Gtzetal 2020

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/342171483

Children, COVID-19 and the media A STUDY ON THE CHALLENGES CHILDREN


ARE FACING IN THE 2020 CORONAVIRUS CRISIS

Article · June 2020

CITATIONS READS

0 84

57 authors, including:

Maya Götz Dafna Lemish


Internationales Zentralinstitut für das Jugend- und Bildungsfernsehen Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey
47 PUBLICATIONS 73 CITATIONS 104 PUBLICATIONS 1,756 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Anne-Linda Camerini Macarena García González


University of Lugano University of Zurich
32 PUBLICATIONS 268 CITATIONS 18 PUBLICATIONS 19 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

MEDIATICINO View project

Encyclopedia item View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Maya Götz on 15 June 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


RESEARCH

Children, COVID-19 and the media


A STUDY ON THE CHALLENGES CHILDREN ARE FACING IN THE
2020 CORONAVIRUS CRISIS
Maya Götz, Caroline Mendel, Dafna Lemish, Nancy Jennings, Rebecca Hains,
Fatima Abdul, Meryl Alper, Hania Asgari, Hadiza Babayaro, Catherine Blaya, Dina Borzekowski, Sandra Cadavid, Anne-
Linda Camerini, Beth Carmona, Cynthia Carter, Mussa Chale, Mareike Düssel, Aldana Duhalde, Andria Gayed, Patricia
Núñez Gómez, Macarena García González, Yuval Gozansky, Eileen Sanabria Herrera, Kirsten Huang, Liselot Hudders,
Yisra Al-Haj Hussein, Hayuki Ishikawa, Ruchi Kher Jaggi, Colleen Russo Johnson, Andreas Klempin, Jennifer Kotler, Cheryl
Kotwal, Geoff Lealand, Sun Sun Lim, Thomas Enemark Lundtofte, Mónica Maruri, Giovanna Mascheroni, Markus Mendel,
Máire Messenger-Davis, Joél Mulongo, Diana Nastasia, Sorin Nastasia, Christina Ortner, Grace Torres Panganiban, Nilüfer
Pembecioğlu, Martina Peštaj, Adrianna Ruggiero, Ulises Soto Ruiz, Isolde Stanczak, Jeanette Steemers, Fadi Taher, Sandra
Téllez, Moses Tholley, Ekatarina Yakusheva, Andrew Zi Han Yee

An international study in 42 coun- Traumatic Stress Disorder). A press


tries inquired children’s perception release by Save the Children (2020) METHOD
of the coronacrisis, their knowledge summarizing the results of a survey
on COVID-19 and the role the media among 1,002 parents and children in Based on a qualitative pre-study with
play in this. Germany revealed that 65% of the chil- 20 children between 10 and 15 years of
dren who responded reported feelings age in Germany, an international team
The COVID-19 pandemic is causing a of boredom and isolation. developed and revised a questionnaire.
worldwide crisis. Many countries have Children are dependent on the cir- The questionnaire comprised 19
closed schools and daycare centers, cumstances they grow up in, and these questions, mainly closed-ended, but
canceled events, imposed stay-at-home vary widely in different countries. But also featuring a few open-ended
orders, and closed borders. Everyday as the pandemic is global, it is creating questions. Our team translated the
activities like going to school, doing at least some similar circumstances questionnaire into 26 languages and
leisure activities, and meeting friends across many countries and regions. As programmed it into an online format
have been canceled and prohibited researchers interested in children’s lives using the SoSci Survey system. The
with short notice. and media engagement, we wondered: team then conducted the study in
A study of 101 children and teens in 13 How do children around the world per- 53 countries, with local teams within
countries conducted by World Vision ceive their situations in this pandemic? each country sampling and inviting
(2020) found that their everyday life What are their levels of knowledge and children between 9 and 13 years of
has changed significantly. As schools emotional statuses with respect to the age to participate, according to ethi-
are closed, many children report that coronavirus and crisis? And, what roles cal research requirements applying in
social distancing, isolation, and loneli- do the media play in their situations? each country.
ness have caused emotional hardship. In this unique situation, the Interna- In total, 9,563 children started and
In Germany, interviews with employees tional Central Institute for Youth and 4,267 children finished the online
and volunteers of helplines for children Educational Television (IZI) and the questionnaire plus 55 children in Cuba
(see Pütz in this issue) revealed that PRIX JEUNESSE Foundation – together who wrote their answers into a PDF
the situation is problematic and the with over 50 scholars and producers document. The data collection time
range of emotional distress is broad. worldwide – conducted an interna- was March 31th to April 26th 2020, i.e.
Likewise, a Chinese study (Liang et al., tional study of children in relation to the high peak of the shutdown in most
2020) found that 2 weeks following the COVID-19 and the media. Our goal was countries. Only countries with at least
outbreak of COVID-19, 2 out of 5 ado- to learn more about the challenges n=50 children were included in the
lescents and adults (14 to 35 years old) children are facing in this time of crisis analysis. The final sample includes 42
suffered from psychological problems, and their ways of coping with these countries on all continents and 4,322
and 14% showed signs of PTSD (Post- challenges. children, with a relatively balanced age

4 33/2020/E
RESEARCH

distribution (between 18% to stay at home and can’t


and 21% per age cohort).1 visit friends, except in Cuba
The number of children per and Taiwan, where this
country varied between applied to fewer children.
n=50 and n=698 (see also Half of the children (49%)
map with sample descrip- said that their parents
tion in this issue). don’t go to work anymore.
Please note that the sample Only a few reported feeling
cannot claim to be repre- sick themselves or having
sentative: It has a clear friends who felt sick.
bias towards middle-class, In their responses to our
© steklo/123rf

higher-educated house- open-ended questions,


holds since it required the children most often
participation from children mentioned that they were
with internet access on a bored, missed their friends,
home computer and good and described feelings of
reading skills to fill out the stress, anxiety, worry, and
questionnaire. Neverthe- frustration. As a nonbi-
less, the results demon- nary/gender-diverse child
strate some clear patterns reported:
that can inform parents “Anxiety issues and no
© Maria Symchych-Navrotska/123rf

and educators, as well as friends around to help.”


the work of researchers (UK, 12)
and producers of children’s A few children described
media. feeling under stress be-
For our analysis, we statisti- cause they had to do so
cally weighed the data many things, and that the
according to gender and worst thing was that they
country. We grouped the couldn’t get away from it
countries by world regions, all:
in consultation with local “That I can’t flee from
partners to ensure we made home when everything is
the right categorizations, getting too much.” (boy,
considering the regions of Austria, 12)
each country where the
© Pongsiri Onkhum/123rf

study had taken place. Some have to face


special ­challenges
THE ­RESULTS The circumstances the
Ill. 1: Children in 42 countries (ages 9-13) were asked how they perceive children are living in vary,
the coronacrisis, what they know about COVID-19 and what role the
Children’s situation media play in this situation
and more than 6 out of 10
under the shutdown (63%) reported living in
circumstances that make
“I haven’t been able to go their situations even more
out for a long time and it critical. More than a quar-
feels strange to be locked up 24/7.” ducted. 6 out of 10 (59%) said they were ter of these children reported that they
(boy, Chile, 11) doing online schooling; of those who are an only child with no siblings to play
were not, some reported a lack of in- with; this was particularly common
What has changed? ternet access, like in Cuba, or expensive among the 9-year-olds in our sample,
access, like in the Democratic Republic of whom more than a third (35%) re-
Most of the children who participated of the Congo, while in other cases, on- ported being an only child. More than 1
reported that they no longer attended line schooling was not offered. Most of in 5 children reported living in a home
school at the time our study was con- the children reported that they have they perceive as “very loud” and “very

33/2020/E 5
RESEARCH

small,” and they described Taiwan (86%) and the


that it was difficult for their lowest in Austria (35%)
parents to buy food, and/or and Germany (39%).
that they lived together with Fewer children, however,
someone belonging to a risk were worried that their
group. pet will get ill.
When children described More children who de-
their special challenges, scribed having to deal
some mentioned that their with special challenges
family was facing a difficult Ill. 2: Percentage of children who are “very much worried” on a 6-point reported feeling fearful
economic situation. For ex- scale (n=4,110) – a difference of up to
ample: “My mom’s salaries 10 percentage points –
are reduced because of the compared to children
less income of the company who do not live in a
she is working in.” (girl, Taiwan, 13) We also noted clear differences in small, loud and crowded place, for
Some named their family itself as chal- world regions (Ill. 2): While in Sub- example.
lenge, responding to our question by Saharan Africa 4 out of 10 children
just writing the family member’s role said they were “very worried,” around
such as “father” or “mother,” writ- 1 out of 10 did so in North America KNOWLEDGE ABOUT CORO-
ing “father” much more often than (8%), Oceania (9%) and Europe (10%). NA/COVID-19
“mother.” Some children (mainly boys) The highest percentage was found in
wrote about fighting more with their Tanzania (73%). 2 Where did the virus appear first?
brothers, and others reported that it
is too crowded at home: The most common fears We asked the children dif ferent
“We have a big family, everybody is at questions about their knowledge
home now all day, it’s hard to find a Children‘s greatest reported fears were on corona/COVID-19. Most chil-
place not to be troubled by someone.“ that a family member will fall ill and dren (97%) indicated that they
(boy, Russia, 10) that they won’t be able to visit grand- knew the country in which the
parents and other relatives for a long virus first appeared, and most of
Emotions time. These fears are found worldwide them (95%) were right, indicating
across our sample to a very similar China. In Cuba, 5 children wrote
More than a third (35%) of the chil- extent (Ill. 3). into the f ield “other“ another
dren perceived their parents as being 6 out of 10 children reported being country: USA. This may be due to
“very worried” about the situation. worried that they themselves fall the US embargo, which may make
The highest numbers in this context ill. The percentage highly differs children think that the USA is the
were found in Brazil (75%) and Egypt between countries and the highest aggressor and to be blamed for the
(68%), and the lowest numbers in rate was found in Japan (93%) and current crisis.
Denmark (9%) and Austria (9%). This
is similar to the results of a study on
the stress level of adults in differ-
ent countries, in which Denmark is
one of the countries with the lowest
reported stress level among adults
(Travaglino et al., 2020).
We asked our participants to assess
on a 6-point scale how worried they
felt personally. In response, a little bit
more than half of the children (52%)
reported feeling worried at least a bit,
while the other half indicated that
they were not worried (48%). 1 out of
5 children (20%) said they were “very lll. 3: The most common fears of children on the subject of the coronavirus (n=4,200)
much worried.”

6 33/2020/E
RESEARCH

Vulnerable groups touch your face with your hands” (748 children) gave 2 wrong answers
(18%), “avoid crowds” (18%) and “don’t and a minority answered 3, 4 or all
Regarding the children’s knowledge get too close to sick people” (15%). fake news incorrectly (3=292; 4=148;
on the groups most vulnerable to all=147).
COVID-19, 9 out of 10 children (92%) Fake news and rumors Overall, most children demonstrated
knew that adults aged 60 years or older some knowledge about the corona-
are considered an at-risk group. A little In our questionnaire, we also presented virus and vulnerable groups, could
bit less, but still the majority of the the children 5 statements – 4 rumors identify main symptoms and basic
children (76%), knew that people with and 1 fact – and we asked them measures to prevent themselves and
lung diseases are vulnerable. However, whether those statements were right others from getting infected (as far as
only half of the children knew that or wrong. 87% of the children correctly medical research knows to date), and
people with diabetes are more vul- responded that Ibuprofen can’t cure could identify rumors and fake news.
nerable than others. A relatively large COVID-19, with no visible age or gender Taken as a whole, our findings in this
number of children (17%) guessed that tendencies in their responses. Mean- area suggest that children aged 9 to
babies are especially vulnerable in this while, although 84% knew that garlic 13 would benefit from receiving more
context, and 12% of them think adults can’t prevent infection, 16% didn’t age-appropriate information on the
aged 40 to 59 years are at risk. know that this is fake news. COVID-19 pandemic. Parents and
8 out of 10 children knew that it is not teachers could help in this regard, but
Main symptoms true that spraying alcohol or chlorine we posit that children’s media could
all over your body will kill viruses that do more to serve this need, as well.
Most children could identify the symp- have already entered your body, yet 2 Note that this need is especially noted
toms of COVID-19. 9 out of 10 ticked out of 10 children didn’t know that this among our 9-year-old participants,
the answer “coughing,” and about is misinformation. We found similar who were significantly more likely to
8 out of 10 ticked “fever” (83%) and results regarding the rumor that the respond with incorrect answers, as
“breathing difficulties” (79%) as obvi- coronavirus is spread by a foreign well as participants from Sub-Saharan
ous symptoms. While we found hardly government as a weapon, with 79% Africa and Oceania, who offered the
any age differences with respect to of the children stating that this is false, highest numbers of incorrect answers
knowledge on “coughing” and “fever” and 21% indicating that it is true. In (compared with the lowest numbers
as frequent symptoms, the symptom this context, the 13-year-old age group found in Europe), suggesting different
“breathing difficulties” was a little bit believed this fake news more often levels of informational needs in differ-
less known among 9-year-olds (72%) than did the other age-groups (29%). ent countries, as well.
and better known among 13-year-olds We also presented a true statement
(82%). More than 1 out of 5 children that several coronaviruses have been Knowledge and worries
(23%) considered “sneezing” a main known for years, but that special one
symptom of COVID-19. is new. 7 out of 10 children correctly When combining our data regarding
identified this as a true statement. children’s knowledge and their fears,
How to protect yourself and others The majority of the children surveyed a tendency becomes quite clear: The
could identify fake news, but some children who described themselves
Around the topic “How to protect could not. Out of the 4,200 children, as “very worried” were more likely to
yourself and others from getting in- 1,444 identified all fake news cor- know fewer facts and answer more
fected?” we offered the children 14 an- rectly (34%). Another 1,421 children questions incorrectly. Meanwhile,
swer choices, 8 of which were right. We gave 1 wrong answer (34%). 18% among the children who answered all
asked the participants to the questions correctly,
choose 3 options. Their only 15% described them-
most common answers selves as “very worried”;
were “wash your hands” and of those children who
(90%) and “stay at home” answered every question
(70%), followed, at a incorrectly, the percentage
considerable distance, by of “very worried” children
“keeping distance” (38%), was 35% (Ill. 4).
“cover your mouth and We found the same
nose when coughing or Ill. 4: Percentage of “very worried” children in relation to knowledge tendencies in relation to
sneezing” (21%), “don’t their knowledge of fake

33/2020/E 7
RESEARCH

For our study, we asked with boredom; talking to friends and


the children who par- sharing interests in times of the shut-
ticipated to name 3 down; and finding school materials.
media (from a list with 7 out of 10 children reported using
21 options) that they media to access information and to
have been using more know what is going on; and 6 out of
during the coronavirus 10 children reported using media
pandemic. As illustrated to take their minds off from what is
in Ill. 5, nearly half of the going on (escapism). Fewer children
children (47%) chose reported using the media to develop
TV, with the 9-year-olds a daily schedule or to get away from
selecting this option their family. In each of these cases,
Ill. 5: Percentage of children reporting the 3 media they have more often (61%) than reports of using media to fulfil this
been using more since the COVID-19 lockdown did the 13-year-olds function were more common in
(39%). Comparing data older than younger children; girls
internationally, children more than boys; and children facing
in Sub-Saharan Africa special challenges more than children
news. The more rumors the children and the Arab/Persian region reported without those.
believed, the more they reported an increase in TV use the most (79%
feeling “very worried.” While this is and 61%). The highest percentages in a Media to regulate emotions
a correlation that does not necessar- cross-country comparison were found
ily indicate causality, meaning that in Cuba (91%) and in Tanzania (90%), Media also play a role in regulating
there is a relationship between the where 9 out of 10 children reported emotions. Half of the children re-
2, but we don’t know if one causes watching more TV now than they had ported using media when they feel
the other, we can hypothesize from previously. sad. Nearly 6 in 10 reported doing so
this pattern that for children, being when they are lonely, and more than
well-informed and knowledgeable a third when feeling scared. 8 out of
may be linked with a reduction in THE FUNCTIONS OF MEDIA 10 children reported feeling calmed
uncertainty and thus in reduction of IN TIMES OF LOCKDOWN by communicating with friends and
worried feelings. relatives through social media (e.g.,
Children reported 3 major reasons Instagram, WhatsApp). Also, from
for turning to media during the the children’s subjective perspec-
WHICH MEDIA ARE DEFINITE- lockdown, as shown in Ill. 6: dealing tives, they reported that watching
LY USED MORE IN TIMES OF
LOCKDOWN?
Obviously, under the circumstances of
this global pandemic, most children
have to stay at home. In that environ-
ment, many become dependent on
media as sources of or facilitators
of their education, information, and
entertainment.
In their yearly trend report, conducted at
the same time as this study, the market
research company Dubit found that with
children in the UK, the use of television,
smartphones, streaming services and so-
cial media etc. increased by around 15 to
20 percentage points. Parents reported
feeling less concerned about the media
use of their children than they did the Ill. 6: Percentage of children agreeing for what reason they use media more
year before (Dubit, 2020).

8 33/2020/E
RESEARCH

TV or YouTube relaxed them from


stress. SUMMARY: THE NEED FOR NOTES
Studies indicate that while media AGE-APPROPRIATE AND 1
Argentina, Australia, Austria, Belgium, Brazil,
can play a great role in children’s ­RELIABLE INFORMATION Canada, Chile, Colombia, Cuba, Czech Republic,
identity work (Götz, 2014), it may Democratic Republic of the Congo, Denmark, Ec-
uador, Egypt, France, Germany, Iran, Israel, Italy,
also contribute to negative effects In this period of lockdown, children Japan, Lebanon, Malaysia, Mexico, Poland, Russia,
such as emotional stress (e.g., Abi- face an extraordinary situation. Many Singapore, Spain, Switzerland, Taiwan, Thailand, Tur-
key, USA, Dominican Republic, India, New Zealand,
Jaoude et al., 2020; Hoge et al., 2017), everyday routines and activities, such Philippines, Slovenia, South Africa, Syria, Tanzania,
especially when used too long (e.g., as attending school or participating in Nigeria, United Kingdom

MacDool et al., 2016). From the chil- leisure activities, are no longer possible. Background circumstances may be, among other
2

things, inconsistent actions. For example, schools


dren’s perspective, however, these Now, children must stay at home. This are closed and the international air traffic is stopped,
yet adults as far as possible still go to work as usual
effects are difficult to see. For them, sometimes means living in narrow and and use crowded public transport; church services
media serve as information carriers, loud circumstances, causing much and funerals still take place. The Tanzanian govern-
ment doubts that protective clothing is effective and
facilitate remote schooling, and offer more tension within families. announced that praying was better than wearing
a “COVID-19-safe” way of staying in One-third of the children in our study protection masks (Ohikere, 2020).
contact with friends. Given media’s perceived their parents as “very wor-
double-edged functions, children ried,” and half of the children expressed
need competencies and media feeling worried themselves, with the REFERENCES
literacy skills to manage their own biggest fear being the possibility of
media use. COVID-19 infection among family or Abi-Jaoude, Elia, Naylor, Karline Treurnicht & Pigna-
friends. Most children demonstrated tiello, Antonio (2020). Smartphones, social media use
and youth mental health. Canadian Medical Asso-
Media for information about a basic knowledge about coronavirus ciation Journal (CMAJ), 192(6), 136-141. Available at:
and how they can protect themselves https://www.cmaj.ca/content/cmaj/192/6/E136.full.
coronavirus pdf [18.5.20]
and others. Further, most children Dubit (2020). Kids and media in the time of corona.
Nearly half of the children in our could identify rumors and fake news, Unpublished report, April 24, 2020. London: Dubit.
study reported feeling fed up with although a proportion of the children Götz, Maya (2014). TV-Hero(in)es of boys and girls.
Reception studies of favorite characters. Frankfurt:
news on the coronavirus and stated we surveyed could not. Peter Lang.
that they didn’t want to see, hear Faced with extraordinary times, chil- Hoge, Elizabeth, Bickham, David & Cantor,
or read anything more about it. We dren reported using media more than Joanne (2017). Digital media, anxiety, and depression
in children. Pediatrics, 140(2), 76-80. Available at:
noted the largest numbers of such before, especially to beat boredom, do https://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/140/
responses in our sample from Iran their homework, stay in contact with Supplement_2/S76 [18.5.20]
(72%), and the lowest in our Singa- friends, get information, and regulate Liang, Leilei et al. (2020). The effect of COVID-19 on
youth mental health. Psychiatric Quarterly, April 21,
pore sample (15%). their emotions. These findings demon- 2020. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub-
Nearly half of the children who par- strate that children’s media are bearing med/32319041 [18.5.20]

ticipated agreed that news on coro- a great responsibility, serving impor- MacDool, Emily, Powell, Philip, Roberts, Jennifer &
Taylor, Karl (2016). Social media use and children’s
navirus scares them. In fact, 4 out of tant functions for children during the wellbeing. Bonn: Institute of Labor Economics (IZA).
10 children reported avoiding news COVID-19 crisis. Available at: http://ftp.iza.org/dp10412.pdf [18.5.20]

about C ­ OVID-19 because they feel We wish to emphasize the results Ohikere, Onize (2020). Tanzania’s troubling response
to COVID-19 International. The country’s leadership
that the news fosters their worries. dealing with children’s knowledge faces criticism for its reaction to the pandemic. World.
We found the highest percentage of and worries, because we observed a Posted 5/08/20. Available at: https://world.wng.org/
content/tanzania_s_troubling_response_to_covid_19
such responses in the Arab/Persian correlation between more accurate [18.5.20]
world (62%) and the lowest in the Asian knowledge about the coronavirus and Save the Children (Ed.) (2020). Corona in Deutschland:
Was Kindern und Eltern Sorgen macht. 16.04.2020.
region (22%). fewer reports of feeling “very worried.” Available at: https://www.savethechildren.de/news/
At the same time, more than 6 out These findings underline that children’s corona-in-deutschland-was-kindern-und-eltern-sor-
gen-macht [18.5.20]
of 10 children (65%) stated that they media have the potential to play an
Travaglino, Giovanni, Lieberoth, Andreas & Coll Mar-
wished that children’s television important role in this pandemic by tín, Tao (2020). How is the COVID19 Pandemic affect-
talked more about the coronavirus. helping children to 1) cope with this ing Europeans’ lives? A report of the COVIDiSTRESS
global survey. Report on data collection March 30th
We found the highest such numbers special situation in age-appropriate, - April 20th.
in Tanzania (92%) and the Dominican readily-understandable ways; 2) de- World Vision (Ed.) (2020). Children’s voices in times
Republic (89%), where the children’s velop understandings of responsible of COVID-19. Continued child activism in the face
of personal challenges. World Vision International.
news Notichicos is the only local behavior for themselves and towards Available at: https://www.worldvision.de/sites/worldvi-
production, the rest is bought-in others; and 3) to react appropriately sion.de/files/pdf/WV_report_childrens_voices_in%20
times_of_COVID-19%20Final.pdf [18.5.20]
entertainment. in the given situation.

33/2020/E 9
View publication stats

You might also like