Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp

Communication increases collaborative corruption☆


Mathilde H. Tønnesen a, *, Christian T. Elbæk a, Stefan Pfattheicher b, Panagiotis Mitkidis a
a
Department of Management, Aarhus BSS, Aarhus University, 8210 Aarhus V, Denmark
b
Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Aarhus BSS, Aarhus University, 8000 Aarhus, Denmark

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Despite being a pivotal aspect of human cooperation, only a few studies within the field of collaborative
Communication dishonesty have included communication between participants, and none have yet experimentally compared this
Dishonesty to non-communicative contexts. As a result, the impact of communication on unethical collaborations remains
Collaboration
unclear. To address this gap, we conducted two well-powered studies (Ntotal = 1187), closely replicating and
Commitment
Replication
extending seminal research by Weisel and Shalvi (2015), introducing communication as a manipulated variable
within a dyadic cheating task. Across both studies, we found evidence that communication increases the
magnitude of cheating—even when coordination on the task is not allowed. Importantly, the effect of commu­
nication was linked to a stronger experienced collaboration among the communicating dyads, highlighting that
communication is not only key to everyday ethically sound collaborations, but also to corrupt collaborations.

1. Introduction decision and reports one after each other (e.g., Gross et al., 2018; Wouda
et al., 2017).
Humans are a cooperative species, whose amicable ability to When working in collaborative settings, dishonesty can be conta­
collaborate is regarded as one of the most fundamental factors in their gious and spread among group members (Gross et al., 2018) and the
evolution (Bowles & Gintis, 2011) and morality (Tomasello, 2016), and impact of collaboration on corrupt behaviors has been related to various
still constitute an important pillar of modern societal life. However, factors, including diffusion of responsibility and moral justifications
despite the clear benefits of collaboration, research in behavioral ethics (Conrads et al., 2013; Kocher et al., 2018), perceived similarity (Irlen­
and collaborative corruption has shown that collaborative settings in­ busch et al., 2020), commitment (Zickfeld et al., 2022), parochial
crease the magnitude of economic dishonesty compared to instances altruism (Cadsby et al., 2016; Shalvi & De Dreu, 2014), exposure to
where individuals behave independently (e.g. Beck et al., 2020; Conrads dishonest descriptive norms (Gross & De Dreu, 2020; Soraperra et al.,
et al., 2013; Gerlach et al., 2019; Gross et al., 2018; Kocher et al., 2018; 2017) and conformity (Jamaluddin et al., 2020). Conversely, commit­
Weisel & Shalvi, 2015, see also; Leib et al., 2021; Zickfeld et al., 2022 for ting to an honesty oath (Beck et al., 2020; Zickfeld et al., 2023),
recent review and meta-analyses). Thus, human collaborative ten­ increasing the salience of negative externalities (Castillo et al., 2022),
dencies might, paradoxically, also play a key role in shaping corrupt and being monitored (Mitkidis et al., 2023; Schild et al., 2019) have
behaviors. been found to decrease dishonesty.
The field of collaborative corruption has provided valuable insights Despite having greatly advanced our understanding of the role of
into the interplay between cooperation and (un)ethical behaviors. collaboration in corrupt and fraudulent behaviors, many studies within
Weisel and Shalvi (2015) was among the first to show that participants the field are based on sequential decision-making with limited interac­
collaborating in a dyadic die rolling game, cheated more than in­ tion between participants. However, true joint decision-making often
dividuals playing the game alone. Similar results have been observed in involves some interaction, wherein group members engage in commu­
different decision contexts; for instance, dishonesty increases (1) when nication about possible courses of action (Beck et al., 2020). Conse­
participants make decisions individually but report simultaneously (e.g., quently, a central element integral to collaborations outside the
Kocher et al., 2018), (2) when the group makes one joint decision (e.g., laboratory is often absent in these studies: verbal communication.
Beck et al., 2020; Korbel, 2017) or (3) when each group member makes a Verbal communication refers to communication through the use of


This paper has been recommended for acceptance by Dr. Joris Lammers.
* Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: mtt@mgmt.au.dk (M.H. Tønnesen), chel@mgmt.au.dk (C.T. Elbæk), sp@psy.au.dk (S. Pfattheicher), pm@mgmt.au.dk (P. Mitkidis).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104603
Received 12 December 2023; Received in revised form 20 February 2024; Accepted 22 February 2024
Available online 28 February 2024
0022-1031/© 2024 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

language (words) in written and oral form and differs from nonverbal studies have allowed communication between participants (e.g., Beck
communication (e.g., gestures and facial expressions (Buck & VanLear, et al., 2020; Kocher et al., 2018), the present paper is the first to directly
2002)). Verbal communication is perhaps the most pervasive form of test the impact of communication in collaborative corrupt behaviors.
human communication (De Saussure & Rocci, 2016) and is essential for Thirdly, by varying the kind of social communicative exchanges, our
coordinating behaviors to achieve common goals (Tomasello, 2008) and findings illuminate the conditions that exacerbate challenges related to
for fostering commitment among individuals (Kerr & Kaufman- collaborative dishonesty, shedding light on why and how communication
Gilliland, 1994). Furthermore, both oral and written communication might facilitate such behaviors. Lastly, by relating dishonesty to expe­
have been found to increase collaboration in various games, such as rienced collaboration, we contribute knowledge into the experiential
ultimatum games (Roth, 1995; Zultan, 2012), trust games (Charness & nature of behaviors that arise in the settings used to study corrupt col­
Dufwenberg, 2006), and dictator games (Andreoni & Rao, 2011). Yet, laborations, thereby providing new and important insights into the de­
despite its evident importance and although collaborative dishonesty gree and nature of collaboration captured in collaborative dishonesty
has been defined as “joint unethical acts” (Shalvi et al., 2016, p. 134), studies.
experimental paradigms employed to study collaborative dishonesty
frequently only allow minimal opportunities for joint decisions.1 2. Study 1: Does communication corrupt?
Theoretically, verbal communication can both foster and hinder
dishonesty. It provides a platform for coordination and justifications The importance of verbal communication in human social behavior
that may ease dishonest acts (Kocher et al., 2018). However, as repu­ is clear. First, the origin of verbal communication is one of the most
tational concerns increase in more social settings (De Cremer & Barker, essential cornerstones in human evolution (Nowak, 2000). Second, it
2003), it also amplifies reputational risks, potentially deterring serves as a primary channel through which we transmit socially shared
dishonesty. Moreover, directly discussing the opportunity of behaving knowledge (Mesoudi, 2011) and establish joint intentions and goals to
dishonestly may enhance ethical considerations by making the unethical effectively coordinate our behaviors to achieve a common goal (Toma­
aspect of dishonesty more salient and less ambiguous, in turn potentially sello, 2008), which is crucial for cooperation (Mitkidis et al., 2013).
mitigating dishonesty. Indeed, research have indicated that increasing Therefore, we reasoned that verbal communication would also be
the salience of unethical aspects of behavior or moral awareness reduces instrumental when people collude on deceiving others for collective or
dishonesty (e.g., Mazar et al., 2008; Sturm, 2017). This disparity raises a monetary gain.
central question: How does the introduction of communication influence Few studies on collaborative corruption have included communica­
collaborative dishonesty? More broadly, does the inclusion of commu­ tion between participants. In one study, participants in groups of three
nication lead to an increase in dishonest behavior, and if so, what are the were given the opportunity to chat with each other via a chat box during
underlying mechanisms? a die-roll game. This finding suggests that communication could facili­
In response to these inquires, we present two well-powered experi­ tate dishonesty in group settings by allowing participants to coordinate
mental studies that directly investigate the role of communication in their actions and justify deceitful behavior (Kocher et al., 2018). Simi­
fostering collaborative dishonesty (Fig. 1). First, we examined a scenario larly, another study, in which participants were able to engage in joint
where individuals had the opportunity to engage in dishonesty for decision-making in face-to-face interactions, found further evidence that
monetary gains. In this context, we compared a non-communicative groups act more dishonest, because they are more readily exposed to
setting to a setting where participants were allowed to communicate justifications for dishonesty (Beck et al., 2020). While such studies
and further related this to levels of collaboration by directly measuring provide important insights into how communication can facilitate
participants experience of collaborating with their partner (Study 1, corrupt collaborations, these studies do not provide direct evidence that
Fig. 1a). communication increases dishonesty. In fact, although a recent meta-
Then, because communication plays a key role in fostering analysis indicates that communication is linked to increased dishon­
commitment to others (Kerr & Kaufman-Gilliland, 1994), we reasoned esty (Zickfeld et al., 2022), no studies have systematically isolated the
that being able to communicate in itself (without coordination and jus­ effect of communication by comparing it to non-communicative con­
tifications) would increase dishonesty through feelings of commitment texts. Consequently, the extent to which communication influences
to one’s partner. Indeed, feeling committed to others may constitute a collaborative dishonesty remains largely understudied.
crucial motivation for engaging in dishonesty (Zickfeld et al., 2022). It Considering this, we closely replicated and extended the work by
has been found that this tendency increases as people become better Weisel and Shalvi (2015), which has played a central role in establishing
acquainted with each other (Beck et al., 2020) and when the dishonesty the corruptive role of collaboration. In the original study, participants
benefits the group or other individuals (Cadsby et al., 2016; Erat & engaged in a private die roll game either individually or in pairs, in
Gneezy, 2012). Therefore, in a second study (Study 2, Fig. 1b) we which they could cheat for monetary gains by misreporting the result of
manipulated the content of participants conversation varying whether their die rolls. Specifically, in the authors’ aligned incentives treatment,
participants were allowed to coordinate and discuss the task. In doing participants playing in dyads would receive the same payoff based on
so, we examined how different communicative settings would impact the number of doubles they reported. Participants playing alone instead
collaborative dishonest behavior. rolled the die twice and reported the results of their die rolls. Notably,
By including and manipulating communication, this paper advances compared to individual players, pairs exhibited significantly greater
the current literature on behavioral ethics in several ways. Firstly, levels of cheating (indicated by higher numbers of reported doubles
whereas the majority of past work on collaborative dishonesty have above chance level), underscoring the facilitative role of collaboration in
restricted participant’s ability to communicate and engage in joint de­ dishonest behavior. Nevertheless, the interaction in the pairs was
cisions with their partner, this paper introduces this important aspect. confined to receiving feedback on the partner’s die-roll on a computer,
This significantly broadens the applicability of findings to real-world minimizing the salience of collaboration. While participants might still
scenarios, such as large corporate scandals, where communication be able to communicate their intention and willingness to engage in
plays a central role in coordinating such actions. Secondly, while a few dishonesty through their reporting behavior, participants could not
directly discuss the task and explicitly coordinate on how to proceed
during the game. We therefore extended Weisel and Shalvi (2015)
1
While communication within the joint action literature has been described original aligned paradigm to enhance the conditions for collaboration by
as a joint action in and of itself (e.g., Moore, 2018; Tomasello, 2008), we focus manipulating whether participants in the dyadic conditions could
on communication as a means of collaboration, serving as an important channel communicate with each other or not. In doing so, we aimed to investi­
through which people can collaborate more effectively. gate the interplay of communication and collaborative dishonesty.

2
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

Fig. 1. Experimental design in Study 1 (a) and Study 2 (b). In Study 1 participants were randomly assigned to either an individual condition, a no-communication
condition, or a communication condition. After a practice round, participants would begin phase 2, consisting of 10 trials of a die-roll task adapted from previous
work by Weisel and Shalvi (2015). After completing the trials, participants indicated their subjective level of experienced collaboration in the dyadic conditions.
Study 2 followed a similar procedure. Participants were randomly assigned to a no-communication condition, an unrestricted communication condition (reflecting
the communication condition in Study 1) or a restricted communication condition, wherein participants were not allowed to talk about the task and instead were
given specific topics to discuss.

Building on Weisel and Shalvi (2015), we hypothesized that dyads 2.1. Method
(both those that could communicate and those that could not) would
report more doubles compared to individuals (H1). Additionally, we 2.1.1. Participants
expected that permitting communication would allow participants to A total of 430 participants from Prolific completed the study. Seven
align their goals and more easily coordinate on which numbers to report. participants failed the attention check placed in the end of the study in a
We therefore further hypothesized that dyads who were able to measure of the personality trait Honesty-Humility (see Supplementary
communicate would report a higher number of doubles compared to Material; SM section 2.4.2.), and two participants were missing a partner
dyads that could not communicate (H2). after we removed the participants who failed the attention check.
Furthermore, because communication is important to collaboration Following our pre-registration, these participants were excluded from
and joint decision-making (Beck et al., 2020; Zultan, 2012), constituting the herein presented analyses, leaving a final sample size of N = 421
a channel to effectively coordinate behavior and exchange justifications (161 dyads and 99 individual players; 47.5% women, 50.6% men, Mage
(Kocher et al., 2018), we reasoned that the effect of communication on = 40.32, SDage = 12.57). Results including all participants are reported
number of reported doubles would depend on the experience of in SM (see section 2.5.).
collaborating with the other player. Thus, by allowing participants to Due to the magnitude of the effect size in the original study (d =
communicate, we increase the conditions for collaboration and goal 0.94), which yielded a rather small sample size estimate (34 participants
coordination, which may also increase coordinated dishonesty. To test [12 dyads and 22 individual players]) in an a priori power analysis (d =
this, we therefore measured the subjectively perceived collaboration 0.94, 1-β = 0.80, α = 0.05, allocation ratio N1/N2 = 1.8, two-tailed
within both dyadic conditions, hypothesizing that the effect of Mann-Whitney U test), we determined our sample size based on the
communication on dishonest reporting would be mediated by an smallest effect size of interest.
increased experience of collaborating with the other player (H3).2 We follow Simonsohn (2015), and define a small effect as the effect
that would give the original study a power of 33% (d33%). Based on this,
we estimated the minimum sample size of 53 dyads (n = 106) in each
dyadic condition and 95 participants in the individual condition (d33% =
0.44, α = 0.05, 1-β = 0.80, allocation ratio N1/N2 = 1.8, one-tailed

2
We changed the wording of this hypothesis from the pre-registration. In the
pre-registration we termed the collaboration items as “joint goal” items. This
was subsequently changed to “sense of collaboration.”

3
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

Mann-Whitney U test3). Additionally, due to more financial resources the external website, roll the die, and report their respective outcome.
than originally estimated, we continued recruiting until our resources After this, participants were shown a table explaining the outcome of the
were depleted.4 A one-way ANOVA sensitivity power analysis (α = 0.05, round. After the practice round (Phase 1), participants began Phase 2,
1-β = 0.8) indicated that our sample size (N = 260, dyads n = 161, in­ which followed the same procedure (see SM section 2.1.).
dividuals n = 99), is sensitive to detect an effect of Cohen’s d = 0.19 Dyads in the Communication condition underwent the same pro­
(G*Power 3.1; Faul et al., 2007). cedure as in the no-communication condition. However, in this condi­
tion Player A and B additionally had the possibility to chat with each
2.1.2. Design and procedure other using an online chat. Participants were instructed that they had 2
In this study, we report all measures, manipulations and exclusions. min to discuss and exchange information of their choice, except for
A thorough description of measures and manipulations can be found in sensitive personal information. They could end the chat at any point, but
Supplemental Material. Study 1 was conducted online using Qualtrics the chat would automatically close after 2 min. Participants could chat
and the add-on application SMARTRIQS (Molnar, 2020), which allows after the practice round, before beginning the 10 trials, as well as after
for real-time interaction between participants on the Qualtrics platform. every round of the 10 trials. In the Individual condition, participants
To study the impact of communication on cheating behavior and engaged in the die roll task by playing both the role of A and B as in
experienced collaboration, the study used a between-subjects design, in Weisel and Shalvi (2015).
which participants were randomly assigned to either of three conditions; To account for the possible extra time in between trials introduced by
(1) Communication (n = 164; 82 dyads), (2) No-communication (n = the chats in the Communication condition, participants in the individual
158; 79 dyads) or (3) Individual (n = 99).5 and No-communication conditions answered a set of filler questions
Following the protocol of Weisel and Shalvi (2015) participants in after each trial. These filler items were in the form of mundane episodic
the No-communication condition engaged in 10 trials of the dyadic die recall questions, such as “What did you have for dinner yesterday?” or
roll game, on a computer. We used 10 trials, instead of 20 as in Weisel “What is your favorite hobby?”. We chose these writing tasks to simulate
and Shalvi (2015). We did this to reduce fatigue among participants the interactive chats as much as possible and to minimize the risk of
since the study took place in an online setup, and the finding that inducing unintended confounds in the experimental setup. After the 10
behavior in collaborative cheating paradigms typically stabilizes around trials, one round was randomly selected as payoff for Phase 2 and
the 10th round in sequential cheating paradigms (Zickfeld et al., 2022). showed to the participants.
Participants were given a base payment for taking part in the study and
were informed that they would be able to earn an additional bonus, 2.1.3. Measures
which was determined by random selection of one of the 10 die-rolls. After having completed both experimental phases, participants were
After having provided informed consent, participants began Phase 1 asked a set of questions related to their experience throughout the
of the experiment, which consisted of thorough instructions of the rules experiment. Because shared goals and intentions are central concepts
of the die-roll game, including the payment structure and procedure. within the joint actions literature (Bratman, 1992; Butterfill, 2012;
Participants were made aware that they would be rolling a die on an Tuomela, 2020) we measured participants’ experienced collaboration
external website (www.random.org) and report their outcome of this and having a shared goal, by including three items measuring the degree
die-roll in an allotted text box. We used a reputable external website to to which participants experienced (1) that they were collaborating with
ensure that the die-rolls remained truly private to the participants in line the other player, (2) that they had a shared goal to obtain a certain result
with the original study. Because the die rolls were truly private, par­ together during the game, and (3) that they had a shared intention with
ticipants could inflate their profit by misreporting the actual outcome of the other player to report a certain outcome during the game. To
their die rolls. Following Weisel and Shalvi (2015) aligned incentives examine personality differences related to dishonesty, we included the
condition, participants were informed that their payment was based on Honesty-Humility subscale of 10 items from the HEXACO personality
the reported outcomes and that they would only receive money, if both inventory (Lee & Ashton, 2004). This scale has, repeatedly, been found
Player A and B reported the same number. In this case, participants to correlate with higher honesty and trustworthiness in previous work
would earn their reported number in Experimental Currency Units (Ashton & Lee, 2007; Heck et al., 2018; Ścigała et al., 2020; Thielmann
(ECU’s), in which 1 ECU = £0.30. That is, if both participants reported a & Hilbig, 2015). At the end of the survey, participants were asked a
1, they would both earn £0.30 in that round, while they would earn £1.8 range of socio-demographic questions, including gender, age, education,
if they both reported a six. household income, and number of people in the household.
After reading the instructions and filling out a payment scheme to
ensure that participants understood the payment structure, participants 2.1.4. Analysis
were paired with another anonymous player, and then randomly In line with the original study (Weisel & Shalvi, 2015), we calculated
assigned either the role of Player A or Player B. Dyads then engaged in a (1) the percentage of reported doubles (i.e., identical die-rolls by Player
practice round of the dyadic die-roll task. First, Player A would be asked A and B) in each condition, (2) the mean reported number of doubles in
to go to the external website, click on “roll die” and then report their each condition, and (3) the percentage of (totally) brazen B and A
outcome. This information was sent to Player B, who would then go to players (i.e., dyads who always report a double) across conditions. The
number of reported doubles was calculated based on the number of
times participants were shown feedback for an equal outcome across the
3
We calculated the power analysis on a one-tailed test in the pre-registration 10 trials in Phase 2.
for Study 1. A two-tailed power analysis revealed a sample of 67 dyads and 121 We also followed the same analytical strategy as in Weisel and Shalvi
individual players. (2015). First, we used a Wilcoxon-signed rank test to test for differences
4
Our funding was specifically earmarked for the respective study by the in percentage of reported doubles with the expected percentage
funder. assuming honesty (16.7%)6 in each condition. However, as the present
5
To get a similar allocation ratio as the original study and thus avoid over- study included three conditions (vs. two in Weisel & Shalvi, 2015), we
sampling in the Individual condition compared to the dyadic conditions, the
deviated from the original study’s Mann-Whitney U test and instead
allocation to the Individual condition was set to stop at 100 participants. After
used a Kruskal-Wallis test to test for differences in mean reported
this point, participants were randomly allocated to one of the two dyadic
conditions. Additional correlation analyses showed no significant association
between time-of-participation and individual characteristics, such as age (r =
6
− 0.05, p = .757), gender (r = 0.06, p = .757) and Honesty-Humility (r = − 0.01, The probability of rolling a double is 1/6, leading to an expected number of
p = .768). doubles of 1.67 (16.7%) over 10 trials.

4
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

number of doubles between the conditions. Multiple comparisons were (overall aggregate and individual items) and Honesty-Humility were
tested using Dunn’s test. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) was calculated using analyzed using Pearson’s correlations (see SM section 2.5.1. for a cor­
the R package effsize (Torchiano, 2016). We further used a Fisher’s exact relation matrix).
test to analyze whether the proportion of (totally) brazen B players Chatlogs in the Communication condition were cleaned10 and
differed between conditions. This was similarly done for brazen A analyzed based on word frequencies in terms of top 30 words written in
players.7 the chats to get insights into the most commonly used words in the social
To test whether participants in the dyads affected each other’s exchanges (See SM section 2.5.8. for results).11 All analyses were con­
behavior, we modelled a linear-mixed effects model for each player ducted in the statistical environment R (version 4.2.2. R Core Team,
(Player A and Player B). For Player A we predicted the die-roll reported 2022).
starting from round 2 (due to the sequential nature of the study design)
with fixed effects for 1) Player B’s report in the prior round (lagged), 2)
round number, 3) gender, and 4) age.8 As participants interacted in the 2.2. Results
practice round prior to the main trials, we also conducted an exploratory
linear mixed effects model using Player B’s report as an unlagged vari­ 2.2.1. Number of reported doubles
able. We deviated from the pre-registration and specified a random Overall, the pattern of the results replicates the findings of Weisel
intercept for group ID instead of participant ID with a random slope for and Shalvi (2015), although with lower levels of cheating.12 Participants
round number. Because the analyses involved dyads, we used group ID and dyads across all conditions reported significantly more doubles than
to avoid singular fits for participant ID. Similarly for Player B, we pre­ what would be expected when assuming honesty. Dyads in the No-
dicted Player B’s reported die-roll with fixed effects for: 1) reported die- communication condition reported 44.6% doubles over the 10 trials,
roll of Player A, 2) round number, 3) gender, and 4) age, and the same which is significantly higher than the expected 1.67 number of doubles
random effects structure as for the model on Player A’s behavior. (16.7%) when assuming honesty (W = 295.00, p < .001; r (rank biserial)
Exploratory linear mixed effects models were used to investigate the = − 0.81, 95% CI [− 0.88; − 0.71]). Dyads in the Communication con­
differences in mean reported doubles predicted by condition and round dition reported 60.5% doubles on average (W = 151.00, p < .001; r (rank
number (time), with an expected interaction between condition and biserial) = − 0.91, 95% CI [− 0.95; − 0.86]), while participants in the
round number (see SM section 2.5.3.). The interaction between condi­ Individual condition reported 32.8% doubles on average (W = 1136.00,
tion and round number is based on previous research finding that p < .001, r (rank biserial) = − 0.54, 95% CI [− 0.68; − 0.36]).
verifying behavior among leaders in collaborative dishonest settings Next, an independent-samples Kruskal-Wallis test showed significant
increased over time, when the leader and subordinate had conflicting differences in the mean reported number of doubles across conditions
incentives, but not when incentives were disentangled (Karg, 2021). (χ 2 (2) = 29.76, p < .001, Hedges’ g = 0.108, 95% CI(g) [0.05; 0.20],
Mean experienced collaboration and shared goals were calculated for Fig. 2). In line with the original study, dyads reported significantly more
each condition. The three items were highly correlated (Pearson’s r > doubles compared to individuals (Individual (M = 3.28, Median = 2.00,
0.50), and were therefore aggregated to create a mean score for each SD = 2.70) vs. No communication (M = 4.46, Median = 4.00, SD = 3.01),
participant (cf. Mitkidis et al., 2022). We used an independent sample t- z = 2.69, adj. p = .022, d = 0.41; Individual vs Communication (M =
test to test for differences in experienced collaboration between the two 6.05, Median = 6.00, SD = 3.50), z = 5.45, adj. p < .001, d = 0.88), with
dyadic conditions. We formulated a mediation model using the R- medium to large effects, thus supporting H1. Moreover, dyads that had
package mediation (Tingley et al., 2014) to further test whether the effect the opportunity to communicate with each other reported significantly
of condition on mean reported number of doubles was mediated by higher numbers of doubles compared to dyads that could not commu­
experienced collaboration.9 We hypothesized that communication nicate (z = 2.59, adj. p = .029), with a medium effect of d = 0.49), thus
would increase participants sense of collaboration by allowing them to supporting H2.
coordinate and make joint decisions, thereby increasing the number of An exploratory mixed effects model predicting number of reported
doubles reported. However, we acknowledge that an alternative direc­ doubles based on condition and number of rounds did not find cheating
tion for the relationship may be equally viable. Specifically, it is possible increased over the course of the 10 rounds of the task (OR = 1.02, 95%
that participants experience increased collaboration due to the shared CI [0.95, 1.07], p = .416; see also Supplemental Material section 3.2.7
success of reporting doubles during the task. for full model results). Furthermore, a correlation analysis indicated that
Correlations between number of doubles, experienced collaboration the number of reported doubles was negatively correlated with the
personality measure of Honesty-Humility (r = − 0.14, 95% CI [− 0.23,
− 0.02], p = .027).
Overall, Wilcoxon-signed rank tests revealed that dishonesty was not
7
We only pre-registered brazen B players as a main dependent variable. only related to an inflated number of doubles but also to the magnitude
However, analyses related to brazen A players were pre-registered under of reports (i.e., higher numbers yielding greater monetary outcomes).
“analysis” in the pre-registration. See SM section 2.5.5 for results. Totally Both A and B players reported significantly higher numbers than one
brazen A players are conceptualized as players who try to maximize their would expect when assuming honesty (3.5).13 Both A and B players in
earnings by consequently reporting a 6, while totally brazen B players are the Individual (A: M = 3.81, SD = 0.84, W = 1439.00, p = .004; B: M =
defined as players who consistently report a double (see Weisel & Shalvi, 2015).
8 3.77, SD = 0.85, W = 1470.50, p = .029), the No-communication (A: M
We included age and gender due to previous research suggesting that
= 3.78, SD = 0.81, W = 810,50, p = .007; B: M = 3.76, SD = 0.62, W =
dishonesty differs for gender and age, with males and younger participants
often cheating more than females and older participants (Conrads et al., 2013,
719.00, p < .001), and the Communication condition (A: M = 4.17, SD
see Gerlach et al., 2019). = 1.09, W = 678.50, p < .001; B: M = 4.26, SD = 0.99, W = 408.00, p <
9
We deviate from the pre-registration. Instead of using the shared goal item
for the mediation analysis, we changed this to the single item for experienced
10
collaboration. Because the experimental task in itself provides participants with E.g., automatic generated text in the chat logs, such as “<br>” and “***
a shared goal of getting doubles, the shared goal item was deemed less suitable Player A has joined the task ***” was removed from the chat logs.
11
for capturing variance between the conditions. Furthermore, in hindsight, the The most often used words (e.g., time, roll, 6), refer to the task at hand,
questions about having shared goals and intentions might not be as intuitive for indicating that the participants were coordinating on the task (whether it being
the participants as question about the degree they experienced they were coordination on dishonesty or dishonesty).
12
collaborating with their partner. The experienced collaboration item thus may In Weisel and Shalvi (2015) dyads in the aligned incentives treatment re­
more accurately capture the participants’ experience of the collaboration dur­ ported 81.5% doubles on average.
13
ing the task. We therefore chose to focus on the experienced collaboration item. As (1 + 2 + 3 + 4 + 5 + 6)/6 = 3.5

5
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

Fig. 2. Differences in mean number of reported doubles between conditions. Pairwise comparison using Dunn’s test; adj. p = Bonferroni. 1) Individual vs. No
communication, z = 2.69, adj. p = .022, d = 0.41; 2) Individual vs Communication, z = 5.45, adj. p < .001, d = 0.88; 3) No communication vs Communication, z =
2.59, adj. p = .029 d = 0.49.

.001), reported significantly higher numbers than expected. There was had a medium to large effect of d = 0.57 on experienced collaboration,
no difference between conditions for A players (χ2 (2) = 4.592, p = .10), while there was no significant differences between conditions for the
while B players reported higher numbers in the communication condi­ single items measuring experience of joint goals and shared intention.
tion (Individual: p < .01; No-communication: p = .010). Lastly, the effect of communication on number of reported doubles
Furthermore, the proportion of brazen A and B players,14 were was partly mediated by experienced collaboration, thereby supporting
higher in the Communication condition (14% A players, p = .009) and H3. The regression coefficient between Communication and number of
30.5% B players, p < .001, see SM section 2.5.5.). The full description reported doubles and the coefficient between experienced collaboration
and analyses for personality measures, differences in magnitude of and number of reported doubles was significant (Fig. 3.). The boot­
dishonesty, brazen players and linear mixed effects models predicting strapped unstandardized indirect effect was ACME = 0.704, and the
reports based on partners’ behavior are presented in SM (section 2.5.1., 95% confidence interval ranged from 0.40 to 1.07 (p < .001; 1000
2.5.4–2.5.6.). simulations). The effect of communication remained significant (ADE =
0.889, 95% CI [0.187; 1.59], p < .008).
2.2.2. Experienced collaboration
Overall, independent-samples t-tests revealed that dyads in the 3. Study 2: Does mere communication without task-related
Communication condition experienced a higher level of collaboration exchanges promote collaborative dishonesty?
compared to the dyads who did not communicate (t(319.27), − 2.85, p =
.005, d = 0.32, see Table 1). This was primarily driven by an increased Overall, Study 1 provides evidence suggesting that communication is
level of experienced collaboration with the other player, captured by a associated with increased experiences of collaboration, leading to
single item (i.e., experienced collaboration). Namely, communication greater magnitudes of dishonesty. However, the main underlying
mechanism for this effect remains unclear. We therefore aimed at
isolating different potential mechanisms, by investigating whether
14
Totally brazen A players are conceptualized as players who tries to maxi­ communication could corrupt irrespective of the possibility to coordi­
mize their earning by consequently reporting a 6, while totally brazen B players nate and share justifications for dishonesty (e.g., Kocher et al., 2018)
are defined as players who consistently report a double (Weisel & Shalvi, 2015). through an increased feeling of being committed with one’s co-

6
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

Table 1
Mean differences in experienced collaboration in dyadic conditions.
No Communication Communication

M SD M SD t p d
(df) (95% CI)

Overall collaboration 4.28 1.75 4.86 1.91 − 2.85 0.005 0.32


(319.27) (− 0.98, − 0.18)
Experienced collaboration 3.76 2.18 4.98 2.09 − 5.14 <0.001 0.57
(317.86) (− 1.69–0.75)
Shared goal 5.07 1.95 5.30 2.15 − 1.03 0.304 0.12
(318.78) (− 0.68, 0.21)
Shared intention 4.02 2.25 4.30 2.47 − 1.09 0.278 0.12
(318.92) (− 0.80, 0.23)

A thorough description of measures and manipulations can be found in


Supplemental Material. Participants were randomly assigned to one of
three conditions; 1) No-communication (n = 260; 130 dyads); 2) Un­
restricted communication (n = 256; 128 dyads); and 3) Restricted
communication (n = 250; 125 dyads). As in Study 1, the experiment was
programmed in Qualtrics using the SMARTRIQS application (Molnar,
2020). The experimental setup was identical to Study 1, except for
exclusion of the Individual condition, and the new Restricted commu­
Fig. 3. Mediation model of experienced collaboration on the effect of
communication on number of reported doubles. nication condition. In the Restricted communication condition, partici­
pants were given the opportunity to chat together using an online chat
function after each round, but, importantly, they were instructed to not
conspirator. We did this by varying whether participants in the
chat about the die-roll task. Instead, participants in this condition were
communicative settings were allowed to discuss and coordinate on the
given specific neutral topics to discuss. To reduce variance between
task.
conditions, we based the topics on the filler items in the No-
Overall, Study 2 aimed to replicate the finding that (unrestricted)
communication condition (e.g., What is your favorite hobby?).
communication increases dishonesty (H1, Study 1 H2). Since commu­
nication may facilitate a sense of commitment, we further expected that
3.1.3. Measures
dyads that were restricted from task-relevant communication would
We used the same items as in Study 1 to capture participants’
report a higher number of doubles compared to pairs that were not able
experienced collaboration with the other player and also included a
to communicate at all (H2). Similarly to Study 1, we expected that the
measure of participants’ sense of commitment (Michael et al., 2016b).
effect of communication would be mediated by an increased experience
Inspired by Michael et al. (2016a), we included three items in terms of 1)
of collaborating with the other player (H3).
how likely the participants think it is that they would help the other
participant if they were to collaborate again (Likert-type scale from 1 =
3.1. Methods very unlikely to 7 = very likely), 2) how likely they think it is that the
other participant would help them if they were to collaborate again (1 =
3.1.1. Participants very unlikely to 7 = very likely), and 3) how obligated they feel to help
We recruited a total of 796 participants from Prolific. Eleven par­ the other participant (from 1 = not at all to 7 = very much). The items
ticipants failed an attention check, and eleven participants were missing were used based on the prediction that if participants felt a sense of
a partner after we removed the participants who failed the attention commitment with their partner, they would be more likely to help (and
check and were subsequently removed from the analyses. Four dyads in expect help from) their partner. Following Study 1, the Honesty-
the Restricted communication condition did not comply with the rules of Humility subscale (Lee & Ashton, 2004) and socio-demographic mea­
their conditions and discussed coordinating their rolls in the die-roll sures were also included.
game, leaving a final sample size of N = 766 (n = 383 dyads; 49.35%
women, 49.61% men, mean age = 41.04, SD = 12.85). Identification 3.1.4. Analysis
and exclusion plan as well as results including participants who failed We replicated all analyses from Study 1. Inspired by (Zultan, 2012),
attention checks are reported in the SM (section 3.1.2.). we further conducted an exploratory analysis testing differences in
Based on the effect size of communication (vs. no-communication) in proportion of coordination on Nash equilibrium under money-
Study 1, a Mann-Whitney U a priori power analysis yielded a required maximization (i.e., Player A reports a 6 and Player B matches Player
sample size of 56 dyads in each condition (d = 0.49, 1-β = 0.80, α = A’s report)16 over the 10 trials between conditions using Chi-square
0.05).15 Additionally, due to more financial resources than originally tests. In addition to analyses for experienced collaboration included in
estimated, we continued recruiting until our resources were depleted. A Study 1, mean differences in commitment between conditions were
one-way ANOVA sensitivity power analysis (α = 0.05, 1-β = 0.8) indi­ analyzed using a one-way ANOVA. This was followed by separate
cated that our sample size (N = 383 dyads), is sensitive to detect an mediation analyses for the unrestricted and restricted communication
effect of Cohen’s d = 0.16 (G*Power 3.1). conditions testing whether the effect of the two communication condi­
tions on number of reported doubles was mediated by experienced
3.1.2. Design and procedure
In this study, we report all measures, manipulations and exclusions.
16
The Nash equilibrium is a game theoretic concept and refers to situations,
where no player obtains a higher utility by changing their strategy, if the other
15
The pre-registration was based on a one-tailed t-test a priori power analysis. players stick to their strategy (Kreps, 1989). In a dyadic die roll setting, this
To be conservative we ran a two-tailed Mann-Whitney U test a priori power would equate to situations, where Player A reports a 6, and Player B matches
analysis, indicating a required sample size of 70 dyads in each condition. the report of Player A.

7
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

collaboration. We further ran a similar exploratory mediation analysis increased collaborative cheating: dyads in the Unrestricted condition
for sense of commitment. Similar to Study 1, the chatlogs in both (mirroring the communication condition in Study 1) reported signifi­
communication conditions were cleaned and analyzed based on word cantly more doubles on average (M = 5.16, Median = 4.50, SD = 3.27)
frequencies in terms of top 30 words written in the chats (see SM section compared to dyads that were not able to communicate (M = 3.38, Me­
3.2.8.). dian = 3.00, SD = 2.62, z = 4.33, adj. p < .001), with a medium to large
effect of d = 0.59. Furthermore, dyads that were able to communicate,
3.2. Results but not about the task (i.e., Restricted communication condition), re­
ported significantly more doubles (M = 4.36, Median = 3.00, SD = 3.06)
3.2.1. Number of reported doubles than non-communicative dyads (z = 2.46, adj. p = .041, d = \0.34) with
Overall, Study 2 replicated the results of Study 1. Dyads across all a small effect of d = 0.34, supporting H2.
conditions reported significantly more doubles than what would be There was no significant difference between the two communication
expected assuming honesty. Dyads in the No-communication condition conditions (z = − 1.84, adj, p = .198, d = 0.25). However, an exploratory
reported the least doubles with an average of 34% of reported doubles chi-square analysis revealed that there was a higher proportion of co­
on average (W = 1711.00, p < .001; r (rank biserial) = − 0.60, 95% CI ordination on the Nash equilibrium under money maximization in the
[− 0.71, − 0.46]). This was followed by dyads in the Restricted Unrestricted condition (20.47%) compared to both the No-
communication condition with 43.6% reported doubles on average (W communication (8.14%; χ2(1) = 71.362, p < .001) and the Restricted
= 819.00, p < .001; r (rank biserial) = − 0.79, 95% CI [− 0.86, − 0.70]). communication condition (11.62%; χ2(1) = 29.157, p < .001), indi­
Dyads in the Unrestricted communication condition reported the highest cating higher levels of coordination between participants on maximizing
number of doubles, with dyads reporting 51.6% doubles on average (W their payment, when task-related communication was allowed.
= 534.00, p < .001; r (rank biserial) = − 0.87, 95% CI [− 0.91, − 0.81]). Similar to Study 1, an exploratory mixed effects model predicting
An Independent-Samples Kruskal-Wallis test revealed significant number of reported doubles by condition and round number revealed no
differences in the mean reported number of doubles across conditions significant effect of round (odds ratio = 1.02, 95% CI [0.99, 1.05], p =
(χ 2 (2) = 18.93, p < .001, Hedges’ g = 0.05, 95% CI(g) [0.01–0.10], .127, see SM section 3.2.3.). Results for magnitude of cheating, pro­
Fig. 4). In line with Study 1 and in support of H1, communication portion of brazen players, and linear mixed effects models predicting

Fig. 4. Differences in number of reported doubles between conditions. Pairwise comparison using Dunn’s test; adj. p = Bonferroni. 1) No communication (M =
3.38, SD = 2.62) vs. Unrestricted communication (M = 5.16, SD = 3.27), z = 4.33, adj. p < .001, d = 0.59; 2) No-communication vs Restricted communication (M =
4.36, SD = 3.06), z = 2.46, adj. p = .041, d = 0.34; 3) Unrestricted communication vs Restricted communication, z = − 1.84, adj, p = .198, d = 0.25.

8
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

reports based on partners behavior, are also presented in SM (section communication and Restriction communication condition (t(503) =
3.2.4. – 3.2.6.). − 1.38, p = .355, 95% CI [− 0.17, 0.66], d = 0.12).
Concerning the obligation question, participants in the Unrestricted
3.2.2. Experienced collaboration and sense of commitment condition reported higher level of being obligated to help their co-player
A one-way ANOVA revealed that the overall experienced collabo­ than participants in the No communication condition (t(512) = − 3.37, p
ration (aggregated measure) differed significantly across groups (F = .003, 95% CI [0.18, 1.04], d = 0.30). There were no differences be­
(2,763) = 10.28, p < .001, η2 = 0.026, see Table 2 for an overview of tween the No communication and Restricted communication condition
ANOVA results, means and variances of the different conditions).17 (t(507) = − 1.89, p = .145, 95% CI [− 0.09, 0.78], d = 0.17) and the two
Tukey’s post-hoc tests showed that dyads in the Unrestricted commu­ communication conditions (t(503) = 1.40, p = .328, 95% CI [− 0.70,
nication condition reported significantly higher levels of experienced 0.17], d = 0.13).
collaboration compared to the No-communication condition (t(513) = In line with Study 1, we found a significant partial mediation effect of
− 3.55, p < .001, 95% CI [0.19, 0.96], d = 0.31) and the Restricted experienced collaboration on the effect of Unrestricted communication,
communication condition, (t(503) = 4.17, p < .001, 95% CI [− 1.07, supporting H3. The bootstrapped unstandardized indirect effect was
0.32], d = 0.37). There was no significant difference between the No- ACME = 0.472 and the 95% confidence interval ranged from 0.28 to
communication and Restricted communication condition (t(503) = 0.70. The direct effect of Unrestricted communication was ADE = 1.287
0.78, p = .718, 95% CI [− 0.52, 0.26], d = 0.07). (95% CI [0.80; 1.77]). The effect of Restricted communication was
In line with Study 1, this was primarily driven by a higher level of likewise partly mediated by experienced collaboration. The mediation
experienced collaboration on the single item measure. Both the Unre­ analysis revealed a significant bootstrapped unstandardized indirect
stricted (t(513) = − 6.54, p < .001, 95% CI [0.78, 1.66], d = 0.58) and effect of ACME = 0.089 (95% CI [0.16; 0.19]). The direct effect of
Restricted condition (t(505) = − 2.93, p = .010, 95% CI [0.11, 1.00], d = Restricted communication was ADE = 0.869 (95% CI [0.40; 1.37]). All
0.26) had a higher average experienced collaboration compared to the mediation analyses used 1000 simulations to bootstrap.
No-communication. Participants in the Unrestricted condition similarly We performed similar exploratory analyses for commitment. There
scored higher than the participants in the Restricted condition (t(505) = was a significant indirect effect of Unrestricted communication through
2.93, p < .001, 95% CI [− 1.11, 0.22], d = 0.31). commitment (ACME = 0.351, 95% CI [0.17; 0.57]), with a significant
There were no significant differences in experience of shared goal direct effect of ADE = 1.416 (95% CI [0.97; 1.88]). Thus, the effect of
between the No-communication and the Unrestricted communication (t Unrestricted communication was partly mediated by commitment. The
(514) = − 1.91, p = .145, 95% CI [− 0.09, 0.82], d = 0.17) and the effect of Restricted communication was, however, not mediated by
Restricted communication condition (t(505) = 2.25, p = .061, 95% CI commitment. The indirect effect was not statistically significant (ACME
[− 0.90, 0.02], d = 0.20). Dyads in the Unrestricted condition, however, = 0.069, 95% CI [− 0.01; 0.16]) (see SM Fig. S4 section 3.2.7. for
scored higher on experiences a shared goal compared to dyads in the overview of mediations for experienced collaboration and
Restricted communication conditions (t(501) = 4.10, p < .001, 95% CI commitment).
[− 1.27, − 0.35], d = 0.37). Furthermore, participants in the Restricted
condition reported lower levels of shared intentions compared to both 4. General discussion
the Unrestricted (t(503) = 3.12, p = .004, 95% CI [− 1.11, − 0.17], d =
0.28) and No-communication condition (t(506) = 2.54, p = .038, 95% CI Instead of deterring dishonesty, our results suggest that including
[− 0.96, − 0.02], d = 0.22). There was no significant difference between communication facilitates dishonesty in collaborative settings. Overall,
the Unrestricted and No-communication condition (t(507) = − 0.73, p = Study 1 successfully replicated the core findings of Weisel and Shalvi
.741, 95% CI [− 0.32, 0.62], d = 0.06). (2015), finding that participants engaged in a dyadic die roll paradigm
When investigating the effect of the condition on feelings of cheated more for monetary gains compared to when people played
commitment, a one-way ANOVA revealed an overall effect (F(2,763) = alone. Study 1 offered further compelling evidence indicating that
7.23, p < .001, eta2 = 0.019, see Table 2 for overview of means and communication between participants increases the magnitude of
variances of the different conditions). Again, participants in the Unre­ cheating beyond the original study’s non-communicative settings.
stricted communication condition scored higher in overall commitment Importantly, this effect was linked to a stronger experienced collabo­
compared to participants in the No-communication condition (t(508) = ration among the communicating dyads, highlighting that communica­
− 3.88, p < .001, 95% CI [0.23, 0.97], d = 0.34), but not compared to the tion is not only key to everyday ethically sound collaborations, but also
Restricted communication condition(t(503) = 2.07, p = .082, 95% CI to corrupt collaborations. Study 2 replicated this finding, showing that
[− 0.71, 0.03], d = 0.18). There was no difference between the No- communication significantly increases collaborative cheating compared
communication and Restricted communication condition (t(496) = to non-communicative contexts, even when coordination is restricted.
− 0.164, p = .238, 95% CI [− 0.12, 0.63], d = 0.15). Both commitment and experienced collaboration mediated the effect of
When asked how likely it was that they would help their co-player unrestricted communication on dishonesty. However, when dyads could
again, this was higher for participants in the Unrestricted condition chat but were not allowed to coordinate on the task, dishonesty was only
compared to the No-communication condition (t(513) = − 2.40, p = mediated by the experience of collaborating and not a sense of
.045, 95% CI [0.01, 0.85], d = 0.21). There was no significant differ­ commitment.
ences between the No-communication and Restricted condition (t(506) Study 2’s findings illuminate the role of communication in collabo­
= − 0.99, p = .585, 95% CI [− 0.25, 0.60], d = 0.09) and between the two rative dishonesty. Similar to the presumptions of prior research (Beck
communication condition (t(503)4 = 1.37, p = .350, 95% CI [− 0.68, et al., 2020; Kocher et al., 2018), the ability to communicate about the
0.17], d = 0.12). Similarly, participants in the Unrestricted condition task, potentially enabling coordination and justification exchange,
scored higher when asked how likely they thought it was that their co- resulted in the highest levels of dishonesty. Interestingly, this was not
player would help them, compared to the No-communication (t(512) significantly different from dyads who were not allowed to discuss the
= − 4.31, p < .001, 95% CI [0.34, 1.16], d = 0.38) and Restricted task. This suggests that discussing everyday topics alone might increase
communication condition (t(503) = 2.80, p = .012, 95% CI [− 0.92, collaborative dishonest behavior, regardless of coordination and justi­
− 0.09], d = 0.25). There was no significant difference between the No- fication exchange. This effect was partly linked to experienced collab­
oration, but not significantly connected to commitment.
However, despite the non-significant difference in number of re­
17
See also Table S15 in SM section 3.2.7. for an overview of results and ported doubles between the two communicative settings in Study 2, the
pairwise comparisons. results also indicate that restricting the communication to non-task

9
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

Table 2
Differences in mean experienced collaboration and commitment across conditions.
No communication Unrestricted communication Restricted communication ANOVA

M SD M SD M SD F(df) p Eta2

Overall collaboration 3.78 1.81 4.36 1.88 3.65 1.92 10.28 <0.001 0.026
(2763)
Experienced collaboration 3.29 2.08 4.51 2.15 3.84 2.17 21.12 <0.001 0.052
(2763)
Shared goal 4.39 2.18 4.76 2.17 3.95 2.27 8.52 0.001 0.022
(2763)
Shared intention 3.67 2.17 3.82 2.40 3.18 2.22 5.53 0.004 0.014
(2763)
Overall commitment 4.07 1.66 4.66 1.83 4.32 1.86 7.23 <0.001 0.019
(2.76)
Likelihood of helping Person X 4.65 1.99 5.07 2.07 4.82 2.05 2.88 0.057 0.007
(2763)
Belief that Person X would help 4.17 1.93 4.92 2.02 4.39 2.05 9.43 0.001 0.024
(2763)
Feeling of obligation to help 3.38 2.00 3.99 2.11 3.72 2.14 5.59 0.004 0.014
(2763)

F statistic based on one-way ANOVA. Overall collaboration is based on an aggregated measure of the three items. Overall Commitment is likewise based on an
aggregate measure of three items. NC = No communication, UC = Unrestricted communication, RC = Restricted communication, M = mean, SD = standard deviation,
df = degrees of freedom.

related topics reduces the magnitude of the cheating in terms of coor­ see Frollová et al., 2021; Heyman et al., 2020; Karg, 2021). Neverthe­
dination on obtaining a maximum payoff. This is in line with previous less, this does not diminish the actual dishonesty of misreporting in the
research studying the differential effect of task-related and non-task- present studies. Furthermore, people differ in how (dis)honest they are
related communication in collaboration. For instance, Zultan (2012) (Thielmann et al., 2023) and several studies have shown that behavior in
found that removing strategic communication by excluding game- cheating paradigms is also related to basic personality traits, such as the
related discussion led to similar levels of offers in the ultimatum Honesty-Humility trait (e.g., Pfattheicher et al., 2019; Ścigała et al.,
game, while the responders were more willing to accept lower offers 2020; Thielmann & Hilbig, 2015). If dishonesty in cheating paradigms
when communication was restricted. However, there was a slightly were only due to demand effects, it would arguably be unlikely that
higher proportion of equal splits between participants when communi­ interindividual differences would be linked to basic personality traits.19
cation was not restricted, indicating higher coordination on a mutually Indeed, in line with previous research, dishonesty was negatively
beneficial outcome for the participants (Zultan, 2012). Together with correlated with the trait of Honesty-Humility in the present study.
the study by Zultan (2012), our study suggests that behavior under Thirdly, as previous research has shown that coordinating in a die
different communicative settings may be related to different mecha­ roll task increases trust and social bonds (Karg et al., 2023), the direction
nisms. While we did not find a significant effect of commitment when of the mediations in the present studies may be reversed. More research
communication was restricted, other mechanisms, such as prosocial that disentangles the relationship between communication, collabora­
concerns for the other person may be at play. Thus, rather than looking tion, and dishonesty is needed. Lastly, our commitment items were
at a unified explanation for the effect of communication, more research focused on future help, which may not fully capture the commitment
into the differential mechanisms is important, both when it comes to fostered by communication. Instead, the effect could be related to
ethical and unethical collaborations, respectively. relationship building. The restricted communication topics constituted
different forms of self-disclosure (i.e., the sharing og personal informa­
tion; Cozby, 1973), that allowed the participants to get to know each
4.1. Limitations other. In fact, previous research has indicated that people become more
generous towards others the more they know about them (Bohnet &
Besides certain differences between the present studies and the Frey, 1999; Charness & Gneezy, 2008). Further research into the role of
original study of Weisel and Shalvi (2015),18 some general limitations such social exchanges in collaborative dishonest behaviors is warranted.
deserve consideration. The first is related to the inherent nature of In conclusion, this paper highlights two key directions for future
experimental studies prioritizing high internal validity at the expense of research. First, it emphasizes the role of communication in shaping
external validity. Transitioning from die roll tasks to real-life cases of collaborative dishonest behaviors, urging future investigation into its
collaborative dishonesty outside the lab is indeed a substantial leap. various mechanisms. Although Study 2 offers initial insights into such
Some studies have found that dishonesty in the lab correlate positively mechanisms, future studies should differentiate between the exchange
with real-world dishonesty (e.g., Dai et al., 2018; Schild et al., 2021). of justifications, coordination, commitment, and other factors such as
However, because communication between individuals has often been a relationship building to understand how communication influences
necessary prerequisite for corruption to evolve in many major fraud collaborative dishonesty. Specifically, it is crucial to further develop
scandals, including communication into collaborative paradigms is a strong measures and manipulations to help differentiate between rele­
step towards increasing the generalizability towards real-world unethi­ vant mechanisms. This also relates to measures of experienced collab­
cal collaborations. oration and commitment in these settings.
Secondly, another issue pertains to the demand-effects associated Additionally, exploring situations where communication not only
with the die roll task. Cheating paradigms often provide participants facilitates but rather hinders collaborative dishonesty is pivotal. How
with ambiguous instructions that may indicate an expectation for strategic (unrestricted) communication may alter the beliefs and
cheating, potentially influencing their behavior (for similar concerns,

18 19
A description of deviances from the original study is provided in SM (section We would like to thank one of the anonymous reviewers for this insightful
2.3.). point.

10
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

preferences for (dis)honesty of players in these settings is especially Declaration of Competing interest
important. That is, under which circumstances may strategic commu­
nication be more effectful in either persuading others to act dishonestly, The authors declare no competing interest.
or more importantly, when may it serve as an important tool to talk
someone out of dishonesty? Furthermore, the reduced coordination Data availability
when communication was restricted to non-task related topics warrants
further examination. All data is openly available at the Open Science Framework
Another interesting avenue is to study the impact of communication (https://osf.io/gwn3r/?
in a more real-word context, where individuals can perceive both their view_only=3de67cf4bd0a451f87aa38e065c13516)
own and other’s reputations as being even more at stake. For instance,
research has suggested that reducing distance and anonymity may Acknowledgments
decrease dishonesty (Conrads & Lotz, 2015). Conversely, face-to-face
communication may increase the effect of communication. Indeed, We would like to thank the Centre for Integrative Business Psy­
face-to-face communication has previously been related to stronger ef­ chology, Aarhus University, for providing funding for this research.
fects than those observed when using written communication (Brosig,
Weimann, & Ockenfels, 2003; Zultan, 2012). Thus, studying the impact
Appendix A. Supplementary data
of communication in face-to-face interactions in the lab would provide
important knowledge on how communication impact dishonesty when
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
anonymity is reduced. As collaborative endeavors rarely happen
org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104603.
without prior interaction and communication, future research investi­
gating how these behaviors unfold in already established social groups
(e.g., employees or friends), would further help move the field of References
collaborative dishonesty closer to real-life corrupt collaborations.
Andreoni, J., & Rao, J. M. (2011). The power of asking: How communication affects
Although few studies have included friends (e.g., Okano & Goto, 2023), selfishness, empathy, and altruism. Journal of Public Economics, 95(7–8), 513–520.
we are not aware of studies investigating these behaviors among col­ https://EconPapers.repec.org/RePEc:eee:pubeco:v:95:y:2011:i:7-8:p:513-520.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the
leagues in organizations.
HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11
Second, given that our results reveal different levels of experienced (2), 150–166. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868306294907
collaboration between conditions, future research should consider var­ Beck, T., Bühren, C., Frank, B., & Khachatryan, E. (2020). Can honesty oaths, peer
iations in collaboration. Investigating how different levels of collabo­ interaction, or monitoring mitigate lying? Journal of Business Ethics, 163(3),
467–484. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-018-4030-z
ration are related to distinct forms of corrupt behaviors, will offer Bohnet, I., & Frey, B. S. (1999). Social Distance and Other-Regarding Behavior in Dictator
important insights for mitigation strategies aimed at hindering corrup­ Games. Comment. American Economic Review, 89(1), 335–339. https://doi.org/
tion and corporate fraud. Indeed, some collaborations may be initiated 10.1257/aer.89.1.335
Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2011). A cooperative species: Human reciprocity and its evolution.
by private, self-directed goals, other may be based on the goal of the Princeton University Press.
group (Tuomela, 2020), potentially giving rise to different forms of Bratman, M. E. (1992). Shared cooperative activity. The Philosophical Review, 101(2),
corrupt collaborations. Finally, if we want to study people colluding on 327–341. https://doi.org/10.2307/2185537
Brosig, J., Weimann, J., & Ockenfels, A. (2003). The Effect of Communication Media on
deceiving others together, it is essential understand the circumstances for Cooperation. German Economic Review, 4(2), 217–241. https://doi.org/10.1111/
this type of collaboration and to develop experimental designs that can 1468-0475.00080
captures the nuances of collaboration. Our assertion is that the inte­ Buck, R., & VanLear, C. A. (2002). Verbal and nonverbal communication: Distinguishing
symbolic, spontaneous, and pseudo-spontaneous nonverbal behavior. Journal of
gration of communication serves as the initial stride in advancing our
Communication, 52(3), 522–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.
understanding of the collective dimension inherent in collaborative tb02560.x
dishonesty. Butterfill, S. (2012). Joint action and development. The Philosophical Quarterly, 62(246),
23–47. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9213.2011.00005.x
Cadsby, C. B., Du, N., & Song, F. (2016). In-group favoritism and moral decision-making.
Open science practices Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 128, 59–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jebo.2016.05.008
All materials, data, and analyses codes for Studies 1 and 2 are pub­ Castillo, G., Choo, L., & Grimm, V. (2022). Are groups always more dishonest than
individuals? The case of salient negative externalities. Journal of Economic Behavior
licly accessible at the OSF [https://osf.io/gwn3r/]. The hypotheses, & Organization, 198, 598–611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2022.04.014
exclusion criteria, sampling, and analysis plan were pre-registered. Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2006). Promises and partnership. Econometrica, 74(6),
The preregistration for Study 1 can be found at https://doi.org/10 1579–1601. http://www.jstor.org/stable/4123084.
Charness, G., & Gneezy, U. (2008). What’s in a name? Anonymity and social distance in
.17605/OSF.IO/NP96H. dictator and ultimatum games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 68(1),
The preregistration for Study 2 can be found at https://doi.org/10 29–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2008.03.001
.17605/OSF.IO/SJYFD. In these studies, we report all measures, ma­ Conrads, J., Irlenbusch, B., Rilke, R. M., & Walkowitz, G. (2013). Lying and team
incentives. Journal of Economic Psychology, 34, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
nipulations and exclusions. joep.2012.10.011
The studies were approved by the Ethical Committee of the Institu­ Conrads, J., & Lotz, S. (2015). The effect of communication channels on dishonest
tional Review Board at the authors’ institution (BSS-2022-073). behavior. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 58, 88–93. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.socec.2015.06.006
Cozby, P. C. (1973). Self-disclosure: A literature review. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 73–91.
CRediT authorship contribution statement https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033950
Dai, Z., Galeotti, F., & Villeval, M. C. (2018). Cheating in the lab predicts fraud in the
field: An experiment in public transportation. Management Science, 64(3),
Mathilde H. Tønnesen: Writing – review & editing, Writing –
1081–1100. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2016.2616
original draft, Visualization, Software, Project administration, Method­ De Cremer, D., & Barker, M. (2003). Accountability and cooperation in social dilemmas:
ology, Investigation, Funding acquisition, Formal analysis, Data cura­ The influence of others’ reputational concerns. Current Psychology, 22(2), 155–163.
tion, Conceptualization. Christian T. Elbæk: Writing – review & https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-003-1006-6
De Saussure, L., & Rocci, A. (2016). Verbal communication: An introduction. In Verbal
editing, Methodology, Formal analysis, Conceptualization. Stefan communication (Handbook of Communication Sciences, vol. 3) (pp. 3–20).
Pfattheicher: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Methodology, Erat, S., & Gneezy, U. (2012). White lies. Management Science, 58(4), 723–733. https://
Conceptualization. Panagiotis Mitkidis: Writing – review & editing, doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1110.1449
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical
Supervision, Methodology, Conceptualization. power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior
Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146

11
M.H. Tønnesen et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 112 (2024) 104603

Frollová, N., Vranka, M., & Houdek, P. (2021). A qualitative study of perception of a Moore, R. (2018). Gricean communication, joint action, and the evolution of
dishonesty experiment. Journal of Economic Methodology, 28(3), 274–290. https:// cooperation. Topoi, 37(2), 329–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-016-9372-5
doi.org/10.1080/1350178X.2021.1936598 Nowak, M. A. (2000). Evolutionary biology of language. Philosophical Transactions of the
Gerlach, P., Teodorescu, K., & Hertwig, R. (2019). The truth about lies: A meta-analysis Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 355(1403), 1615–1622. https://
on dishonest behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 145(1), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/ doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2000.0723
bul0000174 Okano, Y., & Goto, E. (2023). Groups versus individuals, partial lying, and social image
Gross, J., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2020). Rule following mitigates collaborative cheating concern in a dice-rolling experiment. The Japanese Economic Review.. https://doi.
and facilitates the spreading of honesty within groups [article]. Personality and Social org/10.1007/s42973-023-00139-0
Psychology Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167220927195 Pfattheicher, S., Schindler, S., & Nockur, L. (2019). On the impact of honesty-humility
Gross, J., Leib, M., Offerman, T., & Shalvi, S. (2018). Ethical free riding: When honest and a cue of being watched on cheating behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 71,
people find dishonest partners. Psychological Science, 29(12), 1956–1968. https:// 159–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2018.06.004
doi.org/10.1177/0956797618796480 R Core Team. (2022). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna,
Heck, D. W., Thielmann, I., Moshagen, M., & Hilbig, B. E. (2018). Who lies? A large-scale Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.
reanalysis linking basic personality traits to unethical decision making. Judgment and Roth, A. E. (1995). Bargaining experiments. Handbook of Experimental Economics, 1,
Decision making, 13, 356–371. 253–348.
Heyman, T., Vankrunkelsven, H., Voorspoels, W., White, A., Storms, G., & Verheyen, S. Schild, C., Heck, D. W., Ścigała, K. A., & Zettler, I. (2019). Revisiting REVISE: (re)testing
(2020). When cheating is an honest mistake: A critical evaluation of the matrix task as a unique and combined effects of REminding, VIsibility, and SElf-engagement
measure of dishonesty (p. 6). Collabra: Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1525/ manipulations on cheating behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 75(Part A).
collabra.294 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2019.04.001
Irlenbusch, B., Mussweiler, T., Saxler, D. J., Shalvi, S., & Weiss, A. (2020). Similarity Schild, C., Lilleholt, L., & Zettler, I. (2021). Behavior in cheating paradigms is linked to
increases collaborative cheating [article]. Journal of Economic Behavior and overall approval rates of crowdworkers. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 34(2),
Organization, 178, 148–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2020.06.022 157–166. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2195
Jamaluddin, S. F., Adi, S. P., & Lufityanto, G. (2020). Social influences on cheating in Ścigała, K. A., Schild, C., & Zettler, I. (2020). Dishonesty as a signal of trustworthiness:
collectivistic culture: Collaboration but not competition. Group Dynamics: Theory, Honesty-humility and trustworthy dishonesty. Royal Society Open Science, 7(10),
Research, and Practice. Article 200685. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200685
Karg, S. T. (2021). Partners in Crime: Investigating influence factors and outcomes of Shalvi, S., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2014). Oxytocin promotes group-serving dishonesty.
ocllaborative corruption. Aarhus University. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(15), 5503–5507. https://doi.
Karg, S. T., Kim, M., Mitkidis, P., & Young, L. (2023). Collaborative cheating in org/10.1073/pnas.1400724111
hierarchical teams: Effects of incentive structure and leader behavior on subordinate Shalvi, S., Weisel, O., Kochavi-Gamliel, S., & Leib, M. (2016). Corrupt collaboration: A
behavior and perceptions of leaders. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(8), behavioral ethics approach. In J.-W. V. Prooijen, & P. A. M. V. Lange (Eds.), Cheating,
1166–1183. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672221090859 corruption, and concealment: The roots of dishonesty (pp. 134–148). Cambridge
Kerr, N. L., & Kaufman-Gilliland, C. M. (1994). Communication, commitment, and University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316225608.009.
cooperation in social dilemma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, Simonsohn, U. (2015). Small telescopes: Detectability and the evaluation of replication
513–529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.3.513 results. Psychological Science, 26(5), 559–569. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Kocher, M. G., Schudy, S., & Spantig, L. (2018). I lie? We lie! Why? Experimental 0956797614567341
evidence on a dishonesty shift in groups [article]. Management Science, 64(9), Soraperra, I., Weisel, O., Zultan, R. I., Kochavi, S., Leib, M., Shalev, H., & Shalvi, S.
3995–4008. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2017.2800 (2017). The bad consequences of teamwork. Economics Letters, 160, 12–15. https://
Korbel, V. (2017). Do we lie in groups? An experimental evidence. Applied Economics doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2017.08.011
Letters, 24(15), 1107–1111. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504851.2016.1259734 Sturm, R. E. (2017). Decreasing unethical decisions: The role of morality-based
Kreps, D. M. (1989). Nash Equilibrium. In J. Eatwell, M. Milgate, & P. Newman (Eds.), individual differences. Journal of Business Ethics, 142(1), 37–57. https://doi.org/
Game theory (pp. 167–177). Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1007/978- 10.1007/s10551-015-2787-x
1-349-20181-5_19. Thielmann, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2015). The traits one can trust:dissecting reciprocity and
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality kindness as determinants of trustworthy behavior. Personality and Social Psychology
inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 329–358. https://doi.org/ Bulletin, 41(11), 1523–1536. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215600530
10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8 Thielmann, I., Hilbig, B. E., Klein, S. A., Seidl, A., & Heck, D. W. (2023). Cheating to
Leib, M., Köbis, N., Soraperra, I., Weisel, O., & Shalvi, S. (2021). Collaborative benefit others? On the relation between honesty-humility and prosocial lies. Journal
dishonesty: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 147(12), 1241–1268. of Personality, n/a(n/a). https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12835
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000349 Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2014). Mediation: R package
Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of for causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 59(5), 1–38. https://doi.
self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633–644. https:// org/10.18637/jss.v059.i05
doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.45.6.633 Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. MIT Press.
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution: How darwinian theory can explain human culture Tomasello, M. (2016). A Natural History of Human Morality. Harvard University Press.
and synthesize the social sciences. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/ http://www.jstor.org/stable/j.ctv1g13jjn.
9780226520452.001.0001 Torchiano, M. (2016). Effsize - A package for efficient effect size computation. https://doi.
Michael, J., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2016a). Observing joint action: Coordination org/10.5281/zenodo.196082
creates commitment. Cognition, 157, 106–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Tuomela, R. (2020). We-thinking, we-mode, and group agents. Social Ontology,
cognition.2016.08.024 Normativity and Law, 11.
Michael, J., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2016b). The sense of commitment: A minimal Weisel, O., & Shalvi, S. (2015). The collaborative roots of corruption. Proceedings of the
approach [hypothesis and theory]. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/ National Academy of Sciences, 112(34), 10651–10656. https://doi.org/10.1073/
10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01968 pnas.1423035112
Mitkidis, P., Chrysochou, P., Obolevich, V., & Mitkidis, K. (2022). Effectiveness of Wouda, J., Bijlstra, G., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2017). The
environmental health and loss framing on household pharmaceutical take-back collaborative roots of corruption? A replication of Weisel & Shalvi (2015). Collabra:
schemes. Waste Management, 143, 61–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Psychology, 3(1). https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.97
wasman.2022.02.017 Zickfeld, J., Karg, S. T., Engen, S. S., Gonzalez, A. S. R., Michael, J., & Mitkidis, P. (2022).
Mitkidis, P., Perkovic, S., Nichols, A., Elbæk, C. T., Gerlach, P., & Ariely, D. (2023). Committed dishonesty: A systematic meta-analysis of the effect of social commitment on
Morality in minimally deceptive environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: dishonest behavior.
Applied. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000476. No Pagination Specified-No Zickfeld, J. H., Ścigała, K. A., Weiss, A., Michael, J., & Mitkidis, P. (2023). Commitment
Pagination Specified. to honesty oaths decreases dishonesty, but commitment to another individual does
Mitkidis, P., Sørensen, J., Nielbo, K. L., Andersen, M., & Lienard, P. (2013). Collective- not affect dishonesty. Communications Psychology, 1(1), 27. https://doi.org/10.1038/
goal ascription increases cooperation in humans. PLoS One, 8(5), Article e64776. s44271-023-00028-7
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064776 Zultan, R. I. (2012). Strategic and social pre-play communication in the ultimatum game.
Molnar, A. (2020). How to implement real-time interaction between participants in Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 425–434. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
online surveys: A practical guide to SMARTRIQS. The Quantitative Methods for joep.2011.12.009
Psychology, 16(4), 334–354. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.16.4.p334 (IN FILE)

12

You might also like