree shrews Specialized genital openings and bear live young that require little Somatosensory, motor, and premotor areas differentiated in neocortex.
maternal care. Can show restraint in sexual activity and eating, e.g. Approx. 20 distinct cortical regions compared to >200 in human beings [38].
no excess leading to intoxication when consuming fermented fruits.
6 Protoprimates Enhanced physical agility to grasp food and maternal care of young. (Fossils only: functions suggest similar to prosimians except eyes not
Eyes still laterally directed. forward-directed)
7 Prosimians Nocturnal solitary foragers with skill in finding and selecting food. Taste is processed in primary gustatory cortex prior to hypothalamus and
Brain size varies with foraging complexity, flexibility of diet and amygdala (frontal operculum and insula) [77]. Eye–hand coordination facilitated
activity patterns, not social variables. Extensive maternal care by greater topical ordering of inputs to nuclei for hand and foot and expansion
provided to young. Teeth are distinctive for highly variable diets of of parietal association cortex. Ventromedial hypothalamus has many oxytocin
C.R. CLONINGER

primates. receptors, allowing regulation of feeding behaviour in favour of reproductive role.


Differentiation of DPIC supports awareness of the affective aspects of
sensation [78].
8 Monkeys Diurnal and social with increased metabolic rate able to support larger PFC (mainly orbital and medial PFC) expands and projects directly to
body and brain. Enduring social relationships, much time in social hypothalamus, thalamus, septum, basal amygdala, and striatum.
activities of large groups. Mirror neuron system appears in monkeys, allowing mirroring of observed

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


behaviours by neurons in speech motor area (BA 44 posterior inferior
frontal gyrus), VPC, and IPL (BA 40) [40]. The VPC supports both action
understanding and imitation, a precursor to language. In monkeys, affective
information is also relayed to the middle insular cortex, which has extensive
reciprocal connections with the amygdala and hypothalamus, so it is
well-positioned for the regulation of sensuality.
9 Great apes Highly social, warm emotional expression and affectivity, flexible Somatosensory processing becomes serial and less parallel for greater depth
dominance hierarchies, imitation learning. Australopiths developed of processing. Differentiation of parietal association cortex for integration of
skilled bipedal walking. visual, auditory, and somatosensory information. Great apes and humans,
and not other primates, have von Economo neurons that allow reciprocal
connections of AIC and ACC.
Mirror neurons are also present in great apes in Broca’s area (BA 44) and IPL. Also
in great apes there is differentiation of the AIC for enhanced emotional awareness,
which supports the communication of social emotions in great apes [8].
1001

(Continued)
1002 FUNCTIONAL STRUCTURE OF HUMAN CONSCIOUSNESS

ticularly around the lateral sulcus in regions related to language [55,79]. a progression of five transitions whereby the neocortex takes control
Hemispheric asymmetry is observed in Homo and some Australopiths, par-

Fossils only but enlarged frontal opercular area and IPL, suggesting emer-

Fossils only but findings indicate language ability with prominent development
of IPL (BA 39/40) and emergence of brain default network that supports
daydreaming, holistic attention, and subconscious problem-solving. Default
brain network includes subsystems in the ventral medial PFC, dorsal medial
PFC, IPL, posterior cingulate/retrosplenial cortex, and hippocampal forma-

Autonoetic awareness depends on a distributed frontotemporoparietal network


with encoding via hippocampus [12]. The same brain regions are most
recently differentiated in evolution and are late in myelinating. The whole
neocortex becomes a functional whole by linking all associative areas

ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; AIC, anterior insular cortex; BA, Brodmann area; DPIC, dorsal posterior insular cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; PFC, prefrontal cortex; VPC,
of central regulatory functions from the hypothalamus. In early mam-
mals and tree shrews, the major neocortical function is voluntary
control of copulation, which is reflexive in reptiles [84]. Early mam-
mals noted for their prolific sexuality, such as rodents and rabbits, are

tion and related entorhinal and parahippocampal cortex [9,10].


a sister clade of tree shrews [28]. Primary somatosensory cortex is
clearly developed in early mammals and tree shrews (clades 6/7), but
there is little or no differentiation of sensory neocortex from motor
neocortex [38,39].
Emergent structures

In proto-primates and prosimians there is functional development


gence of language and default brain network.

of motor agility, better discrimination of the taste of a varied diet,


more maternal care of young, and more time spent in grooming and
related form of assuagement. These functions involve regulation of
through projections of visual system

material things such as food and activities of daily living. Unlike


rodents, in primates there is no direct path from the brainstem taste
areas such as the nucleus of the solitary tract to the hypothalamus and
amygdala. Information about taste in primates, in contrast, reaches
the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex from the primary taste cortex,
which is in the frontal operculum and insula [77].
Prosimians have well-differentiated sensory and motor neocortical
areas, in contrast to tree shrews. Detailed studies of galagos indicated
several changes in brain structure that support enhanced motor agility
with advanced grasping and leaping adaptations [38,39]. The findings
Table 6. (Continued)

include greater topographical ordering of sensory input for the hands


and feet, premotor and supplementary motor areas, at least two motor
Precision grip for making stone tools, reduced size of post-canine
teeth, fishing, and hunting in groups for meat. Enlarged brain size

Creation of aesthetically refined tools, language, and culture with

areas in the cingulate cortex, and feedback circuits between prefrontal


cortex, premotor cortex, and primary motor cortex. In addition, prosim-
ians have an enlarged posterior parietal cortex for processing visual,
Self-aware consciousness with art, science, and spirituality

auditory, and somatosensory information to form and relay instructions


about hand and eye movements to premotor areas.
In simians there is emergence of affectivity with patterns of emotional
expression, attachment, and friendship that are similar to human affectiv-
Emergent functions

ity as noted by Darwin [14] and Bowlby [49,61,85]. Related brain


widespread migration in social groups.

changes include the development of prefrontal cortex for regulation of


emotional functions [80], a distinctive system for interoceptive process-
but body similar to australopiths.

ing of sensual aspects of touch [8,78,86,87], and the emergence of the


mirror neuron system on frontal and parietal cortical areas [40].
In early Homo there is emergence of symbolism, including capaci-
ties for taboo, allegory, language, empathy, and discernment [79].
Symbolism builds on the abilities needed for cooperative group forag-
ing in simians [79]. Homo erectus showed aesthetic appreciation in the
making of refined tools in their Acheulian culture [81,82]. These sym-
bolic functions depend on processing in the inferior parietal cortex,
which is a convergence area for touch, hearing, and vision, allowing
cross-modal transformations important for language and other forms
ventral premotor cortex.

of symbolism [55,79,88]. The angular gyrus in particular has an


important role in the comprehension of metaphor and allegory [89].
sapiens sapiens
12 Modern Homo
11 Ancient Homo
10 Homo habilis

Symbolic activity enhances the capacity of the brain default network,


which allows first-person perspective taking and daydreaming, as when
sapiens

a person is letting his or her mind freely wander about inner thoughts
Clade

and feelings [9,10].


Finally, in modern human beings there is self-aware perception
of a sense of unity, manifest by emergent capacities for harmony,

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


C.R. CLONINGER 1003

Table 7. Cladistic staging of evolution of the functional components of self-aware consciousness in human
beings: five basic stages
Clades Emergent brain network Major function Component functions†
(Voluntary)

4/5 Early mammals Somatosensory neocortex regulating Copulation Sex drive


sexuality Longing
(Pleasure)
(Eroticism)
(Tenderness)
6/7 Early primates Differentiation of sensory and motor Materiality Rhythmicity
neocortex; neocortex regulating taste Agility
Sensation
(Movement)
(Transformation)
8/9 Simians Prefrontal cortex regulating limbic system Affectivity Fulfilment
[80]; von Economo neurons in AIC/ Friendship
ACC; mirror neuron system Emotional
awareness
Romanticism
(Devotion)
10/11 Early Homo Auditory association cortex regulates Symbolism Taboo
cross-modal symbolism; brain default Allegory
network regulates attention and Empathy
daydreaming; frontoparietal percep- Discernment
tual–motor praxis system permits Aesthetics
refined tool-making [81,82]
12 Modern Homo sapiens Autonoetic system unifying frontopari- Unity Harmony
etotemporal association areas (linked Sublimation
by visual projection system) [6,12] Adoration
Contemplation
Universal awareness

ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; AIC, anterior insular cortex.


†Functions in parenthesis are found in human beings but not in original ancestor in which major function came under neocortical regulation.

sublimation, spiritual adoration, and contemplation [7]. These abilities called the five planes of being: sexuality, materiality,
give modern human beings their potential in art, science, and spiritual- emotionality, intellectuality, and spirituality [7]. A plane
ity, sometimes leading to transcendent joy, oceanic feelings or even can refer to a level of existence, thought, or development.
cosmic consciousness [7,58]. Such integrated awareness is supported The term ‘plane’ is particularly appropriate for describ-
by autonoetic system of learning and memory [6]. Such self-aware ing these components of self-aware consciousness. It
consciousness allows a person to travel in space and time in their recol- is appropriate because a plane refers to a particular level
lection of episodic events. Such autobiographical thinking involves a of existence, perhaps sexual, material, emotional, and
distributed frontoparietotemporal network [12]. Essentially the visual so on. It also refers to particular kinds of thoughts in
projection system connects all tertiary association cortices so that the self-aware consciousness. Finally, it refers to components
brain can function as a coherent whole. of consciousness that are inseparably connected and
dynamically developing, even when its manifestation
is latent.
Discussion My last statement about latent development is impor-
tant, even though it may seem obscure at first. It is obscure
I have sketched the ancestral lineage of human beings because throughout this brief sketch of evolutionary devel-
and identified five major transitions in functional capac- opment I have traced a specific manifest line of develop-
ity: copulation, materiality, affectivity, symbolism, and ment. I have talked only about emergent functions, but
self-awareness of unity. These five functions are core latent processes are expected to be already under develop-
abilities for the regulation of what I have elsewhere ment before they are overtly manifested. Remember that

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


1004 FUNCTIONAL STRUCTURE OF HUMAN CONSCIOUSNESS

Table 8. Matrix of functional components of consciousness and the animal clade in


which they are first manifest†
Subplane Sexual plane Material plane Emotional plane Intellectual plane Spiritual plane
Spiritual Union 7 Giving of yourself 7 Exaltation 7 Philosophy 7 Unity 7
Intellect Communion 5 Expression 5 Exhortation 5 Symbolism 5 Teaching 7
Emotional Sensuality 4 Sensitivity 4 Affectivity 4 Symbiosis 5 Humanism 7
Material Satisfaction 3 Materiality 3 Resurgence 4 Artistic creation 5 Incarnation 7
Sexual Copulation 2 Reproduction 3 Intimacy 4 Suggestivity 5 Temperance 7
†Animal groups in which functional component is first manifest: 1, reptiles; 2, early mammals and tree shrews; 3, protoprimates and
prosimians; 4, simians and Australopiths; 5, early Homo; 6, neanderthals and archaic Homo sapiens; 7, modern Homo sapiens
sapiens.
Bolded text designates the crucial functions upon which the other functions are dependent.

evolution and development are complex adaptive systems personality, physicality, emotionality, cognition, and
within a whole in which everything is interconnected and spirituality in a unified developmental framework. Each
interdependent. Language did not develop wholly in one of the components of this functional matrix can function
abrupt step [55,79,90]. Functional precursors that are either well or poorly, thereby providing the foundation
developed in response to one challenge are often co-opted for a positive science of well-being. I am pleased to
for another use in different situations. introduce the evolutionary foundation of the science of
Each plane of being involves neocortical functions well-being on the anniversary of Darwin’s birth and his
organized around one of the five special senses. The publication of The origin of species [91].
interactions among these five planes gives rise to a 5 ×
matrix of subplanes, which are functions that coarsely
describe the focus of neocortical regulation (Table 8). Acknowledgements
I have deliberately focused only on the five functions in
bold along the diagonal of Table 7. These diagonal com- This work was supported by non-government funds from
ponents are the core functions upon which the others are the Washington University Center for Psychobiology and
built. For example, the material subplane of sexuality the Anthropedia Foundation.
(satisfaction, such as assuagement by grooming) depends
on the differentiation of sensory and motor functions
underlying materiality. Satisfaction and enhanced mater- References
nal care (a facet of reproduction) emerge together with
materiality in protoprimates and prosimians. Likewise, 1. Hall CS, Lindzey G. Theories of personality. New York: John
Wiley & Sons, 1978.
the emotional subplane of sexuality (sensuality) depends 2. Maslow AH. Higher and lower needs. J Psychol 1948; 25:
on emotional awareness (affectivity), and they emerge 433–436.
together in simians. 3. WHO. Mental health: new understanding, new hope. Geneva:
Within each of the five planes of being, there are World Health Organization, 2001.
4. Vaillant GE, Vaillant CO. Natural history of male psychological
components of each plane that emerge in succession in
health, XII: a 45-year study of predictors of successful aging at
the ancestral lineage leading to human beings. For exam- age 65. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:31–37.
ple, in the sexual plane, copulation emerges in early 5. Collins. Collins essential English dictionary. New York: Harper
mammals, satisfaction in protoprimates, sensuality in Collins, 2006.
simians, and soon. 6. Tulving E. Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Hum
Neurobiol 1987; 6:67–80.
Each of these 25 neocortical functions regulates each
7. Cloninger CR. Feeling good: the science of well-being. New York:
of five basic motives or drives that can be measured as Oxford University Press, 2004.
temperaments (harm avoidance, novelty seeking, reward 8. Craig AD. How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human
dependence, persistence, plus coherence-seeking) or awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10:59–70.
basic emotions like those described by Darwin and 9. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default
network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad
Ekman (fear, anger, disgust, surprise, and happiness/
Sci 2008; 1124:1–38.
sadness) [14]. The resulting 5 × 5 × 5 matrix of human 10. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA,
characteristics provides a general testable model of the Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad
functional structure of human consciousness that includes Sci USA 2001; 98:676–682.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


C.R. CLONINGER 1005

11. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial 33. Janecka JE, Miller W, Pringle TH et al. Molecular and genomic
prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a data identify the closest living relative of primates. Science 2007;
default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001; 318:792–794.
98:4259–4264. 34. Sussman RW. Primate origins and the evolution of angiosperms.
12. Levine B. Autobiographical memory and the self in time: brain Am J Primatol 1991; 23:209–223.
lesion effects, functional neuroanatomy, and lifespan develop- 35. Sargis EJ. Paleontology. Primate origins nailed. Science 2002;
ment. Brain Cogn 2004; 55:54–68. 298:1564–1565.
13. Pribram K. Rethinking neural networks: quantum fields and bio- 36. Eaton SB, Eaton SB 3rd, Konner MJ. Paleolithic nutrition revisited:
logical data. Hillsdale, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1993. a twelve-year retrospective on its nature and implications. Eur J
14. Darwin C, Ekman P, Prodger P. The expression of the emotions in Clin Nutr 1997; 51:207–216.
man and animals. London: Harpercollins Publishers, 1998. 37. Wright S. The shifting balance theory and macroevolution. Annu
15. Chomsky N. Rules and representations. Behav Brain Sci 1980; Rev Genet 1982; 16:1–19.
3:1–61. 38. Kaas JH, The evolution of the complex sensory and motor systems
16. Chomsky N. Universals of human nature. Psychother Psychosom of the human brain. Brain Res Bull 2008; 75:384–390.
2005; 74:263–268. 39. Kaas JH. Evolution of the neocortex. Curr Biol 2006; 16:
17. Cloninger CR. A systematic method for clinical description R910–R914.
and classification of personality variants. A proposal. Arch Gen 40. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev
Psychiatry 1987; 44:573–588. Neurosci 2004; 27:169–192.
18. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A psychobiological 41. Keltikangas-Jarvinen L, Raikkonen K, Ekelund J, Peltonen L.
model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; Nature and nurture in novelty seeking. Mol Psychiatry 2004;
50:975–990. 9:308–311.
19. Cloninger CR. Completing the psychobiological architecture 42. Keltikangas-Jarvinen L, Pulkki-Raback L, Elovainio M, Raitakari
of human personality development: temperament, character, OT, Viikari J, Lehtimaki T. DRD2 C32806T modifies the effect of
and coherence, In: Staudinger UM, Lindenberger UER, eds. child-rearing environment on adulthood novelty seeking. Am J Med
Understanding human development: dialogues with lifespan Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2009; 150B:389–394.
psychology. London: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 2003: 43. Janvier P. Craniata: animals with skulls. The Tree of Life Web
159–182. Project. University of Arizona College of Agriculture and Life
20. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM. Integrative psychobiological approach Sciences and University of Arizona Library, 1997. Available from
to psychiatric assessment and treatment. Psychiatry 1997; 60: URL: http://tolweb.org/
120–141. 44. Shubin N. Your inner fish: a journey into the 3.5-billion-year
21. Crain W. Theories of development: concept and applications. history of the human body. New York: Pantheon, 2008.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 2000. 45. MacLean PD. Evolutionary psychiatry and the triune brain.
22. Staudinger UM, Lindenberger U. Understanding human deve- Psychol Med 1985; 15:219–221.
lopment: dialogues with lifespan psychology. London: Kluwer 46. Radhakrishna S. From Purgatorius ceratops to Homo sapiens:
Academic Publishers, 2003. primate evolutionary history. Resonance 2006; 11:51–60.
23. Cloninger CR. Foreword. In: Lilienfeld SO, O’Donohue WT, 47. Clemens WA. Purgatorius, an early paromomyid primate
Fowler KA, eds. Personality disorders: toward the DSM-V. Los (Mammalia). Science 1974; 184:903–905.
Angeles: Sage Publications, 2007:vii–xvi. 48. Sussman RW. Primate ecology and social structure, vol. 1: lorises,
24. Bjorklund DF. In search of a metatheory for cognitive develop- lemurs and tarsiers. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom
ment (or, Piaget is dead and I don’t feel so good myself). Child Publishing, 2003.
Dev 1997; 68:144–148. 49. Sussman RW, Chapman AR. The origins and nature of sociality.
25. Cloninger CR. The science of well-being: an integrated approach New York: Aldine de Gruyter, 2004.
to mental health and its disorders. World Psychiatry 2006; 50. Bekoff M. The emotional lives of animals. Novato, CA: New World
5:71–76. Library, 2007.
26. Gruber HE. Darwin on man, together with Darwin’s early and 51. Goodman M. The genomic record of humankind’s evolutionary
unpublished notebooks. New York: EP Dutton, 1974. roots. Am J Hum Genet 1999; 64:31–39.
27. Murphy WJ, Pringle TH, Crider TA, Springer MS, Miller W. Using 52. Wood B, Collard M/ The human genus. Science 1999;
genomic data to unravel the root of the placental mammal phylog- 284:65–71.
eny. Genome Res 2007; 17:413–421. 53. Lee SH, Wolpoff MH. Habiline variation: a new approach using
28. Murphy WJ, Eizirik E, Johnson WE, Zhang YP, Ryder OA, STET. Theory Biosci 2005; 124:25–40.
O’Brien SJ. Molecular phylogenetics and the origins of placental 54. Asfaw B, White T, Lovejoy O, Latimer B, Simpson S, Suwa G.
mammals. Nature 2001; 409:614–618. Australopithecus garhi: a new species of early hominid from
29. Smith HF, Grine FE. Cladistic analysis of early Homo crania from Ethiopia. Science 1999; 284:629–635.
Swartkrans and Sterkfontein, South Africa. J Hum Evol 2008; 55. Eccles JC. Evolution of the brain: creation of the self. London:
54:684–704. Routledge, 1989.
30. Hammer MF, Karafet T, Rasanayagam A et al. Out of Africa and 56. Hawks J, Hunley K, Lee S-H, Wolpoff M. Population bottlenecks
back again: nested cladistic analysis of human Y chromosome and Pleistocene human evolution. Mol Biol Evol 2000; 17:2–22.
variation. Mol Biol Evol 1998; 15:427–441. 57. Lewin R. Principles of human evolution. Malden, MA: Blackwell
31. Schierwater B, Eitel M, Jakob W et al. Concatenated analysis sheds Science, 1993.
light on early metazoan evolution and fuels a modern ‘urmetazoon’ 58. Mithen S. The prehistory of the mind: the cognitive origins of art,
hypothesis. PLoS Biol 2009; 7:e20. religion and science. London: Thames and Hudson, 1996.
32. Bloch JI, Silcox MT, Boyer DM, Sargis EJ. New Paleocene 59. Wiens F, Zitzmann A, Lachance MA et al. Chronic intake of
skeletons and the relationship of plesiadapiforms to crown-clade fermented floral nectar by wild treeshrews. Proc Natl Acad Sci
primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007; 104:1159–1164. USA 2008; 105:10 426–10 431.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016


1006 FUNCTIONAL STRUCTURE OF HUMAN CONSCIOUSNESS

60. Sussman RW, Andrianasolondraibe O, Soma T, Ichino I. Social 76. Raup DM, Sepkoski JJ Jr, Stigler SM. Mass extinctions in the
behavior and aggression among ringtailed lemurs. Folia Primatol fossil record. Science 1983; 219:1240–1241.
(Basel) 2003; 74:168–172. 77. Verhagen JV, Kadohisa M, Rolls ET. Primate insular/opercular
61. Preston SD, De Waal FB. The communication of emotions and taste cortex: neuronal representations of the viscosity, fat texture,
the possibility of empathy in animals. In: Post SG, ed. Altruism grittiness, temperature, and taste of foods. J Neurophysiol 2004;
and altruistic love. New York: Oxford University Press, 2002: 92:1685–1699.
284–308. 78. Craig AD. Forebrain emotional asymmetry: a neuroanatomical
62. Hawks J, Hunley K, Lee SH, Wolpoff M. Population bottlenecks basis? Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9:566–571.
and Pleistocene human evolution. Mol Biol Evol 2000; 17:2–22. 79. Deacon TW. The symbolic species: the co-evolution of language
63. UCMP. University of California Museum of Paleontology Geology and the brain. New York: WW Norton, 1997.
Wing. Berkeley, CA: University of California at Berkeley, 2009. 80. Semendeferi K, Armstrong E, Schleicher A, Zilles K, Van
64. Bryant LJ, Clemens WA, Hutchison JH. Cretaceous–Tertiary Hoesen GW. Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: a
dinosaur extinction. Science 1986; 234:1172–1173. comparative study of area 10. Am J Phys Anthropol 2001; 114:
65. Clemens WA. Eocene and earlier deposits. Science 1982; 215: 224–241.
962–964. 81. Stout D, Toth N, Schick K, Chaminade T. Neural correlates of early
66. Armstrong E. Relative brain size in monkeys and prosimians. Am stone age tool-making: technology, language, and cognition in
J Phys Anthropol 2005; 66:263–273. human evolution. Philos Trans R Soc London B Biol Sci 2008;
67. Badgley C, Barry JC, Morgan ME et al. Ecological changes in 363:1939–1949.
Miocene mammalian record show impact of prolonged climatic 82. Stout D, Toth N, Schick K, Stout J, Hutchins L. Stone tool-making
forcing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008; 105:(12)145–12 149. and brain activation: PET studies. J Archaeol Sci 2000; 27:
68. Cerling TE, Harris JM, MacFadden BJ et al. Global vegetation 1215–1223.
change through the Miocene/Pliocene boundary. Nature 1997; 83. Nieuwenhuys R. The neocortex. An overview of its evolutionary
389:153–158. development, structural organization and synaptology. Anat
69. Lee-Thorp JA, Sponheimer M, Luyt J. Tracking changing envi- Embryol (Berl) 1994; 190:307–337.
ronments using stable carbon isotopes in fossil tooth enamel: an 84. Wersinger SR, Baum MJ. Sexually dimorphic processing of
example from the South African hominin sites. J Hum Evol 2007; somatosensory and chemosensory inputs to forebrain luteinizing
53:595–601. hormone-releasing hormone neurons in mated ferrets. Endocrino-
70. Sponheimer M, Lee-Thorp JA. Isotopic evidence for the diet of an logy 1997; 138:1121–1129.
early hominid, Australopithecus africanus. Science 1999; 283: 85. Bowlby J. Attachment, 2nd edn. New York: Basic Books, 1983.
368–370. 86. Craig AD. Human feelings: why are some more aware than others?
71. Sponheimer M, Lee-Thorp JA. Differential resource utilization by Trends Cogn Sci 2004; 8:239–241.
extant great apes and australopithecines: towards solving the C4 87. Craig AD. Emotional moments across time: a possible neural basis
conundrum. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol 2003; for time perception in the anterior insula. Philos Trans R Soc Lond
136:27–34. B Biol Sci 2009; 364:1933–1942.
72. Richards MP. A brief review of the archaeological evidence for 88. McGeoch PD, Brang D, Ramachandran VS. Apraxia, metaphor and
Palaeolithic and Neolithic subsistence. Eur J Clin Nutr 2002; 56:16 mirror neurons. Med Hypotheses 2007; 69:1165–1168.
p following 1262. 89. Ramachandran VS. A brief tour of consciousness. Upper Saddle
73. Stanford CB. The hunting apes: meat eating and the origins of River, NJ: Pi Press, 2005.
human behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 2001. 90. Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT. The faculty of language:
74. Richards MP, Schulting RJ, Hedges RE. Archaeology: sharp shift what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 2002;
in diet at onset of Neolithic. Nature 2003; 425:366. 298:1569–1579.
75. Raup DM, Sepkoski JJ Jr. Mass extinctions in the marine fossil 91. Darwin C. On the origin of species by means of natural selection,
record. Science 1982; 215:1501–1503. 1st edn. London: Murray, 1859.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at CIDADE UNIVERSITARIA on February 12, 2016

You might also like