Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Developmental Science 10:2 (2007), pp 183– 189 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00548.

REPORT
Blackwell Publishing Ltd

Crawling is associated with more flexible memory retrieval


by 9-month-old infants
Jane Herbert,1 Julien Gross2 and Harlene Hayne2
1. Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, UK
2. Department of Psychology, University of Otago, New Zealand

Abstract
In the present experiment, we used a deferred imitation paradigm to explore the effect of crawling on memory retrieval by
9-month-old human infants. Infants observed an experimenter demonstrate a single target action with a novel object and their
ability to reproduce that action was assessed after a 24-hr delay. Some infants were tested with the demonstration stimulus in
the demonstration context and some infants were tested with a different stimulus in a different context. Half of the infants in
each test condition were crawling at the time of participation and half were not. Both crawling and non-crawling infants exhibited
retention when tested with the demonstration stimulus in the demonstration context, but only infants who were crawling by
9 months of age exhibited retention when tested with a different stimulus in a different context. These findings demonstrate
that the onset of independent locomotion is associated with more flexible memory retrieval during the first year of life.

Introduction the mobile conjugate reinforcement paradigm (Hayne,


Greco, Earley, Griesler & Rovee-Collier, 1986; Sullivan,
During the course of daily life, we typically encounter Rovee-Collier & Tynes, 1979). For example, Hayne et al.
stimuli that cue retrieval of prior memories. For most trained 3-month-old infants to kick their feet to produce
normal adults, the range of effective retrieval cues is movement in a five-object mobile. When infants were
remarkably broad. A scent, a word, or a picture can tested 24 hrs later, those who were tested with the origi-
activate highly detailed memories of experiences that nal training mobile exhibited perfect retention, but those
took place hours, days, or even years earlier. In some who were tested with a mobile that contained more than
instances, the cue that initiates retrieval may bear little one novel object exhibited no retention at all. The highly
resemblance to the original experience. Furthermore, the specific nature of infant memory retrieval has been
complex way in which our memories are organized replicated under a number of different conditions. For
means that retrieving one memory may cue retrieval of example, following training in the mobile conjugate
other, related memories as well. The flexible nature of reinforcement paradigm, the absence of a distinctive
adult memory retrieval is a key ingredient in many odor that was present during training has been shown to
highly sophisticated cognitive skills, allowing us to impair memory retrieval, and substituting the training
access and use our memories in circumstances that are odor with another distinctive odor eliminates memory
very different from those in which the memories were retrieval altogether (Rubin, Fagen & Carroll, 1998).
originally established. Further research has shown that the specificity of
Although memory retrieval by human adults is highly infant memory retrieval originally documented in the
flexible, memory retrieval by human infants is not (for a mobile conjugate reinforcement paradigm is not simply
review, see Hayne, 2004). A large body of research has a reflection of the operant training procedure that is
shown that, for infants, memory retrieval occurs if and used to establish the memory in the first place. For
only if the cues encountered at the time of retrieval example, deferred imitation involves an observation-only
match the cues present at the time of encoding almost learning procedure whereby infants watch an experi-
exactly. The requisite relation between encoding and menter perform a series of actions with objects and the
retrieval during infancy was originally demonstrated in infants’ ability to reproduce those actions is assessed

Address for correspondence: Jane Herbert, Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S10 2TN, United Kingdom; e-mail:
J.S.Herbert@sheffield.ac.uk

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and
350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.
184 Jane Herbert et al.

after a delay. Deferred imitation tasks are widely used in Robinson & Pascalis, 2004), but the precise timetable for
studies of infant memory development and are considered change is unique to the task. Furthermore, within the
to yield a nonverbal measure of declarative memory deferred imitation task in particular, making the memory
(McDonough, Mandler, McKee & Squire, 1995; for a demands less difficult by allowing infants the opportu-
review, see Hayne, 2004). Memory performance by young nity to practice the target actions prior to the test (Bauer
infants in the deferred imitation task, like memory per- & Dow, 1994; Hayne, 2006; Hayne, Barr & Herbert, 2003)
formance by young infants tested in operant paradigms, or by increasing the similarity between the demonstration
is also disrupted by a change in the target stimulus at the and the test stimuli (Hayne, MacDonald & Barr, 1997)
time of retrieval. In one study, Hayne, Boniface and Barr contracts the timetable over which changes in flexibility
(2000) found that 6-month-old infants who watched as are typically observed (cf. Hayne et al., 2000; Herbert &
an experimenter performed a three-step sequence of Hayne, 2000).
actions using a puppet failed to reproduce these actions In addition to changes related to age, memory
when they were tested 24 hours later if the color or the retrieval is also facilitated by experiences provided by an
form of the puppet was different from that encountered adult during the original learning episode. In Herbert
during the original demonstration. Similarly, Herbert and Hayne (2000), for example, the experimenter pro-
and Hayne (2000) found that 18-month-old infants who vided a novel verbal label for the stimuli at the time of
watched as an experimenter performed a three-step the original demonstration. Infants were then re-presented
sequence of actions to make a rattle or an animal failed with the same verbal label when they were tested with
to reproduce those actions if the stimuli were changed the novel stimulus. At 24 months of age, infants were
at the time of the test. able to use this verbal information to facilitate memory
The highly specific nature of infant memory retrieval retrieval, but 18-month-olds were not. Subsequent
also extends to cues associated with the environmental research has shown that although memory retrieval is
context in which encoding originally occurred. In the not facilitated by verbal information at 18 months of age
mobile conjugate reinforcement paradigm, for example, (Herbert & Hayne, 2000), it is facilitated by physical
changes to proximal contextual cues such as the color experience with the stimuli during encoding (Hayne et al.,
and pattern of a crib liner that was present during 2003). Similarly, flexible memory retrieval is enhanced in
training (Borovsky & Rovee-Collier, 1990; Butler & 3-month-olds by exposing them to different mobiles or
Rovee-Collier, 1989), or to more distal contextual cues to different contexts during original encoding (Amabile
such as the room in which training occurs (Hayne, & Rovee-Collier, 1991; Fagen, Morrongiello, Rovee-
Rovee-Collier & Borza, 1991) disrupts memory retrieval Collier & Gekoski, 1984; Greco, Hayne & Rovee-Collier,
by 3- to 6-month-olds. Similarly, in the deferred imitation 1990; Rovee-Collier & DuFault, 1991).
task, 6-month-old infants fail to retrieve a memory Taken together, a number of studies have shown that
acquired in one physical location (e.g. the laboratory) experiences provided by an adult during a learning
when they are tested in a different location (e.g. their session can facilitate the flexibility of subsequent memory
own home) even when all other aspects of the target retrieval by infant participants. In contrast, considerably
objects remain the same (Hayne et al., 2000). less is known about the effect of prior experience that
Over the course of human development, the high infants bring to the experimental setting. During the
degree of specificity that characterizes infant memory course of their daily lives, infants undoubtedly accumul-
retrieval gradually declines. In the deferred imitation ate different experiences that might contribute to indi-
task, for example, 6-month-old infants fail to retrieve the vidual differences in memory retrieval. One potential
target memory when they are tested in a different con- source of individual differences in experience might be
text, but 12-month-old infants exhibit the same level of the onset of independent locomotion. As infants begin
performance irrespective of the context at the time of the to crawl, their ability to explore the environment increases
test (Hayne et al., 2000). Furthermore, 12-month-olds dramatically. Many researchers have argued that this
fail to retrieve the target memory if the characteristics increased opportunity for exploration may contribute to
of the focal cue (e.g. the puppet) are changed, but when advances in other areas of psychological development
faced with the same change in cue, 18-month-olds (Adolph, 1997; Gibson, 1988). In fact, a growing body
exhibit excellent memory performance. A similar age- of research has shown that infants’ social, emotional,
related increase in the flexibility of memory retrieval is and cognitive skills do change once they learn to crawl
observed in other infant memory tasks such as operant (for a review, see Campos, Anderson, Barbu-Roth,
conditioning paradigms (e.g. Hartshorn, Rovee-Collier, Hubbard, Hertenstein & Witherington, 2000). With
Gerhardstein, Bhatt, Klein, Aaron, Wondolski & Wurtzel, respect to memory retrieval in particular, as infants
1998) and the visual paired comparison procedure (e.g. begin to crawl, they are faced with the new challenge of

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


Independent locomotion 185

Figure 1 The cow (left) and duck (right) stimuli that were used in the experiment.

transferring information acquired in one situation to groups did not differ. Infants’ crawling status was ini-
similar (but not identical) problems encountered in tially established by parental report and was confirmed
another. In light of this new challenge, Rovee-Collier by experimenter observation at the time of the original
(1996) has hypothesized that memory retrieval may demonstration and again at the time of the test. Crawl-
become more flexible as infants learn to crawl; in essence, ing was defined as the ability to continuously transverse
as infants begin to explore their environment on their at least one meter using arms and/or knees. In no
own, they rapidly learn a more general rule that some instance was there a discrepancy between parents’
changes are more (or less) important than others. The reports of their infant’s crawling status and what the
present experiment was designed to test the hypothesis experimenter observed during the experimental session.
that the onset of independent locomotion is associated This high degree of reliability is consistent with recent
with more flexible memory retrieval. In order to provide research by Bodnarchuk and Eaton (2004) showing that
a particularly challenging test of the flexibility of memory parents provide reliable reports of their infants’ attain-
retrieval, we changed both the target stimulus and the ment of motor milestones, including crawling.
context at the time of the test.
Apparatus
Method The stimuli consisted of a black and white cow and an
orange duck that were constructed specifically for these
experiments and were not commercially available (see
Participants
Figure 1). Both stimuli were 60 cm in height and con-
Ninety-six 9-month-old infants (48 male, 48 female) sisted of a wooden head attached vertically to a wooden
who were born within 2 weeks of their due date were base. A wooden button (6 cm in diameter) was recessed
recruited from public birth records and by word of into the center of the wooden base. The button was
mouth. All infants were tested within 2 weeks of their either white or orange and matched the color of the cow
9-month birthdays. Infants were predominantly of Euro- or duck respectively. The center of the button was
pean descent and came from a range of socioeconomic marked with a 1 cm in diameter circle made of black felt.
backgrounds. Five additional infants were excluded When the button was pressed, the cow stimulus ‘mooed’
from the final sample due to scheduling difficulties, and the duck stimulus ‘quacked’ for approximately 6 s.
maternal interference, failure to touch the experimental A small LED in each eye also flashed for approximately
stimulus, or crawling backwards as their primary means 6 s. The stimuli (cow or duck) were counterbalanced
of independent locomotion. within and between groups.
At the time of their participation, half of the infants
were crawling and half were not. The average age of the
Procedure
infants in the crawling (M chronological age = 9.13
months, SD = .18; M gestational age = 78.99 weeks, SD Infants were randomly assigned to one of two demon-
= 1.43) and noncrawling (M chronological age = 9.14, stration conditions (n = 64; 32 males, 32 females) or
SD = .13; M gestational age = 78.85 weeks, SD = 1.35) to the non-demonstration control condition (n = 32;

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


186 Jane Herbert et al.

16 males, 16 females). All infants were tested at a time infants, the test occurred in the laboratory ( n = 16;
of the day that the caregiver identified as an alert/play 8 males, 8 females) and for the remaining half, the test
period. During the initial visit, the purpose of the study occurred in their own home (n = 16; 8 males, 8 females).
was explained to the caregiver, and informed consent
was obtained.
Results
Demonstration condition
Each videotaped session was scored by two independent
For half of the infants, the demonstration occurred in observers, one of whom was unaware of the infant’s
the laboratory (n = 32; 16 males, 16 females) and for the experimental condition or crawling status. Each observer
remaining half, the demonstration occurred in their own scored the presence or absence of the target action
home (n = 32; 16 males, 16 females). The female experi- during a 90-s test period. A button press was coded as
menter was positioned directly opposite the infant and present if it resulted in the stimulus producing an
both were seated on the floor. At the beginning of the audible sound. Infants were given credit only for the first
demonstration and the test session, the experimenter time that they produced the target action. Interobserver
interacted with the infant for 5 minutes or until a smile reliability, calculated using percentage of agreement, was
was elicited. If the infant failed to orient toward the 100%.
experimenter, the experimenter attracted his or her Preliminary analyses indicated that there was no
attention by saying ‘look’ and using the infant’s name. difference in infants’ performance with the two sets of
For infants in the demonstration condition, the experi- stimuli; therefore data were averaged across both stimuli
menter demonstrated the novel action with one version for all subsequent analyses.
of the stimulus (cow or duck). The action was modeled Figure 2 shows the number of infants who imitated
three times in succession out of reach of the infant. The the target action as a function of crawling status and test
experimenter did not verbally describe the action or group (control, no change, cue/context change). Within
label the stimulus. The entire demonstration session lasted each crawling status, separate chi-square tests were con-
approximately 30 seconds. ducted across test group; significant group differences
Independent groups of crawling and non-crawling were further analysed using Fisher’s Exact Test. Retention
infants were tested after a 24-hr delay (± 2 hours). was inferred when more infants in a given demonstration
Within each crawling-status condition, infants were condition performed the target action than did infants
randomly assigned to one of two test groups (8 males, in the non-demonstration control condition. As shown
8 females per group). One group was tested with the
demonstration stimulus in the demonstration context
(No Change group). The other group was tested with a
different stimulus in a different context (Cue/Context
Change group). During the test, the female experimenter
was again positioned directly opposite the infant and
both were seated on the floor. The stimulus was placed
within the infant’s reach and his/her behavior was vide-
otaped for a 90-s period. The duration of the test period
was based on prior research on deferred imitation with
infants less than 1 year old (Barr, Dowden & Hayne,
1996; Hayne et al., 2000).

Non-demonstration control condition


To assess the spontaneous production of target actions
in the absence of adult demonstration, additional con-
trol groups of crawling and non-crawling infants were
exposed to the stimulus for the first time during the test. Figure 2 The number of infants who performed the target
For these 32 infants (16 males, 16 females), the test was action as a function of crawling-status condition and
also scheduled within 2 weeks of their 9-month birthdays. experimental group. An asterisk indicates that the performance
The test procedure was identical to that experienced by of a demonstration group is significantly above the performance
infants in the demonstration condition. For half of the of the status-matched non-demonstration control group.

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


Independent locomotion 187

in Figure 2 (see asterisks), non-crawling infants only target task. In the present experiment, for example,
exhibited retention when they were tested with the infants were specifically prevented from crawling during
same stimulus in the same context (X 2 [2] = 8.52, p < .05, the demonstration and during the test.
Fisher’s Exact Test, p < .05). Crawling infants, on the The present data add to a growing body of research
other hand, exhibited retention when they were tested showing that independent locomotion enhances at least
with the same stimulus in the same context and when some aspects of cognitive development (Bertenthal, Campos
they were tested with a different stimulus in a different & Barrett, 1984; Clearfield, 2004; Gustafson, 1984; Ker-
context (X 2 [2] = 14.40, p < .001, Fisher’s Exact Test, moian & Campos, 1988; for a review, see Campos et al.,
p < .01). 2000). Across each of these cognitive domains, Campos
et al. (2000) have argued that crawling is neither a neces-
sary nor a sufficient condition to produce developmental
Discussion change; rather they propose that the onset of independent
locomotion yields a range of new experiences that help
Taken together, these data provide the first empirical push infants’ extant skills to a new level. Within the
support for Rovee-Collier’s (1996) hypothesis that domain of memory retrieval, independent locomotion
independent locomotion is associated with more flexible undoubtedly provides infants with new opportunities to
memory retrieval. Although both crawling and non- retrieve their memories in a range of different situations.
crawling infants exhibited excellent retention when tested We hypothesize that these particular experiences facilitate
with the demonstration stimuli in the demonstration memory retrieval more generally. Partial support for
context, only infants with crawling experience exhibited this hypothesis comes from experimental research in
retention when tested with a different stimulus in a which infants are given specific (albeit brief) experience
different context. The manipulation of both the cue and with novel test cues. Under these conditions, solving one
the context at the time of the test provided a particularly memory retrieval problem helps infants to solve addi-
challenging memory retrieval problem, yet crawling tional, more difficult memory retrieval problems (Hayne,
infants exhibited no difficulty at all. Whether crawling 2006). This research has shown that infants do not
infants would exhibit a similar retrieval advantage if require large amounts of additional experience to push
only the cue or the context (but not both) were changed them to a new level of performance. As such, crawling
at the time of the test awaits further research. Similarly, would provide more than enough additional experience
further research is also required to determine whether to push infants to a new level of memory flexibility.
crawling infants would exhibit a similar retrieval advantage In the final analysis, the answer to the question of
if the imitation task was even more difficult than the mechanism rests on the answers to at least two addi-
one we used here (e.g. multiple target actions, a longer tional questions. First, how much crawling experience is
delay). required for performance to improve on a particular
Given that we have now shown that crawling is task? In the present experiment, as in others, crawling
associated with more flexible memory retrieval, the next was treated as a dichotomous variable – infants had
obvious question to answer is why? At this stage in our either achieved independent locomotion or they had
research, we can rule out at least two simple explana- not. On the basis of maternal report, crawling experience
tions for the relation between crawling and memory varied widely; some infants had been crawling for as
retrieval. First, assuming that infants of the same chrono- long as 4 months, others for only 1 week. We are currently
logical (and gestational) age are at the same level of conducting a prospective study of the role of locomotor
maturity, the differences observed in the present experi- experience in cognitive development in which caregivers
ment cannot be attributed to maturation alone because have kept detailed daily records of their infant’s locomotor
the age of the infants was the same irrespective of their experience. This ongoing study will help us to answer
crawling status. Second, although the beneficial effects the question of how much crawling experience infants
of active exploration within the context of an experiment require.
have been well established (Bensen & Uzgiris, 1985; Finally, how long do these differences last? In other
Feldman & Acredolo, 1979; for review, see Yan, Thomas words, at what point does the performance of the non-
& Downing, 1998), this kind of proximal mechanism crawling infants catch up with that of their crawling
cannot explain the effect of crawling reported here. In counterparts? Furthermore, do the facilitative effects of
the present experiment, and in others, infants were not crawling extend to other forms of locomotor experience?
allowed to engage in exploratory behavior during the That is, do infants who walk early have a cognitive
task per se; rather, independent locomotion outside advantage over infants who walk later when they are
the experimental context enhanced performance on the tested with age-appropriate imitation tasks that are more

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


188 Jane Herbert et al.

difficult than those used here (cf. Herbert & Hayne, Bodnarchuk, J.L., & Eaton, W. (2004). Can parent reports be
2000)? If so, at what point do the late walkers catch up? trusted? Validity of daily checklists of gross motor mile-
Again, our ongoing longitudinal research on the relation stone attainment. Applied Developmental Psychology, 25,
between motor development and cognitive development 481–490.
Borovsky, D., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1990). Contextual constraints
will help us to answer this question as well.
on memory retrieval at six months. Child Development, 61,
In conclusion, although our memories comprise a
1569–1583.
record of the past, their most important function is to Butler, J., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1989). Contextual gating of
guide behavior in the future. In order to achieve this memory retrieval. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 533–
function, we must be able to retrieve our memories in 552.
situations that are different from those in which they Campos, J.J., Anderson, D.I., Barbu-Roth, M.A., Hubbard,
were initially established. Young infants’ ability to W.M., Hertenstein, M.J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel
accomplish this task is extremely limited, but over the broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149–219.
course of development, older infants begin to exploit Clearfield, M.W. (2004). The role of crawling and walking
retrieval cues that differ from those encountered at the experience in infant spatial memory. Journal of Experimental
time of original encoding. Previous research has identi- Child Psychology, 89, 214–241.
Fagen, J.W., Morrongiello, B.A., Rovee-Collier, C., & Gekoski,
fied at least three variables that enhance the flexibility of
M.J. (1984). Expectancies and memory retrieval in 3-month-
infant memory retrieval (Hayne et al., 2003; Herbert &
old infants and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
Hayne, 2000): the age of the infant, the complexity of chology, 22, 272–281.
the task, and additional experience provided by an Feldman, A., & Acredolo, L. (1979). The effect of active versus
adult. The results of the present experiment suggest that passive exploration on memory for spatial location in chil-
crawling experience can now be added to this list. dren. Child Development, 50, 698–704.
Gibson, E.J. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development
of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge.
Acknowledgements Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 1–41.
Gustafson, G.E. (1984). Effects of the ability to locomote on
This research was supported by a Marsden grant from infants’ social and exploratory behaviors: an experimental
study. Developmental Psychology, 20, 397–405.
the Royal Society of New Zealand to Harlene Hayne.
Greco, C., Hayne, H., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1990). Roles of
The authors would like to thank Jackie Clearwater and
function, reminding, and variability in categorization by
Bridget McDonald for help with data collection, Brendon 3-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Sturgeon for constructing the stimuli, and the infants and Memory, and Cognition, 16, 617–633.
parents who participated in this project. Hartshorn, K., Rovee-Collier, C., Gerhardstein, P., Bhatt,
R.S., Klein, P.J., Aaron, F., Wondoloski, T.L., & Wurtzel, N.
(1998). Developmental changes in the specificity of memory
References over the first year of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 33,
61–78.
Adolph, K.E. (1997). Learning in the development of infant Hayne, H. (2004). Infant memory development: implica-
locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child tions for childhood amnesia. Developmental Review, 24, 33–
Development, 62 (3, Serial No. 251). 73.
Amabile, T.A., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1991). Contextual varia- Hayne, H. (2006). Bridging the gap: the relation between
tion and memory retrieval at 6 months. Child Development, learning and memory during infancy. In M.H. Johnson &
62, 1155 –1166. Y. Munakata (Eds.), Attention and performance XXI: Pro-
Barr, R., Dowden, A., & Hayne, H. (1996). Developmental cesses of change in brain and cognitive development (pp. 209–
changes in deferred imitation by 6- to 24-month-old infants. 231). London: Oxford University Press.
Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 159 –170. Hayne, H., Barr, R., & Herbert, J. (2003). The effect of prior
Bauer, P.J., & Dow, G. (1994). Episodic memory in 16- and practice on memory reactivation and generalization. Child
20-month-old children: specifics are generalized but not Development, 74, 1615–1627.
forgotten. Developmental Psychology, 30, 403 – 417. Hayne, H., Boniface, J., & Barr, R. (2000). Age-related
Bensen, J.B., & Uzgiris, I.C. (1985). Effect of self-initiated changes in the specificity of memory retrieval: implications
locomotion on infant search activity. Developmental Psy- for cognitive development. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 77–
chology, 21, 923 – 931. 83.
Bertenthal, B.I., Campos, J.J., & Barrett, K. (1984). Self- Hayne, H., Greco, C., Earley, L., Griesler, P., & Rovee-Collier,
produced locomotion: an organizer of emotional, cognitive, C. (1986). Ontogeny of early event memory: II. Encoding
and social developments in infancy. In R. Emde & R. Harmon and retrieval by 2- and 3-month-olds. Infant Behavior and
(Eds.), Continuities and discontinuities in development Development, 9, 461–472.
(pp. 175–210). New York: Plenum. Hayne, H., MacDonald, S., & Barr, R. (1997). Developmental

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


Independent locomotion 189

changes in the specificity of memory over the second year of Rovee-Collier, C. (1996). Shifting the focus from what to why?
life. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 237–249. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 385–400.
Hayne, H., Rovee-Collier, C., & Borza, M. (1991). Infant memory Rovee-Collier, C., & DuFault, D. (1991). Multiple contexts
for place information. Memory and Cognition, 19, 378–386. and memory retrieval at 3 months. Developmental Psychobi-
Herbert, J., & Hayne, H. (2000). Memory retrieval by 18–30- ology, 24, 39–49.
month-olds: age-related changes in representational flexibility. Rubin, G.B., Fagen, J.W., & Carroll, M.H. (1998). Olfactory
Developmental Psychology, 36, 473 – 484. context and memory retrieval in 3-month-old infants. Infant
Kermoian, R., & Campos, J.J. (1988). Locomotor experience: Behavior and Development, 21, 641–658.
a facilitator of spatial cognitive development. Child Develop- Sullivan, M.W., Rovee-Collier, C.K., & Tynes, D.M. (1979).
ment, 59, 908–917. A conditioning analysis of infant long-term memory. Child
McDonough, L., Mandler, J.M., McKee, R.D., & Squire, L.R. Development, 50, 152–162.
(1995). The deferred imitation task as a nonverbal measure Yan, J.H., Thomas, J.R., & Downing, J.H. (1998). Locomotion
of declarative memory. Proceedings of the National Academy improves children’s spatial search: a meta-analytic review.
of Sciences, 92, 7580 –7584. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 67–82.
Robinson, A.J., & Pascalis, O. (2004). Development of flexible
visual recognition memory in human infants. Developmental Received: 4 July 2005
Science, 7, 527– 533. Accepted: 17 March 2006

© 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

You might also like